Available via license: CC BY-NC 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
CORRECTED PROOF
Journal of Future Robot Life -1 (2021) 1–10 1
DOI 10.3233/FRL-210010
IOS Press
Men with paraphilic interests and their desire to interact with a
sex robot
Alena Mareˇ
cková a,b,∗,∗∗, Renáta Androviˇ
cová a,b,∗∗, Klára Bártová b,∗∗,LucieKrej
ˇ
cová a,b,∗∗
and Kateˇ
rina Klapilová a,b,∗∗
aNational Institute of Mental Health, Czech Republic
bCharles University, Czech Republic
Abstract. Sex robots may present an opportunity for a clinical management of individuals with paraphilic interests whose
realisation would result in infliction of harm and/or legal consequences. We explored the prevalence of desire for sex robot
experience in a control group and two target groups: one with paraphilic interests in minors, the other with interest in
non-consent/violence. We expected both target groups to express a greater desire for sex robot experience than the control
group, because sex robots would enable them to express paraphilic sexuality within legal limitations. We used data from two
samples of male respondents. The control sample consisted of respondents from a representative online Czech sample (N =
806). Target groups of individuals with paraphilic interests consisted of 48 subjects sexually interested in minors and 57
subjects sexually interested in non-consent/violence against adults. Sex robot experience was desired by 18% of respondents
in the control group, 37.5% of respondents in the group interested in minors, and 26% of respondents interested in non-
consent/violence. This indicates that individuals with paraphilic interest in minors are slightly more open to the idea of sex
robot experience. Studies exploring preferred features of sex robots and interest in their use in therapy are needed.
Keywords: Sex robots, sex robot experience, paraphilic interest
1. INTRODUCTION
Sex robots are no longer just science fiction: recently, their development had accelerated and com-
panies are putting considerable effort in technical, behavioural, and visual innovations (Aoki and
Kimura, 2021). Sex robots can be defined as human-like, full-sized, anatomically correct humanoid
service robots that use a range of different materials and technologies and whose price range varies
widely. What they have in common is being designed for sexual use (Döring and Pöschl, 2018). They
can differ in apparent age and appearance (e.g., different eye colour, pubic hair, hair, skin colour,
makeup) and have several removable orifices for sexual use. Sex robots differ from their predeces-
sors, passive sex dolls, in applied artificial intelligence, which makes them active and responsive.
Synthetic speech and simple motor skills allow them to speak, move, and display a rudimentary per-
sonality. Although their range of behaviours is still limited, sex robots are likely to become highly
sophisticated in the near future.
Attitudes toward sex robot experience (SRE) in the general population have been explored by sev-
eral small, nonrepresentative, web-based surveys (Appel et al., 2019; Edirisinghe and Cheok, 2016;
Richards et al., 2016; Scheutz and Arnold, 2016;2017; Szczuka and Krämer, 2016). The mean age
of respondents in those studies ranged around 30–35 years. The results varied: for instance, in a US
*Corresponding author. E-mail: alena.mareckova@nudz.cz.
**Department of Psychology and Life Sciences, Faculty of Humanities, Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic
2589-9953 © 2021 – The authors. Published by IOS Press. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial License (CC BY-NC 4.0).
CORRECTED PROOF
2A. Mareˇcková et al. / Men with paraphilic interests and their desire to interact with a sex robot
sample of male and female respondents (N =100; 57 male respondents), 70% of male respondents in-
dicated they would use a sex robot (Scheutz and Arnold, 2016), but only 9% of Malaysian respondents
of both sexes (N =32; 16 male respondents) wanted to entertain this idea (Edirisinghe and Cheok,
2016). In a German study focused only on male respondents (N =263), 40.3% indicated they would
buy a sex robot now or within the next five years (Szczuka and Krämer, 2016). This indicates highly
varying rates of acceptance of SRE. In the US sample (Scheutz and Arnold, 2016), men were rather
accepting of SRE in most areas of use including paraphilic interests (PI), such as unusual sexual prac-
tices (e.g., rough sex or sadistic behaviour), but the use of child-like sex robots was strongly rejected
by respondents. In a very large Dutch sample (N =1,162), de Graaf and Allouch (2016) reported
that 22% of respondents thought that robotic sexual partners could decrease the incidence of sexual
assault and rape. Furthermore, 17% of participants indicated their belief that robotic sexual partners
could sexually assist special target groups, for instance people with social or sexual difficulties.
So far, no empirical study investigated the effects of SRE on their user´s partnerships as well as
emotional and sexual functioning. Some evidence is emerging from studies on sex doll use, but it
is fairly limited and cannot be generalised (Döring et al., 2020). A study on sex doll owners found
that they report more issues in their sexual functioning when the sexual activity involves a human
partner, while sex dolls provide them with sexual gratification and stimulation (Valverde, 2012). Other
studies likewise reported that sex dolls seem to have a soothing and healing effect on users, offer a
viable alternative to having a human partner (Knafo, 2015; Knafo and Bosco, 2017), and can alleviate
loneliness (Ciambrone et al., 2017).
There is currently a high clinical demand for evidence-based guidelines regarding interactions with
sex robots because it is an issue with implications for the physical, mental, and social wellbeing of
their users (Cox-George and Bewley, 2018).
2. THE USE OF SEX ROBOTS IN PREVENTION OF SEXUAL OFFENDING AND CLINICAL
MANAGEMENT OF PARAPHILIC DISORDERS
Within the context of sex robot use, two issues tend to attract the greatest controversy: the use of sex
robots to prevent actual sexual crimes – such as violent assault, rape, or sexual abuse of children –
and their potential use in clinical management of paraphilias.
Disorders of sexual preference (paraphilic disorders) are defined in the International Classification of
Diseases, 11th revision (World Health Organization, 2018) as ‘persistent, intense patterns of atypical
sexual arousal, manifested by sexual thoughts, fantasies, urges, or behaviours, the focus of which
involves others whose age or status renders them unwilling or unable to consent and on which the
person has acted or by which he or she is markedly distressed’. The presence of paraphilic interest
(PI) itself has no moral or legal implications, but acting upon it in a way that includes another person
presents a potential risk to public safety.
Individuals with PI in minors who deliberately suppress their sexual urges report that their primary
motivation for abstaining from offending behaviour is the awareness of harm it would inflict upon a
child as well as potential legal consequences for themselves (Walker, 2017). Identified strategies to
manage paraphilic urges include the viewing of images and videos of children depicted in mainstream
media or computer-generated images of children popularised by the Japanese culture (Walker, 2017),
maintenance of sexual contacts with adults, and adult pornography or fantasising during masturbation
(Houtepen et al., 2016). Child-like sex robot (or child-like sex dolls, e.g., a model by Trottla) could
CORRECTED PROOF
A. Mareˇcková et al. / Men with paraphilic interests and their desire to interact with a sex robot 3
be viewed as a legal surrogate of their desired partner which has not only a child-typical body scheme
but, unlike the child-like sex dolls, also the basic age-typical behavioural displays.
Sex robots, such as the Roxxxy model by True Companion with ‘Frigid Farrah’ personality, may offer
an outlet to individuals with PI in non-consent/violence, that is, people who are sexually aroused by
infliction of physical or mental pain or by overcoming the active resistance of others (Harris et al.,
2012;Setoetal.,2012). Robots such as the abovementioned model can mimic the basic signals of
non-consent but the range of emotions or reactions they can display (e.g., defiance, resistance, and
suffering) is currently fairly limited.
A recent study that mapped the rates of acceptance of sex robots by therapists and physicians in
Germany, Austria, and Switzerland (N =72), showed that 30% of approached experts could imagine
using sex robots for patients with paraphilic disorders (based on ICD-10), although contrasts between
respondents’ opinions on this subject were high. Respondents stressed that the benefits of SRE must
be decided individually for each specific patient. For some, it could offer an opportunity to express
their sexuality with robot as a substitute, whereas for others, it could intensify their sexual urges and
lower barriers to committing a sexual offence (Eichenberg et al., 2019). The survey also reported
significant differences in attitudes towards the use of sex robot in therapeutic setting between younger
and older therapists, with younger therapists being more open to the subject (Eichenberg et al., 2019).
Parallel information regarding the acceptance of and desire for SRE among potential users is largely
missing. The goal of this study is to fill in this knowledge gap: after all, willingness to engage with
sex robots would be a necessary prerequisite of any potential therapeutic use. Our aim is to explore
the level of desire to engage in SRE in individuals with PI, particularly those for whom finding a
desired partner is unrealistic and/or illegal (e.g., individuals with PI in minors) or at least difficult and
mostly unsatisfying because their partners’ sexual behaviour is not complementary to their PI (e.g.,
individuals with PI in non-consent and/or sexual violence).
First, we explore the percentage of men in a representative sample of Czech population interested in
SRE and the percentage of men with PI in minors and in non-consent/violence interested in SRE. We
focus solely on men because paraphilic interests seem far more prevalent amongst men than women
(Bártová et al., 2021; Bouchard et al., 2017). Secondly, we investigate the differences in SRE desire
between the abovementioned samples. In particular, we hypothesise that due to the impossibility to
fully express their innate sexual desires with actual people without harming a child/adult and breaking
the law, both of these groups with PI would express greater desire in SRE than men in the general
population (control group). Furthermore, we expect that younger participants would be more open
to the idea of SRE due to their generally greater acceptance of new technologies (Eichenberg et al.,
2019), which is why we will control for the effect of age in a complex model which analyses the
differences in the level of desire for SRE across the groups.
3. METHODS
The prevalence of SRE desire in the general population was assessed using a sample of Czech men
engaged in an online representative survey on ‘Love and intimacy in the Czech population’. The
sampling was conducted by a respected sociodemographic agency. Separately, a sample of men who
met the criteria of PI in minors and PI in non-consent/violence was pre-screened from an online
pool of Czech respondents to assess the prevalence of SRE in the two target groups. All participants
completed a questionnaire which contained a question regarding their desire for SRE. They expressed
their level of agreement with the following statement ‘I desire sexual experience with a sex robot/doll.’
CORRECTED PROOF
4A. Mareˇcková et al. / Men with paraphilic interests and their desire to interact with a sex robot
using a 4-category ordinal scale (strongly disagree, disagree, agree, strongly agree). The study was
approved by the Ethics Committee of the National Institute of Mental Health under the Nr. 41/15.
3.1. A representative sample of Czech men (control group)
Respondents were recruited online from a national pool of respondents using the following strata:
sex, age, education, region, and size of the place of residence to represent general Czech adult male
population. The study was part of a survey project ‘Love and intimacy in the Czech population’, which
took place on 8–31 January 2020. All respondents were Czech citizens, fluent in Czech, and 18+years
of age. The sampling procedure was conducted by a respected sociodemographic agency, STEM /
MARK (www.stemmark.cz), using a Czech National Panel of 55,000 active panellists and a Dialog
panel of 10,000 active panellists (https://www.nationalpanel.eu/). The procedure was compliant with
the ICC/ESOMAR International Code of Marketing and Social Research (https://www.esomar.org/).
For detailed information on the sampling procedure, see Table 1.
Tab le 1
The procedure of selection of male respondents, with interim and final sample size and total response rate
Stages of the selection process, reason for exclusion N
Approached 3,748
Not surveyed due to quota fulfilment 15
Survey not completed 258
Complete answers entered into analyses 806
Response rate 22%
3.2. The sample of men with PI
In parallel with the national representative survey, respondents with PI were pre-screened from the
Czech National Panel. Men who participated in a national survey of sexual behaviour and preferences
in 2016 (see Bártová et al., 2021) were asked whether they would be willing to participate in a project
mapping the sexual behaviour and needs of people with PI for a newly launched project of primary
prevention of sexual offending in the Czech Republic (Krejˇ
cová et al., 2020). The survey was held on
8–7 February 2020. A total of 5,422 individuals were approached, 753 did not complete the survey,
and inclusion criteria for PI target groups were met by 135 respondents.
Inclusion in PI target groups was assessed based on answers to screening questions focused on sexual
interests. PI was assessed using a five-point Likert scale (ranging from ‘definitely not’ to ‘definitely
yes’). Respondents were asked whether they find the idea of the following activities sexually arous-
ing: Firstly, ‘Intimate contact with a minor without physical signs of adolescence (up to 12 years of
age)’ and ‘Intimate contact with pubescent girls/boys (minors with signs of adolescence aged 12–15
years)’. Answering ‘yes’ or ‘definitely yes’ (i.e., answer 4 or 5 on the Likert scale) to either of these
items resulted in inclusion in the group with PI in minors (N =48). Secondly, we asked respondents
whether they found the following sexually arousing: ‘Complete immobilisation of a stranger (female
or male), potentially using violence, and preventing them from resisting you’; ‘stalking and seizing a
stranger’; ‘sadomasochistic activities involving physical or mental subordination or humiliation’, and
‘sadomasochistic activities that involve beating or other forms of torture’. Answering ‘yes’ or ‘defi-
nitely yes’ (i.e., 4 or 5 on the Likert scale) to either of these items resulted in respondent’s inclusion
in a group with PI in non-consent/violence (N =57). Respondents who met both criteria (N =30)
were not included in the latter analysis.
CORRECTED PROOF
A. Mareˇcková et al. / Men with paraphilic interests and their desire to interact with a sex robot 5
3.3. Statistical analysis
Data analyses were conducted on an IBM SPSS 24.0 (IBM Corp., 2016). First, we assessed the preva-
lence of desire for SRE in each group using a SRE scale (completely disagree/disagree/agree/com-
pletely agree). Additionally, we transformed the scale indicating agreement with desiring a SRE into
a binary variable (‘completely disagree’ or ‘disagree’ transformed into ‘no’; ‘agree’ or’ completely
agree’ into ‘yes’). Then we conducted a two way-contingency table analysis with Chi-Square Test of
Independence to evaluate whether group membership was associated with SRE desire. The two vari-
ables were membership in a group (control, PI in minors, PI in non-consent/violence) and binary SRE
desire. To explore differences between the groups, a Chi-Square Test was conducted for the control
group vs. PI in minors and control group vs. PI in non-consent/violence.
Next, we ran a CATREG analysis with SPSS Optimal Scaling option to examine the effect of group
(control, PI in minors, PI in non-consent/violence) and age on SRE desire rated by scale. Normality
of the raw data was checked using a procedure suggested by Field (2018): it showed that the data were
not normally distributed, and nonparametric tests were to be preferred.
Group was scaled as a nominal variable, age as a numeric one, and desire for SRE as an ordinal vari-
able (completely disagree/disagree/agree/completely agree), with the latter two variables discretised
by the means of ranking. Then we selected a random initial configuration. The analysis was followed
by multiple Mann–Whitney (M–W) U tests for pairwise comparisons. Effect sizes represented by
Pearson’s rfor M–W U tests were computed after Rosenthal (1991)asr=z
√N. The association
between age and SRE desire was computed with Spearman’s ρ, while effect size R2
s, interpreted as
a proportion of variance in the ranks which the two variables share, was computed by squaring its
value. Practical significance was assessed as effect size anchored to a minimum of r=.2 (Ferguson,
2009).
4. RESULTS
A detailed summary of descriptive parameters of the groups is presented in Table 2.
Tab le 2
Distribution of SRE desire scale variable and age categories across all groups. Percentages are given relative to the total
number of men in each group
Control group
(N =806)
PI in minors
(N =48)
PI in non-consent/violence
(N =57)
SRE scale
completely disagree 476 (59%) 13 (27%) 22 (39%)
disagree 187 (23%) 17 (35%) 20 (35%)
agree 116 (14%) 13 (27%) 12 (21%)
completely agree 27 (3%) 5 (10%) 3 (5%)
Mean age (yrs ±SD) 51 ±16 46 ±14 48 ±14
Age category
18–29 90 (11%) 4 (8%) 7 (11%)
30–44 232 (29%) 22 (46%) 17 (30%)
45–54 155 (19%) 8 (17%) 11 (19%)
55+329 (41%) 4 (29%) 22 (40%)
CORRECTED PROOF
6A. Mareˇcková et al. / Men with paraphilic interests and their desire to interact with a sex robot
SRE desire was expressed by 18%, 37.5%, and 26% of respondents in the group control, group with
PI in minors, and group with PS in non-consent/violence, respectively (Table 3).
Tab l e 3
Distribution of SRE binary variable across all groups. The percentages are given relative to the total number of men in each
group
Control group (N =806) PI in minors (N =48) PI in non-consent/violence (N =57)
n Relative frequency
[95% CI]
n Relative frequency
[95% CI]
n Relative frequency
[95% CI]
SRE binary
No 663 82% [79,85]30 62.5% [49,76]42 74% [63,85]
Yes 143 18% [15,21]18 37.5% [24,51]15 26% [15,37]
Across the three groups, we found a significant difference in the distribution of binary SRE desire
(X2(2)=13.256; p=0.001; Cramer’s V =.121). The control group and group with PI in minors
differed significantly in expressed binary SRE desire (X2(1)=11.56; p=0.001; Phi ϕ=−.116).
Between the control group and group with PI in non-consent/violence, expressed desire for SRE did
not significantly differ (X2(1)=2,617; p=0.106; Phi ϕ=−.055).
Categorical regression (CATREG) model explained nearly 7% of variation in SRE desire scale (R2=
.069; R2
adj =.065). The model was statistically significant (F(3,907)=22.25; p<.001) and SRE
desire scale was significantly predicted by both group membership (β=.159; F(2)=20.17; p<
.001) and age (β=−.195; F(1)=38.48; p<.001).
Multiple Mann–Whitney (M–W) U tests for pairwise comparisons revealed that the group with PI in
minors (N =48) differed from the control group (N =806): M–W U =12,577.00; p<.001; r =
.16 (medianminor =‘disagree’, mediancontrol =‘completely disagree’). The situation was parallel for a
comparison between the group with PI in non-consent/violence (N =57) and the control group: M–W
U=18,286.50; p<.01; r =.10 (mediannon-consent/violence =‘disagree’; mediancontrol =‘completely
disagree’; see Fig. 1). Nevertheless, neither of the differences was practically significant because the
size of the effects did not reach the recommended minimum effect size for a ‘practically’ significant
effect in the context of social science (Ferguson, 2009).
Fig. 1. A violin plot showing expressed SRE desire rated on a four-point scale (1 – completely agree, 2 – agree, 3 –
disagree, 4 – completely disagree) by groups.
Visual explorations revealed that younger respondents tended to be more accepting of the idea of sex
with a robot, but the size of this effect only reached the recommended minimum effect size for a
CORRECTED PROOF
A. Mareˇcková et al. / Men with paraphilic interests and their desire to interact with a sex robot 7
‘practically’ significant effect (Ferguson, 2009): Spearman’s ρ=.204, p<.001, N =911, R2
s=
.042.
5. DISCUSSION
Our study explored desire for a sex robot experience (SRE) in three groups of male respondents: first,
a general sample of the Czech population, second, individuals with PI in minors, and third, individuals
with PI in non-consent/violence. We expected both target groups (with PI in minors and with PI in
non-consent/violence) to have a greater desire for SRE than the control group. We also expected that
younger individuals would express greater SRE desire. The proportion of interested individuals ranged
from 18% to 37.5% across groups. Statistical differences in the desire for SRE were found between
the control group and the group with PI in minors but not between the control group and group with
PI in non-consent/violence. Differences between the control group vs. group with PI in minors and
the control group vs. group with PI in non-consent/violence in the desire for SRE were calculated
using a model controlled for the effect of age. Differences between the groups were not practically
significant (Ferguson, 2009) and the effect of age reached only the recommended minimum effect size
(Ferguson, 2009).
The control sample expressed the least desire in SRE of all groups (18%). The desire for SRE in our
control group was lower than that of respondents from a general US population sample (70%; Scheutz
and Arnold, 2016) but higher than in the Malaysian sample (9%; Edirisinghe and Cheok, 2016).
It should be noted, though, that studies by Scheutz and Arnold (2016) and Edirisinghe and Cheok
(2016) used small convenience samples (N =100, N =32 respectively), whereas our control group
was a representative sample of Czech male population (N =806) recruited online from a national
pool of respondents. Other reasons for different levels of desire for SRE may include differences in
the cultural background, which could affect openness to new technological advances, or the mean age
of respondents.
Although differences in the desire for SRE did not reach the level of practical significance (Ferguson,
2009) across the groups, we found that a relatively greater desire for SRE was expressed by the group
with PI in minors. This could be attributed to the fact that persons with PI in minors cannot fully
express their innate sexual desires without violating the law, which might motivate them to look for
alternative routes of sexual expression. A sex robot might be perceived as a strategy of managing their
PI.
Individuals with PI in non-consent/violence also expressed a greater desire for SRE than the control
group. This might may be associated with either their general greater interest in impersonal sexual
encounters, reflected in their greater use of commercial sex services (Farley et al., 2011;2017), or
with perceiving SRE as a way to satisfying one’s PI without inflicting harm by sexual assault or rape.
The minimal effect of age on SRE desire was surprising because younger individuals are thought to
be more accepting of new technologies and novel sexual experiences. Along similar lines, Eichenberg
et al. (2019) reported that younger therapists were more open regarding a possible use of sex robots
as part of therapy than older therapist were. On the other hand, Scheutz and Arnold (2016) reported
somewhat different results: in their study, younger people did not express a greater acceptance of sex
robots than older respondents. On the contrary, younger people were less accepting of the idea of
interacting with sex robots rather than with prostitutes than older people were Scheutz and Arnold
(2016). It might be due to the fact that younger generations are sensitised to issues pertaining to
consent and ethical sexual conduct.
CORRECTED PROOF
8A. Mareˇcková et al. / Men with paraphilic interests and their desire to interact with a sex robot
6. LIMITATIONS AND SUGGESTIONS FOR A FUTURE RESEARCH
The study explored the prevalence of desire for SRE across three groups. The results should be in-
terpreted cautiously for the following reasons: First of all, recruitment strategies for the samples and
the numbers of respondents across groups differed, with the control group being distinctly larger than
either of the PI groups. This is a consequence of the way the target groups were defined (since our aim
was to recruit people with a potential medical diagnosis): by their nature, samples of people with PI
cannot be representative. Secondly, the study did not explore respondents’ preferences regarding sex
robot’s features (apparent age, physical appearance, or behavioural displays). We can only speculate
whether such preferences would indeed reflect, in the relevant groups, the nature of the PI. Instead,
we only assume that people with PI in minors would prefer child-like robots with age-typical be-
havioural displays and people with PI in non-consent/violence would choose adult-like sex robots
with clear displays of resistance or strong negative emotions. Thirdly, the survey did not include a
question whether respondents would welcome a sex robot as part of their therapy, and it should be
noted that expressed SRE desire is not the same as interest in using a sex robot as part of therapy.
And finally, the survey insufficiently differentiated between the terms ‘sex robot’ and ‘sex doll’ when
asking about respondents’ interests: the statement in point was worded as ‘I desire sexual experience
with a sex robot/doll.’ Sex dolls may be more familiar to respondents because they have been on the
market much longer than sex robots and they might have a connotation of passivity. As a consequence,
respondents may have failed to consider the sex robots’ interactive features.
These shortcomings should be considered and addressed in future research to help us better understand
the potential of sex robots in therapeutic settings. Finally, as noted above, the present study did not
aspire to solving the question of whether sex robots are a suitable therapeutic tool for people with PI
or whether they could play a role in the potentiation/suppression of sexual offending behaviour.
Exploration of desire for SRE within the clinical population is a prerequisite for the currently ex-
tensively developing discussions about the use of sex robots in clinical management of paraphilic
disorders. Our study is the first to provide data in this direction.
FUNDING
This study was supported by the Institutional Support for Longterm Development of Research Orga-
nizations – Charles University, Faculty of Humanities (Charles Univ, Fac Human 2021), by Specific
University Research 2021 – 260 610, by Charles University Research Centre of the Charles University,
Czech Republic (UNCE 204056), and by project ‘Sustainability for the National Institute of Mental
Health’ under grant no. LO1611, with financial support from the Ministry of Education, Youth, and
Sports of the Czech Republic under the NPU I program.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We would like to thank Anna Pilátová for English proofreading.
REFERENCES
Aoki, B.Y. & Kimura, T. (2021). Sexuality and affection in the time of technological innovation:
Artificial partners in the Japanese context. Religions,12(5), 296. doi:10.3390/rel12050296.
CORRECTED PROOF
A. Mareˇcková et al. / Men with paraphilic interests and their desire to interact with a sex robot 9
Appel, M., Marker, C. & Mara, M. (2019). Otakuism and the appeal of sex robots. Frontiers in Psy-
chology,10, 1–11. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00569.
Bártová, K., Androviˇ
cová, R., Krejˇ
cová, L., Weiss, P. & Klapilová, K. (2021). The prevalence of
paraphilic interests in the Czech population: Preference, arousal, the use of pornography, fantasy, and
behavior. The Journal of Sex Research,58(1), 86–96. doi:10.1080/00224499.2019.1707468.
Bouchard, K.N., Dawson, S.J. & Lalumière, M.L. (2017). The effects of sex drive and paraphilic
interests on paraphilic behaviours in a nonclinical sample of men and women. Canadian Journal of
Human Sexuality,26(2), 97–111. doi:10.3138/cjhs.262-a8.
Ciambrone, D., Phua, V.C. & Avery, E. (2017). Gendered synthetic love: Real dolls and the construc-
tion of intimacy. International Review of Modern Sociology,43(1), 59–78.
Cox-George, C. & Bewley, S. (2018). I, Sex robot: The health implications of the sex robot industry.
BMJ Sexual & Reproductive Health,44(3), 161–164. doi:10.1136/bmjsrh-2017-200012.
de Graaf, M.M. & Allouch, S.B. (2016). Anticipating our future robot society: The evaluation of
future robot applications from a user’s perspective. In 25th IEEE International Symposium on Robot
and Human Interactive Communication, RO-MAN, 2016 (pp. 755–762). doi:10.1109/ROMAN.2016.
7745204.
Döring, N., Mohseni, M.R. & Walter, R. (2020). Design, use, and effects of sex dolls and sex robots:
Scoping review. Journal of Medical Internet Research,22(7), e18551. doi:10.2196/18551.
Döring, N. & Pöschl, S. (2018). Sex toys, sex dolls, sex robots: Our under-researched bed-fellows.
Sexologies,27(3), e51–e55. doi:10.1016/j.sexol.2018.05.009.
Edirisinghe, C. & Cheok, A.D. (2016). Robots and intimacies: A preliminary study of perceptions,
and intimacies with robots. In A.D. Cheok, K. Devlin and D. Levy (Eds.), Love and Sex with Robots
(pp. 137–147). Cham: Springer.
Eichenberg, C., Khamis, M. & Hübner, L. (2019). The attitudes of therapists and physicians on the
use of sex robots in sexual therapy: Online survey and interview study. Journal of Medical Internet
Research,21(8), e13853. doi:10.2196/13853.
Farley, M., Golding, J.M., Matthews, E.S., Malamuth, N.M. & Jarrett, L. (2017). Comparing sex buy-
ers with men who do not buy sex: New data on prostitution and trafficking. Journal of interpersonal
violence,32(23), 3601–3625. doi:10.1177/0886260515600874.
Farley, M., Macleod, J., Anderson, L. & Golding, J.M. (2011). Attitudes and social characteristics of
men who buy sex in Scotland. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy,3(4),
369–383. doi:10.1037/a0022645.
Ferguson, C.J. (2009). An effect size primer: A guide for clinicians and researchers. Professional
Psychology: Research and Practice,40(5), 532–538. doi:10.1037/a0015808.
Field, A. (2018). Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statictics (5th ed.). London: Sage.
Harris, G.T., Lalumiere, M.L., Seto, M.C., Rice, M.E. & Chaplin, T.C. (2012). Explaining the erectile
responses of rapists to rape stories: The contributions of sexual activity, non-consent, and violence
with injury. Archives of Sexual Behavior,41(1), 221–229. doi:10.1007/s10508-012-9940-8.
Houtepen, J.A., Sijtsema, J.J. & Bogaerts, S. (2016). Being sexually attracted to minors: Sexual de-
velopment, coping with forbidden feelings, and relieving sexual arousal in self-identified pedophiles.
Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy,42(1), 48–69. doi:10.1080/0092623X.2015.1061077.
IBM Corp. (2016). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows (Version 24). Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.
CORRECTED PROOF
10 A. Mareˇcková et al. / Men with paraphilic interests and their desire to interact with a sex robot
Knafo, D. (2015). Guys and dolls: Relational life in the technological era. Psychoanalytic Dialogues,
25(4), 481–502. doi:10.1080/10481885.2015.1055174.
Knafo, D. & Bosco, R.L. (2017). The Age of Perversion: Desire and Technology in Psychoanalysis
and Culture. Taylor & Francis.
Krejˇ
cová, L., Plášilová, L., Potyszová, K., ˇ
Ciženkova, A., Šinkner, F. & Klapilová, K. (2020). Parafi-
lik: Program primární prevence pro jedince s parafilní preference a jejich blízké. Sexuologia, 13–17.
Richards, R., Coss, C. & Quinn, J. (2016). Exploration of relational factors and the likelihood of a
sexual robotic experience. In A.D. Cheok, K. Devlin and D. Levy (Eds.), Love and Sex with Robots
(pp. 97–103). Cham: Springer.
Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-Analytical Procedures for Social Research. London: Sage.
Scheutz, M. & Arnold, T. (2016). Are we ready for sex robots? In 2016 11th ACM/IEEE International
Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI), Christchurch, New Zealand, 2016 (pp. 351–358).
doi:10.1109/HRI.2016.7451772.
Scheutz, M. & Arnold, T. (2017). Intimacy, bonding, and sex robots: Examining empirical results and
exploring ethical ramifications. In J. Danaher and N. McArthur (Eds.), Robot Sex: Social and Ethical
Implications, London: The MIT Press.
Seto, M.C., Lalumière, M.L., Harris, G.T. & Chivers, M.L. (2012). The sexual responses of sexual
sadists. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,121(3), 739. doi:10.1037/a0028714.
Szczuka, J.M. & Krämer, N.C. (2016). Influences on the intention to buy a sex robot. In A.D. Cheok,
K. Devlin and D. Levy (Eds.), Love and Sex with Robots (pp. 72–83). Cham: Springer.
Velverde, S. (2012). The modern sex doll-owner: A descriptive analysis. [Master thesis, California
State Polytechnic University, San Luis Obispo]. Digital Commons.
Walker, A. (2017). Understanding resilience strategies among minor-attracted individuals. [Doctoral
dissertation, The City University of New York]. CUNY Academic Works.
World Health Organization (2018). International Classification of Diseases for Mortality and Mor-
bidity Statistics (11th ed.). World Health Organization.