ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Artificial intelligence and machine learning capabilities are developing at lightning speed. However, these machines lack the module of emotional processing, which makes artificial intelligence fundamentally different from human intelligence. The intellectual development of humans follows the path of the biological evolution towards the accumulation and transfer of information with increasing efficiency. Apart from the cognitive constant of speech, which dramatically enhanced the transfer of information, humans have advanced their computational power by using various devices, from the abacus to the quantum computer. Yet the mental lives of humans cannot be separated from feelings. The neurodynamics of emotion is still poorly understood; this is the greatest challenge to creating artificial intelligence on par with human intelligence. Keywords: artificial intelligence; embodied cognition; emotion; social matrix; triune brain
Emotion,(embodied(cognition,(and(Artificial(Intelligence((
Marina&Korsakova-Kreyn&
1
ACADEMIA Letters
Academia Letters, August 2021
Corresponding Author: Marina Korsakova-Kreyn, mnkors@gmail.com
Citation: Korsakova-Kreyn, M. (2021). Emotion, embodied cognition, and Artificial Intelligence. Academia Letters,
Article 2883. https://doi.org/10.20935/AL2883.
©2021 by the author Open Access Distributed under CC BY 4.0
Emotion, embodied cognition, and Artificial Intelligence
Marina Korsakova-Kreyn, PhD
Thus, age by age - oh, how soon, my Lord? -
Under the art and nature’s scalpel,
Our spirit screams, and flesh is worn
By giving birth to the sixth sense’s organ.
Nikolai Gumilev
Artificial intelligence and machine learning capabilities are developing at lightning speed.
However, these machines lack the module of emotional processing, which makes artificial
intelligence fundamentally different from human intelligence. The intellectual development
of humans follows the path of the biological evolution towards the accumulation and transfer
of information with increasing efficiency. Apart from the cognitive constant of speech, which
dramatically enhanced the transfer of information, humans have advanced their computational
power by using various devices, from the abacus to the quantum computer. Yet, the mental
lives of humans cannot be separated from feelings. The neurodynamics of emotion is still
poorly understood; this is the greatest challenge to creating artificial intelligence on par with
human intelligence.
The primordial origin of human consciousness is emotion (Panksepp, 1998a). The
survival of living organisms depends on affective responsiveness, whether the binary response
of a paramecium (lacking a nervous system) or complex coordination of various systems in
the human organism. For example, the emotion of fear triggers a cascade of reactions beginning
with the precognitive response of the autonomic nervous system, complex homeostatic
Emotion,(embodied(cognition,(and(Artificial(Intelligence((
Marina&Korsakova-Kreyn&
2
adjustments, activation of the motor system, and up to moral reasoning involving high-level
cognitive function.
Psychologists define emotions as superordinate programs (Tooby & Cosmides, 2008),
which coordinate and synchronize the behaviors of living organisms at all levels. Basic affective
responses to the environment are biologically determined by their survival value: all
sensory systems of living organisms are designed to register changes in the environment.
Biological evolution created and fine-tuned the visual, auditory, and somatosensory system as
survival tools first and foremost. When components of these systems fail, the organism’s life
is endangered. For example, congenital insensitivity to pain severely hinders a baby’s growth
into adulthood (Zhang et al., 2016).
Coordinated and integrated responses of a complex living organism to its environment,
and the very existence of the organism, relies on homeostatic processes that proceed via
electrochemical communication. In humans, this communication engages an enormous colony
of small organisms: some are our cells, and some are companion microbes, viruses, bacteria,
and protozoa that form a microbiome. Cooperation within the colony exemplifies the
evolutionary advantage of mutual support, found everywhere from prokaryote communication
to the human being and advanced civilization. Along with accumulating various biological
survival tools, evolution produced the human mind capable of highly abstract and elaborate
reasoning.
Even when humans think about abstract things (the expansion of the universe,
mathematical proofs), their rational agency remains inseparable from their feelings. According
to our current understanding of human emotional processing, the core of affective
responsiveness is the limbic system or paleomammalian brain. Because humans share neural
correlates of emotional processing with mammals, we can study emotions with animal models
(Panksepp, 2005). A complete definition of the limbic system’s components is still developing,
though some structures are invariably included in all descriptions. Principal among them are
the hippocampus (learning and memory), the hypothalamus (homeostatic regulation), the
amygdala (anxiety), and the periaqueductal gray (defensive behavior, pain mediation).These
subcortical structures support the rise of affective states that influence our consciousness and
provide a sense of self. The neuropsychology of “self” was articulated in two important models:
the “Core-Self” by Panksepp (1998b) and the “proto-Self” by Damasio (1999). Both models
imply that consciousness is embodied (Damasio, 1994, 1996; Foglia & Wilson, 2013; Lakof &
Johnson, 1999; Merleau-Ponty, 1945/2012; Varela et al., 1991), meaning that our motor
behaviors and our mental lives are interwoven, and that our thoughts and imagination be-
come sculpted through our active interaction with the environment (Aziz-Zadeh et al., 2006;
Aziz-Zadeh & Ivry, 2009; Hostetter & Alibali, 2008; Glenberg & Kaschak, 2002; James &
Maouene, 2009; Wellsby & Pexman, 2014).
Emotion,(embodied(cognition,(and(Artificial(Intelligence((
Marina&Korsakova-Kreyn&
3
Currently, the biologically-determined basic emotions are explained as a set of seven
primary emotional systems (Panksepp & Biven, 2012), each with its well-defined survival value.
The systems are: FEAR, ANGER, SEEKING/LEARNING, LUST, CARE/NURTURE, PANIC,
and PLAY. These systems activate through interaction with the environment. On top of the
basic emotions, neurologically intact humans learn the code of human behaviors, which is
also conditioned by emotions. This learning begins in infancy. The ‘extended brain’ of caring
adults provides a baby with the necessary training to enter human society. In the absence
of human caregivers, a feral child misses the critical window of opportunity (Robson, 2002)
for normal human development. The matrix of human society provides the framework and
environment for the emergence and development of our mental lives. Overall, affective
consciousness is molded by the social matrix, personal experience, memories, and
psychological makeup (even small children differ in temperament). As a result, members of
human societies exhibit a tremendous range of personality differences.
We can trace the origin of emotions to the binary response of a simple organism, such
as paramecium, as it moves toward positively-valenced information (nutrients) and away from
negatively-valenced information (toxicity). To highlight the principal feature of living things,
Pearl (1988) and Friston (2010) introduced the concept of Markov’s blanket to the cognitive
sciences. This concept represents the boundaries of a biological system in a statistical
sense (Kirchhof et al., 2018). These boundaries can be explained as ‘membranes’ that maintain
the integrity of a living organism in its fight against entropy. The ‘membrane’ allows
the organism to receive information from the environment and to initiate, when necessary,
energy-consuming protective action (Free Energy Principle, Friston, 2010). As compared to
positively-valenced information, negatively-valenced information is prioritized and receives
greater resources, at all levels (negativity bias, Ito et al., 1998). Negative information bad
news, loud or dissonant sound, or a dangerous situation generally elicits physical tension, thus
showing the intimate relationship between emotions and the motor system.
Curiously, perceived tension is the main morphological principle in music (Korsakova-
Kreyn, 2014, 2018), an artform dubbed the “universal language of emotions.” In music, we
sense degrees of tonal tension in consonant versus dissonant sound and in stable versus
unstable tones. By using melodic triggers that differ in perceived tonal tension, music conveys
virtual emotions in progress, over time. Perceived tonal tension connects to the subtle responses
of the motor system, which means that studying music perception and cognition can
further our understanding of the relationship between emotion, consciousness, and the motor
system.
In discussing consciousness, it is helpful to mention two neurobiological models: the
triune brain model of Paul McLean (1952, 1990) and the model of principal functional units
of the brain by Aleksander Luria (1973). Both models approach human consciousness from
Emotion,(embodied(cognition,(and(Artificial(Intelligence((
Marina&Korsakova-Kreyn&
4
an evolutionary perspective. The McLean’s model delineates the brainstem and limbic system
as crucially important primeval structures supporting instinctual responses and affective
consciousness, whereas the highest (tertiary) level of consciousness grants humans the ability
to think about thought. According to Luria’s theory of the dynamic organization of higher
mental functions, advanced (youngest) areas of the brain engage in multimodal integration.
The higher the brain damage in the evolutionary hierarchy, the more specific and bizarre the
disorders of perception (Koziol, Budding, & Chidekel, 2011). While lesions to the neocortex do
not erase consciousness (Merker, 2007), lesions to the emotion-supporting subcortical
structures produce a collapse of mental activity (Panksepp, 1998b; Watt & Pincus, 2004).
The deep structures of the brain, which support our instinctual affective responsiveness, are
critical for maintaining our consciousness.
In conclusion:
The creation of AI on par with human intelligence necessitates research in affective neuro-
science. Human consciousness cannot be separated from emotions. Moreover, human
consciousness is embodied and intimately connected to the motor system. It remains to be seen
whether forthcoming models of AI can emulate the workings of the human limbic system to
approach the source of emotional consciousness defining human relationships and creativity.
Bibliography:
Aziz-Zadeh, L., Wilson, S. M., Rizzolatti, G., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Congruent embodied
representations for visually presented actions and linguistic phrases describing actions.
Current Biology 16 (18), 1818-1823
Aziz-Zadeh L., Ivry R.B. (2009) The human mirror neuron system and embodied repre
sentations. In: Sternad D. (eds) Progress in Motor Control. Advances in Experimental
Medicine and Biology, vol. 629. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-
387-77064-2_18
Damasio, A. (2010). The Self comes to mind. Constructing the conscious brain. New York,
NY: Pantheon
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York,
NY: Penguin Publishing Group
Damasio, A.R. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the
prefrontal cortex, Philosophical Transaction: Biological Sciences, 351, 14131420
Edelman, G. M. (1992). Bright air, brilliant fire: On the matter of the mind. New York, NY:
Basic Books
Emotion,(embodied(cognition,(and(Artificial(Intelligence((
Marina&Korsakova-Kreyn&
5
Foglia, L. & Wilson, R. A. (2013). Embodied cognition. WIREs Cognitive Science, doi:
10.1002/wcs.1226
Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11
(2), 127138. https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2787.
Glenberg, A. M. & Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic
Bulletin & Review, 9, 558565
Hostetter, A.B. & Alibali, M.W. (2008). Visible embodiment: gestures as simulated action.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15 (3), 495-514
Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Smith, N. K., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs
more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 75 (4), 887-900
James, K. H. & Maouene, J. (2009). Auditory verb perception recruits motor systems in the
developing brain: an fMRI investigation. Developmental Science, 12, F26F34
Kirchhof, M., Parr, T., Palacios, E., Friston, K., & Kiverstein, J. (2018). The Markov blankets
of life: autonomy, active inference and the free energy principle. Journal of the Royal
Society. Interface. 15(138). DOI: 10.1098/rsif.2017.0792
Koziol, L. F., Budding, D. & Chidekel, D. (2011). Sensory integration, sensory processing,
and sensory modulation disorders: Putative functional neuroanatomic underpinnings, The
Cerebellum 10(4), 770-92. DOI: 10.1007/s12311-011-0288-8
Korsakova-Kreyn, M. (2018). Two-level model of embodied cognition in music. Psychomusicology:
Music, Mind, and Brain, 28(4), 240-259. DOI: 10.1037/pmu0000228
Korsakova-Kreyn, M. (2019). Language of music and its psychophysical foundations. Mod-
ern technologies in Medicine. 11 (1), 40-45. DOI: http://doi.org/10.17691/stm2019.11.1.
Lakof, G. & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to
Western thought. New York, NY: Basic Books
Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain: An introduction to neuropsychology. New York,
NY: Basic Books
MacLean, M.D. (1952). Some psychiatric implications of physiological studies on frontotemporal portion
of limbic system (visceral brain). Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 4 (4),
407418
MacLean, P.D. (1990). The triune brain in evolution. Role in paleocerebral functions. New
Emotion,(embodied(cognition,(and(Artificial(Intelligence((
Marina&Korsakova-Kreyn&
6
York, NY: Plenum Press
Merker, B. (2007). Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: A challenge for neuroscience
and medicine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 6381. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07000891
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1945/1962). Phenomenology of perception. Trans. C. Smith. London,
UK: Routledge and Kegan Paul
Panksepp, J. (1998a). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New
York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Panksepp, J. (1998b). The periconscious substrates of consciousness: affective states and the
evolutionary origins of the SELF. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 5, 566582
Panksepp, J., and Biven, L. (2012). The archaeology of mind: Neuroevolutionary origins of
human emotion. New York, N Y: W. W. Norton & Company
Panksepp, J. (2005). Affective consciousness: core emotional feelings in animals and hu-
mans. Consciousness and Cognition. 14, 3080. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2004.10.004
Pearl, J. (1988). Probabilistic reasoning in intelligent systems: Networks of plausible inference. San
Francisco: Morgan Kaufmann
Robson, A. L. (2002). Critical/sensitive periods. Child Development. Ed. Neil J. Salkind.
New York: Macmillan USA
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2008). The evolutionary psychology of the emotions and their
relationship to internal regulatory variables. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F.
Barrett (Eds.), 3rd edition. Handbook of emotions. New York, NY: Guilford
Varela, F.J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind. Cognitive science and
human experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Watt, D. F., & Pincus, D. I. (2004). Neural substrates of consciousness: implications for
clinical psychiatry. In Textbook of Psychiatry, ed. J. Panksepp (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley),
75110
Wellsby, M. & Pexman, P.M. (2014). Developing embodied cognition: Insights from chil-
dren’s concepts and language processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1-10
Zhang, S., Sharif, S. M. S., Chen, Y-C., et al. (2016). Clinical features for diagnosis and management of
patients with PRDM12 congenital insensitivity to pain. Journal of Medical Genetics, 53(8). DOI:
10.1136/jmedgenet-2015-103646
Emotion,(embodied(cognition,(and(Artificial(Intelligence((
Marina&Korsakova-Kreyn&
7
Academia Letters, August 2021
Corresponding Author: Marina Korsakova-Kreyn, mnkors@gmail.com
Citation: Korsakova-Kreyn, M. (2021). Emotion, embodied cognition, and Artificial Intelligence.
Academia Letters, Article 2883. https://doi.org/10.20935/AL2883.
©2021 by the author Open Access Distributed under CC BY 4.0
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Application of music in rehabilitation medicine requires an understanding of the mechanisms of music perception. This article discusses differences in character of the basic melodic elements and addresses some of the specifics of tonal space. Colloquially, music is called the language of emotion. Bearing in mind that high-level cognitive functioning in humans is inseparable from affective consciousness, investigating the mechanisms of emotional processing belongs to an important area of cognitive sciences. Studying music perception can help to advance methods of music therapy and to elucidate major aspects of human consciousness.
Article
Full-text available
In this paper, I propose that embodied cognition in music has two distinct levels. The “surface” level relates to the apparent corporeal articulation such as the activated psychomotor program of a music performer, visible gestures in response to music, and rhythmic entrainment. The primary (though concealed) “deep” level of embodied cognition relates to the main coding aspects in music: the tonal relationships arranged in time. Music is made of combinations of a small number of basic melodic intervals that differ by their psychophysical characteristics, among which the level of tonal stability and consonant-dissonant dichotomy are the most important for the formation of tonal expectations that guide music perception; tonal expectations are at the heart of melodic intentionality and musical motion. The tonal/temporal relationships encode musical content that dictates the motor behavior of music performers. The proposed two-level model of embodied cognition connects core musicology with the data from studies in music perception and cognition as well as studies in affective neuroscience and musicianship-related brain plasticity. The paper identifies the need for collaboration among various subdisciplines in musicology and cognitive sciences in order to further the development of the nascent field of embodied cognition in music. The presented discourse relies on research in the tonal music of European tradition and it does not address either aleatoric music or the exotic musics of non-Western traditions. To make the proposed model of embodied cognition in music available for nonmusicians, the paper includes the basics of music theory.
Article
Full-text available
This work addresses the autonomous organization of biological systems. It does so by considering the boundaries of biological systems, from individual cells to Home sapiens, in terms of the presence of Markov blankets under the active inference scheme-a corollary of the free energy principle. A Markov blanket defines the boundaries of a system in a statistical sense. Here we consider how a collective of Markov blankets can self-assemble into a global system that itself has a Markov blanket; thereby providing an illustration of how autonomous systems can be understood as having layers of nested and self-sustaining boundaries. This allows us to show that: (i) any living system is a Markov blanketed system and (ii) the boundaries of such systems need not be co-extensive with the biophysical boundaries of a living organism. In other words, autonomous systems are hierarchically composed of Markov blankets of Markov blankets-all the way down to individual cells, all the way up to you and me, and all the way out to include elements of the local environment.
Article
Full-text available
Background Congenital insensitivity to pain (CIP) is a rare extreme phenotype characterised by an inability to perceive pain present from birth due to lack of, or malfunction of, nociceptors. PRDM12 has recently been identified as a new gene that can cause CIP. The full phenotype and natural history have not yet been reported. Methods We have ascertained five adult patients and report their clinical features. Results Based on our findings, and those of previous patients, we describe the natural history of the PRDM12-CIP disorder, and derive diagnostic and management features to guide the clinical management of patients. Conclusions PRDM12-CIP is a distinct and diagnosable disorder, and requires specific clinical management to minimise predictable complications.
Article
Full-text available
In this paper, I propose that embodied cognition in music has two distinct levels: 1) the apparent corporeal articulation of music by performers and listeners, which reflects either a desire to make visible their emotional responses to the music or rhythmic entrainment, and 2) the principal (though concealed) level of transient muscular reactions to the main coding aspects in music: the tonal relationships arranged in time. This paper argues that the apparent corporeal articulation with regard to the entrainment effect and dance (Leman & Maes, 2014) is more related to the multimodal integration that is characteristic of attending to such a multidisciplinary performing art as opera and ballet than to purely musical content. I also present empirical data on the perception of tonal distances (Korsakova-Kreyn & Dowling, 2014) and suggest an explanation of why listeners' intuitive navigation in tonal and temporal space lies at the heart of emotional responses to music, including corporeal articulation. In addition, the paper touches on the research into temporality in music, such as memory constraints in the perception of tonal structures (Tillmann & Bigand, 2004). The main emphasis of this paper is on the principal two dimensions of music: tonal relationships and time. Understanding the primacy of these dimensions is important for defining music cognition and music in general. The paper also identifies the need for collaboration among various subdisciplines in musicology and the cognitive sciences so as to further the development of the nascent field of embodied cognition in music. Expressive distinctions are easily encoded by the listeners through the verbal labels, but they are practically untranslatable by bodily mediation, when body expression is induced by the musical stimulus. Frances and Bruchon-Schweitzer (1983) The concept of embodied cognition is based on the understanding that our emotions, memory, speech, and imagination are inseparable from the experiences of our bodies. To say it differently, a mind is shaped by the motor and somatosensory experience of the body that houses that mind. Music, a very special form of communication between humans, illustrates two levels of embodied cognition. The first, " surface " level is the influence of visible bodily movement on music perception and cognition. The second, " deep " level deals with melodic morphology. Our minds read melodic information by comparing differences in the perceived tonal stability of melodic elements. The sense of stability is directly related to a physical sensation of perceived tension, which means that melodic morphology is based on a highly primitive principle of perception that involves changes in somatic tension (Radchenko et al., 2015)—changes that most likely include transient actions of the musculature in response to tonal and temporal patterns. This is why tonal music presents what is probably the most obvious and holistic example of embodied cognition. SPEECH AND EMBODIED COGNITION The theory of embodied cognition postulates that sensory information and motor activity are essential for understanding the surrounding world and for developing the abilities that are important for abstract reasoning (Foglia & Wilson, 2013). Because both memory and speech include sensorimotor representations, our imagination relies on previously experienced gestures and movements (Wellsby &
Article
Application of music in rehabilitation medicine requires an understanding of the mechanisms of music perception. This article discusses the origin of differences in the character of the basic melodic elements and addresses some of the specifics of tonal space. Colloquially, music is called the language of emotion. Bearing in mind that high-level cognitive functioning in humans is inseparable from affective consciousness, investigating the mechanisms of emotional processing makes an important area of cognitive sciences. Studying music perception can help to advance methods of music therapy and to elucidate major aspects of human consciousness.
Book
A new edition of a classic work that originated the “embodied cognition” movement and was one of the first to link science and Buddhist practices. This classic book, first published in 1991, was one of the first to propose the “embodied cognition” approach in cognitive science. It pioneered the connections between phenomenology and science and between Buddhist practices and science—claims that have since become highly influential. Through this cross-fertilization of disparate fields of study, The Embodied Mind introduced a new form of cognitive science called “enaction,” in which both the environment and first person experience are aspects of embodiment. However, enactive embodiment is not the grasping of an independent, outside world by a brain, a mind, or a self; rather it is the bringing forth of an interdependent world in and through embodied action. Although enacted cognition lacks an absolute foundation, the book shows how that does not lead to either experiential or philosophical nihilism. Above all, the book's arguments were powered by the conviction that the sciences of mind must encompass lived human experience and the possibilities for transformation inherent in human experience. This revised edition includes substantive introductions by Evan Thompson and Eleanor Rosch that clarify central arguments of the work and discuss and evaluate subsequent research that has expanded on the themes of the book, including the renewed theoretical and practical interest in Buddhism and mindfulness. A preface by Jon Kabat-Zinn, the originator of the mindfulness-based stress reduction program, contextualizes the book and describes its influence on his life and work.