ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Introduction: The study examined age-related differences between young and older adults’ emotional and psychological experience as well as cognitive functioning throughout different phases of the COVID-19 pandemic in Italy. Materials and Methods: Participants were interviewed by phone when confined at home during the national lockdown (T1-May 2020; N = 138 young adults; N = 119 older adults) and after the first wave of contagions, when restrictions were discarded (T2-September 2020; N = 52 young adults; N = 59 older adults). A sub-sample also participated in a third assessment (T3-December 2020). Participants completed questionnaires assessing their emotional and psychological functioning (i.e., positive and negative affect, perceived social and emotional loneliness, resilience) along with memory tasks (Backward Digit Span task and words list recall). Results: Although individuals reported less positive and more negative emotions during the lockdown than at T2, results showed that older adults displayed overall fewer negative emotions and greater resilience than young adults. The latter were those who reported feeling more emotionally lonely when compared to their older counterpart during the lockdown than afterward. Older adults’ advantage in emotional and psychological functioning was also confirmed 7 months after the national lockdown. Only age-related differences in favor of young adults for the memory tasks were found. The measures of interest were also susceptible to mood and/or concerns of COVID-19 effects. Discussion: These findings further highlight the age-related advantage of older adults managing the emotional and psychological experience even when facing an unexpected, prolonged, and unpredictable, stressful life event such as the COVID-19 pandemic.
Content may be subject to copyright.
fnagi-13-712369 September 4, 2021 Time: 17:34 # 1
BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT
published: 10 September 2021
doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.712369
Edited by:
Yang Jiang,
University of Kentucky, United States
Reviewed by:
Massimiliano Palmiero,
University of Bergamo, Italy
Yaakov Shmuel Gershon Hoffman,
Bar-Ilan University, Israel
Mirco Fasolo,
University “G. d’Annunzio”
Chieti-Pescara, Italy
*Correspondence:
Elena Carbone
elena.carbone@unipd.it
Erika Borella
erika.borella@unipd.it
Received: 20 May 2021
Accepted: 23 August 2021
Published: 10 September 2021
Citation:
Carbone E, Palumbo R, Sella E,
Lenti G, Di Domenico A and Borella E
(2021) Emotional, Psychological,
and Cognitive Changes Throughout
the COVID-19 Pandemic in Italy: Is
There an Advantage of Being an
Older Adult?
Front. Aging Neurosci. 13:712369.
doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.712369
Emotional, Psychological, and
Cognitive Changes Throughout the
COVID-19 Pandemic in Italy: Is There
an Advantage of Being an Older
Adult?
Elena Carbone1*, Rocco Palumbo2, Enrico Sella1, Graziana Lenti1, Alberto Di Domenico2
and Erika Borella1*
1Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padua, Italy, 2Department of Psychological, Health and Territorial
Sciences, G. d’Annunzio University of Chieti-Pescara, Chieti, Italy
Introduction: The study examined age-related differences between young and older
adults’ emotional and psychological experience as well as cognitive functioning
throughout different phases of the COVID-19 pandemic in Italy.
Materials and Methods: Participants were interviewed by phone when confined at
home during the national lockdown (T1-May 2020; N= 138 young adults; N= 119
older adults) and after the first wave of contagions, when restrictions were discarded
(T2-September 2020; N= 52 young adults; N= 59 older adults). A sub-sample
also participated in a third assessment (T3-December 2020). Participants completed
questionnaires assessing their emotional and psychological functioning (i.e., positive and
negative affect, perceived social and emotional loneliness, resilience) along with memory
tasks (Backward Digit Span task and words list recall).
Results: Although individuals reported less positive and more negative emotions
during the lockdown than at T2, results showed that older adults displayed overall
fewer negative emotions and greater resilience than young adults. The latter were
those who reported feeling more emotionally lonely when compared to their older
counterpart during the lockdown than afterward. Older adults’ advantage in emotional
and psychological functioning was also confirmed 7 months after the national lockdown.
Only age-related differences in favor of young adults for the memory tasks were
found. The measures of interest were also susceptible to mood and/or concerns of
COVID-19 effects.
Discussion: These findings further highlight the age-related advantage of older
adults managing the emotional and psychological experience even when facing an
unexpected, prolonged, and unpredictable, stressful life event such as the COVID-
19 pandemic.
Keywords: emotional functioning, psychological functioning, cognitive performance, age-related differences,
COVID-19 pandemic
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 1September 2021 | Volume 13 | Article 712369
fnagi-13-712369 September 4, 2021 Time: 17:34 # 2
Carbone et al. Emotions, Cognition Throughout COVID-19 Pandemic
INTRODUCTION
The COVID-19 pandemic has represented an unexpected and
prolonged stressful situation, with the potential of impacting
individuals’ emotional and psychological functioning. Hence, a
growing number of studies are interested in understanding the
emotional and psychological consequences of the COVID-19
outbreak in young and healthy older adults.
The studies exploring age differences between young and older
adults’ emotional reactions experienced during the first acute
phase of the pandemic (March-June 2020) found that older adults
showed higher emotional well-being (Carstensen et al., 2020),
lower stress, and negative affect (Ceccato et al., 2020;Young
et al., 2021) than younger adults when facing the unexpected
and inescapable stressors imposed by the COVID-19 lockdown.
During the lockdown, older individuals also felt less lonely than
younger adults, thereby showing less vulnerability to the effects
of social distancing (e.g., Bu et al., 2020;Luchetti et al., 2020;
Varga et al., 2021). Furthermore, when considering resilience, a
dynamic process of recovering from adversity which it configures
as a crucial (psychological) protective factor to cope with the
COVID-19 pandemic implications (e.g., Holmes et al., 2020),
older adults’ perceived ability to cope with adversities was less
influenced by COVID-19-related stressful events compared to
younger adults (e.g., McCleskey and Gruda, 2021;Rossi et al.,
2021). Such a pattern of findings showed how aging seemed to
be associated with better emotional and psychological outcomes
during the COVID-19 lockdown, which is in line with a well-
proven theoretical framework on aging and emotions such as the
Socioemotional Selectivity Theory (SST; Carstensen et al., 2003).
According to the SST, due to their constrained temporal horizons,
older adults experience motivational shifts toward prioritizing
emotionally meaningful and positive goals and experiences with
advancing age (e.g., Lockenhoff and Carstensen, 2004;Charles
and Carstensen, 2010). Empirical evidence has, for instance,
demonstrated that older adults display selective attention toward
positive stimuli and show aversiveness toward negative ones (e.g.,
Fairfield et al., 2015). As a result, older adults experience high
level of psychological well-being and display greater emotional
regulation than young adults (Charles and Carstensen, 2010;
Burr et al., 2021). Such a positive emotions-oriented attitude
characteristic of older age may have therefore allowed older adults
to display greater emotional and psychological functioning,
than their younger counterparts, even when facing a stressful
emergency such as the COVID-19 pandemic.
It is worth highlighting that these studies depicted the
unique effects of the first wave of the COVID-19 outbreak
(when individuals were confined at home due to the lockdown
restrictions) and lack an “as-usual” everyday normal condition
to compare data with. Moreover, the health emergency lasted
throughout 2020. Like other life-stressful events, COVID-
19 might have caused detrimental effects on individuals’
psychological and emotional well-being in the long term (Wang
et al., 2020). Therefore, it is worth investigating to what extent the
emotional and psychological effects of the pandemic and the age
advantage, shown in such domains by older adults, change –or is
maintained– over time.
Apart from young and older adults’ psychological and
emotional fallout, which the SST can account, lockdown
restrictions have also led individuals to cope with an
impoverished environment, in terms of cognitive and socially
stimulating activities and experiences, that are, however, known
to have a role in counteracting/delaying age-related cognitive
decline (Reuter-Lorenz and Park, 2014). At the same time,
cognitive functioning is known also to be affected when stressful
situations occur (e.g., Scott et al., 2015;Boals and Banks,
2020). Therefore, the lack of an “enriched” and stimulating
environment, due to lockdown restrictions, and the unexpected
and prolonged stressful emergency individuals had to manage,
might have likely broadly impacted cognitive functioning as well.
Interestingly, little attention has been devoted to understanding
whether and to what extent the COVID-19 lockdown and the
following restrictions have affected such other crucial domain
of our functioning, as the cognitive one, however. The two
studies that examined the COVID-19 lockdown influence in
this domain, highlighted that attention, executive functions,
and temporal orientation (Fiorenzato et al., 2021), as well as
daily functioning (Reading Turchioe et al., 2021), were perceived
as the most affected cognitive abilities during the lockdown,
whereas an improvement in the memory domain was perceived
(Fiorenzato et al., 2021). Also, younger adults reported higher
cognitive complaints than their older counterpart (Fiorenzato
et al., 2021;Reading Turchioe et al., 2021). These studies,
however, focused on the very first wave of the pandemic and
were based on self-reported measures that assessed participants’
perception of changes in their cognitive abilities during the
lockdown compared to before. Thus, it is still unknown whether
objective cognitive outcomes might have changed in young and
older adults through the pandemic.
For the first time (at least to our knowledge), the present
study aimed to ascertain age-related differences between young
and older adults’ emotional and psychological experiences
throughout different phases of the COVID-19 health emergency
in Italy. In addition, changes in cognitive functioning, in
particular in the memory domain, were jointly investigated using,
for the first time, classical tasks assessing two memory domains
sensitive to age-related decline, i.e., working memory -WM-
(Borella et al., 2007, 2019;Gamboz et al., 2009) and episodic
memory (Park, 2002).
To these aims, young and community-dwelling healthy older
participants completed several questionnaires assessing different
emotional and psychological factors (positive and negative
affects, social and emotional loneliness, resilience), along with
WM and long-term memory tasks (Backward Digit Span task
and words list recall, respectively) on two occasions: (i) during
the first national lockdown, when several activities (e.g., shops
selling non-essential goods, educational –schools, university–,
social and leisure activities services and infrastructures) were
closed and people were allowed to leave their homes only for
a short amount of time and for documented purposes (e.g.,
health issues, shopping for basic necessities, to go to work if
this cannot be done from home) (T1; May 2020); and (ii) in
the period following the first wave of the pandemic, when the
stay-at-home and travel ban rules were dismissed and people
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 2September 2021 | Volume 13 | Article 712369
fnagi-13-712369 September 4, 2021 Time: 17:34 # 3
Carbone et al. Emotions, Cognition Throughout COVID-19 Pandemic
were allowed again to engage work, social and leisure activities as
usual (T2; September 2020). These two timepoints allowed us to
depict age-related differences in the emotional and psychological
experience and memory outcomes throughout the pandemic due
to changes in restriction regulations. T2 indeed also served as an
“as-usual” lifestyle routine situation condition (no restrictions)
to be compared with the lockdown timepoint (T1). A third
interview was also planned 7 months after the first lockdown (T3;
December 2020), to follow up on the impact of the COVID-19
outbreak on psychological, emotional, and cognitive outcomes.
What appeared to be the end of the emergency in our country in
September (T2), however, was then followed by a second wave
of contagions (since October 2020) that made it necessary to
restore stricter restrictions rules encompassing national curfew,
travel restrictions, and limited work, social and leisure activities.
Our third assessment occasion (T3) was therefore unexpectedly
characterized again by restrictions, thereby configuring as a
condition that mirrored the one that occurred during the first
timepoint (the national lockdown) more than an “as-usual” post-
pandemic situation.
In line with previous studies (e.g., Carstensen et al., 2020), we
expected older adults to show an advantage in terms of emotional
and psychological outcomes during the different phases of the
COVID-19 pandemic. Specifically, we expected older adults to
(i) feel more positive and less negative emotions compared to
younger adults (e.g., Luchetti et al., 2020); and (ii) be more
resilient and less lonely than younger adults throughout the
different timepoints (e.g., Rossi et al., 2021). Regarding the WM
and episodic memory tasks, we explored whether any changes
occurred between the two considered timepoints. Since the
lockdown has impacted the everyday life cognitive and social
“enriched” environment, we expected an accentuation of the age-
related differences in favor of younger adults (e.g., Borella et al.,
2007, 2019). However, given the short period (about 4 months)
between the two timepoints, no cognitive changes due to the
different restriction regulations were expected. Therefore, we
finally explored whether changes in the examined measures were
maintained or emerged in the longer term (T3). Since mood
(depression) and worries of contagion from COVID-19 have
been suggested to be among the vulnerability factors on mental
health and cognitive outcomes associated with this stressful
situation (e.g., Carstensen et al., 2020;De Pue et al., 2021), their
impact on the psychological and emotional as well as cognitive
outcomes considered was examined.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants
One hundred and forty-one young adults (age range: 18–44 years)
and 157 older adults (age range: 64–82 years), all Italian and
recruited by word of mouth, volunteered for the study and
completed an individual phone interview during the national
lockdown (T1; May 2020). Participants were contacted again in
a subsequent period, and 52 young adults and 69 older adults
agreed to participate in a second interview (T2; September 2020).
Twenty-four young adults and 37 older adults also completed
a third interview -which corresponded to a second wave of the
pandemic– (T3; December 2020).
Exclusion criteria were: (i) current or past COVID-19
infection, (ii) serious health issues and/or use of medication
(antidepressants, anxiolytics), as assessed by a semi-structured
interview (De Beni et al., 2008), and (iii) for older adults, signs
of cognitive impairment, i.e., a score below 17 to the Montreal
Cognitive Assessment-BLIND classical and widely used cognitive
functioning screening measure (Wittich et al., 2010).
The final sample included 138 young adults and 119 older
adults at T1, 52 young adults and 59 older adults at T2, and 18
young adults and 31 older adults at T3.
Materials
Emotional and Psychological Functioning
The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson
et al., 1988) consists of 20 adjectives describing different feelings
assessing positive and negative affective states. Participants were
asked to rate how they felt from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely)
in the previous month (T1: April, T2: August, T3: November),
higher scores corresponding to greater positive and negative
affects, respectively (maximum = 50).
The Social and Emotional Loneliness Scale (Capotosto et al.,
2017) comprises six items assessing emotional and social
loneliness. Participants were asked to rate their agreement
with each item from 1 (absolutely true) to 5 (absolutely not
true) regarding the previous month (T1: April, T2: August,
T3: November). The dependent variables were the sum of the
three items for emotional and social loneliness, respectively,
with lower scores indicating higher social and emotional
loneliness (maximum = 15).
The Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale–10 items (Campbell-
Sills and Stein, 2007) assesses the ability to cope with adversity.
Participants were asked to rate each item from 0 (not true at
all) to 4 (true nearly all the time) in relation to the previous
month (T1: April, T2: August, T3: November), with higher scores
corresponding to a greater resilience (maximum = 40).
Memory Performance
The Backward Digit Span task (adapted from the battery by
De Beni et al., 2008) involves presenting a series of digits (1 s
per digit). Participants had to repeat the series of digits in
the backward order. The series started from two digits and
rose to eight, each level containing two strings of digits. One
point was assigned for each sequence correctly recalled. The
final score corresponded to the total number of correct trials
recalled (maximum = 14).
The word list recall task involves presenting an ad hoc list of
15 words (adapted from Carretti et al., 2011) at a rate of 2 s per
word. Participants had to repeat as many words as they could
remember immediately after the list was presented (immediate
recall) and after 10 min (delayed recall). One point was assigned
for each word correctly recalled. The final scores corresponded to
the total number of correct words recalled for both the immediate
and delayed recall tasks (maximum = 15), respectively.
Three parallel versions for each memory measure were created
and used in a counterbalanced fashion across timepoints.
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 3September 2021 | Volume 13 | Article 712369
fnagi-13-712369 September 4, 2021 Time: 17:34 # 4
Carbone et al. Emotions, Cognition Throughout COVID-19 Pandemic
Mood and Fear of COVID-19
The Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II; Beck et al., 1996) and
the Geriatric Depression Scale- 15 items (GDS; Yesavage et al.,
1982) are self-report of depressive symptoms for young and older
adults, respectively.
The Fear of COVID-19 questionnaire (Di Crosta et al., 2020)
assesses the conviction of being infected by COVID-19, either
in the past or in the future (Beliefs of Contagion Scale -BCS),
and the possibility of suffering severe consequences (i.e., being
hospitalized or dying) due to the contagion (Consequences of
Contagion Scale -CCS), referred to either self or loved ones’
health. Participants answered on a scale from 0 (not at all) to
100 (extremely) in relation to the previous month (T1: April,
T2: August, T3: November), with higher scores corresponding to
greater worries of being infected or suffering severe consequences
due to the contagion, respectively.
Procedure
Participants were contacted by phone to complete a single 90 min
interview while confined at home due to the COVID-19 outbreak
(T1; 1–20 May 2020). Participants were asked to place in a
quiet area of their home to avoid hearing issues. After obtaining
their consent, the experimenter guided participants through the
completion of tasks and questionnaires -ensuring that they were
able to hear and understand the instructions and stimuli clearly-
as following: a semi-structured interview assessing demographic
characteristics as well as physical and mental health status, the
MOCA-BLIND (cognitive functioning screening for older adults
only), the Backward Digit Span task, the word list-immediate
recall, the Resilience scale, the word list-delayed recall, the Social
and Emotional Loneliness Scale, the PANAS, the BDI-II (young
adults) or the GDS (older adults), and the Fear of COVID-
19 questionnaire. Participants completed the same survey also
during the second (T2; 1–20 September 2020) and the third (T3;
1–20 December 2020) interviews.
Statistical Analyses
To assess age-related differences between young and older
adults’ mood (depression), worries of contagion from COVID-
19, psychological and emotional experience, and memory
performance throughout different phases of the COVID-19
pandemic, linear mixed-effects models (Pinheiro and Bates, 2000)
were run for all the measures of interest with Age group (young
adults vs. older adults) and Time (T1 vs. T2) as predictors,
and random intercepts for participants. Maximum likelihood
estimation was used. The significance of the effects was tested
with the likelihood ratio test for nested models based on chi
square distribution. Then, the psychological, emotional, and
memory outcomes models were run again with mood (depression
scores)1, BCS, and CCS as covariates to control for such
vulnerability factors.
The same analyses (linear mixed-effects models with
covariates) were also run considering the third interview (T3)
1To obtain a unique measure for depressive symptoms to be used as a covariate in
the analyses, standardized scores, age group-centered considering the scores at T1,
were computed.
to ascertain whether the pattern of results between T1 and T2
was confirmed. However, due to the small sample of participants
that completed the third interview, these analyses have mainly
descriptive/qualitative purposes.
RESULTS
The descriptive statistics of demographic characteristics, the
measures of interest by age group and assessment occasion,
and results from independent t-tests between young and older
adults who did complete both interviews at T1 and at T2 and
those who did complete only T1 are shown in Supplementary
Materials-Section A.
Mood and Fear of COVID-19
Regarding mood, a main effect of Time emerged for young
adults only, who reported lower BDI-II scores at T2 than at T1
(see Supplementary Materials-Section B). The 28.26% of young
adults and 15.96% of older adults at T1, and 7.69% of young
adults and 15.25% of older adults at T2, scored above the cut-off
of the BDI-II and the GDS, respectively.
As for the Fear of COVID-19 questionnaire, younger adults
reported a higher belief of being infected by COVID-19
than older adults. In contrast, older adults reported greater
worries of suffering severe consequences from contagion (see
Supplementary Materials-Section B).
Emotional and Psychological
Functioning
Results showed only a significant main effect of Time for the
PANAS-Positive emotions, participants reporting lower positive
emotions at T1 than at T2. Significant main effects of Age group
and Time emerged for the PANAS-Negative emotions. Older
adults reported lower negative emotions than younger adults.
Overall, participants reported higher negative emotions at T1
than at T2 (see Table 1).
For the Emotional loneliness scale, significant main effects
of Age group and Time emerged. Older adults reported lower
emotional loneliness than younger adults. All participants
reported higher emotional loneliness at T1 compared to T2 (see
Table 1). The Age group ×Time interaction was significant
(see Table 1), and planned contrasts indicated that both young
and older adults reported a higher emotional loneliness at T1
compared to T2 (ß = 1.94 [1.42; 2.46], p<0.001; ß = 0.70
[0.19; 1.21], p= 0.032), and young adults perceived a higher
emotional loneliness than older adults at T1 (ß = 1.36 [0.84; 1.88],
p<0.001), but not at T2 (ß = 0.12 [0.62; 0.85], p= 1.00).
For the Social loneliness scale, no significant effects emerged (see
Table 1).
Regarding the Resilience scale, results showed only a
significant main effect of Age group. Older adults reported higher
resilience than younger adults (see Table 1).
When adding mood (depression), BCS, and CCS as covariates
in the models, the mood was a significant covariate for all
the emotional and psychological outcomes and the BCS was
a significant covariate for the PANAS-Negative emotions (see
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 4September 2021 | Volume 13 | Article 712369
fnagi-13-712369 September 4, 2021 Time: 17:34 # 5
Carbone et al. Emotions, Cognition Throughout COVID-19 Pandemic
Table 2 and Figure 1). Results were confirmed for all the
outcomes except for the Social loneliness scale, for which a
main effect of Time emerged. Individuals reported lower social
loneliness at T1 compared to T2 (see Table 2).
Memory Performance
As for the Backward Digit Span task, results showed only
a significant main effect of Age group with younger adults
outperforming older adults (see Table 1).
Concerning the word list recall tasks, results showed a
significant main effect of Age group for both the immediate and
delayed recall with younger adults outperforming older adults.
A significant main effect of Time also emerged for both the
immediate and delayed recall. Participants recalled fewer words
during the lockdown (T1) than at T2 (see Table 1).
When adding mood, the BCS, and the CCS as covariates in
the model, the main effect of Time for both the immediate and
delayed recall was no longer significant. The CCS scale was a
significant covariate for the WM measure, and the BCS was a
significant covariate for the word list delayed recall (see Table 2
and Figure 1).
Additional Analyses
When considering T3 (see Supplementary Materials- Additional
analyses section), older adults reported more positive and less
negative emotions than younger adults for the PANAS scales.
Regardless of age group, participants reported less positive and
TABLE 1 | Results of the linear mixed-effects models for the measures of interest.
Measure of interest Effect χ2df p
PANAS-Positive emotions Age group 1.620 1 0.203
Time 24.872 1 <0.001
Age group ×Time 1.936 1 0.164
PANAS-Negative emotions Age group 32.561 1 <0.001
Time 13.174 1 <0.001
Age group ×Time 0.015 1 0.901
Emotional loneliness Age group 7.352 1 0.007
Time 41.139 1 <0.001
Age group ×Time 9.982 1 0.002
Social loneliness Age group 0.987 1 0.321
Time 3.058 1 0.080
Age group ×Time 1.206 1 0.272
Resilience Age group 16.829 1 <0.001
Time 0.138 1 0.710
Age group ×Time 0.031 1 0.860
Backward Digit Span task Age group 40.748 1 <0.001
Time 0.448 1 0.503
Age group ×Time 0.546 1 0.460
Word list-immediate recall Age group 19.464 1 <0.001
Time 4.396 1 0.036
Age group ×Time 2.171 1 0.141
Word list-delayed recall Age group 20.160 1 <0.001
Time 4.525 1 0.033
Age group ×Time 1.319 1 0.251
more negative emotions at T1 than T2, but no differences
between T3 and T1 and T2 emerged.
Results on emotional loneliness confirmed lower scores for
older adults than younger ones. Participants reported higher
emotional loneliness at T1 than at T2 and felt less emotionally
lonelier at T2 than at T3; the latter did not differ from T1. The Age
group ×Time interaction was no longer significant. For Social
loneliness, the main effect of Time was no longer significant.
For the Resilience scale, the pattern of results found between
T1 and T2 was confirmed. Older adults reported a higher
resilience than their younger counterpart also in the long term.
Results at T3 also confirmed the age-related differences in
favor of younger adults for the WM measure and the word
list recall tasks.
The mood was always a significant covariate for the emotional
and psychological outcomes and emerged as a significant
covariate for the word list recall tasks. The BCS was always a
significant covariate for the PANAS-Negative emotions and the
word list delayed recall, while the CCS scale for the WM measure.
Finally, as a further additional analysis to investigate
any associations between changes in emotional, psychological,
and cognitive outcomes, correlations between change indexes
expressing the difference between scores at T1 and scores at T2
for each measure of interest (controlled for mood, BCS, and
CCS change indexes) were run by age group. Results showed
that younger adults, who displayed better performance in the
word list immediate recall task between the lockdown and the
subsequent timepoint, showed greater positive affect between T1
and T2 (r= 0.31, p<0.05). Furthermore, those who showed
a better performance between T1 and T2 in the WM measure
reported lower emotional loneliness (r= 0.31, p<0.05). Instead,
no significant associations between changes in the emotional,
psychological, and cognitive outcomes emerged for older adults.
DISCUSSION
The present study examined age-related differences throughout
the COVID-19 pandemic in Italy between young and older
adults’ emotional and psychological experience and classical
cognitive measures assessing memory abilities (not self-reported)
important for everyday life functioning (WM and long-term
memory) and sensitive to age-related effects. The lockdown
phase (T1, restrictions), a subsequent timepoint mirroring an
“as-usual” lifestyle routine situation (T2, no restrictions), and
a third timepoint (T3, analyzed for qualitative purposes) were
considered. The latter coincided with the second wave of
the pandemic characterized by the reimplementation of the
restrictions (a condition similar to T1).
In line with our expectations and previous evidence (e.g.,
Carstensen et al., 2020;Ceccato et al., 2020), our results showed
that young and older adults displayed a different psychological
and emotional experience of the COVID-19 lockdown. Although
all individuals perceived higher negative and lower positive affect
during the lockdown (T1) than in the subsequent “as-usual”-
like situation (T2), older adults reported overall lower negative
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 5September 2021 | Volume 13 | Article 712369
fnagi-13-712369 September 4, 2021 Time: 17:34 # 6
Carbone et al. Emotions, Cognition Throughout COVID-19 Pandemic
FIGURE 1 | Models plots by age group (young adults vs. older adults) and time (T1-May 2020 vs. T2-September 2020) for each measure of interest. PANAS positive
emotions scale (A); PANAS negative emotions scale (B); Emotional loneliness scale (C); Social loneliness scale (D); Resilience scale (E); Backward Digit Span Task
(F); Word list immediate recall (G); Word list delayed recall (H). Plots of the models with mood, BCS, and CCS as covariates. For the Social and Emotional
Loneliness scales lower scores correspond to higher perceived social and emotional loneliness, respectively.
emotions, as well as a higher resilience (as discussed below),
compared to their younger counterpart. The lockdown was
also found to have a significant role on emotional loneliness,
i.e., individuals reported negative feelings of detachment and
lack of deep and meaningful relationships while confined at
home (T1) more than in the subsequent timepoint (T2). This
was particularly true for younger adults, who reported feeling
emotionally lonelier than older adults, at T1 compared to T2. It is
then worth highlighting that, in line with previous evidence (e.g.,
Rossi et al., 2021), compared with older adults, younger adults
were those who reported higher depressive symptoms during the
lockdown than afterward.
Such a pattern of findings for the emotional and psychological
outcomes was also confirmed when considering T3 -albeit for
qualitative purposes due to the small sample of young and
older participants completing the third interview-. Older adults
still showed better emotional and psychological functioning,
together with higher positive emotions (not showed when
considering only T1 and T2) than younger adults. However, at
T3, participants’ scores on the Emotional loneliness scale tended
to align with those reported at T1, and ratings on the PANAS
scales were in between those reported at T1 and T2. Although
T3 results need to be considered with caution, they further
highlight the impact of restrictions imposing physical distancing
on individuals’ emotional and psychological functioning and
call upon the need to examine the effects of this emergency in
the longer term.
It is worth mentioning that, though restrictions might be
seen to have likely impacted young adults’ usual lifestyle habits
the most, our older adult sample also reported to have a quite
active lifestyle (i.e., engagement in several leisure, social and
physical activities), and have had to cope with disrupted daily
routines during the lockdown. Moreover, these results were
found even though older adults reported greater worries of
suffering severe consequences (i.e., being hospitalized or dying)
due to the contagion than younger adults, but lower concerns
about being infected by COVID-19, as suggested by the scores
for the BCC and the CCS. Findings, therefore, are in line with the
SST (Carstensen et al., 2003): the adaptive changes in emotion
regulation that characterize older age (in which emotionally
meaningful and positive goals and experiences are prioritized)
might explain why, although both groups were worried about
the COVID-19, older adults, compared to younger adults, were
more able to regulate their emotional reactions, especially the
negative affects also related to emotional loneliness. Furthermore,
such a greater emotion regulation, along with several major life
events faced throughout their life (and therefore a bigger wealth
of experience), might also explain why older adults reported
a higher resilience compared to younger adults (e.g., Rossi
et al., 2021), thereby feeling able to face, cope, and adapt more
easily than their younger counterparts to such an unexpected,
unpredictable and stressful situation.
Intriguingly, when considering the perceived amount of social
support, expressed by the social loneliness facet, young and older
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 6September 2021 | Volume 13 | Article 712369
fnagi-13-712369 September 4, 2021 Time: 17:34 # 7
Carbone et al. Emotions, Cognition Throughout COVID-19 Pandemic
TABLE 2 | Results of the linear mixed-effects models including mood and beliefs
and consequences of contagion scales as covariates.
Measure of
interest
Effect χ2df p
PANAS-positive
emotions
Mood 76.594 1 <0.001
Beliefs of contagion 0.550 1 0.458
Consequences of contagion 0.445 1 0.505
Age group 1.889 1 0.169
Time 17.042 1 <0.001
Age group ×Time 0.566 1 0.452
PANAS-negative
emotions
Mood 84.676 1 <0.001
Beliefs of contagion 4.539 1 0.033
Consequences of contagion 0.145 1 0.703
Age group 30.135 1 <0.001
Time 7.843 1 0.005
Age group ×Time 0.562 1 0.453
Emotional
loneliness
Mood 90.586 1 <0.001
Beliefs of contagion 3.126 1 0.077
Consequences of contagion 0.004 1 0.949
Age group 11.990 1 <0.001
Time 32.221 1 <0.001
Age group ×Time 5.581 1 0.018
Social loneliness Mood 16.328 1 <0.001
Beliefs of contagion 1.928 1 0.165
Consequences of contagion 2.482 1 0.115
Age group 1.034 1 0.309
Time 5.142 1 0.023
Age group ×Time 0.603 1 0.438
Resilience Mood 69.851 1 <0.001
Beliefs of contagion 2.663 1 0.103
Consequences of contagion 0.152 1 0.697
Age group 23.276 1 <0.001
Time 0.629 1 0.428
Age group ×Time 0.419 1 0.518
Backward Digit
Span task
Mood 1.626 1 0.202
Beliefs of contagion 0.066 1 0.797
Consequences of contagion 5.929 1 0.015
Age group 28.712 1 <0.001
Time 0.113 1 0.736
Age group ×Time 0.936 1 0.333
Word
list-immediate recall
Mood 3.587 1 0.058
Beliefs of contagion 3.672 1 0.055
Consequences of contagion 1.568 1 0.210
Age group 10.422 1 0.001
Time 2.608 1 0.106
Age group ×Time 3.016 1 0.082
Word list-delayed
recall
Mood 2.869 1 0.090
Beliefs of contagion 6.561 1 0.010
Consequences of contagion 2.032 1 0.154
Age group 10.014 1 0.002
Time 2.610 1 0.106
Age group ×Time 2.173 1 0.140
adults reported to perceive a higher lack of social support at
T2 than at T1, a pattern that emerged when considering mood
and concerns of COVID-19 effects and which disappeared when
also T3 was considered. The enhanced sense of community (a
shared feeling of “everyone being in it together”), emerged with
the COVID-19 outbreak, together with the use of technology
to maintain social contacts and interactions, might have led
individuals to feel connected with their support network,
especially even when physically isolated (e.g., Luchetti et al.,
2020). We did not ascertain neither participants’ feeling of
connectedness nor their social media usage, however, therefore
these are only speculations that merit further investigations.
Regarding cognitive outcomes, our findings do not seem to
align with previous evidence (Fiorenzato et al., 2021;Reading
Turchioe et al., 2021), which included self-reported measures of
cognitive complaints leading to an over or underestimation of
cognitive changes compared to objective outcomes implemented
in this study. The well-known age-related differences in favor
of younger adults (e.g., Park, 2002;Borella et al., 2007) were
confirmed for both the WM measure and the word list recall
tasks. A different pattern of results emerged depending on the
memory outcomes considered when the different timepoints
effects were examined. Performance in the WM measure was
not affected by restrictions, regardless of controlling or not for
mood and COVID-19 concerns. Long-term memory outcomes,
instead, showed an improvement between T1 and T2. However,
these latter outcomes seemed to be particularly susceptible to
the influence of mood (that emerged more clearly when also
considering T3) and COVID-19 concerns, which overshadowed
any timepoints effects between T1 and T2. Such a pattern
of results suggests how, rather than lockdown restrictions per
se, mood changes and worries related to stressful situations
might impact memory outcomes based on the demands and the
content of the tasks considered, particularly long-term memory
ones considered here. Moreover, associations between positive
changes in terms of emotional functioning and a better memory
performance emerged for younger adults only. These results
might suggest how the less adaptive coping and emotional
responses displayed by younger adults influenced their memory
performance, whereas the functional emotion regulation of older
adults might have allowed them to maintain (not worsen) their
memory performance. It is worth stressing that the relatively
short period of time (about 4 months between T1 and T2 and
about 6 months from T1 when considering T3) occurred between
the considered timepoints, might have prevented to clearly
observe the impact of the lockdown restrictions and the resulted
“impoverished” environment, as well as any interplay between
the psychological and emotional response to the emergency
and cognitive functioning in young and older adults. Further
investigations are therefore needed to ascertain whether the
ongoing stressful situation might have a fallout on cognitive
functioning in the longer term, also as a function of the different
psychological and emotional responses displayed by young and
older adults and the demands and content of the tasks in hand –
e.g., considering memory tasks with emotional stimuli, as well
as other cognitive functions, essential in everyday life (e.g.,
Meneghetti et al., 2011, 2014).
Despite these interesting findings, some limitations have to
be acknowledged. A pre-pandemic baseline assessment would
have helped to better depict the emotional, psychological,
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 7September 2021 | Volume 13 | Article 712369
fnagi-13-712369 September 4, 2021 Time: 17:34 # 8
Carbone et al. Emotions, Cognition Throughout COVID-19 Pandemic
and cognitive fallout of such stressful and extraordinary
times throughout the different pandemic phases (in terms of
restrictions). Although one of our study’s strengths consists
of having data not limited to the restrictions period per se,
results on T3 should be considered with caution and for
qualitative purposes only, due to the small sample of individuals
completing the assessment. However, based on our results, and
since the pandemic is still ongoing, it seems paramount to keep
ascertaining the psychological, emotional, and cognitive fallout
of such extraordinary times also as a function of the changes in
the restrictions that lead to an unpredictable variation of lifestyle
habits, and availability of cognitive stimulating experiences. In
doing so, another aspect that should be considered is whether
the spread of COVID-19 (in terms of number of contagions
in different geographic areas) affected individuals’ emotional
and psychological experience and reactions to the emergency
differently. We attempted to control this aspect by administering
the two Fear of COVID-19 scales, created to assess individuals’
concerns toward being infected by COVID-19 and suffering
from severe consequences due to contagion. Moreover, other
individual characteristics (e.g., personality dispositions) and
protective or risk factors (i.e., coping styles), as well as other
cognitive functions, not examined in this study, should be
considered in the future as they might help delineate individuals
at risk of suffering from negative consequences when this
emergency will end.
Overall, our results suggest that older adults take advantage of
their emotional strengths and resilience even when facing new,
unexpected, and prolonged threats as the COVID-19 pandemic.
However, findings also call upon the need to implement
interventions that promote functional coping strategies to
improve emotion regulation, resilience, and the quality of social
relationships, especially for young adults, during unforeseeable
and prolonged circumstances that impose physical distancing.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be
made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
ETHICS STATEMENT
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and
approved by the Ethical Committee for the Psychological
Research of the School of Psychology, University of Padova,
Padua, Italy. The patients/participants provided their written
informed consent to participate in this study.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
EC contributed to supervising the data collection, analyzing and
interpreting the data, and writing the manuscript. RP contributed
to designing the study, interpreting the data, and writing
the manuscript. ES supervised the data collection and drafted
the manuscript. GL contributed to enrolling and interviewing
participants, supervising the data collection, and organizing the
database. ADD contributed to designing the study and drafted
the manuscript. EB designed the study, contributed to analyzing,
interpreting the data, and writing the manuscript. All authors
read and approved the final manuscript.
FUNDING
This study was supported by a grant Visiting Programme from
Cassa di Risparmio di Padova e Rovigo (Cariparo) Foundation
to EB. This work was also carried out within the scope of the
“use-inspired basic research” project for which the Department
of General Psychology at the University of Padova has been
recognized as a “Dipartimento di eccellenza” by the Italian
Ministry for the University and Research.
SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found
online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.
2021.712369/full#supplementary-material
REFERENCES
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., and Ball, R. (1996). Comparison of beck depression
inventories IA and II in psychiatric outpatients. J. Pers. Assess. 67, 588–597.
doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13
Boals, A., and Banks, J. B. (2020). Stress and cognitive functioning during a
pandemic: thoughts from stress researchers. Psychol. Trauma 12, S255–S257.
doi: 10.1037/tra0000716
Borella, E., Carretti, B., and Cornoldi, C. (2007). Working memory, control of
interference and everyday experience of thought interference: when age makes
the difference. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 19, 200–206. doi: 10.1007/BF03324690
Borella, E., Pezzuti, L., De Beni, R., and Cornoldi, C. (2019). Intelligence and
working memory: evidence from administering the WAIS-IV to Italian adults
and elderly. Psychol. Res. 84, 1622–1634. doi: 10.1007/s00426-019-01173- 7
Bu, F., Steptoe, A., and Fancourt, D. (2020). Loneliness during a strict
lockdown: trajectories and predictors during the COVID-19 pandemic in
38,217 United Kingdom adults. Soc. Sci. Med. 265:113521. doi: 10.1101/2020.
05.29.20116657
Burr, D. A., Castrellon, J. J., Zald, D. H., and Samanez-Larkin, G. R. (2021).
Emotion dynamics across adulthood in everyday life: older adults are more
emotionally stable and better at regulating desires. Emotion 21, 453–464. doi:
10.1037/emo0000734
Campbell-Sills, L., and Stein, M. B. (2007). Psychometric analysis and
refinement of the Connor–Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC): validation
of a 10-item measure of resilience. J. Trauma Stress 20, 1019–1028.
doi: 10.1002/jts.20271
Capotosto, E., Belacchi, C., Gardini, S., Faggian, S., Piras, F., Mantoan, V., et al.
(2017). Cognitive stimulation therapy in the Italian context: its efficacy in
cognitive and non-cognitive measures in older adults with dementia. Int. J.
Geriatr. Psychiatry 32, 331–340. doi: 10.1002/gps.4521
Carretti, B., Borella, E., Zavagnin, M., and De Beni, R. (2011). Impact of
metacognition and motivation on the efficacy of strategic memory training
in older adults: analysis of specific, transfer and maintenance effects. Arch.
Gerontol. Geriatr. 52, e192–e197. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2010.11.004
Carstensen, L. L., Fung, H. H., and Charles, S. T. (2003). Socioemotional selectivity
theory and the regulation of emotion in the second half of life. Motiv. Emot. 27,
103–123. doi: 10.1023/A:1024569803230
Carstensen, L. L., Shavit, Y. Z., and Barnes, J. T. (2020). Age advantages in
emotional experience persist even under threat from the COVID-19 pandemic.
Psychol. Sci. 31, 1374–1385.
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 8September 2021 | Volume 13 | Article 712369
fnagi-13-712369 September 4, 2021 Time: 17:34 # 9
Carbone et al. Emotions, Cognition Throughout COVID-19 Pandemic
Ceccato, I., Palumbo, R., Di Crosta, A., La Malva, P., Marchetti, D., Maiella, R.,
et al. (2020). Age-related differences in the perception of COVID-19 emergency
during the Italian outbreak. Aging Ment. Health 25, 1305–1313. doi: 10.1080/
13607863.2020.1856781
Charles, S. T., and Carstensen, L. L. (2010). Social and emotional aging. Annu. Rev.
Psychol. 61, 383–409.
De Beni, R., Borella, E., and Carretti, B. (2008). Portfolio Per la Valutazione del
Benessere e Delle AbilitaÌ Cognitive Nell’etaÌ Adulta e Avanzata [The Assessment
of Well- Being and Cognitive Abilities in Adulthood and Aging]. Firenze: Giunti
OS.
De Pue, S., Gillebert, C., Dierckx, E., Vanderhasselt, M. A., De Raedt, R., and Van
den Bussche, E. (2021). The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on wellbeing
and cognitive functioning of older adults. Sci. Rep. 11:4636. doi: 10.1038/
s41598-021- 84127-7
Di Crosta, A., Palumbo, R., Marchetti, D., Ceccato, I., La Malva, P., Maiella, R.,
et al. (2020). Individual differences, economic stability, and fear of contagion as
risk factors for PTSD symptoms in the COVID-19 emergency. Front. Psychol.
11:2329. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.567367
Fairfield, B., Mammarella, N., Palumbo, R., and Domenico, D. A. (2015). Emotional
meta-memories: a review. Brain Sci. 5, 509–520. doi: 10.3390/brainsci5040509
Fiorenzato, E., Zabberoni, S., Costa, A., and Cona, G. (2021).
Cognitive and mental health changes and their vulnerability factors
related to COVID-19 lockdown in Italy. PLoS One 16:e0246204.
doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0246204
Gamboz, N., Borella, E., and Brandimonte, M. A. (2009). The role of switching,
inhibition and working memory in older adults’ performance in the Wisconsin
Card Sorting Test. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 16, 260–284. doi: 10.1080/
13825580802573045
Holmes, E. A., O’Connor, R. C., Perry, V. H., Tracey, I., Wessely, S., Arseneault, L.,
et al. (2020). Multidisciplinary research priorities for the COVID-19 pandemic:
a call for action for mental health science. Lancet Psychiatry 7, 547–560. doi:
10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30168- 1
Lockenhoff, C. E., and Carstensen, L. L. (2004). Socioemotional selectivity theory,
aging, and health: the increasingly delicate balance between regulating emotions
and making tough choices. J. Pers. 72, 1395–1424. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.
2004.00301.x
Luchetti, M., Lee, J. H., Aschwanden, D., Sesker, A., Strickhouser, J. E., Terracciano,
A., et al. (2020). The trajectory of loneliness in response to COVID-19. Am.
Psychol. 75, 897–908. doi: 10.1037/amp0000690
McCleskey, J., and Gruda, D. (2021). Risk-taking, resilience, and state anxiety
during the COVID-19 pandemic: a coming of (old) age story. Pers. Individ. Dif.
170:110485. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110485
Meneghetti, C., Borella, E., Pastore, M., and De Beni, R. (2014). The role of spatial
abilities and self-assessments in cardinal point orientation across the lifespan.
Learn Individ. Differ. 35, 113–121. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2014.07.006
Meneghetti, C., Fiore, F., Borella, E., and De Beni, R. (2011). Learning a map of
environment: the role of visuo-spatial abilities in young and older adults. Appl.
Cogn. Psychol. 25, 952–959. doi: 10.1002/acp.1788
Park, D. C. (2002). Aging, cognition, and culture: a neuroscientific perspective.
Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 26, 859–867. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00072-6
Pinheiro, J. C., and Bates, D. M. (2000). “Linear mixed-effects models: basic
concepts and examples,” in Mixed-Effects Models in S and S-Plus, eds J. C.
Pinheiro and D. M. Bates (New York, NY: Springer-Verlag), 3–56. doi: 10.1007/
0-387- 22747-4_1
Reading Turchioe, M., Grossman, L. V., Myers, A. C., Pathak, J., and Creber,
R. M. (2021). Correlates of mental health symptoms among US adults during
COVID-19. Public Health Rep. 136, 97–106. doi: 10.1177/0033354920970179
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., and Park, D. C. (2014). How does it STAC up? Revisiting
the scaffolding theory of aging and cognition. Neuropsychol. Rev. 24, 355–370.
doi: 10.1007/s11065-014- 9270-9
Rossi, R., Jannini, T. B., Socci, V., Pacitti, F., and Lorenzo, G. D. (2021). Stressful
life events and resilience during the COVID-19 lockdown measures in Italy:
association with mental health outcomes and age. Front. Psychiatry 12:236.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.635832
Scott, S. B., Graham-Engeland, J. E., Engeland, C. G., Smyth, J. M., Almeida, D. M.,
Katz, M. J., et al. (2015). The effects of stress on cognitive aging, physiology and
emotion (ESCAPE) project. BMC Psychiatry 15:146. doi: 10.1186/s12888-015-
0497-7
Varga, T. V., Bu, F., Dissing, A. S., Elsenburg, L. K., Herranz Bustamante,
J. J., Matta, J., et al. (2021). Loneliness, worries, anxiety, and precautionary
behaviours in response to the COVID-19 pandemic: a longitudinal analysis of
200,000 Western and Northern Europeans. Lancet Reg. Health Eur. 2:100020.
doi: 10.1016/j.lanepe.2020.100020
Wang, C., Pan, R., Wan, X., Tan, Y., Xu, L., McIntyre, R. S., et al. (2020). A
longitudinal study on the mental health of general population during the
COVID-19 epidemic in China. Brain Behav. Immun. 87, 40–48. doi: 10.1016/
j.bbi.2020.04.028
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and
validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS
scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.
1063
Wittich, W., Phillips, N., Nasreddine, Z. S., and Chertkow, H. (2010). Sensitivity
and specificity of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment modified for individuals
who are visually impaired. J. Vis. Impair. Blind 104, 360–368. doi: 10.1177/
0145482X1010400606
Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., et al.
(1982). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale:
a preliminary report. J. Psychiatr. Res. 17, 37–49. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(82)
90033-4
Young, N. A., Waugh, C. E., Minton, A. R., Charles, S. T., Haase, C. M., Mikels,
J. A., et al. (2021). Reactive, agentic, apathetic, or challenged? Aging, emotion,
and coping during the COVID-19 pandemic. Gerontologist 61, 217–227. doi:
10.1093/geront/gnaa196
Conflict of Interest: The authors declare that the research was conducted in the
absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a
potential conflict of interest.
The reviewer MF declared a shared affiliation, though no other collaboration, with
several of the authors RP and ADD to the handling editor.
Publisher’s Note: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors
and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of
the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in
this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or
endorsed by the publisher.
Copyright © 2021 Carbone, Palumbo, Sella, Lenti, Di Domenico and Borella. This
is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons
Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums
is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited
and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted
academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not
comply with these terms.
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org 9September 2021 | Volume 13 | Article 712369
... Only a few studies looked at the impact of the early stages of the pandemic on cognitive functioning. Overall, they showed a decline in long-term memory [7] associated with pandemic-related worries, and mild declines in subjective cognitive functioning [8]. ...
... For older adults, longitudinal studies on wellbeing showed mixed ndings. In line with observations for the general population, some studies observed that stress due to isolation and pandemic worry uctuated with the severity of the pandemic [7], [17]. Living with a partner and stronger relationships with family and friends played a protective role in these uctuations over time [17]. ...
... For wellbeing, our results were in line with other research, showing uctuations in wellbeing mimicking uctuations in pandemic severity [7], [17]. However, our study showed that levels of wellbeing during the last phase (M3) were still signi cantly lower than before the pandemic, indicating a sustained impact of the pandemic. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
In earlier survey research, we observed a severe impact of the first peak of the COVID-19 pandemic on the subjective wellbeing, sleep and activity of adults aged 65 years or older in Flanders, Belgium. The impact on subjective cognitive functioning, however, was limited. Since then, periods of lockdown and periods with less strict regulations alternated, but social distancing remained, especially for older adults. To study the longer-term impact of the pandemic on wellbeing and cognitive functioning, we re-assessed the older adults from the first measurement moment ( n = 371) in a second (June-July 2020) and third (December 2020) wave of the survey. Results indicated that wellbeing fluctuated with the severity of the pandemic. Results for self-reported cognitive functioning were mixed. While participants indicated a slightly better general cognitive functioning at the end of the study, experienced problems with most cognitive subdomains significantly increased over time. The presence of depressive and anxiety symptoms influenced the longer-term impact of the pandemic on wellbeing and cognitive functioning. Our study shows the long-lasting impact of the pandemic on the wellbeing and cognitive functioning of older adults, without full recovery from the first wave.
... Only a few studies looked at the impact of the early stages of the pandemic on cognitive functioning in older adults. Overall, they showed a decline in long-term memory 13 associated with pandemic-related worries, and mild declines in subjective cognitive functioning 14 . From this, we can conclude that the first peak of the pandemic significantly decreased wellbeing and certain aspects of cognitive functioning to a limited extent. ...
... Specifically for older adults, longitudinal studies on wellbeing showed mixed findings. In line with observations for the general population, some studies observed that stress due to isolation and pandemic worry fluctuated with the severity of the pandemic 13,21 . Living with a partner and stronger relationships with family and friends played a protective role in these fluctuations over time 21 . ...
Article
Full-text available
In earlier survey research, we observed a severe impact of the first peak of the COVID-19 pandemic on the subjective wellbeing, sleep and activity of adults aged 65 years or older in Flanders, Belgium. The impact on subjective cognitive functioning, however, was limited. Since then, periods of lockdown and periods with less strict regulations alternated, but social distancing remained, especially for older adults. To study the longer-term impact of the pandemic on wellbeing and subjective cognitive functioning, we re-assessed the older adults from the first measurement moment (May–June 2020) in a second (June–July 2020) and third (December 2020) wave of the survey (n = 371, M = 72 years old, range 65–97 years old). Results indicated that wellbeing fluctuated with the severity of the pandemic. Results for self-reported cognitive functioning were mixed. While participants indicated a slightly better general subjective cognitive functioning at the end of the study, experienced problems with most cognitive subdomains significantly increased over time. The presence of depressive and anxiety symptoms were related to the longer-term impact of the pandemic on wellbeing and subjective cognitive functioning. Our study shows the long-lasting impact of the pandemic on the wellbeing and subjective cognitive functioning of older adults, without full recovery from the first wave.
... Several studies indicate that the COVID-19 pandemic has had a significant impact, albeit temporary, on the personality profile, well-being, and educational performance of secondary school students. This impact is attributed to cognitive decrease observed during the period of confinement [6,34,35]. Hammerstein [36] indicates that cognitive regression is primarily caused by social isolation resulting from the emergence of social confinement. This isolation significantly affects the risks of anxiety and depression, even after individuals have become accustomed to the environmental changes in their lifestyle. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to assess whether students’ learning styles have changed due to the environmental context changes caused by the COVID-19 pandemic outbreak. This analysis is important because learning styles, in addition to predicting vocational outcomes, must inform the design of pedagogical practices and supports used in the teaching environment. To achieve this objective, a cross-sectional analysis was conducted on three cohorts of students who enrolled in an engineering course. Students in group 2 have been under confinement conditions during the previous year, while students in group 3 have been under confinement conditions for the previous two years. Group 1 consists of students who did not experience any confinement. The obtained results lead to the following conclusions: (i) The pandemic outbreak has led to environmental changes, pedagogical practices, and changes in the personality profiles of students, which can significantly impact the learning styles of students entering an engineering degree. (ii) While some learning styles are more susceptible to change, others remain resilient. (iii) The duration of exposure to the environmental changes caused by the pandemic outbreak has a greater impact on the changes in these learning styles.
... Studies revealed that older adults tend to report greater emotional well-being than younger people [19,20] and better coping strategies [21]. The latter may be due to the fact that the ability of young people to cope with distressed situations and emotions may not be fully developed yet [22,23]. Research further showed that the elderly had less negativity towards the COVID-19 crisis than younger adults [24]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Coronavirus 2019 (COVID-19) lockdown periods had a significant negative impact on people’s lives and psychological well-being. However, the impact of lockdowns differed between individuals. This study aimed to identify vulnerable groups and investigated the relationship between mood and perceived immune fitness and the number and severity of coronavirus 2019 (COVID-19) symptoms during the first COVID-19 lockdown in the Netherlands. In addition, the impact of emotion regulation and other preventive measures was considered. The aim of the study was to identify possible differences according to sex, age, and the presence of underlying disease. A two-part online survey among N = 1415 individuals of the Dutch population (18 to 94 years old) was conducted in the summer of 2020. N = 541 of these participants also completed part 2 of the survey. A series of questionnaires was completed on mood, quality of life, lifestyle, immune fitness, and the number and severity of COVID-19 symptoms. Retrospectively, the period before the first lockdown (15 January–14 March 2020) was compared with the first lockdown (15 March–11 May 2020). The analysis revealed that the lockdown period was associated with significantly poorer mood, poorer immune fitness, and reduced quality of life. Poorer mood was associated with a significantly reduced immune fitness and a significant increase in the number and severity of COVID-19 symptoms. Mood changes did not differ significantly between men and women. Some mood effects were significantly more pronounced for individuals with underlying diseases (depression, fatigue, and stress) and younger individuals (depression and loneliness). Regarding lifestyle factors, no significant lockdown effects were seen according to underlying disease status. During the lockdown period, women reported a decline in nutrition scores, which was not seen in men, whereas they reported receiving more support from family and friends than men. Regarding age, younger individuals reported a significantly greater negative impact on physical activity and being active than the older participants. No differential effects for the groups were found for health correlates. In conclusion, significant negative lockdown effects on mood, quality of life, and immune fitness were observed across the population. The effects were significantly more pronounced among young individuals and those with underlying disease.
... Given that age is one of the main risk factors for serious illness or death due to COVID-19 (Esai Selvan, 2020), it is not surprising that it is related to enhanced COVID-19 related distress. In addition, the restrictions imposed on the population affect the daily lives of younger adults (i.e., working from home, not being able to go to school or university, resulting in loneliness) differently than older adults (Carbone et al., 2021;Varma et al., 2021), which may result in more general distress in the younger population (e.g., Ribeiro et al., 2021;Rutland-Lawes et al., 2021;Varma et al., 2021;Xiong et al., 2020). Furthermore, women showed higher scores on traumatic stress symptoms, and general stress, which is in line with previous research showing women to be more vulnerable towards (COVID-19 related) mental health problems (Ribeiro et al., 2021;Taylor et al., 2020a;Xiong et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Trait mindfulness could function as a protective factor against distress. The current study investigated the relationship of the five distinct facets of mindfulness and COVID-19 related distress. Methods An online self-report study was conducted in a Dutch sample (N = 811; Mage = 49 ± 17; 54.5 % female) in February 2021. Dispositional mindfulness was assessed with the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire, and the COVID Stress Scale was used to assess COVID-19 related distress. Results Multivariate regression analyses showed that describing and non-judging were inversely related to COVID-19 related distress. Specifically, describing was related to fewer socioeconomic worries, while describing and non-judging were negatively associated with traumatic stress symptoms. Non-judging was negatively associated with COVID-19-related compulsive checking. Conclusions The results indicate that individuals high in mindfulness traits describing and non-judging, experience less distress during the pandemic. Teaching mindfulness skills and practice could be helpful in global situations like pandemics.
... Wellness resulting from exposure to the natural environment is both physical and psychological. The latter aspect is one of the impacted factors during and after the COVID-19 pandemic, with differences depending on age [2]. Climate and environmental ...
Article
Full-text available
The post-pandemic era has raised awareness on the importance of physical and psychological well-being for decreasing the vulnerability of both individuals and populations. Citizens in urban areas are subject to numerous stress factors which can be mitigated by green spaces such as parks and gardens. Sensor and internet technologies support nature-based solutions in various ways. In this paper, we show the results of ongoing research on the use of spatially distributed IoT sensors that collect climate data in an ~8 ha urban garden. The novelty resides in the method for merging the IoT data with a detailed 3D model created by a laser scan survey from a drone flight. The end products are 1 m resolution thermal comfort maps of user-defined scenarios, e.g., at specific times or aggregated in daily/monthly/yearly statistics that represent a thermal comfort distribution. For full replicability, the code is open source and available as an R package on Github.
... Some studies, on the contrary, showed lower levels of anxiety and depression, and they motivated this result with a possible higher resilience and coping mechanisms in older people in comparison to the younger population or with a greater acceptance of death with maturity developed by age [24,25]. Moreover, when one considers resilience in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, a few studies have shown that older people seem to better manage their emotional and psychological consequences [26][27][28][29]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The COVID-19 health emergency and restrictive measures have increased psychological problems, particularly anxiety and depression, in the general population. However, little is known about mental health conditions and the possible risk and protective factors of specific population groups, such as institutionalized vs. community-dwelling elderly. We investigated the abovementioned aspects in a sample of 65–89-year-old people during the third wave of COVID-19 in Italy. We employed a sociodemographic survey and four questionnaires on health-related quality of life (SF-36), loneliness (UCLA), spirituality (FACIT-Sp), and anxiety/depression (HADS). Our findings suggest that the physical, psychological, and spiritual well-being of the elderly had not been seriously impaired by the events related to the pandemic, although most of the participants reported a worsening of their social life and a moderate/high fear of COVID-19. In regression analyses, these two latter aspects turned out to be predictors of higher anxiety, while spiritual well-being and the possibility to get out of the house/institution emerged as protective factors against anxiety and for preserving quality of life, respectively. Our findings help refine the picture of the condition of the elderly in the aftermath of the pandemic, giving some hints about how to continue supporting their well-being and quality of life.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Older adults are at a greater risk for contracting and experiencing severe illness from COVID-19 and may be further affected by pandemic-related precautions (e.g., social distancing and isolation in quarantine). However, the longitudinal impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on older adults is unclear. The current study examines changes in health behaviors, psychosocial factors, and cognitive functioning in a large sample of older adults using a pre-pandemic baseline and longitudinal follow-up throughout 9 months of the COVID-19 pandemic. Methods: One hundred and eighty-nine older adults (ages 65-89) were recruited from a multisite clinical trial to complete additional virtual assessments during the COVID-19 pandemic. Mixed effects models evaluated changes in health behaviors, psychosocial factors, and cognitive functioning during the pandemic compared to a pre-pandemic baseline and over the course of the pandemic (i.e., comparing the first and last COVID-19 timepoints). Results: Compared to their pre-pandemic baseline, during the pandemic, older adults reported worsened sleep quality, perceived physical health and functioning, mental health, slight increases in depression and apathy symptoms, reduced social engagement/perceived social support, but demonstrated better performance on objective cognitive tasks of attention and working memory. Throughout the course of the pandemic, these older adults reported continued worsening of perceived physical health and function, fewer depression symptoms, and they demonstrated improved cognitive performance. It is important to note that changes on self-report mood measures and cognitive performance were relatively small regarding clinical significance. Education largely served as a protective factor, such that greater years of education was generally associated with better outcomes across domains. Conclusions: The present study provides insights into the longitudinal impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on health behaviors, psychosocial factors, and cognitive functioning in a population disproportionately affected by the virus. Replicating this study design in a demographically representative older adult sample is warranted to further inform intervention strategies targeting older adults negatively impacted by the COVID-19 pandemic.
Article
Full-text available
Background: The COVID-19 pandemic, due to its disproportionated higher morbidity and mortality rates in the older age, has been considered to be a “geropandemic.” Several studies, however, have found that older age is associated with lower psychological distress in relation to the COVID-19 outbreak and related lockdown measures. Aim: To explore the role of Resilience as a mediator between stressful COVID-19 related life events and depressive and, anxiety symptoms and perceived stress, and to ascertain the role of age as a moderator of the mediator's effect. Methods: An on-line survey was spread through social networks during the first lockdown in Italy. Depressive and anxiety symptoms and perceived stress were measured using the Italian version of the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9), the Generalized Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire-7 (GAD-7) and the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS). Resilience was measured using the Italian version of the Resilience Scale for Adults (RSA). Stressful COVID-19 related life events were explored using a checklist of events derived from the International Adjustment Disorder Questionnaire (IADQ). After a preliminary panel of linear regressions, mediation was tested using Structural Equation Modeling and inspecting the bootstrapped indirect effects. Afterwards, age was introduced as a mediator of the indirect effect in a moderated mediation analysis. Results: Twenty one thousand three hundred and thirty four subjects completed the questionnaire, 17,178 (80.52%) were female, 748 (3.5%) were >60 years old. In the whole sample, the presence of any stressful event was associated with depressive and anxiety symptoms and perceived stress. Resilience mediated the effects of stressful COVID-19-related events on depressive and anxiety symptoms and perceived stress. The moderated mediation analysis revealed that age moderated the mediation effect of Resilience between the presence of a stressful event and the selected outcomes. Conclusion: Taken together, our results show that age moderates the mediating effect of Resilience in the relationship between COVID-19-related stressful events and depressive and anxiety symptoms and perceived stress. Older adults' Resilience was less influenced by stressful events, and this could be one of the reasons accounting for the better mental health outcomes observed in the older age.
Article
Full-text available
COVID-19 took a heavy toll on older adults. In Belgium, by the end of August, 93% of deaths due to COVID-19 were aged 65 or older. Similar trends were observed in other countries. As a consequence, older adults were identified as a group at risk, and strict governmental restrictions were imposed on them. This has caused concerns about their mental health. Using an online survey, this study established the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on adults aged 65 years or older, and which factors moderate this impact. Participants reported a significant decrease in activity level, sleep quality and wellbeing during the COVID-19 pandemic. Depression was strongly related to reported declines in activity level, sleep quality, wellbeing and cognitive functioning. Our study shows that the COVID-19 pandemic had a severe impact on the mental health of older adults. This implies that this group at risk requires attention of governments and healthcare.
Article
Full-text available
The COVID-19 pandemic and government imposed social restrictions like lockdown exposed most individuals to an unprecedented stress, increasing mental health disorders worldwide. We explored subjective cognitive functioning and mental health changes and their possible interplay related to COVID-19-lockdown. We also investigated potential risk factors to identify more vulnerable groups. Across Italy, 1215 respondents completed our Qualtrics-based online-survey during the end of a seven to 10-week imposed lockdown and home confinement (from April 29 to May 17, 2020). We found subjective cognitive functioning and mental health severely changed in association with the lockdown. Under government regulations, cognitive complaints were mostly perceived in routine tasks involving attention, temporal orientation and executive functions—with no changes in language abilities. A paradoxical effect was observed for memory, with reduced forgetfulness compared to pre-lockdown. We found higher severity and prevalence of depression, anxiety disorders, abnormal sleep, appetite changes, reduced libido and health anxiety: with mild-to-severe depression and anxiety prevalence climbing to 32 and 36 percent, respectively, under restrictions. Being female, under 45 years, working from home or being underemployed were all identified as relevant risk factors for worsening cognition and mental health. Frequent consumers of COVID-19 mass media information or residents in highly infected communities reported higher depression and anxiety symptoms, particularly hypochondria in the latter. If similar restrictions are reimposed, governments must carefully consider these more vulnerable groups in their decisions, whilst developing effective global and long-term responses to the cognitive and mental health challenges of this type of pandemic; as well as implementing appropriate psychological interventions with specific guidelines: particularly regarding exposure to COVID-19 mass-media reports.
Article
Full-text available
Background In response to the COVID-19 pandemic, governments around the world instituted various public-health measures. Our project aimed to highlight the most significant similarities and differences in key mental-health indicators between four Western and Northern European countries, and identify the population subgroups with the poorest mental-health outcomes during the first months of the pandemic. Methods We analysed time-series survey data of 205,084 individuals from seven studies from Denmark, France, the Netherlands, and the UK to assess the impact of the pandemic and associated lockdowns. All analyses focused on the initial lockdown phase (March–July 2020). The main outcomes were loneliness, anxiety, and COVID-19-related worries and precautionary behaviours. Findings COVID-19-related worries were consistently high in each country but decreased during the gradual reopening phases. While only 7% of the respondents reported high levels of loneliness in the Netherlands, percentages were higher in the rest of the three countries (13-18%). In all four countries, younger individuals and individuals with a history of mental illness expressed the highest levels of loneliness. Interpretation The pandemic and associated country lockdowns had a major impact on the mental health of populations, and certain subgroups should be closely followed to prevent negative long-term consequences. Younger individuals and individuals with a history of mental illness would benefit from tailored public-health interventions to prevent or counteract the negative effects of the pandemic. Individuals across Western and Northern Europe have thus far responded in psychologically similar ways despite differences in government approaches to the pandemic. Funding See the Funding section.
Article
Full-text available
Background and Objectives Advanced age is generally associated with improved emotional well-being, but the COVID-19 pandemic unleashed a global stressor that gravely threatened the physical well-being and ostensibly challenged the emotional well-being of older adults disproportionately. The current study investigated differences in emotional experiences and coping strategies between younger and older adults during the pandemic, and whether these differences were accounted for by age differences in appraisal of the pandemic. Research Design and Methods We asked younger (n = 181) and older adult (n = 176) participants to report their stress, appraisals the pandemic, emotions, and the ways in which they were coping with the pandemic. Results Results indicated that older adults experienced less stress and less negative affect and used greater problem-focused coping and less avoidant coping in response to the pandemic than younger adults. Further, age differences in affect and coping were partially accounted for by age differences in appraisals of the pandemic. Discussion and Implications Despite their objectively higher risk of illness and death due to the pandemic, older adults experienced less negative affect and used more agentic coping strategies than younger adults.
Article
Full-text available
Rationale There are increasing worries that lockdowns and ‘stay-at-home’ orders due to the COVID-19 pandemic could lead to a rise in loneliness, which is recognised as a major public health concern. But profiles of loneliness during the pandemic and risk factors remain unclear. Objective The current study aimed to examine if and how loneliness levels changed during the strict lockdown and to explore the clustering of loneliness growth trajectories. Methods Data from 38,217 UK adults in the UCL COVID -19 Social Study (a panel study collecting data weekly during the pandemic) were analysed during the strict lockdown period in the UK (23/03/2020–10/05/2020). The sample was well-stratified and weighted to population proportions of gender, age, ethnicity, education and geographical location. Growth mixture modelling was used to identify the latent classes of loneliness growth trajectories and their predictors. Results Analyses revealed four classes, with the baseline loneliness level ranging from low to high. In the first a few weeks of lockdown, loneliness levels increased in the highest loneliness group, decreased in the lowest loneliness group, and stayed relatively constant in the middle two groups. Younger adults (OR = 2.17–6.81), women (OR = 1.59), people with low income (OR = 1.3), the economically inactive (OR = 1.3–2.04) and people with mental health conditions (OR = 5.32) were more likely to be in highest loneliness class relative to the lowest. Further, living with others or in a rural area, and having more close friends or greater social support were protective. Conclusions Perceived levels of loneliness under strict lockdown measures due to COVID-19 were relatively stable in the UK, but for many people these levels were high with no signs of improvement. Results suggest that more efforts are needed to address loneliness.
Article
Full-text available
The current COVID-19 pandemic has created an unprecedented hostile psychological environment for individuals. Against the backdrop of this exogenous shock and applying a Cumulative Prospect Theory framework, we examined a relationship between risk-taking, trait resilience, and state anxiety, wherein age moderates the relationship between trait resilience and risk-taking, on state anxiety during the pandemic. We assess risk-taking using a behavioral measure and assess trait anxiety, big five personality traits, and other demographic factors in a sample of 515 individuals in the United States. Regression analysis revealed that age moderates the relationship between risk-taking and state anxiety and that highly resilient, risk-tolerant individuals experience lower state anxiety than less resilient risk-averse individuals. In contrast, older, more resilient, risk-averse individuals experience lower state anxiety than their younger, more resilient, risk-averse counterparts. Study limitations are noted, and additional research is suggested.
Article
Objectives An understanding of mental health symptoms during the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic is critical to ensure that health policies adequately address the mental health needs of people in the United States. The objective of this study was to examine mental health symptoms among US adults in an early stage of the COVID-19 pandemic. Methods We conducted a cross-sectional study in late March 2020 with a national sample of 963 US adults using an online research platform. Participants self-reported state of residence, psychosocial characteristics, and levels of anxiety, depression, anger, cognitive function, and fatigue in the context of COVID-19 using validated patient-reported outcomes scales in the Patient-Reported Outcome Measurement Information System measures. We used analysis of variance and multivariate linear regression to evaluate correlates of mental health symptoms. Results Overall, participants reported high levels of anxiety (mean [SD], 57.2 [9.3]) and depression (mean [SD], 54.2 [9.5]). Levels of anger, anxiety, cognitive function, depression, and fatigue were significantly higher among the Millennial Generation and Generation X (vs Baby Boomers), those with not enough or enough (vs more than enough) financial resources, females vs males), those with self-reported disability (vs no self-reported disability), and those with inadequate (vs adequate) health literacy. In adjusted models, being in Generation X and the Millennial Generation (vs Baby Boomer), having not enough or enough vs more than enough) financial resources, and having inadequate (vs adequate) health literacy were most strongly correlated with worse mental health symptoms. Conclusions Results suggest that mental health symptoms during the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic were prevalent nationally, regardless of state of residence and especially among young, psychosocially vulnerable groups.
Article
Objectives Older adults have been identified as a high-risk population for COVID-19, therefore it is crucial to understand how they perceived and reacted to the emergency. We examined age-related differences in emotions, cognitive attitudes, and behavioral responses to the COVID-19 crisis. Based on the Socioemotional Selectivity Theory, we expected to find a positive approach in older adults, which may translate into lower compliance with restrictive measures. Methods: We analyzed data (n = 306) from a nation-wide online survey conducted between April 1st and April 16th, 2020. We compared young (18–29 years), middle-aged (30–50 years), and older (65–85 years) adults’ self-reported emotions, attitudes toward the emergency, and compliance with governmental rules. Results: Older adults showed lower negative emotions than young and middle-aged adults. Also, older adults were more confident about COVID-related information received, more favorable toward the restrictive measures, and perceived lower underestimation of the emergency compared to the other age groups. However, older people anticipated a longer time for the emergency to resolve. No age-related differences in compliance with the rules emerged. Conclusion: Older people showed a positive attitude toward the emergency. This attitude was confined in the here and now and did not extend to expectations for the future. Compliance with rules was high across our sample. However, less compliant individuals were also less confident in COVID-related information received by the media and official sources, suggesting the importance of providing precise and reliable information to promote adherence to restrictive measures.
Article
The COVID-19 pandemic is creating unprecedented, sustained, and unavoidable stress for the entire population, and older people are facing particularly heightened risk of contracting the virus and suffering severe complications, including death. The present study was conducted when the pandemic was spreading exponentially in the United States. To address important theoretical questions about age differences in emotional experience in times of crisis, we surveyed a representative sample of 945 Americans between the ages of 18 and 76 years and assessed the frequency and intensity of a range of positive and negative emotions. We also assessed perceived risk of contagion and complications from the virus, as well as personality, health, and demographic characteristics. Age was associated with relatively greater emotional well-being both when analyses did and did not control for perceived risk and other covariates. The present findings extend previous research about age and emotion by demonstrating that older adults’ relatively better emotional well-being persists even in the face of prolonged stress.