ArticlePDF Available

Public acceptance of evolution in the United States, 1985–2020

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The public acceptance of evolution in the United States is a long-standing problem. Using data from a series of national surveys collected over the last 35 years, we find that the level of public acceptance of evolution has increased in the last decade after at least two decades in which the public was nearly evenly divided on the issue. A structural equation model indicates that increasing enrollment in baccalaureate-level programs, exposure to college-level science courses, a declining level of religious fundamentalism, and a rising level of civic scientific literacy are responsible for the increased level of public acceptance.
Content may be subject to copyright.
https://doi.org/10.1177/09636625211035919
Public Understanding of Science
1 –16
© The Author(s) 2021
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/09636625211035919
journals.sagepub.com/home/pus
P U S
Public acceptance of evolution
in the United States, 1985–2020
Jon D. Miller
University of Michigan, USA
Eugenie C. Scott
National Center for Science Education, USA
Mark S. Ackerman
University of Michigan, USA
Belén Laspra
University of Oviedo, Spain
Glenn Branch
National Center for Science Education, USA
Carmelo Polino
University of Oviedo, Spain; Centro Redes, Argentina
Jordan S. Huffaker
University of Michigan, USA
Abstract
The public acceptance of evolution in the United States is a long-standing problem. Using data from a series
of national surveys collected over the last 35 years, we find that the level of public acceptance of evolution
has increased in the last decade after at least two decades in which the public was nearly evenly divided on
the issue. A structural equation model indicates that increasing enrollment in baccalaureate-level programs,
exposure to college-level science courses, a declining level of religious fundamentalism, and a rising level of
civic scientific literacy are responsible for the increased level of public acceptance.
Corresponding author:
Jon D. Miller, Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, 426 Thompson Street, Ann Arbor, MI 48106, USA.
Email: jondmiller@umich.edu
1035919PUS0010.1177/09636625211035919Public Understanding of ScienceMiller et al.
research-article2021
Research article
2 Public Understanding of Science 00(0)
Keywords
biology/evolution, public acceptance, structural equation model, time series, United States
Acceptance of evolution in the United States increased substantially in the last decade, following
two decades of parity in the acceptance or rejection of evolution by American adults. Since 1985,
national samples of US adults have been asked to agree or disagree with the statement “Human
beings, as we know them today, developed from earlier species of animals.” During the last decade,
the percentage of US adults agreeing with this statement increased from 40% to 54%—a majority.
The level of acceptance of evolution in the United States is atypically low for a developed nation.1
In a study of the acceptance of evolution in 34 developed nations in 2005, only Turkey—at 27%—
scored lower than the United States (Miller et al., 2006). Even with the level of acceptance of evolu-
tion at 54% in 2019–2020, it is likely that the United States remains low in international rankings.2
This article quantifies and analyzes recent trends in the relative influence of American funda-
mentalist religious beliefs, ideological polarization, college-level science education, and other fac-
tors affecting the public acceptance of evolution in the United States. To do this, we utilize a
three-decade-long time series of US national surveys. We provide a statistical description of the
pattern of acceptance of evolution and use a structural equation model (SEM) to identify the pri-
mary variables that predict the acceptance of biological evolution among American adults in 2019.
The data
The data for this time series analysis come from a combination of (a) several years of biennial
National Science Board surveys that were used to produce the Science and Engineering Indicators
reports to Congress, (b) several national surveys funded by other units of the National Science
Foundation, and (c) a new time series focused on adult civic scientific literacy (CSL) funded by the
National Aeronautics and Space Administration. During the three decades covered by this analysis,
the modes and methods of survey data collection have changed. The items used in this analysis
were collected by a mix of telephone interviews and online questionnaires, but there is no correla-
tion between the modes of data collection and acceptance of evolution. For a description of spon-
sorship and methods, see the Supplemental Material.
The patterns of evolution acceptance and rejection since 1985
There are no adequate probability surveys on American attitudes toward evolution prior to the
Second World War. Researchers of American views of evolution employed a variety of questions
over the postwar years (Plutzer and Berkman, 2008). Despite the variety of questions, American
opinion about evolution was divided during the second half of the twentieth century and fewer than
half of Americans accepted evolution in these surveys (Brenan, 2019; IPSOS, 2011; Kampourakis,
2020; Miller et al., 2006; Pew Research Center, 2013, 2015, 2019b; Plutzer and Berkman, 2008;
Swift, 2017).
Starting in 1985, a series of surveys used “Human beings, as we know them today, developed
from earlier species of animals” to investigate attitudes toward evolution. An examination of data
from these surveys indicates that Americans were evenly divided on the evolution question from
1985 through 2007 with the exception of the 1999 respondents,3 but more than half of American
adults now indicate that they accept evolution (see Figure 1). The increase in acceptance of evolu-
tion reflects a small decline in overt rejection and a larger decline in the proportion of American
adults who are unsure about the evolution issue.
Miller et al. 3
Gallup (Brenan, 2019; Swift, 2017) and the Pew Research Center (2019b) have used a similar
question that asks respondents to indicate whether they think that humans have evolved over time
and, if so, whether God had any role in this process. Plutzer and Berkman (2008), the Pew Research
Center (2019b), and Kampourakis (2020) have examined the impact of question wording and
found conflicting advantages and disadvantages to the inclusion of multiple dimensions in the
same question. We think that the simple question asking whether humans evolved over a long
period is a useful and clearer indicator of respondent acceptance or rejection of evolution.
To explain the observed increase in acceptance of evolution by American adults, we look at
several factors that we hypothesize are associated with the acceptance or rejection of evolution.
The factors predicting acceptance or rejection of evolution
Bivariate cross-tabulations allow us to examine the strength of each variable’s association with the
public acceptance of evolution and to see how this relationship changed between 1988 and 2019.
We first discuss the demographic variables (age, gender, educational attainment, college science
courses taken, minor children at home), in decreasing order of strength (as measured by the abso-
lute value of the ordinal correlation coefficient gamma) in the 2019 data (see Table 1).4
Figure 1. Public acceptance and rejection of evolution in the United States, 1985–2020.
The following question was used in all of the years in this analysis:
For each statement below, please indicate if you think that it is definitely true, probably true, probably false, or definitely
false. If you don’t know or aren’t sure, please check the “not sure” box.
Human beings, as we know them today, developed from earlier species of animals.
The number of respondents in each year and the confidence intervals are provided in the Supplemental Material in SI
Table 1.
4 Public Understanding of Science 00(0)
Completion of one or more college science courses was the strongest predictor of adult accept-
ance of evolution among the demographic variables (see Table 1). An important part of American
higher education is a unique requirement for students to complete a year of general education
courses, including science courses, to earn an accredited baccalaureate (Hyman et al., 1975; Muller,
2010). The gamma for this relationship was 0.23 in 1988 and 0.25 in 2019, indicating the stable and
substantial influence of college science courses.
The level of educational attainment (highest degree earned) has been positively associated with
the acceptance of evolution during the last 30 years (see Table 1). It is important to note that the
percentage of American adults who earned a baccalaureate increased from 20% in 1988 to 35% in
2018 (United States Census Bureau, 2019).
Over the last 30 years, age has had a negative relationship with the acceptance of evolution (see
Table 1). In 1988, the gamma for this relationship was –0.18 and 30 years later it was –0.17, indi-
cating that the influence of age on the acceptance of evolution has remained stable over the last
Table 1. The acceptance of evolution, by selected factors: 1988, 2019.
1988 2019
% accept
evolution
gamma % accept
evolution
gamma
All adults 46 54
Age
18–24 54 –0.18 68 –0.17
25–34 50 65
35–44 53 54
45–54 41 50
55–64 36 45
65 and above 37 45
Gender
Male 52 –0.23 57 –0.13
Female 41 51
Education
Less than high school 41 0.19 53 0.20
High school grad/GED 44 47
Associate degree 38 54
Baccalaureate 61 58
Graduate/prof degree 67 69
College science courses
None 42 0.23 47 0.25
1–3 courses 51 58
4 or more courses 59 65
Minor children at home
None 46 ns 54 –0.06
One or more 46 52
Number of cases 2,041 2,738
ns = not significant at the .05 level.
Although data concerning the proportion of American adults who accept evolution is available from 1985 through 2020,
not all of the predictor variables were asked in each of those years. Some items measuring religious fundamentalism
were not asked in 1985 and 1988. The year-to-year variation is not significant at the .05 level, so we utilize 1988 and
2019 as the end points of a three-decade distribution for illustration purposes.
Miller et al. 5
three decades. The negative sign indicates that younger respondents are slightly more likely to
accept evolution than older respondents.
Throughout the last three decades, a higher proportion of men than women have accepted evo-
lution, but the magnitude of this difference is declining (see Table 1). This narrowing of the gender
difference may reflect the growth of educational attainment during the same period—the percent-
age of women with a baccalaureate increased from 17% to 35% over the last 30 years (United
States Census Bureau, 2019).
Finally, another family demographic of interest is the number of minor children in the respond-
ent’s home. Although we often think of adults as transmitting science information to their children,
research in recent decades has shown that many parents learn new science information through
helping their children with homework, assisting with science fair projects, and answering questions
that occur during science museum visits or the viewing of science-related events on television
(Bengtson et al., 2002; Shumow and Miller, 2001). Increasingly, children may ask their parents
about issues like climate change. Educational scholars often refer to this as the science fair effect.
In this analysis, the results from 1988 indicated that the presence of minor children at home was
not related to the acceptance of evolution, but the 2019 results show a small negative relationship
(gamma = –.06), indicating that the parents of minor children were slightly less likely to accept
evolution than adults without minor children at home. We will return to this issue in our SEM
analysis below.
In addition to personal and family demographic variables, other relevant variables were consid-
ered (religious fundamentalism, civic scientific literacy [CSL], ideological partisanship, level of
interest in selected science policy issues, the use of informal science education resources, and pat-
terns of individual science information acquisition). We discuss them below in decreasing order of
strength (as measured by the absolute value of the ordinal correlation coefficient gamma) in the
2019 data.
First, any analysis of the acceptance of evolution by American adults must take into account the
impact of religious fundamentalism (Chaves, 2017; Emerson and Hartman, 2006; Putnam and
Campbell, 2010; Wilcox and Larson, 2006). All of the surveys used in this analysis include some
measures of religious fundamentalism, but they sometimes used slightly different questions. For
those years in which all five of the variables5 used to measure religious fundamentalism were avail-
able, we used a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to determine that all of the items loaded on a
single factor. The factor scores were used to compute a zero-to-10 index for each year. The gamma
for the relationship between the CFA-derived Index of Religious Fundamentalism and the accept-
ance of evolution was –0.71 in 2007 and –0.71 in 2019, indicating that there was no change in this
relationship over the last 12 years. A description of the 2007 and 2019 CFAs is included in the
Supplemental Material for this article.
The next strongest factor was CSL, a measure of the ability of citizens to read about and make
sense of public policy issues involving science and technology6 (Miller, 1983, 1987, 1998, 2000,
2010a, 2012; Miller et al., 1997; Shen, 1975). The number of college science courses taken and
educational attainment (discussed earlier) are strong predictors of CSL. CSL was assessed in the
1988 and 2019 surveys, and the results demonstrate a strong relationship to the acceptance of evo-
lution over the last 30 years, with gammas of 0.59 and 0.51, respectively (see Table 2). The small
decline in the strength of the association may reflect the growing proportion of American adults
who qualified as civic scientifically literate over the last three decades—an increase from 11% in
1988 to 30% in 2019 (Miller, 2010a).
For the last 100 years, the acceptance or rejection of evolution has been politicized in the United
States. The growth of ideological partisanship and affective partisanship in the postwar years and
its acceleration in recent decades has made evolution a component of the alliance of evangelical
6 Public Understanding of Science 00(0)
Table 2. The acceptance of evolution, by selected factors: 1988, 2019.
1988* 2019
% accept
evolution
gamma % accept
evolution
gamma
All adults 46 54
Religious fundamentalism (2007 and 2019)
Index score 0–1 78 –0.70 91 –0.71
Index score 2–3 74 69
Index score 4–6 39 39
Index score 7–8 11 18
Index score 9–10 8 32
Civic scientific literacy
Not scientifically literate 43 0.59 45 0.51
Scientifically literate 74 74
Ideological partisanship (2007 and 2019)
Conservative Republican 25 0.26 34 0.34
Moderate Republican 40 49
Conservative independent 37 41
Independent 41 51
Liberal independent 65 78
Moderate Democrat 35 53
Liberal Democrat 73 83
Science information acquisition
None 45 0.22 50 0.21
1–26 times/year 37 56
27–52 times/year 41 67
53–104 times/year 43 57
105 or more times/year 59 71
Science issue interest
No science issue interest 44 0.16 44 0.19
Very interested in one issue 36 49
Very interested in two issues 50 53
Very interested in three issues 51 56
Very interested in 4+ issues 56 65
Informal science learning resource use
0–12 times/year 46 0.08 50 0.07
13–24 times/year 44 57
25–36 times/year 42 56
37–48 times/year 47 53
49–60+ times/year 54 58
Number of respondents 2041 2738
*Although data concerning the proportion of American adults who accept evolution is available from 1985 through
2020, some of the predictor variables were not asked in each of those years. Items measuring religious fundamentalism
were not asked in 1985 and 1988 and are reported for 2007 instead.
Christianity and the Republican Party (Abramowitz., 2010, 2018; Levandusky, 2009; Williams,
2012); evolution has emerged as an ideological litmus test of conservatism. Using our seven-point
scale of ideological partisanship in 2019, we observe that 83% of liberal Democrats accept
Miller et al. 7
evolution compared to 34% of conservative Republicans (see Table 2). The relationship is roughly
linear. To assess the relative importance of ideological partisanship in the context of religious fun-
damentalism and college science course exposures, the same seven-point scale of ideological par-
tisanship is included in our SEM.
An increasingly important—and changing—measure concerns adult science information acqui-
sition. The traditional broadcast model of information dissemination is being replaced by a just-in-
time information acquisition system that takes advantage of the Internet and related digital
communication technologies (Case and Given, 2016; Miller, 2010b, 2010c; Miller et al., 2021).
Because of the changes in information technologies over the 30 years, our measures have varied to
reflect the dominant technologies and practices of the time. For example, the 1988 survey did not
ask about acquiring information from the Internet because the first home-based Internet service
was not introduced until 1989. In 2019, the relationship between the frequency of science informa-
tion acquisition (which includes the Internet, various wireless communication services, electronic
and print books and newspapers, and face-to-face and electronic discussions with friends and fam-
ily) and acceptance of evolution was relatively strong, with a gamma of 0.21 (see Table 2).
The level of individual interest in various science and technology policy issues might be
expected to be an important indicator to include in this analysis. Some individuals frequently read
about and pay attention to (a) new scientific discoveries, (b) new medical discoveries, (c) new
inventions and technologies, (d) space exploration, and/or (e) climate change. In this analysis,
respondents were asked about all five of these issues in 2019, but respondents to our 1988 survey
were not asked about climate change. In 1988, the major environmental issue concerned acid rain;
climate change was not a part of the public policy vocabulary for most American adults. Although
the measures are not identical, they are comparable, and the results indicate that adults who follow
science policy issues are only slightly more likely to accept evolution (see Table 2).
A related measure of adult involvement with science and technology is the frequency of use of
traditional science learning resources such as science museums, natural history museums, planetar-
iums, zoos, aquariums, and arboretums. The results indicate that the frequency of use of informal
science learning resources is positively related to the likelihood of accepting evolution, although
not strongly (see Table 2).
A model to predict the acceptance of evolution
The factors discussed in our bivariate analysis do not operate independently. The acceptance or
rejection of evolution does not occur spontaneously; it is a dependent belief mediated by mental
schemas and life experiences (Axelrod, 1973; Schank and Abelson, 1977; Sternberg and Ben-Zeev,
2001). Each individual has an age and a gender, has completed some level of education, has expe-
rienced or not experienced some college-level science courses, has some ideological partisan
views, and engages in various forms of adult learning and information acquisition. He or she may
or may not hold fundamentalist religious views. A SEM provides an analysis that can estimate the
relative influence of each of these factors in a multivariate context (Hayduk, 1987; Jöreskog and
Sörbom, 1993). We use our 2019 data.
A SEM is a path model that takes chronological and logical order into account in seeking to
understand the predictive power of independent variables in the model (Hayduk, 1987; Jöreskog
and Sörbom, 1993). In the diagram of a SEM, influence flows from left to right, reflecting chrono-
logical or logical order (see Figure 2). For example, the level of education that individuals attain
may be influenced by their age or gender or both, but their level of educational attainment cannot
influence or change their age or gender, so age and gender are placed to the left of educational
attainment. Whenever there is a statistically significant relationship between two variables in this
8 Public Understanding of Science 00(0)
Variables Total
Effect
Religious fundamentalism -0.60(.03)
Civic scientific literacy 0.32(.06)
College science courses 0.23(.03)
Education (highest degree attained) 0.19(.03)
Age (in six ordinal categories) -0.19(.03)
Ideological partisanship (liberal Democrat high) 0.18(.03)
Interest in science, technology, medical, space, or climate issues 0.08(.03)
Minor children in the home -0.08(.02)
Science information acquisition activities 0.07(.02)
Gender (female) -0.05(.01)
Use of informal science learning resources 0.00(.00)
R2 0.50
Chi-squares = 141.8; degrees of freedom = 31; Root Mean Square Error of
Approximation (RMSEA) = 0.009; the upper 10% confidence interval of
RMSEA = 0.019; N = 2,146.
Figure 2. A model to predict acceptance of evolution, 2019.
model, they are connected by an arrow reflecting the direction of the influence. Each arrow is
associated with a coefficient ranging from –1.0 to +1.0, a measure of the relative strength of that
relationship. The absence of an arrow means that the relationship between the variables was not
statistically significant at the .05 level.
Miller et al. 9
To estimate the total influence of each variable in the model on the outcome variable—here the
acceptance of evolution—the SEM computes a total effect, ranging from –1.0 to +1.0, which is the
sum of the products of all of the path coefficients in any path that runs from a specific variable to
the outcome variable (see the example in the Supplemental Material). While the total effect lacks
a Proportional Reduction in Error (PRE) interpretation, it is a good indicator of the relative influ-
ence of variables in any specific model.
The SEM for the 2019 data indicates that religious fundamentalism is the strongest predictor of
acceptance of evolution, with a total effect of –0.60 (see Figure 2). The negative effect means that,
holding constant the preceding variables in the model, individuals with a high level of religious
fundamentalism are unlikely to accept evolution.
The level of CSL is the second strongest predictor of the acceptance of evolution, with a total
effect of 0.32 (see Figure 2). The successful acquisition of CSL is strongly influenced by the num-
ber of college science courses completed and the highest level of education attained (Miller, 1998,
2000, 2010a), which are the third and fourth strongest predictors of the acceptance of evolution,
with total effects of 0.23 and 0.19, respectively (see Figure 2). Age, with a total effect of –0.19, is
the fourth strongest predictor.
Consistent with prior research, individuals with more formal education and more experience
with college science courses tend to have a higher level of interest in science and technology
broadly and in public policy regarding science and technology specifically. In the 2019 model,
individuals with more interest in science and technology issues were more likely to accept evolu-
tion (total effect = 0.08). Adults with higher levels of educational attainment and exposure to col-
lege science courses were significantly less likely to hold fundamentalist religious beliefs.
The level of liberal ideological partisanship is weakly predicted by female gender (0.10) and col-
lege science courses (0.07). The intensity of ideological partisanship is U-shaped, with the highest
level reported by conservative Republicans and liberal Democrats. This means that the small posi-
tive total effect between female gender and liberal ideological partisanship would translate into a
similar small positive relationship between male gender and conservative ideological partisanship.
The scale on which ideological partisanship is scored is nominal and has no substantive meaning per
se. But the two ends of the ideological partisanship scale have very different views of evolution with
a total effect of 0.18, meaning that individuals with liberal ideological partisan views are more likely
to accept evolution than individuals with conservative political views. We will return to a discussion
of the relative impact of ideological partisanship in our closing discussion.
Prior research indicates that higher levels of educational attainment and participation in college
science courses provides a set of cognitive tools that enable individuals to more effectively seek
science information in the Internet era (Miller, 2010b, 2010c; Miller et al., 2021; van Dijk, 2020).
The 2019 model is consistent with this view: individuals who report a higher level of overt infor-
mation-seeking on the Internet and related digital platforms were slightly more likely to accept
evolution than individuals who did not engage in this kind of science information-seeking (total
effect = 0.07), holding constant the preceding demographic and educational variables.
The total effect of minor children in the respondent’s household was –0.08 (see Figure 2). The
SEM indicates that the total effect of gender (with female coded as 1 and male as 0) on acceptance
of evolution is –0.05, lower than might be suggested by the raw percentages of men and women
who accept evolution, reflecting the differential gender impact on other intervening variables in the
model.
Older adults were less likely to accept evolution than younger adults (total effect = –0.19). The
paths in Figure 2 from age to the acceptance of evolution indicate that this result is the product of
having a lower exposure to college science courses, holding more fundamentalist religious beliefs,
and being less active in seeking current science information.
10 Public Understanding of Science 00(0)
Overall, the SEM is a good fit for the data, accounting for 50% of the total covariance in the
model (see Figure 2). This level of fit indicates that the structural assumptions of the model fit the
available data, and the path diagram shows a clear and understandable set of pathways of variables
relevant to the acceptance of evolution.
Discussion
The level of public acceptance of evolution in the United States has increased in the last decade—a
majority of American adults now accept biological evolution (see Figure 1). Given the changes over
time in this series, we infer that the increase in acceptance of evolution after 2008 is largely due to
changes in the “unsure” population, rather than the conversion of deniers of evolution, although there
was a small decline in the proportion of Americans who held fundamentalist views during this period.
Considering the comparison of the 1988 and 2019 data and the SEM for 2019, it is now possible
to address two important questions: What factors are related to the increased level of acceptance of
evolution in the United States? And what factors are related to the level of rejection of evolution in
the United States—still higher than that found in the vast majority of developed countries (Branch,
2009) and even in some countries with less industrialized and emerging economies (Branch 2009;
Kampourakis, 2020; Pew Research Center, 2020).
To answer the first question, education plays a significant role in the increased level of accept-
ance of evolution. Survey results from 1988 and 2019 found a stable positive association between
the level of educational attainment and the acceptance of evolution over the last three decades. The
proportion of American adults who earned at least a baccalaureate almost doubled over this 30-year
period—from 20% in 1988 to 35% in 2018 (United States Census Bureau, 2019). The influence of
participation in college science courses increased over the same period, demonstrating the impor-
tance of the uniquely American requirement for general education at the baccalaureate level. From
the 1980s, with John Moore’s seminal “Science as a Way of Knowing” series of essays on the
teaching of undergraduate biology to a recent Gordon Conference on understanding undergraduate
biology education, biologists have been debating how to improve undergraduate education (Austin,
2018; Dolan et al., 2020; Society for Integrative Comparative Biology, n.d.). Notably, teaching
evolution was central to Moore’s project, as well as to subsequent efforts by the AAAS, the National
Research Council, the Howard Hughes Medical Institute, and others. It is not a surprise to find an
increase in college-level science course associated with an increase in the acceptance of evolution.
The association plausibly may be both direct and mediated through secondary science education
since public high school biology teachers are more likely to teach evolution as a matter of scientific
consensus if they have studied evolution themselves at the college level (Plutzer et al., 2020).
The proportion of American adults qualifying as civic scientifically literate increased from 11%
in 1988 to 31% in 2019 (Miller, 2010a), and the relationship between CSL and acceptance of evo-
lution was steady with a gamma of approximately .50 throughout the last three decades. This
underscores the linkage between formal science education and the acceptance of evolution. The
weak positive relationship between the level of overt science information seeking and acceptance
of evolution suggests that information acquisition activities and informal science learning broadly
may provide important assistance to adults to sustain and enrich their level of CSL during the adult
years following post-secondary study. But there is no evidence that informal science learning or
focused information acquisition can substitute for formal education.
Changes in the American religious landscape may have influenced the change in the acceptance
of evolution shown here. Religious participation and adherence to organized religion are declining
(although still high in comparison to other developed countries) and confidence in the literal truth
of the Bible has declined (Chaves, 2017). A reduction in adherence to conservative Protestant
Miller et al. 11
beliefs might contribute to an increase in the acceptance of evolution. The percentage of Americans
who reject evolution remains high in comparison to other developed countries, which leads to the
second question regarding the continuing high level of rejection of evolution in the United States.
Religious fundamentalism plays a significant role in the rejection of evolution. The historical
explanation of the low rate of acceptance of evolution in the United States involves the central
place of the Bible in American Protestantism. In a country settled piecemeal by colonists of vary-
ing religious views and without a state church, it was natural for people of faith, especially
Protestants who already accepted the principle of sola Scriptura, to privilege the Bible—or their
interpretation of it—as the primary source of religious authority and an inerrant source of informa-
tion about history and science as well as faith and morals. In contrast, religion in European coun-
tries is strongly structured by ecclesiastic institutions and the public receptivity to creationism has
been limited as a result (Blancke et al., 2014; Branch, 2009).
It is thus a particular form of religion that is at the foundation of American anti-evolutionism of
the twentieth and twenty-first centuries, not religion in general (see Coyne, 2012, for a dissenting
view). Indeed, evolution is routinely taught in Catholic parochial schools in the United States, and
mainstream Protestant denominations similarly accept evolution (Martin, 2010). While not all anti-
evolutionism originates in Fundamentalism and its inerrantism about the Bible, it largely reflects a
conservative form of Protestantism with relatively inflexible and inerrantist religious views (Scott,
2009), which we have been calling fundamentalism.
Hill (2014) argues that religious beliefs are a stronger predictor of attitude toward evolution
than education in the United States. Using odds-ratio data in a set of logistic regression models,
Hill employs only a tripartite measure of educational attainment, including no measures of scien-
tific literacy or understanding, and finds that respondents’ view of the Bible is a stronger predictor.
He also finds that living in a homogeneous religious community with reinforcement from a reli-
gious social network increases the impact of Biblical beliefs. While agreeing with the primacy of
religious belief in predicting attitudes toward evolution, we contend that Hill’s use of ordinary least
squares regression models with limited measures of educational attainment or scientific literacy
produces an enhanced estimate of the impact of religiosity and a reduced estimate of the influence
of education and literacy. We believe that the SEM analysis model we use provides a more accurate
portrait of the factors associated with the acceptance or rejection of evolution. Applying binary
logistic regression models to data from a different national survey, Baker (2013) offers an assess-
ment of the influence of both religion and educational variables similar to ours, concluding that
higher education influences the acceptance of evolution only for individuals who do not hold a
literalist interpretation of the Bible.
Our 2019 SEM documents the continuing tension between religious fundamentalism and the
acceptance of evolution in the United States. Although scientific literacy has grown, and science
continues to have pervasive influence in American society, a tension between religious fundamen-
talism and evolution remains. We think that there are two primary reasons.
First, a significant portion of American adults continues to hold fundamentalist religious beliefs.
Although there is some disagreement over the definition of religious fundamentalism (Marsden,
1991; Williams, 2012), in broad terms, approximately 30% of American adults hold fundamentalist
religious beliefs largely rooted in Protestantism. Religious fundamentalists are not only more likely
to reject evolution themselves, but are also more likely to support efforts to undermine the teaching
of evolution in the public schools (Baker, 2013; Berkman and Plutzer, 2010; Branch et al., 2010).
Over the last century, such efforts have taken various forms—banning the teaching of evolution
outright, balancing evolution with biblical creationism, creation science, or intelligent design, or
belittling evolution as controversial. Even if most of the efforts have not been successful, the
intended effect is to encourage students to reject evolution.
12 Public Understanding of Science 00(0)
Second, the acceptance or rejection of evolution has been politicized in recent decades.
According to the Pew Research Center (2015), 54% of Republicans and 64% of Democrats
accepted human evolution in 2009; this 10-point gap grew to a 24-point gap 4 years later, when
43% of Republicans and 67% of Democrats accepted evolution. The Republican Party has culti-
vated conservative Christians who share “a Protestant-based moral order” (Williams, 2012) as part
of its base. In local elections, such as for school board, the teaching of evolution may be electorally
salient, especially in smaller communities with a large proportion of religiously conservative citi-
zens. While not as much a “litmus test” of Republican identity as abortion or the rejection of
anthropogenic climate change (Pew Research Center, 2019a), anti-evolutionism is part of the polit-
ical mix, and may augment fundamentalism in maintaining the core of anti-evolutionism in
American society.
To provide a more precise estimate of the role of ideological partisanship in the acceptance or
rejection of evolution, we included a measure of ideological partisanship in our 2019 SEM. In this
model, ideological partisanship has a total effect of 0.18, indicating that liberal Democrats were
more likely to accept evolution holding constant other preceding variables and, conversely, that
conservative Republicans were less likely to accept evolution holding constant other preceding
variables. Because this model is cross-sectional rather than longitudinal, we have no evidence
concerning the early development of attitudes toward evolution, but this model indicates that
among adults, ideological partisanship is not the driving force in influencing evolution attitude.
What does the future hold for the acceptance of evolution in the United States? On one hand,
our model indicates that post-secondary education and exposure to college science courses were
positively related to the acceptance of evolution throughout the last three decades, but the propor-
tion of American adults earning a baccalaureate and participating in college science courses has
increased markedly during this period. The continued growth of educational attainment among
American adults in the twenty-first century suggests that we might expect a moderate rate of
growth in the public acceptance of evolution in the United States in the decades ahead. On the other
hand, changes in the religious profile of Americans have been taking place; both the number of
atheists and the number of Americans who do not identify with organized religion (the “nones”)
have increased in the last decade. Some analyses have found increased political and social liberal-
ism (e.g. increased acceptance of homosexuality) in recent surveys of religious Americans (Pew
Research Center, 2015). If this is correct, a reduction in the influence of fundamentalism along
with growing civic scientific literacy is likely to continue these trends in increasing acceptance of
evolution.
Funding
The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publica-
tion of this article: The data reported in this article was collected with the support of the U.S. National Science
Foundation (awards: SRS85-17581, SRS88-07409, SRS90-02467, SRS92-17876, SRS99-06416); the
Foundation BBVA (Spain); the U.S. National Aeronautics and Space Administration (coopoerative agree-
ment: NNX16AC66A). We are grateful for this support, but all of the findings and conclusions reported in this
work are the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of any funding agency.
ORCID iDs
Jon D. Miller https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8714-0126
Eugenie C. Scott https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3795-8207
Belén Laspra https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4553-4885
Glenn Branch https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4931-3935
Miller et al. 13
Carmelo Polino https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1789-8024
Jordan S. Huffaker https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2876-9842
Supplemental material
Supplemental material for this article is available online.
Notes
1. We use “developed nations” to denote countries that are wealthier and industrialized. The member coun-
tries of the Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) reflect our use of this
term (see oecd.org).
2. Using different wording, a 20-country survey from the Pew Research Center (2020) estimated the rate
of acceptance of evolution in the United States to be 64%, ranking the United States 15th, ahead of only
Poland, Singapore, India, Brazil, and Malaysia.
3. We have examined the mode, vendors, and procedures used during the 1985 through 2007 period and we
can find no methodological explanation of the higher rate of public acceptance in 1999. The magnitude
of this increase barely exceeded the levels required for statistical significance at the .05 level, but this
pattern was not repeated in subsequent years during the first decade of the twenty-first century.
4. Gamma is a proportional reduction of error (PRE) measure and should be interpreted in the same way as
an R2 in standard regression. Costner (1965) has provided the classic definitional discussion of gamma
and related PRE statistics.
5. The five items used are the following: (1) agreement that “There is a personal God that hears the prayers
of individuals,” (2) agreement that “The Bible is the actual word of God and is to be taken literally,” (3)
the self-reported number of times that each respondent attends a religious service in a typical week, (4)
the self-reported number of times that each respondent prays during a typical week, and (5) agreement
that “We depend too much on science and not enough on faith.”
6. A discussion of the measurement of civic scientific literacy (CSL) is provided in the Supplemental
Material.
References
Abramowitz A (2010) The Disappearing Center: Engaged Citizens, Polarization, and American Democracy.
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Abramowitz A (2018) The Great Alignment: Race, Party Transformation, and the Rise of Donald Trump.
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Axelrod R (1973) Schema theory: An information processing model of perception and cognition. American
Political Science Review 67: 1248–66.
Austin A (2018) Vision and Change in Undergraduate Biology Education: Unpacking a movement and shar-
ing lessons learned. Meeting Report. American Association for the Advancement of Science. Available
at: https://live-visionandchange.pantheonsite.io/wp-content/uploads/2018/09/VandC-2018-finrr.pdf
Baker JO (2013) Acceptance of evolution and support for teaching creationism in public schools: The con-
ditional impact of educational attainment. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 52(1): 216–228.
Bengtson VL, Biblarz TJ and Roberts REL (2002) How Families Still Matter: A Longitudinal Study of Youth
in Two Generations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Berkman M and Plutzer E (2010) Evolution, Creationism, and the Battle to Control America’s Classrooms.
New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Blancke S, Hjermitslev HH and Kjaergaard PC (eds) (2014) Creationism in Europe. Baltimore, MD: Johns
Hopkins University Press.
Branch G (2009) Creationism as a global phenomenon. In: Robbins RH and Cohen MN (eds) Darwin and the
Bible: The Cultural Confrontation. Boston, MA: Pearson Education, pp. 137–151.
Branch G, Scott EC and Rosenau J (2010) Dispatches from the evolution wars: Shifting tactics and expanding
battlefields. Annual Review of Genomics and Human Genetics 11: 317–338.
14 Public Understanding of Science 00(0)
Brenan M (2019) 40% Of Americans Believe in Creationism. Available at: https://news.gallup.com/
poll/261680/americans-believecreatioism.aspx
Case DO and Given LM (2016) Looking for Information: A Survey of Research on Information Seeking,
Needs, and Behavior. Bingley: Emerald.
Chaves M (2017) American Religion: Contemporary Trends. 2nd ed. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University
Press.
Costner HL (1965) Criteria for measures of association. American Sociological Review 30(3): 341–353.
Coyne J (2012) Science, religion, and society: The problem of evolution in America. Evolution 66(8): 2654–
2663.
Dolan E, Borrero M, Callis-Duehl K, Musgrove MMC, de Lima J, Ero-Tolliver, et al. (2020) Undergraduate
biology education research Gordon research conference: A meeting report. CBE – Life Sciences
Education 19(20): mr1
Emerson MO and Hartman D (2006) The rise of fundamentalism. Annual Review of Sociology 32: 127–144.
Hayduk LA (1987) Structural Equation Modeling with LISREL. Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins
University Press.
Hill JP (2014) Rejecting evolution: The role of religion, education, and social networks. Journal for the
Scientific Study of Religion 53(3): 575–594 (Reprinted in Mirola WA, Emerson MO and Monahan SC
(eds) Sociology of Religion: A reader. New York, NY: Routledge. 2019. pp. 361–75).
Hyman HH, Wright CR and Reed JS (1975) The Enduring Effects of Education. Chicago, IL: University of
Chicago Press.
Ipsos (2011) Supreme Being, the Afterlife, and Evolution. Available at: https://www.ipsos.com.
Jöreskog K and Sörbom D (1993) LISREL 8. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International.
Kampourakis K (2020) Understanding Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Levendusky M (2009) The Partisan Sort: How Liberals Became Democrats and Conservatives Became
Republicans. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Marsden G (1991) Understanding fundamentalism and evangelicalism. Grand Rapids, MI: Eerdmans
Publishing.
Martin JW (2010) Compatibility of major U.S. Christian denominations with evolution. Evolution: Education
and Outreach 3(3): 420–431.
Miller JD (1983) Scientific literacy: A conceptual and empirical review. Daedalus 112(2): 29–48.
Miller JD (1987) Scientific literacy in the United States. In: Evered D and O’Connor M (eds) Communicating
Science to the Public. London: Wiley, pp. 19–40.
Miller JD (1998) The measurement of civic scientific literacy. Public Understanding of Science 7: 1–21.
Miller JD (2000) The development of civic scientific literacy in the United States. In: Kumar DD and Chubin
D (eds) Science, Technology, and Society: A Sourcebook on Research and Practice. New York, NY:
Plenum Press, pp. 21–47.
Miller JD (2010a) The conceptualization and measurement of civic scientific literacy for the 21st century. In:
Hildebrand JG and Meinwald J (eds) Science and the Educated American: A Core Component of Liberal
Education. Cambridge, MA: American Academy of Arts and Sciences, pp. 241–255.
Miller JD (2010b) Civic scientific literacy: The role of the media in the electronic era. In: Kennedy D and
Overholser G (eds) Science, Technology, and the Media. Cambridge, MA: American Academy of Arts
and Sciences, pp. 44–63.
Miller JD (2010c) Adult science learning in the Internet era. Curator 53(2): 191–208.
Miller JD (2012) The sources and impact of civic scientific literacy. In: Bauer MW, Shukla R and Allum
N (eds) The Culture of Science: How the Public Relates to Science across the Globe. New York, NY:
Routledge, pp. 217–240.
Miller JD, Ackerman MA, Laspra B and Huffaker J (2021) The acquisition of health and science information
in the 21st century. The Information Society 37(2): 82–98.
Miller JD, Pardo R and Niwa F (1997) Public Perceptions of Science and Technology: A Comparative Study
of the European Union, the United States, Japan, and Canada. Madrid: BBV Foundation Press.
Miller JD, Scott EC and Okamoto S (2006) Public acceptance of evolution. Science 313: 765–766.
Miller et al. 15
Muller RA (2010) Physics for Future Presidents. In: Hildebrand JG and Meinwald J (eds) Science and the
Educated American: A Core Component of Liberal Education. Cambridge, MA: American Academy of
Arts and Sciences, pp. 112–129.
Pew Research Center (2013) Public’s Views on Human Evolution. Available at: https://www.pewforum.
org/2013/12/30/publics-views-on-human-evolution/
Pew Research Center (2015) America’s Changing Religious Landscape. Available at: https://www.pewfo-
rum.org/2015/05/12/americas-changing-religious-landscape/
Pew Research Center (2019a) U.S. Public Views on Climate and Energy. Available at: https://www.pewre-
search.org/science/2019/11/25/u-s-public-views-on-climate-and-energy/
Pew Research Center (2019b) The Evolution of Pew Research Center’s Survey Questions about the Origins
and Development of Life on Earth. Available at: https://www.pewforum.org/2019/02/06/the-evolution-
of-pew-research-centers-survey-questions-about-the-origins-and-development-of-life-on-earth/
Pew Research Center (2020) Biotechnology Research Viewed with Caution Globally, but Most Support Gene
Editing for Babies to Treat Disease. Available at: https://www.pewrearch.org/2020/12/10/biotechnol-
ogy-research-viewed-with-caution-globally-but-most-support-gene-editing-for-babies-to-treat-disease/
Plutzer E and Berkman M (2008) Evolution, creationism, and the teaching of human origins in schools. Public
Opinion Quarterly 72(3): 540–553.
Plutzer E, Branch G and Reid A (2020) Teaching evolution in U.S. public schools: a continuing challenge.
Evolution: Education and Outreach 13(4).
Putnam RD and Campbell DE (2010) American Grace: How Religion Divides and Unites Us. New York,
NY: Simon and Schuster.
Schank RC and RP Abelson (1977) Scripts, Plans, Goals, and Understanding: An Inquiry in Human
Knowledge Structures. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Scott EC (2009) Evolution vs. Creationism: An Introduction. 2nd ed. Berkeley, CA: University of California
Press.
Shen BSJ (1975) Scientific literacy and the public understanding of science. In: Day S (ed.) Communication
of Scientific Information. Basel: Karger, pp. 44–52.
Shumow L and Miller JD (2001) Parents’ at-home and at-school academic involvement with adolescents.
Journal of Early Adolescence 21(1): 68–91.
Society for Integrative Comparative Biology (n.d.). Available at: https://sicb.org/rer/saawok.php3
Sternberg RJ and Ben-Zeev T (2001) Complex Cognition: The Psychology of Human Thought. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Swift A (2017) In U.S. belief in creationist view of humans at new low. Available at: https://news.gallup.com/
poll/210956/belief-creationist-view-humans-new-low.aspx
United States Census Bureau (2019) CPS historical time series. Table A-2. Available at: https://www.census.
gov/data/tables/time-series/demo/educational-attainment/cps-historical-time-series.html
van Dijk J (2020) The Network Society. 2nd ed. New York, NY: SAGE.
Wilcox C and Larson C (2006) Onward Christian Soldiers? The Religious Right in American Politics.
Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Williams DK (2012) God’s Own Party: The Making of the Christian Right. New York, NY: Oxford University
Press.
Author biographies
Jon D. Miller is Director of the International Center for the Advancement of Scientific Literacy in the Institute
for Social Research at the University of Michigan, Ann Arbor. He has studied the public understanding of
science and technology and public engagement in the formulation of science and technology policy for the
last 50 years.
Eugenie C. Scott is the founding executive director of the National Center for Science Education, which pro-
motes and defends the teaching of evolution and climate change.
16 Public Understanding of Science 00(0)
Mark S. Ackerman is the George Herbert Mead Collegiate Professor of Human-Computer Interaction and a
Professor in the School of Information and in the Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science
at the University of Michigan, Ann Arbor. His major research area is social computing (also called CSCW).
Belén Laspra is an Assistant Professor at the Department of Philosophy of the University of Oviedo (Spain).
She is a member of the Social Studies of Science Research Team. Her current research interests are in the
meaning and measures of scientific culture.
Glenn Branch is deputy director of the National Center for Science Education. He received the Evolution
Education Award for 2020 from the National Association of Biology Teachers.
Carmelo Polino is an Assistant Professor in the Department of Philosophy at the University of Oviedo (Spain)
and a researcher at Centro Redes (Buenos Aires, Argentina). His research interests are public understanding
of science and sociology of science communication.
Jordan Huffaker is a PhD student in Computer Science and Engineering at the University of Michigan, Ann
Arbor. His area of research is social computing.
... While some evidence suggests that acceptance in the United States has increased recently (Miller et al. 2021;Swift May 22, 2017) and that biology teachers are now teaching more evolution to their students (Plutzer, Branch, and Reid 2020), rejection of evolutionary science Evolution is one of the most robust and widely accepted principles of modern science. It is the foundation for research in a wide array of scientific fields and, accordingly, a core element in science education. ...
... In addition to the Scopes trial, other major court cases have originated in southern states, two of which originated in Arkansas (Matsumura and Mead 2016). Despite encouraging reports that public acceptance of evolution in the US has increased (Miller et al. 2021;Swift May 22, 2017), and teachers are teaching more evolution than they used to (Plutzer, Branch, and Reid 2020), anti-evolution sentiments remain firmly in place in the Mid-South. Arkansas is not alone in this regard, however, as anti-science legislation aimed at evolution has been introduced in other states in recent years (Catalyzing Action n.d.). ...
... It is important for the students to not only understand evolution but to also accept it as the best scientific explanation for the unity and diversity of life on earth (Misheva et al., 2023). A large body of research has examined knowledge and acceptance of the evolution of university and high school students (Barnes et al., 2022;Gefaell et al., 2020;Kuschmierz et al., 2021;Nadelson & Hardy, 2015;Oliviera et al., 2022), among teachers (Beniermann et al., 2023;Tekkaya et al., 2012;Venetis & Mavrikaki, 2017), the general public (Kuschmierz et al., 2020;Miller et al., 2022;Zhu et al., 2020), and recently in popular media and online spaces (Ferguson et al., 2022;Park & Seo, 2023). ...
... (3) political orientation and having a role model discuss the compatibility between religion and evolution (Ferguson & Jensen, 2021;Miller et al., 2022), and (4) scientific literacy (Dunk et al., 2017;Gefaell et al., 2020;Weisberg et al., 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Evolutionary theory provides the foundation for modern biology with evolution as a core concept and unifying principle. This research aimed to examine evolutionary knowledge and acceptance among first-year university students. The participants considered for analysis consisted of 87 students enrolled in the first-year bachelor's in biology and biotechnology at the University of Tirana, Albania. The respondents' average age was 18.95 ± 0.78 years, with a pronounced dominance of females (90.2%). The study used the standardized research instrument "evolution education questionnaire on acceptance and knowledge" to measure knowledge and acceptance about evolution in an international context. We employed descriptive statistics to analyze the collected data. The findings of research revealed that over half of the respondents, 61%, fall into the 'very low knowledge' category on evolution and none fall in the 'high knowledge' category. Only 60.9% accept evolution. The need for a revision of the evolutionary concepts and topics in the curricula, from primary school, is recommended coupled with the incorporation of philosophical dialogue into teaching practice.
... Nevertheless, the American public views trail scientific ones when it comes to support for these concepts. Regarding evolution, public views are strongly related to religion, with the more religious being the least likely to say that life evolved over time from natural processes (Barnes et al. 2019;Dunk et al. 2017;Miller et al. 2021;Tom 2018). On the other hand, American's views on climate change are strongly associated with political party or ideology, with Republicans and those who lean Republican being less likely to attribute global warming to human activity (Ballew et al. 2019;Leiserowitz et al. 2019;Pew Research Center 2019;Tom 2018). ...
... On the other hand, American's views on climate change are strongly associated with political party or ideology, with Republicans and those who lean Republican being less likely to attribute global warming to human activity (Ballew et al. 2019;Leiserowitz et al. 2019;Pew Research Center 2019;Tom 2018). Moreover, there is a strong generational difference for both as well, with older Americans being less likely to say that humans evolved from natural processes (Miller et al. 2021) and to agree the Earth is warming due to human activity (Ballew et al. 2019;Pew Research Center 2020). These anti-science views, or views that dismiss scientific information and evidence, inevitably make it into science classrooms. ...
Article
Full-text available
Both evolution and climate change have broad scientific consensus, and yet they are the most contested scientific concepts in the US K‐12 education system. This study aimed to explore trends in proposed US state legislation employed from 2003 to 2023 by anti‐evolution and anti‐climate change education movements to constrain the teaching of these sciences. Using a historical qualitative research design, document analysis was used to evaluate state legislation and reports from the National Center for Science Education (NCSE). Two hundred and seventy‐three climate and evolution‐related House and Senate bills, concurrent resolutions, and joint resolutions were identified, coded, and analyzed. Eleven anti‐science education legislative tactics were employed from 2003 to 2023. Five were first identified in the literature review: academic freedom (42.1%), rebranding (12.1%), balanced treatment (12.1%), censorship (2.6%), and disclaimers (2.6%). Six new tactics were revealed in the analysis: anti‐indoctrination (16.8%), standards (12.1%), instructional materials (10.3%), religious liberty (8.8%), avoidance (4.4%), and religious instruction (4.0%). One‐quarter of bills and resolutions employed a combination of tactics. The most ubiquitous tactics were academic freedom bills, which urge science teachers to introduce ideas like intelligent design or climate change denial under the mantle of academic freedom, and anti‐indoctrination bills, which prevent teachers from advocating for controversial topics deemed political. Since 2017, anti‐indoctrination has become the preferred tactic. Southern, southeastern, and midwestern states were the most prolific in their contribution to anti‐science education legislation. Qualitative analysis revealed bill and resolution language was often recycled across years and states with slight changes to wording. From 2003 to 2023, the total number of anti‐science education state legislative efforts increased, as did the number of passed bills and resolutions. The implications of these tactics and trends are considered.
... This gap is potentially problematic because perception of consensus is strongly related to beliefs about evolution, even after accounting for religious beliefs and ideology (Pew Research Center, 2015). In the United States, public opinion has remained divided for decades, with recent acceptance rates slightly increasing but still below 65% (Miller et al., 2022). By contrast, acceptance levels in Europe have varied widely, from 27% in Turkey to approximately 70% in Germany, and nearly 90% in Scandinavian countries (Miller et al., 2006). ...
... Regarding the topic of evolution, our results revealed a pre-existing high level of acceptance. Future studies in evolutionary education could use a sample with lower pretest acceptance, possibly from the United States (Miller et al., 2022). Alternatively, using different measurement instruments or focusing on different aspects of evolution, such as human evolution, could be considered as this might be more effective in addressing strong personal scientific beliefs. ...
Article
Full-text available
Evolutionary theory is considered the unifying theory of modern biosciences, but the public continues to misperceive the prevailing scientific consensus. Science communication studies emphasise the influence of perceived scientific and social consensus on personal beliefs, which can be manipulated through communication strategies. This study investigates the impact of consensus messaging on (1) perceptions of scientific and social consensus, (2) students’ feelings of certainty about these perceptions, and (3) acceptance of evolutionary theory. Employing a 2 × 2 factorial design (Factor 1: scientific consensus messaging versus consensus uncertainty; Factor 2: social consensus messaging), we randomly assigned 218 German upper secondary school students to one of four text variants to complete a close-ended pre – post questionnaire. We found positive correlations of evolutionary theory acceptance with perceived scientific and social consensus, and with feeling of certainty regarding perceived scientific consensus. Scientific consensus messaging positively affected perceived scientific consensus and feeling of certainty. Assessed with all items of MATE 2.0, acceptance decreased in the consensus uncertainty condition. When measured with two items, acceptance increased with scientific consensus messaging. The role of consensus messaging in teaching evolution and its use as a component of teaching strategies in evolutionary education and other fields are discussed.
... The perceived conflict between religion and evolution impacts college student evolution acceptance Evolution is the foundation of biology [1][2][3], yet it has remained controversial in the public and in schools since Darwin first published The Origin of Species [4]. In the United States, about half of the public rejects human evolution [5,6] and this rejection also extends to American college biology students. Surveys indicate that in 2020, up to 35% of introductory college biology students in the United States did not think life shares common ancestry [7]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Evolution is an important part of biology education, but many college biology students do not accept important components of evolution, like the evolution of humans. Practices that reduce perceived conflict between religion and evolution have been proposed to increase student evolution acceptance. This study investigates college student experiences of conflict reducing practices in evolution education and how these experiences are related to their gains in acceptance of human evolution during evolution instruction. We measured the natural variation in student experiences of conflict reducing practices among 6,719 college biology students in 55 courses and 14 states including (1) their experiences of an instructor demonstrating religion-evolution compatibility by presenting examples of religious leaders and scientists who accept evolution and (2) their experiences of an instructor emphasizing students’ autonomy in their own decision to accept evolution or not. We also measured student acceptance of human evolution before and after instruction so that we could test whether any changes in evolution acceptance were associated with student experiences of the conflict reducing practices. Linear mixed models showed that highly religious Christian students accepted evolution more when they perceived more compatibility practices. Further, students from all religious and non-religious affiliations accepted human evolution more after instruction when they perceived more autonomy practices. These results indicate that integrating examples of religion compatibility in evolution education will positively impact Christian students’ views on evolution and that emphasizing students’ autonomy over their decision to accept evolution may be important for students more broadly. If instructors incorporate practices that emphasize compatibility and one’s personal choice to accept or not accept evolution, then these results suggest that students will leave their college biology classes accepting evolution more. Perhaps by using more conflict reducing practices, instructors can help increase evolution acceptance levels that have remained low in the United States for decades.
... Parallel to this pattern of educational decisions and postsecondary science and policy-related learning, some LSAL young adults adopt or retain a set of fundamentalist religious beliefs. Other work on student acceptance of biological evolution (Miller et al. 2022c) has found that parents with fundamentalist religious beliefs tend to influence their children through joint participation in religious services and personal discussion, but we do not have a direct measure of student religious beliefs during high school. Collecting data from students in public schools is the art of the possible, and the principals of middle and high schools tend not to approve questionnaires with questions about the Bible or other specific religious beliefs. ...
Article
Research on public attitudes toward science and technology policy has relied on surveys taken at single points in time. These surveys fail to indicate how these attitudes develop or change. In this study, we use data from the Longitudinal Study of American Life that has followed a national sample of Generation X for 33 years—from middle school to midlife. We demonstrate that the critical period for the formation of attitudes toward science and technology is the 15–18 years after high school—college, work, family, and career. The attitudes formed in this period remain stable for most individuals during midlife. This work provides an important perspective for scientists, engineers, and the leadership of the scientific community in their efforts to foster positive attitudes toward science and technology and to understand the roots of concerns and reservations about science.
Article
The teaching and learning of macroevolutionary processes have received limited attention in the evolution education literature despite their importance in evolution acceptance and evolution understanding. This necessitates the development of pedagogical content knowledge, including best practices in curriculum and instruction, on macroevolutionary processes that support student understanding and acceptance of macroevolution. One promising approach is to consider macroevolution at the microbiological level by teaching endosymbiotic theory while capitalizing on pre-existing pedagogical content knowledge of tree thinking and bioinformatics. Here, we present a computational laboratory activity that guides students through the construction of a phylogeny based on the universal small subunit ribosomal RNA gene. The resulting phylogenetic tree demonstrates that the photosynthetic organelles of the protist Paulinella chromatophora evolved independently of the chloroplasts of plants and algae. This not only addresses the need for pedagogical content knowledge in macroevolution in an interdisciplinary and integrative fashion, but also serves as a foundation for future research into the teaching of endosymbiosis. This activity is designed for a 15–20 student introductory/intermediate biology laboratory.
Article
Trust in science is crucial to resolving societal problems. Americans across political ideologies have high levels of trust in science—a stable pattern observed over the past 50 years. Yet, trust in science varies by individual and group characteristics and faces several threats, from political actors, increased political polarization, or global crises. We revisit historical trends of trust in science among Americans by political orientation. We find steadily diverging trends by political views since the 1990s, and a drastically and rapidly opening gap since 2018. Recent unprecedented changes are driven not only by decreases in trust among conservatives but also by increases among liberals. Existing theoretical accounts do not fully explain these patterns. Diverging attitudes toward the institution of science can diminish capacity for collective problem-solving, eroding the shared foundation for decision-making and political discourse.
Chapter
The ‘deficit model’ of science and society holds that the more you know about science the more you will love it, so rejection of science is based on ignorance. The conflict narrative presents the domains of science and religion in a necessary and enduring conflict due to their essential and epistemic differences. Both the deficit model and the conflict narrative are simplistic stories that simultaneously purport to explain yet actively obscure understanding of various publics’ orientations towards science. Both these discourses intersect in the topic of attitudes towards evolution, creation, and human origins. In this chapter, I argue that critiques of the deficit model and conflict narrative provide their most valuable contribution in opening space to reimagine and more accurately describe public orientations to science. Focusing on literature at the intersection of the public understanding of science (PUS) and what I term science and religion studies (SRS), and using public attitudes towards evolution and creation as a case study, I delineate four avenues that have opened once the dominant narratives of deficit and conflict have been surpassed: beyond acceptance, complexity mapping, moral conflicts, and second-order perspectives.
Article
Full-text available
Background Over a decade ago, the first nationally representative probability survey concerning the teaching of evolution revealed disquieting facts about evolution education in the United States. This 2007 survey found that only about one in three public high school biology teachers presented evolution consistently with the recommendations of the nation’s leading scientific authorities. And about 13% of the teachers emphasized to their students that creationism was a valid scientific alternative to modern evolutionary biology. In this paper, we investigate how the quality of evolution teaching, as measured by teachers’ reports of their teaching practices with regard to evolution and creationism, has changed in the intervening 12 years. Results We find substantial reductions in overtly creationist instruction and in the number of teachers who send mixed messages that legitimate creationism as a valid scientific alternative to evolutionary biology. We also report a substantial increase in the time that high school teachers devote to human evolution and general evolutionary processes. We show that these changes reflect both generational replacement—from teachers who are new to the profession—and changes in teaching practices among those who were teaching in the pre-Kitzmiller era. Conclusion Adoption of the Next Generation Science Standards, along with improvements in pre-service teacher education and in-service teacher professional development, appears to have contributed to a large reduction in both creationist instruction and mixed messages that could lead students to think that creationism is a scientific perspective. Combined with teachers devoting more hours to evolution—including human evolution—instruction at the high school level has improved by these measures since the last national survey in 2007.
Article
Full-text available
The 2019 Undergraduate Biology Education Research Gordon Research Conference (UBER GRC), titled "Achieving Widespread Improvement in Undergraduate Education," brought together a diverse group of researchers and practitioners working to identify, promote, and understand widespread adoption of evidence-based teaching, learning, and success strategies in undergraduate biology. Graduate students and postdocs had the additional opportunity to present and discuss research during a Gordon Research Seminar (GRS) that preceded the GRC. This report provides a broad overview of the UBER GRC and GRS and highlights major themes that cut across invited talks, poster presentations, and informal discussions. Such themes include the importance of working in teams at multiple levels to achieve instructional improvement, the potential to use big data and analytics to inform instructional change, the need to customize change initiatives, and the importance of psychosocial supports in improving undergraduate student well-being and academic success. The report also discusses the future of the UBER GRC as an established meeting and describes aspects of the conference that make it unique, both in terms of facilitating dissemination of research and providing a welcoming environment for conferees.
Article
The ways that humans acquire information is undergoing a fundamental change comparable to the introduction of Gutenberg’s printing press and broadcast systems. Using the literature and a growing body of empirical evidence, we describe a model of information acquisition that specifies the roles of education, salience, subject-matter literacy, and navigation skills in the decision to seek information. We refer to this model as a just-in-time information acquisition system. We focus on the acquisition of health information and science information separately and discuss the similarities and differences. We utilize a national U.S. survey to describe adult information acquisition behaviors empirically.
Book
Alan I. Abramowitz has emerged as a leading spokesman for the view that our current political divide is not confined to a small group of elites and activists but a key feature of the American social and cultural landscape. The polarization of the political and media elites, he argues, arose and persists because it accurately reflects the state of American society. Here, he goes further: the polarization is unique in modern U.S. history. Today’s party divide reflects an unprecedented alignment of many different divides: racial and ethnic, religious, ideological, and geographic. Abramowitz shows how the partisan alignment arose out of the breakup of the old New Deal coalition; introduces the most important difference between our current era and past eras, the rise of “negative partisanship”; explains how this phenomenon paved the way for the Trump presidency; and examines why our polarization could even grow deeper. This statistically based analysis shows that racial anxiety is by far a better predictor of support for Donald Trump than any other factor, including economic discontent.
Article
Most Americans say they believe in God, and more than a third say they attend religious services every week. Yet studies show that people do not really go to church as often as they claim, and it is not always clear what they mean when they tell pollsters they believe in God or pray. American Religion presents the best and most up-to-date information about religious trends in the United States, in a succinct and accessible manner. This sourcebook provides essential information about key developments in American religion since 1972, and is the first major resource of its kind to appear in more than two decades. Mark Chaves looks at trends in diversity, belief, involvement, congregational life, leadership, liberal Protestant decline, and polarization. He draws on two important surveys: the General Social Survey, an ongoing survey of Americans' changing attitudes and behaviors, begun in 1972; and the National Congregations Study, a survey of American religious congregations across the religious spectrum. Chaves finds that American religious life has seen much continuity in recent decades, but also much change. He challenges the popular notion that religion is witnessing a resurgence in the United States--in fact, traditional belief and practice is either stable or declining. Chaves examines why the decline in liberal Protestant denominations has been accompanied by the spread of liberal Protestant attitudes about religious and social tolerance, how confidence in religious institutions has declined more than confidence in secular institutions, and a host of other crucial trends.
Article
This book traces the history of the Christian Right from its early twentieth-century origins to its involvement in the presidential election of 2008. The book argues that evangelicals' success in transforming American politics was primarily a result of their ability to link their political agenda to the Republican Party. Evangelicals who believed in reclaiming America as a Christian nation began developing an alliance with the Republican Party during the early years of the Cold War, when Billy Graham cultivated a relationship with the Eisenhower administration. Evangelicals strengthened that tie during the culture wars of the late 1960s and 1970s, when grassroots activists forged alliances with the national conservative movement during their campaigns against sex education, the Equal Rights Amendment, abortion, and gay rights. The development of an organized Religious Right in the late 1970s, which resulted partly from evangelicals' rising socioeconomic status and the growth of the Sunbelt, gave evangelicals such as Jerry Falwell and Pat Robertson national political influence, but their frustration with their lack of legislative success prompted them to make increased demands of the GOP. As conservative evangelicals gained control of the Republican Party, they pushed the party further to the right. By the early twenty-first century, the Christian Right was the most powerful interest group in the Republican Party, a position that conservative evangelicals used to reshape the nation's political agenda.