ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Underwater noise has been identified as a relevant pollutant affecting marine ecosystems in different ways. Despite the numerous studies performed over the last decades regarding the affections of underwater noise to marine life, a lack of knowledge and methodological procedures still exists and results are often tentative or qualitative. A monitoring methodology for behavioural response of bluefin tuna (Thunnus thynnus) when exposed to ship and wind turbine operational noises was implemented and tested in a fixed commercial tuna feeding cage in the Mediterranean sea. Fish behaviour was continuously monitored combining synchronised echosounder and video recording systems. Automatic information extracted from acoustical echograms was used to describe tuna reaction to noise in terms of average depth and vertical dimensions of the school, and indicators of swimming speed and tilt direction. Video recordings allowed to detect changes in swimming patterns. Different kinds of stimuli were considered during bluefin tuna cage monitoring such as noise generated by feeding boats, wind farm operational noise and other synthetic signals projected in the medium using a broadband underwater projector. The monitoring system design was revealed as a successful methodological approach to record and to quantify reactions to noise. The obtained results pointed out that observed reactions presented a strong relationship with insonification pressure level and time. Behavioral changes associated with noise are difficult to observe specially in semi-liberty conditions, thus the presented approach offered the opportunity to link anthropogenic activity with possible affections to a given marine species, bringing the possibility to achieve a more realistic framework to assess underwater noise impacts on marine animals.
Content may be subject to copyright.
sensors
Article
Monitoring of Caged Bluefin Tuna Reactions to Ship and
Offshore Wind Farm Operational Noises
Vicente Puig-Pons 1,2,* , Ester Soliveres 1,2, Isabel Pérez-Arjona 1,2 , Victor Espinosa 1,2, Pedro Poveda-Martínez 3,
Jaime Ramis-Soriano 3, Patricia Ordoñez-Cebrián 4, Marek Moszy ´nski 5, Fernando de la Gándara 2,6,
Manuel Bou-Cabo 2,6, José L. Cort 2,6 and Eladio Santaella 2,6


Citation: Puig, V.; Ordoñez, P.;
Soliveres, E.; Pérez-Arjona, I.;
Espinosa, V.; Poveda, P.; Ramis, J.;
Moszy´nski, M.; de la Gándara, F.;
Bou-Cabo, M.; Cort, J.L.; Santaella, E.
Monitoring of Caged Bluefin Tuna
Reactions to Ship and Offshore Wind
Farm Operational Noises. Sensors
2021,21, 6998.
https://doi.org/10.3390/s21216998
Academic Editor: Teruhisa Komatsu
Received: 7 July 2021
Accepted: 15 October 2021
Published: 21 October 2021
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral
with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional affil-
iations.
Copyright: © 2021 by the authors.
Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland.
This article is an open access article
distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons
Attribution (CC BY) license (https://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/
4.0/).
1Intitut d’Investigació per a la Gestió Integrada de Zones Costaneres, Universitat Politécnica de València,
C/Paranimf 1, Grau de Gandia, 46730 València, Spain; essogon@upv.es (E.S.); iparjona@upv.es (I.P.-A.);
vespinos@fis.upv.es (V.E.)
2Unidad Mixta de Investigación en Tecnología para Estudios Marinos IEO-UPV (UTEM), Muelle Frutero,
Port de Gandia, Grau de Gandia, 46730 València, Spain; fernando.delagandara@ieo.es (F.d.l.G.);
manuel.bou@ieo.es (M.B.-C.); jose.cort@ieo.es (J.L.C.); esantael@gmail.com (E.S.)
3
Institut Universitari de Física Aplicada a les Ciències i les Tecnologies, Universitat d’Alacant, Ap. de Correus
99, 03080 Alacant, Spain; pedro.poveda@ua.es (P.P.-M.); jramis@ua.es (J.R.-S.)
4Zunibal S.L., Idorsolo Kalea, 1, 48160 Derio, Spain; patricia.ordonez@zunibal.com
5Faculty of Electronics, Telecommunications and Informatics, Politechnika Gda ´nska, Gabriela Narutowicza
11/12, 80233 Gda´nsk, Poland; marmo@eti.pg.gda.pl
6Instituto Español de Oceanografía, Centro Ocenográfico de Murcia, C/Varadero, 1, San Pedro del Pinatar,
30740 Murcia, Spain
*Correspondence: vipuipon@upvnet.upv.es
Abstract: Underwater noise has been identified as a relevant pollution affecting marine ecosystems
in different ways. Despite the numerous studies performed over the last few decades regarding the
adverse effect of underwater noise on marine life, a lack of knowledge and methodological procedures
still exists, and results are often tentative or qualitative. A monitoring methodology for the behavioral
response of bluefin tuna (Thunnus thynnus) when exposed to ship and wind turbine operational noises
was implemented and tested in a fixed commercial tuna feeding cage in the Mediterranean sea. Fish
behavior was continuously monitored, combining synchronized echosounder and video recording
systems. Automatic information extracted from acoustical echograms was used to describe tuna
reaction to noise in terms of average depth and vertical dimensions of the school and the indicators
of swimming speed and tilt direction. Video recordings allowed us to detect changes in swimming
patterns. Different kinds of stimuli were considered during bluefin tuna cage monitoring, such as
noise generated by feeding boats, wind farm operational noise, and other synthetic signals projected
in the medium using a broadband underwater projector. The monitoring system design was revealed
as a successful methodological approach to record and quantify reactions to noise. The obtained
results suggested that the observed reactions presented a strong relationship with insonification
pressure level and time. Behavioral changes associated with noise are difficult to observe, especially
in semi-free conditions; thus, the presented approach offered the opportunity to link anthropogenic
activity with possible effects on a given marine species, suggesting the possibility of achieving a
more realistic framework to assess the impacts of underwater noise on marine animals.
Keywords: underwater noise; bluefin tuna; offshore windmill; behavior; anthropogenic impact
1. Introduction
Human activities cause pressure on the marine environment, affecting it in numerous
ways. One of the activities that has increased during the last decades is marine traffic.
Some authors have pointed out that from 1992 to 2012, marine traffic has increased at
global level of almost 60%. Some areas even experienced an increase ranging from 100%
to 200% [
1
]. Commercial globalization has led to an increase in goods traffic through
Sensors 2021,21, 6998. https://doi.org/10.3390/s21216998 https://www.mdpi.com/journal/sensors
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 2 of 23
oceans and seas around the world. Related not only to marine traffic but also other types
of human activities, underwater noise has captured a considerable amount of attention
as a pollutant, being promoted to the category of threat and widely studied by different
organizations, such as ACCOBAMS (Agreement on the Conservation of Cetaceans of the
Black Sea, Mediterranean Sea and contiguous Atlantic Area), ASCOBAMS (Agreement
on the Conservation of Small Cetaceans of the Baltic, North East Atlantic, Irish and North
Seas), IFAW (International Fund for Animal Welfare), and IMO (International Maritime
Organization), among others. Many actions have been taken by both Europe and the
USA with the aim of establishing standards and methodologies to assess the threat that
underwater noise represents [
2
,
3
]. One of the more ambitious actions developed during
the last years with respect to the study of the marine environment is the Marine Strategy
Framework Directive (hereafter referred to as MSFD) adopted in 2008 by EU member
states. The main aim of MSFD is to protect the marine ecosystem and biodiversity by
establishing the concept of Good Environmental Status (or GES). The directive defines
GES as “the environmental status of marine waters where these provide ecologically
diverse and dynamic oceans and seas which are clean, healthy and productive”. GES
assessment is conducted using eleven qualitative/quantitative descriptors. That related
to underwater noise is defined in Descriptor 11 (D11), which accounts for impulsive or
continuous noise separately due to the differences among sources and effects produced.
The framework related to the assessment of underwater noise typically considers the
acoustic sources and sound pressure level generated and propagated through the medium,
but, ultimately, the studies linked to the effect of marine biota due to noise are gaining
relevance. To perform studies examining the risk of the effect linked to underwater noise,
it is necessary to know the influence of sound on animal life. According to [
4
], most
noise effect studies are related to fish, specifically 52%, while 21% correspond to marine
mammals. The remaining studies are based on reptiles, mollusks, and arthropods. It
is commonly accepted that all fish studied to date are sensitive to noise [
5
7
], and this
was confirmed by using two sensory systems depending on the species: the inner ear
and the lateral line system. Despite the numerous studies considering different kinds of
animals and types of noise, there exists a lack of research with respect to underwater noise
effects due to the vastness of the theme. In this work, we report on the behavioral changes
observed in bluefin tuna due to continuous noise emitted by an underwater projector. One
of the most relevant sources of impulsive noise is related to the installation of turbines in
offshore wind farms. The effect of piling noise on fish has been investigated in regard to
some target species, with a variety of results, from no evidence of injury or reaction [
8
,
9
]
to immediate death [
10
], but fewer studies are related to operational continuous noise.
However, different works have addressed the characterization and possible effects of
the operational noise of marine turbines on marine life. The effects of installation and
operational phases on marine mammals were investigated in [
11
], cetaceans being the initial
focus. Attention was also paid to the effects of such noise on fish and invertebrates, as well
as to all of the potential impacts of operating windmills and their emitting characteristics
(see, for instance, [
12
15
], and references therein). Bluefin tuna (Thunnus thynnus) is an
emblematic species and also a high value economic resource, and, thus, it has been the
subject of many studies over the last decade due to the possible worldwide extinction at the
turn of the century. Even assuming the increasing interest in the impact of anthropogenic
noise on marine life and the general concern regarding bluefin tuna, only a few studies
have attempted to characterize the hearing threshold of similar species such as Thunnus
orientalis [
16
], and, thus, there exists a lack of knowledge regarding bluefin tuna as a
receptor of noise pollution. Bluefin tuna form schools that migrate at ocean scales crossing
the Gibraltar strait from Atlantic Ocean to Mediterranean sea. Their migration routes pass
nearby coasts, these regions representing the main candidates to host offshore facilities
such as wind farms. The aim of the present work is to contribute to the knowledge of
the potential effects of the operational noise of wind turbines on the behavior of bluefin
tuna, which could affect their feeding and reproductive migration. Additionally, as a
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 3 of 23
first step, we aim to validate methodologies with semi-captive tuna to study such effects.
In order to investigate reactions to operational turbine noise, bluefin tuna located in a
feeding cage off the Mediterranean coast were exposed to wind turbine noise and other
recordings. The animals were previously monitored for a number of weeks using active
acoustics and video systems to ensure that the possible reactions after noise emission were
distinguishable from the usual behavior of the fish. Behavioral experiments in cages cannot
be directly extrapolated to wild bluefin tuna, and similar methodologies used with other
pelagic species [
17
] or with species with more limited geographical movements should
be applied [
18
,
19
]. An example of this is the use of acoustical and other types of sensing
tagging (dive loggers) that have been successfully applied to harbor porpoises to investigate
the relationships between their reaction and noise sources. However, monitoring tuna is
extremely difficult because of their great mobility and the required delicate manipulation,
mainly in wild conditions but also in the semi-captive condition presented here [
20
]. Our
experiment was deemed an exceptional opportunity due to the potential implication for
tuna farming. The presence or absence of a response of animals to particular stimuli, in this
case, an acoustical disturbance, depends on different factors, such as stress, individual
characteristics, previous experience, and the presence of prey or predators, among others.
The obtained results on semi-captive tuna must be interpreted carefully, but the observed
effects caused by wind turbine noise could allow us to interpret future reactions observed
in the wild.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Location and Measurement Conditions
Activities reported via this communication were carried out at facilities located in
L’Ametlla de Mar (latitude = 40
52
0
11.7
00
N and longitude 0
48
0
15.2
00
E), Mediterranean Sea.
The experiment was performed from 23 to 25 July 2013. Bluefin tuna are usually caught
along their migration route, close to the Balearic Islands in the Mediterranean Sea. Animals
after capturing by purse seiners were transferred to floating cages and towed during a
period of 10–15 days from the Balearic coastal region to the fattening farms, located near
the continental coast traveling about 200 km at a speed of 1 knot. The dimensions of the
final feeding cage were 50 m in diameter and 28 m in depth. The total number of fish was
approximately 900 bluefin tuna, weighing 200 kg on average and caught during the first
week of June 2013. During their stay in the cages until the time of the experiment, the tuna
were subjected to ship operational acoustical stimuli, which must be taken into account
when discussing the results of the work; the presence of feeding boats twice per day and
other operational works (cleaning, repairing, surveillance, etc.) will most likely alter the
response of semi-captive tuna, thereby distinguishing them from the wild ones. Moreover,
during the trip from Balearic waters, the tuna were continuously subjected to the towing
boat noise, an aspect that should be also taken into account.
2.2. Background Tuna Behavior
In order to study tuna behavior in sea cages in normal production conditions, the be-
havioral patterns of the tuna school in the farm were continuously monitored for 6 weeks
from January to February 2013. The studied tuna were caught during the fishing season
in June 2012. The acoustical recording system consisted of an autonomous single-beam
Knudsen Engineering ROVER echosounder (Figure 1), working at 200 kHz and covering an
angle of 25
at
3 dB from the maximum emission level on the transducer axis. The trans-
ducer was located facing upwards toward the surface at a depth of 24 m at the bottom of a
floating commercial cage that was 50 m in diameter and anchored to the middle of the trap
radius. In addition, an underwater video camera system was installed, working together
with the acoustic system and creating a redundant system of monitoring. The system was
powered by external batteries and solar panels located on the cage and installed inside
a waterproof box. Data transfer was ensured by a Wi-Fi link to the shore providing the
possibility of remote control of the system and real-time acquisition. Nevertheless, all
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 4 of 23
data were also stored locally on the computer incorporated by the Knudsen echosounder.
This combination of video cameras and echosounders for the behavioral monitoring of
fish when exposed to underwater noise was also used by [
21
] in herring cages. Later,
a combination of two different echo sounding systems (vertical echosounder and sidescan
sonar) was also used by [17] for behavioral response observations in the wild.
Figure 1.
Scheme of the continuous long-term monitoring system, composed of a control echosounder, a video camera,
and a real-time transmission system. A floating platform with the video camera and the ultrasonic transducer was placed at
a depth of 24 m and cabled to the surface where the autonomous echosounder and communication systems were placed in
a waterproof box. The solar power electronics and batteries were placed in a second box together in a structure fixed to the
cage rim. The resulting synchronized video and echogram are also depicted.
2.3. Exposure to Pure Tones, Synthetic Noises, and Hydrophone Recording Playback
The experiment was originally designed to test the effect of operational turbine noise
on bluefin tuna behavior, but other sounds were also reproduced for a better understanding
of the possible tuna reactions and validation of the observing system. The noise of a wind
turbine was previously recorded at 50 m from the source for 30 s and sampled at 350
kHz. It is possible to observe the spectrum of the wind turbine in Figure 2. The sound
could be understood as a broad band noise with similar levels, around 120 dB ref 1
µ
Pa,
along the whole spectrum defined within 30 Hz–10k Hz, with a maximum sound pressure
level centered at 50 Hz (142 dB ref 1
µ
Pa). Considering the audibility threshold of similar
species and the sound pressure level generated by the source, bluefin tuna were expected
to react to the projected sound, even more so when maximum levels overlapped with
the frequency range of sound generated by tuna, probably produced by swimbladder
contraction [
22
]. The emitter used to project the recorded noise to the medium was Data
Physics GW350, an underwater sound projector provided by the Spanish Navy. This
device is specially designed to simulate the acoustic signatures of specific sonar targets.
Typically, it is used to train the crews of surface ships, submarines, and helicopters in
anti-submarine warfare techniques. The source allowed the emission of low-frequency
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 5 of 23
sound in the band of 20 Hz to 3200 Hz, suitable for the pursued objectives. The projector
was assembled in a metal structure, implementing a pressure compensator in order to
increase the operation depth of the source. The system was deployed to a depth of 10
m by means of a crane. The projector was connected to a 300 W power amplifier into
which a signal was fed from an National Instruments PXI-5412 100 MS/s signal generator
connected to a laptop computer. The electric power was provided by the boat battery
array, and during the measurements, both the main ship’s engine and the auxiliary electric
generator were switched off. Sound pressure level measurements during the signal’s
playback were made by means of two calibrated hydrophones, an ITC1032 (sensitivity:
194 dB ref 1 V/
µ
Pa, ranging from 10 Hz to 50 kHz) and a B&K 8103 (sensitivity:
211 dB
ref 1 V/
µ
Pa, from 0.1 Hz to 180 kHz), located at distances of 25 m and 10 m from the source,
respectively. The recorded signals were amplified using a B&K Nexus Signal Conditioner
Type 2693 and digitized using an NI PXI-5102 oscilloscope at sample rates of 44,100 Hz
and 96,000 Hz, and data were stored on a laptop computer. The sound pressure level (SPL)
was calculated for each frequency applying an exponential RMS averaging (expressed as
dB ref 1 V/
µ
Pa). The emitted signals were classified in three different groups: single tones,
synthetic broadband noises, and recorded noises. The first group was composed of pure
tones of 30 Hz, 50 Hz, 100 Hz, 150 Hz, 200 Hz, 300 Hz, 500 Hz, 1000 Hz, and 4000 Hz.
The amplitude of each tone could be as high as 137, 150, 162, 160, 155, 158, 152, 155, or 150
dB (ref 1 V/
µ
Pa), respectively. The second group, synthetic broadband noises, consisted of
four different signals: white noise (average SPL 120 dB), maximum length sequences (MLS)
(125 dB), time-stretched pulses (TSP) (150 dB), and sine sweeps (140 dB) [
23
]. The last
group consisted of recorded submarine noises, produced by tuna farm ships (Figure 3) and
the previously mentioned recording of an offshore wind turbine in operation (Figure 2).
Additionally to continuous noise, a recording of an impulsive noise, regular in the farm
acoustic landscape, was registered. This last recorded sound came from a lupara shot,
which is a tool commonly used in the slaughter of tuna with a peak SPL above 216 dB ref
1
µ
Pa (Figure 4). Previously to our experiment, it was visually observed that fish reacted
with sudden accelerations when an individual slaughter was made.
Figure 2.
Original recorded turbine emission (black) and reproduced noise used in the experiment
(red). Dashed curves correspond to the third-octave levels (dB ref 1 µPa RMS) of the same signals.
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 6 of 23
Figure 3.
Echograms corresponding to feeding process on 15 January 2013. In the left column raw
echograms are shown. In the rigth column echograms of the school had been isolated from noise
following the procedure explained in Section 2.4. Average school depth and upper and lower limits
were calculated automatically. In all figures the red solid line represents the average depth of the
school; the lower dashed line represents the lower limit of the school; and the upper dashed line
represents the upper limit of the school. From the inspection of the processed echograms the change
in the vertical distribution of fish can be observed as a consequence of boat operations:
(a
) boat
approached, and school dived at an average distance from transducer of 12.2 m (before 07:44:00)
to 8.9 m (after 07:44:00).
(b
) Boat moored and unloaded food block. Tuna dived deeper to 10.4 m.
(c
) Food block through the acoustic beam. School dived deeper to 9.7 m.
(d
) Boat departed from the
cage and tuna rose again at an average distance from the transducer at the bottom of 12.3 m.
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 7 of 23
Figure 4.
Impulsive noise temporal recording of a lupara used for tuna slaughter (
a
) and its spectral
content (b, inset).
During the exposure to the turbine noise, an extra single-beam DT-X Biosonics scien-
tific echosounder (Figure 5), also working at 200 kHz and an aperture at
3 dB of 20
, was
installed at the opposite side of the cage to gather more information about the school’s dis-
tribution and behavior. The video camera was located alongside the Knudsen echosounder
(Figure 5).
Figure 5. Right
panel: Setup used for insonification experiments. The Knudsen echosounder and the video camera were
placed at point A in the diagram. The Biosonics echosounder was placed at point B, also looking up toward the sea surface.
The underwater projector was suspended from the observation boat at point C, and two calibrated hydrophones were
positioned at points E and D.
Left
panel: Image of operations with underwater projector being installed and the control
desk with both image and acoustic real-time observations.
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 8 of 23
2.4. Data Analysis
Acoustic data were analyzed automatically using software developed specifically
for this purpose in the framework of Matlab(R). First, data were split using two criteria
according to the intended end use. In the case of background tuna behavior, data were
divided into 1-hour files for subsequent analysis. In the noise exposure case, data were
divided into events. Every event refers to one sound stimulus to which tuna were subjected
(pure tones and synthetic and recorded noises).
The behavior was characterized in terms of three variables:
Average depth and upper and lower limits of the school;
Average length of traces;
Average tilt of traces.
The average depth of the school was calculated similar to the center of mass of
the acoustic volume backscattering strength following the method of [
24
] and references
therein. To analyze trace length and tilt, digital imaging processing techniques were used
to transform the echogram into a binary image using the threshold level method [
25
].
In order to obtain isolated traces, a sequence of morphological operations was applied
to achieve more compact regions and to remove noise [
26
]. Figure 6shows the image
binarization, morphological operations , region-based segmentation and trace isolation of
school presented in Figure 3. In regard to this, to obtain data from the school, the average
depth and upper and lower limits were calculated for every ping. Based on the ping average
data, an average value per hour was calculated in the behavior monitoring experiment
and an average value of three parameters was estimated for each event. Afterward, region-
based segmentation was carried out. To separate tuna traces from unwanted targets,
a region size threshold and an echo level threshold were used. The geometrical parameters
of each trace were stored with its distance to the transducer and its duration. Acoustic data
processing produced a collection of traces for each hour or event. Finally, the tilt of traces
was calculated using the maximum backscattering value of each ping of the trace and the
distance from each maximum to the transducer (Figure 6B). A linear fit was applied to
the range values and the slope of the line was used as a tilt of the trace indicator. The tilt
of traces was used to assess whether tuna swam upward or downward. The sequence of
acoustic data processing algorithms is shown in Figure 7A.
Figure 6.
Image processing thechniques used and tested in Figure 3top-right echogram.
(a
) Image
binarization.
(b
) Morphological operation applied: thickening to provide more compact regions,
opening to remove protrusions (noise), breaking weak connections, and closing to smooth out contours
and fill small holes. c) Region-based segmentation results. d) Isolated trace from this school.
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 9 of 23
Figure 7. (a
) Sequence of data processing algorithm.
(b
) Trace of fish swimming upward. Dotted line
shows the distance of maximum backscattering value for each ping. The solid line represents a linear
fit applied to range values shown by the dashed line.
2.5. Statistics Analysis
Statistical analysis was carried out to analyze the suitability of measured variables
to describe the tuna’s reaction to acoustic stimuli, as has previously been performed in
previous studies [
27
,
28
]. In order to measure the tuna’s behavioral response to the different
stimuli described above, three types of behavior were listed:
B0: No response to stimuli;
B1: Moderate response, with the tuna presenting slight changes in vertical position,
swimming velocity, or swimming tilt.
B2: Severe response referring to abrupt changes in vertical position, swimming veloc-
ity, or swimming tilt.
In order to establish a relationship between the acoustic stimuli and the behavioral
variables, statistical analysis of the data was conducted. First, the assumptions necessary for
the use of analysis of variance (ANOVA) were determined. In this case, although the values
did not comply with the required normality for some of the variables, homoscedasticity was
satisfied according to Leven’s test (p> 0.05). Consequently, and assuming the robustness
of the method against violations in data normality (the central limit theorem states that
the sample measurements must be approximately normal), the ANOVA method was the
approach applied to the present analysis. To describe the three types of behavior in a
quantitative manner, a linear combination of the measured parameters obtained from
principal component analysis was used. That quantitative value could be used to describe
B0, B1, and B2 as dependent on the measured parameters. The ANOVA test was carried
out to analyze the effect of source type and source level on the tuna’s behavioral response.
Levene’s test was used to test normality and homogeneity of variances in ANOVA analysis.
All statistics analysis was carried out using Statgraphics Centurion XVIII®[29].
3. Results
3.1. Background Tuna Behavior
The monitoring of the usual behavior of the tuna in the cage resulted in 700 h of
acoustic echograms and 150 h of video recordings. As a result, it could be inferred that the
tuna school usually swam in a circular or elliptical pattern [
30
,
31
] covering a large area of
the cage, swimming closer to the cage nets. As expected [
32
], the school depth exhibited
circadian rhythms. During the middle of the day, the school tended to be closer to the
surface, going deeper overnight. This behavior was repeatedly observed during the period
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 10 of 23
of continuous monitoring and recording. An average night/day depth difference of 2.8 m
was noticed. It was also found that the school reacted to the feeding boat’s approach. Tuna
were fed with frozen mackerel blocks thrown using a tube from the boat to the middle of
the cage surface twice per day. As the boat approached, tuna dove downward from the
surface and swam deeper even before the boat or its shadow were visible. Then, the school
remained far from the surface as the boat arrived, the boat was moored beside the cage,
and food was given. Only when the boat departed did the school rise up again. Figure 3
shows the echograms corresponding to the described process, where the depicted distance
of the school gravity center from the cage bottom to the transducer is represented.
This behavior can be interpreted in relation to feeding boat-produced noise, avoidance
movement, and feeding maneuver. However, it is clear that, as expected [
33
], tuna reacted
significantly to noise. Figure 8shows the recorded spectra of the acoustic landscape of the
farm when the feeding boat was moving alongside the cages.
Figure 8.
Spectra of farm acoustic landscape in third-octave levels (dB ref 1
µ
Pa RMS). Lower curve:
in the absence of operations or ships in the farm perimeter; upper curve: during maneuvers of the
feeding boat upper curve.
3.2. Reaction of Tuna to Sound Playback
The farm manager authorized the experiments on the following conditions:
Duration of the experiment is limited to a maximum of three days to avoid possible
cumulative stress and prevent a decrease in tuna meat quality. In the case of a sudden
rise in physiological stress indicators, some experimental time must be disposed of to
ensure normal conditions before the next slaughter period.
Maximum acoustic levels to which the tuna are commonly exposed to at the feeding
installations are not exceeded.
With the aim of accomplishing the previously mentioned goals, the acoustic sound-
scape related to the cage was monitored. The reference conditions inferred from the
conducted measurements consider the maximum sound pressure level generated by ship
noise and lupara shots. In addition, a caution principle was applied regarding the du-
ration of the signals, and it was limited to 15 s. The first assays with tone pulses were
addressed to evaluate whether a panic reaction would compromise the security of the
studied animals. These measurements also allowed for the determination of the presence of
behavioral responses related to the threshold defined by the expected sensitive curve [
16
].
After a period of 48 hours taking data and working with the highest SPL emissions for
each single frequency, it was shown that physiological indicators in the fish slaughtered for
commercial purposes did not show any significant changes that could reveal additional
stress associated with the acoustical study. The signals were projected to the medium every
15 min and had a duration of 15 s. After this first trial, a second experiment was developed
using longer signals with a higher averaged SPL. In the following sections, the results
related to the behavioral reactions under short (from 10 to 15 seconds) and long (from 10 to
15 minutes) emission periods are summarized.
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 11 of 23
The time-table activities carried out during the observation period followed the scheme
summarized below:
The ship arrived everyday early in the morning and was moored alongside the
experiment cage.
The recording system was set up, and feeding boats approached recording tuna
reactions.
During the feeding operations, the ship’s engine was turned on to achieve maximum
battery charge. The sound projector was set up.
All measurements related to recording playbacks were developed in absence of the
ship’s noise (or alternator noise) in the proximity of the cage.
Average background measurements suggested that SPL was around 93 dB ref 1
µ
Pa
with peaks lower than 100 dB ref 1 µPa below 1 kHz. (see Figure 8for details).
The experiment was performed over two daily periods after tuna feeding (morning
and afternoon–evening) and was repeated for 3 days.
In order to understand the results obtained in this experiment, data were analyzed per
day taking into account all of the events emitted during each day. However, some of the
events produced more striking reactions due to the emitted signal characteristics. For this
reason, these events were studied in detail.
3.3. Day 1 Results
During the course of the first day of the experiment, we had the chance to test the
monitoring system and the performance of the sound projector in free-field conditions and
high-power emissions.
The first observations resulted from the comparison of both echosounders readings:
acoustic data from the Knudsen echosounder were closer to saturation (with some values
reaching it) for the given fish mean size and densities than the Biosonics echosounder
data. This was a consequence of the chosen Knudsen gains, resulting in a given dynamic
range for the acoustic backscattering volume strength measurements. We emitted 35 sound
events that allowed us to compare the results of both echosounders when applying the
described parameterization of behavior in terms of the school mass center. A discrepancy
was detected between Knudsen and Biosonics recorded data, with differences in the
capability of detecting slight vertical displacements of the school. The differences were
primarily due to the saturated values produced by the Knudsen echosounder digital gain,
which was solved on the following days. Some other differences could be attributed to the
distance between two echosounders described in the measurement setup.
Regarding the underwater projector performance, it appeared that while reproducing
pure tones at very low frequencies and the highest allowed power excitation, nonlinearities
arose at the dynamic loudspeaker generating super-harmonics with enough amplitude to
overlap the emitted signal (see Figure 9).
Figure 9.
Emitted spectrum affected by loudspeaker nonlinearities for the 50 Hz pure tone excitation.
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 12 of 23
In Figure 10, the results of the first test day are presented. This figure depicts two
graphics: Knudsen echosounder data are shown in the upper part of the graphic (Figure
10a) and Biosonics echosounder in the lower part (Figure 10b). In both, the most important
events are labeled. In Table 1, labeled events are described. A variation in the average
depth of the school was observed in events 4 to 8 (label B in Figure 10a) that corresponds
to a pure tone of 20 Hz with a increasing SPL from 140 dB (event 4) to 165 dB ref 1
µ
Pa
(event 8). The same reaction was shown in events 20 to 23 (label C in Figure 10a), in this
case caused by a 500 Hz pure tone (with the same increase in the level among the events).
On the other hand, for the Biosonics echosounder data (Figure 10b), the average depth and
upper and lower limit variations were determined. When a 1000 Hz pure tone was emitted,
the school was contracted near the surface. In Figure 10b, label A presents an increase in
the lower limit of school due to the 1000 Hz pure tone. The average trace length decreased
with respect to the previous situation, which could indicate an increase in the swimming
velocity of the tuna. Moreover a swimming tilt change was taking place, and the tuna
began to swim upwards. The 20 Hz pure tone, according to the Biosonics echosounder
data, caused school expansion and contraction (label B Figure 10b). The same applied
for the 500 Hz pure tone (label C Figure 10b) and windmill playback (label D in Figure
10b). This was further supported by the measurement of trace length and tilt. Trace length
decreased, and, thus, swimming speed increased. In addition, a rapid change in average
trace tilt occurred, changing from swimming upward to swimming downward repeatedly.
This behavior is normally associated with an alarm situation that causes an increase in
activity and in the expansion and closure of the school [34,35].
Table 1. Description of labeled events presented in Figure 10.
Label Event Emitted Signal Average Trace Length Swimming Tilt (*)
A 1 to 3 1000 Hz 20 pings up
B 4 to 8 20 Hz 15 pings up/down
C 20 to 23 500 Hz 15 pings up/down
D 30 to 34 Windmill 15 pings up/down
(*) Swimming tilt must be understood as change in swimming tilt with respect to the previous situation.
0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35
-30
-20
-10
0
Depth (m)
0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35
Event
-30
-20
-10
0
a)
b)
CD
B
A
BC
Figure 10. (a
) Knudsen echosounder data recorded on day 1;
(b
) Biosonics echosounder data recorded
on day 1. In both cases, the solid line represents the average depth of the school; the dashed line
represents the lower limit of the school; and the point-dashed line represents the upper limit of
the school.
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 13 of 23
3.4. Day 2 Results
During the second working day, 41 events were emitted. Knudsen echosounder digital
gain was balanced, and different pure tones were sequentially emitted. Pure tones of 19 Hz
(events 3 to 5 in Figure 11), 50 Hz (events 6 to 8 in Figure 11), and 300 Hz (events 9 to 11
in Figure 11) were reproduced using the underwater sound projector. Those pure tone
emissions caused an increase in school activity that could be observed at two echosounders
(Figure 11a,b, label A). In Table 2, changes in swimming tilt and length of the traces are
described. Changes in the length of the traces were documented to correspond to changes
in the swimming velocity of the tuna. In the same way, changes in average swimming tuna
tilt were observed, and tuna swam upward and downward. These changes occurred in
expansions and contractions of the school. Additional measurements were carried out by
performing the acoustic emission but considering other types of noise, in this case, a lupara
shot (Figure 11, label B). Specifically, the lupara emission was performed with a peak SPL
of 216.4 dB (ref 1
µ
Pa). As a result, the average depth of the tuna school and the upper and
lower limits decreased. In addition, swimming tilt changed (Table 2), and, consequently,
the school swam farther from the sea surface. After this moment, broadband noises were
emitted. During broadband noises (label C in Figure 11) school behavior remained unstable.
Contractions and expansions of the school were detected. Finally, a 50 Hz pure tone was
emitted with an SPL of 165 dB ref 1
µ
Pa. This emission was repeated from event 38 to
event 40, and it is illustrated in Figure 11, label D. These events caused a large contraction
of the school, so that the difference between the upper and lower limits decreased from 19
to 13 m. Results obtained on the second day indicated that tuna presented the same alarm
behavior as in the previous day.
Table 2. Description of labeled events presented in Figure 11.
Label Event Emitted Signal Average Trace Length Swimming Tilt (*)
A 3 to 11 19 Hz, 50 Hz, and 300 Hz 14 pings down/up/down
B 12 to 13 lupara 11 pings down/up
C 14 to 25 broadband noises 15 pings down/up
D 38 to 40 50 Hz 10 pings up/down
(*) Swimming tilt must be understood as change in swimming tilt respect to the previous situation.
0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40
-30
-20
-10
0
Depth (m)
0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40
Event
-30
-20
-10
0b)
a)
A
AD
D
C
B
BC
Figure 11. (a
) Knudsen echosounder data recorded on day 2;
(b
) Biosonics echosounder data recorded
on day 2. In both cases, the solid line represents the average depth of the school; the dashed line
represents the lower limit of the school; and the point-dashed line represents the upper limit of
the school.
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 14 of 23
3.5. Day 3 Results: Windmill Recorded Playback Increases Exposure
On the last measurement day, 27 events were reproduced, and the results are depicted
in Figure 12. Pure tones of 30 Hz, 50 Hz, and 150 Hz were emitted in events 5 to 9 with a
SPL of 185 dB ref 1
µ
Pa (Figure 12, label A). These emissions caused school contraction and
expansion. As in all other previous cases, low-frequency emissions induced higher activity
of fish. The average values of trace length and trace tilt shown in Table 3support this
statement. A qualitative change occurred when reproducing wind turbine noise recordings
for 15 s after a long pause in the experiments while waiting for the absence of the operational
shipping noise. The results can be observed in Figure 12, label B, and detailed results of the
echogram are given in Figure 13. Behavioral changes were found during the emission of
turbine sound when considering the fact that the SPL equivalent was expected at a distance
of 50 m from the source (windmill). The observed movement pattern of the tuna could be
interpreted as a maneuver to avoid this noise. Once the sound ceased, they recovered their
original distribution in the cage.
Table 3. Description of labeled events presented in Figure 12.
Label Event Emitted Signal Average Trace Length Swimming Tilt (*)
A 5 to 9 30 Hz, 50 Hz, and 150 Hz 14 pings
up/down/up/down
B 11 windmill 15” 10 pings down/up/down
C 24 windmill 15’ 7 pings up/down
D 25 windmill 15’ 8 pings up/down
(*) Swimming tilt must be understood as a change in swimming tilt with respect to the previous situation.
0 5 10 15 20 24 25 27
-30
-20
-10
0
depth (m)
0 5 10 15 20 24 25 27
Event
-30
-20
-10
0b)
a)
A
A
B
B
C
CD
D
Figure 12. (a
) Knudsen echosounder data recorded on day 3;
(b
) Biosonics echosounder data recorded
on day 3. In both cases, the solid line represents the average depth of the school; the dashed line
represents the lower limit of the school; and the point-dashed line represents the upper limit of
the school.
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 15 of 23
Time
Depth of cage (m)
13:33:55 13:34:05 13:34:20
5
10
15
20
25
Time
Depth of cage (m)
13:34:20 13:34:30 13:34:45
5
10
15
20
25
Figure 13.
Echogram corresponding to the reaction to the first short-time (15 s) emission of a windmill
recording, SPL between 110 and 145 dB. The tuna moved upward (upper panel) and recovered their
normal distribution later (lower panel).
With the aim of identifying reactions to acoustic stimulus, the time of exposure to
noise was increased considering turbine sound with an SPL of 182 dB being continuously
emitting for 15 min (label C in Figure 12). During the first emission of sound, the tuna
started to show behavioral changes after 8 min of sound exposure. The observed reaction
can be described in terms of three variables, namely, the position of the school along the
water column, changes in the school swimming pattern considering the size and position,
and the changes in the swimming direction.
School depth: a few minutes after starting the noise emission, the school moved
upward (see Figure 14). The tuna remained closer to the surface even when acoustic
emissions had finished, and only some minutes later did they recover their origi-
nal distribution.
Swimming pattern size and position: the tuna bunched together and swam closer to
one to each other. They still acted like a school with a circular pattern but displaying
circles of a smaller radius and occupying only half of the cage (see Figure 15).
Swimming direction: to identify possible changes in swimming direction, ten ran-
dom intervals of five minutes of video recordings were analyzed before and after
long acoustic emissions. During the intervals before the emission, none of the tuna
changed their swimming direction from that of the school. However, after a long noise
emission, an average of 15 tuna individuals were registered to swim in the opposite
direction at a higher speed. This reaction was observed in addition to the two changes
described above.
A second acoustic emission (label D in Figure 12) was performed 30 min after the first
one. In this case, the same behavioral changes were observed but the reaction appeared
at minute 11 of the emission (3 min later with respect to the previous emission). A third
emission was carried out assuming again 30 min to allow the tuna to recover their normal
behavior. In this case, the tuna did not show any kind of reaction to the noise.
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 16 of 23
Figure 14.
: Echograms showing evolution of the average school depth during a long-time emission
(10 min). The relative absence of tuna tracks in the third echogram can be noted in the left lower
panel. The solid line represents average depth of the school; the dashed line represents the upper
limit and the lower limit of the school. In the lower limit case, the dashed line is split to mark the real
lower limit of the school versus previous lower limit.
Figure 15.
Images recorded during a 10-minute emission. The absence of tuna after a few minutes
should be noted (third frame). Tuna shapes are slightly masked by the presence of copepods on the
camera lens.
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 17 of 23
3.6. Statistics Analysis Results
A two-way ANOVA was conducted to examine the effect of source type and source
level on behavioral variables (Table 4). There was a statistically significant interaction
between the effects of source type and source level on average tilt (F(4, 94) = 4.558, p= 0.002)
and, to a lesser extent, on average length (F(4, 94) = 2.517, p= 0.046). However, a simple
main effects analysis only showed a significant difference in TSP and MLS sources for a
high source level (SPL between 150 and 165 dB ref 1
µ
Pa). This result may be due to the
small number of samples recorded with these sources. Focusing on the independent effect
of each acoustic property of the stimuli, there was a statistically significant difference in
the levels for all variables (p< 0.05). However, a Tukey’s honest significance test (Table
5) revealed that only the average length of traces obtained for very high source level
(SPL > 170 dB ref 1
µ
Pa) was significantly lower than that for the others (47.13
±
22.3,
p< 0.05). This result may be due to the high levels of emitted noise causing an increase
in swimming speed and, therefore, the decrease in the average length of traces. In the
second case, the average tilt was clearly lower when the source was excited with a low
level (SPL between 120 and 140 dB ref 1
µ
Pa) (
4.35
±
0.35, p< 0.05), which denotes a
response of tuna to all of the sound stimuli used in the test with a level clearly higher than
that for the background noise (Table 6). Regarding the type of source, independently, it
has a significant effect on the average depth (F(6, 94) = 5.797, p= 0.000036) and average
tilt (F(6, 94) = 14.471,
p= 1.25 ×1011
). In order to establish a clear separation between the
results obtained for each variable, TSP and MLS sources were removed from the analysis
(
number of samples = less than 2
). Using a Tukey post hoc test, it was possible to establish
two groups divided by sources for average depth. In this case, there was a statistically
significant difference between the group of sources formed by pure tones (
9.61
±
1.54,
p= 0.008
) and windmill noises (
9.32
±
1.39) with regard to chirp (
5.89
±
1.61, p< 0.05),
as shown in Table 7. On the other hand, in Table 8, average tilt shows a clear difference
between the values obtained for background noise (SPL < 120 dB ref 1 µPa) (4.35 ± 0.35,
p< 0.05) and other sources (
Ftone
: 1.35
±
1.92;
Fwindmill
: 3.15
±
1.61;
Flu par a
: 4.80
±
0.01;
Fchir p
:
4.50
±
0.01;
Ftwo-tones
: 3.53
±
0.67; p< 0.05). This result is in accordance with that obtained
for the source-level analysis, that is, the lower level and smallest angle, as the background
noise presents a minimum level.
On the basis of the results presented in Table 9, it can be observed that interaction
between source level and source type did not cause significant differences in the behavioral
reactions of the tuna. However, when the effects of source type and source level are
evaluated independently, a moderate response to acoustic stimuli (B1) presents significant
differences (p« 0.05) with both factors. Unfortunately, this does not occur for the cases of
B0 and B2. The low numbers in B0 (five samples) and B2 (25 samples) measurements may
explain the observed results.
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 18 of 23
Table 4.
Two-way ANOVA conducted on the effect of source type and source level on the behav-
ioral variables.
Source MS df AF p(*)
Upper limit
Source type 4.667 6 1.182 0.323
Source level 22.340 3 11.318 0.000
Source type ×source level 0.375 4 0.142 0.966
Residuals 61.847 94
Lower limit
Source type 41.158 6 1.507 0.184
Source level 41.214 3 3.017 0.034
Source type ×source level 14.326 4 0.951 0.438
Residuals 427.981 94
Average depth
Source type 55.048 6 5.797 0.000
Source level 27.899 3 5.876 0.001
Source type ×source level 5.171 4 0.817 0.518
Residuals 15.5756 94
Average length of traces
Source type 1551.111 6 0.977 0.445
Source level 24,558.073 3 30.939 0.000
Source type ×source level 2663.504 4 2.517 0.046
Residuals 24,870.886 94
Average tilt of traces
Source type 186.391 6 14.471 0.000
Source level 17.505 3 2.718 0.049
Source type ×source level 39.136 4 4.558 0.002
Residuals 201.78 94
(*) p-values « 0.05 = factors with a statistically significant effect with a 95.0% confidence level.
Table 5. Tukey post hoc test for average length. Homogeneous groups for source level.
Source Level N Subset 1 (**) Subset 2 (**)
Very high (SPL > 170 dB ref 1 µPa) 30 47.13
High (SPL = 150–165 dB ref 1 µPa) 53 93.04
Medium (SPL = 140–150 dB ref 1 µPa) 15 79.40
Low (SPL 120–140 dB ref 1 µPa) 9 75.56
Background (SPL < 120 dB ref 1 µPa) 2 77.00
Sig. 1.000 0.312
(**) Subset for alpha = 0.05.
Table 6. Tukey post hoc test for average tilt. Homogeneous groups for source level.
Source Level N Subset 1 (**) Subset 2 (**)
Very high (SPL > 170 dB ref 1 µPa) 30 1.0367
High (SPL = 150–165 dB ref 1 µPa) 53 2.7811
Medium (SPL = 140–150 dB ref 1 µPa) 15 0.9053
Low (SPL = 120–140 dB ref 1 µPa) 9 0.8789
Background (SPL < 120 dB ref 1 µPa) 2 4.3500
Sig. 1.000 0.428
(**) Subset for alpha = 0.05.
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 19 of 23
Table 7. Tukey post hoc test for average depth. Homogeneous groups for source type.
Source Type N Subset 1 (**) Subset 2 (**)
Tone 72 9.6147
Windmill 23 9.3226
Lupara 2 9.2800
Background 2 8.1850 8.1850
Two-tone 6 8.1017 8.1017
Chirp 2 5.8950
Sig. 0.747 0.313
(**) Subset for alpha = 0.05.
Table 8. Tukey post hoc test for average tilt. Homogeneous groups for source type.
Source Type N Subset 1 (**) Subset 2 (**)
Tone 72 1.3582
Windmill 23 3.1565
Lupara 2 4.8000
Background 2 4.3500
Two-tone 6 3.5333
Chirp 2 4.5000
Sig. 1.000 0.123
(**) Subset for alpha = 0.05.
Table 9.
Two-way ANOVA conducted on the effect of source type and source level on the reaction
responses of tuna.
Source MS df AF p(*)
BO
Source type 1.155 1 0.07 0.835
Source level 1.108 1 0.07 0.838
Source type ×Source level 0 0 0 –
Residuals 16.368 1
B1
Source type 261.8 14 2.71 0.006
Source level 867.64 3 41.98 0.800
Source type ×Source level 171.7 18 1.38 0.189
Residuals 289.33 42
B2
Source type 211.401 10 1.57 0.235
Source level 89.385 1 1.63 0.125
Source type ×Source level 24.93 2 0.92 0.4253
Residuals 148.239 11
(*) p-values « 0.05 = factors with a statistically significant effect with a 95.0% confidence level.
4. Discussion
Reactions of tuna to underwater noise were identified by applying a monitoring
methodology based on the combined use of vertical echosounders and video cameras,
which allowed us to parameterize and quantify them. The observed reactions were related
to the emission of signals with a high-power, low-frequency projector, and especially to
low frequencies, pure tones, broadband noises, long exposure activities, and the highest
SPL-emitted values. These reactions can be summarized as follows:
(i) Position change in the water column of the fish school.
(ii) Increase in school activity: contraction and expansion of the school (alarm).
(iii) Displacement and contraction of the school (avoidance).
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 20 of 23
(iv)
After the longest emissions, some specimens swam in the opposite direction to the
rest of school, which could be interpreted as slight disorientation.
(v) increased speed.
Reactions to noise summarized above were repeatedly observed during the various
emissions. Emission duration had to be increased progressively to observe similar reactions
via various measurements. Therefore, for semi-captive bluefin tuna, a high degree of
adaptability to noise could be considered.
To reinforce this, a statistical analysis was carried out to verify the relationship between
the observed reactions and the applied stimuli. Behavioral parameters have a strong
dependence on source level. On the other hand, only two parameters, average depth and
average tilt angle, presented significant differences with source type. This suggests that
behavioral responses are potentially affected by level regardless of the type of emitted noise.
Moreover, average tilt length showed a statistically significant difference with interaction
in regard to the effects of source type and source level. A deeper analysis revealed that
average tilt differences were found when the sound level of stimuli was higher than that
of the background noise. However, the average tilt length depends on very high levels
of sound. The two-way ANOVA test results, as shown in Table 9, indicate that only
moderate reactions presented significant statistical differences with source level and source
type effects when they were independently evaluated. This reaction level was the most
commonly found in this study, being observed in 69% of cases. Severe reactions were
observed in a lower number of cases, which could be too low for a significant analysis.
Emission conditions could have been forced in order to observe a higher number of severe
reactions. However, this could have caused unintended damages to fishes, and this is the
reason why this test was not carried out.
The current experiment proved that tuna behavior can be affected by noisy stimuli,
but more exhaustive experiments should be developed to obtain a complete characteri-
zation of time and intensity thresholds and to properly determine the effect of turbine
operational noise on bluefin tuna. It would be especially interesting to increase the time
duration of exposure to noise as well as the SPL of the emitted signals in order to obtain
results related to tuna’s adaptability to noise or accumulative stress effects. It is important
to also remark that we were working with semi-captive animals, which do not have the
same constraints as those of tuna in the wild (e.g., they do not need to hunt for prey; they
swim in a limited space; they may have developed a level of tolerance to ship noise).
It is difficult to study the effect of human activities on the marine environment. Some-
times, the obtained results are tentative or inconclusive, but the existing lack of knowledge
should encourage the development of specific studies. In relation to underwater noise,
knowledge about physiology or behavioral changes is required to develop methodolo-
gies and tools that allow for the relationship between pollutants and the effect that they
have to be determined. The proposed methodology offers a way to develop objective
observations with acoustical and image recordings susceptible of automatic processing to
parameterize behavioral changes. While the use of echosounders and video cameras (single
or stereoscopic) can allow one to properly describe the produced reactions, a discussion
regarding the availability of low-frequency underwater projectors for scientific studies
is still necessary. In our work, and thanks to the collaboration of the Spanish Army, we
had the rare opportunity to use a high-power, low-frequency projector, usually restricted
to military uses (mainly for budget reasons), which offered the possibility of emitting
signals with a source level above 150 dB between 20 Hz and 20 KHz. There is a general
concern about the lack of proper low-frequency sources for research purposes. Piezoelectric
technologies usually cannot achieve such lower frequencies, and they do not have flat fre-
quency responses. Moreover, underwater electrodynamic loudspeakers are usually found
with limitations in operational depth and have efficient frequencies only above 100 Hz and
below 20 kHz. Therefore, there is a need for new commercial developments that facilitate
access to military technology for marine research and assessment, including hydrostatic
compensation mechanisms and high-power emissions with limited nonlinearities due
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 21 of 23
to source performance. High source levels are associated with nonlinear propagation
because of the high-pressure wave interacting with the medium, but undesired nonlinear
oscillations due to device aging (as attributed in our case by the army maintenance service)
or the oscillator’s characteristics can arise at higher power levels, altering the desired
spectral content of the emitted signal. This aspect must be examined, and it can also be
compensated for by characterizing the transfer function of the emitting device and properly
designing the excitation signals.
In migratory species, as in the case of bluefin tuna, behavioral changes can be highly
relevant to their spawning periods and routes. Considered recently a species with en-
dangered populations, it is very important to study the habits of bluefin tuna and their
possible conditioning. Bluefin tuna are extremely difficult to manipulate in captivity, and it
is challenging to study them in the wild because of their great mobility. The restrictions
in catching quotas also limit the access to individuals for experimentation. The activities
presented in this work support the initial insight into the problems concerning the effects of
anthropogenic noise and particularly offshore wind turbine noise on bluefin tuna behavior.
Author Contributions:
Conceptualization, Vicente Puig-Pons, Isabel Pérez-Arjona, Víctor Espinosa,
Fernando De La Gándara and Eladio Santaella; Data curation, Vicente Puig-Pons and Pedro Poveda-
Martínez; Formal analysis, Vicente Puig-Pons, Víctor Espinosa, Jaime Ramis Soriano and Marek
Moszy´nski; Funding acquisition, Eladio Santaella; Investigation, Ester Soliveres, Víctor Espinosa,
Pedro Poveda-Martínez, Jaime Ramis Soriano, Patricia Ordoñez-Cebrián, Fernando De La Gándara,
José Luis Cort and Eladio Santaella; Methodology, Vicente Puig-Pons, Isabel Pérez-Arjona, Víctor
Espinosa, Jaime Ramis Soriano, Fernando De La Gándara, Manuel Bou-Cabo and Eladio Santaella;
Project administration, Víctor Espinosa; Software, Vicente Puig-Pons; Supervision, Víctor Espinosa
and Manuel Bou-Cabo; Validation, Marek Moszy ´nski; Visualization, Pedro Poveda-Martínez; Writing
– original draft, Vicente Puig-Pons; Writing – review & editing, Isabel Pérez-Arjona, Víctor Espinosa,
Marek Moszy´nski, Fernando De La Gándara and Manuel Bou-Cabo. All authors have read and
agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding:
This research was partially funded by Instituto Español de Oceanografia (IEO) through
the contract "Servicio de caracterizacion de comportamiento del atun rojo (Thunnus thynnus) ante
estimulos externos y mediante sistemas acusticos en granjas de engorde de atun rojo - EXP. N
º
67/12"
Institutional Review Board Statement:
The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the
Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical review and approval were waived for this study, because underwater
sound produced for checking the buefin tuna behaviour was equal or lower in intensity to the sound
usually produced by the boats distributed the feeding to tunas in the cages and therefore without
causing them any suffering. The feeding activity is a normal activity in the commercial maintenance
of bluefin tuna in cages.
Informed Consent Statement: Not applicable
Acknowledgments:
The authors would like to acknowledge the efforts of Balfegó and Balfegó S.L. in
the project, without whose collaboration this work could not have been developed. Special thanks are
also given to the Spanish Navy and its personnel at the Submarines Base of Cartagena for providing
the underwater low-frequency projector and assisting in its operation. Further special thanks go to
the crew of the trawler Nuevo Tomás y Carmen (Santa Pola, Spain), our “research vessel”, who assisted
greatly during the measurements in the sea off the cost of L’Ametlla (Tarragona), Spain.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
1.
Tournadre, J. Anthropogenic pressure on the open ocean: The growth of ship traffic revealed by altimeter data analysis. Geophys.
Res. Lett. 2014,41, 7924–7932.
2.
Technical Guidance for Assessing the Effects of Anthropogenic Sound on Marine Mammal Hearing (Version 2.0): Underwater Thresholds for
Onset of Permanent and Temporary Threshold Shifts; National Marine Fisheries Service: Silver Spring, MD, USA, 2018; pp. 1–167.
3.
Dekeling, R.P.A.; Tasker, M.L.; Van der Graaf, A.J.; Ainslie, M.A.; Andersson, M.H.; André, M.; Borsani, J.F.; Brensing, K.;
Castellote, M.; Cronin, D.; et al. Monitoring Guidance for Underwater Noise in European Seas-Part II: Monitoring Guidance Specifications;
Publications Office of the European Union: Luxembourg, 2014.
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 22 of 23
4.
Peng, C.; Zhao, X.; Liu, G. Noise in the Sea and Its Impacts on Marine Organisms. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health
2015
,
12, 12304–12323.
5. Fay, R.R.; Popper, A.N. Evolution and hearing in vertebrates: The inner ears and processing. Hear. Res. 2000,149, 1–10.
6.
Popper, A.N.; Hawkins, A.D. An overview of fish bioacoustics and the impacts of anthropogenic sounds on fishes. J. Fish Biol.
2019,94, 692–713.
7. Kasumyan, A.O. Structure and function of the auditory system in fishes. J. Ichthyol. 2005,45, 223–270.
8.
Nedwell, J.R.; Turnpenny, A.; Langworthy, J.; Edwards, B. Measurements of Underwater Noise during Piling at the Red Funnel Terminal,
Southampton, and Onservations of Its Effect on Caged Fish; Subacoustech Ltd.: Hampshire, UK, 2003; Volume 558.
9.
Ruggerone, G.T.; Goodman, S.E.; Miner, R. Behavioral Response and Survival of Juvenile Coho Salmon to Pile Driving Sounds; Natural
Resources Consultants, Inc.: Seattle, WA, USA, 2008.
10.
Pile Installation Demonstration Project, Fisheries Impact Assessment; San Francisco Oakland Bay Bridge East Span Seismic Safety
Project; Caltran: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2001.
11.
Madsen, P.T.; Wahlberg, M.; Tougaard, J.; Lucke, K.; Tyack, A.P. Wind turbine underwater noise and marine mammals: Implica-
tions of current knowledge and data needs. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 2006,309, 279–295.
12.
Bailey, H.; Brookes, K.L.; Thompson, P.M. Assessing environmental impacts of offshore wind farms: Lessons learned and
recommendations for the future. Aquat. Biosyst. 2014,10, 1–13.
13.
Tougaard, J.; Hermannsen, L.; Madsen, P.T. How loud is the underwater noise from operating offshore wind turbines? J. Acoust.
Soc. Am. 2020 ,148, 2885.
14.
Mooney, T.A.; Andersson, M.H.; Stanley, J. Acoustic Impacts of Offshore Wind Energy on Fishery Resources: An Evolving Source
and Varied Effects Across a Wind Farm’s Lifetime. Oceanography 2020,33, 82–95.
15.
Farr, H.; Ruttenberg, B.; Walter, R.K.; Wang, Y.H.; White, C. Potential environmental effects of deepwater floating offshore wind
energy facilities. Ocean Coast. Manag. 2021,207, 105611.
16.
Dale, J.J.; Gray, M.D.; Popper, A.N.; Rogers, P.H.; Block, B.A. Hearing thresholds of swimming Pacific bluefin tuna Thunnus
orientalis. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2015,201, 441–454.
17.
Hawkins, A.D.; Roberts, L.; Cheesman, S. Responses of free-living coastal pelagic fish to impulsive sounds. J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
2014,135, 3101–3116.
18.
Wisniewska, D.M.; Teilmann, J.; Hermannsen, L.; Johnson, M.; Miller, L.A.; Siebert, U.; Madsen, P.T. Quantitative Measures
of Anthropogenic Noise on Harbor Porpoises: Testing the Reliability of Acoustic Tag Recordings. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol.
2016
,
875, 1237–1242.
19.
van Beest, F.M.; Teilmann, J.; Hermannsen, L.; Galatius, A.; Mikkelsen, L.; Sveegaard, S.; Balle, J.D.; Dietz, R.; Nabe-Nielsen, J.
Fine-scale movement responses of free-ranging harbour porpoises to capture, tagging and short-term noise pulses from a single
airgun. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2018,5, 170110.
20.
Hoyle, S.D.; Leroy, B.M.; Nicol, S.J.; Hampton, W.J. Covariates of release mortality and tag loss in large-scale tuna tagging
experiments. Fish. Res. 2015,163, 106–118.
21.
Doksæter, L.; Handegard, N.O.; Godø, O.R.; Kvadsheim, P.H.; Nordlund, N. Behavior of captive herring exposed to naval sonar
transmissions (1.0–1.6 kHz) throughout a yearly cycle. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2012,131, 1632.
22.
Allen, S.; Demer, D.A. Detection and characterization of yellowfin and bluefin tuna using passive-acoustical techniques. Fish. Res.
2003,63, 393–403.
23.
Stan, G.-B.; Embrechts, J.-J.; Archambeau, D. Comparison of different impulse response measurement techniques. J. Audio Eng.
Soc. 2002,50, 249–262.
24.
Urmy, S.S.; Horne, J.K.; Barbee, D.H. Measuring the vertical distributional variability of pelagic fauna in Monterey Bay. ICES J.
Mar. Sci. 2014,69, 184–196.
25. Otsu, N.A. Threshold Selection Method from Gray-Level Histograms. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man. Cybern 1979,9, 62–66.
26.
Puig-Pons, V.; Muñoz-Benavent, P.; Espinosa, V.; Andreu-García, G.; Valiente-González, J.; Estruch, V.; Ordóñez, P.;
Pérez-Arjona, I.;
Atienza, V.; Mèlich, B.; Gándara, F.; et al. Automatic Bluefin Tuna (Thunnus thynnus) biomass estimation during
transfers using acoustic and computer vision techniques. Aquac. Eng. 2019,85, 22–31.
27.
La Manna, G.; Manghi, M.; Perretti, F.; Sarà, G. Behavioral response of brown meagre (Sciaena umbra) to boat noise. Mar. Pollut.
Bull. 2016,110, 324–334.
28.
Herbert-Read, J.E.; Kremer, L.; Bruintjes, R.; Radford, A.N.; Ioannou, C.C. Anthropogenic noise pollution from pile-driving
disrupts the structure and dynamics of fish shoals. Proc. R. Soc. Biol. Sci. 2017,284, 20171627.
29. StatPoint Technologies, Inc. Statgraphics Centurion XVIII; StatPoint Technologies, Inc.: Warrenton, VA, USA, 2021.
30.
Komeyama, K.; Kadota, M.; Torisawa, S.; Suzuki, K.; Tsuda, Y.; Takagi, T. Measuring the Swimming Behaviour of a Reared Pacific
Bluefin Tuna in a Submerged Aquaculture Net Cage. Aquat. Living Resour. 2011,24, 99–105.
31.
Komeyama, K.; Kadota, M.; Torisawa, S.; Takagi, T. Three-Dimensional Trajectories of Cultivated Pacific Bluefin Tuna Thunnus
Orientalis in an Aquaculture Net Cage. Aquac. Environ. Interact. 2013,4, 81–90.
32.
Nucci, M.E.; Costa, C.; Scardi, M.; Cataudella, S. Preliminary observations on bluefin tuna (Thunnus thynnus, Linnaeus 1758)
behaviour in captivity. J. Appl. Ichthyol. 2010,26, 95–98.
33.
Sarà, G.; Dean, J.M.; d’Amato, D.; Buscaino, G.; Oliveri, A.; Genovese, S.; Ferro, S.; Buffa, G.; Martire, M.L.; Mazzola, S. Effect of
boat noise on the behaviour of bluefin tuna Thunnus Thynnus in the Mediterranean Sea. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser.
2007
,331, 243–253.
Sensors 2021,21, 6998 23 of 23
34. Popper, A.N; Schilt. C.R. Hearing and Acoustic Behavior: Basic and Applied Considerations. Fish Bioacoust. 2008,32, 17–48.
35. Popper, A.N.; Hastings, M.C. The effects of anthropogenic sources of sound on fishes. J. Fish Biol. 2009,75, 455–489.
... Thus, anything that interferes with the ability of animals to detect sounds has the potential to significantly impair survival of individuals and populations (see Slabbekoorn et al., 2018). Some sounds produced by anthropogenic sources may also elicit behavioral responses and/or physiological effects that interfere with biological activities, such as feeding or spawning (Carroll et al., 2017;Jones et al., 2021;Puig-Pons et al., 2021). ...
... While some research exists on the responses of fishes and aquatic invertebrates to a variety of different sounds, less is known about how sounds specifically emitted from OSW energy development could potentially alter behaviors (e.g., Wahlberg and Westerberg, 2005;Siddagangaiah et al., 2021;Zhang et al., 2021). However, the available data suggest that behavioral changes resulting from exposure to sounds from OSW energy development could be a concern for at least some species (e.g., Perrow et al., 2011;Thomsen et al., 2012;Hawkins et al., 2014;Iafrate et al., 2016;Methratta and Dardick, 2019;Kok et al., 2021;Puig-Pons et al., 2021). A range of behavioral changes with potential fitness consequences have been hypothesized, in part, based on observations or inference from responses to other anthropogenic or environmental noise sources. ...
... Few studies have examined the influence of offshore wind farm operational noise, indicating a varied response in different fish species (Svendsen-Erquiaga et al. 2022 ), mainly observed over a short period (10 to 40 days). Bluefin tuna in semi-free conditions exhibited altered movement patterns when exposed to recordings of wind farm operational noise measured ∼50 m from the turbine (Puig-Pons et al. 2021 ). The wind farm harmonics at 125 Hz, with sound levels reaching 96 dB re 1 μPa, can potentially interfere with the hearing thresholds of the Marbled rockfish (Zhang et al. 2021 ). ...
... When exposed to continual operational noise in the laboratory for three days to a week, the milkfish showed elevated mRNA levels of hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase (Wei et al. 2018 ). However, studies have emphasized that species' behaviors and physiological changes associated with operational noise are usually difficult under semi-free and laboratory conditions (Puig-Pons et al. 2021 ) and cannot be extrapolated to the species' behaviors in their natural habitats (Wei et al. 2018, Svendsen et al. 2022. Thus, it is necessary to understand the specific behavior of the species during the several phases of the offshore wind farm project, which may help identify the specific stressors and the corresponding phases that affect the species' behaviors. ...
Article
Full-text available
Offshore wind farms have recently emerged as a renewable energy solution. However, the long-term impacts of wind turbine noise on fish chorusing phenology are largely unknown. We deployed a hydrophone 10 m from a foremost turbine in Taiwan situated at the Miaoli offshore wind farm (Taiwan Strait) for two years to investigate sound levels and assess the potential influence of turbine noise on seasonal fish chorusing patterns during 2017 and 2018. Wind turbine noise (measured in the 20–250 Hz frequency band) was significantly higher in autumn and winter (mean SPL: 138–143 dB re 1 μPa) and was highly correlated with wind speed (r = 0.76, P < 0.001). During both years, fish chorusing exhibited a consistent trend, that is, beginning in spring, peaking in summer, decreasing in autumn, and absent in winter. Our results show the noise from a single turbine during the two-year monitoring period did not influence the seasonal fish chorusing (r = −0.17, P ≈ 1). Since the offshore wind farm installations are growing in magnitude and capacity across the Taiwan Strait, this study for the first time provides baseline operational sound levels and an understanding of the fish seasonal vocalization behavior at the foremost turbine of the first wind farm in Taiwan. The results presented here provide useful insights for policymakers and constitute a reference starting point for advancing knowledge on the possible effects of wind turbines on fish chorusing in the studied area.
... Thus, anything that interferes with the ability of animals to detect sounds has the potential to significantly impair survival of individuals and populations (see Slabbekoorn et al., 2018). Some sounds produced by anthropogenic sources may also elicit behavioral responses and/or physiological effects that interfere with biological activities, such as feeding or spawning (Carroll et al., 2017;Jones et al., 2021;Puig-Pons et al., 2021). ...
... While some research exists on the responses of fishes and aquatic invertebrates to a variety of different sounds, less is known about how sounds specifically emitted from OSW energy development could potentially alter behaviors (e.g., Wahlberg and Westerberg, 2005;Siddagangaiah et al., 2021;Zhang et al., 2021). However, the available data suggest that behavioral changes resulting from exposure to sounds from OSW energy development could be a concern for at least some species (e.g., Perrow et al., 2011;Thomsen et al., 2012;Hawkins et al., 2014;Iafrate et al., 2016;Methratta and Dardick, 2019;Kok et al., 2021;Puig-Pons et al., 2021). A range of behavioral changes with potential fitness consequences have been hypothesized, in part, based on observations or inference from responses to other anthropogenic or environmental noise sources. ...
Article
No PDF available ABSTRACT There are substantial knowledge gaps regarding both the bioacoustics and the responses of animals to sounds associated with pre-construction, construction, and operations of offshore wind (OSW) energy development. A workgroup of the 2020 State of the Science Workshop on Wildlife and Offshore Wind Energy recommended priority studies for the next five years to help stakeholders better understand potential cumulative biological impacts of sound and vibration to fishes and aquatic invertebrates as the OSW industry develops. The workgroup identified seven short-term priorities that include a mix of primary research and coordination efforts. Key research needs include the examination of animal displacement and other behavioral responses to sound, as well as hearing sensitivity studies related to particle motion, substrate vibration, and sound pressure. Other needs include: identification of priority taxa on which to focus research; standardization of methods; development of a long-term highly instrumented field site; and examination of sound mitigation options for fishes and aquatic invertebrates. Effective assessment of potential cumulative impacts of sound and vibration on fishes and aquatic invertebrates is currently precluded by these and other knowledge gaps. Filling critical gaps in knowledge will improve our understanding of possible sound-related impacts of OSW energy development to populations and ecosystems.
... Thus, anything that interferes with the ability of animals to detect sounds has the potential to significantly impair survival of individuals and populations (see Slabbekoorn et al., 2018). Some sounds produced by anthropogenic sources may also elicit behavioral responses and/or physiological effects that interfere with biological activities, such as feeding or spawning (Carroll et al., 2017;Jones et al., 2021;Puig-Pons et al., 2021). ...
... While some research exists on the responses of fishes and aquatic invertebrates to a variety of different sounds, less is known about how sounds specifically emitted from OSW energy development could potentially alter behaviors (e.g., Wahlberg and Westerberg, 2005;Siddagangaiah et al., 2021;Zhang et al., 2021). However, the available data suggest that behavioral changes resulting from exposure to sounds from OSW energy development could be a concern for at least some species (e.g., Perrow et al., 2011;Thomsen et al., 2012;Hawkins et al., 2014;Iafrate et al., 2016;Methratta and Dardick, 2019;Kok et al., 2021;Puig-Pons et al., 2021). A range of behavioral changes with potential fitness consequences have been hypothesized, in part, based on observations or inference from responses to other anthropogenic or environmental noise sources. ...
Article
Full-text available
There are substantial knowledge gaps regarding both the bioacoustics and the responses of animals to sounds associated with pre-construction, construction, and operations of offshore wind (OSW) energy development. A workgroup of the 2020 State of the Science Workshop on Wildlife and Offshore Wind Energy identified studies for the next five years to help stakeholders better understand potential cumulative biological impacts of sound and vibration to fishes and aquatic invertebrates as the OSW industry develops. The workgroup identified seven short-term priorities that include a mix of primary research and coordination efforts. Key research needs include the examination of animal displacement and other behavioral responses to sound, as well as hearing sensitivity studies related to particle motion, substrate vibration, and sound pressure. Other needs include: identification of priority taxa on which to focus research; standardization of methods; development of a long-term highly instrumented field site; and examination of sound mitigation options for fishes and aquatic invertebrates. Effective assessment of potential cumulative impacts of sound and vibration on fishes and aquatic invertebrates is currently precluded by these and other knowledge gaps. However, filling critical gaps in knowledge will improve our understanding of possible sound-related impacts of OSW energy development to populations and ecosystems.
... Operational noise may impact bluefin tuna's behavior, potentially influencing their feeding habits and reproductive migration patterns (Vicente, 2021). If addressing noise is necessary within a plan or consent condition, it is preferable to establish a maximum allowable level at the point of reception rather than specifying turbine location, distance, or type. ...
Research
Full-text available
Expanding renewable energy, particularly wind farms, is crucial in addressing global energy demands and mitigating environmental issues associated with fossil fuel consumption. Within New Zealand, using renewable energy sources like wind power is essential for reducing dependence on non-renewable alternatives and advancing efforts towards decarbonization (Wind Farm Development in New Zealand, 2013). Otahome, located in the Masterton District approximately 60 kilometers east of Masterton, is positioned to contribute to New Zealand's renewable energy objectives by establishing a wind farm along Otahome Road adjacent to the Whareama River. Oversight for this initiative falls under the jurisdiction of the Masterton District Council and the Greater Wellington Regional Council. The proposed wind farm in Otahome is strategically sited to harness the region's abundant wind resources. The Wairarapa Plains benefit from consistent wind patterns conducive to wind energy generation, complemented by a generally sunny climate boasting over 2000 hours of sunshine annually. However, variations in rainfall levels and occasional rapid temperature fluctuations are influenced by the local terrain, particularly the Rimutaka and Tararua Ranges (Proposed Wairarapa Combined District Plan, 2023). These geographical features significantly impact weather patterns, with sheltered areas experiencing calmer conditions and lower rainfall, while stronger winds and higher precipitation levels characterize the southern plains. Furthermore, the proposed wind farm site's nearness to existing infrastructure, such as transmission lines and road networks, facilitates project development and connectivity to the grid, thereby enhancing project feasibility (Authority, 2004). Additionally, the minimal residential presence in Otahome is conducive to effective wind farm operations, minimizing potential conflicts and optimizing safety, efficiency, and environmental impact (Proposed Wairarapa Combined District Plan, 2023). However, the development of wind farms necessitates adherence to various policies and planning requirements to ensure sustainable resource management and environmental protection. The Resource Management Act (RMA) of 1991 serves as the primary legislative framework governing ecological management in New Zealand, mandating projects to consider environmental effects and engage with stakeholders 3 (Wind farm development in New Zealand, 2013). Furthermore, adherence to regional and district plans designed for wind farm development in the Wairarapa region is vital, addressing aspects such as land use and environmental impact assessments (Proposed Wairarapa Combined District Plan, 2023). To mitigate potential social and environmental impacts, demanding assessment methodologies and approaches are recommended, including visual impact assessment, noise impact assessment, avian mortality studies, land use change analysis, and social impact assessment (Gkeka-Serpetsidaki et al., 2022; Fredianelli et al., 2017; Jenkins et al., 2018; Pekkan et al., 2021; Shah et al., 2023). This assessment aims to evaluate and address the project's effects on landscape aesthetics, noise pollution, wildlife, land use patterns, and community dynamics. The proposed wind farm project in Otahome offers a significant opportunity to contribute to New Zealand's renewable energy objectives while addressing various social, environmental, and regulatory considerations. By leveraging the region's abundant wind resources and adhering to sustainable development practices, the project aims to facilitate the transition towards a more environmentally sustainable energy landscape.
... (1) Laboratory tank (lab tank; (e.g., Hubert et al., 2022;Jézéquel et al., 2021;Jimenez et al., 2020;Lara and Vasconcelos, 2021;McCormick et al., 2019;Mooney et al., 2020;Neo et al., 2015;Olivier et al., 2022;Roberts et al., 2016a;Smith et al., 2011;Spiga et al., 2017;Voellmy et al., 2014)); (2) In-ground pond/tank or above-ground tank (outdoor tank; (e.g., Davidson et al., 2009;Jones et al., 2023;Song et al., 2021)); (3) Large water bodies such as a pond, river, lake, or ocean/bay with animals confined in cages (open-water, confined; (e.g., Buscaino et al., 2010;Day et al., 2017, p. 201;Hawkins and Chapman, 2020;Hubert et al., 2020;Jézéquel et al., 2022aJézéquel et al., , 2023cJézéquel et al., , 2023aJézéquel et al., , 2023bMagnhagen et al., 2017;Sarà et al., 2007)); and (4) Large water body with free-ranging animals (open-water, free-ranging; (e.g., McQueen et al., 2022Puig-Pons et al., 2021;Simpson et al., 2005Simpson et al., , 2016bSimpson et al., , 2016aStaaterman et al., 2020)). ...
Technical Report
Full-text available
The report provides recommendations on research methodologies to address fishes and aquatic invertebrates behavioral and physiological effects from particle motion and substrate-borne vibration exposure.
... This disturbance can reduce the foraging efficiency of fish and make them more susceptible to predation [6][7][8][9][10]. Furthermore, an increase in noise levels can be traced to significant alterations in fish behavior and physiology [11][12][13][14]. Exposure to high-intensity noise can even lead to fish mortality [15]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study assessed the impact of an acoustic stimulus on the behavioral responses and physiological states of the large yellow croaker (Larimichthys crocea). The test fish, with an average body weight of approximately 352.81 ± 70.99 g, were exposed to one hour of acoustic stimulation at seven different frequencies: 100 Hz, 125 Hz, 160 Hz, 200 Hz, 500 Hz, 630 Hz, and 800 Hz. The aim was to delineate the specific effects of acoustic stimulation on the behavior and physiological indices. The results show that acoustic stimulation significantly altered the behavioral patterns of the large yellow croaker, predominantly manifested as avoidance behavior towards the sound source. At a stimulus frequency of 630 Hz, the test fish exhibited continuous irregular motion and erratic swimming. Physiologically, one hour of exposure to acoustic stimulation notably affected the endocrine system. The levels of Epinephrine and thyroxine were significantly elevated at 200 Hz, while the cortisol levels did not show significant differences. Additionally, the lactic acid content significantly increased at 800 Hz, and the blood glucose content peaked at 630 Hz. This study discovered that sound frequencies of 200 Hz, 630 Hz, and 800 Hz led to a significant increase in the levels of Epinephrine, glucose, thyroid hormones, and lactate in large yellow croaker, consequently affecting their behavior. The changes in these physiological indicators reflect the stress response of the large yellow croaker in specific sonic environments, providing crucial insights into the physiological and behavioral responses of fish to acoustic stimuli.
Technical Report
Full-text available
Canarias is one of the regions with the greatest diversity of cetaceans at the European level, and the development of offshore wind energy projects in the islands' waters poses challenges in terms of potential impacts on this group of animals. Despite the rapid expansion expected for the marine wind industry, there is a significant lack of assessment regarding its environmental impacts or benefits, as well as a shortage in quantifying and studying all species present in our waters. The objective of this report is to compile existing information on the possible impacts of offshore wind energy development on cetacean populations in the Canary Islands.
Article
Full-text available
Over the last few decades, the offshore wind energy industry has expanded its scope from turbines mounted on foundations driven into the seafloor and standing in less than 60 m of water, to floating turbines moored in 120 m of water, to prospecting the development of floating turbines moored in ~1,000 m of water. Since there are few prototype turbines and mooring systems of these deepwater, floating offshore wind energy facilities (OWFs) currently deployed, their effects on the marine environment are speculative. Using the available scientific literature concerning appropriate analogs, including fixed-bottom OWFs, land-based wind energy facilities, wave and tidal energy devices, and oil and gas platforms, we conducted a qualitative systematic review to estimate the potential environmental effects of deepwater, floating OWFs during operation, as well as potential mitigation measures to address some of the effects. We evaluated six categories of potential effects: changes to atmospheric and oceanic dynamics due to energy removal and modifications, electromagnetic field effects on marine species from power cables, habitat alterations to benthic and pelagic fish and invertebrate communities, underwater noise effects on marine species, structural impediments to wildlife, and changes to water quality. Our synthesis of 89 articles selected for the review suggests that many of these potential effects could be mitigated to pose a low risk to the marine environment if developers adopt appropriate mitigation strategies and best-practice protocols. This review takes the necessary first steps in summarizing the available information on the potential environmental effects of deepwater, floating OWFs and can serve as a reference document for marine scientists and engineers, the energy industry, permitting agencies and regulators of the energy industry, project developers, and concerned stakeholders such as coastal residents, conservationists, and fisheries.
Article
Full-text available
Offshore wind farms are proliferating around the world, and their presence is expected to expand substantially within US waters. Wind farm lifetimes involve 40–50-year commitments, including site surveys, construction, operation, and eventual decommissioning. Because their areas often overlap with essential fisheries habitats, there is a need to understand, mitigate, and manage offshore wind farm impacts on fisheries and ecosystems. Activities during all phases of wind farm lifetimes produce underwater sound, a concern because high noise levels and/or persistent anthropogenic noise can impact marine life in many ways. Here, we review the current understanding of impacts of wind energy activities on fisheries resources, taking into account the varied noise conditions that occur from site survey to decommissioning. For certain portions of wind farm development, such as construction and operation, there is a small amount of available data that allows stakeholders to evaluate impacts for at least some taxa. Yet, we are data deficient for most species’ populations, life stages, and other phases as they relate to wind farm development. Thus, it is difficult to evaluate impacts with any certainty, underscoring the need for further studies to adequately address impacts of offshore wind farms on vulnerable and ecologically and economically important taxa.
Article
Full-text available
Offshore wind turbines are increasingly abundant sources of underwater low frequency noise. This increase raises concern for the cumulative contribution of wind farms to the underwater soundscape and possible impact on marine ecosystems. Here, available measurements of underwater noise from different wind turbines during operation are reviewed to show that source levels are at least 10-20 dB lower than ship noise in the same frequency range. The most important factor explaining the measured sound pressure levels from wind turbines is distance to the turbines with smaller effects of wind speed and turbine size. A simple multi-turbine model demonstrates that cumulative noise levels could be elevated up to a few kilometres from a wind farm under very low ambient noise conditions. In contrast, the noise is well below ambient levels unless it is very close to the individual turbines in locations with high ambient noise from shipping or high wind speeds. The rapid increase in the number and size of offshore wind farms means that the cumulative contribution from the many turbines may be considerable and should be included in assessments for maritime spatial planning purposes as well and environmental impact assessments of individual projects.
Article
Full-text available
Fishes use a variety of sensory systems to learn about their environments and to communicate. Of the various senses, hearing plays a particularly important role for fishes in providing information, often from great distances, from all around these animals. This information is in all three spatial dimensions, often overcoming the limitations of other senses such as vision, touch, taste and smell. Sound is used for communication between fishes, mating behaviour, the detection of prey and predators, orientation and migration and habitat selection. Thus, anything that interferes with the ability of a fish to detect and respond to biologically relevant sounds can decrease survival and fitness of individuals and populations. Since the onset of the Industrial Revolution, there has been a growing increase in the noise that humans put into the water. These anthropogenic sounds are from a wide range of sources that include shipping, sonars, construction activities (e.g., wind farms, harbours), trawling, dredging and exploration for oil and gas. Anthropogenic sounds may be sufficiently intense to result in death or mortal injury. However, anthropogenic sounds at lower levels may result in temporary hearing impairment, physiological changes including stress effects, changes in behaviour or the masking of biologically important sounds. The intent of this paper is to review the potential effects of anthropogenic sounds upon fishes, the potential consequences for populations and ecosystems and the need to develop sound exposure criteria and relevant regulations. However, assuming that many readers may not have a background in fish bioacoustics, the paper first provides information on underwater acoustics, with a focus on introducing the very important concept of particle motion, the primary acoustic stimulus for all fishes, including elasmobranchs. The paper then provides background material on fish hearing, sound production and acoustic behaviour. This is followed by an overview of what is known about effects of anthropogenic sounds on fishes and considers the current guidelines and criteria being used world‐wide to assess potential effects on fishes. Most importantly, the paper provides the most complete summary of the effects of anthropogenic noise on fishes to date. It is also made clear that there are currently so many information gaps that it is almost impossible to reach clear conclusions on the nature and levels of anthropogenic sounds that have potential to cause changes in animal behaviour, or even result in physical harm. Further research is required on the responses of a range of fish species to different sound sources, under different conditions. There is a need both to examine the immediate effects of sound exposure and the longer‐term effects, in terms of fitness and likely impacts upon populations.
Article
Full-text available
Knowledge about the impact of anthropogenic disturbances on the behavioural responses of cetaceans is constrained by lack of data on fine-scale movements of individuals. We equipped five free-ranging harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) with high-resolution location and dive loggers and exposed them to a single 10 inch3 underwater airgun producing high-intensity noise pulses (2–3 s intervals) for 1 min. All five porpoises responded to capture and tagging with longer, faster and more directed movements as well as with shorter, shallower, less wiggly dives immediately after release, with natural behaviour resumed in less than or equal to 24 h. When we exposed porpoises to airgun pulses at ranges of 420–690m with noise level estimates of 135–147 dB re 1 μPa2s (sound exposure level), one individual displayed rapid and directed movements away from the exposure site and two individuals used shorter and shallower dives compared to natural behaviour immediately after exposure. Noise-induced movement typically lasted for less than or equal to 8 h with an additional 24 h recovery period until natural behaviour was resumed. The remaining individuals did not show any quantifiable responses to the noise exposure. Changes in natural behaviour following anthropogenic disturbances may reduce feeding opportunities, and evaluating potential population-level consequences should be a priority research area.
Article
Full-text available
Noise produced from a variety of human activities can affect the physiology and behaviour of individual animals, but whether noise disrupts the social behaviour of animals is largely unknown. Animal groups such as flocks of birds or shoals of fish use simple interaction rules to coordinate their movements with near neighbours. In turn, this coordination allows individuals to gain the benefits of group living such as reduced predation risk and social information exchange. Noise could change how individuals interact in groups if noise is perceived as a threat, or if it masked, distracted or stressed individuals, and this could have impacts on the benefits of grouping. Here, we recorded trajectories of individual juvenile seabass (Dicentrarchus labrax) in groups under controlled laboratory conditions. Groups were exposed to playbacks of either ambient background sound recorded in their natural habitat, or playbacks of pile-driving, commonly used in marine construction. The pile-driving playback affected the structure and dynamics of the fish shoals significantly more than the ambient-sound playback. Compared to the ambient-sound playback, groups experiencing the pile-driving playback became less cohesive, less directionally ordered, and were less correlated in speed and directional changes. In effect, the additional-noise treatment disrupted the abilities of individuals to coordinate their movements with one another. Our work highlights the potential for noise pollution from pile-driving to disrupt the collective dynamics of fish shoals, which could have implications for the functional benefits of a group's collective behaviour.
Article
Full-text available
In recent years, several sound and movement recording tags have been developed to sample the acoustic field experienced by cetaceans and their reactions to it. However, little is known about how tag placement and an animal's orientation in the sound field affect the reliability of on-animal recordings as proxies for actual exposure. Here, we quantify sound exposure levels recorded with a DTAG-3 tag on a captive harbor porpoise exposed to vessel noise in a controlled acoustic environment. Results show that flow noise is limiting onboard noise recordings, whereas no evidence of body shading has been found for frequencies of 2-20 kHz.
Article
Full-text available
With the growing utilization and exploration of the ocean, anthropogenic noise increases significantly and gives rise to a new kind of pollution: noise pollution. In this review, the source and the characteristics of noise in the sea, the significance of sound to marine organisms, and the impacts of noise on marine organisms are summarized. In general, the studies about the impact of noise on marine organisms are mainly on adult fish and mammals, which account for more than 50% and 20% of all the cases reported. Studies showed that anthropogenic noise can cause auditory masking, leading to cochlear damage, changes in individual and social behavior, altered metabolisms, hampered population recruitment, and can subsequently affect the health and service functions of marine ecosystems. However, since different sampling methodologies and unstandarized measurements were used and the effects of noise on marine organisms are dependent on the characteristics of the species and noise investigated, it is difficult to compare the reported results. Moreover, the scarcity of studies carried out with other species and with larval or juvenile individuals severely constrains the present understanding of noise pollution. In addition, further studies are needed to reveal in detail the causes for the detected impacts.
Article
In this work, acoustic and computer vision techniques are combined to develop an automatic procedure for biomass estimation of tuna during transfers. A side scan sonar working at 200 kHz and a stereo camera, positioned facing towards the surface to record the ventral aspect of fish, are set as acquisition equipment. Moreover, a floating structure has been devised to place the sensors between cages in transfers, creating a transfer canal that allows data acquisition while fish swim from donor to receiving cage. Biomass assessment is computed by counting transferred tuna and sizing a representative sample of the stock. The number of transferred tuna is automatically deduced from acoustic echograms by means of image processing techniques, whereas tuna size is computed from the stereo videos using our automatic computer vision procedure based on a deformable model of the fish ventral silhouette. The results show that the system achieves automatic tuna counting with error below 10%, achieving around 1% error in the best configuration, and automatic tuna sizing of more than 20% of the stock, with highly accurate Snout Fork Length estimation when compared to true data from harvests. These results fulfil the requirements imposed by International Commission for the Conservation of Atlantic Tunas for compliant transfer operations.