ArticlePDF Available

Evidence that infant and early childhood developmental impairments are associated with hallucinatory experiences: results from a large, population-based cohort study

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Background Cognitive and motor dysfunction are hallmark features of the psychosis continuum, and have been detected during late childhood and adolescence in youth who report psychotic experiences (PE). However, previous investigations have not explored infancy and early childhood development. It remains unclear whether such deficits emerge much earlier in life, and whether they are associated with psychotic, specifically hallucinatory, experiences (HE). Methods This study included data from Gen2 participants of The Raine Study ( n = 1101), a population-based longitudinal cohort study in Western Australia. Five areas of childhood development comprising: communication; fine motor; gross motor; adaptive (problem-solving); and personal-social skills, were assessed serially at ages 1, 2 and 3 years. Information on HE, depression and anxiety at ages 10, 14 and 17 years was obtained. HE were further subdivided into those with transient or recurrent experiences. Mixed effects logistic regression models and cumulative risk analyses based on multiple domain delays were performed. Results Early poorer development in multiple areas was noted from ages 1, 2 and 3 years among youth who reported HE. Early developmental delays significantly increased the risk for later HE. This association was particularly marked in the recurrent HE group, with over 40% having early developmental delays in multiple domains. There was no significant association between early childhood development and later anxiety/depression apart from lower gross motor scores at age 3. Conclusions The findings suggest that early pan-developmental deficits are associated with later HE, with the effect strongest for young people who report recurrent HE throughout childhood and adolescence.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Psychological Medicine
cambridge.org/psm
Original Article
*Joint senior authors.
Cite this article: Carey E, Healy C, Perry Y,
Gillan D, Whitehouse AJO, Cannon M, Lin A
(2023). Evidence that infant and early
childhood developmental impairments are
associated with hallucinatory experiences:
results from a large, population-based cohort
study. Psychological Medicine 53, 21162124.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003883
Received: 28 April 2021
Revised: 3 September 2021
Accepted: 3 September 2021
First published online: 29 September 2021
Keywords:
Cognition; Early childhood development;
Hallucinatory experiences; Motor
Author for correspondence:
Eleanor Carey,
E-mail: eleanorcarey1@rcsi.com
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by
Cambridge University Press. This is an Open
Access article, distributed under the terms of
the Creative Commons Attribution licence
(https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/),
which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution
and reproduction, provided the original article
is properly cited.
Evidence that infant and early childhood
developmental impairments are associated
with hallucinatory experiences: results from a
large, population-based cohort study
Eleanor Carey1,2 , Colm Healy1, Yael Perry3, Diane Gillan4,
Andrew J. O. Whitehouse3, Mary Cannon1,2,5,*and Ashleigh Lin3, *
1
Department of Psychiatry, Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland, Dublin, Ireland;
2
Trinity College Institute of
Neuroscience, Dublin, Ireland;
3
Telethon Kids Institute, The University of Western Australia, Perth, WA, Australia;
4
Department of Psychology, Beaumont Hospital, Dublin, Ireland and
5
Department of Psychiatry, Beaumont
Hospital, Dublin, Ireland
Abstract
Background. Cognitive and motor dysfunction are hallmark features of the psychosis
continuum, and have been detected during late childhood and adolescence in youth who
report psychotic experiences (PE). However, previous investigations have not explored infancy
and early childhood development. It remains unclear whether such deficits emerge much
earlier in life, and whether they are associated with psychotic, specifically hallucinatory,
experiences (HE).
Methods. This study included data from Gen2 participants of The Raine Study (n= 1101), a
population-based longitudinal cohort study in Western Australia. Five areas of childhood
development comprising: communication; fine motor; gross motor; adaptive (problem-
solving); and personal-social skills, were assessed serially at ages 1, 2 and 3 years.
Information on HE, depression and anxiety at ages 10, 14 and 17 years was obtained.
HE were further subdivided into those with transient or recurrent experiences. Mixed
effects logistic regression models and cumulative risk analyses based on multiple domain
delays were performed.
Results. Early poorer development in multiple areas was noted from ages 1, 2 and 3 years
among youth who reported HE. Early developmental delays significantly increased the risk
for later HE. This association was particularly marked in the recurrent HE group, with
over 40% having early developmental delays in multiple domains. There was no significant
association between early childhood development and later anxiety/depression apart from
lower gross motor scores at age 3.
Conclusions. The findings suggest that early pan-developmental deficits are associated with
later HE, with the effect strongest for young people who report recurrent HE throughout
childhood and adolescence.
Introduction
Poor mental health is being reported at increasingly younger ages, and there has been much
focus traditionally on affective disorders and anxiety (Caspi et al., 2020; Merikangas et al.,
2010). However, there is growing recognition of the importance of psychotic phenomena in
youth. Psychotic experiences (PE), the subclinical expressions of psychotic symptoms, occur
in the absence of a psychotic disorder and mainly concern auditory (both verbal and non-
verbal) and, less commonly, visual hallucinatory experiences (HE) (Kelleher, Harley,
Murtagh, & Cannon, 2011). PE are most prevalent during late childhood, with 17% of 912
year olds within the general population reporting these phenomena (Kelleher et al., 2012a),
and a further 12% of children and adolescents reporting auditory hallucinations specifically
(Maijer, Begemann, Palmen, Leucht, & Sommer, 2018). A growing body of research suggests
that PE are not only homotypically or inevitably linked to psychotic disorders (Healy et al.,
2019), but also represent markers of vulnerability for a range of psychopathology and poorer
functional outcomes into adulthood (Carey et al., 2021). PE have also been shown to be asso-
ciated with suicidality and substance abuse in young people (Cederlöf et al., 2017; Honings,
Drukker, Groen, & van Os, 2016; Kelleher, Cederlöf, & Lichtenstein, 2014). For the majority
of young people (7590%), PE are transient in nature (Calkins et al., 2017), but, in those for
whom PE persist, greater risk of adverse outcomes have been shown (Calkins et al., 2017). The
recurrence of PE therefore may be indicative of a more serious underlying psychopathological
process.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003883 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Early childhood development, which includes physical, social
and cognitive development, strongly influences mental health
and well-being throughout the lifespan and is critical for influen-
cing a childs developmental trajectory (Irwin, Siddiqi, &
Hertzman, 2007). Early childhood represents a critical time for
the early detection of risks for later psychopathology (Green
et al., 2019). Cognitive variances in early life lead to a very high
risk of not only developmental issues for children, but also
social-emotional and behavioural problems at later ages (Carter,
Briggs-Gowan, & Davis, 2004), and should be signalled for clinical
intervention.
Investigations of early childhood development and its role in
predicting later schizophrenia have been carried out using birth
cohorts and data linkage approaches but less research has been
carried out with HE as the outcome. Motor deficits during devel-
opment likely represent an endophenotype for schizophrenia
(Burton et al., 2016). Clarke et al. (2011), in a study analysing
data from the Finnish population register, found that delayed
milestones in infancy significantly increased the risk of a later
diagnosis of schizophrenia, in a doseresponse manner. Cannon
et al. (2002), in analyses from a New Zealand birth cohort
study, found that subtle but significant impairments in neuromo-
tor, language and cognitive development were detectable from age
3 in children who reported psychotic symptoms at age 11 and
were also detectable in individuals later diagnosed with schizo-
phreniform disorder (but not depression, anxiety or mania) at
age 26. However, developmental data were not available before
the age of 3. Hameed et al. (2018) showed that a decline in
early social and communication skills over the first 4 years of
life, and a declining pattern of fine motor skills in males, was
predictive of later PE (at age 12 years) in a UK birth cohort.
Most studies in this area have focused on childhood and
adolescence, with research showing that cognitive and motor dys-
function at this age is also associated with PE (Carey et al., 2019,
2021; Mollon et al., 2016). A lag in cognitive development from
age 8 years has been detected in children who later report PE
(Gur et al., 2014). However, whether differences in developmental
trajectory can be noted from infancy remains unclear. Questions
pertaining to whether there is an association between early child-
hood developmental delays and poorer outcomes also remain.
Using data from Gen1 and Gen2 of the Raine study, our study
addressed the following aims:
(1) To determine if early childhood development in separate or
multiple domains of development in the first 3 years of life
is associated with HE in childhood and adolescence.
(2) To compare these associations with those of early childhood
development and anxiety/depression in childhood and
adolescence.
(3) To investigate whether the presence of early childhood devel-
opmental delays are associated with recurrence of HE in ado-
lescence, which is a marker for poorer outcomes.
Methods
Participants
Ethical approval for the study was granted by the University of
Western Australia Human Research Ethics Committee (HREC).
Data were taken from The Raine Study, a pregnancy cohort
study that began in 1989, in which 2900 pregnant women were
enrolled with an average recruitment age of 18 weeks of gestation,
from the public antenatal clinic at the major obstetric hospital in
Perth, Western Australia, and nearby private practices. The
ongoing study has followed 2446 infants born over a 3-year
period, with numerous follow-up assessments in childhood and
adolescence. Inclusion criteria for mothersparticipation in the
study included gestational age between 16 and 20 weeks, profi-
ciency in English and an intention to remain in Western
Australia so to allow for long-term participation. Demographic
and social information of parents/guardians was collected
throughout the study. The rate of attrition in the Raine study is
29% from age 1 year to age 17 years (Straker et al., 2017).
Participants are representative of the general Western Australian
population at the time of recruitment and into infancy (Straker
et al., 2017).
Measures
Early childhood development
The Infant Monitoring Questionnaire (Bricker, Squires,
Kaminski, & Mounts, 1988) (IMQ; now known as the Ages and
Stages Questionnaire) was completed by parents/guardians at
ages 1, 2 and 3 years. The IMQ is a screening tool for monitoring
early childhood development in five broad domains: communica-
tion, gross motor, fine motor, adaptive (problem-solving) and
personal-social. A 35-item version of the IMQ was administered
at ages 1 and 2 years, and a 30-item version was administered
at age 3 years. Upon agreement with the authors, age 1 and age
2 IMQ data were condensed to 30-items for standardisation
within the analyses, due to the inclusion of one item that is 150
developmental quotient in each domain. This item was therefore
removed from each domain at age 1 and age 2 years, and no sig-
nificant differences were noted. Parents were asked to assess their
childs behaviour by domain, and score yesif the child does
behave in such way, sometimesif the child occasionally behaves
in such way or noif the child does not behave in such way.
Higher scores indicate better development. The IMQ contain
age-specific developmental items. The IMQ has good criterion
validity, sensitivity, specificity and testretest reliability (Bricker
et al., 1988). The IMQ also has good criterion validity when com-
pared with the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, and the
StanfordBinet Intelligence Test (r= 0.89) (Bricker & Squires,
1989) and is endorsed for use by the American Academy of
Pediatrics (Schonhaut, Armijo, Schönstedt, Alvarez, & Cordero,
2013).
The IMQ was scored as per the instructions in the manual
(Squires, 1993) and the suggested cut-off scores were used to
define low-functioning infants and normal-functioning infants
in each domain (see online Supplementary Table S3). Scoring
below this cut-off was labelled as Delay, while scoring above
this cut-off was labelled as Average. A further measure of low
functioning was calculated based on scores within the lowest
10th percentile for each domain. The data were also dichotomised
and labelled as Low(10 percentile) and Average(11
percentile).
Childhood/adolescent outcomes: hallucinatory experiences and
anxiety/depression
The Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL)/Youth Self-Report (YSR)
(Achenbach, 1991; Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983) are question-
naires used to measure emotional and behavioural problems in
children and adolescents, and contain comparable items
(Achenbach, Dumenci, & Rescorla, 2002). They include eight
Psychological Medicine 2117
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003883 Published online by Cambridge University Press
scales: anxious/depressed, depressed, somatic complaints, social
problems, thought problems, attention problems, rule-breaking
behaviour and aggressive behaviour. For the purpose of the cur-
rent analyses, we use CBCL/YSR data collected at ages 10 years
(parent-report) and 14 and 17 (self-report) years.
HE were indexed by two items on the thought problemssub-
scale of the CBCL/YSR, I hear sounds or voices that other people
think arent there(item 40) and I see things that other people
think arent there(item 70). These items have been found to
have good predictive power for measuring PE (Kelleher et al.,
2011). HE were considered as the endorsement of somewhat/
sometimes trueor very true/often truefor either or both
items, at one or more of the three time-points. Subgroups of
those with transient experiences (reported at one time-point
only) and recurrent experiences (reported at more than one
time-point) were also created.
Anxious/depressed symptomology was determined using
standardised t-scores from the CBCL/YSR anxious/depressed
subscale. A cut-off point for t-scores over and including 70 was
applied, deemed to be clinically significant by Achenbach
(1991). Participants with a t-score of 70 at one or more of the
three time-points (ages 10, 14 and 17 years) and who had never
reported PE were assigned to the anxious/depressed group for
comparison.
Covariates
Covariates entered in the analyses included childs age (in
months) at the time of their developmental assessment; maternal
highest qualification, birthweight, sex at birth and family income
at the time of the developmental assessments. Maternal highest
qualification was measured when the participants were aged 8
years and was scaled from 0 to 8, and ranged from no qualifica-
tion to postgraduate diploma/higher degree. Family income was
measured at ages 1, 2 and 3 years and was based on five categories
(less than $7000, $700011 999, $12 00023 999, $24 00035 999
and more than $36 000).
Statistical analysis
Participants were divided into the HE group (those who reported
PE at any time-point) and the anxious/depressed group (those
who scored above the cut-off on the CBCL/YSR at any time-
point). Controls were defined as those who did not report HE
nor had anxious/depressed symptoms above the cut-off at any
time-point. The HE group was subdivided into those with transi-
ent HE (those who reported HE at one time-point only) and
recurrent HE (those who reported PE at either two or three
time-points).
The HE and anxious/depressed groups were compared to con-
trol participants on demographic characteristics using independ-
ent ttests and a chi-squared test. Raw IMQ scores were
converted to z-scores, in order to standardise each version admi-
nistered at ages 1, 2 and 3 years. The data were analysed using
complete case approaches.
Using Stata ver. 16 (StataCorp, 2009), we fitted a series of
mixed-effect logistic models with a random effect accounting
for the within-subject variance. We report the main effects of
early childhood development in the first 3 years of life and later
outcomes in childhood and/or adolescence. The exposure vari-
ables were the five subscales of the IMQ (communication, gross
motor, fine motor, adaptive and personal-social) at ages 1, 2
and 3 years and the primary outcome variables were HE and
anxious/depressed group membership. This approach allowed
for the identification of associations between developmental
scores and HE or anxious/depressed symptoms by detecting
unique variances within the scores after accounting for within-
and between-subject variance. Each of the aforementioned covari-
ates was adjusted for throughout all of the analyses.
Developmental time-specific investigations were then con-
ducted using separate logistic regression models (at ages 1, 2
and 3 years, respectively) to investigate the association between
early childhood development at specific ages and later HE or anx-
ious/depressed symptoms.
Cumulative risk based on multiple domain delays was also
calculated. Cut-off scores for the IMQ were applied at each year
(ages 1, 2 and 3 years). Three categories were created, 0 domain
delays (reference group), 1 or 2 domain delays and 35 (multiple)
domain delays, where the outcome variables were HE and anx-
ious/depressed. Cumulative risk at age 3 years based on scores
within the lowest 10th percentile on each domain was also
analysed.
All analyses were also performed to determine associations
between the early childhood developmental scores and subgroups
of HE, transient and recurrent.
Results
Of the 2446 participants who initially took part, data from
n= 1101 participants were available for the present analysis. An
attrition analysis is presented in online Supplementary Table S1.
Those who were included in the present analysis were younger,
had a higher birthweight and better overall development at age
2 years, as well as having mothers with higher qualifications,
than those who were not included.
Hallucinatory experiences
The prevalence of HE was 11.1% in the overall cohort. Among the
participants in the HE group (n= 228), 48% reported at age 17;
38% reported at age 14 and 14% at age 10 (parent-report). At
age 10 years, auditory HE were reported at higher rates (83%)
while at 14 years, visual HE were more prevalent (84%). At age
17 years, rates of auditory and visual HE were comparable (50%
respectively), and 48% of those who reported HE at this age
reported both. Transient experiences were reported in 82% of
cases, while recurrent experiences were reported in 18% of
cases. A total of n= 40 participants reported HE at two time-
points, and a further n= 2 participants reported HE at three
time-points.
Anxious/depressed
The prevalence of anxious/depressed symptoms at or above cut-
off was 5.6% in the overall cohort. A total of n=114 participants
scored on or above cut-off at least once at ages 10, 14 or 17 years.
Seven (6%) had symptoms at more than one time-point. A total of
32% had symptoms at age 17; 18% had symptoms at age 14 and
32% had symptoms at age 10. Of note, n=43 (37.7%) of the anx-
ious/depressed group also reported PE, and were therefore only
included in the HE group.
No significant group differences in demographic characteris-
tics were found between the HE and control groups, with
the exception of maternal highest level of education (t=3.80,
p< 0.01) and family income at age 1 (t=3.76, p< 0.01), where
2118 Eleanor Carey et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003883 Published online by Cambridge University Press
less maternal education and a lower family income at age 1 were
noted for the HE group. No significant group differences were
found between the anxious/depressed and control groups (see
Table 1).
Association between early childhood development and PE
Mean scores for the IMQ subscales at each age (1, 2 and 3 years)
for the HE, anxious/depressed and control groups, including the
subgroups of the HE group (transient and recurrent HE), are pre-
sented in online Supplementary Table S2.
In the overall random effects model, which accounted for
development over the first 3 years of age, there was no significant
association between any of developmental domains and HE. In
the age-specific analysis, lower adaptive scores at age 1 year
were associated with HE. At age 2 years, lower scores in 4 of
the 5 developmental domains (with the exception of gross
motor) were associated with HE. At age 3 years, lower scores in
any of the five domains (communication, gross motor, fine
motor, adaptive and personal-social) were associated with HE
(see Table 2).
These results are presented, stratified by gender, in Tables S4
and S5 of the online Supplementary materials.
Association between early childhood development and
anxious/depressed symptoms
No significant associations between any of developmental
domains and anxious/depressed symptoms were found. In the
age-specific analysis, only lower gross motor scores at age 3
years were associated with anxious/depressed symptoms
(see Table 2).
Association between early childhood development and
transient and recurrent HE
No significant associations between any of developmental
domains and transient HE were found. In the age-specific ana-
lysis, lower adaptive scores at ages 1, 2 and 3 years, as well as
lower fine motor scores at age 3 years, were significantly asso-
ciated with transient HE.
Table 1. Demographic characteristics for the HE group and anxious/depressed groups when compared to the control group
Descriptive statistics Analysis (reference: control group)
HE group
Mean (S.D.)
Anxious/depressed
group
Mean (S.D.)
Control group
Mean (S.D.)
HE group
χ
2
or t p-value
Anxious/depressed
group
χ
2
or t p-value
Demographic variable
Sex at birth, n(%) 1.03 0.31 1.66 0.20
Males 119 (52%) 40 (56%) 388 (48%)
Females 109 (49%) 31 (44%) 414 (52%)
Ethnicity
a
Caucasian (European descent) 203 (91%) 69 (99%) 703 (90%)
Aboriginal 1 0 5
Polynesian 0 0 8
Vietnamese 0 0 4
Chinese 13 0 37
Indian 6 1 20
Other 0 0 7
Birthweight (in g) 3281.7 (671.8) 3281.7 (626.2) 3341.3 (560.1) 1.35 0.18 0.09 0.93
Age 1 (months) 1.50 (0.11) 1.14 (0.11) 1.14 (0.10) 0.70 0.48 0.21 0.84
Age 2 2.13 (0.15) 2.16 (0.18) 2.13 (0.14) 0.07 0.94 1.46 0.14
Age 3 3.15 (0.16) 3.16 (0.18) 3.12 (0.12) 3.13 0.002 2.94 0.003
Age 10 10.59 (0.20) 10.62 (0.17) 10.57 (0.17) 1.39 0.17 2.14 0.03
Age 14 13.99 (0.21) 14.02 (0.23) 13.99 (0.19) 0.70 0.48 1.26 0.21
Age 17 17.07 (0.27) 17.03 (0.29) 17.02 (0.22) 2.80 0.005 0.39 0.70
Maternal highest qualification 2.42 (2.49) 2.60 (2.35) 3.14 (2.30) 3.80 <0.001 1.84 0.07
Family income age 1 2.77 (1.20) 2.78 (1.40) 3.11 (1.24) 3.76 <0.001 1.90 0.06
Family income age 2 2.83 (1.70) 3.03 (2.11) 3.05 (1.41) 1.61 0.11 0.07 0.94
Family income age 3 2.95 (1.63) 2.80 (1.61) 3.10 (1.32) 1.23 0.22 1.74 0.08
S.D., standard deviation.
a
Some missing data.
Psychological Medicine 2119
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003883 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Lower communication scores in the first 3 years were asso-
ciated with recurrent HE. In the age-specific analysis, lower
gross motor and fine motor scores at age 1 year were significantly
associated with recurrent HE. At age 2 years, lower scores in 4 of
the 5 developmental domains (with the exception of gross motor)
were associated with recurrent HE. At age 3 years, lower scores in
4 of the 5 early childhood development domains (communica-
tion, gross motor, adaptive and personal-social) were significantly
associated with recurrent HE. All data are presented in Fig. 1.
Cumulative risk based on multiple domain delays
Based on cut-off scores, having 35 developmental domain delays
at age 2 led to an almost 5-fold increased risk for HE (adjusted
odds ratio 4.86, 95% confidence interval 1.6914.05, p= 0.004).
Having 12 developmental domain delays at age 3 led to an
almost 2-fold increased risk for HE and having multiple domain
delays increased the risk of HE over 3-fold (see Table 3). Among
young people who reported recurrent HE, 30.9% had develop-
mental domain delays at age 2 years and 40.5% had developmen-
tal domain delays at age 3 years.
In the percentile analysis, delays (lowest 10th percenile for all
participants) in 12 domains led to over a 2-fold increase risk for
HE, while delays in multiple domains led to over a 3-fold
increased risk for HE (see Table 4). For the HE subgroups, delays
(by percentile) in 12 developmental domains led to over a 2-fold
increased risk for transient HE. Delays in 35 developmental
domains led to over a 7-fold increased risk for recurrent HE.
Multiple developmental domain delays did not lead to a
significant increase in cumulative risk for anxious/depressed
symptoms, in either the cut-off or percentile analysis.
Discussion
In this study, we examined longitudinally the association between
early childhood development and HE in childhood and adoles-
cence in a population-based, representative sample of young peo-
ple. Participants who reported HE in childhood and/or
adolescence showed lower early developmental scores from ages
1 and 2 years on a range of measures. By age 3 years, lower scores
in any of the five developmental domains were significantly asso-
ciated with the risk of later reporting HE. Cumulative risk based
on multiple (35) domain delays at age 3 years led to over a 3-fold
increased risk for HE. For those participants who would later
report clinically significant anxious/depressed symptomology,
no association was found for overall development over the first
3 years, with the exception of gross motor scores at age 3, and
there was also no increased cumulative risk based on multiple
domains delays.
Further analyses within the HE group showed that when sub-
dividing the group by transient and recurrent experiences, lower
adaptive scores at ages 1, 2 and 3 years were specifically associated
Table 2. Overall and age-specific developmental scores between ages 1 and 3 and risk of later HE (n= 228) [also subdivided into transient (n= 114) or recurrent
(n= 42)] and anxiety/depression (n= 71) (compared with Controls (n= 802) (reference group is controls)
Communication Gross motor Fine motor Adaptive Personal-social
OR (CI) OR (CI) OR (CI) OR (CI) OR (CI)
HE group
Mixed effect model 1.04 (0.921.19) 1.02 (0.901.07) 0.99 (0.841.16) 1.05 (0.921.05) 1.01 (0.871.18)
Age 1 year 1.14 (0.961.35) 1.09 (0.931.27) 1.08 (0.931.28) 1.27 (1.081.49) 1.10 (0.941.30)
Age 2 years 1.39 (1.181.64) 1.16 (0.991.39) 1.23 (1.031.47) 1.39 (1.181.66) 1.20 (1.011.45)
Age 3 years 1.32 (1.121.54) 1.25 (1.061.47) 1.32 (1.141.54) 1.35 (1.161.56) 1.20 (1.021.43)
Anxious/depressed group
Mixed effect model 0.98 (0.631.54) 1.19 (0.781.79) 1.12 (0.721.75) 1.17 (0.761.82) 1.19 (0.761.85)
Age 1 year 1.02 (0.771.35) 0.91 (0.681.22) 1.01 (0.761.33) 1.01 (0.761.35) 1.20 (0.931.56)
Age 2 years 0.92 (0.651.28) 1.20 (0.931.54) 1.20 (0.921.58) 1.30 (1.001.72) 1.08 (0.791.47)
Age 3 years 1.05 (0.791.39) 1.39 (1.111.75) 1.20 (0.931.54) 1.14 (0.881.47) 1.19 (0.901.56)
Transient HE group
Mixed effect model 1.03 (0.601.78) 1.05 (0.631.75) 1.02 (0.621.66) 1.28 (0.851.92) 1.08 (0.721.59)
Age 1 year 1.05 (0.841.32) 0.94 (0.761.19) 0.89 (0.701.12) 1.33 (1.081.64) 1.00 (0.801.23)
Age 2 years 1.12 (0.881.43) 1.06 (0.851.33) 0.97 (0.761.25) 1.28 (1.021.61) 1.05 (0.821.35)
Age 3 years 1.14 (0.931.41) 1.16 (0.931.45) 1.33 (1.001.61) 1.27 (1.041.54) 1.15 (0.921.43)
Recurrent HE group
Mixed effect model 2.04 (1.143.70) 1.72 (0.793.85) 1.72 (0.963.03) 1.37 (0.782.43) 1.47 (0.812.70)
Age 1 year 1.39 (0.991.92) 1.37 (1.061.75) 1.43 (1.091.89) 1.19 (0.861.63) 1.30 (0.951.78)
Age 2 years 2.17 (1.543.03) 1.28 (0.901.82) 2.04 (1.432.94) 1.44 (1.012.08) 1.56 (1.062.27)
Age 3 years 1.85 (1.452.38) 1.64 (1.272.17) 1.82 (0.981.82) 1.41 (1.061.92) 1.59 (1.142.17)
OR, odds ratio; CI, confidence intervals.
Bolded text represents analyses where 95% CI do not cross 1.
Odds ratios are adjusted for sex at birth, age, birthweight, maternal highest qualification and family income at each year.
2120 Eleanor Carey et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003883 Published online by Cambridge University Press
with transient HE. Delays in 1 or 2 developmental domains at age
3 years led to over a 2-fold increased risk for transient HE.
Stronger effects were found for those who reported recurrent
HE, in that a greater number of associations were found with
early developmental scores compared to those who only reported
HE at one time-point in childhood or adolescence. Both at ages 2
and 3 years, lower scores in 4 of the 5 developmental domains
were associated with recurrent HE. At age 3 years, over 40% of
the recurrent HE group had at least one developmental delay, as
measured by cut-off scores. Multiple (35) domain delays led to
over a 7-fold increased risk. These findings convey the strong
associations between poorer early childhood development and
mental health outcomes later in life. Our findings indicate a spe-
cific risk associated with the psychosis spectrum, and a strong
association with recurrent HE throughout childhood and adoles-
cent development.
Fig. 1. Longitudinal unadjusted mean z-scores with standard deviation bars for each developmental domain (ages 1, 2 and 3 years) for all groups.
Table 3. Cumulative risk for later HE (n= 228) [Transient (n= 114) or recurrent (n= 42)] and anxiety/depression (n= 71) based on number of developmental delays
(below cut-off) at ages 1, 2 and 3 years for each group
Cumulative risk
(0 delays reference category)
12 delays
N(% below cut-off) OR (CI) p-value
35 delays
N(% below cut-off) OR (CI) p-value
Age 1
HE group 41 (18) 1.70 (1.102.62) 0.02 4 (1.8) 0.75 (0.163.52) 0.72
Anxious/depressed group 8 (11.3) 0.85 (0.371.95) 0.70 0 ––
Transient HE group
a
17 (14.9) 1.46 (0.822.61) 0.20 1 (0.9) 0.75 (0.095.99) 0.78
Recurrent HE group
a
9 (21.4) 1.88 (0.834.23) 0.13 0 ––
Controls 86 (10.7) 10 (1.2)
Age 2
HE group 50 (21.9) 1.81 (1.202.72) 0.004 8 (3.5) 4.86 (1.6914.05) 0.004
Anxious/depressed group 10 (14.1) 1.00 (0.492.08) 0.99 1 (1.4) 1.66 (0.1914.08) 0.64
Transient HE group
a
20 (17.5) 1.24 (0.722.13) 0.45 1 (0.9) 0.95 (0.118.02) 0.96
Recurrent HE group
a
10 (23.8) 3.49 (1.468.33) 0.005 3 (7.1) 21.11 (4.22105.91) <0.001
Controls 118 (14.7) 7 (0.9)
Age 3
HE group 60 (26.3) 1.80 (1.242.62) 0.002 17 (7.5) 3.60 (1.707.23) 0.001
Anxious/depressed group 18 (25.4) 1.53 (0.842.78) 0.17 3 (4.2) 1.31 (0.295.97) 0.72
Transient HE group
a
38 (33.3) 2.17 (1.383.42) 0.001 3 (2.6) 1.33 (0.374.70) 0.66
Recurrent HE group
a
10 (23.8) 1.45 (0.643.31) 0.37 7 (16.7) 7.68 (2.7321.58) <0.001
Controls 154 (19.2) 19 (2.4)
a
Subgroups of the HE group.
Psychological Medicine 2121
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003883 Published online by Cambridge University Press
The present study has extended previous research on early
childhood development and PE. Using the Denver Developmental
Screening Test (which includes four measures of early develop-
ment: fine and gross motor, social and communication skills),
Hameed et al. (2018) showed that declining communication
and social skills in the first 4 years of life were significantly asso-
ciated with PE at one time-point at age 12 years. Our study has
extended these findings by measuring adaptive (problem-solving)
skills, and by examining HE over three time-points in childhood
and adolescence. In addition, we explored the comparison
between the associations between poorer early development and
later HE and clinically significant anxious/depressed symptomol-
ogy as outcomes. Lower scores in gross motor development at age
3 years were associated with later clinically significant anxious/
depressed symptomology. Although the co-occurrence of PE
and anxiety/depression has been established (Wigman et al.,
2011), our findings suggest that poorer development is driven
by variability associated with psychosis spectrum symptomology
rather than psychopathology more generally, a pattern that has
also been shown in other studies (Cannon et al., 2002).
Our findings pertaining to early infant communication devel-
opment and its relationship with HE, in particular recurrent HE,
are in-line with those of Hameed et al. (2018). Impairments in
language abilities and the dysconnectivity of language networks
have been a central topic in the research of cognitive dysfunction
in psychosis, and are thought to contribute to the development of
auditory hallucinations (Benetti et al., 2015). Altered white matter
in language areas has also been seen in young people with PE
(Dooley et al., 2020). Our results suggest that language and com-
munication deficits may be detectable as early as the first year of
life in young people at risk for psychosis.
Poorer early adaptive development or problem-solving skills
were also associated with HE, and in particular, transient HE.
The transient HE group were characterised by persistently lower
scores in adaptive skills throughout the first 3 years. Reasoning,
control and planning are core to adaptive development and delays
in these areas have been described in the later stages of childhood
in individuals who go on to develop schizophrenia (Reichenberg
et al., 2009). Transient HE, or once-off PE, are linked to poorer
global functioning throughout adolescence and early adulthood
(Healy et al., 2018).
The recurrent HE group had poorer developmental scores in
both cognitive and motor domains throughout the first 3 years
of life. Motor dysfunction may be reflective of a core feature of
psychosis and schizophrenia. Psychomotor developmental devia-
tions are more marked in those later diagnosed with schizophre-
nia, when compared those later diagnosed with bipolar disorder,
and have an earlier onset (Parellada, Gomez-Vallejo, Burdeus, &
Arango, 2017). Keskinen et al. (2015) hypothesised that delayed
motor development may be a marker of other risk processes
that interact with genetic liability for psychosis and schizophrenia.
Murray et al. (2006) retrospectively found that individuals with
schizophrenia had early delays in neuromotor milestones.
Motor development therefore may be an important trait marker
for recurrent HE, which is of important clinical utility, as young
people with persistent symptoms have poorer functional out-
comes (Healy et al., 2018), higher levels of stress reactivity
(Collip et al., 2013) and are at greater risk of conversion to psych-
osis (Dominguez, Wichers, Lieb, Wittchen, & van Os, 2011).
Recurrence is thought to be indicative of a more serious under-
lying psychopathological process, and represents an important,
early, non-specific marker of later mental health problems
(Kelleher et al., 2012b).
Strengths of the study include the availability of very early
developmental data from infancy from a population-based longi-
tudinal birth cohort and the use of a robust and widely-used
assessment of early childhood development, as well as the con-
sistency of measures used across time-points. The HE group
also included those with clinically significant anxious/depressed
symptomology, due to the established co-occurrence (Kounali
et al., 2014) and known overlap with psychopathology more gen-
erally (Trotta et al., 2020). Kounali and colleagues also found that
markers of abnormal neurodevelopment showed stronger associ-
ation with PE, when compared to depression. However, limita-
tions of the study should also be noted. The measurement of
HE was confined to the self-report of two types of symptomology
(namely, auditory and visual HE); however, these are the most
common and validated in childhood community samples
(Laurens, Hobbs, Sunderland, Green, & Mould, 2012). HE were
indexed by responses somewhat/sometimes trueor very true/
often truewhich may have been inflated prevalence rates.
Furthermore, at age 10 years, a parent-report of HE was used
which may have led to an underestimation of HE. The prevalence
of clinically significant anxious/depressed symptoms might also
be underestimated, particularly as a high proportion who also
reported HE were counted within the HE group rather than the
Anxious/depressed group as this was the main focus of our ana-
lysis. Within the analyses, the omission of parental psychopath-
ology as a covariate must also be noted. This was not
consistently measured throughout The Raine Study.
Furthermore, the impact of multiple comparisons should be con-
sidered, particularly for the results in the anxious/depressed
group. Finally, attrition rates within The Raine Study may have
led to follow-up bias; however, this may have led to underestima-
tion rather than over estimation of effect sizes since those who
were lost to follow-up had poorer development at age 2 years.
Table 4. Cumulative risk for later HE (transient or recurrent) and anxious/depressed based on number of domain delays at age 3 years for each group
Cumulative risk (0 delays reference category)
12 domains
OR (CI) p-value
35 domains
OR (CI) p-value
HE group 2.21 (1.543.18) <0.001 3.60 (1.826.99) <0.001
Anxious/depressed group 1.57 (0.872.81) 0.13 1.07 (0.244.78) 0.93
Transient HE group
a
2.31 (1.483.60) <0.001 1.54 (0.514.67) 0.16
Recurrent HE group
a
1.98 (0.924.25) 0.08 7.23 (2.6219.98) <0.001
OR, odds ratio; CI, confidence intervals.
Statistically significant results ( p< 0.05) are presented in bold text.
a
Subgroups of the HE group.
2122 Eleanor Carey et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003883 Published online by Cambridge University Press
In conclusion, our study has shown that cognitive and motor
developmental delays can be detected from infancy in young
people who go on to report HE in childhood and adolescence.
Our findings are reminiscent of the early insulttheory in psych-
osis, where early genetic or environmental insults lead to aberrant
brain development (Murray & Lewis, 1987) which is exacerbated
thereafter by childhood trauma and adversity (Croft et al., 2019).
Exploration of early childhood development as both a meaningful
marker for later mental health outcomes and a viable critical per-
iod for early intervention is an exciting emerging area in psychosis
research.
Supplementary material. The supplementary material for this article can
be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003883.
Acknowledgements. We are grateful to the Raine study participants and their
families and we thank the Raine study staff for cohort coordination and data col-
lection. The core management of the Raine study is funded by The University of
Western Australia, Curtin University, Telethon Kids Institute, Women and
Infants Research Foundation, Edith Cowan University, Murdoch University,
The University of Notre Dame Australia and the Raine Medical Research
Foundation. The study is funded by the National Health and Medical
Research Council (NHMRC).
Author contributions. Each author contributed significantly to the
manuscript. E. Carey (corresponding author) had full access to the data and
had complete freedom to direct its analysis and reporting. C. Healy, A. Lin
and M. Cannon were involved in the conception and design of the study, as
well as the analysis of the data and interpretation of the results. Y. Perry,
D. Gillan and A. Whitehouse contributed to the preparing and editing of previ-
ous drafts of the manuscript and the critical revision of important intellectual
content. All authors gave their approval of the present version to be published.
Financial support. EC, CH and MC are funded by a European Research
Council Consolidator Award to MC (724809 iHEAR). AL is funded by a
National Health and Medical Research Council Career Development
Fellowship (no. 1148793). YP is funded by a fellowship from the Giorgetta
Charity Fund. AW is supported by an Investigator Grant from the National
Health and Medical Research Council (no. 1173896).
Conflict of interest. None.
Role of the funder or sponsor. The funders had no role in the design and
conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis and interpretation of
the data. The Raine study approved final manuscript and agreed upon its sub-
mission for publication.
References
Achenbach, T.M. (1991). Manual for the youth self-report and 1991 profile (pp.
113). Burlington: Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont.
Achenbach, T. M., Dumenci, L., & Rescorla, L. A. (2002). Ten-year comparisons
of problems and competencies for national samples of youth: Self, parent, and
teacher reports. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders,10(4), 194203.
Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. S. (1983). Manual for the child behavior
checklist and revised child behavior profile.
Benetti, S., Pettersson-Yeo, W., Allen, P., Catani, M., Williams, S., Barsaglini,
A., Mechelli, A. (2015). Auditory verbal hallucinations and brain dyscon-
nectivity in the perisylvian language network: A multimodal investigation.
Schizophrenia Bulletin,41(1), 192200.
Bricker, D., & Squires, J. (1989). The effectiveness of parental screening of
at-risk infants: The infant monitoring questionnaires. Topics in Early
Childhood Special Education,9(3), 6785.
Bricker, D, Squires, J, Kaminski, R, & Mounts, L. (1988). The validity, reliabil-
ity, and cost of a parent-completed questionnaire system to evaluate at-risk
infants. Journal of pediatric psychology,13(1), 5568.
Burton, B. K., Hjorthøj, C., Jepsen, J. R., Thorup, A., Nordentoft, M., &
Plessen, K. J. (2016). Research review: Do motor deficits during
development represent an endophenotype for schizophrenia? A
meta-analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,57(4), 446456.
Calkins, M. E., Moore, T. M., Satterthwaite, T. D., Wolf, D. H., Turetsky, B. I.,
Roalf, D. R., Gur, R. C. (2017). Persistence of psychosis spectrum
symptoms in the Philadelphia neurodevelopmental cohort: A prospective
two-year follow-up. World Psychiatry,16(1), 6276.
Cannon, M., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Harrington, H., Taylor, A., Murray, R.
M., & Poulton, R. (2002). Evidence for early-childhood, pan-developmental
impairment specific to schizophreniform disorder: Results from a longitu-
dinal birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry,59(5), 449456.
Carey, E., Dooley, N., Gillan, D., Healy, C., Coughlan, H., Clarke, M., Cannon,
M. (2019). Fine motor skill and processing speed deficits in young people
with psychotic experiences: A longitudinal study. Schizophrenia Research,
204, 127132.
Carey, E., Gillan, D., Healy, C., Dooley, N., Campbell, D., McGrane, J.,
Cannon, M. (2021). Early adult mental health, functional and neuropsycho-
logical outcomes of young people who have reported psychotic experiences:
A 10-year longitudinal study. Psychological Medicine,51(11), 18611869.
Carter, A. S., Briggs-Gowan, M. J., & Davis, N. O. (2004). Assessment of young
childrens social-emotional development and psychopathology: Recent
advances and recommendations for practice. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry,45(1), 109134.
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Ambler, A., Danese, A., Elliott, M. L., Hariri, A.,
Ramrakha, S. (2020). Longitudinal assessment of mental health disorders
and comorbidities across 4 decades among participants in the Dunedin
birth cohort study. JAMA Network Open,3(4), e203221e203221.
Cederlöf, M., Kuja-Halkola, R., Larsson, H., Sjölander, A., Östberg, P.,
Lundström, S., Lichtenstein, P. (2017). A longitudinal study of adolescent
psychotic experiences and later development of substance use disorder and
suicidal behavior. Schizophrenia Research,181,1316.
Clarke, M. C., Tanskanen, A., Huttunen, M., Leon, D. A., Murray, R. M., Jones,
P. B., & Cannon, M. (2011). Increased risk of schizophrenia from additive
interaction between infant motor developmental delay and obstetric com-
plications: Evidence from a population-based longitudinal study.
American Journal of Psychiatry,168(12), 12951302.
Collip, D., Wigman, J. T., Myin-Germeys, I., Jacobs, N., Derom, C., Thiery, E.,
van Os, J. (2013). From epidemiology to daily life: Linking daily life
stress reactivity to persistence of psychotic experiences in a longitudinal
general population study. PLoS ONE,8(4), e62688.
Croft, J., Heron, J., Teufel, C., Cannon, M., Wolke, D., Thompson, A.,
Zammit, S. (2019). Association of trauma type, age of exposure, and fre-
quency in childhood and adolescence with psychotic experiences in early
adulthood. JAMA Psychiatry,76(1), 7986.
Dominguez, M., Wichers, M., Lieb, R., Wittchen, H.-U., & van Os, J. (2011).
Evidence that onset of clinical psychosis is an outcome of progressively
more persistent subclinical psychotic experiences: An 8-year cohort study.
Schizophrenia Bulletin,37(1), 8493.
Dooley, N., OHanlon, E., Healy, C., Adair, A., McCandless, C., Coppinger, D.,
Frodl, T. (2020). Psychotic experiences in childhood are associated with
increased structural integrity of the left arcuate fasciculus A population-
based case-control study. Schizophrenia Research,215, 378384.
Green, M. J., Tzoumakis, S., Laurens, K. R., Dean, K., Kariuki, M., Harris, F.,
Carr, V. J. (2019). Early developmental risk for subsequent childhood
mental disorders in an Australian population cohort. Australian & New
Zealand Journal of Psychiatry,53(4), 304315.
Gur, R. C., Calkins, M. E., Satterthwaite, T. D., Ruparel, K., Bilker, W. B.,
Moore, T. M., Gur, R. E. (2014). Neurocognitive growth charting in
psychosis spectrum youths. JAMA Psychiatry,71(4), 366374.
Hameed, M. A., Lingam, R., Zammit, S., Salvi, G., Sullivan, S., & Lewis, A. J.
(2018). Trajectories of early childhood developmental skills and early ado-
lescent psychotic experiences: Findings from the ALSPAC UK birth cohort.
Frontiers in Psychology,8, 2314.
Healy,C.,Brannigan,R.,Dooley,N.,Coughlan,H.,Clarke,M.,Kelleher,I.,
Cannon, M. (2019). Childhood and adolescent psychotic experiences and
risk of mental disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological
Medicine,49(10), 15891599.
Healy, C., Campbell, D., Coughlan, H., Clarke, M., Kelleher, I., & Cannon, M.
(2018). Childhood psychotic experiences are associated with poorer global
Psychological Medicine 2123
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003883 Published online by Cambridge University Press
functioning throughout adolescence and into early adulthood. Acta
Psychiatrica Scandinavica,138(1), 2634.
Honings, S., Drukker, M., Groen, R., & van Os, J. (2016). Psychotic experiences
and risk of self-injurious behaviour in the general population: A systematic
review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine,46(2), 237251.
Irwin,L.G.,Siddiqi,A.,&Hertzman,G.(2007).Early child development: A power-
ful equalizer. Human Early Learning Partnership (HELP) Vancouver, BC.
Kelleher, I., Cederlöf, M., & Lichtenstein, P. (2014). Psychotic experiences as a
predictor of the natural course of suicidal ideation: A Swedish cohort study.
World Psychiatry,13(2), 184188.
Kelleher, I., Connor, D., Clarke, M. C., Devlin, N., Harley, M., & Cannon, M.
(2012a). Prevalence of psychotic symptoms in childhood and adolescence:
A systematic review and meta-analysis of population-based studies.
Psychological Medicine,42(9), 18571863.
Kelleher, I., Harley, M., Murtagh, A., & Cannon, M. (2011). Are screening
instruments valid for psychotic-like experiences? A validation study of
screening questions for psychotic-like experiences using in-depth clinical
interview. Schizophrenia Bulletin,37(2), 362369.
Kelleher, I., Keeley, H., Corcoran, P., Lynch, F., Fitzpatrick, C., Devlin, N.,
Harley, M. (2012b). Clinicopathological significance of psychotic experi-
ences in non-psychotic young people: Evidence from four population-based
studies. The British Journal of Psychiatry,201(1), 2632.
Keskinen, E., Marttila, A., Marttila, R., Jones, P., Murray, G., Moilanen, K.,
Jääskeläinen, E. (2015). Interaction between parental psychosis and early
motor development and the risk of schizophrenia in a general population
birth cohort. European Psychiatry,30(6), 719727.
Kounali, D., Zammit, S., Wiles, N., Sullivan, S., Cannon, M., Stochl, J., Heron,
J. (2014). Common versus psychopathology-specific risk factors for psychotic
experiences and depression during adolescence. Psychological Medicine,44
(12), 25572566.
Laurens,K.R.,Hobbs,M.,Sunderland,M.,Green,M.J.,&Mould,G.(2012).
Psychotic-like experiences in a community sample of 8000 children aged 9
to 11 years: An item response theory analysis. Psychological Medicine,42(7),
14951506.
Maijer, K., Begemann, M. J., Palmen, S. J., Leucht, S., & Sommer, I. E. (2018).
Auditory hallucinations across the lifespan: A systematic review and
meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine,48(6), 879888.
Merikangas, K. R., He, J.-P., Burstein, M., Swanson, S. A., Avenevoli, S., Cui, L.,
Swendsen, J. (2010). Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in US ado-
lescents: Results from the national comorbidity survey replicationadoles-
cent supplement (NCS-A). Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Adolescent Psychiatry,49(10), 980989.
Mollon,J.,David,A.S.,Morgan,C.,Frissa,S., Glahn,D.,Pilecka,I.,
Reichenberg, A. (2016). Psychotic experiences and neuropsychological
functioning in a population-based sample. JAMA Psychiatry,73(2), 129138.
Murray, G., Jones, P., Moilanen, K., Veijola, J., Miettunen, J., Cannon, T., &
Isohanni, M. (2006). Infant motor development and adult cognitive func-
tions in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research,81(1), 6574.
Murray, R. M., & Lewis, S. W. (1987). Is schizophrenia a neurodevelopmental
disorder? British Medical Journal (Clinical Research Ed.),295(6600), 681.
Parellada, M., Gomez-Vallejo, S., Burdeus, M., & Arango, C. (2017).
Developmental differences between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.
Schizophrenia Bulletin,43(6), 11761189.
Reichenberg, A., Caspi, A., Harrington, H., Houts, R., Keefe, R. S., Murray, R.
M., Moffitt, T. E. (2009). Static and dynamic cognitive deficits in child-
hood preceding adult schizophrenia: A 30-year study. American Journal of
Psychiatry,167(2), 160169.
Schonhaut, L., Armijo, I., Schönstedt, M., Alvarez, J., & Cordero, M. (2013).
Validity of the ages and stages questionnaires in term and preterm infants.
Pediatrics,131(5), e1468e1474.
Squires, J. (1993). Infant/child monitoring questionnaires procedures manual.
Oregon: University of Oregon Center of Human Development.
Straker, L., Mountain, J., Jacques, A., White, S., Smith, A., Landau, L.,
Eastwood, P. (2017). Cohort profile: The western Australian pregnancy
cohort (Raine) studygeneration 2. International Journal of Epidemiology,
46(5), 13841385j.
Trotta, A., Arseneault, L., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Danese, A., Pariante, C.,
Fisher, H. L. (2020). Mental health and functional outcomes in young adult-
hood of children with psychotic symptoms: A longitudinal cohort study.
Schizophrenia Bulletin,46(2), 261271.
Wigman, J., Lin, A., Vollebergh, W. A., van Os, J., Raaijmakers, Q. A., Nelson,
B., Yung, A. (2011). Subclinical psychosis and depression: Co-occurring
phenomena that do not predict each other over time. Schizophrenia
Research,130(13), 277281.
2124 Eleanor Carey et al.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003883 Published online by Cambridge University Press
... Indeed, beyond conferring a high risk of developing later psychotic disorders, PEs have been found to be implicated in other mental disorders onset (Healy et al., 2019). In this regard, research has demonstrated that PEs are associated with a broad range of subsequent psychopathology and behavioral problems , including depression, anxiety , suicidal behavior, higher rates of substance disorders (Cederlöf et al., 2017), and poorer global functioning (Carey et al., 2021;Healy et al., 2018), as well as internalizing and externalizing behaviors during the childhood and adolescence (Gin et al., 2021). Therefore, an in-depth examination of the behavioral problems associated with the development and persistence of PEs may expand our knowledge of the mechanisms underlying increased vulnerability to psychosis and have potential clinical implications for prevention and early intervention of psychotic disorders. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Although internalized (i.e., anger, hostility) and externalized (i.e., physical/verbal aggression, bullying) aggressive behaviors have been found to predict later psychotic experiences (PEs) onset in vulnerable young people, the mediating pathways are unclear. We aimed to test the hypothesis that insomnia has a significant indirect mediating effect on the relationship between aggression variables and PEs. Method: We performed a cross-sectional study among Lebanese young adults from the general population (N=4158, mean age of 21.91 ± 3.79, 64.4% females). Results: A total of 22.1% of participants reported at least one positive PE ‘nearly always’. After adjusting for demographics (age and gender), substance use, and psychological distress, mediation analyses showed that insomnia severity mediated the association between the four aggression variables and positive PEs. Higher aggression, anger, hostility and bullying were significantly associated with more insomnia severity and directly associated with increased positive PEs. Finally, higher insomnia severity was significantly associated with more frequent positive PEs. Conclusion: Improving insomnia severity may decrease the effect of aggression on PEs. This finding is clinically relevant, especially since insomnia is a modifiable factor which could easily and efficiently be targeted for interventions aimed at preventing or delaying psychosis in vulnerable populations.
... infrequent, PEs may be potentially distressing and disruptive, interfering with daily activity and functioning (Carey et al., 2021;Healy et al., 2018). PEs have also been shown to contribute to elevated healthcare costs and increased use of mental health services (Bhavsar et al., 2018;Rimvall et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: The present study followed the newly hypothesized “suicidal drive for psychosis” suggesting that psychosis may be consequential to suicidal ideation (SI) and behavior, and attempted to explain parts of the pathway between these variables. To this end, we aimed to test whether problematic smartphone use (PSU) has an indirect mediating effect in the cross-sectional relationship between SI and positive psychotic experiences (PEs). Methods: Lebanese community young adults (N=4158; 64.4% females; mean age 21.91±3.79) have been invited to participate to a cross-sectional, web-based study in the period from June to September 2022. Results: After adjusting for potential confounders (i.e., the living situation, marital status, household crowding index, economic pressure, cannabis use, other drugs use, and past history of mental illness other than psychosis), we found that higher levels of suicidal ideation was significantly associated with greater PSU, which was also positively and significantly associated with more positive PEs. Finally, greater suicidal ideation was significantly and directly associated with more positive PEs. Conclusion: Our findings suggest that SPU may be regarded as a potential target for prevention and intervention in psychosis. Clinicians, educators and school administrators ought to give greater attention to PSU among vulnerable young people who present with SI.
... Carey et al. (2021), which reported that early poorer development in domains such as motor and cognitive function is associated with hallucinatory experiences. Nev-ertheless, accumulation of pre-existing and environmental risk factors, particularly those that have a broader impact on development (M.-d.-G. ...
Thesis
Psychotic-like experiences (PLEs) include experiences such as hallucinations and delusional thinking that can occur in the absence of a clinical disorder. PLEs peak in middle childhood but appear to spontaneously remit in most children. Nevertheless, their presence, and particularly their persistence, are associated with an increased risk of developing later psychosis and poor psychiatric and social outcomes. However, the mechanisms by which they are generated during childhood are unclear. Existing models of psychosis cite early life experience and childhood neurodevelopment as important but often examine these retrospectively. Furthermore, they suggest mechanisms for how psychotic experiences are generated that are assumed to apply across the lifespan. This assumption has remained untested, however. Consequently, in this thesis, I investigated neurocognitive mechanisms of PLEs in 9-10 year-old-children using data from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study. In Study 1, I examined whether the established mechanistic risk factors in adult psychosis – affective symptoms, traumatic experiences, cognitive function, structural brain changes – are associated with PLEs using network analysis, finding only that depression-related symptoms were associated with PLEs. In Study 2, I tested whether fMRI activation in striatal reward pathways was associated with PLEs in children, as this is an established finding in adults. This study found no strong evidence for alteration to striatal reward pathways with a high likelihood that it was absent, rather than undetectable. Given the prognostic and aetiological important of persistence of PLEs, in Study 3, I tested affective, trauma-related, cognitive and striatal reward activation predictors of 1-year PLE persistence. Only depressive symptoms were substantial predictors. Depressive symptoms emerged as the strongest predictor of PLEs at this developmental stage, both cross-sectionally and longitudinally. The findings indicate that PLEs in childhood are not a ‘mini psychosis syndrome’ and developmental-stage specific models of psychotic experiences in children are required.
Article
Background and Hypothesis Familial high-risk (FHR) studies examining longitudinal associations between neurocognition and psychotic experiences are currently lacking. We hypothesized neurocognitive impairments at age 7 to be associated with increased risk of psychotic experiences from age 7 to 11 in children at familial high risk of schizophrenia (FHR-SZ) or bipolar disorder (FHR-BP) and population-based controls (PBC), and further, impaired functioning in some neurocognitive functions to be associated with greater risk of psychotic experiences in children at FHR-SZ or FHR-BP relative to PBC. Study Design Neurocognition was assessed at age 7 (early childhood) and psychotic experiences from age 7 to 11 (middle childhood) in 449 children from the Danish High Risk and Resilience Study. The neurocognitive assessment covered intelligence, processing speed, attention, visuospatial and verbal memory, working memory, and set-shifting. Psychotic experiences were assessed through face-to-face interviews with the primary caregiver and the child. Study Results Set-shifting impairments at age 7 were associated with greater risk of psychotic experiences from age 7 to 11 in children at FHR-SZ. Children at FHR-BP and PBC showed no differential associations. Working memory and visuospatial memory impairments were related to increased risk of psychotic experiences across the cohort. However, adjusting for concurrent psychopathology attenuated these findings. Conclusions Early childhood neurocognitive impairments are risk markers of middle childhood psychotic experiences, of which impaired set-shifting appears to further increase the risk of psychotic experiences in children at FHR-SZ. More research is needed to examine longitudinal associations between neurocognitive impairments and psychotic experiences in FHR samples.
Article
Full-text available
Background While the psychopathological sequalae of childhood maltreatment are widely acknowledged, less is known about the underlying pathways by which childhood maltreatment might lead to an increased risk for mental health problems. Recent studies indicated that impaired personality functioning might mediate this relationship. The aim of the present paper was to extend the current literature by investigating the mediating effect of impaired personality functioning between different types of childhood maltreatment and self-reported mental health problems in a high-risk sample. Methods Overall, 173 young adults (mean age = of 26.61 years; SD = 3.27) with a history of residential child welfare and juvenile justice placements in Switzerland were included in the current study. The Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ-SF), Semi-structured Interview for Personality Functioning DSM-5 (STiP-5.1) and the self-report questionnaires of the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment scales (ASEBA) were used. Mediation analyses were conducted through structural equation modeling. Results Overall, 76.3% (N = 132) participants indicated at least one type of childhood maltreatment, with emotional neglect being most commonly reported (60.7%). A total of 30.6% (N = 53) participants self-reported mental health problems. Emotional abuse (r = 0.34; p < .001) and neglect (r = 0.28; p < .001) were found to be most strongly associated with mental health problems. In addition, impaired personality functioning was fond to be a significant mediator for overall childhood maltreatment ( β = 0.089; p = 0.008) and emotional neglect ( β = 0.077; p = 0.016). Finally, impaired self-functioning was found to be a significant mediator when both self-functioning and interpersonal functioning were included as potential mediators in the relationship between overall childhood maltreatment ( β 1 = 0.177, p 1 = 0.007) and emotional neglect ( β 1 = 0.173, p 1 = 0.003). Conclusion Emotional neglect may be particularly important in the context of childhood maltreatment, personality functioning, and mental health problems and, therefore, should not be overlooked next to the more “obvious” forms of childhood maltreatment. Combining interventions designed for personality functioning with trauma-informed practices in standard mental health services might counteract the psychopathological outcomes of maltreated children and adolescents.
Article
Full-text available
Psychotic experiences (PE) are common in the general population, in particular in childhood, adolescence and young adulthood. PE have been shown to be associated with an increased risk for later psychotic disorders, mental disorders, and poorer functioning. Recent findings have highlighted the relevance of PE to many fields of healthcare, including treatment response in clinical services for anxiety & depression treatment, healthcare costs and service use. Despite PE relevance to many areas of mental health, and healthcare research, there remains a gap of information between PE researchers and experts in other fields. With this review, we aim to bridge this gap by providing a broad overview of the current state of PE research, and future directions. This narrative review aims to provide an broad overview of the literature on psychotic experiences, under the following headings: (1) Definition and Measurement of PE; (2) Risk Factors for PE; (3) PE and Health; (4) PE and Psychosocial Functioning; (5) Interventions for PE, (6) Future Directions.
Article
Full-text available
Childhood maltreatment (CM) has been associated with adverse psychosocial outcomes during the pandemic, but the underlying mechanisms are unclear. In a prospective online study using baseline and 10-week follow-up data of 391 German participants, we applied multiple mediation analyses to test to what extent COVID-19 perceived stressors mediate the association between CM and later adverse psychosocial outcomes compared to established mediators of rumination and insecure attachment. We also explored the relative importance of different COVID-19 related stressors in predicting adverse psychological trajectories using elastic net regression. Results showed that CM was longitudinally associated with all adverse psychosocial outcome. COVID-19 perceived stressors, rumination, and insecure attachment mediated this relationship and full mediation was observed for the outcomes anxiety, stress and psychological well-being. COVID-19-related concerns about the future was most strongly and consistently associated with adverse psychosocial functioning. These findings provide preliminary evidence that COVID-19 perceived stressors, in particular concerns about the future, may be a key mechanism underlying the development of adverse psychosocial outcomes in individuals with a CM history. Thus, COVID-19 perceived stressors may require a higher priority for prevention and treatment efforts in vulnerable groups. Our results warrant replication in more representative cross-cultural samples.
Article
Full-text available
Healthy sleep supports robust development of the brain and behavior. Modern society presents a host of challenges that can impair and disrupt critical circadian rhythms that reinforce optimal physiological functioning, including the proper timing and consolidation of sleep. While the acute effects of inadequate sleep and disrupted circadian rhythms are being defined, the adverse developmental consequences of disrupted sleep and circadian rhythms are understudied. Here, we exposed mice to disrupting light–dark cycles from birth until weaning and demonstrate that such exposure has adverse impacts on brain and behavior as adults. Mice that experience early-life circadian disruption exhibit more anxiety-like behavior in the elevated plus maze, poorer spatial memory in the Morris Water Maze, and impaired working memory in a delayed match-to-sample task. Additionally, neuron morphology in the amygdala, hippocampus and prefrontal cortex is adversely impacted. Pyramidal cells in these areas had smaller dendritic fields, and pyramidal cells in the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus also exhibited diminished branching orders. Disrupted mice were also hyperactive as adults, but otherwise exhibited no alteration in adult circadian locomotor rhythms. These results highlight that circadian disruption early in life may have long lasting and far-reaching consequences for the development of behavior and the brain.
Article
Full-text available
Importance Mental health professionals typically encounter patients at 1 point in patients’ lives. This cross-sectional window understandably fosters focus on the current presenting diagnosis. Research programs, treatment protocols, specialist clinics, and specialist journals are oriented to presenting diagnoses, on the assumption that diagnosis informs about causes and prognosis. This study tests an alternative hypothesis: people with mental disorders experience many different kinds of disorders across diagnostic families, when followed for 4 decades. Objective To describe mental disorder life histories across the first half of the life course. Design, Setting, and Participants This cohort study involved participants born in New Zealand from 1972 to 1973 who were enrolled in the population-representative Dunedin Study. Participants were observed from birth to age 45 years (until April 2019). Data were analyzed from May 2019 to January 2020. Main Outcomes and Measures Diagnosed impairing disorders were assessed 9 times from ages 11 to 45 years. Brain function was assessed through neurocognitive examinations conducted at age 3 years, neuropsychological testing during childhood and adulthood, and midlife neuroimaging-based brain age. Results Of 1037 original participants (535 male [51.6%]), 1013 had mental health data available. The proportions of participants meeting the criteria for a mental disorder were as follows: 35% (346 of 975) at ages 11 to 15 years, 50% (473 of 941) at age 18 years, 51% (489 of 961) at age 21 years, 48% (472 of 977) at age 26 years, 46% (444 of 969) at age 32 years, 45% (429 of 955) at age 38 years, and 44% (407 of 927) at age 45 years. The onset of the disorder occurred by adolescence for 59% of participants (600 of 1013), eventually affecting 86% of the cohort (869 of 1013) by midlife. By age 45 years, 85% of participants (737 of 869) with a disorder had accumulated comorbid diagnoses. Participants with adolescent-onset disorders subsequently presented with disorders at more past-year assessments (r = 0.71; 95% CI, 0.68 to 0.74; P < .001) and met the criteria for more diverse disorders (r = 0.64; 95% CI, 0.60 to 0.67; P < .001). Confirmatory factor analysis summarizing mental disorder life histories across 4 decades identified a general factor of psychopathology, the p-factor. Longitudinal analyses showed that high p-factor scores (indicating extensive mental disorder life histories) were antedated by poor neurocognitive functioning at age 3 years (r = −0.18; 95% CI, −0.24 to −0.12; P < .001), were accompanied by childhood-to-adulthood cognitive decline (r = −0.11; 95% CI, −0.17 to −0.04; P < .001), and were associated with older brain age at midlife (r = 0.14; 95% CI, 0.07 to 0.20; P < .001). Conclusions and Relevance These findings suggest that mental disorder life histories shift among different successive disorders. Data from the present study, alongside nationwide data from Danish health registers, inform a life-course perspective on mental disorders. This perspective cautions against overreliance on diagnosis-specific research and clinical protocols.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Childhood psychotic symptoms have been associated with various psychiatric disorders in adulthood but their role as early markers of poor outcomes during the crucial transition to adulthood is largely unknown. Therefore, we investigated associations between age-12 psychotic symptoms and a range of mental health problems and functional outcomes at age 18. Methods: Data were used from the Environmental Risk Longitudinal Twin Study, a nationally representative birth cohort of 2232 twins born in 1994-1995 in England and Wales, followed to age 18 with 93% retention. Childhood psychotic symptoms were assessed in structured interviews at age 12. At age 18, study members' mental health problems, functional outcomes, risky behaviors, and offending were measured using self-reports and official records. Results: Children with psychotic symptoms (N = 125, 5.9%) were more likely to experience a range of mental health problems in young adulthood than children without such symptoms. They were also more likely to be obese, smoke cigarettes, be lonely, be parents, and report a lower quality of life, but not more likely to commit crimes. Childhood psychotic symptoms predicted these poor outcomes over and above other emotional and behavioral problems during childhood. Nevertheless, twin analyses indicated that these associations were largely accounted for by shared family factors. Conclusions: Psychotic symptoms in childhood signal risk for pervasive mental health and functional difficulties in young adulthood and thus may provide a useful screen for an array of later problems. However, early psychotic symptoms and poor outcomes may be manifestations of shared environmental and genetic risks.
Article
Full-text available
Importance Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have consistently reported associations between childhood trauma and psychotic experiences and disorders. However, few studies have examined whether the age of exposure or specific trauma types are differently associated with the risk of developing psychotic experiences. Objective To examine whether exposure to trauma, assessed at multiple age periods between 0 and 17 years of age, is associated with an increased risk of psychotic experiences by age 18 years and whether this association varies according to trauma type as well as age and frequency of exposure. Design, Setting, and Participants This study used data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, a large population-based birth cohort in the United Kingdom that recruited women who resided in the Avon Health Authority area and had an expected delivery date between April 1, 1991, and December 31, 1992. Data on psychotic experiences were included in the study, along with trauma variables derived from assessments completed by the parents or self-reported by the participants. The variables represent exposure to any trauma type between ages 0 and 17 years; any trauma type within a distinct age period: early childhood (0-4.9 years), middle childhood (5-10.9 years), or adolescence (11-17 years); specific trauma types between ages 0 and 17 years; and specific trauma types within early childhood, middle childhood, or adolescence. Data were analyzed from January 9, 2017, to November 30, 2017. Main Outcomes and Measures Suspected or definite psychotic experiences were assessed using the psychosis-like symptoms semistructured interview at age 12 years and then at age 18 years. Results The sample of 4433 participants included 2504 (56.5%) females, with a mean (SD) age of 17.8 (0.38) years. Exposure to any trauma up to age 17 years was associated with increased odds of psychotic experiences at age 18 years (adjusted odds ratio, 2.91; 95% CI, 2.15-3.93). All trauma types from age 0 to 17 years were associated with an increased odds of psychotic experiences. The population-attributable fraction for childhood and adolescent trauma on psychotic experiences at age 18 years was 45% (95% CI, 25%-60%). Effect sizes for most trauma types were greater for exposure that was more proximal to the outcome, although CIs overlapped with those for more distal trauma. Evidence supported dose-response associations for exposure to multiple trauma types and at multiple age periods. In an analysis aimed at minimizing reverse causality, adolescent trauma was also associated with past-year incident psychotic experiences at age 18 years. Conclusions and Relevance These findings are consistent with the thesis that trauma could have a causal association with psychotic experiences; if so, identification of modifiable mediators is required to inform prevention strategies.
Article
Full-text available
Background Psychotic experiences (PEs) are common in childhood and have been associated with concurrent mental disorder and poorer global functioning. Little is known about the effects of childhood PEs on future functioning. We investigated the effects of childhood PEs on global functioning from childhood into early adulthood. Method Fifty‐six participants from a community sample completed all three waves of the Adolescent Brain Development study (T1 Age: 11.69, T2 Age: 15.80 T3Age: 18.80). At each phase, participants completed a clinical interview assessing for PEs, mental disorder and global function. Repeated measures models, adjusted for mental disorder and gender, were used to compare current (C‐GAF) and most severe past (MSP‐GAF) functioning in participants who had reported PEs in childhood and controls. Results Participants with a history of PEs had significantly poorer C‐GAF (P < 0.001) and MSP‐GAF scores (P < 0.001). Poorer functioning was evident in childhood (C‐GAF: P = 0.001; MSP‐GAF: P < 0.001), adolescence (C‐GAF: P < 0.001; MSP‐GAF: P = 0.004) and early adulthood (C‐GAF: P = 0.001; MSP‐GAF: P = 0.076). Discussion Children who report PEs have persistently poorer functioning through to early adulthood. The longitudinal association between childhood PEs and global functioning highlights the underlying global vulnerability in children reporting PEs, beyond what can be explained by mental disorder.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: The aim of this study was to use prospective data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) to examine association between trajectories of early childhood developmental skills and psychotic experiences (PEs) in early adolescence. Method: This study examined data from n = 6790 children from the ALSPAC cohort who participated in a semi-structured interview to assess PEs at age 12. Child development was measured using parental report at 6, 18, 30, and 42 months of age using a questionnaire of items adapted from the Denver Developmental Screening Test – II. Latent class growth analysis was used to generate trajectories over time for measures of fine and gross motor development, social, and communication skills. Logistic regression was used to investigate associations between developmental trajectories in each of these early developmental domains and PEs at age 12. Results: The results provided evidence that decline rather than enduringly poor social (adjusted OR = 1.28, 95% CI = 1.10–1.92, p = 0.044) and communication skills (adjusted OR 1.12, 95% CI = 1.03–1.22, p = 0.010) is predictive of suspected or definite PEs in early adolescence, than those with stable and/or improving skills. Motor skills did not display the same pattern of association; although gender specific effects provided evidence that only declining pattern of fine motor skills was associated with suspected and definite PEs in males compared to females (interaction OR = 1.47, 95% CI = 1.09–1.97, p = 0.012). Conclusion: Findings suggest that decline rather than persistent impairment in social and communication skills were most predictive of PEs in early adolescence. Findings are discussed in terms of study’s strengths, limitations, and clinical implications.
Article
Background: Psychotic experiences (PE) are highly prevalent in childhood and are known to be associated with co-morbid mental health disorders and functional difficulties in adolescence. However, little is known about the long-term outcomes of young people who report PE. Methods: As part of the Adolescent Brain Development Study, 211 young people were recruited in childhood (mean age 11.7 years) and underwent detailed clinical interviews, with 25% reporting PE. A 10 year follow-up study was completed and 103 participants returned (mean age 20.9 years). Structured clinical interviews for DSM-5 (SCID-5) and inter- viewer-rated assessments of functioning were conducted. A detailed neuropsychological battery was also administered. Analyses investigated group differences between those who had ever reported PE and controls in early adulthood. Results: The PE group was at a significantly higher risk of meeting DSM-5 criteria for a current (OR 4.08, CI 1.16–14.29, p = 0.03) and lifetime psychiatric disorder (OR 3.27, CI 1.43–7.47, p = 0.005). They were also at a significantly higher risk of multi-morbid lifetime psychiatric disorders. Significantly lower scores on current social and global functioning measures were observed for the PE group. Overall, there were no differences in neuropsychological performance between groups apart from significantly lower scores on the Stroop Word task and the Purdue Pegboard task for the PE group. Conclusions: Our findings suggest that reports of PE are associated with poorer mental health and functional outcomes in early adulthood, with some persisting cognitive and motor deficits. Young people who report such symptoms could be considered a target group for interventions to aid functional outcomes.
Article
Background Psychotic experiences (PEs) are common in childhood and adolescence and their association with mental disorders is well-established. We aim to conduct a quantitative synthesis the literature on the relationship between childhood and adolescent PEs and (i) any mental disorder; and (ii) specific categories of mental disorder, while stratifying by study design. Method Three electronic databases (PUBMED, PsycINFO and EMBASE) were searched from inception to August 2017 for all the published literature on childhood and adolescent PEs and mental disorder (outcome) in non-help-seeking community samples. Study quality was assessed using a recognised quality assessment tool for observational studies. Two authors conducted independent data extraction. Pooled odds ratios were calculated for mental disorders using random-effects models. Additional analyses were conducted investigating different categories of mental disorder while stratifying by study design. Results Fourteen studies from 13 community samples ( n = 29 517) were identified with 9.8% of participants reporting PEs. PEs were associated with a three-fold increased risk of any mental disorder [odds ratio (OR) 3.08, confidence interval (CI) 2.26–4.21, k = 12]. PEs were associated with four-fold increase risk of psychotic disorder (OR 3.96, CI 2.03–7.73, population-attributable-fraction: 23.2%, k = 5). In addition, PEs were associated with an increased risk of affective disorders, anxiety disorders, behavioural disorders and substance-use disorders. Few longitudinal studies have investigated childhood and adolescent PEs and subsequent non-psychotic disorders which limited a meaningful synthesis and interpretation of these results. Conclusion This meta-analysis confirms that PEs are prevalent in childhood and adolescent community samples and are associated with a variety of mental disorders beyond psychotic disorders. Further longitudinal research is necessary to fully determine the longitudinal relationship between PEs and non-psychotic disorders.
Article
Objective: We examined associations between developmental vulnerability profiles determined at the age of 5 years and subsequent childhood mental illness between ages 6 and 13 years in an Australian population cohort. Methods: Intergenerational records from New South Wales (NSW) Government Departments of Health and Child Protection spanning pre-birth to 13 years of age were linked with the 2009 Australian Early Development Census records for 86,668 children. Mental illness indices for children were extracted from health records between 2009 and 2016 (child's age of 6-13 years). Associations between mental disorder diagnoses and membership of early childhood risk groups, including those with established 'special needs' (3777, 4.3%) at school entry, or putative risk classes delineated via latent class analysis of Australian Early Development Census subdomains - referred to as 'pervasive risk' ( N = 3479; 4.0%), 'misconduct risk' ( N = 5773; 6.7%) or 'mild generalised risk' ( N = 9542; 11%) - were estimated using multinomial logistic regression, relative to children showing 'no risk' ( N = 64,097; 74%). Poisson regression models estimated the relative risk of a greater number of days recorded with mental health service contacts among children in each Australian Early Development Census risk group. Adjusted models included child's sex, socioeconomic disadvantage, child protection contacts and parental mental illness as covariates. Results: The crude odds of any mental disorder among children aged 6-13 years was increased approximately threefold in children showing pervasive risk or misconduct risk profiles at the age of 5 years, and approximately sevenfold in children with special needs, relative to children showing no risk; patterns of association largely remained after adjusting for covariates. Children with special needs and the misconduct risk class used mental health services over a greater number of days than the no risk class. Conclusion: Patterns of early childhood developmental vulnerability are associated with subsequent onset of mental disorders and have the potential to inform interventions to mitigate the risk for mental disorders in later childhood and adolescence.
Article
Objective: To identify neuropsychological and motor changes from adolescence to early adulthood in young people with psychotic experiences (PE). Methods: A community-based sample of 56 young people attended the study over a 9 year follow-up period. Participants were assessed over 3 time-points at T1, T2 and T3 aged x̅=11.69, x̅=15.80 and x̅=18.80 years respectively. PE were assessed using the Kiddie Schedule for Affective and Depressive Symptoms (K-SADS). Neuropsychological assessments, including subtests of the MATRICS battery, and motor assessments were examined at T2 and T3. Two groups were compared: those who ever reported PE during their adolescence or early adulthood (n=21) and a healthy control group (n=35). Further group analysis was conducted within the PE group subdividing into those with transient PE (n=10) and those with persistent PE (n=11). Results: At T3, a significant group difference was found between the PE and control groups in the fine motor skill task, the Pegboard task (F=4.8, p=.03) and the processing speed task, the Digit-Symbol Coding task (F=5.36, p=.03). Furthermore, a significant group difference was found between the transient PE and control groups on the Digit-Symbol Coding task (F=5.61, p=.02). A significant group difference was found between the persistent PE and control groups on the Pegboard task (F=7.84, p=.01). Conclusion: This study shows that fine motor skill and processing speed deficits persist in young people who report PE, even in those with transient PE. The current research advances the knowledge about the trajectory and precursors of sub-clinical symptoms of psychosis in young people.