ArticlePDF Available

Use of appeasement display and recruitment by an adult Striated Caracara (Phalcoboenus australis) to overcome territorial defense

Authors:
Use of appeasement display and recruitment by an adult
Striated Caracara (Phalcoboenus australis) to overcome
territorial defense
Authors: Harrington, Katie J., and Meiburg, Jonathan
Source: The Wilson Journal of Ornithology, 133(1) : 127-131
Published By: The Wilson Ornithological Society
URL: https://doi.org/10.1676/20-00060
BioOne Complete (complete.BioOne.org) is a full-text database of 200 subscribed and open-access titles
in the biological, ecological, and environmental sciences published by nonprofit societies, associations,
museums, institutions, and presses.
Your use of this PDF, the BioOne Complete website, and all posted and associated content indicates your
acceptance of BioOne’s Terms of Use, available at www.bioone.org/terms-of-use.
Usage of BioOne Complete content is strictly limited to personal, educational, and non - commercial use.
Commercial inquiries or rights and permissions requests should be directed to the individual publisher as
copyright holder.
BioOne sees sustainable scholarly publishing as an inherently collaborative enterprise connecting authors, nonprofit
publishers, academic institutions, research libraries, and research funders in the common goal of maximizing access to
critical research.
Downloaded From: https://bioone.org/journals/The-Wilson-Journal-of-Ornithology on 03 Nov 2021
Terms of Use: https://bioone.org/terms-of-useAccess provided by University of Ottawa
The Wilson Journal of Ornithology 133(1):127–131, 2021 DOI: 10.1676/20-00060
Use of appeasement display and recruitment by an adult Striated Caracara
(Phalcoboenus australis) to overcome territorial defense
Katie J. Harrington
1
* and Jonathan Meiburg
2
ABSTRACT—Adult animals across many taxa use
appeasement signaling to minimize or avoid costly conflict
in social settings by jointly communicating their consent to
defer and an interest to remain. When foraging, however,
appeasement may not be enough to gain access to a resource
controlled by a dominant conspecific. In this case,
conspecific recruitment can shift the social dynamic, so the
resource becomes accessible to all. Striated Caracara
(Phalcoboenus australis) are Near Threatened scavenging
falconids that breed in high densities on the extreme
southern coasts of South America and the Falkland
Islands. While juvenile caracaras recruit other caracaras to
concentrated food resources, neither recruitment nor
appeasement has been reported in adults. Here we describe
a female adult caracara that appeared to use both
appeasement signaling and recruitment to access food in
another pair’s defended territory on New Island, Falkland
Islands. Appeasement and recruitment calls are one of
several behavioral parallels that researchers have noted
among Striated Caracara, Common Raven (Corvus corax),
and Kea (Nestor notabilis). Although falconids are not
typically considered in comparative studies of avian
cognition and social behavior, we suggest that caracaras
merit further attention, especially given their relationship to
parrots in the clade Eufalconimorphae. Received 11 May
2020. Accepted 9 June 2021.
Key words: Eufalconimorphae, Falconidae, foraging,
scramble competition, territory, vocal repertoire.
Uso de un despliegue de apaciguamiento y reclutamiento
de un adulto del caracara Phalcoboenus australis para
sobreponerse a defensa territorial
RESUMEN (Spanish)—Los adultos de muchos taxa usan se ˜
nales
de apaciguamiento para minimizar o evitar el costoso conflicto en
situaciones sociales, comunicando su consentimiento para ceder y un
1
Acopian Center for Conservation Learning, Hawk
Mountain Sanctuary, Orwigsburg, PA, USA
2
Dripping Springs, TX, USA
* Corresponding author: katiejharrington@gmail.com
127Short Communications
Downloaded From: https://bioone.org/journals/The-Wilson-Journal-of-Ornithology on 03 Nov 2021
Terms of Use: https://bioone.org/terms-of-useAccess provided by University of Ottawa
inter´
es de permanecer. Sin embargo, cuando esta´n forrajeando, la
se˜
nal de apaciguamiento podr´
ıa no ser suficiente para tener acceso a
un recurso controlado por un conespec´
ıfico dominante. En este caso,
el reclutamiento podr´
ıa modificar la dina´mica social para que el
recurso est´
e disponible para todos. Los caracaras Phalcoboenus
australis son falc´
onidos carro˜
neros Casi Amenazados que anidan a
altas densidades en las costas extremas del sur de Sudam´
erica y las
Islas Malvinas. Si bien los caracaras juveniles reclutan a otros
caracaras hacia recursos alimenticios concentrados, dicho
reclutamiento y apaciguamiento no se han reportado en adultos.
Aqu´
ı describimos una hembra adulta de caracara que parece usar
ambos, se˜
nales de apaciguamiento y reclutamiento, para acceder a
alimento en un territorio bajo defensa de otra pareja en Isla
Goicoechea, Islas Malvinas. Los llamados para apaciguar y reclutar
son uno de varios paralelos conductuales que los investigadores han
notado entre este caracara, el cuervo (Corvus corax) y el kea (Nestor
notabilis). Aunque los falc´
onidos no son t´
ıpicamente considerados
en estudios comparativos de capacidad cognitiva y comportamiento
social aviar, sugrimos que estos caracaras merecen mayor atenci´
on,
especialmente dada su relaci´
on con los loros en el clado
Eufalconimorphae.
Palabras clave: competencia trepadora, Eufalconimorphae,
Falconidae, forrajeo, repertorio vocal, territorio.
Adult animals across many taxa use appease-
ment signaling to minimize or avoid costly conflict
in social settings by jointly communicating their
consent to defer and an interest to remain (Signe
and Van Schaik 2000). For example, rhesus
macaque (Macaca mulatta) navigate socially
dominant conspecifics by silently baring their
teeth, and meerkat (Suricata suricatta) grovel at
the feet of dominant females (Kutsukake and
Clutton-Brock 2006). These social tactics also
have been documented in birds, as when juvenile
Kea (Nestor notabilis) hunch in the presence of
dominant adult males to avoid displacement at a
feeding event (Diamond and Bond 1991), or when
Common Guillemot (Uria aalge) use a ritualized
walking behavior to appease neighbors while
moving through high-density colonies (Birkhead
1978).
When foraging, however, appeasement may not
be enough to gain access to a resource controlled
by a dominant conspecific. In this case, conspecific
recruitment is sometimes used to shift the social
dynamic from contest to scramble competition, in
which the resource becomes accessible to all
(Nicholson 1954, Parker 2000). For example,
juvenile Common Raven (Corvus corax) attempt-
ing to scavenge at carcasses produce recruitment
calls that attract conspecifics who overwhelm
territorial adult pairs (Heinrich 1988).
Striated Caracara (Phalcoboenus australis; here-
after ‘‘caracaras’’ ) are Near Threatened scavenging
falconids that breed in high densities on the
extreme southern coasts of South America and
the Falkland Islands (Balza et al. 2017, Reeves et
al. 2018, BirdLife International 2020). During the
austral summer, they forage in colonies of
breeding seabirds; in winter, when most seabirds
migrate offshore, caracaras consume invertebrates,
penguin and seal excreta, and bonanzas of carrion
(e.g., carcasses of Gentoo Penguin [Pygoscelis
papua], cast sheep [Ovis aries], and pinnipeds;
Strange 1996, Rexer-Huber and Bildstein 2013).
While juvenile caracaras recruit other caracaras to
concentrated food resources (Strange 1996, Autilio
et al. 2019), this behavior has not been reported in
adults. Here we describe an adult caracara that
appeared to use both appeasement signaling and
recruitment to access food in another pair’s
defended territory.
New Island (51843012 00 S, 618180000 W) is a 22
km
2
formerly farmed nature preserve in the
Falkland Islands. Since becoming a preserve in
1972, its breeding population of caracaras has
rebounded from 0 to over 80 pairs, reaching a
density of 4.3 nests/km
2
. Caracara age classes are
easily defined by plumage and bill color (Strange
1996), and much of the coastline is occupied by
territorial adult pairs (Catry et al. 2008). Two
wardens who live at a central settlement are the
island’s only human inhabitants. GPS data indicate
an adult pair of banded caracaras (M30 female and
M31 male, hereafter the ‘‘M pair’’ ) includes the
settlement in their territory (KJH, 2019, unpubl.
data), which they defend against intruders and in
which they have nested and fledged young (G.
Hazell, New Island Warden, 2020, pers. comm.).
On 9 August 2019 (austral winter), during a
trapping effort (see Harrington et al. 2018 for
methods) in the M pair’s territory, we observed an
adult female intrude and use appeasement displays
and recruitment calls to access food. Prior to
opening the trap, we had surveyed an 800 m radius
around the settlement. The only birds present were
the M pair and the neighboring pair, H37 and H38
(hereafter, the ‘‘H pair’’), who hold an adjacent
territory (KJH, 2019, unpubl. data).
At 0940 h (GMT-3), we opened the trap. After 2
min, M30 approached, fed, and then defended the
bait against 2 circling Turkey Vultures (Cathartes
aura). At 0950 h, we trapped and released M30,
128 The Wilson Journal of Ornithology Vol. 133, No. 1, March 2021
Downloaded From: https://bioone.org/journals/The-Wilson-Journal-of-Ornithology on 03 Nov 2021
Terms of Use: https://bioone.org/terms-of-useAccess provided by University of Ottawa
who remained close to the trap. Meanwhile, 2
unbanded caracaras, a juvenile and an adult,
landed within 20 m of the trap. The H pair visibly
remained 150 m from the trap and performed a
territorial ‘‘duetting’’ display with heads thrown
back while calling simultaneously (Strange 1996).
At 1001 h, we trapped and banded the juvenile
female as R30. We next trapped and banded the
adult female as R31, while the M pair duetted 40
m away. Both R30 and R31 were trapped within 1
min of approaching the bait, and neither was able
to feed before ensnaring their tarsus.
When we released R31 at the trap, she
immediately postured with her back hunched,
body feathers ruffled, head bowed, and wings
spread downward (Fig. 1), and began uttering a
soft, high-pitched, intermittent vocalization, a
behavior commonly seen in fledgling Striated
Caracara and Northern Crested Caracara (Caraca-
ra cheriway) when begging for food from their
parents (KJH, pers. obs.; J. Morrison, Trinity
College, 2020, pers. comm.). The M pair
approached and supplanted R31, who flew to a
perch within 15 m of the trap.
M30 approached R31 again and made brief
agonistic contact. R31 flew off with one talon
dangling slightly, then returned to within 2 m of
the trap and resumed the ‘‘begging’’ call and
posture for over 2 min, while the M pair remained
at 40 m distance, duetting.
We stood atop the trap to prevent R31 from
being recaptured. R31 walked around the trap
begging and posturing, then flew to a perch within
5 m of the trap and switched to a loud, repeating
cry we recognized as a ‘‘recruitment’’ call (Strange
1996, Autilio et al. 2019). Within 60 s, 9 additional
unbanded adult caracaras arrived, along with one
unbanded subadult and R30. We moved to stand 5
m from the trap. The newly arrived birds swarmed
the trap and consumed the bait. The M pair joined
in, and we observed no agonistic interactions
between birds as they fed. At 1040 h we closed
and removed the trap. By 1052 h, M30 and M31
had chased away all birds except R30 and R31.
Striated Caracara throughout the Falkland
Islands show little fear of humans (Strange
1996), and the presence of observers at or near
the trap during trapping efforts does not appear to
deter them (Harrington et al. 2018). We acknowl-
edge that the trap is an artificial situation, but their
behavior at traps appears similar to their behavior
at naturally occurring carcasses of injured Upland
Geese (Chloephaga picta) and cast sheep. Fur-
thermore, we observed another instance of adult
begging on New Island 5 d prior, in which a
banded adult female (G6) attempting to forage in a
Gentoo Penguin colony assumed the same posture
as R31 and was tolerated by an adult pair who had
previously been defending against intruders (Fig.
2).
R31’s glossy-black plumage, white terminal tail
band, pale striations on head and neck, yellow
legs, and silver bill indicated that she was an adult
of breeding age. Her sex was later confirmed via
DNA analysis (following the methods in Morrison
1999). Many adult female birds perform begging
displays for several reasons, although they do so
primarily during the breeding season to signal
Figure 1. Adult female R31 calling and displaying in the
appeasement posture: head bowed, back hunched, wings
held away from the body. New Island, Falkland Islands, 9
August 2019. Photo by K. Harrington.
Figure 2. Adult female G6 (left) performing appeasement
posture in the presence of territorial adult pair (right) at
Gentoo Penguin colony. New Island, Falkland Islands, 4
August 2019. Photo by K. Harrington.
129Short Communications
Downloaded From: https://bioone.org/journals/The-Wilson-Journal-of-Ornithology on 03 Nov 2021
Terms of Use: https://bioone.org/terms-of-useAccess provided by University of Ottawa
nutritional need (Ellis et al. 2009). As this occurred
outside the breeding season, we suggest that R31’s
most likely motivation was ‘‘appeasement’’ in
which she signaled to avoid conflict (Lorenz 1935,
Heinrich et al. 1993, Ellis et al. 2009). Juvenile
Common Raven, when attacked by dominant
adults, perform a similar submissive display and
begging call, with head bowed, feathers fluffed,
and wings spread (Heinrich 1988, 1989).
We speculate that R31, like a juvenile raven,
was acknowledging her subordinate status and
signaling to the M pair that she was not an
immediate threat so she could be permitted to feed.
While she may have been a floater frequenting this
territory (sensu Smith et al. 1978), it is also
possible that she was seasonally territorial; al-
though New Island appeared to support a similar
number of adult birds as in the previous summer,
an island-wide survey revealed fewer pairs main-
taining territories through the winter (KJH, unpubl.
data). It is also possible that R31 was a previous
offspring of the M pair, and thus perhaps more
likelytoshowbeggingbehaviortowardher
parents, since Striated Caracara are long-lived
birds (at least 12 years old in the wild [KJH, 2020,
unpubl. data] and 32 in captivity [A. Wallace,
Falconry and Me, 2020, pers. comm.]) and our
survey data suggests site fidelity among females
(KJH, 2019, unpubl. data). Ravens show long-term
memory of group members and can distinguish
affiliates from nonaffiliates (Boeckle and Bugnyar
2012), although further research is needed to
understand these capabilities in Striated Caracara.
We also surmise that R31’s subsequent recruit-
ment behavior may have been a response to our
guarding the trap. Her recruitment call, function-
ally similar to that of juvenile Common Raven
recruitment calls to a carcass (Heinrich 1989), and
the swift response of conspecifics, suggests
caracaras are aurally observant and primed to take
advantage of sudden feeding opportunities sig-
naled by this call. We also note that the
neighboring territorial H pair, though visible, did
not respond to R31’s recruitment call. This may be
evidence of the ‘‘dear enemy effect’’ (Fisher 1954),
a phenomenon in which territorial neighbors
respect territorial boundaries, which would suggest
that the newly arrived birds were not holding
territories adjacent to the M pair. Prior to these
observations, we had observed juvenile Striated
Caracara recruiting at food sources, but we had
never seen adults use both appeasement and
recruitment to overcome a territorial pair.
We also note the similarity of R31’s behavior,
not only to ravens, but to the ‘‘hunch’’ display used
by juvenile Kea to appease dominant conspecifics
(Diamond and Bond 1991), one of several
behavioral parallels that researchers have noted
between these 2 species (Rexer-Huber and Bild-
stein 2013, Autilio et al. 2019). Despite their close
relationship to parrots in the recently defined clade
Eufalconimorphae (Suh et al. 2011), falconid birds
are not typically considered in comparative studies
of avian group dynamics, social intelligence, and
cognitive evolution (i.e., Emery and Clayton 2004,
Guti´
errez-Iba´ ˜
nez et al. 2018). We suggest that
caracaras, whose lineage is basal to the widespread
‘‘true’’ falcons of the genus Falco, merit further
attention. Recent studies of social learning and
behavior in Chimango Caracara (Phalcoboenus
chimango; Biondi et al. 2010, Solaro and Sarasola
2019) and group dynamics in Northern Crested
Caracara and Striated Caracara (Dwyer and
Cockwell 2011, Dwyer 2014, Dwyer et al. 2018)
suggest that caracaras could show similar social
behavior and cognitive abilities to parrots and
crows, including social learning and object play
(Diamond and Bond 2003, Miller et al. 2014).
Striated Caracara’s high density, sociality, ap-
proachability, and behavioral flexibility (sensu
Mayr 1974) make them ideal candidates for further
study, and we suggest that research into their social
landscape and cognitive skills could shed light on
the ancestral cognitive abilities of their clade.
Acknowledgments
Funding for this study was provided by Falklands
Conservation, the Falkland Islands Government Environ-
mental Studies Budget, the John Cheek Trust, and Hawk
Mountain Sanctuary. We thank the Falkland Islands
Government Environmental Planning Department for pro-
viding permission to work on the islands and the New Island
wardens, Alec and Giselle Hazell, for being gracious hosts
and providing logistical support during our stay. We thank
K. Bildstein for initiating Hawk Mountain Sanctuary’s
longitudinal study of Striated Caracara in the Falklands. We
also thank J. Morrison, J. Dwyer, A. Bond, and 2
anonymous reviewers for providing critical comments that
improved an earlier version of this manuscript. All research
was conducted in accordance with the Falkland Island
Government Conservation of Wildlife and Nature Ordinance
under Research License No. R22/2015. This is Hawk
Mountain Contribution to Conservation Science No. 349.
130 The Wilson Journal of Ornithology Vol. 133, No. 1, March 2021
Downloaded From: https://bioone.org/journals/The-Wilson-Journal-of-Ornithology on 03 Nov 2021
Terms of Use: https://bioone.org/terms-of-useAccess provided by University of Ottawa
Literature cited
Autilio AR, Bechard MJ, Bildstein KL. 2019. Social
scavenging by wintering Striated Caracaras (Phalco-
boenus australis) in the Falkland Islands. Behavioral
Ecology and Sociobiology. 73:27.
Balza U, Lois NA, Raya Rey A. 2017. Status and
reproductive outcome of the breeding population of
Striated Caracaras (Phalcoboenus australis) at Franklin
Bay, Staten Island, Argentina. Wilson Journal of
Ornithology. 29:890–898.
Biondi LM, B´
o MS, Vassallo AI. 2010. Inter-individual and
age differences in exploration, neophobia and problem-
solving ability in a Neotropical raptor (Milvago
chimango). Animal Cognition. 13:701–710.
BirdLife International. 2020. Species factsheet: Phalcoboe-
nus australis [cited 28 Apr 2020]. http://www.birdlife.
org
Birkhead TR. 1978. Behavioural adaptations to high density
nesting in the Common Guillemot Uria aalge. Animal
Behaviour. 26:321–331.
Boeckle M, Bugnyar T. 2012. Long-term memory for
affiliates in ravens. Current Biology. 22:801–806.
Catry P, Lecoq M, Strange IJ. 2008. Population growth and
density, diet and breeding success of Striated Caracaras
Phalcoboenus australis on New Island, Falkland
Islands. Polar Biology. 31:1167–1174.
Diamond J, Bond AB. 1991. Social behavior and the
ontogeny of foraging in the Kea (Nestor notabilis).
Ethology. 88:128–144.
Diamond J, Bond AB. 2003. A comparative analysis of
social play in birds. Behaviour. 140:1091–1115.
Dwyer JF. 2014. Correlation of cere color with intra- and
interspecific agonistic interactions of Crested Caraca-
ras. Journal of Raptor Research. 48:240–247.
Dwyer JF, Cockwell SG. 2011. Social hierarchy of
scavenging raptors on the Falkland Islands, Malvinas.
Journal of Raptor Research. 45:229–235.
Dwyer JF, Fraser JD, Morrison JL. 2018. Evolution of
communal roosting: A social refuge–territory prospect-
ing hypothesis. Journal of Raptor Research. 52:407–419.
Ellis JM, Langen TA, Berg EC. 2009. Signalling for food
and sex? Begging by reproductive female White-
throated Magpie-Jays. Animal Behaviour. 78:615–623.
Emery NJ, Clayton NS. 2004. Comparing the complex
cognition of birds and primates. In: Rogers LJ, Kaplan
G, editors. Comparative vertebrate cognition: Are
primates superior to non-primates? New York (NY):
Springer; p. 3–55.
Fisher J. 1954. Evolution and bird sociality. In: Huxley J,
Hardy AC, Ford EB, editors. Evolution as a process.
London (UK): Allen & Unwin; p. 71–83.
Guti´
errez-Ib´
a˜
nez C, Iwaniuk AN, Wylie DR. 2018. Parrots
have evolved a primate-like telencephalic-midbrain-
cerebellar circuit. Scientific Reports. 8:1.
Harrington KJ, Pole-Evans S, Reeves M, Bechard M,
Bobowski M, et al. 2018. Seasonal micro-migration in
a farm-island population of Striated Caracaras (Phal-
coboenus australis) in the Falkland Islands. Movement
Ecology. 6:4.
Heinrich B. 1988. Winter foraging at carcasses by three
sympatric corvids, with emphasis on recruitment by the
raven, Corvus corax. Behavioral Ecology and Socio-
biology. 23:141–156.
Heinrich B. 1989. Ravens in winter. New York (NY): Simon
and Schuster.
Heinrich B, Marzluff JM, Marzluff CS. 1993. Common
Ravens are attracted by appeasement calls of food
discoverers when attacked. Auk. 110:247–254.
Kutsukake N, Clutton-Brock TH. 2006. Aggression and
submission reflect reproductive conflict between fe-
males in cooperatively breeding meerkats Suricata
suricatta. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
59:541–548.
Lorenz K. 1935. Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels
[Companions as factors in the bird’s environment].
Journal f¨
ur Ornithologie. 83:137–213, 289–413. Ger-
man.
Mayr E. 1974. Behavior programs and evolutionary
strategies. Journal of American Science. 62:650–659.
Miller R, Schiestl M, Whiten A, Schwab C, Bugnyar T.
2014. Tolerance and social facilitation in the foraging
behaviour of free-ranging crows (Corvus corone
corone;C. c. cornix). Ethology. 120:1248–1255.
Morrison J. 1999. Methods for gender determination in
Crested Caracaras. Journal of Raptor Research.
33:128–133.
Nicholson AJ. 1954. An outline of the dynamics of animal
populations. Australian Journal of Zoology. 2:9–65.
Parker GA. 2000. Scramble in behaviour and ecology.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of
London B. 355:1637–1645.
Reeves M, Crofts S, Bildstein KL. 2018. Distribution and
abundance of breeding Striated Caracaras in the
Falkland Islands (Malvinas). Journal of Raptor Re-
search. 52:309–315.
Rexer-Huber K, Bildstein KL. 2013. Winter diet of Striated
Caracara Phalcoboenus australis (Aves, Polyborinae)
at a farm settlement on the Falkland Islands. Polar
Biology. 36:437–443.
Signe P, Van Schaik CP. 2000. Dominance and communi-
cation: Conflict management in various social settings.
In: Aureli F, de Waal FBM, editors. Natural conflict
resolution. Berkeley (CA): University of California
Press; p. 77–105.
Smith SM. 1978. The ‘‘underworld’’ in a territorial sparrow:
Adaptive strategy for floaters. American Naturalist.
112:571–582.
Solaro C, Sarasola JH. 2019. Urban living predicts
behavioural response in a Neotropical raptor. Behav-
ioural Processes. 169:103995.
Strange IJ. 1996. The Striated Caracara (Phalcoboenus
australis) in the Falkland Islands. Author’s edition.
Falkland Islands.
Suh A, Paus M, Kiefmann M, Churakov G, Franke FA, et al.
2011. Mesozoic retroposons reveal parrots as the
closest living relatives of passerine birds. Nature
Communications. 2:1–7.
131Short Communications
Downloaded From: https://bioone.org/journals/The-Wilson-Journal-of-Ornithology on 03 Nov 2021
Terms of Use: https://bioone.org/terms-of-useAccess provided by University of Ottawa
... Striated caracaras (Phalcoboenus australis) are Falconiformes, sister clade to parrots and passerines, 13-15 and those endemic to the Falkland Islands (Malvinas) show curiosity and neophilia similar to notoriously neophilic kea parrots 16,17 and face similar socio-ecological pressures to corvids and parrots. 18,19 We tested wild striated caracaras as a new avian model for technical cognition and innovation using a field-applicable 8-task comparative paradigm (adapted from Rö ssler et al. 20 and Auersperg et al. 21 ). ...
... Falklands striated caracaras' ecology and behavior overlaps extensively with an established parrot model for technical innovation: Like kea parrots (Nestor notabilis), Falklands striated caracaras face seasonal resource unpredictability and are neophilic, exploratory, and playful. 16,19,31,52 Moreover, they are behaviorally flexible, e.g., they associate with anthropogenic resources, seasonally alter activity levels, extractively forage, and albeit historically persecuted, remain unwary of humans similar to how they were first described by sailors prior to human settlement in the Falklands. 31,53-55 Furthermore, Falklands striated caracaras are known for foraging innovations, including predation of an octopus 56,57 and unearthing invasive invertebrates in invasive grasslands. ...
Article
Full-text available
Innovation (i.e., a new solution to a familiar problem, or applying an existing behavior to a novel problem) plays a fundamental role in species’ ecology and evolution. It can be a useful measure for cross-group comparisons of behavioral and cognitive flexibility and a proxy for general intelligence. Among birds, experimental studies of innovation (and cognition more generally) are largely from captive corvids and parrots, though we lack serious models for avian technical intelligence outside these taxa. Striated caracaras (Phalcoboenus australis) are Falconiformes, sister clade to parrots and passerines, and those endemic to the Falkland Islands (Malvinas) show curiosity and neophilia similar to notoriously neophilic kea parrots, and face similar socio-ecological pressures to corvids and parrots. We tested wild striated caracaras as a new avian model for technical cognition and innovation using a field-applicable 8-task comparative paradigm. The setup allowed us to assess behavior, rate, and flexibility of problem solving over repeated exposure in a natural setting. Like other generalist species with low neophobia, we predicted caracaras to demonstrate a haptic approach to solving tasks, flexibly switching to new, unsolved problems and improving performance over time. Striated caracaras performed comparably to tool-using parrots, nearly reaching ceiling levels of innovation in few trials, repeatedly and flexibly solving tasks and rapidly learning. We attribute our findings to the birds’ ecology, including geographic restriction, resource unpredictability and opportunistic generalism, and encourage future work investigating their cognitive abilities in the wild.
Article
Full-text available
Avian scavengers perform vital ecosystem services by removing waste and slowing disease, yet few details are known about the process of carcass depletion, or the role of social interactions among groups of scavengers. The striated caracara (Phalcoboenus australis) is a social scavenging falconid of the Falkland Islands, whose boldness and reliance on human settlements as winter foraging grounds make it an ideal species with which to closely examine carcass use over the entire period of a carcass’ availability. By providing and monitoring experimental carcasses, we estimated the mass of food consumed per individual during 5-min intervals and compared the rate of group formation in the presence and absence of conspecific vocalizations. We found (1) that food obtained per individual was greater toward the beginning of carcass availability, when competition was fierce; (2) that vocalizations, by birds at and approaching the carcass, preceded periods of faster group formation; and (3) that on average birds would approach a speaker playing conspecific calls more closely than one playing a control recording. Our observations add to those of social foraging in other scavengers, providing a study of carcass use and vocalization at these ephemeral resources. Significance statement Currently ranked as Near Threatened by BirdLife International, the remote and restricted range of the striated caracara (Phalcoboenus australis) has made it a difficult study subject, though conservation concerns show it warrants deeper research. A social scavenging falconid of the Falkland Islands, striated caracaras have been persecuted as livestock pests since the 1800s, but their reliance on human settlements as winter foraging grounds makes them an ideal species with which to examine social scavenging behavior. This study was undertaken to investigate group feeding at carcasses by striated caracaras in austral winter, and to better understand the social function of their vocalizations. Furthering our knowledge of avian vocalizations is essential to certain wildlife management strategies, and empirically testing the effects of playbacks on target species is a crucial prior step to the use of any bio-acoustic tool.
Article
Full-text available
Avian communal roosts provide insight into evolution and serve as focal points for conservation. Nonbreeding Crested Caracaras (Caracara cheriway; hereafter caracaras) use communal roosts, but evolutionary implications have not been explored. Though nonbreeding caracaras are nonmigratory, the scientific literature fails to explain seasonal differences in their movement and survival concurrent with seasonal consistency in their habitat and social ecology. In the Social Refuge-Territory Prospecting hypothesis we propose, socially subordinate nonbreeding caracaras precluded from breeding by habitat limitation use communal roosts as social refuges to avoid aggression from dominant territory holders during nonbreeding seasons, and engage in territory prospecting during breeding seasons. Communal roosts thus become central places from which to forage not for food, but for a breeding territory. Because foraging gains are stored as remembered information, competition costs resulting from public information are preempted. For the Social Refuge-Territory Prospecting hypothesis to be valid, two criteria need to be met. First, communal roost use needs to be higher during nonbreeding seasons. Second, a measure of fitness needs to be used to evaluate the hypothesis as an evolutionarily stable strategy. To meet these criteria, in this study we report that numbers of nonbreeding caracaras using a communal roost in Florida are higher during nonbreeding seasons (mean = 111.8 individuals/night) than during breeding seasons (mean = 60.7 individuals/night) as counted from August 2006-April 2009 (n = 407 counts). We also compare differential survival by season from previous work to demonstrate that by limiting exploratory movements to times when prospecting is most informative, nonbreeding caracaras balance maximizing survival against the likelihood of securing a breeding territory. Our hypothesis provides a unifying explanation for otherwise unexplained paradoxes in the ecology of nonbreeding caracaras, and may be useful in guiding conservation and expanding our understanding of the ecology of other communally roosting birds.
Article
Full-text available
We conducted surveys of breeding pairs of the endemic and globally "Near Threatened" Striated Caracara (Phalcoboenus australis) on 79 outer islands in the Falklands archipelago (Islas Malvinas) during the austral-summer breeding seasons of 2013-2014 and 2014-2015. A total of 354 breeding pairs (defined as pairs with evidence of egg-laying), or an archipelago-wide adult breeding population of at least 0.05 birds/km² was recorded. An additional 477 probable breeding pairs also were found. The distribution of breeding pairs remains similar to that found during surveys in the 1990s and 2000s. Grand Jason, Steeple Jason, New Island, Beauchêne Island, and Bird Island, the five islands with the highest numbers of breeding pairs, accounted for half of the total breeding population. "Vegetationally pristine" islands with dense areas of tussac grass (Poa flabellata) and high densities of small burrowing seabirds-namely Bird Island, Saddle Island, and Beauchêne Island-supported the highest densities of breeding pairs. Eighty percent of all pairs occurred on National Nature Reserves or on privately owned nature sanctuaries. Our surveys suggest that sustained protection of key breeding sites is critical for the long-term conservation of this species. We recommend that populations of Striated Caracaras at Tierra del Fuego also be surveyed and that the global population status of the species be reassessed.
Article
Full-text available
It is widely accepted that parrots show remarkable cognitive abilities. In mammals, the evolution of complex cognitive abilities is associated with increases in the size of the telencephalon and cerebellum as well as the pontine nuclei, which connect these two regions. Parrots have relatively large telencephalons that rival those of primates, but whether there are also evolutionary changes in their telencephalon-cerebellar relay nuclei is unknown. Like mammals, birds have two brainstem pontine nuclei that project to the cerebellum and receive projections from the telencephalon. Unlike mammals, birds also have a pretectal nucleus that connects the telencephalon with the cerebellum: the medial spiriform nucleus (SpM). We found that SpM, but not the pontine nuclei, is greatly enlarged in parrots and its relative size significantly correlated with the relative size of the telencephalon across all birds. This suggests that the telencephalon-SpM-cerebellar pathway of birds may play an analogous role to cortico-ponto-cerebellar pathways of mammals in controlling fine motor skills and complex cognitive processes. We conclude that SpM is key to understanding the role of telencephalon-cerebellar pathways in the evolution of complex cognitive abilities in birds.
Article
Full-text available
Background The extent to which seasonal changes in food availability affect small-scale movements in free-ranging populations of birds of prey is relatively little studied. Here we describe a seasonal “micro-migration” of a farm-island population of striated caracaras (Phalcoboenus australis) in the Falkland Islands in response to seasonal changes in the availability of seabird carcasses. We banded more than 450 individuals on Saunders Island, deployed archival and satellite GPS data loggers on 17 individuals, and monitored movements within and between two feeding areas on Saunders Island, a “marine-subsidized” site near seabird colonies and an anthropogenic “human-subsidized” farm site 16 km to the southeast. Results During 67 observation days between 2010 and 2015, resightings of 312 banded caracaras were greater at the marine-subsidized site during austral summer than winter, and the total daily resightings varied significantly between spring versus summer, summer versus winter, autumn versus spring, and autumn versus winter. Resightings were higher at the human-subsidized site in austral winter than summer and the total daily resightings varied significantly across all bi-seasonal comparisons. Resightings indicated that at least 12 of 197 birds (6.1%) moved between the human- and marine-subsidized sites at least once during the same winter, 15 of 335 birds (4.5%) did so in spring, none of 164 birds did so in summer, and 16 of 297 birds (5.4%) did so in autumn. Individuals fitted with archival GPS data loggers at the marine-subsidized site in summer maintained highly localized 95% kernel core areas (0.55 ± 0.12 km² [mean ± SD]), whereas those at the human-subsidized site in winter maintained larger 95% kernel core areas (3.8 ± 4.6 km²). Two of 6 satellite-tagged individuals that summered at known caracara breeding colonies 80 km WNW of Saunders Island were subsequently resighted in winter at the human-subsidized site. Conclusion Our results suggest that seasonal shifts in food resource availability drive seasonal micro-migrations in a farm-island population of striated caracaras, and that farm sites can be critical in providing nutritional resources for caracaras when naturally occurring marine-subsidized resources become less available. Our results have important implications for striated caracara spatial ecology and conservation, as increased winter survival could improve the status of this globally Near-Threatened population.
Article
Full-text available
The Striated Caracara (Phalcoboenus australis) is a poorly known raptor associated with seabird colonies, restricted to the islands of southern South America, and with an estimated extant population of <2,500 mature individuals. We evaluated the number of breeding pairs, described the characteristics and spatial pattern of nest sites, and estimated breeding output of a population of Striated Caracaras in Franklin Bay, Staten Island, Argentina. We found one of the lower breeding density values reported for this species, although this population is associated with one of the biggest colonies of Rockhopper Penguins. The main material used for the construction of Striated Caracaras' nests was tussac grass, though only half of nests were placed in grassland. The spatial pattern for nest sites corresponds with global and local clustering. All successful Striated Caracaras' nests were at least 250 m from the nearest neighboring nest, were generally closer to the colony of Rockhopper Penguins than failed nests, and had more Rockhopper patches around them. Breeding success was 0.73 successful nests/active nests, productivity was 1.27 ± 1.01 young/active nest, and brood size was 1.75 ± 0.71 young/successful nest. The presence of invasive wild goats and red deer is proposed as a factor that could be restricting nest site availability in the study area.
Article
Full-text available
Social foraging provides animals with opportunities to gain knowledge about available food. Studies indicate that animals are influenced by social context during exploration and are able to learn socially. Carrion and hooded crows, which are opportunistic generalists with flexible social systems, have so far received little focus in this area. We combined observational and experimental approaches to investigate social interactions during foraging and social influences on crow behaviour within a free-ranging population at Vienna Zoo, which included 115 individually marked crows. We expected the crows to be tolerant of conspecifics during foraging due to high food abundance. We predicted that social context would enhance familiar object exploration, as well as a specific foraging strategy: predation by crows on other species. We found that crows were highly tolerant of one another, as reflected by their high rates of cofeeding – where they fed directly beside conspecific(s) – relative to affiliative or agonistic interactions. Evidence for social facilitation – when the observer's behaviour is affected by the mere presence of a model – was found in both object exploration and predation behaviour. Specifically, crows touched the objects more frequently when others were present (whilst only approaching the objects when alone), and conspecifics were present more frequently during predation events involving the high-risk target species. Evidence for enhancement during object exploration – where the observer's attention is drawn to a place or object by a model's actions – was not confirmed in this context. Our results highlight the role played by the presence of conspecifics across different contexts: natural foraging behaviour, familiar object exploration and a specific foraging strategy. To our knowledge, this is one of the first corvid studies aimed at teasing apart specific social influence and learning mechanisms in the field. These crows therefore make promising candidates for studying social learning and its consequences under naturalistic conditions.
Article
Behaviour is expected to be one of the most important factors driving urban living of bird species because it largely determines how animals interact with their environments. The contribution of different behavioural traits as facilitators of contemporary process of bird colonization of urban environments, however, is still unclear. Here we examined the differences in three behavioural traits: neophobia (avoidance of a new object) and solving success and solving latency of a problem solving test related to obtain food, in rural and urban living individuals of a common diurnal Neotropical raptor, the chimango caracara (Phalcoboenus chimango). Moreover, for solving success and solving latency behaviours, we tested the plasticity (i.e., habituation) in birds. Urban and rural chimangos showed similar neophobia of a new object. All chimangos showed an improvement in their output in problem-solving test as the five-day testing passed but urban chimangos showed higher solving capabilities than rural conspecifics as they were more successful in solving the problem test. More, urban birds shown lower solving latency than rural, as they were able to improve their test performance by opening doors in less time as the day passed; in rural birds this relation was not so abrupt. In addition, those individuals that opened more doors, opened these faster too. Lastly, both solving success and latency showed very low individual consistency (repeatability < 0.275). Our results provide evidence of plasticity in solving capabilities of chimango caracaras which reveal that a habituation process in these behavioural traits could be associated to establishment of birds in urban environments.