ArticlePDF Available

Do Odontomachus brunneus nestmates request for help and are taken care of when caught?

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

In social insects, situations can arise that threaten an individual or an entire colony. When the call for help goes out, different behavioral responses are elicited by signals emitted from nestmates. In ants, the response can be one of redemptive behavior by the worker receiving it. However, little is known about the evolution of this behavior and in which group of ants it manifests. Therefore, this study investigates whether workers of Odontomachus brunneus Patton can act as rescuers, able to detect and respond to calls for help from nestmates. Laboratory experiments were carried out in which the legs of ants were trapped by tape, simulating capture by a predator. Nearby were nestmates able to receive and respond to a request for help. Two experiments were performed: 1. Calls for help were made at different distances, in order to test the response latency. 2. Evaluation of whether rescuers would respond differently to calls for help from nestmates, non-nestmates of the same species, and ants of another species. Finally, evaluation was made of the behaviors of the rescuers when they responded to requests for help from nestmates and ants of another species. It could be concluded from the results that O. brunneus workers respond to signals emitted by workers who may have been captured by a potential predator, prompting the performance of behaviors related to rescue attempts. The signals involved appear to have an optimal range and are species-specific. When exposed to a capture situation, this species transmits audible signals by stridulation, so it is possible that this type of signal may be involved, in addition to chemical signaling.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Open access journal: http://periodicos.uefs.br/ojs/index.php/sociobiology
ISSN: 0361-6525
DOI: 10.13102/sociobiology.v68i3.6022
Sociobiology 68(3): e6022 (September, 2021)
Introduction
One of the benets of living in social groups is the
availability of defense, and one of the most effective ways of
group defense is attacking the presumed predator (Siebenaler
& Caldwell, 1956; Vogel & Fuentes-Jiménez, 2006). For example,
wasp and bee colonies can group attack a potential predator,
usually mobilized by volatile chemicals, such as the alarm
pheromone (Morse & Laigo, 1969; Breed et al., 1990, 2004).
Abstract
            
           
  
   

Odontomachus brunneus 
             

           
           
   
 
     

    
O. brunneus
           




Sociobiology
An international journal on social insects
1,232
Arcle History
Edited by





Keywords


Corresponding author









In some groups of ants, the morphologically modied
caste of larger workers has, among other functions, the role
of group defense when the colony is put under possible threat
from an invader (Wilson, 1976; Hölldobler & Wilson, 1990;
Powell & Clark, 2004; Pepper & Herron, 2008; Powell, 2008;
Hou et al., 2010; Strassmann & Queller, 2010). Ants defend
their colonies using various structures, when threatened
by an individual from outside of the colony. In this way,
bites and stings are commonly used in coordinated attacks




Do Odontomachus brunneus
Luiz C Santos-Junior, Emerson P Silva, William F Antonialli-Junior – Odontomachus brunneus Nestmates Request Help
2
(Hölldobler & Wilson, 1990), as a strategy for group defense
of the colony.
Some ant species have developed a unique means
of defending themselves (Beponis et al., 2014). If an ant
is caught, nestmates may exhibit rescue behavior to save it
(Czechowski et al., 2002; Nowbahari et al., 2009; Nowbahari
& Hollis, 2010; Miler, 2016). This behavior can be exhibited
by one or more workers, known as rst responders, and is
directed towards another individual (the victim), in order to
rescue it from a predator situation (Nowbahari & Hollis, 2010;
Miler, 2016). Such behavior can involve relatively simple
digging around the victim, with pulling of its limbs, to more
precise behaviors such as directly attacking and stinging the
trapping animal or object (Taylor & Visvader, 2013; Miler, 2016).
Independent of the species that can perform this type
of behavior, there must be some exchange of signals between
the ant that is requesting help and the rescuers. Ants and
other social hymenopterans can produce and release volatile
substances in the form of pheromones (Wheeler & Blum,
1973; Jafé & Marcuse, 1983; Lahav et al., 1999; Morgan et
al., 1999; Lenoir et al., 2001; Hernández et al., 2002; Howard
& Blomquist, 2005; Blomquist & Bagnères, 2010; Sainz-
Borgo et al., 2011). They can also emit acoustic signals
(Markl, 1973; Golden & Hill, 2016), such as those produced
by the stridulatory organ, located between the petiole and the
gaster in ants (Markl, 1973; Taylor, 1978; Stuart & Bell, 1980;
Hölldobler & Wilson, 1990; Golden & Hill, 2016). This organ
emits a “beep” that has different roles in the functioning of the
colony (Markl & Hölldobler, 1978; Chiu et al., 2011).
In the Odontomachus genus, workers, in particular,
produce sounds in the form of stridulation, when they feel
threatened (Markl, 1973; Golden & Hill, 2016). Therefore,
it is likely that ants of the species Odontomachus brunneus
Patton, 1894 may emit this type of signaling as a way of
recruiting their nestmates, when they are exposed to a
dangerous situation. The signaling may involve chemicals,
sound signals, or a combination of both methods.
Most of the studies on this topic have been performed
for ants inhabiting sandy areas, with exposure to lion ant
larvae (Neuroptera: Myrmeleontidae). This relationship seems
to have contributed to the evolution of rescue behavioral
patterns that prevail in ants, especially in species of the
genera Cataglyphis, Formica, and Lasius, all belonging to
the subfamily Formicinae (Gotelli, 1996; Czechowski et al.,
2002; Hollis & Nowbahari, 2013a; Miler, 2016; Hollis, 2017).
However, studies show that ants from other subfamilies, such
as Myrmicinae and Ponerinae, are also capable of exhibiting
some kind of rescue behavior (Hollis & Nowbahari, 2013a;
Frank et al., 2017, 2018). Hence, the occurrence of rescue
behavior in relatively unrelated ant species suggests that this
behavior is not phylogenetically restricted and that many
factors may contribute to its occurrence.
Unfortunately, little is known about rescue behavior in
poneromorphic ants. Therefore, the aim of this study was to
evaluate whether workers of O. brunneus, when exposed to a
capture situation, emit some kind of signal that provokes help
in the form of attempted rescue by nestmates.
Materials and Methods
Six colonies of O. brunneus were collected in the
urban area of Dourados, in Mato Grosso do Sul State, Brazil
(22º13’16’’ S; 54º48’20’’ W), between September 2016 and
September 2017.
All the ants were collected from hollow trunks of
Caesalpinia pluviosa (Fabaceae), using tweezers, and were
placed in plastic pots. In the laboratory, the ants were housed
in articial nests constructed using plastic trays (10 x 20 cm).
Inside there were plaster molds that simulated the nest
chambers, which were connected to a foraging arena, where
food was offered to the colonies.
The colonies were kept at a controlled temperature of
around 25.0 ± 1 ◦C, relative humidity of 65 ± 5%, and 12-hour
photoperiod, for a seven-day habituation period, and were fed
ad libitum with water and honey on moistened cotton inside
an Eppendorf tube. As a protein source, last instar Tenebrio
molitor Linnaeus, 1758 larvae were offered every ve days.
The behavioral tests were performed after the habituation period.
Latency time and call for help to nestmate rescuer ants
Tests were conducted in order to understand if O.
brunneus rst responders could respond to their nestmates
call for help, by simulating capture at different distances from
the rst responders.
A system of plastic chambers and connectors (15 x 10
x 8 cm) was constructed, allowing for the insertion of rescuers
and a trapping chamber (10 x 8 cm) where an ant was attached
to simulate its capture. These two sites were connected by a
tube 2 cm in diameter and with different lengths (30, 60, and
90 cm), in order to assess whether the call for assistance might
vary as a function of distance (Fig 1).
Ten foraging workers were inserted in the arena as
potential rescuers. In the trapping chamber, a forager from
the same colony was secured by tape, following the modied
methodology used in rescue behavior studies by articial
imprisonment in ants (Nowbahari et al., 2009, 2012, 2016;
Hollis & Nowbahari, 2013a; Duhoo et al., 2017). Prior to each
test, the arena, the trapping chamber, and the connector tube
were sterilized with an alcohol-soaked lter paper. The tests
were conducted under laboratory conditions, at a constant
temperature of 25.0 ± 1 ◦C and humidity of 65 ± 5%.
For each of the three different connector distances, 30
different ant groups were tested, with each group including 10
rescuers and 1 trapped ant. In each test, the observation time
was 15 min, from the moment the ant was immobilized in the
trapping chamber and all the rescuers were released into the
arena. At the end of each test, the number of rescuers entering
the trapping chamber was counted, so it was possible to assess
Sociobiology 68(3): e6022 (September, 2021) 3
whether distance was a factor affecting the number of ants
able to respond to the call for help.
Rescuer latency in responding to the call for aid was
calculated as described by Nowbahari et al. (2016). This
was dened as the period between the time of entry of the
rst rescuer into the connector tube and the time of the rst
attempt at rescuing the trapped ant performed by that rescuer.
As a control, the same parameters were measured
under the same conditions and with the same group of ants
used in each test, but without the ant in the trapping chamber.
Fig 1. Scheme used for the latency tests of rescue behavior between O. brunneus nestmates. A: Arena connected to the trapping
chamber through a 30 cm connector tube; B: Arena connected to the trapping chamber through a 60 cm connector tube; C: Arena
connected to the trapping chamber through a 90 cm connector tube.
Test to evaluate the specicity of the response to help requests
Three different help request situations were simulated,
in order to evaluate whether workers from a particular colony
might respond to a request for help from non-nestmate ants
(Nowbahari & Hollis, 2010; Miler, 2016; Uy et al., 2018).
The chamber system and connectors used in this test
were adapted from Yusuf et al. (2014). The system included
an arena where 10 rescuers were inserted, connected to a tube
25 cm long and 2 cm in diameter, the end of which had a
connector allowing bifurcation into two other tubes, each 5
cm long and with the same diameter. Each led to a plastic
trapping chamber (10 x 8 cm), where at least one of them had
a nestmate attached by tape. Then, either a rescue ant from the
same colony was inserted, or an ant from another colony and/
or species was inserted (Fig 2).
In each test, the observation time was 15 min, from the
moment the ants were trapped in the imprisonment chambers
and all the rescuers were released into the arena.
Three types of experiments were conducted. In the
rst, rescuers were tested for their ability to receive the call
for help and respond to it, using a Y-maze system. For this,
an O. brunneus nestmate worker was xed in only one of
the entrapment chambers (Fig 2-A), while the other chamber
contained a loose nestmate. In the second experiment,
rescuers were tested for their ability to distinguish between
the requests for help made by a nestmate and a non-nestmate.
For this test, a nestmate was trapped in one imprisonment
chamber, while the other chamber contained a trapped non-
nestmate (Fig 2-B). In the third experiment, a nestmate
was immobilized in one entrapment chamber, while the
other contained a worker of another species, in this case
Odontomachus chelifer, Latreille, 1802 (Fig 2-C). Each of the
three experimental designs was performed using thirty tests
with different groups of rescuers and trapped workers.
All the behaviors exhibited by the rescuers upon
entering the trapping chamber were observed and described
according to the methodology of Nowbahari et al. (2016).
To obtain the average frequency of each behavioral act
performed by the rescuers, the behaviors at the end of all the
tests were summed and divided by the sum of execution of all
the behavioral acts.
Luiz C Santos-Junior, Emerson P Silva, William F Antonialli-Junior – Odontomachus brunneus Nestmates Request Help
Statistical analysis
Differences among the treatments and the controls
were evaluated using the Student’s t-test applied to the
average values for the number of rescuers remaining in the
trapping chamber in the tests performed with the connectors
of three different lengths (distances).
The same test was used to evaluate differences between
the average latency times of the rescuers in answering the
requests for help by the trapped workers in the tests performed
with the different connectors, as well as when there was no
trapped worker (controls).
The Kruskal-Wallis test was applied to determine any
signicant differences among the average latency times in
responding to the request for help from a trapped worker, for
the three different distances.
A t-test was applied to evaluate any signicant
differences among the mean values of the number of rst
responders that remained in the trapping chamber during the
Y-maze decision tests.
Results
In all the tests, independent of distance, rescuers went
to their nestmate and performed rescue behaviors, using bites
and stings against the tape that held it. However, the number
of rescuers reaching the trapping chamber was signicantly
higher when the distance was 30 cm. The average numbers of
ants in the trapping chamber with the trapped ant and in the
control situation are shown in Table 1.
The latency times showed that the rescuers took less
time to respond to the call for help within 30 cm. At this
distance, there was a signicant difference between the
times recorded with an ant trapped in the chamber and with
no ant (Table 2).
In the tests to assess specicity in responding to requests
for help, the rescue workers opted for a trapping chamber
containing a trapped worker from the same colony, as opposed
to a nestmate loose in a chamber and/or an ant of another
species in the chamber. No signicant differences were found
between the numbers of times rescuers opted for a chamber
Fig 2. Scheme used for the Y-maze decision tube tests with the rescue workers in the arena and the workers
in the trapping chambers. A: Test with nestmate workers loose and trapped in the trapping chambers; B: Test
with a trapped nestmate worker in one chamber and a trapped non-nestmate worker in the other chamber; C:
Test with a trapped nestmate in one of the chambers and a trapped O. chelifer worker in the other chamber.
Sociobiology 68(3): e6022 (September, 2021) 5
DISTANCE
30 cm 60 cm 90 cm
With ant Without ant With ant Without ant With ant Without ant
Latency (minutes) 0:29 ± 0.01 2:43 ± 0.01 3:00 ± 0.02 3:03 ± 0.02 9:27 ± 0.21 8:52 ± 0.20
p-value <0.0001 0.732 0.955
t-test -2.5 0.34 0.05
DECISION TEST
Nestmate (trapped) Nestmate (loose) Nestmate Non-nestmate Nestmate O. chelifer
Average number
of rescuers 8.1 ± 1.24 1.00 ± 1.31 5.33 ± 3.45 4.13 ± 3.61 7.60 ± 1.67 1.06 ± 1.17
p-value <0.001 0.35 <0.001
t-test 0.80 0.93 1.31
Table 2. Average latency times of rescue ants present in the capture chamber positioned at different distances.
Table 3. Average numbers of rescue ants present in the trapping chambers when an ant was trapped on one side of the connector tube.
with a nestmate, compared to a chamber with a non-nestmate
(Table 3). The behaviors exhibited by the rescuers when they
arrived at the trapping chamber are described in Table 4.
The behaviors suggested that some kind of rescue was only
performed to assist either a nestmate or a non-nestmate of the
same species (Table 4).
Discussion
The range of signals involved was even more evident
from analysis of the signicant differences between the
distances in terms of latency time. Alarm pheromones are
volatile chemical compounds used for communication by
various social insects (Crewe et al., 1972; Traniello, 1982;
Blomquist & Bagnères, 2010). These include various species
of less derived ants (Robertson, 1971; Hölldobler & Taylor,
1983; Hölldobler & Wilson, 1990), such as O. bauri (Sainz-
Borgo et al., 2011). Hölldobler and Wilson (1990) reported
that these alarm pheromones are transmitted over short
distances and are coded by workers able to respond in
various ways, such as by attacking. The diffusion model of
Bossert and Wilson (1963) predicts that the alarm pheromone
emitted by ants can reach a radius of approximately 20cm,
in the absence of a draft. Therefore, in this study, if the
ants responded only to chemical signals, the range of these
compounds would be greater, since some response occurred
even at a distance of 90 cm. However, it should be noted
that the testing employed a chamber-and-tube system, which
may have reduced dispersion and assisted the targeting of the
volatile compounds.
Another consideration is that when immobilized, the
workers of O. brunneus make an audible sound produced
by stridulation. Although this would need to be tested, it
should be highlighted here that the workers could emit this
complementary signal, in order to enlist help. This acoustic
signal is produced by friction between the petiole and the
gaster (Markl, 1965; Taylor, 1978; Hölldobler & Wilson,
1990; Grasso et al., 2000), as observed previously in ants
of this genus (Markl, 1973; Golden & Hill, 2016). Among
other functions, these signals emitted by ants may be a call
for help, as indicated in several ant rescue behavior studies
(Czechowski et al., 2002; Nowbahari et al., 2009; Nowbahari
& Hollis, 2010; Miler, 2016; Frank et al., 2017, 2018).
Therefore, it is possible that nestmates may have emitted a
chemical and/or audible signal that was coded by rescuers,
DISTANCE
30 cm 60 cm 90 cm
With ant Without ant With ant Without ant With ant Without ant
Average number
of rescuers 8.73 ± 1.05 1.53 ± 1.66 3.53 ± 1.31 3.33 ± 1.49 3.03 ± 1.27 2.06 ± 1.54
p-value <0.001 0.54 0.16
t-test 21.81 0.61 1.43
Table 1. Average numbers of rescue ants attending nestmates trapped at different distances.
Luiz C Santos-Junior, Emerson P Silva, William F Antonialli-Junior – Odontomachus brunneus Nestmates Request Help
enabling rescue behavior to be directed towards their
nestmates. The evidence suggested that the rst responders
could have responded to the call for help at distances greater
than reported in the literature (Bossert & Wilson, 1963).
In contrast, the number of rst responders responding
to the request for help by the ant in the trapping chamber
decreased signicantly as the distance increased. Recent
work has evidenced rescue behavior among nestmates in
poneromorphic ants (Frank et al., 2017, 2018), but no available
data could be found describing the maximum distance of such
signaling. Rescue behavior in ants was discussed by Hollis
and Nowbahari (2013b), who demonstrated that a greater
number of workers involved in the rescue attempt could
improve the chances of success. Group defensive behavior in
social insects, especially ants, is a well-known phenomenon
(Hölldobler & Wilson, 1990) and includes rescue behavior
(Czechowski et al., 2002; Hollis & Nowbahari, 2013a; Hollis
et al., 2015; Frank et al., 2017). The present results suggested
that distance could be a major factor determining the number
of ants recruited for rescue, indicating that distance may be a
key factor inuencing rescue success. In this case, it appeared
that the request for help would receive a better response if the
captured ant was not more than 30 cm distant, corresponding
to relatively close proximity to the entrance of the nest, if the
ant was captured outside it.
The emission of signals by the captured ant is a major
determinant of rescue success. Miler (2016) evaluated rescue
behavior among nestmates of Formica cinerea, comparing
capture by the lion ant (Myrmeleon bore) with articial
capture, from which it was concluded that the latency time
was shorter for articially captured ants. When ants are
captured by a lion ant, they are anesthetized by the action of
the chemical compound injected by the Myrmeleontidae. For
this reason, the ants are slow to emit a help signal, resulting
in a longer latency time for rescue. It was also concluded that
the longer latency time in the aid request resulted in a lower
expectation of rescue, compared to ants who promptly issued
the aid request (Miler, 2016).
The results also suggested that the signals involved
are similar between colonies of the same species. The rescue
workers responded to requests for help from nestmates and
non-nestmates, without any signicant difference. Previous
studies with ants have also reported this rescue behavior for
non-nestmates (Taylor et al., 2013; Uy et al., 2018). However,
in studies of such behavior in the ant Oecophylla smaragdina
Fabricius, 1775, Uy et al. (2018) concluded that colonies with
greater similarity in odor models could present a recognition
error, with non-nestmates likely to be confused with nestmates,
consequently being rescued. In this way, colonies that are
closer are more likely to be genetically related, resulting in
greater tolerance among the nearest colonies, which increases
the possibility of rescuing non-nestmates (Errard et al.,
2006; Newey et al., 2010; Uy et al., 2018). This provides an
explanation for the fact that the O. brunneus rst responders
responded to requests for help from non-nestmates, as well as
from nestmates, since the colonies were collected at relatively
close distances.
On the other hand, the rescue workers responded to
the request for help from O. chelifer ants with aggressive
behaviors, stinging and biting the immobilized ant (Table 4).
This was different from how they acted when helping the
ants of the same species, when the rescuers delivered bites
and stings to the duct tape, suggesting an attempt to free their
nestmate. These results corroborated those of Hollis and
Nowbahari (2013a), who investigated the requests for help
using ve different species of ants, nding that all of them
rescued their co-specics, but not ants of other species, in the
latter case also being aggressive towards the immobilized
ant. The evidence suggests that the signals involved in this
type of behavior are specic, at least at the species level.
However, further analysis is needed to assess the level of
specicity, since rescue behavior is onerous for the ants that
perform it, and rescuing an ant from another colony without a
degree of kinship would not make sense from an evolutionary
point of view.
It could be concluded from the results obtained in this
work that O. brunneus workers respond to signals emitted by
a worker who may have been captured by a potential predator,
leading to an attempt at rescue. The signals involved seem to
have an optimal range, in addition to being species-specic.
This species emits sound signals by stridulation, when exposed
to a capture situation, suggesting that this type of signal may
be involved, in addition to chemical signals. However, further
experiments will be necessary to test this possibility.
Table 4. Frequencies of behaviors exhibited during interactions between rescue workers and the trapped ant in the decision-making experiments.
FREQUENCY DURING THE MEETING (%)
Behaviors of rescue workers Nestmate
(loose)
Nestmate
(trapped) Nestmate Non-nestmate O. brunneus O. chelifer
Recognition of the trapped individual 0.00 40.32 36.25 33.87 49.56 56.25
Pull the trapped individual with the jaws 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00
Bite or pull the tape with the jaws 0.00 13.16 19.37 23.38 17.98 6.25
Sting or bite the trapped individual 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 34.37
Sting the tape 0.00 46.50 44.37 42.74 32.45 3.12
Sociobiology 68(3): e6022 (September, 2021)
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Fundação de Apoio ao
Desenvolvimento do Ensino Ciência e Tecnologia do
Estado de Mato Grosso do Sul (FUNDECT) for a doctoral
scholarship awarded to the third author (FUNDECT
03/2014), Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de
Nível Superior (CAPES) for doctoral scholarships awarded
to the rst and second authors, and Conselho Nacional de
Desenvolvimento Cientíco e Tecnológico (CNPq) for a
productivity scholarship (WFAJ, grant number 307998/2014-2).
Author contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: Santos-Junior,
L.C. & Antonialli-Junior, W.F.; Performed the experiments:
Santos-Junior, L.C. & Silva, E.P.; Analyzed the data: Santos-
Junior, L.C., Silva, E.P., & Antonialli-Junior, W.F.; Wrote the
paper: Santos-Junior, L.C. & Antonialli-Junior, W.F.
References
Breed, M.D., Guzmán-Novoa, E. & Hunt, G.J. (2004). Defensive
behavior of honey bees: Organisation, genetics, and comparison
with other bees. Annual Review of Entomology, 49: 271-298.
doi: 10.1146/annurev.ento.49.061802.123155
Breed, M.D., Robinson, G.E. & Page R.E.-Jr. (1990). Division
of labor during honeybee colony defense. Behavioral Ecology
and Sociobiology, 27: 395-401
Beponis, L.M., O’Dea, R.E., Ohl, V.A., Ryan, M.P., Backwell,
P.R.Y., Binning, S.A. & Haff, T.M. (2014). Cleaning up after
a meal: The consequences of prey disposal for pit-building
antlion larvae. Ethology, 120: 873-880. doi: 10.1111/eth.12257
Bossert, W.H. & Wilson, E.O. (1963). The analysis of olfactory
communication among animals. Journal of Theoretical
Biology, 5: 443-469 doi: 10,101/0022-5193(63)90089-4
Blomquist, G.J. & Bagnères, A.G. (2010). Insect Hydrocarbons
Biology, Biochemistry, And Chemical Ecology. Cambridge
University Press, 528 p.
Chiu, Y.K., Mankin, R.W. & Lin, C.C. (2011). Context-
dependent stridulatory responses of Leptogenys kitteli
(Hymenoptera: Formicidae) to social, prey, and disturbance
stimuli. Annals of the Entomological Society of America,
104: 1012-1020. doi: 10.1603/an11027
Czechowski, W., Godzińska, E.J. & Kozłowski M.W. (2002).
Rescue behavior shown by workers of Formica sanguinea
Latr., F. fusca L. and F. cinerea (Hymenoptera: Formicidae)
in response to their nestmates caught by an ant lion larva.
Annales Zoologici, 52: 423-431
Crewe, R.M., Blum, M.S. & Collingwood, C.A. (1972).
Comparative analysis of alarm pheromones in the ant genus
Crematogaster. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology,
43: 703-716. doi: 10.1016/0305-0491(72)90155-1
Duhoo, T., Durand, J.L., Hollis, K.L. & Nowbahari, E.
(2017). Organization of rescue behaviour sequences in ants,
Cataglyphis cursor, reects goal-directedness, plasticity and
memory. Behavioural Processes, 139: 12-18. doi: 10.1016/j.
beproc.2017.02.006
Errard, C., Hefetz, A. & Jaisson, P. (2006). Social discrimination
tuning in ants: template formation and chemical similarity.
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 59: 353-363. doi: 10.10
07/s00265-005-0058-z
Frank, ET., Schmitt, T., Hovestadt, T., Mitesser, O., Stiegler,
J. & Linsenmair, K.E. (2017). Saving the injured: rescue
behavior in the termite-hunting ant Megaponera analis.
Science Advances, 3: e1602187. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1602187
Frank, E.T., Wehrhahn, M. & Linsenmair, K.E. (2018).
Wound treatment and selective help in a termite-hunting ant.
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
285: 1-8. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2017.2457
Grasso, D.A.T., Wenseleers, T., Mori, A., Moli, F. & Billen,
J. (2000). Thelytokous worker reproduction and lack of
Wolbachia infection in the harvesting ant Messor capitatus.
Ethology Ecology and Evolution, 12: 309-314. doi: 10.1080/
08927014.2000.9522803
Golden, T.M.J. & Hill, P.S. (2016). The evolution of stridulatory
communication in ants, revisited. Insectes Sociaux, 63:309-
319 doi: 10.1007/s00040-016-0470-6
Gotelli, N.J. (1996). Ant community structure: Effects of
predatory ant lions. Ecology, 77: 630-638. doi: 10.2307/2265636
Hernández, J.V., Lopez, H. & Jaffé, K. (2002). Nestmate
recognition signals of the leaf cutting ant Atta laevigata.
Journal of Insect Physiology, 48: 287-295. doi: 10.1016/
S0022-2973 1910(01)00173-1
Hölldobler, B. & Taylor, R.W. (1983) A behavioral study of
the primitive ant, Nothomyrmecia macrops Clark. Insectes
Sociaux, 30: 384-401. doi: 10.1007/BF02223970
Hölldobler, B. & Wilson, E.O. (1990). The Ants. Cambridge:
Harvard University Press, 732 p
Hollis, K.L. (2017). Ants and antlions: The impact of ecology,
coevolution and learning on an insect predatory prey
relationship. Behavioural Processes, 139: 4-11. doi: 10.1016/j.
beproc.2016.12.002
Hollis, K.L. & Nowbahari, E. (2013-a). A comparative analysis
of precision rescue behavior in sand-dwelling ants. Animal
Behaviour, 85: 537-544. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.12.005
Hollis, K.L. & Nowbahari, E. (2013-b). Toward a behavioral
ecology of rescue behavior. Evolutionary Psychology, 11:
647-664. doi: 10.1177/147470491301100311
Luiz C Santos-Junior, Emerson P Silva, William F Antonialli-Junior – Odontomachus brunneus Nestmates Request Help
Howard, R.W. & Blomquist, G.J. (2005). Ecological, behavioral,
and biochemical aspects of insect hydrocarbons. Annual
Review of Entomology, 50: 371-393. doi: 10.1146/annurev.
ento.50.071803.130359
Hollis, K.L., Harrsch, F.A. & Nowbahari, E. (2015). Ants vs.
antlions: An insect model for studying the role of learned and
hard-wired behavior in coevolution. Learning Motivation 50:
68-82. doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2014.11.003
Hou, C., Kaspari, M., Zanden, H.B.V. & Gillooly, J.F. (2010).
Energetic basis of colonial living in social insects. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences, 107: 3634-3638. doi:
10.1073/pnas.0908071107
Jaffé, K. & Marcuse, M. (1983). Nestmate recognition and
territorial behaviour in the ant Odontornachus bauri Emery
(Formicidae: Ponerinae). Insectes Sociaux, 30: 466-481
Lahav, S., Soroker, V., Hefetz, A. & Vander-Meer, R.K.
(1999). Direct behavioral evidence for hydrocarbons as ant
recognition discriminators. Naturwissenschaften, 86: 246-249
.doi: 10.1007/s001140050609
Lenoir, A., Hefetz, A., Simon, T. & Soroker, V. (2001).
Comparative dynamics of gestalt odour formation in two
ant species Camponotus fellah and Aphaenogaster senilis
(Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Physiological Entomology, 26:
275-283. doi: 10.1046/j.0307-6962.2001.00244.x
Markl H., Hölldobler B. (1978). Recruitment and food-retrieving
behavior in Novomessor (Formicidae, Hymenoptera): II
vibration signals. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 4:
183-216
Markl, H. (1965). Stridulation in leaf-cutting ants. Science,
149: 1392-1393 doi: 10.1126/science.149.3690.1392
Markl, H. (1973). The evolution of stridulatory communication
in ants. In: International Union for the Study of Social Insects
(7th), Proceedings. London, 258-265 p.
Miler, K. (2016). Moribund ants do not call for help. PLoSOne
11: e0151925 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0151925
Morse, R.A. & Laigo, F.M. (1969). Apis dorsata in the
Philippines. Philippine Association of Entomologists, 1: 1-96
Morgan, E.D., Nascimento, R.R., Keegans, S.J. & Billen, J.
(1999). Comparative study of mandibular gland secretions of
workers of ponerine ants. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 25:
1395-1409. doi: 10.1023/A:1020987028163
Nowbahari, E., Amirault, C. & Hollis, K.L. (2016). Rescue
of newborn ants by older Cataglyphis cursor adult workers.
Animal Cognition, 19: 543-553. doi: 10.1007/s10071-016-0955-8
Nowbahari, E., Hollis, K.L. & Durand, J.L. (2012). Division
of labor regulates precision rescue behavior in sand dwelling
Cataglyphis cursor ants: to give is to receive. PLoS One, 7:
e48516. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048516
Nowbahari, E., Scohier, A., Durand, J. & Hollis, K.L. (2009).
Ants, Cataglyphis cursor, use precisely directed rescue behavior
to free entrapped relatives. PLOS One, 4: e6573. doi: 10.1371/
journal.pone.0006573 PMID: 19672292
Nowbahari, E. & Hollis, K.L. (2010). Rescue behavior.
Distinguishing between rescue, cooperation and other forms of
altruistic behavior. Communicative and Integrative Biology,
3: 77-79. doi: 10.4161/cib.3.2.10018 PMID: 20585494
Newey, P.S., Robson, S.K.A. & Crozier, R.H. (2010). Weaver
ants Oecophylla smaragdina encounter nasty neighbors rather
than dear enemies. Ecology, 91: 2366-2372. doi: 10.1890/09-
0561.1
Powell, S. & Clark, E. (2004). Combat between large derived
societies: A subterranean army ant established as a predator of
mature leaf- cutting ant colonies. Insectes Sociaux, 51: 342-
351. doi: 10.1007/s00040-004-0752-2
Powell S. (2008). Ecological specialization and the evolution
of a specialized caste in Cephalotes ants. Functional Ecology,
22: 902-911. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3046 2435.2008.01436.x
Pepper, J.W. & Herron, M.D. (2008). Does biology need an
organism concept? Biological Reviews, 83: 621-627. doi:
10.1111/j.1469-185X.2008.00057.x
Robertson, P.L. (1971). Pheromones involved in aggressive
behaviour in the ant Myrmeciagulosa, ft. Journal of Insect
Physiology, 17: 691-715. doi: 10.1016/0022-1910(71)90117-X
Siebenaler, J.B. & Caldwell, D.K. (1956). Cooperation among
adult dolphins. Journal of Mammalogy, 37: 126-128. doi: 10.23
07/1375558
Stuart, R.J. & Bell, P.D. (1980). Stridulation by workers of
the ant Leptothorax muscorum (Nylander) (Hymenoptera:
Formicidae). Psyche, 87: 199-210. doi: 10.1155/1980/46583
Strassmann, J.E. & Queller, D.C. (2010). The social organism:
congresses, parties, and committees. Evolution, 64, 605-616
doi: 10.1111/j.1558- 5646.2009.00929.x
Sainz-Borgo, C., Cabrera, A.E. & Hernández, J.V. (2011).
Nestmate recognition in the ant Odontomachus bauri
(Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Sociobiology, 58: 1-18 doi: 10.10
07/BF02223978
Taylor, K., Visvader, A., Nowbahari, E. & Hollis, K.L. (2013).
Precision rescue behavior in North American ants. Evolutionary
Psychology,11: 665-677 doi: 10.1177/147470491301100312
Taylor, F. (1978). Foraging behavior of ants: theoretical
considerations. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 71: 541-565.
doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(78)90324-7
Traniello, J.F.A. (1982). Population structure and social
organization in the primitive ant Amblyopone pallipes
(Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Psyche, 89: 65-80. doi: 10.1155/
1982/79349
Sociobiology 68(3): e6022 (September, 2021)
Uy, F.M.K., Adcock, J.D., Jeffries, S.F. & Pepere, E. (2018).
Intercolony distance predicts the decision to rescue or attack
conspecics in weaver ants. Insectes Sociaux, 66: 185-192.
doi: 10.1007%2Fs00040-018-0674-z
Vogel, E.R. & Fuentes-Jiménez, A. (2006). Rescue behavior
in white-faced capuchin monkeys during an intergroup attack:
Support for the infanticide avoidance hypothesis. American
Journal of Primatology, 68: 1012-1016. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20286
Wheeler, J.W. & Blum, M.S. (1973). Alkylpyrazine alarm
pheromones in ponerine ants. Science, 182: 501-503 doi:
10.1126/science.182.4111.501
Wilson, E.O. (1976). A social ethogram of the neotropical
arboreal ant, Zacryptocerus varians (Fr. Smith). Animal
Behaviour, 24: 354-363. doi: 10.1016/S0003- 3472(76)80043-7
Yusuf, A.A., Crewe, R.M. & Pirk, C.W. (2014). Olfactory
detection of prey by the termite-raiding ant Pachycondyla
analis. Journal of Insect Science, 14: 53 doi: 10.1093/jis/14.1.53
... About twenty years ago Czechowski et al. (2002) employed the term "rescue behaviour" in the title and the text of the paper describing rescue behaviour displayed by workers of three ant species (Formica sanguinea, Formica fusca and Formica cinerea) in response to ant victims captured by predatory antlion larvae (Myrmeleon formicarius) [27]. This study provided inspiration for extensive further research carried out both in the field and in the laboratory with the use of two main bioassays: antlion larva capture bioassay, during which ant rescue behaviour was elicited in response to an ant captured by an antlion larva [88][89][90][91][92], and artificial snare bioassay, during which ant rescue behaviour was elicited in response to a victim ant entrapped in an artificial snare [28,88,90,91,[93][94][95][96][97][98][99][100][101][102][103][104][105][106]. ...
... Such an artificial snare bioassay was used in numerous laboratory and field experiments to test a wide range of ant species from the subfamilies Formicinae [28,88,90,91,[93][94][95][96][97][98][99]101,[103][104][105][106], Myrmicinae [88,90,96], and Dolichoderinae [90]. Modified versions of that bioassay consisted of a confrontation of potential rescuers with a victim immobilized by means of a duct tape applied to its legs (Odontomachus brunneus, subfamily Ponerinae) [102], and of simultaneous confrontation of potential rescuers with two artificial snares, one empty and one containing a victim, or containing two different victims (Cataglyphis niger, subfamily Formicinae) [106]. Yet other studies of ant rescue behaviour investigated the responses of weaver ants Oecophylla smaragdina (subfamily Formicinae) and harvester ants Veromessor pergandeyi (subfamily Myrmicinae) to victims wrapped in spider silk [100,107]. ...
... In these studies rescue attempts directed precisely to the object responsable for the victim's entrapment were labeled by several similar terms including such expressions as "precision rescue", "precision rescue behaviour" and "precise rescue behaviour patterns" [18,88,93,94,96,103,131], "precisely directed rescue behaviour" [28,29,97,131], "precisely targeted rescue behaviour" [29,95,97,101], and "precisely tuned rescue behaviour" [109]. Responses to the snare responsible for the entrapment of the victim were also investigated and/or discussed in some other research papers in which these terms were not used [90,91,99,102,104,105], as well as in several review papers [10,18,40,109]. However, surprisingly, in some studies pulling at the snare and at the victim's appendages were not analysed separately, but pooled together to form a more general subcategory of pulling behaviour [97,106]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Ant rescue behaviour belongs to the most interesting subcategories of prosocial and altruistic behaviour encountered in the animal world. Several studies suggested that ants are able to identify what exactly restrains the movements of another individual and to direct their rescue behaviour precisely to that object. To shed more light on the question how precise is the identifi-cation of the source of restraint of another ant, we investigated rescue behaviour of workers of the red wood ant Formica polyctena using a new version of an artificial snare bioassay in which a nestmate victim was bearing on its body two wire loops, one placed on the petiole and acting as a snare, and an additional one on the leg. The tested ants did not direct preferentially their rescue behaviour to the snare. Moreover, the overall strategy adopted by the most active rescuers was not limited to precisely targeted rescue attempts directed to the snare, but consisted of attempts to find a solution to the victim’s problem through frequent trial-and-error switching between various subcategories of rescue behaviour. These findings highlight the importance of precise identifica-tion of cognitive processes and overall behavioural strategies for better understanding of causal factors underlying animal helping behaviour.
... In another recent study conducted by Santos-Junior et al. [58], the authors modified laboratory simulations of entrapment proposed by Nowbahari et al. [32] even further. In their version of the test, the legs of Odontomachus brunneus ants were attached to the floor of the trapping chamber by tape. ...
... In terms of antlion predation and rescue actions as an antipredatory strategy of ants in response to selective pressures imposed on them by antlions, this has not been satisfactorily demonstrated to date, and the fact that some ants display rescue in this context might even be an artefact. Curiously, the last-mentioned study on ants, by Santos-Junior et al. [58], provided another indirect clue in favor of this argument. Specifically, O. brunneus ants, in which the authors demonstrated rescue of nestmates trapped by tape, nevertheless show an alternative tactic when faced with antlion predation, namely, escape jumps from the traps [95]. ...
... Anoplolepis gracilipes artificial ensnarement in the laboratory antlion traps in the laboratory Undetected * [48] Aphaenogaster senilis artificial ensnarement in the field detected but weak and/or infrequent [34] Atta cephalotes entrapment using soil detected and pronounced * [24] Camponotus korthalsiae artificial ensnarement in the laboratory antlion traps in the laboratory Undetected * [48] Cataglyphis floricola artificial ensnarement in the field detected and pronounced [34] Cataglyphis cursor artificial ensnarement in the field and in the laboratory detected and pronounced [32,33,40,42,54] Formica cinerea artificial ensnarement in the field and in the laboratory antlion traps in the field and in the laboratory detected and pronounced [27,38,43,48,50,55,57] Formica fusca antlion traps in the field undetected [27] Formica polyctena artificial ensnarement in the laboratory detected but weak and/or infrequent * [48] Formica sanguinea antlion traps in the field detected and pronounced [27] Iridomyrmex anceps artificial ensnarement in the laboratory detected but weak and/or infrequent * [48] Megaponeraanalis confrontation with termites in the field detected and pronounced [49,51] Messor barbarus artificial ensnarement in the field undetected [34] Messor marocanus artificial ensnarement in the field detected but weak and/or infrequent [34] Myrmica ruginodis artificial ensnarement in the laboratory Undetected * [48] Lasius grandis artificial ensnarement in the field detected and pronounced [34] Pogonomyrmex occidentalis entrapment using sand detected and pronounced * [25] Prenolepis imparis artificial ensnarement in the field antlion traps in the laboratory detected but weak and/or infrequent [35] Oecophylla smaragdina spider webs in the field detected and pronounced [53] Odontomachus brunneus artificial ensnarement in the laboratory detected but limited to aggression towards the tape * [58] Solenopsis geminata artificial entrapment in the laboratory detected and pronounced * [26] Tetramorium sp. E artificial ensnarement in the field antlion traps in the laboratory detected and pronounced [35] Veromessor pergandei spider webs in the field and in the laboratory detected and pronounced [52] ...
Article
Full-text available
Simple Summary Ants provide an outstanding example of organisms capable of risky acts. When ants engage in rescue behavior, for example, they do so for a chance of saving another individual from a dangerous situation. What contributes to whether a particular ant engages in rescue behavior? Why do some species of ants show high rescue activity while other species show no such behavior at all? How is rescue behavior triggered in ants? Finally, but no less importantly, how risky engaging in a rescue action really is and what benefits it brings to both the rescuing and rescued ant? These are the fundamental questions we address here. We demonstrate the progress in the research field and, in doing so, we expose the extent to which the abovementioned questions are unanswered. In this comprehensive review, we present a summary of relevant published works and hope to spin higher interest in the fascinating area of study that is ant rescue behavior. Abstract Altruism is defined as an action that decreases the lifetime direct fitness of an actor and benefits one or more recipients. This phenomenon, which is generally difficult to understand and explain, requires special research attention. The subject of this review, rescue, is a type of altruistic behavior in which the actor puts itself at risk to save another individual, the recipient, that is in danger. The highest numbers of published empirical works have been devoted to rescue behavior in ants and they have enormous potential for further study. We review studies devoted to the subject and group them into four main areas of research on ant rescue actions: (1) variation in rescue behavior activity on a between-individual scale, (2) factors contributing to the evolution of rescue behavior on a between-species scale, (3) rescue behavior releaser signals and (4) rescue behavior benefits and costs. We highlight the progress in research on rescue behavior in ants, indicate that this behavior is probably much more common than previously thought yet thus far demonstrated in only a few species, and uncover research gaps and open questions that remain unexplored. We additionally point out some gaps in knowledge that become evident when research devoted to rescue behavior in rats, the second most studied group of animals in this context, is briefly overviewed. We hope to help navigate among studies on rescue behavior and provide the most up-to-date summary of the relevant literature. Moreover, we hope to encourage and facilitate researchers in behavioral ecology and other subdisciplines to further experimentally analyze rescue behavior, not only in ants but also in other taxa.
... Natural history: Odontomachus cupreus has been found nesting in the soil, under rotten trunks, and inside the nests of Cornitermes cumulans (Kollar, 1832). There are records of specimens collected in hollow trunks of Caesalpinia pluviosa DC. (Santos et al. 2021). In this study, the authors identified the specimens as O. brunneus, but their vouchers were checked and identified as O. cupreus (INPA-HYM 033677 and Voucher 6010). ...
Article
Full-text available
We provide a taxonomic revision of the Neotropical species of Odontomachus, including description of gynes, their wing venation, and description of males. We examined 2633 workers, 156 gynes, and 142 males, resulting in four new species, which are described here: O. chicomendesi sp. nov., O. cupreus sp. nov., O. dubius sp. nov. and O. xeta sp. nov. We propose the synonym of O. mayi n. syn. under O. panamensis, resulting in 27 species of Odontomachus in the neotropics. Dichotomous keys, illustrated with high quality images, are provided for workers, gynes, and males. We describe/redescribe 21 gynes and 15 males. The worker of O. allolabis is redescribed and the status of the type specimens as parasitized is confirmed. Odontomachus chelifer shows wide variation of the petiolar node, metasternal process and male propodeum sculpturing indicating that it is possibly a complex of cryptic species. The metasternal process is an important diagnostic character for most of the species. The morphology of the worker and gynes are congruent with the current molecular phylogeny for the genus. We discuss the hastatus and haematodus species groups relationships and keep O. cornutus, O. mormo, and O. bradleyi in their respective monospecific groups. It is still necessary to investigate male genitalia to shed light on the relations within the haematodus group and detect possible cryptic species.
Article
Full-text available
Ant rescue behaviour belongs to the most interesting subcategories of prosocial and altruistic behaviour encountered in the animal world. Several studies suggested that ants are able to identify what exactly restrains the movements of another individual and to direct their rescue behaviour precisely to that object. To shed more light on the question of how precise the identification of the source of restraint of another ant is, we investigated rescue behaviour of red wood ant Formica polyctena workers, using a new version of an artificial snare bioassay in which a nestmate victim bore two wire loops on its body, one (acting as a snare) placed on its petiole and an additional one on its leg. The tested ants did not preferentially direct their rescue behaviour towards the snare. Moreover, the overall strategy adopted by the most active rescuers was not limited to precisely targeted rescue attempts directed towards the snare, but consisted of frequent switching between various subcategories of rescue behaviour. These findings highlight the importance of precise identification of cognitive processes and overall behavioural strategies for better understanding of causal factors underlying animal helping behaviour in light of new facts discovered by testing of various successive research hypotheses.
Article
Full-text available
Jednym z najważniejszych elementów społecznego trybu życia są zachowania prospołeczne, czyli działania, z których korzyści odnoszą inne osobniki. Do najbardziej interesujących podkategorii ryzykownych zachowań prospołecznych należą zachowania ratunkowe, czyli spieszenie z pomocą osobnikom, które znalazły się w niebezpiecznej sytuacji. W świecie zwierząt zachowania ratunkowe zostały po raz pierwszy opisane u mrówek legionistek z gatunku Eciton hamatum, a następnie badano je również u mrówek z wielu innych gatunków należących do podrodzin Formicinae, Myrmicinae, Ponerinae i Dolichoderinae. Badania te były prowadzone zarówno w terenie, jak i w warunkach laboratoryjnych i stosowano w nich szeroką gamę testów behawioralnych, podczas których potencjalne ratowniczki konfrontowano bądź z osobnikami o ograniczonej mobilności (unieruchomionymi w wyniku zasypania lub też uwięzionymi w sztucznym sidle), bądź też z mrówkami zaatakowanymi przez wrogów naturalnych, takich jak larwy mrówkolwa, pająki lub termity. W badaniach tych analizowano wpływ licznych czynników wpływających na gotowość mrówek do angażowania się w zachowania ratunkowe oraz na ujawniające się w tym kontekście wzorce behawioralne, w tym przede wszystkim tzw. precyzyjne zachowania ratunkowe stanowiące świadectwo nieoczekiwanie zaawansowanych możliwości poznawczych tych owadów. Do czynników tych należały wiek, specjalizacja behawioralna oraz oczekiwana dalsza długość trwania życia zarówno ofiar, jak i potencjalnych ratowniczek, a także przebyte doświadczenia, czynniki genetyczne oraz gatunek i specyfika jego ekologii. Sprawdzano też, czy ratowane są jedynie towarzyszki z własnej kolonii, czy również osobniki pochodzące z obcych kolonii własnego gatunku oraz z gatunków obcych. Podejmowano też próby identyfikacji sygnałów emitowanych przez ofiary w ramach tzw. wołania o pomoc. Zaobserwowano również, że niektóre mrówki otaczają opieką sanitarną towarzyszki zranione przez wrogów naturalnych lub też cierpiące wskutek infekcji i stwierdzono, że opieka taka może być korzystna zarówno dla osobników poszkodowanych, jak i dla ich zdrowych towarzyszek, gdyż uratowane osobniki mogą kontynuować pracę na rzecz kolonii, zaś kontakty z zakażonymi towarzyszkami mogą wywierać stymulujący wpływ na procesy odpornościowe zdrowych osobników i dzięki temu stanowić element profilaktyki przeciwdziałającej rozprzestrzenianiu się chorób zakaźnych. Niemniej jednak, skrócona oczekiwana dalsza długość trwania życia często idzie u mrówek w parze z tzw. wycofaniem społecznym, które może się wyrazić również w mniejszej gotowości do angażowania się w zachowania ratunkowe. Osobniki takie są również mniej chętnie ratowane przez towarzyszki. Zachowania ratunkowe opisywano i analizowano również u kręgowców: ptaków (dwa gatunki) i licznych ssaków (szczury, myszy, psy, mangusty, dziki, słonie, a także różne walenie i naczelne). Badania zachowań ratunkowych zwierząt rzucają istotne światło zarówno na ewolucję altruizmu i zachowań prospołecznych, jak i na zdolności poznawcze zwierząt i czynniki, które mogą je wspomagać lub osłabiać. Wyniki badań zachowań ratunkowych mrówek stanowią też świetną ilustrację tego, w jaki sposób badania porównawcze przyczyniają się do formułowania ogólnych zasad i prawidłowości ukazując pełniejszy obraz badanych zagadnień i pozwalając na wyciągnięcie wniosków o wyższym stopniu ogólności.
Article
Full-text available
Open wounds are a major health risk in animals, with species prone to injuries likely developing means to reduce these risks. We therefore analysed the behavioural response towards open wounds on the social and individual level in the termite group-hunting ant Megaponera analis. During termite raids, some ants get injured by termite soldiers (biting off extremities), after the fight injured ants get carried back to the nest by nest-mates. We observed treatment of the injury by nest-mates inside the nest through intense allogrooming at the wound. Lack of treatment increased mortality from 10% to 80% within 24 h, most likely due to infections. Wound clotting occurred extraordinarily fast in untreated injured individuals, within 10 min. Furthermore, heavily injured ants (loss of five extremities) were not rescued or treated; this was regulated not by the helper but by the unresponsiveness of the injured ant. Interestingly, lightly injured ants behaved ‘more injured’ near nest-mates. We show organized social wound treatment in insects through a multifaceted help system focused on injured individuals. This was not only limited to selective rescuing of lightly injured individuals by carrying them back (thus reducing predation risk), but, moreover, included a differentiated treatment inside the nest.
Article
Full-text available
Predators of highly defensive prey likely develop cost-reducing adaptations. The ant Megaponera analis is a specialized termite predator, solely raiding termites of the subfamily Macrotermitinae (in this study, mostly colonies of Pseudocanthotermes sp.) at their foraging sites. The evolutionary arms race between termites and ants led to various defensive mechanisms in termites (for example, a caste specialized in fighting predators). Because M. analis incurs high injury/mortality risks when preying on termites, some risk-mitigating adaptations seem likely to have evolved. We show that a unique rescue behavior in M. analis, consisting of injured nestmates being carried back to the nest, reduces combat mortality. After a fight, injured ants are carried back by their nestmates; these ants have usually lost an extremity or have termites clinging to them and are able to recover within the nest. Injured ants that are forced experimentally to return without help, die in 32% of the cases. Behavioral experiments show that two compounds, dimethyl disulfide and dimethyl trisulfide, present in the mandibular gland reservoirs, trigger the rescue behavior. A model accounting for this rescue behavior identifies the drivers favoring its evolution and estimates that rescuing enables maintenance of a 28.7% larger colony size. Our results are the first to explore experimentally the adaptive value of this form of rescue behavior focused on injured nestmates in social insects and help us to identify evolutionary drivers responsible for this type of behavior to evolve in animals.
Article
Full-text available
When an antlion captures a foraging ant, the victim’s nestmates may display rescue behaviour. This study tested the hypothesis that the expression of rescue behaviour depends on the life expectancy of the captured ant. This hypothesis predicts that the expression of rescue behaviour will be less frequent when the captured ant has a lower life expectancy than when it has a higher life expectancy because such a response would be adaptive at the colony level. Indeed, significant differences were found in the frequency of rescue behaviours in response to antlion victims with differing life expectancies. In agreement with prediction, victims with lower life expectancies were rescued less frequently, and those rescues had a longer latency and shorter duration. There was also a qualitative difference in the behaviour of rescuers to victims from the low and high life expectancy groups. Several explanations for these findings are proposed.
Article
Full-text available
The presence and use of a stridulatory organ (SO) to produce vibrational signals is highly variable and structured within the ants. The file and scraper that make up the SO are specialized morphological features not used for functions other than stridulation (a vibratory signal production mechanism) and not found in non-stridulating species. It has been hypothesized and generally accepted that the stridulatory organ first evolved to alert nest mates of burial and need for rescue. Based on this premise, arboreal species are expected to exhibit a reduction of use of vibrational signals when compared to terrestrial species. Using ancestral state reconstruction, we have mapped the presence of the SO on a molecular phylogeny of the ants and found support for the hypothesis that the SO evolved multiple times in the ants. We quantitatively tested the hypothesis that stridulation evolved initially to signal burial/rescue by comparing the presence of the SO to general foraging and nesting preferences for 76 genera evenly spread throughout the currently accepted ant phylogeny. We found that a greater proportion of genera that are considered primarily arboreal possess a SO, as opposed to the ground nesting genera, and none of the five entirely subterranean genera included in this study possess a SO. We therefore reject the previous hypothesis regarding burial/rescue signaling.
Article
Full-text available
Cataglyphis cursor worker ants are capable of highly sophisticated rescue behaviour in which individuals are able to identify what has trapped a nestmate and to direct their behaviour towards that obstacle. Nonetheless, rescue behaviour is constrained by workers’ subcaste: whereas foragers, the oldest workers, are able both to give and to receive the most help, the youngest workers, inactives, neither give nor receive any help whatsoever; nurses give and receive intermediate levels of aid, reflecting their intermediate age. Such differences in rescue behaviour across subcastes suggest that age and experience play a critical role. In this species, as in many others in which a sensitive period for nestmate recognition exists, newly enclosed ants, called callows, are adopted by ants belonging not only to different colonies but also to different species; foreign callows receive nearly the same special care provided to resident newborns. Because callows are younger than inactives, which are incapable of soliciting rescue, we wondered whether entrapped callows would receive such aid. In the present study, we artificially ensnared individual callows from their own colony (homocolonial), from a different colony (heterocolonial), and from a different species (heterospecific), and tested each one with groups of five potential C. cursor rescuers, either all foragers or all nurses. Our results show that all three types of callows are able to elicit rescue behaviour from both foragers and nurses. Nonetheless, nurse rescuers are better able to discriminate between the three types of callow victims than are foragers.
Article
Full-text available
Altruistic behavior, in which one individual provides aid to another at some cost to itself, is well documented. However, some species engage in a form of altruism, called rescue, that places the altruist in immediate danger. Here we investigate one such example, namely rescuing victims captured by predators. In a field experiment with two North American ant species, Tetramorium sp. E and Prenolepis imparis, individuals were held in artificial snares simulating capture. T. sp. E, but not P. imparis, exhibited digging, pulling, and snare biting, the latter precisely targeted to the object binding the victim. These results are the first to document precision rescue in a North American ant species; moreover, unlike rescue in other ants, T. sp. E rescues conspecifics from different colonies, mirroring their atypical social behavior, namely the lack of aggression between non-nestmate (heterocolonial) conspecifics. In a second, observational study designed to demonstrate rescue from an actual predator, T. sp. E victims were dropped into an antlion's pit and the behavior of a single rescuer was observed. Results showed that T. sp. E not only attempted to release the victim, but also risked attacking the predator, suggesting that precision rescue may play an important role in this species' antipredator behavior.
Article
Full-text available
We report the results of field observations and experiments demonstrating that workers of Formica sanguinea Latr. and F. cinerea Mayr caught by a larva of an ant lion (Myrmeleon formicarius L.) can induce rescue behaviour in their nestmates. Typical rescue behaviour involves both the attempts to pull away the attacked ant by tugging at its limbs, and rapid, intense digging behaviour. In natural mixed colonies of F. sanguinea and F. fusca L., enslaved F. fusca workers display rescue behaviour when their heterospecific nestmate is caught by an ant lion larva. On the other hand, we did not observe nestmate rescue behaviour in monospecific F. fusca colonies. These data suggest that workers of F. sanguinea and F. cinerea caught by an ant lion emit some signals which summon their nestmates to arrive at their rescue. Workers of F. fusca either do not emit such signals, or their danger signals fail to elicit rescue behaviour in other ants. Exprcssion of rescue behaviour in the studied ant species is discussed in the context of their life strategies, of their position in the interspecific competitive hierarchy, and of our knowledge about nestmate rescue behaviour, and signals eliciting alarm and digging behaviour in ants.
Article
The experimental study of rescue behaviour in ants, behaviour in which individuals help entrapped nestmates in distress, has revealed that rescuers respond to victims with very precisely targeted behaviour. In Cataglyphis cursor, several different components of rescue behaviour have been observed, demonstrating the complexity of this behaviour, including sand digging and sand transport to excavate the victim, followed by pulling on the victim’s limbs as well as the object holding the victim in place, behaviour that serves to free the victim. Although previous work suggested that rescue was optimally organized, first to expose and then to extricate the victim under a variety of differing circumstances, experimental analysis of that organization has been lacking. Here, using experimental data, we characterize the pattern of individual rescue behaviour in C. cursor by analysing the probabilities of transitions from one behavioural component to another. The results show that the execution of each behavioural component is determined by the interplay of previous acts. In particular, we show not only that ants move sand away from the victim in an especially efficient sequence of behaviour that greatly minimizes energy expenditure, but also that ants appear to form some kind of memory of what they did in the past, a memory that directs their future behaviour.
Article
A behavioural ecological approach to the relationship between pit-digging larval antlions and their common prey, ants, provides yet another example of how the specific ecological niche that species inhabit imposes selection pressures leading to unique behavioural adaptations. Antlions rely on multiple strategies to capture prey with a minimal expenditure of energy and extraordinary efficiency while ants employ several different strategies for avoiding capture, including rescue of trapped nestmates. Importantly, both ants and antlions rely heavily on their capacity for learning, a tool that sometimes is overlooked in predator-prey relationships, leading to the implicit assumption that behavioural adaptations are the result of fixed, hard-wired responses. Nonetheless, like hard-wired responses, learned behaviour, too, is uniquely adapted to the ecological niche, a reminder that the expression of associative learning is species-specific. Beyond the study of ants and antlions, per se, this particular predator-prey relationship reveals the important role that the capacity to learn plays in coevolutionary arms races.