ArticlePDF Available

Pathways to Political Persuasion: Linking Online, Social Media, and Fake News With Political Attitude Change Through Political Discussion

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

There is a vast research tradition examining the antecedents that lead people to be politically persuaded. However, political opinion and attitude change in social media has received comparatively scarce attention. This study seeks to shed light on this strand of the literature by theoretically advancing and empirically testing a structural equation model linking online social media, and fake news exposure, with political discussion and political persuasion in social media. Drawing on autoregressive causal tests from two waves of US survey panel data collected in 2019 and 2020, our results indicate that online, social media fake news, and political discussion are all positive predictors of individual political attitude change. Furthermore, structural equation tests reveal that online and social media news lead individuals to be exposed to fake news, which, in turn, predict higher levels of political discussion, ultimately facilitating political persuasion in the social media realm. Limitations and further suggestions for future research are also included in the study.
Content may be subject to copyright.
https://doi.org/10.1177/00027642221118272
American Behavioral Scientist
1 –22
© 2022 SAGE Publications
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/00027642221118272
journals.sagepub.com/home/abs
Article
Pathways to Political
Persuasion: Linking Online,
Social Media, and Fake News
With Political Attitude Change
Through Political Discussion
Homero Gil de Zúñiga1,2,3 ,
Pablo González-González1,
and Manuel Goyanes1,4
Abstract
There is a vast research tradition examining the antecedents that lead people to be
politically persuaded. However, political opinion and attitude change in social media
has received comparatively scarce attention. This study seeks to shed light on this
strand of the literature by theoretically advancing and empirically testing a structural
equation model linking online social media, and fake news exposure, with political
discussion and political persuasion in social media. Drawing on autoregressive causal
tests from two waves of US survey panel data collected in 2019 and 2020, our results
indicate that online, social media fake news, and political discussion are all positive
predictors of individual political attitude change. Furthermore, structural equation
tests reveal that online and social media news lead individuals to be exposed to fake
news, which, in turn, predict higher levels of political discussion, ultimately facilitating
political persuasion in the social media realm. Limitations and further suggestions for
future research are also included in the study.
1Democracy Research Unit, Political Science Department, College of Law and Public Administration,
University of Salamanca, Salamanca, Spain
2Media Effects Research Lab, Media Studies Department, Donald P. Bellisario College of
Communications, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA
3Facultad de Comunicación y Letras, Universidad Diego Portales, Santiago, Chile
4Department of Communication, Universidad Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
Corresponding Author:
Homero Gil de Zúñiga, Department of Political Science, University of Salamanca, Campus Unamuno s/n,
Salamanca, Castilla y Leon 37007, Spain.
Pennsylvania State University, 201 Carnegie Building, University Park, PA 16802-1503, USA.
Emails: hgz@usal.es; hgzn@psu.edu
1118272ABSXXX10.1177/00027642221118272American Behavioral Scientist</italic>Gil de Zúñiga et al.
research-article2022
2 American Behavioral Scientist 00(0)
Keywords
fake news, misinformation, online news, social media news, fake news, political
discussion, political persuasion
Political persuasion permeates through all layers of political life in well-established
democracies. For instance, political elites and elected officials need to persuade their
constituents to support their policies (Mutz et al., 1996), while the media inform and
“persuade” the public opinion by offering public affair facts and promoting rational
arguments across society (Arias, 2019; Graves & Amazeen, 2019). In addition, opin-
ion leaders and lay citizens regularly persuade others about political matters (Cobb &
Kuklinski, 1997; Iyengar & Simon, 2000). In short, citizens end up being influenced
by a myriad of mechanisms, including voluntary news consumption, incidental news
exposure, and political discussion (Mutz et al., 1996; Wood, 2000). Yet, the growth of
social media ecologies and the Internet have also changed how and when these mecha-
nisms operate. People use social networking sites for a variety of purposes: from stay-
ing connected with friends, relatives, and coworkers (Kahne & Bowyer, 2018; Whiting
& Williams, 2013) to consuming entertainment, getting news, or acquiring novel polit-
ical information (Anderson & Caumont, 2014; Shearer & Mitchell, 2021).
This article structurally examines the antecedents of political persuasion in social
media. Specifically, we examine whether online news consumption, social media
news usage, and fake news exposure lead to political persuasion. First, we empirically
test the direct effects of such factors, using data from a two-wave panel survey of resi-
dents in the United States, and developing causal autoregressive models (Ardèvol-
Abreu et al., 2017; Huckfeldt et al., 2005). In addition, we analyzed direct and indirect
effects of the previous exogenous variables alongside political discussion in a struc-
tural equation model, proposing a theoretical framework that illuminates the specific
pathways toward citizens’ political persuasion in social media. Results from the study
lend support for a positive and direct association between online news consumption,
social media news use, fake news exposure, and political persuasion. Additional struc-
tural equation model results illustrate the potential mechanisms explaining people’s
political attitude change through social media fake news exposure and political discus-
sion. Overall, this study contributes to the scholarly conversation on political persua-
sion in social media, arguing that exposure to online, social media, and fake news
across different platforms and through divergent mechanisms contours people’s politi-
cal attitudes.
Online and Social Media Political Persuasion
There is ample evidence that exposure to political news makes it easier for people to
reconsider their political views (Fishkin, 1991; Mutz, 2002). However, news con-
sumption may elicit distinctive effects on persuasion hinging on individuals’ percep-
tions of issue relevance (McCombs & Shaw, 1972), and perhaps what is more important
Gil de Zúñiga et al. 3
for our study, the overall likelihood for individuals to change their judgments based on
new information acquisition (Mutz et al., 1996; Wood, 2000), and exposure to diverse
viewpoints (Feldman, 2011; Ladd & Lenz, 2008).
While most people typically rely on mass media to be informed about public affairs
and politics (Chaffee & Kanihan, 1997; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986; Gil de Zúñiga &
Chen, 2019), the growing emergence of social media has significantly changed this
landscape.
Recent data suggest that the use of social media for news is growing exponentially,
as more than half of the Americans (53%) rely on such platforms (Shearer & Mitchell,
2021).
Accordingly, it has been argued that such exposure may influence public opinion
(Asur & Huberman, 2010; Kim, Hsu, et al., 2013; Weiksner et al., 2008) and poten-
tially persuade people to change their political beliefs (Allcott & Gentzkow, 2017;
Dimitrova et al., 2014; Kim, Chen, et al., 2013; Valenzuela, 2013).
Despite the myriad of definitions (Mutz et al., 1996; Petty, 2018), political persua-
sion has been theorized as the process of attitude change germane to political matters
(Bohner et al., 2008; Diehl et al., 2016; Perloff, 2008), whereby a communicative
intention to influence the judgments or behavior of a “persuade” tend to be the norm
(Burnell & Reeve, 1984; O’keefe, 2015; Perloff, 2020; Simons, 1976). More recently,
and specifically applied to political communication, political persuasion has been
defined as the reconsideration “of one’s political attitudes based on exposure to new
information” (Gil de Zúñiga et al., 2018, p. 303). This definition best fits our analytical
strategy as it weighs the importance of news stimuli in the cognitive process of attitude
change (e.g., Feldman, 2011). In this regard, examining political persuasion is impor-
tant to better understand how political attitudes and behavior may change due to of
exposure to diverse information hubs nurtured by social media ecologies and online
public spaces.
Numerous political communication scholars have previously examined how news
consumption facilitates people’s political persuasion process, and the complementary
fashion in which political discussion attributes reinforce this attitude change (Feldman
et al., 2012; Gerber et al., 2008; Zaller, 1992). Social media facilitate the creation and
eventual consolidation of larger and more diverse, heterogeneous social networks
(Brundidge, 2010a; Diehl et al., 2016) that may share politically dissimilar news con-
tents (Bakshy et al., 2012; Lee et al., 2014; Messing & Westwood, 2014). This expo-
sure to heterogeneous news prompts users to change their attitudes (Chang et al., 2018;
Diehl et al., 2016) either due to the novel information acquisition or cognitively pro-
cessing the information they receive via news exposure and political exposure
discussion.
First, exposure to counter-attitudinal information can lead to ambivalence in one’s
political opinions and beliefs (Mutz, 2006), which may challenge prior assumptions
stored in the memory. Second, the politically grounded reasons exposed by participants
in interpersonal discussions may also challenge one’s opinion. Third, research has
noted that attitude ambivalence is contingent on the framing and context of the issues
tackled and, not surprisingly, on one’s social network diversity (Keele & Wolak, 2008).
4 American Behavioral Scientist 00(0)
Ambivalence energizes a more open view toward opinion-challenging perspectives
which, in turn, fosters attitude change (Mutz, 2006). More recent research has, indeed,
demonstrated a positive relationship between news consumption and political persua-
sion in social media. Evidence found it to be both direct (Gil de Zúñiga et al., 2018) and
indirect mechanisms, mediated by cognitive elaboration about cross-cutting views (Lee
& Choi, 2017), and through network heterogeneity and discussion disagreement (Diehl
et al., 2016). Based on the preceding theoretical discussion, we propose the following
hypotheses:
H1: Online news consumption (W1) will be positively related over time to political
persuasion on social media (W2).
H2: Social media news use (W1) will be positively related over time to political
persuasion on social media (W2).
Fake News Exposure and Political Persuasion
In the social media ecosystem, people will encounter not only verified factual informa-
tion, but also fabricated news (Allcott & Gentzkow, 2017; Lazer et al., 2018). Such
misleading content is usually theorized under the protection of fake news, but there is
no strong consensus on its structural interpretation (Conroy et al., 2015; Tandoc et al.,
2018; Wardle & Derakhshan, 2017). As pointed out in similar research, simply reduc-
ing the dimensions of false information may not be an effective or productive way to
advance research (Weeks & Gil de Zúñiga, 2021). Thus, our empirical efforts focus on
the effects of fake news exposure on political persuasion, understanding “fake news”
as fabricated, false news that could mislead readers by mimicking news media content
(Allcott & Gentzkow, 2017; Lazer et al., 2018). The literature on the potential impact
of fake news on society paints a rather gloomy and pessimistic picture (Balmas, 2014;
Rapp & Salovich, 2018). Fake news’ persuasive influence stems from its scalability, as
fabricated content spreads deeper and faster than authentic news (Vosoughi et al.,
2018). It can be said that the most vulnerable topics are related to health and medicine
(i.e., abortion, vaccination, etc.), because prior knowledge is usually required to deter-
mine the authenticity of the content (Ha et al., 2021), but it also threatens other areas,
including politics. Ultimately, it has been theorized that when fake news is assumed as
authentic, peoples’ attitudes may change, no matter the topic, and this includes, inex-
tricably, voting behavior (Chadwick et al., 2018; Luo et al., 2020). In this regard, dif-
ferent agents across political systems have explored with relative success misleading
narratives for political gains, including, for example, Donald Trump’s presidential
campaigns (Allcott & Gentzkow, 2017; Guo & Vargo, 2020).
Recent work has focused on the potential impact of fake news on unconscious pro-
cesses (Bastick, 2021). It has been argued that fake news might exert a negative influ-
ence on emotions and political attitudes, such as cynicism, political efficacy, and
polarization (Allcott et al., 2019; Azzimonti & Fernandes, 2018; Balmas, 2014),
which, in turn, may affect political behavior. Given that being exposed to new infor-
mation and factual-based news may inspire people to reconsider their political views,
Gil de Zúñiga et al. 5
we also expect that the distorted narratives of fabricated content that mimics real news
may also prompt users to be politically persuaded and, ultimately, change their atti-
tudes toward political issues. More formally:
H3: Online exposure to fake news (W1) will be positively related in time to political
persuasion on social media (W2).
Fake News and Online Discussion as Pathways to Political Persuasion
Social media provides fertile soil to foster peer-to-peer interactions (Kaplan &
Haenlein, 2010; Van Noorden, 2014). Their affordances enable users to engage in
semi-public activities, such as liking and (re)sharing (Khan, 2017). Such interactions
are deemed “semi-public” because users can either make them fully visible or invisi-
ble (Boyd & Ellison, 2007; Ellison et al., 2011). In addition, another interaction mech-
anism that is particularly relevant to our research is the possibility of commenting.
This activity carries the potential of engaging in discussions with other users, involv-
ing explicit political content (Halpern & Gibbs, 2013; Gil de Zúñiga, Ardèvol-Abreu,
& Casero-Ripollés, 2019), that may influence users’ political opinions.
However, the mainstay of the pathways to political discussions in social media is
arguably news use. When citizens consume news, they find information about public
affairs and political arguments with which they tend to agree or disagree, triggering
their willingness to discuss and express their political opinions (Gil de Zúñiga,
Ardèvol-Abreu, & Casero-Ripollés, 2019; Halpern & Gibbs, 2013). Previous work
has yielded strong, consistent evidence on this association (Brundidge, 2010b; Gil de
Zúñiga et al., 2016). Subsequently, after political discussions take place, users involved
could generally want to seek new information and follow the news on a particular
issue (Jacobs et al., 2009; Jahng, 2018), and check, verify, and contextualize what
other users also said about these issues (Lane et al., 2019; Velasquez & Rojas, 2017).
A large body of research has shown that exposure to discussion disagreement can lead
individuals to be more likely to reconsider their views and opinions (Fishkin, 1991;
Mutz & Martin, 2001).
Therefore, it stands to reason that political discussion could mediate between users’
news consumption and their likelihood of being persuaded.
Beyond fact-checked news, social media also enable the distribution of fake news,
prompting users to comment, discuss, or correct its distorted nature (Bode & Vraga,
2018; Colliander, 2019). As discussed previously, misinformation narratives in the
distribution of inaccurate or misleading news may also elicit changes in people’s polit-
ical attitudes, emotions, and behavior (Balmas, 2014; Bastick, 2021; Rapp & Salovich,
2018). However, the question of which role fake news plays in political persuasion as
a broader, more comprehensive framework remains open. Therefore, this study also
seeks to investigate how variables previously articulated in the political persuasion
literature as influential (e.g., online, social media, fake news exposure, and political
discussion) may work in a theoretically structured system. Drawing upon these theo-
retical rationales, we pose the following overarching research question:
6 American Behavioral Scientist 00(0)
RQ1: How do online news consumption (W1), social media news use (W1), fake
news exposure (W1), and online discussion (W1) relate in time to political persua-
sion (W2) in a theoretical causal structure?
Methods
Data for this study came from a three-wave panel survey that was administered in the
United States (June 2019–October 2019–February 2020). Questionnaires were
designed and distributed in Qualtrics at the Principal Investigator’s university. At the
same time, respondent recruitment was performed by IPSOS Austria, from an online
panel mirroring the US general population using stratified sampling using key demo-
graphic variables such as gender, age, and education (see Table 1). Drawing on a panel
curated by IPSOS Austria, 3,000 subjects were sampled based on quotas to represent
US society according to the US Census. In Wave 1, 1,338 cases were validated and 511
in Wave 2, with a retention and cooperation rate of 44.60% and 38.19%, respectively
(see AAPOR, 2018). Only data from the first and second waves were employed in this
study. The following operationalizations were used for the endogenous and exogenous
variables:
Social Media News Use
This construct computes respondents’ news consumption across social media (adapted
from Gil de Zúñiga et al., 2018), including “Facebook,” “Twitter,” “Snapchat,”
“LinkedIn,” “WhatsApp,” “local news on social media,” and “national news on social
media” (13-item averaged scale, 1 = never to 10 = all the time; Cronbach’s α = .91;
M = 3.20; SD = 1.98).
Online News Consumption
This variable taps into respondents’ online news consumption across different plat-
forms, including “online news sites,” “citizen journalism,” and “local news online
sites” (three-item averaged scale, 1 = never to 10 = all the time; Cronbach’s α = .68;
M = 3.80; SD = 2.30)
Fake News Exposure
This construct computes respondents’ self-evaluation of their exposure to “fake
news” (Allcott & Gentzkow, 2017; Lazer et al., 2018). It was measured by asking
respondent how often they think they see “fabricated information that mimics news
media content and could mislead readers,” “articles that originate from satirical
websites but were transformed by others and put in a misleading context,” and “sto-
ries containing deliberatively misleading elements making the reader believe it is
correct” (three-item averaged scale, 1 = never to 10 = all the time; Cronbach’s α = .92;
M = 5.99; SD = 2.32)
Gil de Zúñiga et al. 7
Social Media/Online Political Discussion
This variable measure respondents’ frequency of online and social media political dis-
cussion. Respondents indicated how often they talk “about politics or public affairs”
online, encompassing closer knit networks or strong ties, as well as discussions
Table 1. Demographic Profile of Study Survey, Other Comparable US Survey, and the US
Census.
Study survey,
June 2019 (%) GSS, 2018 (%)
U.S. Census Bureau
(estimates, 2019) (%)
Gender
Female 53.2 55.2 51.5
Male 46.8 44.8 48.5
Race/ethnicity
White 75.2 71.2 77.5
Black or African American 10.1 16.4 13.0
Hispanic or Latino 6.5 5.4 14.6
Asian 5.6 3.1 6.1
Age (years)
18–35 32.3 5.5 30.6
36–55 39.7 33.2 36.7
Older than 55 28.0 38.2 32.7
Education
High school or less 35.2 61.3 38.9
Some college 25.0 8.3 27.8
Bachelor’s degree or more 39.8 30.3 33.3
Household income
Less than $15,000 11.6 21.2 9.1
$15,000–$24,999 9.8 12.7 8.0
$25,000–$49,999 21.1 30.8 20
$50,000–$99,999 33.3 24.0 28.8
$100,000–$149,999 15.9 3.3 15.5
$150,000–$199,999 4.8 7.9 8.3
$200,000 or more 3.5 0.0 10.3
Source (third column). U.S. Census Bureau, Population Division.
Note. Percentages are calculated for population 18 years old and over. In the second column, “Race”
(block #2) was measured by respondents’ first mention, “Some college” (block #4) includes those who
reported “Junior College” as their highest degree in the GSS, and “Household income” (block #5) was
measured in inflation-adjusted constant dollars (See GSS Methodological Reports). The third column
(blocks #1–4) shows official estimates of the resident population by single year for the United States
(2019). For education attainment, Census figures also refer to population age 18 and older, and “Some
college” also includes those with an associate’s degree. Household-level income data (block #5) in the
third column is based on information from the Current Population Survey Annual Social and Economic
Supplements (CPS ASEC).
GSS = general social survey
8 American Behavioral Scientist 00(0)
maintained with looser social connections or weak ties (Gil de Zúñiga, 2017;
Valenzuela et al., 2012) (six-item averaged scale, 1 = never to 10 = all the time;
Cronbach’s α = .92‚ M = 2.92‚ SD = 2.31).
Social Media Political Persuasion
This construct taps on respondents’ likelihood of being persuaded in social media (see
Diehl et al., 2016). “I have changed my opinion based upon something I saw on social
media,” “I have changed my opinion based upon what someone influential posted on
social media,” and “I take part in changing my mind about political issues because of
information or interaction on social media” (three-item averaged scale, 1 = never to
10 = all the time; W1 Cronbach’s α = .89; M = 4.17; SD = 2.46; W2 Cronbach’s α = .91;
M = 3.75; SD = 2.39).
Controls and Analysis Strategy. As prior scholarship has suggested that the association
between explanatory and response variables may also be affected by other exogenous
antecedents (see Feldman, 2011; Gil de Zúñiga et al., 2018; Weeks et al., 2015), a set
of controls were also included: demographics (age, gender, education, income, and
race), media antecedents (social media use, television use for news, newspaper use,
radio use for news), and political antecedents (political interest, ideology, network
size, and online political discussion). For data analysis, we first run a series of panel
autoregressive causal order ordinary least squares (OLS) regressions, with social
media political persuasion in Wave 2 as the dependent variable, controlling for all
prior theoretical antecedents, as well as prior individual levels of political persuasion
on social media in Wave
1. The models include controls in five different blocks (including the autoregres-
sive term), and we ran a series of OLS regressions for each of our variables of
interest (i.e., social media use for news, online news consumption, and fake
news exposure). Then, a structural equation model test was employed to
account for the direct and indirect effects on an overarching model. Different
competing models were also theorized and compared, with the one presented
in this manuscript being the most parsimonious. Thus, the rationale behind our
analytic strategy was: (1) to test the direct effects of each exogenous variable
independently and (2) to model their direct and indirect effects structurally and
more stringently. Direct regression effects were calculated with SPSS 26,
while the structural equation model was run with MPlus 8. Zero-order correla-
tions are also shown in Table 1.
Results
Before properly testing our hypothesis, an overall set of zero-order correlations
(Table 2) show that all exogenous and endogenous variables of interest in our model
are associated with each other in a bivariate relationship. In order to test H1 through
Gil de Zúñiga et al. 9
H3 (Table 3), we first employed a series of panel autoregressive OLS regressions with
social media political persuasion as the dependent variable and our variables of inter-
est, namely, (1) social media news, (2) online news, and (3) fake news exposure as
independent variables. Regarding the effect of online news on social media political
persuasion (H1), the first autoregressive model indicates a statistically significant and
positive effect of online news consumption on social media political persuasion
(β = .133, p < .05; total R2 = 44.5%). Accordingly, those users who consume more
online news in W1 are more prone to be politically persuaded in social media in W2.
Beyond the autoregressive term (i.e., social media political persuasion in Time 1;
β = .398, p < .001; ΔR2 = 36.9%), social media use (β = .140, p < .001), and online dis-
cussion (β = .124, p < .05) were also statistically significant and positive predictors of
social media political persuasion, supporting H1.
The second autoregressive regression model examines the relationship between
social media use for news and social media political persuasion (H2). The analysis
portrayed in Table 3 shows a positive association between social media use for news
and social media political persuasion (β = .214, p < .001; total R2 = 45.2%). Accordingly,
greater use of social media for being informed in W1 is positively associated with
social media political persuasion in W2. The autoregressive term (β = .364, p < .001;
ΔR2 = 36.9%), social media use (β = .088, p < .05), online discussion (β = .098, p < .05)
were also shown to be statistically significant and positive predictors of social media
political persuasion. H2 is therefore also supported.
Finally, the third autoregressive model tests the effect of fake news exposure on
social media political persuasion (H3). The model reveals a positive and statistically
significant association between fake news exposure and social media political persua-
sion (β = .076, p < .05; total R2 = 44%). Accordingly, those who are exposed to fake
news on social media in W1 are more prone to be politically persuaded in social media
in W2. The autoregressive term (β = .419, p < .001; ΔR2 = 36.9%), social media use
(β = .145, p < .001) and online discussion (β = .134, p < .05) were shown to be signifi-
cant and positive predictors of social media political persuasion, while age (β = −.091,
p < .05) was revealed to be a significant but negative predictor. Thus, H3 was
supported.
Among the remaining controls introduced in our model, use of social media
(β = .088, p < .05, cross-sectional; β = .140, p < .001, lagged; and β = .145, p < .001,
autoregressive) and online political discussion (β = .098, p < .05, cross-sectional;
β = .124, p < .001, lagged; and β = .134, p < .001, autoregressive) are all consistent
positive predictors of political persuasion. Age is also a significant predictor, as
younger people tend to change their political opinion more frequently than their older
counterparts in time (β = −.091, p < .05, in the autoregressive model).
In order to examine the structural influence of our exogenous measurements on
the endogenous variable, the study tested different parsimonious theoretical models
that could explain the relationship between news exposure and discussion in pre-
dicting political persuasion in social media (RQ1). Prior Structural Equation
Modeling (SEM) literature suggests that an appropriate structural model fit should
get a comparative fit index (CFI > 0.90), root mean square error of approximation
10 American Behavioral Scientist 00(0)
(RMSEA < 0.08), and Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI > 0.90) (see Awang, 2014).
Accordingly, the model presented here yielded the most parsimonious fit to the
data, when compared to the baseline model, and other competing theoretical mod-
els: χ2(3) = 4.091, p = .251; CFI = 0.995; TLI = .986; RMSEA = 0.027; and
SRMR = 0.014. Figure 1 represents the standardized betas of direct effects, while
Table 4 shows the results from computing the mediation effects. As reflected, all
exogenous variables have a positive and statistically significant effect on the endog-
enous measurements, except social media political persuasion. Only social media
and online political discussion are directly related to persuasion in time. Regarding
social media news, the structural equation model reveals a positive effect on fake
news (β = .031, p < .05), social media online discussion (β = .479, p < .001), and a
positive (covariate) association with online news (r = .616, p < .001).
As for online news consumption, the structural equation model indicates a statisti-
cally significant and positive effect on fake news (β = .116, p < .05), and social media
online discussion (β = .135, p < .01). Finally, the model reveals a positive effect of fake
new exposure on social media online discussion (β = .085, p < .05), and a positive
effect of social media online discussion on social media political persuasion (β = .150,
p < .01). The exogenous variables had significant explanatory power in accounting for
Figure 1. Autoregressive SEM testing social media news, online news, fake news exposure,
and social media/online discussion effects on social media persuasion.
Note. N = 511. Maximum-likelihood estimation. Continuous path entries are standardized SEM
coefficients (StdYX standardization): *p < .05,**p < .01, ***p < .001. The model controls for all
variables included in Table 4: demographic variables (age, gender, education, income, and race),
traditional media use (TV, radio, and newspapers), political orientations (political intertest, discussion
network size, and ideology), and the autoregressive term (social media political persuasion W¹) by
residualizing all observed variables prior to model fitting. All W¹ variables were brought into the
model by mentioning their variance in the MODEL command. The model includes indirect effects on
persuasion (W2) (see TABLE2Table 2). Model bootstrapped 1,000 iterations. Goodness of fit: χ² = 4.09;
df = 3; p = .25; RMSEA = 0.027, CFI = 0.995, TLI = 0.986, SRMR = 0.022. Explained variance of criterion
variables: SM political persuasion (W2), R2 = .023; SM/online political discussion (W1), R2 = .335; fake
news (W1), R2 = .049. CFI = comparative fit index, RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation,
SEM = structural equation modelling, TLI = Tucker–Lewis Index.
Gil de Zúñiga et al. 11
the endogenous measurements (fake news R2 = 4.9%, social media online discussion
R2 = 35.5%, and social media political persuasion R2 = 2.3%)1.
When it comes to indirect effects, the structural equation model shows three posi-
tive mediating paths. The first path reveals that social media news positively affects
social media persuasion through online/social media political discussion (β = .035,
p < .05). Accordingly, those who consume more news in social media are more prone
to engage in online/social media discussion, which, in turn, influence their likelihood
of being persuaded in social media. Similar effects were found in regard to online
news consumption. According to our structural equation model, those who consume
more online news are more likely to participate in online/social media discussions,
which positively affects their likelihood of being persuaded in social media (β = .009,
p < .05). Finally, a more nuanced picture is depicted when considering the mediating
effect of online/social media discussion over the relationship between fake news and
political persuasion (β = .013, p = .06).
Discussion
There is abundant evidence in the literature suggesting that people who more fre-
quently use social media amplify their social ties within these platforms and also tend
to rely on social media to consume information about current news and public affairs
(Gottfried & Shearer, 2016; Kahne & Bowyer, 2018; Shearer & Mitchell, 2021). In
this process of news consumption and political discussion, users’ political attitudes are
susceptible to change (Cobb & Kuklinski, 1997; Iyengar & Simon, 2000). Drawing on
a two-wave panel survey of US residents, this study contributes to better understand
how political persuasion in social media unfolds. First, our study provides illustrative
causal empirical evidence suggesting that higher levels of online news consumption,
social media news use, and fake news exposure directly lead to social media political
persuasion. In addition, the study proposes a comprehensive social media political
Table 2. Zero-Order Correlations.
Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. TV news use
2. Newspaper .572***
3. Radio news .426*** .420***
4. Online discussion .345*** .363*** .424***
5. Online news .495*** .514*** .444*** .467***
6. Social media news .421*** .392*** .352*** .621*** .647***
7. Fake news exposure .180*** .107*** .229*** .221*** .212*** .218***
8. Social media political
persuasion
.251*** .225** .204*** .495*** .436*** .640*** .149***
Note. Cell entries are Pearson’s correlation coefficients (W1 = 1,338).
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
12 American Behavioral Scientist 00(0)
Table 3. Autoregressive Models Predicting Political Persuasion.
Social media
political
persuasion W2
Social media
political
persuasion W2
Social media
political
persuasion W2
Block 1: Autoregressive term
Social media political
persuasion W1
0.354***
R236.9%
Social media political
persuasion W1
398***
R236.9%
Social media political
persuasion W1
0.419***
R2 36.9%
Block 2: Demographics
Age −0.055 −0.071 −0.091*
Gender (female) −0.031 −0.029 0.022
Education 0.037 0.034 0.038
Income −0.030 −0.017 −0.026
Race (white) −0.033 −0.036 −0.033
R21.8% 1.8% 1.8%
Block 3: Media antecedents
Social media use 0.088* 0.140*** 0.145***
TV news use 0.048 0.056 0.070
Newspaper use −0.025 −0.033 0.007
Radio news use −0.001 −0.006 −0.001
R23.2% 3.2% 3.2%
Block 4: Political orientations
Political interest −0.065 −0.075 −0.076
Network size 0.026 0.021 0.014
Online discussion 0.098* 0.124* 0.134*
Ideology −0.049 −0.043 −0.061
R21.6% 1.6% 1.6%
Block 5: Variables of interest
Social media news 0.214***
Online news 0.133*
Fake news 0.076*
R21.7% 1% 0.5%
Total R245.2% 44.5% 44%
Note. Sample size = 511 (Wave 2). Cell entries are final-entry OLS standardized beta (β) coefficients.
OLS = ordinary least squares.
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
Gil de Zúñiga et al. 13
persuasion theoretical framework by shedding more light into this relationship with a
structural equation model. Overall, our findings suggest that people are politically
persuaded after consuming news and being exposed to fake news, and engaging in
online/social media discussions, which function as a mediating mechanism.
First, our analyses show that online news use is a significant factor influencing
political persuasion. When people consume online news from (professional) media
outlets, users tend to change their political positions. This finding is not entirely sur-
prising, as prior research has consistently shown that news use nurtures political per-
suasion both in offline and online environments (Feldman, 2011; Fishkin, 1991; Ladd
& Lenz, 2008; Mutz et al., 1996). The main driver here is novel information. When
individuals are exposed to factual information, they incorporate these facts into their
prior cognitive and mental map, making more informed decisions. Some of these deci-
sions lead people to change their attitude toward political matters and policies
(Johansen & Joslyn, 2008).
Second, we also provide evidence that supports a positive association between
social media news use and political persuasion. Also consistent with extant research
(Diehl et al., 2016; Gil de Zúñiga et al., 2018), our findings suggest that news con-
sumption in social media may encourage people to reconsider their political assump-
tions and, ultimately, be persuaded.
Beyond the explanatory power of the novel information individuals acquire to pro-
mote political attitude change, the persuasive process may also hinge on a myriad of
mechanisms, including news trust, peers’ news suggestions, algorithms curation pro-
cess, or/and individual filtration tactics (Fletcher & Nielsen, 2018; Goyanes et al.,
2021; Hermida et al., 2012; Messing & Westwood, 2014). All these agents may also be
responsible for activating a cognitive process that prompts users to reconsider their
political attitudes in time.
Table 4. Indirect Effects of Social Media News (W1), Online News (W1), Fake News (W1),
and SM/Online Political Discussion on Persuasion (W2).
Indirect effects
Point
estimate
95% confidence
interval
Social media news (W1) SM/online political discussion
(W2) SM political persuasion (W2)
0.072** 0.006 to 0.065
Social media news (W1) fake news (W2) SM/online
political discussion (W2) SM political persuasion (W2)
0.002#−0.001 to 0.002
Online news (W1) SM/online political discussion (W2)
SM political persuasion (W2)
0.020* 0.002 to 0.021
Online news (W1) fake news (W2) SM/online political
discussion (W2) SM political persuasion (W2)
0.001 −0.001 to 0.002
Fake news (W1) SM/online political discussion (W2)
SM political persuasion (W2)
0.013#−0.005 to 0.030
Note. Indirect effects calculated from the model shown in Figure 1. Standardized coefficients are
reported. W1 = Wave 1; W2 = Wave 2.
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 (two-tailed), #p < .10.
14 American Behavioral Scientist 00(0)
Complementarily to real news, users may also encounter “fake news” (Allcott &
Gentzkow, 2017; Lazer et al., 2018). The low access barriers to information in social
media ecologies enable the massive circulation of misleading content aiming to mod-
ify users’ ideals (Bastick, 2021; Ha et al., 2021). Interestingly, when citizens are
exposed to fake news, they also seem to be politically persuaded in time. This is an
important and novel finding.
A first feasible explanation implies that, although the information is not factual,
some people may be deceived and believe such content is authentic, triggering an
attitude change. This possibility denotes the importance of social media platforms and
policymaker’s efforts in identifying misleading and false content, making social media
users and people aware.
Alternatively, and what we consider more plausibly given our measurement of fake
news, people are able to identify a given content as fake news at the moment of being
exposed to it; however, there are certain features about that fake “information” that
also fosters political attitude change eventually. In other words, individuals may iden-
tify the information as fake at first, but over time, they cannot recall or assess the
provenance of that information, affecting their political attitudes (Jankowski, 2018).
Likewise, the direction of the attitude change is uncertain. It might well be occur-
ring as a change in one’s former attitudes, adopting a counter-prior political position.
However, we should consider the possibility of persuasion happening by embracing a
more polarized opinion consistent with their prior political stance; this would fuel a
reinforcement loop. As social media users can stumble upon fake news shared by like-
minded peers, if its content is consistent with one’s opinions and (political) leanings,
it could potentially reinforce users’ distorted views (Faragó et al., 2020; Moravec
et al., 2018), as trust in the news also matters (Gil de Zúñiga, Ardèvol-Abreu, Diehl,
et al., 2019). In this vein, another potential rationale may stem from users’ inclinations
to believe in any news piece as long as it comes from sources they trust. Put differ-
ently, users may assume as real the information shared by those within their social
network, such as friends, coworkers, and relatives, or other actors such as political
leaders and parties (Anspach, 2017; Turcotte et al., 2015). Many of these are important
considerations for future research, and forthcoming scholarship should pay more
attention to these processes.
Lastly, we aimed to draw a theoretical casual order model to explain how online
news consumption, social media news use, and fake news exposure would relate to
online/social media discussion, and ultimately, to political persuasion. The analysis
shows several insightful pathways. First, online news consumption and social media
news use positively predict discussion in social media/online environments. As peo-
ple get relevant insights from the news, discussions organically emerge. Second,
online news consumption and social media news use also positively predict fake
news exposure. Accordingly, the more the users consume news online and in social
media, the higher their chances of being exposed to misleading content. Third, fake
news exposure also leads to discussions in social media/online environments.
Analogously to real news, fake news stimulates and sparks peer-to-peer discussions,
whether to support or correct the assumptions held in the (fake) contents (Bode &
Gil de Zúñiga et al. 15
Vraga, 2018; Colliander, 2019). Finally, after discussions, some users may change
their political position. This phenomenon might occur because discussing involves
processing new information that challenges one’s previous beliefs. This exposure
might be sufficient to enable attitude ambivalence or directly change one’s political
attitudes and opinions.
The finding that a positive association between news consumption and political
persuasion is encouraging for democratic societies. However, under certain condi-
tions, fake news exposure may exert similar effects. Online/social media discussion
plays a key role here, mediating some of these effects on political persuasion. It is
important to stress that discussion partially mitigates the effect of fake news exposure.
In addition, there might be potential cofounders, such as political orientations or other
political motivations, either dispositional or situational. These could potentially impact
on people’s news consumption online and on social media and further, affect how citi-
zens engage in political discussions. Although this study has included some of these
potential orientations as controls (i.e., political interest, ideology, discussion network
size, etc.), future research should further address this possibility.
In contrast to prior studies (Diehl et al. 2016; Gil de Zúñiga et al., 2018), one of the
primary findings of our article is that social media news use seems not to exert a direct
effect on political persuasion. As explained above, this holds as all the mentioned
variables are considered in our novel, stringent model (Figure 1). Although we do not
focus on the discussion attributes here, this study has unveiled some nuances about the
process of political persuasion that the literature was not fully aware of. Furthermore,
while building on the cited studies, we have considered in our models the different
types of information sources available in the virtual environment, differentiating
between social media news and online news, as well as fake news. This consideration
also contributes to the literature by providing a better glance at how such diverse ways
of being exposed to information online lead to political persuasion. On the whole, our
analysis suggests that political persuasion on social media is most likely to occur when
people (1) consume news online and on social media, (2) are exposed to fake news,
and (3) engage in discussions online/on social media.
As much as this study contributes to better understanding the mechanisms that lead
to individual political attitude change in social media, it is not exempt from noteworthy
limitations and caveats. First, we conduct our analysis on data from a representative
sample of the US general population; this limits our study in comparative terms, and
future research is needed to establish if the causal association reported exists in other
geographic areas. Second, our study’s findings are based upon statistical analyses of
online panel surveys, potentially lacking the causal inference attributable to experimen-
tal methods. Third, our dependent variable (i.e., political persuasion) alongside fake
news exposure is based on self-reports. These kinds of measurements are linked to
several limitations, such as individuals’ cognitive capacity to recall accurately.
Accordingly, respondents might not only correctly remember the extent to which they
were exposed to misleading news pieces, but also may underreport their actual fake
news exposure: social desirability. Finally, we lack information about the content of
discussions, the news consumed, and measures of particular political attitudes or issues
16 American Behavioral Scientist 00(0)
from which persuasion emerges. Therefore, our study may not accurately pinpoint
whether political persuasion is unfolding as changes in individuals’ previous beliefs or
stances regarding a particular issue or whether there is a reinforcement loop of such
beliefs. On the whole, our study fleshes out the idea that political persuasion in social
media unfolds in different pathways hinging on the very nature of news and the political
discussions they trigger.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship,
and/or publication of this article.
Funding
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship,
and/or publication of this article: This work has benefited from the support of the Spanish
National Research Agency’s Program for the Generation of Knowledge and the Scientific and
Technological Strengthening Research + Development Grant PID2020-115562GB-I00. The
first author is funded by the ‘Beatriz Galindo Program’ from the Spanish Ministry of Science,
Innovation & Universities, and the Junta de Castilla y León. The second author is funded by the
Spanish National Research Agency (MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033) and the European
Social Fund Plus (ESF+) (PRE2021-097685). Responsibility for the information and views set
out in this study lies entirely with the authors.
ORCID iD
Homero Gil de Zúñiga https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4187-3604
Note
1. It is noteworthy to state that the R2 (2.3%) explained for social media political persuasion
in the structural equation model is beyond the R2 explained for all other control variables,
included the autoregressive term for a total of R2 of 48.3.
References
Allcott, H., & Gentzkow, M. (2017). Social media and fake news in the 2016 election. Journal
of Economic Perspectives, 31(2), 211–236. https://doi.org/10/gc5t4c
Allcott, H., Gentzkow, M., & Yu, C. (2019). Trends in the diffusion of misinformation on social
media. Research & Politics, 6(2), 2053168019848554. https://doi.org/10/ggff7j
American Association for Public Opinion Research (AAPOR). (2018). Best practices for survey
Research. https://www.aapor.org/Standards-Ethics/Best-Practices.aspx
Anderson, M., & Caumont, A. (2014). How social media is reshaping news. Pew Research
Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2014/09/24/how-social-media-is-reshaping-
news/
Anspach, N. M. (2017). The new personal influence: How our Facebook friends influence the
news we read. Political Communication, 34(4), 590–606.
Ardèvol-Abreu, A., Barnidge, M., & de Zúñiga, H. G. (2017). Communicative antecedents of
political persuasion: Political discussion, citizen news creation, and the moderating role of
strength of partisanship. Mass Communication and Society, 20(2), 169–191.
Gil de Zúñiga et al. 17
Arias, E. (2019). How does media influence social norms? Experimental evidence on the role of
common knowledge. Political Science Research and Methods, 7(3), 561–578.
Asur, S., & Huberman, B. A. (2010). Predicting the future with social media [Conference ses-
sion]. 2010 IEEE/WIC/ACM International Conference on Web Intelligence and Intelligent
Agent Technology, Toronto, ON, Canada, Vol. 1, pp. 492–499.
Awang, Z. (2014). A handbook on SEM for academicians and practitioners: The step by step
practical guides for the beginners. Bandar Baru Bangi, MPWS Rich Resources, 10(3), 32–45.
Azzimonti, M., & Fernandes, M. (2018). Social media networks, fake news, and polarization.
National Bureau of Economic Research.
Bakshy, E., Rosenn, I., Marlow, C., & Adamic, L. (2012). The role of social networks in infor-
mation diffusion. ArXiv:1201.4145 [Physics]. http://arxiv.org/abs/1201.4145
Balmas, M. (2014). When fake news becomes real: Combined exposure to multiple news
sources and political attitudes of inefficacy, alienation, and cynicism. Communication
Research, 41(3), 430–454. https://doi.org/10/f5tnms
Bastick, Z. (2021). Would you notice if fake news changed your behavior? An experiment on
the unconscious effects of disinformation. Computers in Human Behavior, 116, 106633.
https://doi.org/10/ghk5s9
Bode, L., & Vraga, E. K. (2018). See something, say something: Correction of global health
misinformation on social media. Health Communication, 33(9), 1131–1140. https://doi.
org/10/gdgzp3
Bohner, G., Erb, H.-P., & Sieber, F. (2008). Information processing approaches to persuasion:
Integrating assumptions from the dual- and single-processing perspectives. In W. D. Crano
& R. Prislin (Eds.), Attitudes and attitude change (pp. 161–188). Psychology Press.
Boyd, D., & Ellison, N. B. (2007). Social network sites: Definition, history, and scholarship.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 13(1), 210–230. https://doi.org/10/gzn
Brundidge, J. (2010a). Encountering “difference” in the contemporary public sphere: The con-
tribution of the Internet to the heterogeneity of political discussion networks. Journal of
Communication, 60(4), 680–700. https://doi.org/10/b7mbng
Brundidge, J. (2010b). Political discussion and news use in the contemporary public sphere:
The “accessibility” and “traversability” of the Internet. Javnost - The Public, 17(2), 63–81.
https://doi.org/10/gh35gp
Burnell, P., & Reeve, A. (1984). Persuasion as a political concept. British Journal of Political
Science, 14(4), 393–410. https://doi.org/10/bdhsdc
Chadwick, A., Vaccari, C., & O’Loughlin, B. (2018). Do tabloids poison the well of social
media? Explaining democratically dysfunctional news sharing. New Media & Society,
20(11), 4255–4274. https://doi.org/10/gdccxn
Chaffee, S. H., & Kanihan, S. F. (1997). Learning about politics from the mass media. Political
Communication, 14(4), 421–430.
Chang, J.-H., Zhu, Y.-Q., Wang, S.-H., & Li, Y.-J. (2018). Would you change your mind? An
empirical study of social impact theory on Facebook. Telematics and Informatics, 35(1),
282–292. https://doi.org/10/gh4mfr
Cobb, M. D., & Kuklinski, J. H. (1997). Changing minds: Political arguments and political per-
suasion. American Journal of Political Science, 41(1), 88. https://doi.org/10/b4cz3f
Colliander, J. (2019). “This is fake news”: Investigating the role of conformity to other users’
views when commenting on and spreading disinformation in social media. Computers in
Human Behavior, 97, 202–215. https://doi.org/10/ggcgpd
Conroy, N. K., Rubin, V. L., & Chen, Y. (2015). Automatic deception detection: Methods for
finding fake news. Proceedings of the Association for Information Science and Technology,
52(1), 1–4. https://doi.org/10/gfkdfm
18 American Behavioral Scientist 00(0)
Diehl, T., Weeks, B. E., & Gil de Zúñiga, H. (2016). Political persuasion on social media:
Tracing direct and indirect effects of news use and social interaction. New Media & Society,
18(9), 1875–1895. https://doi.org/10/f8336n
Dimitrova, D. V., Shehata, A., Strömbäck, J., & Nord, L. W. (2014). The effects of digital media
on political knowledge and participation in election campaigns: Evidence from panel data.
Communication Research, 41(1), 95–118. https://doi.org/10/b2f36w
Ellison, N. B., Vitak, J., Steinfield, C., Gray, R., & Lampe, C. (2011). Negotiating privacy con-
cerns and social capital needs in a social media environment. In S. Trepte & L. Reinecke
(Eds.), Privacy online (pp. 19–32). Springer.
Faragó, L., Kende, A., & Krekó, P. (2020). We only believe in news that we doctored ourselves:
The connection between partisanship and political fake news. Social Psychology, 51(2),
77–90. https://doi.org/10/gf876t
Feldman, L. (2011). The opinion factor: The effects of opinionated news on information pro-
cessing and attitude change. Political Communication, 28(2), 163–181. https://doi.org/10/
fdqx36
Feldman, L., Maibach, E. W., Roser-Renouf, C., & Leiserowitz, A. (2012). Climate on cable:
the nature and impact of global warming coverage on Fox News, CNN, and MSNBC. The
International Journal of Press/Politics, 17(1), 3–31. https://doi.org/10/fgk5p4
Fishkin, J. S. (1991). Democracy and deliberation: New directions for democratic reform. Yale
University Press.
Fletcher, R., & Nielsen, R. K. (2018). Are people incidentally exposed to news on social media?
A comparative analysis. New Media & Society, 20(7), 2450–2468. https://doi.org/10/
gc7mvk
Gerber, A. S., Green, D. P., & Larimer, C. W. (2008). Social pressure and voter turnout:
Evidence from a large-scale field experiment. American Political Science Review, 102(1),
33–48. https://doi.org/10/bj6qz2
Gil de Zúñiga, H. (2017). Attributes of interpersonal political discussion as antecedents of
cognitive elaboration. Revista Española de Investigaciones Sociológicas, 157(1), 65–84.
https://doi.org/10.5477/cis/reis.157.65
Gil de Zúñiga, H., Ardèvol-Abreu, A., & Casero-Ripollés, A. (2019). WhatsApp political dis-
cussion, conventional participation and activism: Exploring direct, indirect and genera-
tional effects. Information, Communication & Society, 24(2), 201–218.
Gil de Zúñiga, H., Ardèvol-Abreu, A., Diehl, T., Patiño, M. G., & Liu, J. H. (2019). Trust
in institutional actors across 22 Countries. Examining political, science, and media trust
around the world. Revista Latina de Comunicación Social, 74, 237–262.
Gil de Zúñiga, H., Barnidge, M., & Diehl, T. (2018). Political persuasion on social media: A
moderated moderation model of political discussion disagreement and civil reasoning. The
Information Society, 34(5), 302–315. https://doi.org/10.1080/01972243.2018.1497743
Gil de Zúñiga, H., & Chen, T. C. (2019) Digital media and politics: Effects of the great infor-
mation and communication divides. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 63(3),
365–373, https://doi.org/10.1080/08838151.2019.1662019
Gil de Zúñiga, H., Diehl, T., Huber, B., & Liu, J. H. (2019). The citizen communication media-
tion model across countries: A multilevel mediation model of news use and discussion on
political participation. Journal of Communication, 69(2), 144–167.
Gil de Zúñiga, H., Valenzuela, S., & Weeks, B. E. (2016). Motivations for political discussion:
antecedents and consequences on civic engagement. Human Communication Research,
42(4), 533–552. https://doi.org/10.1111/hcre.12086
Gil de Zúñiga et al. 19
Gottfried, J., & Shearer, E. (2016). News use across social media platforms 2016. Pew Research
Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/journalism/2016/05/26/news-use-across-social-media-
platforms-2016/
Goyanes, M., Borah, P., & Gil de Zúñiga, H. (2021). Social media filtering and democ-
racy: Effects of social media news use and uncivil political discussions on social media
unfriending. Computers in Human Behavior, 120, 106759. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
chb.2021.106759
Graves, L., & Amazeen, M. A. (2019). Fact-checking as idea and practice in journalism. In J.
Nussbaum (Ed.), Oxford research encyclopedia of communication. Oxford University Press.
https://oxfordre.com/communication/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228613.001.0001/
acrefore-9780190228613-e-808.
Guo, L., & Vargo, C. (2020). “Fake news” and emerging online media ecosystem: An integrated
intermedia agenda-setting analysis of the 2016 US presidential election. Communication
Research, 47(2), 178–200. https://doi.org/10/gfkdft
Ha, L., Andreu Perez, L., & Ray, R. (2021). Mapping recent development in scholarship on
fake news and misinformation, 2008 to 2017: Disciplinary contribution, topics, and impact.
American Behavioral Scientist, 65(2), 290–315. https://doi.org/10/ghjkm7
Halpern, D., & Gibbs, J. (2013). Social media as a catalyst for online deliberation? Exploring
the affordances of Facebook and YouTube for political expression. Computers in Human
Behavior, 29(3), 1159–1168. https://doi.org/10/f4tj3x
Hermida, A., Fletcher, F., Korell, D., & Logan, D. (2012). Share, like, recommend: Decoding
the social media news consumer. Journalism Studies, 13(5–6), 815–824. https://doi.org/10/
gcx595
Huckfeldt, R., Johnson, P. E., & Sprague, J. (2005). Individuals, dyads, and networks:
Autoregressive patterns of political influence. In A. S. Zuckerman (Ed.), The social logic
of politics: Personal networks as contexts for political behavior (pp. 21–48). Temple
University Press.
Iyengar, S., & Simon, A. F. (2000). New perspectives and evidence on political communication
and campaign effects. Annual Review of Psychology, 51(1), 149–169. https://doi.org/10/
c9fmgk
Jacobs, L. R., Cook, F. L., & Carpini, M. X. D. (2009). Talking together: Public deliberation
and political participation in America. University of Chicago Press.
Jahng, M. R. (2018). From reading comments to seeking news: Exposure to disagreements
from online comments and the need for opinion-challenging news. Journal of Information
Technology & Politics, 15(2), 142–154. https://doi.org/10/gdf335
Jankowski, N. W. (2018). Researching fake news: A selective examination of empirical studies.
Javnost - The Public, 25(1–2), 248–255.
Johansen, M. S., & Joslyn, M. R. (2008). Political persuasion during times of crisis: The effects
of education and news media on citizens’ factual information about Iraq. Journalism &
Mass Communication Quarterly, 85(3), 591–608.
Kahne, J., & Bowyer, B. (2018). The political significance of social media activity and social
networks. Political Communication, 35(3), 470–493. https://doi.org/10/gdskjv
Kaplan, A. M., & Haenlein, M. (2010). Users of the world, unite! The challenges and opportuni-
ties of Social Media. Business Horizons, 53(1), 59–68. https://doi.org/10/gzr
Keele, L., & Wolak, J. (2008). Contextual sources of ambivalence. Political Psychology, 29(5),
653–673. https://doi.org/10/cnkb8m
Khan, M. L. (2017). Social media engagement: What motivates user participation and consump-
tion on YouTube? Computers in Human Behavior, 66, 236–247. https://doi.org/10/f9fkd6
20 American Behavioral Scientist 00(0)
Kim, Y., Chen, H.-T., & Gil de Zúñiga, H. (2013). Stumbling upon news on the Internet:
Effects of incidental news exposure and relative entertainment use on political engage-
ment. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(6), 2607–2614. https://doi.org/10/f5djrt
Kim, Y., Hsu, S.-H., & de Zúñiga, H. G. (2013). Influence of social media use on discussion
network heterogeneity and civic engagement: The moderating role of personality traits.
Journal of Communication, 63(3), 498–516. https://doi.org/10/f42bmm
Ladd, J. M., & Lenz, G. S. (2008). Exploiting a rare shift in communication flows to document
news media persuasion: The 1997 United Kingdom general election. SSRN Electronic
Journal. https://ssrn.com/abstract=1189811
Lane, D. S., Lee, S. S., Liang, F., Kim, D. H., Shen, L., Weeks, B. E., & Kwak, N. (2019).
Social media expression and the political self. Journal of Communication, 69(1), 49–72.
Lazer, D. M. J., Baum, M. A., Benkler, Y., Berinsky, A. J., Greenhill, K. M., Menczer, F.,
Metzger, M. J., Nyhan, B., Pennycook, G., Rothschild, D., Schudson, M., Sloman, S. A.,
Sunstein, C. R., Thorson, E. A., Watts, D. J., & Zittrain, J. L. (2018). The science of fake
news. Science, 359(6380), 1094–1096. https://doi.org/10/gc3jgv
Lee, J. K., Choi, J., Kim, C., & Kim, Y. (2014). Social media, network heterogeneity, and
opinion polarization. Journal of Communication, 64(4), 702–722. https://doi.org/10/cnkc
Lee, J., & Choi, Y. (2017). Shifting from an audience to an active public in social viewing:
Focusing on the discussion network. Computers in Human Behavior, 75, 301–310.
Luo, M., Hancock, J. T., & Markowitz, D. M. (2020). Credibility perceptions and detection
accuracy of fake news headlines on social media: Effects of truth-bias and endorsement
cues. Communication Research, 49(2), 171–195. https://doi.org/10/ggxq7b
McCombs, M. E., & Shaw, D. L. (1972). The agenda-setting function of mass media. The
Public Opinion Quarterly, 36(2), 176–187. https://doi.org/10/dnd7p4
Messing, S., & Westwood, S. J. (2014). Selective exposure in the age of social media:
Endorsements trump partisan source affiliation when selecting news online.
Communication Research, 41(8), 1042–1063. https://doi.org/10/f6rfc5
Moravec, P., Minas, R., & Dennis, A. R. (2018). Fake news on social media: People believe
what they want to believe when it makes no sense at all. SSRN Electronic Journal. https://
ssrn.com/abstract=3269541
Mutz, D. C. (2002). The consequences of cross-cutting networks for political participation.
American Journal of Political Science, 46(4), 838. https://doi.org/10/bwsf88
Mutz, D. C. (2006). Hearing the other side: Deliberative versus participatory democracy.
Cambridge University Press.
Mutz, D. C., & Martin, P. S. (2001). Facilitating communication across lines of political
difference: The role of mass media. American Political Science Review, 95(1), 97–114.
https://doi.org/10/ggd49z
Mutz, D. C., Sniderman, P. M., & Brody, R. A. (Eds.). (1996). Political persuasion and atti-
tude change. University of Michigan Press.
O’keefe, D. J. (2015). Persuasion: Theory and research. Sage Publications.
Perloff, R. M. (2008). Political Persuasion. In J. P. Dillard & L. Shen (Eds.), The SAGE hand-
book of persuasion: Developments in theory and practice. (pp. 258–277). SAGE
Perloff, R. M. (2020). The dynamics of persuasion: Communication and attitudes in the
twenty- first century. Routledge.
Petty, R. E. (2018). Attitudes and persuasion: Classic and contemporary approaches.
Routledge.
Gil de Zúñiga et al. 21
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In
Communication and persuasion (pp. 1–24). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-
4612-4964-1_1
Rapp, D. N., & Salovich, N. A. (2018). Can’t we just disregard fake news? The consequences
of exposure to inaccurate information. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 5(2), 232–239. https://doi.org/10/gf3gq2
Shearer, E., & Mitchell, A. (2021). News use across social media platforms in 2020. https://
www.pewresearch.org/journalism/2021/01/12/news-use-across-social-media-platforms-
in-2020/
Simons, H. W. (1976). persuasion: Understanding, practice, and analysis. Addison-Wesley
Pub. Co.
Tandoc, E. C., Lim, Z. W., & Ling, R. (2018). Defining “fake news”: A typology of scholarly
definitions. Digital Journalism, 6(2), 137–153. https://doi.org/10/gdgzp5
Turcotte, J., York, C., Irving, J., Scholl, R., & Pingree, R. (2015). News recommendations
from social media opinion leaders: Effects on media trust and information seeking.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 20(5), 520–535.
Valenzuela, S. (2013). Unpacking the use of social media for protest behavior: The roles
of information, opinion expression, and activism. American Behavioral Scientist, 57(7),
920–942. https://doi.org/10/f425zn
Valenzuela, S., Kim, Y., & Gil de Zuniga, H. (2012). Social networks that matter: Exploring
the role of political discussion for online political participation. International Journal of
Public Opinion Research, 24(2), 163–184. https://doi.org/10/fqwn6r
Van Noorden, R. (2014). Online collaboration: Scientists and the social network. Nature
News, 512(7513), 126. https://doi.org/10/t6v
Velasquez, A., & Rojas, H. (2017). Political expression on social media: The role of
communication competence and expected outcomes. Social Media + Society, 3(1),
2056305117696521.
Vosoughi, S., Roy, D., & Aral, S. (2018). The spread of true and false news online. Science,
359(6380), 1146–1151. https://doi.org/10/gc3jt6
Wardle, C., & Derakhshan, H. (2017). Information disorder: Toward an interdisciplinary
framework for research and policy making. Council of Europe Report, 27, 1–107.
Weeks, B. E., Ardèvol-Abreu, A., Gil de & Zúñiga, H. (2015). Online influence? Social
media use, opinion leadership, and political persuasion. International Journal of Public
Opinion Research, 29(2), 214–239. https://doi.org/10/ggnfr5
Weeks, B. E., Gil de & Zúñiga, H. (2021). What’s next? Six observations for the future
of political misinformation research. American Behavioral Scientist, 65(2), 277–289.
https://doi.org/10/gf9nw8
Weiksner, G. M., Fogg, B. J., & Liu, X. (2008). Six patterns for persuasion in online social
networks. In H. Oinas-Kukkonen, P. Hasle, M. Harjumaa, K. Segerståhl, & P. Øhrstrøm
(Eds.), Persuasive technology (Vol. 5033, pp. 151–163). Springer Berlin Heidelberg.
Whiting, A., & Williams, D. (2013). Why people use social media: A uses and gratifica-
tions approach. Qualitative Market Research: An International Journal, 16(4), 362–369.
https://doi.org/10/gf6hs4
Wood, W. (2000). Attitude change: Persuasion and social influence. Annual Review of
Psychology, 51, 539–570. https://doi.org/10/b7c9s6
Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge University Press
22 American Behavioral Scientist 00(0)
Author Biographies
Homero Gil de Zúñiga is a Distinguished Research Professor at University of Salamanca and
serves as director of the Democracy Research Unit (DRU). He is also a Professor at Pennsylvania
State University, and a Senior Research Fellow at Universidad Diego Portales, Chile. In general,
his work draws from theoretically driven research, aiming to shed an empirical social scientific
light over how social media, algorithms, AI, and other technologies affect society and
democracy.
Pablo González-González is a doctoral candidate in Political Science at the University of
Salamanca, and a member of the Democracy Research Unit (DRU). His research interests
revolve around quantitative methods applied to political communication, social media, and
political behavior.
Manuel Goyanes (PhD) serves as an assistant professor at Carlos III University in Madrid and
is a former visiting fellow at both the London School of Economics (LSE) and the University of
Vienna. His research addresses the influence of journalism and new technologies over citizens’
daily lives, as well as the effects of news consumption on citizens’ political knowledge and
participation. He is also interested in global inequalities in academic participation, the system-
atic biases towards global South scholars, and publication trends in Communication. His works
appeared in top-tier journals such as News Media & Society, Information, Communication &
Society, Scientometrics, Computers in Human Behaviour, etc. He is editorial board member in
several international journals and the co-PI of a funded interdisciplinary project on fake news
detection on the Internet.
... Moreover, although persuasion is considered to be focused on altering attitudes in three different ways, through changing, forming or reinforcing them (Miller, 2012), many studies on persuasion, including recent studies on social media (e.g., Chang et al., 2018;Gil De Zúñiga et al., 2022), have tended to examine the first outcome. Equating persuasion with attitude change has been criticized before in communication because it creates an artificial boundary around the phenomenon (Holbert et al., 2010). ...
... The interactive nature of social media also facilitates persuasion through other mechanisms, leading users even to persuade themselves through self-effects (Cho et al., 2018;Valkenburg, 2017). On these grounds, the growing body of research on political persuasion in social media has shown a number of the antecedents that may lead users to change their political attitudes, including news use or fake news exposure (Diehl et al., 2016;Gil De Zúñiga et al., 2022). Moreover, recent qualitative studies on trust and news reception may also help to understand how citizens are persuaded in these ecologies. ...
... Studies show that users in social media are not only persuaded by truthful content, but also by fake news, which are rampant in these ecologies and easily disseminated through affordances (Gil De Zúñiga et al., 2022;Tandoc, 2019;Treem and Leonardi, 2013). Fake news is now commonly referred to as false or inaccurate content that mimics the news format and that is designed to confuse audiences and deliberately mislead (Choi and Lee, 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
Research has noted that political content on social media, both accurate and fake, may lead users to reconsider their political opinions and be persuaded. This study contributes to this topic by examining how political attitude change in social media unfolds according to users’ perceptions. Our findings, based on in-depth interviews with 30 Spanish social media users, show how the centrality of social media in citizens’ everyday life nudges them into a persuasive relationship in these ecologies. We conceptualize this dynamic relationship as social media symbiosis, an analogy that draws on the biological concept of symbiosis, and that explains the characteristics of perceived online persuasion: (1) the primacy of social media persuasion over other information sources, (2) its gradual and evolutionary nature, and (3) the persuasive relationships in which users may engage: mutualism, in which both sender and receiver reciprocally benefit and persuade each other; and parasitism, in which fake news creators harmfully persuade receivers while benefitting. Our study contributes to extant literature by theorizing about the persuasive relationship in which users perceive to engage in social media, and the rationales that facilitate it.
... Conceptualising and operationalising transitions while ensuring that no one is left behind (European Commission, 2019) necessitates adaptive and social innovation engagement methods and simplified language in policy communication (Moroni et al., 2020). One social innovation engagement tool is social media (Geissinger et al., 2023), it plays a critical role in influencing both the public's trust in public policies (Gil de Zúñiga et al., 2022) and in adopting new societal participation models (Querol et al., 2011;Hadžialić, 2018). In this contribution, the focus is directed towards gauging how the public discussion on the JGT is articulated in the social virtual space over the 20 months since the introduction of the JTM. ...
... The choice for limiting the inquiry to Twitter has been based upon its reputation in literature for being the main social media political arena, an aspect which fits the requirements for our research aim. Nevertheless, Twitter (now X) does not cover all the voices on social media, not to mention the role of fake news, misinformation, disinformation (Gil de Zúñiga et al., 2022), and the radical influence of the below-the-radar platforms on the political debates on Twitter (Monaci & Persico, 2023). Another issue is that social media reach is socially and spatially relative. ...
Article
Since the European Green Deal was launched (European Commission, 2019), social media and especially Twitter (now X) has become the one main socio-political virtual space where the European Commission is promoting the European Union’s twinned (Just, Green, and Digital) Transitions. This multidisciplinary exploratory research crosscuts Spatial Planning & Territorial Governance (Nadine et al., 2018; Berisha, et al., 2021), and Social Media Studies and Digital Methods (Rogers, 2018; 2019), investigating the existence of the term "Just Green Transitions" on Twitter, highlighting the potential benefits that social media analysis could add to the disciplines of Governance and Policymaking. Utilizing Social Network Analysis, both Mention Analysis and Co-hashtag- Analysis were applied to capture and analyse three million original tweets over the first twenty months since the Just Transition Mechanism entered into force (European Parliament, 2021). The results highlighted the fragmentation of the terminologies and Twitter user communities, discussing the expected transitions of Europe which are expected to be Just and Green (European Commission, 2021).
... By combining new technology with real-world political dynamics, the study examines the intersection of emotion, trust, and identity. As social media increasingly becomes the dominant arena for political persuasion (De Zúñiga et al., 2022a, 2022b, understanding how these factors in uence public opinion is essential for addressing the challenges of modern democracy. ...
Article
Full-text available
Who do people trust in politics, and why? Our online survey experiment reveals that trust and credibility are driven less by emotional victimization narratives and more by partisanship. Political messages resonate most when they align with the audience's ideological beliefs, overshadowing the impact of emotional appeals. These findings highlight the power of identity in shaping perceptions and the challenges of bridging partisan divides in today's polarized landscape. Tailored messaging that speaks to shared values remains key to building trust and engagement.      In today's hyperconnected world, the arena of political debate has shifted from parliaments and rallies to the digital stage of social media. Here, every post has the potential to build trust or spark outrage, amplifying voices and emotions in ways that rede ne public discourse (Huszár et al., 2021; Yarchi et al., 2020). Our study examines this dynamic space, exploring how political af liation and narratives of victimhood shape perceptions of credibility and emotional engagement. At the heart of this research lies a fundamental question: "How do people decide whom to trust?" Furthermore, we ask, "How do identity and emotion shape these judgments?" To explore these questions, we conducted an experiment that re ects the digital realities of political communication. Using AI-generated posts, participants were exposed to messages from representatives of two ideologically distinct UK political parties: the right-wing populist Reform UK and the progressive Green Party. Some posts portrayed the communicators as victims of political persecution, while others focused solely on party platforms. By combining new technology with real-world political dynamics, the study examines the intersection of emotion, trust, and identity. As social media increasingly becomes the dominant arena for political persuasion (De Zúñiga et al., 2022a, 2022b), understanding how these factors in uence public opinion is essential for addressing the challenges of modern democracy. The Online Survey Experiment Imagine scrolling through your social media feed, where political messages compete for attention amid a sea of hashtags and soundbites. This study aimed to replicate that environment by exposing participants to custom-designed posts that mirrored the type of content people encounter daily on platforms like X. After engaging with these posts, participants provided their reactions through a survey, enabling us to measure two critical factors: the extent to which they trusted the messages and their emotional responses. The experiment was designed to explore the relationship between political af liation and emotional appeals. To achieve this, we introduced a ctional candidate representing either the Green Party or Reform UK. The ctional candidate either portrayed themselves as a victim of political persecution or focused solely on communicating their party's agenda, avoiding any mention of personal hardship. This deliberate design allowed us to investigate how the intersection of identity politics, emotional narratives, and party alignment shapes perceptions of trust and credibility in the digital age. By simulating the dynamics of online political discourse, the study offers insights into how emotional and ideological cues in uence the way people perceive and engage with political messaging. In a world where social media serves as the primary battleground for political persuasion, understanding these mechanisms is more critical than ever. Trust and Emotions in A Polarized World In the polarized world of politics, trust and credibility are often elusive goals. Our preliminary ndings offer intriguing insights into what makes political messages resonate-or fail-depending on the audience. Messages framed as coming from a Green Party candidate consistently inspired higher trust and credibility compared to those attributed to a Reform UK candidate, particularly among our predominantly left-leaning sample. This trend held true regardless of whether victimization narratives were employed. In essence, partisanship outweighs narrative. Interestingly, our ndings suggest that victimization narratives-often a powerful emotional tool in political rhetoric-didn't signi cantly in uence trust or credibility within the same political frame. Instead, political ideology appears to be an effective barrier to rhetorical strategy, lending credence to the idea of partisans being stuck in echo-chambers, preventing them from accepting opposing messages. But there's another layer to this story: political alignment. For left-leaning individuals, the Green Party's message was consistently rated as more trustworthy and credible than Reform UK's, across all scenarios. On the other hand, right-leaning individuals were less swayed by the policy frame, though they showed a slight preference for Reform when victimization narratives were included. This dynamic highlights how deeply our ideological beliefs shape the way we perceive political communication, with certain rhetorical strategies being effective for some but not others.
... In this context, an emerging line of inquiry has focused on investigating the antecedents that lead users to be politically persuaded in these ecologies. These studies speak of persuasion and have mostly examined self-reported attitude change, noting that users who use social media to get informed, discuss politics or consume political content may ultimately change opinions about political issues, but also those who use it for social interaction or who encounter fake news (Chang et al., 2018;Diehl et al., 2016;Gil de Zúñiga et al., 2022;Jordá & Goyanes, 2022;Jordá et al., 2024). ...
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter provides an overview of research on political persuasion in the field of communication. It begins with a summary of the four traditional phases of classic media effects research. It then reviews current debates about the effects of social media, describing the two main strands of studies that have provided evidence for and against the alleged contribution of these platforms to political polarization. Throughout the chapter, we emphasize that media effects research has typically revolved around the persuasion dimension of attitude change, although more recent studies have mainly focused on reinforcement. The chapter also includes three suggestions for future work: extending research on narrative persuasion to the realm of social media, investigating the persuasive impact of entertainment-oriented use and content on these platforms in more detail, and introducing a holistic approach to persuasion to better understand how the different dimensions of this phenomenon are interrelated and complementary.
... In view of the trouble brought by fake news, attempts to combat widespread fake news have so far focused on defining fake news [3] [5], investigating factors that caused fake news sharing [6] and threats brought by fake news [7] [8] [9] [10], summarizing features of fake news [11] [12] [13] and pathways of fake news spreading [14] [15], exploring ways to detect fake news [16] [17] and strategies to enhance people's ability to perceive fake news accordingly [18] [19] [20] [21]. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
The present study aimed to scrutinize how fake news have been explored in worldwide scientific articles over the recent years. By employing bibliometric techniques and extracting 1,844 articles from the Web of Science database published between 2013 and 2024, this study attempted to explore different perspectives in fake news researches to obtain understanding of research collaborations among authors, affiliations, and countries, research trends and emerging frontiers related to fake news. R packages including ‘Bibliometrix’ analyzed bibliometric features, visualized international collaborations among different countries, and outlined their volume of publications. VOSviewer was employed to analyze co-authorship across authors, affiliations, and countries, and co-occurrence of keywords. VOSviewer also visualized the analyses with overlay network to display changes of research trends in different periods. And CiteSpace was used to visualize the citation burst of references.
... Over the course of several decades, researchers have proposed various definitions for political persuasion (Mutz, Brody, and Sniderman 1996;Petty and Cacioppo 2019), usually clustering around the idea of attitude changes related to politics (Diehl, Weeks, and Gil De Zúñiga 2016;Perloff 2012) brought about by a deliberate communicative action designed to influence (O′Keefe 2015; Perloff 2010). In recent years, there has been an effort to conceptualize political persuasion in close tandem with political communication, describing it as reassessment of political attitudes on the grounds of new political information to which one was exposed (Gil de Zúñiga, Barnidge, and Diehl 2018;Gil de Zúñiga, González-González, and Goyanes 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Political consumerism has emerged as a popular form of participation among lay citizens. For the most part, research has delved into the main antecedents that explain this type of consumption. However, research has generally neglected to consider political consumerism as an independent variable, and its role influencing and igniting other forms of political behavior. This study theoretically argues that politically motivated consumerism will incite people to generally seek to persuade others of political issues. Using causal order tests and Random Intercept Cross-lagged Panel Structural Equation Models from a two-wave panel survey from the United States, the study shows robust empirical evidence that this is the case.
Article
Full-text available
This study investigates the persuasive strategies employed by Jordanian interlocutors within an academic setting, utilizing Aristotle's rhetorical triangle-ethos (credibility), logos (logic), and pathos (emotion)-as a framework for analysis. Data was collected through observation and memo-writing, allowing in-situ analysis of naturally occurring interactions between students and professors. The study reveals that distinct preferences emerge when students and professors employ all three rhetorical appeals. Professors tend to favor logos-driven strategies, emphasizing logical reasoning and evidence-based arguments. Students, conversely, lean towards pathos-driven approaches, appealing to emotions and shared experiences to achieve their persuasive goals. Furthermore, this research incorporates a translation studies perspective, recognizing that academic discourse in Jordan often involves navigating multiple languages and cultural contexts. This highlights how language can become a persuasion tool, adding complexity to the interplay of rhetorical strategies and cross-cultural communication. This study contributes to a deeper understanding of how cultural context shapes persuasive communication within academic settings, while also offering insights into the dynamics of rhetoric and translation in multilingual environments. Future research could explore the specific linguistic strategies employed within each rhetorical appeal and further investigate the interplay between cultural background and the interpretation of persuasive language. This research has pedagogical implications for enhancing communication skills in academic settings, particularly in multilingual and cross-cultural contexts. |
Article
Full-text available
Exploring the dynamics of online fake political news, media literacy, and political engagement among university students in Kwara State. The information sphere in Africa and Nigeria, in particular, has evolved with the pervasive influence of social media. This has constantly brought to the fore, the pressing concern about fake news which unarguably is capable of undermining the political and democratic foundations of any nation. Experts and scholars have voiced apprehensions in recognizing these potential risks, emphasizing the urgent need for attention and intervention. This study delves into the interplay of online fake political news, media literacy, and political engagement among university students in Kwara State. This research focuses on university students who due to their dynamic energy and active engagement are expected to wield significant influence within the nation's population on issues of governance. We employed a quantitative research approach, surveying 400 respondents across selected universities in Kwara State, namely: the University of Ilorin, Al-Hikmah University, and Kwara State University. Key findings reveal a notable preference among respondents for online platforms as their primary source of political news; a substantial majority of participants exhibit a good level of media literacy, actively discerning and avoiding engagement with political processes tainted by fake news. Furthermore, our research establishes a relationship between exposure to online fake political news and political participation among university students. Our findings underscore the pivotal role of media literacy in shaping civic engagement and fostering a discerning electorate and highlight the need for a nuanced understanding of the use of digital and social media landscape to foster integration and political stability through the youths’ active engagement in governance. Keywords: social media, Fake political news, media literature, political engagement, university students
Book
Full-text available
For over two decades, political communication research has hailed the potentially reinvigorating effect of social media on democracy. Social media was expected to provide new opportunities for people to learn about politics and public affairs, and to participate politically. Building on two systematic literature reviews on social media, and its effects on political participation and knowledge (2000–2020), and introducing empirical evidence drawing on four original US survey data that expands for over a decade (2009–2020), this Element contends that social media has only partially fulfilled this tenet, producing a Social Media Democracy Mirage. That is, social media have led to a socio-political paradox in which people are more participatory than ever, yet not necessarily more informed.
Article
Full-text available
A growing literature is emerging on the believability and spread of disinformation, such as fake news, over social networks. However, little is known about the degree to which malicious actors can use social media to covertly affect behavior with disinformation. A lab-based randomized controlled experiment was conducted with 233 undergraduate students to investigate the behavioral effects of fake news. It was found that even short (under 5-min) exposure to fake news was able to significantly modify the unconscious behavior of individuals. This paper provides initial evidence that fake news can be used to covertly modify behavior, it argues that current approaches to mitigating fake news, and disinformation in general, are insufficient to protect social media users from this threat, and it highlights the implications of this for democracy. It raises the need for an urgent cross-sectoral effort to investigate, protect against, and mitigate the risks of covert, widespread and decentralized behavior modification over online social networks.
Article
Full-text available
This article focuses on message credibility and detection accuracy of fake and real news as represented on social media. We developed a deception detection paradigm for news headlines and conducted two online experiments to examine the extent to which people (1) perceive news headlines as credible, and (2) accurately distinguish fake and real news across three general topics (i.e., politics, science, and health). Both studies revealed that people often judged news headlines as fake, suggesting a deception-bias for news in social media. Across studies, we observed an average detection accuracy of approximately 51%, a level consistent with most research using this deception detection paradigm with equal lie-truth base-rates. Study 2 evaluated the effects of endorsement cues in social media (e.g., Facebook likes) on message credibility and detection accuracy. Results showed that headlines associated with a high number of Facebook likes increased credibility, thereby enhancing detection accuracy for real news but undermining accuracy for fake news. These studies introduce truth-default theory to the context of news credibility and advance our understanding of how biased processing of news information can impact detection accuracy with social media endorsement cues.
Book
Full-text available
While the historical impact of rumours and fabricated content has been well documented, efforts to better understand today’s challenge of information pollution on a global scale are only just beginning. Concern about the implications of dis-information campaigns designed specifically to sow mistrust and confusion and to sharpen existing sociocultural divisions using nationalistic, ethnic, racial and religious tensions is growing. The Council of Europe report on “Information Disorder: Toward an interdisciplinary framework for research and policy making” is an attempt to comprehensively examine information disorder and to outline ways to address it.
Article
Full-text available
Research on political misinformation is booming. The field is continually gaining more key insights about this important and complex social problem. Academic interest on misinformation has consistently been a multidisciplinary effort. But perhaps political communication researchers are particularly well situated to be the leading voices on the public’s understanding of misinformation and many are heeding the call. With that responsibility in mind, in this brief article we offer six observations for the future of political misinformation research that we believe can help focus this line of inquiry to better ensure we address some of the most pressing problems. Our list is not exhaustive, nor do we suggest that areas we do not cover are not important. Rather, we make these observations with the goal of spurring a conversation about the future of political misinformation research.
Article
Full-text available
In this research we aimed to explore the importance of partisanship behind the belief in wish-fulfilling political fake news. We tested the role of political orientation, partisanship, and conspiracy mentality in the acceptance of pro-and anti-government pipedream fake news. Using a representative survey (N = 1,000) and a student sample (N = 382) in Hungary, we found that partisanship predicted belief in political fake news more strongly than conspiracy mentality, and these connections were mediated by the perceived credibility of source (independent journalism vs. political propaganda) and economic sentiment. Our findings suggest that political bias can be more important in predicting acceptance of pipedream political fake news than conspiracy mentality.
Chapter
Persuasion is an integral part of politics and a necessary component of the pursuit and exercise of power. Political persuasion is a process in which communicators try to convince other people to change their attitudes or behavior regarding a political issue through messages, in an atmosphere of free choice (Perloff 2003, 34). As the field of political communication has grown, so too has the number of studies exploring the processes and effects of political persuasive communication (→ Persuasion). Political persuasion involves the application of persuasion principles to a context in which most individuals possess the seemingly incompatible characteristics of harboring strong feelings about a host of issues, yet caring precious little about the context in which these issues are played out.
Article
We study how the structure of social media networks and the presence of fake news affects the degree of misinformation and polarization in a society. For that, we analyze a dynamic model of opinion exchange in which individuals have imperfect information about the true state of the world and exhibit bounded rationality. Key to the analysis is the presence of internet bots: agents in the network that spread fake news (e.g., a constant flow of biased information). We characterize how agents’ opinions evolve over time and evaluate the determinants of long-run misinformation and polarization in the network. To that end, we construct a synthetic network calibrated to Twitter and simulate the information exchange process over a long horizon to quantify the bots’ ability to spread fake news. A key insight is that significant misinformation and polarization arise in networks in which only 15% of agents believe fake news to be true, indicating that network externality effects are quantitatively important. Higher bot centrality typically increases polarization and lowers misinformation. When one bot is more influential than the other (asymmetric centrality), polarization is reduced but misinformation grows, as opinions become closer the more influential bot’s preferred point. Finally, we show that threshold rules tend to reduce polarization and misinformation. This is because, as long as agents also have access to unbiased sources of information, threshold rules actually limit the influence of bots.
Article
In todays’ progressively polarized society, social media users are increasingly exposed to blatant uncivil comments, dissonant views, and controversial news contents, both from their peers and the media organizations they follow. Recent scholarship on selective avoidance suggests that citizens when exposed to contentious stimuli tend to either neglect, avoid, or by-pass such content, a practice scholarly known as users’ filtration tactics or unfriending. Drawing upon a nationally representative panel survey from the United States (W1 = 1,338/W2 = 511) fielded in 2019/2020, this study seeks to a) examine whether social media news use is associated to exposure to uncivil political discussions, and 2) explore the ways in which both constructs causally affect users’ unfriending behavior. Finally, the study investigates the contingent moderating role of uncivil political discussion in energizing the relationship between social media use for news and unfriending. Our findings first find support for the idea that social media news use directly activates citizens’ uncivil discussions and unfriending, while uncivil political discussion directly triggers unfriending behavior and significantly contributes to intensify the effect of social media news use over citizens’ unfriending levels. These findings add to current conversations about the potential motivations and deleterious effects of social media filtering in contemporary democracies.