ArticlePDF Available

One, You, She: Object relations as grounding metaphors for learning mathematics

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

"Where Mathematics Comes From" (Lakoff & Núñez 2000) proposed that mathematical concepts such as arithmetic and counting are constructed cognitively from embodied metaphors of actions on physical objects, and four actions, or 'grounding metaphors' in particular: collecting, stepping, constructing and measuring. This article argues that these actions are accessible too late in children's development to be true grounding metaphors. I explore the possibility of earlier grounding metaphors for learning mathematics based on object relations psychology and the dyadic and triadic relationships with human love objects which babies develop soon after birth: the metaphors of Becoming Two and Becoming Three.
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
This is a post-print version of an article published in For the Learning of Mathematics https://flm-
journal.org. Copyright is held by FLM.
Rumbelow, M. (2021). One, You, She: Object Relations as Grounding Metaphors for Learning Mathematics. For the Learning
of Mathematics, 41(2), 45-51.
One, You, She: Object relations as grounding
metaphors for learning mathematics
Michael Rumbelow
A concept of some kind of ‘inner representation’ of an ‘object’ is required, since there is overwhelming
empirical evidence, in regard to both human and nonhuman species, that attachments or ties may persist
throughout long periods during which the object is absent from perception and can provide no
‘reinforcement’. (Ainsworth, 1969, p. 9)
Many psychologists have traced adult behaviour to bodily experiences in early childhood, from Freud’s oral
fixation and Klein’s splitting of the suckling breast, to Winnicott’s holding and gaze-mirroring, Bion’s containment,
and Ainsworth and Bowlby’s proximity-seeking to attachment figures, among others. In their book ‘Where
Mathematics Comes From’ (WMCF), George Lakoff and Rafael E. Núñez (2000) similarly trace adults’ concepts of
mathematics back to the actions of our bodies on physical objects as children, and to four actions or ‘grounding
metaphors’ in particular: collecting objects into groups, constructing with objects, moving along a path, and using
a measuring stick. They suggest that mathematics, rather than existing on some abstract Platonic plane, is instead
grounded in the brain’s capacity to use embodied actions on physical objects as metaphors to make sense of the
world. WMCF proposes that there are neural mechanisms linking specific sensory-motor experiences cognitively
to mathematical concepts and maps out a network of these links. This article attempts to connect WMCF’s
cognitive network based on sensory-motor experiences of physical objects, with the affective psychology of
experiences of human object relations.
2
Figure 1. Correlates with Lakoff and Núñez’s four grounding metaphors in representations adapted from Maths No
Problem! Textbook 1A (Yeap et al., 2014) aimed at 5-year-olds in England.
Though Lakoff and Núñez characterise WMCF primarily as cognitive neuroscience, they claim “it also can have
an important application in the teaching of mathematics” (p. 7). And for educators of schoolchildren alienated by
the abstraction of mathematics, the idea that the brain builds up abstract concepts from bodily actions on objects,
which almost every child can enact, is alluring. Especially when these actions align with usage of mathematical
manipulatives already established in classroomssuch as collecting counters, constructing with blocks, stepping
along number lines, and measuring with rods (Figure 1). Publication of WMCF seemed to support proponents of
hands-on manipulatives as engaging and motivating to children, as well as a ‘social turn’ in education in the 1990s,
which saw objectmanipulation as a facilitator of dialogue.
Curiously, however, Lakoff and Núñez distanced themselves from key schools of thought in mathematics
education that embraced embodiment as catalytic to both emotional engagement with, and social construction
of, mathematics. In WMCF they emphasise that their approach is not consistent with existing philosophies of
mathematics, “Nor is it consistent with recent post-modernist accounts of mathematics as a purely social
construction” (p. 9). And although Lakoff had previously explored embodied metaphors for emotions, family,
and love extensively with Mark Johnson in ‘Metaphors We Live By’ (MWLB)—for example, “we have the
primary conceptual metaphor Affection Is Warmth because our earliest experiences with affection correspond
to the physical experience of the warmth of being held closely” (Lakoff & Johnson, 2008, p. 256)—links with
emotional metaphors or the psychology of affect are oddly absent from WMCF, as if embodied metaphors for
emotions are somehow segregated cognitively from those for mathematics.
Personally, I am puzzled by WMCF’s lack of affective and social metaphors. As a thought experiment, in this
article I aim to connect Lakoff and Núñez’s four grounding metaphors (‘4Gs’), which they map to learning
arithmetic, to children’s developmental psychology, and its world of affection and family relationships.
3
Firstly, I suggest that the 4Gs may be based on embodied actionsaccurately counting grouped objects, or steps,
or placements of measuring sticks etc.that require fine-motor and linguistic skills typically accessible by children
too late in their development to be true grounding metaphors for learning mathematics. This opens a role for pre-
4G embodied grounding metaphors, to map from the domain of babieswho can apparently innately subitise or
sense at-aglance numbers of objects up to three or fourto the 4G domain of children who can coordinate precise
arrangement of objects with verbal counting.
Secondly, to fill this role, I highlight the theory of object relations from child developmental psychology as a
plausible source of embodied grounding metaphors for dyadic, child-caregiver ‘two-ness’, and triadic, child-
caregiver-other ‘three-ness’ among babies and toddlers. For conciseness, here I use the term caregiver to include
any principal attachment figure who suckles and weans a baby, whilst acknowledging that in many cultures breast-
feeding (and bottle-feeding) is a shared activity, and among attachment theorists there is debate as to whether
babies always prioritise a single attachment figure. I also suggest that these metaphors of dyadic and triadic human
relations may help structure early concepts of first-, second- and third-person pronouns, which support Lakoff and
Núñez’s “fundamental metonymy of algebra” (p. 74).
Thirdly, I briefly highlight the affordances of these objectrelations based metaphors as potential grounding
metaphors for developing a pre-counting, ‘subitisable’ mathematics curriculum to underpin the 4Gs.
Here I wish to emphasise that this article is not meant as a critique of WMCF’s (or MWLB’s) theory of
metaphorical mapping itself, which I find compelling, but rather a tentative attempt to connect it plausibly with
the affective developmental psychology of babies.
Where do grounding metaphors come from?
Lakoff and Núñez are candid about challenges they faced in applying methodologies of cognitive linguistics, used
in MWLB, to mathematics: “To those unfamiliar with the methodology of cognitive linguistics, it will not be obvious
how we arrived at the metaphorical mappings […]. In cognitive linguistics, the main technique is building models
that generalise over the data” (p. 100). Working across multiple disciplines including neuroscience, cognitive
psychology, and developmental psychology, “This is not an easy job […]. We must propose plausible ultimate
embodied groundings for mathematics together with plausible metaphorical mappings” (p. 101).
The process was iterative and collaborative:
In studying arithmetic, for example, we depended on the prior research of Ming Ming Chiu (1996). Chiu’s
dissertation set out some first approximations that met a number of constraints. Starting there, we made
many successive revisions until the constraints were met. (p. 101)
4
Lakoff’s former student Chiu’s dissertation, based on metaphors from MWLB, does include some ‘social’
metaphors for mathematics, for example “Arithmetic is a Social Transaction” (Chiu, 2001, p. 118). However, these
apparently did not meet Lakoff and Núñez’s constraints for grounding metaphors for mathematics.
WMCF evidently represents an enormously complex, interdisciplinary synthesis of research. However, in their
linguistics-based methodology, the plausibility of the grounding metaphors depends critically on the data they
have chosen to generalise across. Thus, if new data are included that affect the plausibility of the mappings, the
4Gs might have to be revised to accommodate this.
The two key pieces of data I wish to introduce are the complexity of counting from educational research, and
the theory of object relations from developmental psychology.
The complexity of counting
Lakoff and Núñez are clear in WMCF that counting is a cognitively complex task, listing seven distinct cognitive
capacities required just to extend subitising to finger counting (p. 51). To map counting further, a grounding
metaphor is required.
Introducing the first grounding metaphorArithmetic Is Object Collection—they say: “No metaphor is more
basic to the extension of our concept of number from the innate cardinal numbers to the natural numbers” (p.
54), implying that this metaphor is necessary to conceptualise numbers beyond the innate, subitisable three or
four. However, from a pedagogical perspective, WMCF does not detail exactly how object collection maps to
counting, until a leap to “every natural number can be conceptualized as a polynomial—that is, a sum of integers
represented by simple numerals times powers of some integer B” (p. 82). There appears to be a paradox here: to
understand counting above four we need to collect objects, but in order to know how many objects we have
collected we need to be able to count above four.
In mathematics education, this assumption that counting can and should be mastered early, before arithmetic,
though common, sits awkwardly with research into children’s development. For example, acoustic counting
saying ‘one, two, three’ etc. in orderdoes not always coordinate with pointing at objects (van den Brink, 1984).
Patterns of sounds in acoustic counting are often inconsistent with the polynomial systemfor example, in English
‘three’, ‘thirteen’, ‘twenty-three’ for 3, 13, 23requiring exceptions to be memorised. In counting with fingers
there is no consensus on which finger is ‘one’, and linguistically some doubt as to whether a thumb is a finger at
all. And to read or write numbers beyond 9, we encounter concepts such as zero, powers and place value that
underpin the counting system and are themselves complex (Coles & Sinclair, 2017). As the mathematics educator
Caleb Gattegno put it in his farewell speech:
5
We have, for centuries, taught people, by offering counting as the basis of elementary arithmetic. It’s wrong!
Shall I say it louder? It’s wrong. Not because I say so, but because counting is a complex activity. It’s a complex
activity asking of children more than is required in order to give them a better foundation. (1989, p. 24)
Below I propose that an alternative foundation, and a possible way of mapping counting more closely to the 4Gs,
would be to introduce key mathematical concepts needed for countingsuch as products, powers and positional
value before counting, using only the ‘innate’ numbers up to 3. These numbers of objects are subitisable, with
little effort, by young babies, freeing cognitive resources, and are also, psychologically, closest to our earliest
concepts of objects, according to object relations theory.
Object relations psychology
Lakoff and Núñez’s grounding metaphors involve the body acting on physical objects, or “things in the world”
(p. 97). For so-called ‘object relations’ psychologists, the term ‘object’ has a psychological meaning. Originally
deriving from Freud’s concept of libidinal drives—sexual desire and aggression—to describe the target or ‘object’
of the drive, the term was adapted by child psychologists Klein, Winnicott, Bion and others to describe the objects
of a child’s love, for example, their mother or principal caregivers, and images of relationships with these objects
which are internalised, or ‘introjected’ in the child’s unconscious. The term object has various interpretations in
different schools of thought in psychoanalysis; however, here I wish to highlight the way Klein uses it to connect
a baby’s first loving relationships with caregivers, with the manipulation of physical objects such as toy bricks, via
the process known as projective identification.
For Klein, at first babies conceive of objects such as a breast-feeding caregiver as embodied ‘part-objects’, for
instance, as a breast. When the hungry baby finds the breast available, the baby is content, and internalises the
image of the suckling breast as a ‘good object’, meeting their needs. However, human babies are highly dependent
on their caregivers for many months and vulnerable in their absence. When the breast is unavailable, the hungry
and helpless baby’s instinctive fear of death causes them anxiety. Struggling to cope emotionally, as a defence the
baby’s psyche may split off their anxiety and project it onto the image of the unavailable breast, which now
becomes internalised as a bad object’, towards which the baby is aggressive, to the point of even biting or
screaming at the caregiver when they return. The baby experiences a split in their personality, a contented, breast-
feeding ‘good self’ and a hungry, anxious and aggressive ‘bad self’ (Spillius et al., 2011).
Over time, with the repeated return of the good breast, the baby begins to realise that these two split-off objects,
the available and unavailable breast, are in reality one, which is a crucial stage in their development. The baby
feels remorse for their aggression against the bad breast and begins to integrate the good and bad objects into
one person, the caregiver. At the same time the baby realises that the two split selves they experienced, the good
and the bad self, also form one personality, the baby’s own. The baby begins to understand themselves and their
caregiver as two separate objects, in a loving relationship.
6
Also, to psychologically survive periods of no good breast, babies may create a mental image of the good breast
as an emotional comfort. This introjected image can be understood as an early ‘metaphor’ or symbol of the union
with the caregiver. And according to Winnicott (1990) a baby may also project this mental image of the loving
relationship onto relationships with external objects, or ‘transitional objects’, such as sucking a thumb, or holding
a piece of blanket or soft toy, which can similarly be understood as comforting embodied metaphors for the
yearned-for union with the absent love object (Coles, 2014).
Soon the infant can project their emotional relationships onto various physical objects in play, which Klein made
use of as psychoanalytical tools:
the brick, the little figure, the car, not only represent things which interest the child in themselves, but in his
play with them they always have a variety of symbolical meanings as well […] Play analysis had shown that
symbolism enabled the child to transfer not only interests, but also phantasies, anxieties and guilt to objects
other than people. Thus a great deal of relief is experienced in play and this is one of the factors which make
it so essential for the child. (Klein & Mitchell, 1986, p. 51)
One implication of object relations theory is that the frustrating loss of a love object—the literal ‘abs-traction’
or pulling away of the breast for examplecan lead to the creation of a mental symbol for it. This can then be
projected onto other people and also onto physical objects, charging them with meaning, and enabling the parting
and reunion to be re-enacted, reflected on, and to an extent controlled. As Kleinian psychologist Hanna Segal puts
it:
The symbols, created internally, can then be re-projected into the external world, endowing it with symbolic
meaning.
The capacity to experience loss and the wish to re-create the object within oneself gives the individual the
unconscious freedom in the use of the symbols. (1988, p. 167)
The grounding metaphor of Becoming Two: splitting and reuniting with the love object
There is, obviously, much more to object relations than is touched on above, however in order to map
metaphorically to mathematical concepts, I wish to highlight the baby’s symbolisation of their original
identification with their caregiver’s body, their ‘splitting’ of both the object and themselves into two parts to cope
emotionally with their separation, and their eventual recognition of the union of these split parts, as a new ‘two-
ness’, a loving, you-I relationship between two embodied individuals.
For object relations psychologists such as Winnicott, the confidence of the child in their own identity, separate
from the caregiver, but secure and confident in their love, is crucial to the idea of the transferable ‘unit’ self, with
7
which other ‘Two-One’, you-I loving relationships are formed. This also carries as a metaphor into the unit, or
number one, in mathematics (Winnicott, 1990, p. 61). The secure twoness of the relationship with the caregiver
becomes a base from which to explore, and a safe haven to return to when anxious.
As developmentally this predates conceptual schemas of numbers, such as number lines, whether splitting
represents a halving or a doubling is moot. Just as cell division and cell multiplication can be understood as the
same thing, the Becoming Two metaphor of splitting and reuniting can be mapped to doubling and halving and
‘whole-ing’, ‘Two— One’, and ‘One—Half’, and any power of ½. It is the metaphor of being whole, split into two
parts, and reunited in a symbolic relationship or ‘two-ness’ that is key.
Figure 2. Becoming Two metaphor for the child-caregiver union.
To illustrate this operation physically I have chosen Cuisenaire rods that are neutral in terms of numberingfor
instance, the red rod is twice the length of the white rod, and they can be thought of as TwoOne, OneHalf etc.
So in Figure 2, pedagogically, a red and two white blocks can represent the multiplicative relationships 2 = 1 × 2,
1 = ½ × 2, and in terms of powers, 20= 21 × 21, and symbolically, r = 2w. The rods are simple to map to the physical
objects of the 4Gs, and also have established use in modelling both language and, pertinently, family relationships
(Paipa, 2010).
Psychologically, in Figure 2 the red rod, centre, is a metaphor for the union of child and caregiver. The righthand
image is a metaphor for the gradually self-sustaining separation into two people in a relationship, facilitated by
the Winnicottian ‘good enough’ caregiver, who gives the child enough love and freedom for them each to develop
as individuals, secure in being loved. On the left, the empty outline above the separated block is a metaphor for
the loss and anxiety felt by a baby who lacks love and nourishment, and may ‘split’ and project a ‘bad object’ to
cope.
As a baby is weaned and gains mobility and starts to explore their environment, though physically separated,
the crawling or toddling baby is still emotionally attached and tends to seek gaze-contact and proximity to their
caregiver, especially when experiencing anxiety, as Ainsworth demonstrated in the much-replicated ‘Strange
8
Situation’ procedure, in which babies are observed being temporarily left by their caregivers in a room with a
stranger (Gillath, Karantzas & Fraley, 2016).
As an extension of the splitting and reuniting metaphor, it seems biologically plausible that, once a baby can
crawl away from their caregiver, they maintain awareness of their distance and directiona line of sightso they
can seek proximity to them. The Becoming Two metaphor can thus also map to simple geometric concepts such
as the straight line connecting two points, directional angle and the radius of a circle (Figure 3). As Bion puts it:
Mathematical elements, namely straight lines, points, circles, and something corresponding to what later
become known by the name of numbers, derive from realizations of two-ness as in breast and infant, two
eyes, two feet, and so on. (2013, p. 180)
Figure 3. Maintaining dyadic and triadic relationships at-adistance through mutual gaze-lines.
The grounding metaphor of Becoming Three: triangulating an Other
There is also in object relations theory the concept of the third ‘other’ object introduced into the dyadic child-
caregiver relationship, for example another caregiver or sibling, which can be related to Becoming Three, or the
third-person relationship, with the addition of the she/he/it object to a dyadic relationship.
The concept of number is acquired early in childhood at the time when the infant is becoming aware of its
relations with significant others […] certain numbers, especially 2 and 3, can be carriers of infantile phantasies.
The number 2 can represent the relation with the other: the child with the mother, the father or another
principal carer. Two is also the parental couple or the rivalry with a sibling. The number 3 can represent the
triangulation of mother, father and child, and also the loss of that triangulation through death or divorce or
the arrival of a sibling. (Brown, 2008, p. 30)
A baby with a ‘good enough’ caregiver, who is handled and held with love, reflected in the gaze of the love
object, will, according to Winnicott, have the confidence to start increasingly paying attention to other objects,
9
both human and physical. Similarly, the baby will be able, eventually, to tolerate the love object giving attention
periodically to a third ‘other’, and thus observes two others in a dyadic relationship which the baby may recognise
as reflecting their own relationships:
To the degree that we ever manage to grasp two-way directionality (that in a relationship, I impact on you as
much as you impact on me), we do so only from the place of the third, a vantage point outside the two.
(Benjamin, 2004, p. 7)
To become a secure triangular relationship, the three people’s gazes and attentions shift sufficiently frequently
for all three not just to feel loved, but to observe the loving dyadic relationship between the other two. In Figure
4, the white cube represents the observer. If the baby is seen as bottom left of each triad, and the caregiver as
bottom right, then in the left-hand triad the baby is relating to the caregiver, with the third other observing. In the
centre triad the baby is relating to the other. In the right-hand triad the baby is observing the caregiver relating to
the other.
Both psychologically and mathematically, this metaphor of triangulation is considerably more complex and
dynamic than the simpler splitting and integrating metaphor of the dyadic relationship, as it contains three
destabilised dyadic relationships within it. Just as a transitional object, such as a dummy or thumb, may help a
baby cope with their caregiver’s absence, play with transitional objects such as a toy or ball can help babies and
caregivers ‘practice’ integration of a third ‘other’, transferring attention to and from the third object and each
other.
When the third object is human, with its own gaze and emotional life, the processing of three dyadic object
relations can stretch the cognitive capacity of a baby, as well as the baby’s emotional tolerance. However, once
dynamic triadic relationships can be integrated, as a ‘three-ness’, Becoming Three provides an embodied
metaphor for extending the ‘Two-One’, not just to ‘Three’ but also beyond. By switching the third object we can
relate to further objects without losing the emotional security of the original ‘TwoOne’ relation. Over time we feel
safe enough in our you-I relationship with this third person that they can substitute the presence of our original
attachment figure, so by substitution we can relate triadically to a fourth ‘other’ and so on. This mature capacity
to relate to others independently of the primary caregiver is critical from a psychological and socio-cultural
perspective when forging sustainable relationships with new people and communities.
Just as in Becoming Two, it seems biologically plausible that a baby would also have awareness not just of the
proximity of the principal caregiver, but of any other person or animal approaching that might trigger a retreat to
the caregiver. Thus, the Becoming Three metaphor for mentally modelling the triadic relationships between three
bodies can be extended to geometric triangulation of three points, their distances and angles, and early
trigonometry and spatial reasoning. Once language starts to develop these triadic relationships may also offer a
proto-algebraic conceptual structure for first, second and third person, ‘I’, ‘you’, and ‘she/he’ pronouns.
10
The metonymic algebra of I-You-She
As well as a child’s formation of a dyadic ‘Two-One’ relation with their caregiver, and the dynamic projection of
this onto a third ‘other’ to make a triangular relationship maintained through switching of gaze and attention, as
language develops there is also the concept in object relations of mentally assuming different roles or perspectives
within relationships symbolically through playing with linguistic metonymy.
From the earliest experiences of holding a new-born baby face-to-face, there is a reciprocal mirroring of
expressions and gestures, analogous to the recursive doubling effect of two mirrors facing each other, or the
reflection of one’s own image in another’s eyes. I reflect you, reflecting me, reflecting you etc. Through mirroring
the two becomes one, and the one becomes two.
As they grow older and learn to talk, children often play at being adults, re-enacting adult-child conversations
but switching the roles of ‘I’ and ‘you’, consciously inverting the structure. As Klein describes:
At times he plays games in which he allots roles to the analyst and himself such as playing shop, doctor and
patient, school, mother and child. In such games the child frequently takes the role of the adult, thereby not
only expressing his wish to reverse the roles but also demonstrating how he feels that his parents should treat
him. (Klein & Mitchell, 1986, p. 41)
Later on, children can explore more sophisticated, threeway role-switching in family relationships and beyond
in adult social life, as Winnicott observed:
A child moves over to a relationship with the father, and in doing so develops an attitude to the mother which
belongs to relating to the father […] This sort of thing goes on as a to-and-fro experience in the daily life of
the child in the home. Of course, it need not be the fathermother relationship; it can be an experience of
going from the mother to the nurse and back again, or it may be an aunt or a grandmother or a big sister.
Gradually in the family all these possibilities can be met with, and experienced, and a child can come to terms
with the fears associated with them. Moreover, the child can come to enjoy the excitements that belong to
all these conflicts, provided they can be contained. (1990, p. 138)
This experience of dynamically switching roles within triadic family relationships also involves linguistic switching
of the objects referred to by ‘I’, ‘you’ and ‘she/he’ in conversation. In one-to-one conversation, the object referred
to as ‘you’ becomes ‘I’ in the mouth of the other,
11
Figure 4. The Becoming Three triangulation metaphor for the third-person ‘other’ she/he/it introduced to a
dyadic relationship.
and vice versa, as the speakers take turns to talk. In private dialogue with her mother a daughter may refer to her
mother asyou’ and to the grandmother, as ‘she’. In a separate conversation with the grandmother, these are
switched, and the grandmother is ‘you’ and the mother is ‘she’. And in overheard conversation between the
grandmother and the mother, the child may be ‘she’. As children transfer these patterns of dialogue to others, the
objects referred to in the first-, second- and third-person are also re-arranged, to maintain the triadic relationships,
not just face-to-face by switching gaze and attention, but also symbolically, by mentally rearranging the objects
represented by the pronouns, in a similar way that equations can be rearranged to put x, y or z on the left-hand
side.
Figure 5. WMCF’s representations of a commutative algebraic group with three elements (left, p. 115) compared
with the Becoming Three triangulation metaphor.
12
For Lakoff and Núñez, this kind of linguistic metonymy, ‘The Fundamental Metonymy of Algebra’, underpins
mathematics:
This everyday conceptual metonymy, which exists outside mathematics, plays a major role in mathematical
thinking: It allows us to go from concrete (case by case) arithmetic to general algebraic thinking. (p. 74)
As WMCF illustrates, the conceptualisation of algebraic structures such as simple groups does not require
counting beyond three (Figure 5). So, it is plausible that algebraic concepts may be accessible to young children
before counting. Mary Boole, editor of her husband George Boole’s ‘Laws of Thought’ (to which WMCF devotes a
chapter), recommends that babies start exploring algebrain the sense of manipulating objects which can stand
for more than one thingeven before talking, for example by repeatedly touching a shiny teapot, which can be
hot or cold:
Everybody ought to be able to make Algebras; and the sooner we begin the better. It is best to begin before
we can talk; because until we can talk, no one can get us into illogical habits; and it is advisable that good
logic should get the start of bad. (Boole & Tahta, 1972, p. 57)
Towards a subitisable mathematics curriculum to underpin the 4Gs
In this article I have proposed, drawing on object relations psychology, that our earliest mathematical objects
may be our closest caregivers, rather than inanimate objects, and that our relationships with them are affective,
reciprocal and dynamic. These are objects that can hold us as much as we can hold them, that can gather us up or
put us down, who move and talk. And, unlike pebbles or wooden blocks, they can appear impermanent, and such
is our emotional dependence on them that when they are gone, we create internal images of a permanent union
with them to comfort ourselvesour first symbolic relationshipswhich we may embody by projection onto
physical transitional objects.
In England, as in many school curricula, the rush to symbolic counting to twenty and higher numbers starts early,
coinciding with starting primary school, and being left alone by our caregiver not just briefly with one stranger
as in Ainsworth’s Strange Situationbut all day, with dozens of strangers. If psychological and mathematical
object relations are linked, this is bound to be overwhelming both emotionally and cognitively for many children.
And, psychologically they may defensively disassociate their anxiety, split it off and project it onto mathematical
objects and symbols.
From a curricular perspective, one possible avenue for future research would be to investigate postponing
counting, to spend more time attending to the relationships between just two or three subitisable objects: halving
and wholing, doubling and double doubling, switching positions, stepping from foot to foot, joining points,
triangulating and squaringthe common dyadic and triadic rhythms and symmetries of bodies and dances, songs
and conversations, and the angles of turning our heads and arms from one object to another and back again. By
prioritising secure internal models of dyadic and triadic relationships with both people and physical objects, I
13
suggest the grounding metaphors of Becoming Two and Becoming Three may emerge as cognitively and
emotionally secure bases for concepts of spatial reasoning, products and powers, that would support place-value
counting, as well as the 4Gs and the metonymy of algebra.
On the cover of my copy of WMCF is a promotional quote: “Adds body heat to the cold and beautiful abstractions
of mathematics”. Object relations psychology may offer a way of adding the emotional warmth of loving
relationships to this body heat.
Acknowledgment
I thank Alf Coles for all his encouragement and insightful reviews.
References
Ainsworth, M.D.S. (1969) Object relations, dependency, and attachment: a theoretical review of the infant-mother
relationship. Child Development 40(4), 9691025.
Benjamin, J. (2004) Beyond doer and done to: recognition and the intersubjective third. Psychoanalytic Quarterly
73, 546.
Bion, W.R. (2013) Second Thoughts: Selected Papers on Psycho-Analysis. Oxford: Butterworth-Heinemann.
Boole, M., Tahta, D.G. (1972) A Boolean Anthology. Selected Writings of Mary Boole on Mathematical Education.
Derby, UK: Association of Teachers of Mathematics.
Brown, T. (Ed.) (2008) The Psychology of Mathematics Education: A Psychoanalytic Displacement. Rotterdam:
Sense Publishers.
Chiu, M.M. (2001) Using metaphors to understand and solve arithmetic problems: novices and experts working
with negative numbers. Mathematical Thinking and Learning 3(23), 93124.
Coles, A. (2014) Transitional devices. For the Learning of Mathematics 34(2), 2430.
Coles, A. & Sinclair, N. (2017) Re-thinking place value: from metaphor to metonym. For the Learning of
Mathematics 37(1), 38.
Gattegno, C. (1989) Reflections on 21 years of ATM. In Brown, L., Hewitt,
D. & Tahta, D. (Eds.) A Gattegno Anthology: Selected Articles by Caleb Gattegno Reprinted from Mathematics
Teaching. Derby, UK: Association of Teachers of Mathematics.
Gillath, O., Karantzas, G.C. & Fraley, R.C. (2016) Adult Attachment: A
Concise Introduction to Theory and Research. London: Academic Press. Klein, M. & Mitchell, J. (Ed.) (1986) The
Selected Melanie Klein. London:
Penguin Books.
Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M. (2008) Metaphors We Live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lakoff, G. & Núñez, R.E. (2000) Where Mathematics Comes from: How the Embodied Mind Brings Mathematics
into Being. New York: Perseus.
14
Paipa, K. (2010) Te whakapapa o te reo i roto i te whanau. MAI Review 3, 115.
Segal, H. (1988). Notes on symbol formation. In: Spillius, E.B. (Ed.) (1988) Melanie Klein Today: Developments in
Theory and Practice. (vol. 1). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Spillius, E.B., Milton, J., Garvey, P., Couve, C. & Steiner, D. (2011) The New Dictionary of Kleinian Thought. New
York: Routledge.
van den Brink, J. (1984) Acoustic counting and quantity counting. For the Learning of Mathematics 4(2), 213.
Winnicott, D. W. (1990). Home Is Where We Start from: Essays by A Psychoanalyst. London: Penguin.
Yeap, B.H., Foong P.Y., Chang S.H., Lim, L.G.P., Wong, O.H. (2014) Maths No Problem! Textbook 1A. Tunbridge
Wells, UK: Maths No Problem.
... In theory [37], mathematics states that when an object has a relationship with another object [38]. It immediately forms two sides mutually perpendicular. ...
Article
Full-text available
I appreciate the comments on earlier drafts by Andy diSessa, Sung Wook Joh, Lawrence Khoo, George Lakoff, Alan Schoenfeld, three anonymous reviewers, and Lyn English (editor).
Book
Full-text available
This book paints an alternative and contemporary portrait of psychology within mathematics education, drawing on psychoanalytic practices and theory. Mathematics education is still a fairly new social science that began as an adjunct to the practice of mathematics in schools some forty years ago, defined by a marriage with cognitive psychology. As a consequence school mathematics has often been seen as a scientific enterprise centred on the operation of individual minds confronting mathematical ideas. Meanwhile, psychoanalysis had earlier come into existence through the work of Sigmund Freud. And for much of his life Freud had similarly seen his work as scientific, a view that later fuelled mainstream practices in psychology. Yet Freud’s engagement with his patients combined with his literary capabilities produced surprising results defining humans in ways that transcended mere scientific assessment. Rather his accounts of humans weaved a rich social tapestry in which individuals were understood relationally to those who shared their world. And through re-telling the story lines of their lives individuals were able to create alternative futures. This dimension of Freud’s work provoked an alternative tradition, best exemplified in the work of Lacan, in which narrative-based understandings linking humans to the social world replaced cognitive models centred on controlling individuals through particular understandings of normality. Through its eleven chapters this book provides accounts of how children, teachers, researchers and mathematical learning can be understood differently, towards emphasising how they are each consequential to the many ways in which the world can be created and described.
Book
The now-classic Metaphors We Live By changed our understanding of metaphor and its role in language and the mind. Metaphor, the authors explain, is a fundamental mechanism of mind, one that allows us to use what we know about our physical and social experience to provide understanding of countless other subjects. Because such metaphors structure our most basic understandings of our experience, they are "metaphors we live by"--metaphors that can shape our perceptions and actions without our ever noticing them. In this updated edition of Lakoff and Johnson's influential book, the authors supply an afterword surveying how their theory of metaphor has developed within the cognitive sciences to become central to the contemporary understanding of how we think and how we express our thoughts in language.
Article
A relationship of trust between students and tutors, built through whakawhanaungatanga(relationship building), lays the foundation for the exploration of a whakapapa approach (te whakapapa o te reo i roto i te whānau) within a Te Ataarangi Māori language learning setting. The approach is built on the creative foundations engendered within the Te Ataarangi environment and has been widened to be an effective tool for exploring the status of the Māori language and a range ofsocial conditions that affect the Māori population. Examined from an insider perspective of a Māori worldview the approach has a pedigree in genealogy, language and Māori social customs and protocols. The approach was pre-tested among Māori whānau (extended family) from a range of social circumstances. Their testimonies bear witness to the effectiveness of whakapapa as a healing and celebratory approach, a tool to reconstitute whānau and a valid research methodology amongst Māori.
Article
3 theoretical approaches to the origin and development of the infant-mother relationship are reviewed: psychoanalytic theories of object relations, social learning theories of dependency (and attachment), and an ethologically oriented theory of attachment. "Object relations," "dependency," and "attachment," although overlapping, are seen to differ substantially. Among the concepts in regard to which there are significant intertheoretical differences, the following are discussed: genetic "biases," reinforcement as compared with activation and termination of behavioral systems and with feedback, strength of attachment behavior versus strength of attachment, inner representation of the object, intraorganismic and environmental conditions of behavioral activation, and the role of intraorganismic organization and structure. Finally, the relation between theory and research methods is considered.
Article
Analytic work based on the intersubjective view of two participating subjectivities requires discipline rooted in an orientation to the structural conditions of thirdness. The author proposes a theory that includes an early form of thirdness involving union experiences and accommodation, called the one in the third, as well as later moral and symbolic forms of thirdness that introduce differentiation, the third in the one. Clinically, the concept of a co-created or shared intersubjective thirdness helps to elucidate the breakdown into the twoness of complementarity in impasses and enactments and suggests how recognition is restored through surrender.