ArticlePDF Available

Passive Prey Discrimination in Surface Predatory Behaviour of Bait-Attracted White Sharks from Gansbaai, South Africa

MDPI
Animals
Authors:
  • Centro Studi Squali - Sharks Studies Centre - Scientific Institute

Abstract and Figures

Simple Summary White sharks, in surface passive prey predatory behaviour, are initially attracted by the olfactory trace determined by the bait and then implement their predatory choices to energetical richer prey, especially thanks to their visual ability, which plays an important role in adults and immatures with dietary shifts in their feeding patterns. Gansbaai represents a hunting training area for white sharks who are changing their diet. Abstract Between the years 2008 and 2013, six annual research expeditions were carried out at Dyer Island (Gansbaai, South Africa) to study the surface behaviour of white sharks in the presence of two passive prey: tuna bait and a seal-shaped decoy. Sightings were performed from a commercial cage-diving boat over 247 h; 250 different white sharks, with a mean total length (TL) of 308 cm, were observed. Of these, 166 performed at least one or more interactions, for a total of 240 interactions with bait and the seal-shaped decoy. In Gansbaai, there is a population of transient white sharks consisting mainly of immature specimens throughout the year. Both mature and immature sharks preferred to prey on the seal-shaped decoy, probably due to the dietary shift that occurs in white sharks whose TL varies between 200 cm and 340 cm. As it is widely confirmed that white sharks change their diet from a predominantly piscivorous juvenile diet to a mature marine mammalian diet, it is possible that Gansbaai may be a hunting training area and that sharks show a discriminate food choice, a strategy that was adopted by the majority of specimens thanks to their ability to visualize energetically richer prey, after having been attracted by the odorous source represented by the tuna bait.
Content may be subject to copyright.
animals
Article
Passive Prey Discrimination in Surface Predatory Behaviour of
Bait-Attracted White Sharks from Gansbaai, South Africa
Primo Micarelli 1, *, Federico Chieppa 1, Antonio Pacifico 1,2 , Enrico Rabboni 1and Francesca Romana Reinero 1


Citation: Micarelli, P.; Chieppa, F.;
Pacifico, A.; Rabboni, E.; Reinero, F.R.
Passive Prey Discrimination in
Surface Predatory Behaviour of
Bait-Attracted White Sharks from
Gansbaai, South Africa. Animals 2021,
11, 2583. https://doi.org/10.3390/
ani11092583
Academic Editors: Martina
Francesca Marongiu and
Tyrone Lucon-Xiccato
Received: 21 July 2021
Accepted: 1 September 2021
Published: 3 September 2021
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral
with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional affil-
iations.
Copyright: © 2021 by the authors.
Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland.
This article is an open access article
distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons
Attribution (CC BY) license (https://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/
4.0/).
1
Sharks Studies Center—Scientific Institute, 58024 Massa Marittima, Italy; chieppafederico@gmail.com (F.C.);
antonio.pacifico86@gmail.com (A.P.); ENRICO.RABBONI@gmail.com (E.R.);
ricerca@centrostudisquali.org (F.R.R.)
2Department of Political Science and CEFOP-LUISS, LUISS Guido Carli University, 00197 Rome, Italy
*Correspondence: direzione@centrostudisquali.org; Tel.: +39-3896732796 or +39-0566919529
Simple Summary:
White sharks, in surface passive prey predatory behaviour, are initially attracted
by the olfactory trace determined by the bait and then implement their predatory choices to energeti-
cal richer prey, especially thanks to their visual ability, which plays an important role in adults and
immatures with dietary shifts in their feeding patterns. Gansbaai represents a hunting training area
for white sharks who are changing their diet.
Abstract:
Between the years 2008 and 2013, six annual research expeditions were carried out at Dyer
Island (Gansbaai, South Africa) to study the surface behaviour of white sharks in the presence of
two passive prey: tuna bait and a seal-shaped decoy. Sightings were performed from a commercial
cage-diving boat over 247 h; 250 different white sharks, with a mean total length (TL) of 308 cm, were
observed. Of these, 166 performed at least one or more interactions, for a total of 240 interactions with
bait and the seal-shaped decoy. In Gansbaai, there is a population of transient white sharks consisting
mainly of immature specimens throughout the year. Both mature and immature sharks preferred to
prey on the seal-shaped decoy, probably due to the dietary shift that occurs in white sharks whose
TL varies between 200 cm and 340 cm. As it is widely confirmed that white sharks change their diet
from a predominantly piscivorous juvenile diet to a mature marine mammalian diet, it is possible that
Gansbaai may be a hunting training area and that sharks show a discriminate food choice, a strategy
that was adopted by the majority of specimens thanks to their ability to visualize energetically richer
prey, after having been attracted by the odorous source represented by the tuna bait.
Keywords: white shark; behaviour; Gansbaai; prey choice; shark vision; shark olfaction
1. Introduction
The white shark (WS) Carcharodon carcharias [
1
] is an important top predator and the
largest fish predator in existence, reaching about 6 m in length, combining many particular
features including large size, regional endothermy (restricted to swimming muscles, viscera,
and brain), and coarsely serrated dentition [
2
]. Predatory behaviour was recently described
by Martin et al. [
3
], who provided ethograms with frequency and event sequence analyses
of behavioural units for the Seal Island white shark population in South Africa, and by
Hammerschlag et al. [
4
], who reported the effects of environmental factors on the frequency
and success rate of predatory attacks. The white shark is an interesting species in the study
of shark behaviour thanks to the relative ease with which it can be observed from the surface,
especially near pinniped colonies on rocky islands where sharks congregate [
5
]. White sharks
have the largest olfactory bulb among sharks, and thanks to their perceptions, they are able
to trace wounded prey, whale carcasses, seal colonies or sea lions even at great distances [
3
].
During hunting, sharks refer mainly to odorous stimuli from prey [
5
], but many sharks are
also thought to rely on their visual system for prey detection, predator avoidance, navigation,
and communication [
6
]. For many sharks, vision plays a vital role in their ecology, particularly
Animals 2021,11, 2583. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11092583 https://www.mdpi.com/journal/animals
Animals 2021,11, 2583 2 of 9
in detecting and identifying prey [
7
9
]. It is widely accepted that white sharks undergo
an ontogenetic shift in prey preference [
2
,
10
13
]; both stomach-content and stable-isotope
analyses indicate that an ontogenetic shift is expressed by a change in the trophic level,
passing from a predominantly piscivorous diet when immature to a marine mammalian diet
when adult [
10
,
12
15
]. The estimated total length (TL) at which they undergo this dietary
shift varies between 200 cm and 340 cm [
2
,
10
13
,
16
,
17
]. White sharks’ teeth also reflect their
ontogenetic diet change: as adults, the teeth of their upper jaw have a single large cusp, which
is triangular and has serrated edges, while those of the lower jaw are tighter, smaller, narrower,
and slightly sharper. The lower teeth penetrate and hold the prey, the upper ones cut the flesh:
this enables both the predation of large prey such as pinnipeds, and the detachment of large
pieces of meat from carcasses [
18
]. Martin et al. [
3
] observed that Sea Island white sharks
appear to select the age class of Cape fur seal Arctocephalus pusillus pusillus [
19
], a group
with a defined size and a clear direction of movement, as well as a choice of hunting during
times and in locations that maximize their probability for predatory success. Regarding
the ontogenetic shift, French et al. [
20
] suggested that gender and individual specialization
are also key drivers in white sharks’ ecological variation, and that they remain important
throughout ontogeny; in fact, individuals may learn a variety of different movement tactics
for encountering and catching prey, and they may develop a preference for a particular tactic
based on their experiences [
21
,
22
]. The individual surface behaviour of white sharks in the
presence of a bait, in the same manner as their predatory [
3
,
23
,
24
] and social [
25
] behaviour,
is not a simple stimulus response reflex, but rather a complex tactical situation with plastic
responses [
24
]. Only a few studies have described the mechanisms that underlie patterns
of prey selection in white sharks
[9,2628]
. The present study carried out along the South
African coasts in Gansbaai was aimed at providing a contribution to confirm whether, by
simulating the natural condition of scavenging, white sharks, in the presence of artificial
passive prey, such as a seal-shaped decoy and tuna bait, implemented a real food choice
based on vision. In the attempt to gather new information on the topic, this study on the
Gansbaai white shark transient population aimed to research the following: (i) the main
surface predatory behaviours of white sharks when using both baits and seal-shaped decoys;
(ii) the existence of possible interlinkages between predatory behaviour and other observed
endogenous factors, such as maturity; and (iii) white sharks’ tendency to exhibit a real food
choice, based on vision, rather than indiscriminate attacks on the two target passive preys,
which are the bait and seal-shaped decoy.
2. Materials and Methods
Observations and data collection were recorded in the Dyer Island Natural Reserve,
located 7.5 km south-east of Gansbaai, South Africa (34
41
0
S; 19
24
0
E). The reserve
includes Dyer Island and Geyser Rock (Figure 1): the first is a low-profile island ca. 1.5 km
long and 0.5 km wide, and it is characterised by the presence of different seabird colonies;
the second is ca. 0.5 km long and 180 m wide, and it hosts a colony of Cape fur seals,
Arctocephalus pusillus pusillus [19].
In Gansbaai (South Africa), at Dyer Island Nature Reserve, a large white shark popu-
lation is present and can be observed thanks to the support of local ecotourism operators
authorised to reach the field observation sites, with a prevalence of immature individuals [
29
].
The greatest proportion of sub-adults and potentially mature sharks also occurs in Seal Island,
False Bay, South Africa [
30
,
31
]. The 2008–2013 study periods occurred in the autumn season
between March and May and required a total of 247 h. Seal-shaped decoys and floating baits
of tuna pieces, of similar size, were the tested passive target preys; in line with the research
protocol adopted by Sperone et al. [
24
], the two baits were chosen first on the basis of the
odourless seal shape, and second, the production of odour. The decoy’s size was as close as
possible to that of a juvenile Cape fur seal (A. pusillus pusillus): 70 cm long and 32 cm wide,
with a diameter of about 60 cm (Figure 2a,b).
Animals 2021,11, 2583 3 of 9
Animals 2021, 11, x 3 of 9
Figure 1. Dyer Island and Geyser Rock Nature Reserve.
In Gansbaai (South Africa), at Dyer Island Nature Reserve, a large white shark pop-
ulation is present and can be observed thanks to the support of local ecotourism operators
authorised to reach the field observation sites, with a prevalence of immature individuals
[29]. The greatest proportion of sub-adults and potentially mature sharks also occurs in
Seal Island, False Bay, South Africa [30,31]. The 2008-2013 study periods occurred in the
autumn season between March and May and required a total of 247 h. Seal-shaped decoys
and floating baits of tuna pieces, of similar size, were the tested passive target preys; in
line with the research protocol adopted by Sperone et al. [24], the two baits were chosen
first on the basis of the odourless seal shape, and second, the production of odour. The
decoy’s size was as close as possible to that of a juvenile Cape fur seal (A. pusillus pusillus):
70 cm long and 32 cm wide, with a diameter of about 60 cm (Figure 2a,b).
(a) (b) (c)
Figure 2. (a) Decoys, (b) interaction shark versus seal-shaped decoy, and (c) tuna bait.
The ethological observations on the sharks were made from the 13 m Barracuda
boat owned by Shark diving unlimited, which was anchored at 100m-150 m off Dyer
Island. The boat was equipped with an upper deck from which it was possible to observe
and photograph the sighted sharks. The observations were also carried out in an anti-
shark cage, which was fixed, for the duration of the observations, to the side of the boat.
Throughout this study, sharks were identified by the same research team (Sharks Studies
CenterCentro Studi SqualiIstituto Scientifico, Massa Marittima, Italy), and the identi-
fication was based on the recognition of different anatomical features such as the dorsal
fin, the caudal fin, and the presence of scars and ectoparasites and their arrangement
[29,32,33]. It was also possible that one specimen was observed at various times through-
out the day and that all exhibited behaviours were recorded. All sharks’ total lengths were
estimated from the boat, referring to structures of a size known as the length of the cage.
Inside the cage, operators were equipped with a mask, boots, a semi-dry suit and weights,
remaining on the surface and maintaining a vertical position. When white sharks ap-
proached, the operators descended in a free dive to the bottom of the cage, in order not to
disturb the sharks with air bubbles and to be able to observe, up close, the behaviour of
the animals, the sex of the specimen and other body features. The cage was, therefore, an
excellent and useful tool for identifying the sex of each individual with precision and for
Figure 1. Dyer Island and Geyser Rock Nature Reserve.
Animals 2021, 11, x 3 of 9
Figure 1. Dyer Island and Geyser Rock Nature Reserve.
In Gansbaai (South Africa), at Dyer Island Nature Reserve, a large white shark pop-
ulation is present and can be observed thanks to the support of local ecotourism operators
authorised to reach the field observation sites, with a prevalence of immature individuals
[29]. The greatest proportion of sub-adults and potentially mature sharks also occurs in
Seal Island, False Bay, South Africa [30,31]. The 2008-2013 study periods occurred in the
autumn season between March and May and required a total of 247 h. Seal-shaped decoys
and floating baits of tuna pieces, of similar size, were the tested passive target preys; in
line with the research protocol adopted by Sperone et al. [24], the two baits were chosen
first on the basis of the odourless seal shape, and second, the production of odour. The
decoy’s size was as close as possible to that of a juvenile Cape fur seal (A. pusillus pusillus):
70 cm long and 32 cm wide, with a diameter of about 60 cm (Figure 2a,b).
(a) (b) (c)
Figure 2. (a) Decoys, (b) interaction shark versus seal-shaped decoy, and (c) tuna bait.
The ethological observations on the sharks were made from the 13 m Barracuda
boat owned by Shark diving unlimited, which was anchored at 100m-150 m off Dyer
Island. The boat was equipped with an upper deck from which it was possible to observe
and photograph the sighted sharks. The observations were also carried out in an anti-
shark cage, which was fixed, for the duration of the observations, to the side of the boat.
Throughout this study, sharks were identified by the same research team (Sharks Studies
CenterCentro Studi SqualiIstituto Scientifico, Massa Marittima, Italy), and the identi-
fication was based on the recognition of different anatomical features such as the dorsal
fin, the caudal fin, and the presence of scars and ectoparasites and their arrangement
[29,32,33]. It was also possible that one specimen was observed at various times through-
out the day and that all exhibited behaviours were recorded. All sharks’ total lengths were
estimated from the boat, referring to structures of a size known as the length of the cage.
Inside the cage, operators were equipped with a mask, boots, a semi-dry suit and weights,
remaining on the surface and maintaining a vertical position. When white sharks ap-
proached, the operators descended in a free dive to the bottom of the cage, in order not to
disturb the sharks with air bubbles and to be able to observe, up close, the behaviour of
the animals, the sex of the specimen and other body features. The cage was, therefore, an
excellent and useful tool for identifying the sex of each individual with precision and for
Figure 2. (a) Decoys, (b) interaction shark versus seal-shaped decoy, and (c) tuna bait.
The ethological observations on the sharks were made from the 13 m “Barracuda” boat
owned by “Shark diving unlimited”, which was anchored at 100–150 m off Dyer Island.
The boat was equipped with an upper deck from which it was possible to observe and
photograph the sighted sharks. The observations were also carried out in an anti-shark cage,
which was fixed, for the duration of the observations, to the side of the boat. Throughout
this study, sharks were identified by the same research team (Sharks Studies Center—
Centro Studi Squali—Istituto Scientifico, Massa Marittima, Italy), and the identification
was based on the recognition of different anatomical features such as the dorsal fin, the
caudal fin, and the presence of scars and ectoparasites and their arrangement [
29
,
32
,
33
].
It was also possible that one specimen was observed at various times throughout the day
and that all exhibited behaviours were recorded. All sharks’ total lengths were estimated
from the boat, referring to structures of a size known as the length of the cage. Inside the
cage, operators were equipped with a mask, boots, a semi-dry suit and weights, remaining
on the surface and maintaining a vertical position. When white sharks approached, the
operators descended in a free dive to the bottom of the cage, in order not to disturb the
sharks with air bubbles and to be able to observe, up close, the behaviour of the animals,
the sex of the specimen and other body features. The cage was, therefore, an excellent and
useful tool for identifying the sex of each individual with precision and for supporting the
identification made on board. The sex of each shark was determined by the surface and by
cage-diving observations, and also with underwater video recordings of the pelvic fin area:
the males were recorded if claspers were seen and the females if the lack of claspers was
verified and their pelvic fin area was filmed [
29
]. All other specimens were categorised as
being of unknown sex. In our study, we estimated the white shark’s size at sexual maturity
according to Hewitt et al. [
31
] and Micarelli et al. [
29
]: a mature male if the TL was
350 cm
and a mature female if the TL was
450 cm. To attract the sharks, olfactory stimulants
(chum) were used, following the methods described in Laroche et al. [
34
], Ferreira and
Ferreira [
35
], and Sperone et al. [
25
]. The chumming was composed of sea water, cod liver
Animals 2021,11, 2583 4 of 9
oil (Gadus sp.), tuna blood and small pieces of fish [
25
,
36
]. Shark predatory behaviour was
induced using two types of surface passive prey placed into the water: the bait, consisting
of tuna pieces tied to a floating buoy positioned at the stern of the boat, and a seal-shaped
decoy, positioned at the bow at a distance from the tuna bait of at least 10 m. A constant
distance between the two-surface passive prey was maintained to isolate the seal-shaped
decoy from the odorous and bloody trail coming from the tuna bait. In order to analyse
the surface behaviour of white sharks in the presence of passive preys, tuna baits and an
odourless seal-shaped decoy, we performed a Chi-squared independence test to check
whether the types of prey (bait and seal-shaped decoy) were associated, and whether the
choice of prey was not causal. To investigate the presence of independency between two
causal variables, we used Pearson’s Chi-squared test, where H0 tests the null hypothesis of
independency between the variables, and H1 the alternative hypothesis of dependency.
To strengthen the results regarding the presence of causality between behaviour and prey,
we also used Cochran’s Q test. This is a non-parametric statistical test used to verify
whether k treatments (or number of studies) have identical effects. Generally, the test
statistic refers to two-way randomized block designs, where the response variable takes
only two possible outcomes coded as 0 or 1 denoting failure or success, respectively. It is
often used to assess whether different observers of the same phenomenon have consistent
results (interobserver variability). Cochran’s Q test is as follows: the null hypothesis (H
0
),
where there is no difference in the effectiveness of treatments (the choice is causal) and the
alternative hypothesis (H
1
), where there is a difference in the effectiveness of treatments
(the choice is not causal).
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive Analysis
Overall, 250 white sharks were sighted, with 166 having at least one or more inter-
actions for a total of 240 interactions: 41 adults, 183 immatures and 16 unsexed. The
variables denoting maturity and prey were transformed into dichotomous variables in
order to be quantitatively evaluated (Table 1). In the statistical analysis, the re-interaction
(more precisely, we observed 64 reinteractions) was counted as additional information
investigating the frequency among white sharks’ individual characteristics. During the
discriminant analysis, when studying the independency and heterogeneity among units,
potential re-interactions were counted as unique observations. The same occurred in the
empirical analysis accounting for non-linear methods, to investigate (potential) interactions
among the observed variables. Multiple interactions were dealt with by shrinking the
dataset with respect to every re-sighting. More precisely, the sample size ‘n’—used in
the analysis—refers to the ‘individuals’ (counted only one time for any observation) and
was weighted by the factors (e.g., behaviour and maturity). In this way, any (potential)
bias—affecting the robustness of estimates—was sufficiently avoided in the statistical
analyses. For the shrinking, a discriminant analysis involved in constructing hypotheses
for a significance test on an individual was used before the estimation procedure.
Table 1. Description of variables. Own computations.
Variable Label
Maturity (=1) adult, (=0) immature
Prey (=1) bait, (=0) seal-shaped decoy
3.2. Statistical Analysis
In the analysis of the white sharks (individual) and the two types of prey (bait and
seal-shaped decoy) (Figure 3), the preference of white sharks for the seal-shaped decoy
rather than the bait was proven through appropriate test statistics. More precisely, we ran
a Chi-squared test (Table 2) and Cochran’s test (Table 3) rejecting any causality concerning
Animals 2021,11, 2583 5 of 9
the shark’s choice under the null hypothesis. In this way, the descriptive statistics were
confirmed, and it was proven that the preference for the seal-shaped decoy was not causal.
Animals 2021, 11, x 6 of 9
Figure 3. Bait and seal-shaped decoy shark choice, between 2013 and 2008.
4. Discussion
Carcharodon carcharias is a top predator that has a wide distribution in temperate
and tropical areas [5,18] with a higher concentration in eight spots around the world [18],
including South Africa. Studies aimed at investigating the various methods of white
sharks’ surface artificial prey approach or their food choices are scarce: between 1989 and
1992, Anderson et al. [32] examined the predatory behaviour of the Californian Farallon
Islands sharks towards decoys with different shapes, with an approximate size of prey
represented by pinnipeds and also including the reproduction of a sea lion shape, for a
total of 159 h of observation. In that study, it was observed that vision played a major role
in the approach and predation activities: the sharks were attracted, not by smell, electric
fields or vibrations coming from the baits, but only by the presence of odourless prey on
the surface. Strong [9] carried out an experiment comparing two floating shapes, one with
the shape of a seal and the other with a square shape: during the first experiences with
these preys, the sighted sharks showed a significant preference for the seal shape. He also
stated that in his study there was little doubt that the sharks initially located the bait via
olfaction, and vision was clearly used to orient their actual approaches, but its relative
importance to most shark species remains, however, poorly understood. The present
study carried out along the South African coasts in Gansbaai was aimed at providing a
contribution to confirm whether, by simulating the natural condition of scavenging, white
sharks, in the presence of artificial passive prey, such as a seal-shaped decoy and tuna
bait, implemented a real food choice. Micarelli et al. [29] stated that, in Gansbaai, the white
shark population was always made up of a prevalence of immature individuals, and that
the mean TL recorded was 308 cm. It is important to remember that the estimated length
at which white sharks undergo dietary shifts varies between 200 cm and 340 cm in TL
[2,1013,16,17]; between 2008 and 2013, only six specimens showed a total length <200cm,
of 250 specimens. Tricas and McCosker [14] showed a clear prevalence of an ichthyopha-
gous diet in immature specimens, while adult sharks preferred marine mammals at Dan-
gerous Reef, South Australia. Moreover, in our study, considering the tests performed to
distinguish the adult from the immature individuals, it emerged that the adult sharks
seemed to prefer the seal-shaped decoy, and the frequency of attacks on tuna bait by the
adult sharks was not significantly greater than that of the immature ones. However, since
the Gansbaai population of white sharks showed a mean TL of 308 cm, it is possible that
the majority of Gansbaai white sharks transient population had already undergone the
Figure 3. Bait and seal-shaped decoy shark choice, between 2013 and 2008.
Table 2.
Pearson’s Chi-squared test of independence. The first column denotes the sub-categories,
and the second column displays the corresponding statistical results in terms of p-values. The
significant codes are as follows: * significance at 10%, ** significance at 5%, and *** significance at 1%.
Chi-Squared Test of Independence on Prey
Full Sample 0.00 ***
Adult 0.00 ***
Immature 0.00 ***
Table 3. Cochran’s Q test (asymptotic p-value).
Test Statistic 175.98
Chi-squared distribution (critical value) 3.840
Df 1
p-value (one-tailed) 0.027
Significance level 0.050
All the test statistics are significant (at 1%) and consistent (p-value < 0.001).
3.3. Frequency Distribution
In order to investigate potential relationships between white sharks and types of
prey, the previous preliminary analysis was expanded to account for related frequency
distributions. More precisely, the relationship was maturity–prey, highlighting possi-
ble interactions between each shark’s life stage and hunting patterns. The relationship
maturity–prey highlighted that adult and even immature sharks preferred the seal-shaped
decoy, confirming previous results. We performed a Chi-squared test with each frequency
distribution accounted for. The main results, summarised in Table 2, highlight that the null
hypothesis of independency can be rejected according to the whole sample. More precisely,
the relationship between the types of prey (bait and seal-shaped decoy) is associated with a
p-value close to zero (=0.00). This finding shows that their choice is not causal. In Cochran’s
test, the treatments denote how the choice of different prey affects the sharks’ behaviour,
as shown in Table 3. Let k
1 = 1 be the degrees of freedom, with k denoting the types of
Animals 2021,11, 2583 6 of 9
prey; as the computed p-value is lower than the significance level (
α
= 5%), one should
reject the null and then accept the alternative hypothesis.
4. Discussion
Carcharodon carcharias is a “top predator” that has a wide distribution in temperate
and tropical areas [
5
,
18
] with a higher concentration in eight spots around the world [
18
],
including South Africa. Studies aimed at investigating the various methods of white sharks’
surface artificial prey approach or their food choices are scarce: between 1989 and 1992,
Anderson et al. [
32
] examined the predatory behaviour of the Californian Farallon Islands
sharks towards decoys with different shapes, with an approximate size of prey represented
by pinnipeds and also including the reproduction of a sea lion shape, for a total of 159 h of
observation. In that study, it was observed that vision played a major role in the approach
and predation activities: the sharks were attracted, not by smell, electric fields or vibrations
coming from the baits, but only by the presence of odourless prey on the surface. Strong [
9
]
carried out an experiment comparing two floating shapes, one with the shape of a seal and
the other with a square shape: during the first experiences with these preys, the sighted
sharks showed a significant preference for the seal shape. He also stated that in his study
there was little doubt that the sharks initially located the bait via olfaction, and vision was
clearly used to orient their actual approaches, but its relative importance to most shark
species remains, however, poorly understood. The present study carried out along the
South African coasts in Gansbaai was aimed at providing a contribution to confirm whether,
by simulating the natural condition of scavenging, white sharks, in the presence of artificial
passive prey, such as a seal-shaped decoy and tuna bait, implemented a real food choice.
Micarelli et al. [
29
] stated that, in Gansbaai, the white shark population was always made
up of a prevalence of immature individuals, and that the mean TL recorded was 308 cm. It
is important to remember that the estimated length at which white sharks undergo dietary
shifts varies between 200 cm and 340 cm in TL [
2
,
10
13
,
16
,
17
]; between 2008 and 2013, only
six specimens showed a total length <200 cm, of 250 specimens. Tricas and McCosker [
14
]
showed a clear prevalence of an ichthyophagous diet in immature specimens, while adult
sharks preferred marine mammals at Dangerous Reef, South Australia. Moreover, in
our study, considering the tests performed to distinguish the adult from the immature
individuals, it emerged that the adult sharks seemed to prefer the seal-shaped decoy, and
the frequency of attacks on tuna bait by the adult sharks was not significantly greater than
that of the immature ones. However, since the Gansbaai population of white sharks showed
a mean TL of 308 cm, it is possible that the majority of Gansbaai white sharks’ transient
population had already undergone the dietary shift, and this would explain why immature
specimens also showed a similar interest towards the seal-shaped decoy (Figure 4).
Regarding our question (which are the white sharks’ main surface predatory be-
haviours when using both baits and seal-shaped decoys?), it is possible to conclude from
our studies that, at least as far as the behaviour of adult specimens is concerned, the same
predatory trend observed near the Californian coasts emerged, but also included most
immatures with dietary shift. White sharks must be selective when there is an abundance
of food; therefore, according to the Optimal Foraging Theory [
37
], they prefer the most
caloric sources over low-energy ones [
38
]. In response to the question “Do white sharks
tend to exhibit a real food choice or indiscriminate attacks on the two passive prey (bait
and seal shaped decoy)?”, it is possible to conclude that there was a real food choice
rather than an indiscriminate attack, and that this strategy was adopted by the majority
of specimens, helped by the ability to visualize the energetically richer preys, also with
respect to the odorous source represented by the tuna bait. In this regard, it was possible to
observe, in this study, that, in the contemporary presence of surface prey, characterized
by different stimulating conditions, the white sharks preferred the odourless seal-shaped
decoy rather than the tuna bait, which produces a strong odour stimulus. In only two of
the six observation years, 2011 and 2008, we recorded the prevalence of the bait choice
instead of the seal-shaped decoy. We suppose that this different behaviour, as proposed
Animals 2021,11, 2583 7 of 9
by Sperone et al. [
39
], could be linked to environmental factors, such as cloud cover and
water visibility. This allows us to confirm that vision, in the Gansbaai transient population
of white sharks, as suggested by Anderson [
32
] and Strong [
9
], plays an important role
during the investigation and choice of prey, helping adults and immatures, which are in
progress with dietary shift, to optimize the result of their predatory activity.
Animals 2021, 11, x 7 of 9
dietary shift, and this would explain why immature specimens also showed a similar in-
terest towards the seal-shaped decoy (Figure 4).
Figure 4. Total length range (meters) of white sharks, between 2008 and 2013.
Regarding our question (which are the white sharks main surface predatory behav-
iours when using both baits and seal-shaped decoys?), it is possible to conclude from our
studies that, at least as far as the behaviour of adult specimens is concerned, the same
predatory trend observed near the Californian coasts emerged, but also included most
immatures with dietary shift. White sharks must be selective when there is an abundance
of food; therefore, according to the Optimal Foraging Theory [37], they prefer the most
caloric sources over low-energy ones [38]. In response to the question Do white sharks
tend to exhibit a real food choice or indiscriminate attacks on the two passive prey (bait
and seal shaped decoy)?”, it is possible to conclude that there was a real food choice rather
than an indiscriminate attack, and that this strategy was adopted by the majority of spec-
imens, helped by the ability to visualize the energetically richer preys, also with respect
to the odorous source represented by the tuna bait. In this regard, it was possible to ob-
serve, in this study, that, in the contemporary presence of surface prey, characterized by
different stimulating conditions, the white sharks preferred the odourless seal-shaped de-
coy rather than the tuna bait, which produces a strong odour stimulus. In only two of the
six observation years, 2011 and 2008, we recorded the prevalence of the bait choice instead
of the seal-shaped decoy. We suppose that this different behaviour, as proposed by Sper-
one et al. [39], could be linked to environmental factors, such as cloud cover and water
visibility. This allows us to confirm that vision, in the Gansbaai transient population of
white sharks, as suggested by Anderson [32] and Strong [9], plays an important role dur-
ing the investigation and choice of prey, helping adults and immatures, which are in pro-
gress with dietary shift, to optimize the result of their predatory activity.
5. Conclusions
It is possible to confirm, based on the data collected in the expeditions between 2008
and 2013, that the Gansbaai white shark transient population shows food preferences that
are similar to those already observed in adult specimens in similar studies carried out
along the Australian and Californian coasts [14,32]. The presence of a white shark popu-
lation showing a mean TL of 308 cm, and only six specimens out of 250 showing a TL
<200cm, implies that for the majority of the Gansbaai white shark population, the change
in diet has already occurred or is in progress and, therefore, the interest in energetically
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
2.0-2.5 2.6-3.0 3.1-3.5 3.6-4.0 4.1-4.5 4.6-5.0
TL Range 2008-2013
Figure 4. Total length range (meters) of white sharks, between 2008 and 2013.
5. Conclusions
It is possible to confirm, based on the data collected in the expeditions between 2008
and 2013, that the Gansbaai white shark transient population shows food preferences that
are similar to those already observed in adult specimens in similar studies carried out along
the Australian and Californian coasts [
14
,
32
]. The presence of a white shark population
showing a mean TL of 308 cm, and only six specimens out of 250 showing a TL <200 cm,
implies that for the majority of the Gansbaai white shark population, the change in diet has
already occurred or is in progress and, therefore, the interest in energetically richer prey
can be important for the overall population. Gansbaai, therefore, could represent a hunting
training area for white sharks who are changing or have already changed their diet. It can
be assumed that white sharks are initially attracted by the olfactory trace [
5
] determined by
the bait and then implement their predatory choices to energetical richer prey, especially
thanks to their visual ability, which plays an important role in adults and immatures with
dietary shifts in their feeding patterns. The behaviour shown in the presence of the two
preys examined in this work is, therefore, linked to the different dietary needs of white
sharks in different stages of development. These needs are linked both to the modification
of the dental system and to the different energy necessary in the immature and adult stages.
As demonstrated by Laroche et al. [
34
] and Sperone et al. [
24
], the results presented in this
article are based on the assumption that white sharks do not respond to the presence of the
boat, but focus their attention mainly on the floating object, because the presence of a boat
has very little influence on the white sharks’ behaviour.
Author Contributions:
Conceptualization, P.M.; methodology, P.M.; software, A.P.; validation, P.M.,
F.R.R., F.C. and A.P.; formal analysis, A.P., P.M. and F.R.R.; investigation, P.M., F.R.R., E.R. and F.C.;
writing—original draft preparation, P.M.; writing—review and editing, P.M., F.R.R., F.C. and A.P.;
funding acquisition, P.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding:
This research was funded by the Sharks Studies Center—Scientific Institute, University of
Calabria and University of Siena.
Animals 2021,11, 2583 8 of 9
Institutional Review Board Statement:
The study and experimentation protocols were reviewed
and approved in accordance 166 with the Directive 2010/63/EU.
Data Availability Statement:
https://www.researchgate.net/project/Great-White-Shark-Carcharodon-
carcharias-Behaviour-Ecology-and-cotoxicology/update/61312b952897145fbd6df39c.
Acknowledgments:
This research was made possible by the financial support of the Sharks Studies
Center—Scientific Institute, University of Calabria and University of Siena. We are grateful to the
CSS team members that carried out 11 expeditions for their indirect financial support of this research,
and thanks are also due to Shark Diving Unlimited for the logistical assistance and all other field
assistance with data collection. All research protocols were approved by the University of Calabria
and Siena.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
1.
Linnaeus, C. Systema Naturae per Regna Tria Naturae, Secundum Classes, Ordines, Genera, Species, Cum Characteribus, Differentiis,
Synonymis, Locis, Tomus 1, 10th ed.; Holimae: Laurentii, Salvii, Stockholm, Sweden, 1758; p. 823.
2.
Compagno, L.J.V. Sharks of the World: An Annotated and Illustrated Catalogue of Shark Species Known to Date, Volume 2: Bullhead,
Mackerel, and Carpet Sharks (Heterodontiformes, Lamniformes and Orectolobiformes); FAO: Rome, Italy, 2001; p. 269.
3.
Martin, A.R.; Hammerschlag, N.; Collier, R.S.; Fallows, C. Predatory behaviour of white sharks (Carcharodon carcharias) at Seal
Island, South Africa. J. Mar. Biol. Assoc. UK 2005,85, 1121–1135. [CrossRef]
4.
Hammerschlag, N.; Martin, R.A.; Fallows, C. Effects of environmental conditions on predator-prey interactions between white
sharks (Carcharodon carcharias) and Cape fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus pusillus) at Seal island, South Africa. Environ. Biol.
Fishes 2006,76, 341–350. [CrossRef]
5. Compagno, L.J.V.; Dando, M.; Fowler, S. A Field Guide to the Sharks of the World; Harper Collins: London, UK, 2005; p. 368.
6.
Ryan, L.A. A Shark Eye View: Motion Vision and Behaviour in Apex Predators. Ph.D. Thesis, School of Animal Biology and
Ocean Institute, Western Australia University, Perth, Australia, 2016.
7. Hobson, E.S. Feeding behavior in three species of sharks. Pac. Sci. 1963,17, 171–194.
8. Gilbert, P.W. Studies on the Anatomy, Physiology, and Behavior of Sharks; Office of Naval Research: Washington, VA, USA, 1970.
9.
Strong, W.R. Shape discrimination and visual predatory tactics in white sharks. In Great White Sharks: The Biology of Carcharodon
Carcharias; Klimley, P., Ainley, D.G., Eds.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1996; pp. 229–240.
10.
Cliff, G.; Dudley, S.F.J.; Davis, B. Sharks caught in the protective gill nets off Natal, South Africa. 2. The great white shark
Carcharodon carcharias (Linnaeus). S. Afr. J. Mar. Sci. 1989,8, 131–144. [CrossRef]
11.
Bruce, B.D. Preliminary observations on the biology of the white shark, Carcharodon carcharias, in south Australian waters. Mar.
Freshw. Res. 1992,43, 1–11. [CrossRef]
12.
Estrada, J.A.; Rice, A.N.; Natanson, L.J.; Skomal, G.B. Use of isotopic analysis of vertebrae in reconstructing ontogenetic feeding
ecology in white sharks. Ecology 2006,87, 829–834. [CrossRef]
13.
Hussey, N.E.; McCann, H.M.; Cliff, G.; Dudley, S.F.J.; Wintner, S.P.; Fisk, A.T. Size-based analysis of diet and trophic position of
the white shark (Carcharodon carcharias) in South African waters. In Global Perspectives on the Biology and Life History of the White
Shark; CRC Press: Florida, FL, USA, 2012; pp. 27–45.
14.
Tricas, T.C.; McCosker, J.E. Predatory behaviour of the white shark (Carcharodon carcharias), with notes on its biology. Proc. Calif.
Acad. Sci. 1984, 221–238.
15.
Klimley, A.P. The areal distribution and autoecology of the white shark, Carcharodon carcharias, off the west coast of North
America. South. Calif. Acad. Sci. Mem. 1985,9, 15–40.
16.
Malcolm, H.; Bruce, B.D.; Stevens, J.D. A Review of the Biology and Status of White Sharks in Australian Waters; CSIRO Marine
Research: Hobart, Tasmania, Australia, 2001.
17.
Bruce, B.D.; Stevens, J.D.; Malcolm, H. Movements and swimming behaviour of white sharks (Carcharodon carcharias) in
Australian waters. Mar. Biol. 2006,150, 161–172. [CrossRef]
18. Martin, A.R. Field Guide to the Great White Shark; RCSR: Vancouver, BC, Canada, 2003.
19.
Schreber, J.C.D. Die Saugethiere in abbildungen nach der nature mit beschreibungen. Erlangen Wolfgang Walther
1775
,2, 223–230.
20.
French, G.C.A.; Rizzuto, S.; Sturup, M.; Inger, R.; Barker, S.; van Wyk, J.H.; Towner, A.V.; Hughes, W.O.H. Sex, size and isotopes:
Cryptic trophic ecology of an apex predator, the white shark Carcharodon carcharias. Mar. Biol.
2018
,165, 1–11. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
21.
Towner, A.V.; Leos-Barajas, V.; Langrock, R.; Schick, R.S.; Smale, M.J.; Kaschke, T.; Jewell, O.J.D.; Papastamatiou, Y.P. Sex-specific
and individual preferences for hunting strategies in white sharks. Funct. Ecol. 2016,30, 1397–1407. [CrossRef]
22.
Micarelli, P.; Spinetti, S.; Andreotti, S.; Muià, C.; Vitaro, M.; Leone, A.; Camigliano, G.; Antonucci, G.; Sperone, E.; Tripepi, S. Is it
good-looking or does it smell good? Preliminary observations about great white shark discriminatory patterns. In Proceedings of
the 13th European Elasmobranch Association, Palma de Mallorca, Palma, Spain, 19–22 November 2009; p. 81.
Animals 2021,11, 2583 9 of 9
23.
Micarelli, P.; Spinetti, S.; Tripepi, S.; Sperone, E. Observations about surface behavior of the great white shark Carcharodon
carcharias (L.) in presence of passive preys at Dyer Island (South Africa). Biol. Mar. Mediterr. 2006,13, 278–279.
24.
Sperone, E.; Micarelli, P.; Andreotti, S.; Brandmayr, P.; Bernabò, I.; Brunelli, E.; Tripepi, S. Surface behaviour of bait-attracted
white sharks at Dyer Island (South Africa). Mar. Biol. Res. 2012,8, 982–991. [CrossRef]
25.
Sperone, E.; Micarelli, P.; Andreotti, S.; Spinetti, S.; Andreani, A.; Serena, F.; Brunelli, E.; Tripepi, S. Social interactions among
bait-attracted white sharks at Dyer Island (South Africa). Mar. Biol. Res. 2010,6, 408–414. [CrossRef]
26. Tricas, T.C. Feeding ethology of the white shark Carcharodon carcharias. Mem. South. Calif. Acad. Sci. 1985,9, 81–91.
27.
Myrberg, A.A.; Nelson, D.R. The behavior of sharks: What have we learned. In Discovering Sharks; Gruber, S.H., Ed.; American
Littoral Society: Highlands, NJ, USA, 1991; pp. 92–100.
28.
Sperone, E.; Circosta, A.L.; Giglio, G.; Tripepi, S.; Micarelli, P. Preliminary observations on Great White Shark (Carcharodon
carcharias) surface predatory activity. Biol. Mar. Mediterr. 2015,22, 184.
29.
Micarelli, P.; Bonsignori, D.; Compagno, L.J.V.; Pacifico, A.; Romano, C.; Reinero, F.R. Analysis of sightings of white sharks in
Gansbaai (South Africa). Eur. Zool. J. 2021,88, 363–374. [CrossRef]
30.
Kock, A.A.; Johnson, R.L. White shark abundance: Not a causative factor in numbers of shark bite incidences. In Finding a Balance:
White Shark Conservation and Recreational Safety in Inshore Waters of Cape Town, South Africa: Proceedings of a Specialist Workshop; Nel,
D.C., Peschak, T.P., Eds.; WWF: Washington, DC, USA, 2006; pp. 1–19.
31.
Hewitt, A.M.; Kock, A.A.; Booth, A.J.; Griffiths, C.L. Trends in sightings and population structure of white sharks, Carcharodon
carcharias, at Seal Island, False Bay, South Africa, and the emigration of subadult female sharks approaching maturity. Environ.
Biol. Fishes 2017,101, 39–54. [CrossRef]
32.
Anderson, S.D.; Henderson, R.P.; Pyle, P. White shark reaction to unbaited decoys. In Great White Sharks: The Biology of Carcharodon
Carcharias; Klimley, A.P., Ainley, D.G., Eds.; Academic Press: San Diego, SD, USA, 1996; pp. 223–228.
33.
Micarelli, P.; Sperone, E.; Giglio, G.; Pecchia, J.; Romano, C.; Scuderi, A.; Vespaziani, L.; Mele, F. Dorsal fin photoidentification:
Tool for long term studies of White shark (Carcharodon carcharias) behavior. Biol. Mar. Mediterr. 2015,22, 144–145.
34.
Laroche, R.; Kock, A.A.; Dill, L.M.; Oosthuizen, W. Effects of provisioning ecotourism activity on the behaviour of white sharks,
Carcharodon carcharias. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 2007,338, 199–209. [CrossRef]
35.
Ferreira, C.A.; Ferreira, T.P. Population dynamics of white sharks in South Africa. In Great White Sharks: The Biology of Carcharodon
Carcharias; Klimley, A.P., Ainley, D.G., Eds.; Academic Press: San Diego, SD, USA, 1996; pp. 381–391.
36.
Strong, W.R.; Murphy, R.C.; Bruce, B.D.; Nelson, D.R. Movements and associated observations of bait-attracted white sharks,
Carcharodon carcharias: A preliminary report. Aust. J. Mar. Freshw. Res. 1992,43, 13–20. [CrossRef]
37. Gerking, S.D. Feeding Ecology of Fish; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2014; p. 416.
38.
Helfman, G.; Collette, B.B.; Facey, D.E.; Bowen, B.W. The Diversity of Fishes: Biology, Evolution, and Ecology; John Wiley and Sons:
Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2009; p. 736.
39.
Sperone, E.; Micarelli, P.; Romano, C.; Giglio, G.; Giovannelli, P.; Reinero, F.R.; Rijillo, G.; Leporati, L.; Fabietti, Y.; Chiriaco, S.
Biological and ecological factors affecting individual surface behaviours of bait attracted White Sharks. In Proceedings of the 3rd
Sharks International Conference, João Pessoa, Brazil, 3–8 June 2018.
... 0.5 km long and 180 m wide, characterized by different seabird colonies and a colony of Cape fur seals Arctocephalus pusillus pusillus (Schreber, 1775), respectively, (Figure 1). The methodology follows and goes further than the one used by Micarelli et al. [10]. The study period, from 2008 to 2013 in the autumn season between March and May, required a total of 247 h of effort (on average 41 h/y). ...
... Passive target preys chosen to attract sharks for passive prey discrimination and predatory observations were an odorless seal-shaped decoy and odorous buoy floating baits of tuna pieces (tuna bait), both of similar size (70 cm long and 32 cm wide juvenile Cape fur seal decoy and 60 cm in diameter floating tuna baits), in line with the research protocol adopted by Sperone et al. [10,15]. Both passive target preys were positioned at the bow 10 m from each other with the aim of testing that the odorless seal-shaped decoy remained isolated from the odorous tuna bait. ...
... Finally, we look more deeply into these sample marginal effects, referring to the average individual effects to investigate the same effects in every period (Table 5). We focus on 2008 and 2011, representing the time periods in which white sharks preferred the tuna bait rather than the seal-shaped decoy [10]. In 2011, according to the total sightings, tide events were exclusively characterized by low tide and the highest water temperature was registered. ...
Article
Full-text available
Simple Summary Predator–prey interactions can be influenced by environmental factors and depend on the sensory capabilities of a predator and its prey. Environmental factors influence the prey discrimination of white sharks in Gansbaai during their passive prey predatory behavior. Tide range is the most important factor that influences the white sharks’ prey choice, followed by underwater visibility, water temperature, and sea conditions. With high tide, better underwater visibility, cooler water temperature, and better sea conditions, sharks choose the energetical richer prey, the seal-shaped decoy, instead of the tuna bait. This study confirms the importance of visual ability in mature and immature white sharks with dietary shifts in their feeding pattern. Abstract The influence of environmental factors on prey discrimination of bait-attracted white sharks was studied over a six-year period (2008–2013) at Dyer Island Nature Reserve (Gansbaai, South Africa). Across 240 bait-attracted feeding events observed in this period, both immature and mature white sharks were attracted by the seal-shaped decoy rather than the tuna bait, except for the years 2008 and 2011. Tide ranges, underwater visibility, water temperature, and sea conditions were, in decreasing order, the factors which drove white sharks to select the seal-shaped decoy. High tide lowered the minimum depth from which sharks could approach seals close to the shore, while extended visibility helped the sharks in making predatory choices towards the more energy-rich prey source, the odorless seal-shaped decoy. On the contrary, warmer water is associated with an increase in phytoplankton that reduces underwater visibility and increases the diversity of teleosts including tuna—a known prey of white sharks—driving the sharks to favor the tuna bait. Overall, sea conditions were almost always slightly rough, ensuring a good average underwater visibility. Recommendations for future research work at this site are presented.
... to three researchers at a time and was moored to the side of the boat. White sharks were sighted at two areas in Gansbaai, Geyser Rock, and Joubertsdam, as reported by Towner et al. (2013a) [34] and Micarelli et al. (2021a) [35], because sharks sighting areas in the Gansbaai gulf changed for unknown reasons, moving from the first area to the second area, and the boat was anchored in both locations at a depth of~8-10 m, with similar bottom characteristics. ...
... Sharks were attracted to the area around the boat by chumming, following methodologies of the Ferreira and Ferreira (1996) [36], Laroche et al. (2007) [37], and Sperone et al. (2010) [17]. The chum was a mixture of seawater, cod liver oil, fish blood, and pilchards, and an additional 2-3 kg of slices of tuna were used as bait, which were maintained at the sea surface by floats following the methods described by Sperone et al. (2010) [17] and Micarelli et al. (2021a) [35]. Observations from the boat lasted 6-8 h per day, whereas underwater observations lasted 2-4 h per day. ...
Article
Full-text available
Knowledge about the social behavior of sharks is a growing research field, but not many observations are available on the social interactions between pairs of sharks in the presence of passive surface bait and mainly related to aggregations. Between 2009 and 2018, in Gansbaai, South Africa, 415 white sharks were sighted, and 525 surface-generated social interactions were identified, exhibited by 169 different white sharks. The mean sighting rate was 0.91 (range 0.18–1.53) white sharks per hour. Eight patterns of social interaction were exhibited: swim by, parallel swim, follow/give way, follow, give way, stand back, splash fights, and piggyback. Non-random interactions occurred when pairs of specimens approached the passive surface bait, confirming that the white sharks made a real choice, showing a dominance hierarchy during the ten years of data collection. Evidence of non-random social interactions in the surface behavior of bait-attracted white sharks Carcharodon carcharias in Gansbaai’s transient population was the goal of this research.
... to three researchers at a time and was moored to the side of the boat. White sharks were sighted at two areas in Gansbaai, Geyser Rock, and Joubertsdam, as reported by Towner et al. (2013a) [34] and Micarelli et al. (2021a) [35], because sharks sighting areas in the Gansbaai gulf changed for unknown reasons, moving from the first area to the second area, and the boat was anchored in both locations at a depth of~8-10 m, with similar bottom characteristics. ...
... Sharks were attracted to the area around the boat by chumming, following methodologies of the Ferreira and Ferreira (1996) [36], Laroche et al. (2007) [37], and Sperone et al. (2010) [17]. The chum was a mixture of seawater, cod liver oil, fish blood, and pilchards, and an additional 2-3 kg of slices of tuna were used as bait, which were maintained at the sea surface by floats following the methods described by Sperone et al. (2010) [17] and Micarelli et al. (2021a) [35]. Observations from the boat lasted 6-8 h per day, whereas underwater observations lasted 2-4 h per day. ...
... The opposite situation occurs for white sharks from Gansbaai (South Africa), where mild observation modules, such as swim by and parallel swimming, are more common, reflecting a calm approach and an initial hesitation of the animals (Micarelli et al., 2023). Hence, white sharks are mild observers in the hunting area, given their sophisticated visual acuity in confronting each other and in the prey selection when the investigation period is of longer duration (Martin et al., 2005;Bromilov, 2014;Micarelli et al., 2021;Micarelli et al., 2023;Reinero et al., 2022). ...
... The absence of significant isotopic differences across sex and size categories suggests similar feeding habits among specimens in this white shark population, contrary to the established literature on dietary shifts in white sharks [37,48,[52][53][54]. As previously demonstrated, dietary changes are either in progress or have been completed by 3 m size in transient Gansbaai populations [55]. Despite the known ontogenetic dietary changes in white sharks, where larger specimens typically shift to marine mammals, this study found no evidence of size-based dietary variation within the sampled population. ...
Article
Full-text available
The great white shark (Carcharodon carcharias), a keystone predator vital to marine ecosystem stability, is increasingly exposed to anthropogenic threats, including endocrine-disrupting chemicals (EDCs). This study highlights the use of non-invasive epidermal biopsies to assess physiological and ecological parameters in 28 live specimens sampled from the Dyer Island Nature Reserve, South Africa. Epidermal tissue was analyzed for vitellogenin (Vtg), a biomarker of estrogenic exposure, while dermal tissue was used for stable isotope analyses of carbon and nitrogen, essential for understanding the feeding habitat of white sharks. Vitellogenin, typically restricted to sexually mature females, was unexpectedly detected in males and immature females, indicating significant exposure to estrogenic pollutants. This finding raises concerns about the potential reproductive and population-level impacts on this vulnerable species. Stable isotope analyses confirmed that dermal tissue alone is sufficient for trophic studies, eliminating the need for deeper muscle sampling. By demonstrating that epidermal and dermal tissues provide critical data for both biomarkers and isotopic studies, this research supports the adoption of minimally invasive techniques. Shallower biopsies reduce stress on the animals, making this method a valuable tool for conservation research and management of C. carcharias.
... Ontogenetic shifts in diet have been accompanied by morphological shifts in the head shape, for example, of tiger sharks (Fu et al., 2016); however, our analyses indicate a continuous growth of the head shape in all variables measured, except for the negative allometric growth observed in the mouth length (Table 2). White sharks are known to be heavily reliant on olfaction for foraging (Martin et al., 2005;Micarelli et al., 2021) and, as such, present the largest olfactory bulb across shark species (Yopak et al., 2014). The continuous increase in the head region anterior to the mouth incorporating the primary senses for potential prey detection (i.e., ampullae of Lorenzini and olfaction) may be an anatomical adaptation to accommodate these acute senses to assist in foraging. ...
Article
Full-text available
The analysis of how biological shape changes across ontogeny can provide us with valuable information on how species adapt behaviorally, physiologically, and ecologically. The white shark Carcharodon carcharias is one of the largest and most widely distributed apex predators globally, yet an understanding of ontogenetic changes in body shape and relative scaling of length and weight measures is limited, especially in relation to foraging ecology. Through analysis of a suite of shape‐related metrics, we identified ontogenetic patterns of scaling throughout development. Isometric growth was exhibited for most metrics, failing to show a significant deviation from an isometric slope of 1.0 for length–length relationships, and 3.0 for weight–length relationships. The most notable difference from this trend was the negative allometric growth observed for the upper caudal‐fin lobe length, trunk length, and the mouth length. The surface area of the fins also presented a strong, positive relationship with precaudal length (PCL) and the girth at the pectoral fin. Negative allometric growth was exhibited for three of the fins (pectoral, upper caudal fin, and lower caudal fin) against PCL, exhibiting a significant deviation from the expected isometric growth of 2.0 for area–length relationships. There were no significant differences in morphometric relationships between geographic regions within Australia that samples were collected from. No differences between the sexes were identified; however, this may be an artifact of the lack of mature animal samples. Conversely, life stage was found to have a significant effect on the girth–length and weight–length relationships. The development of regression equations for morphometric measures allows the assessment of white shark body condition and may serve as an assessment tool to understand the potential impacts of human‐induced environmental change on white sharks.
Article
Full-text available
In Gansbaai (South Africa), at Dyer Island Nature Reserve, a large White shark population is present and can be observed due to the support of local ecotourism operators authorised to reach the field observation sites. Between 2009 and 2019, it was possible to create a database including information about each individual observed. In total, 423 white sharks were sighted during 462 direct observation hours from the boat, that included 220 hours from the diving "cage". The mean sighting rate was 0.91 (range 0.18-1.53) sharks per hour and sighting rates dramatically declined in the last three years of the study period. Ninety-nine unique Photo-Ids of the dorsal fin were collected and only five re-sightings occurred, which indicate a transient behaviour for the Gansbaai White shark population. The sex ratio showed that females were always prevalent over males throughout the duration of the observations: the ratios were 1:2.2:0.8 for males, females, and unsexed sharks, respectively, and showed the prevalence of immature female individuals (immature: 51 males, 201 females, and 40 unsexed; adults: 49 males, 14 females, and 1 unsexed; undefined maturity: 5 males, 19 females, and 43 unsexed sharks). The predominance of immatures only applies to the females; there were as many immature males (51) as mature (49). The total length for all the individuals was between 150 cm and 500 cm (mean 308 cm, n = 423) with few young-of-the-year and adults recorded, indicating that Gansbaai Area is not a nursery area nor an adult aggregation site, but a seasonal feeding ground. The interannual sighting trend showed a consistent long-term increasing peak (ca. 4-5 years) and this could confirm that, in Gansbaai, the White shark frequency is not affected by ecotourism but, since 2017, a consistent loss of sightings was also due to recorded transient killer whales' unusual fatal attacks.
Article
Full-text available
Demographic differences in resource use are key components of population and species ecology across the animal kingdom. White sharks (Carcharodon carcharias) are migratory, apex predators, which have undergone significant population declines across their range. Understanding their ecology is key to ensuring that management strategies are effective. Here, we carry out the first stable isotope analyses of free-swimming white sharks in South Africa. Biopsies were collected in Gansbaai (34.5805°S, 19.3518°E) between February and July 2015. We used Stable Isotope Bayesian Ellipsis in R and traditional statistical analyses to quantify and compare isotopic niches of male and female sharks of two size classes, and analyse relationships between isotopic values and shark length. Our results reveal cryptic trophic differences between the sexes and life stages. Males, but not females, were inferred to feed in more offshore or westerly habitats as they grow larger, and only males exhibited evidence of an ontogenetic niche shift. Lack of relationship between δ13C, δ15N and female shark length may be caused by females exhibiting multiple migration and foraging strategies, and a greater propensity to travel further north. Sharks < 3 m had much wider, and more diverse niches than sharks > 3 m, drivers of which may include individual dietary specialisation and temporal factors. The differences in migratory and foraging behaviour between sexes, life stages, and individuals will affect their exposure to anthropogenic threats, and should be considered in management strategies.
Article
Full-text available
A long-term sightings and photographic identification (photo-ID) database documented the inter-annual and monthly trends in white shark (Carcharodon carcharias) sightings, sex ratios and life-history stages at Seal Island, False Bay, South Africa, over the period 2004–2012. A total of 1105 sightings were recorded during 171 scientific surveys incorporating 577 h of observation (annual mean 64.10 h, range 23.71–178.69 h). The mean annual sighting rate was 1.87 (range 0.90–3.19) sharks per hour and sighting rates declined over the nine-year study period. It is unknown whether the decline resulted from an actual population decline, or was due to changes in shark distribution or environmental conditions, but it highlights the need for ongoing monitoring at this site. The overall sex ratio was 1.0: 1.0: 2.3 for male: female: unsexed sharks, respectively. Of the 1105 sightings, 39% (n = 433) were photo-ID’d, representing 303 individual sharks (112: 111: 80, male: female: unsexed) and 130 resightings. Of the 303 photo-ID’d sharks, 71% were sighted in a single year only, indicative of transient behaviour. Of the 29% of sharks that were resighted in more than one year, 65% were resighted in the following year, indicating site fidelity to Seal Island, whereas 35% skipped one or more years between encounters, indicative of a temporary absence. The majority (60%) of the photo-ID’d sharks were immature, 32% were subadult and only 8% were mature. No young-of-the-year sharks and few adults were recorded, indicating that Seal Island is not an adult aggregation site, nor a pupping or nursery area, but rather is best described as a seasonal feeding ground. Large females were rarely resighted again after they approached length-at-maturity (≥450 cm TL), whereas some adult males were recorded consistently across years. The emigration of subadult female sharks approaching maturity from Seal Island, combined with the small number of mature sharks of both sexes reported from any South African location, indicate that adult aggregation sites, and thus areas of reproductive importance, still remain unknown.
Chapter
Full-text available
AUGUST 2006 White shark conservation and recreational safety in the inshore waters of Cape Town, South Africa P r o c e e d i n g s A u g u s t 2 0 0 6 Foreword The contents of these proceedings are the result of over a year's foresight and planning by the City of Cape Town, the Department of Environmental Affairs and Tourism (DEAT) and the World Wide Fund for Nature (WWF) South Africa. Following a number of shark attacks between 2001 and 2004, the City of Cape Town recognized that a proactive approach to addressing white shark management and recreational safety was needed. Central to this challenge was not only the apparent paradox between the objectives of recreational safety and white shark conservation, but also the lack of clarity about the roles and responsibilities of different government departments. In June 2005, the City and its key partners, DEAT and WWF, started a process to assess the full range of possible causes and potential management responses so as to accurately inform the decision making process. WWF was approached to assist in facilitating a workshop of specialists in the fields of white shark conservation and recreational safety. A number of recognised experts were invited to contribute written papers on a broad range of topics. The workshop, held on 29 & 30 May 2006 in Cape Town, was characterized by very constructive debate and was successful in adopting a number of key conclusions and recommendations based on the information presented. Whilst these recommendations do not carry any legislative weight, they do constitute the collective opinion of the most highly regarded experts in these fields in South Africa and are backed up by fifteen scientific and background papers that were submitted to the workshop and reviewed by the editors. These recommendations have played a central role in informing the City's Draft White Shark and Coastal Recreational Safety Policy and Strategy.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
The White Shark Carcharodon carcharias L. is a top predator worldwide distributed. Its high survival and adaptation capacity is related with high developed senses, used to efficiently interact with a target. During daily activities of cage diving in the Marine Reserve of Dyer Island (South Africa) the behavioural patterns of the white sharks in front of different targets were observed. Previous studies showed that in an unnatural situation, as the cage diving is, the white shark displays a curiosity approach and it long investigate the situation before an attack. The aim of the present study was to understand if a white shark, in front of a still floating decoy similar to the natural prey (Cape fur seal, Arctocephalus pusillus pusillus) and a bloody piece of tuna fish, makes a choice based upon the sigh or the smell. The preliminary data analysis showed that the choice of the target could be related to size (and consequently) age of sharks. We also analyzed the type of approach (vertical, horizontal or diagonal) and observed that in this case the choice could be related to light and weather conditions. A correlation between type of approach and type of target (decoy or bait) was also observed: usually vertical tactics were more often used in presence of sealshaped decoy, while horizontal tactics were showed in presence of tuna bait. Our observations suggest that these patterns appear to be adapted for exploiting a challenging suite of surface-dwelling prey species and may be the basis of a speculative hunting strategy wherein individual sacrifice much of the possibility of identifying a potential prey item in exchange for an increased chance of capture.