ChapterPDF Available

Erotic Target Identity Inversions

Authors:

Abstract

Sexual orientation is conventionally understood as relative attraction to men versus women. It has recently been argued that male sexual orientation in particular can be extended to include other dimensions of sexual attraction besides gender. One such dimension is sexual maturity, or relative attraction to children versus adults. A less familiar dimension is location, or relative attraction to other individuals versus sexual arousal by the fantasy of being one of those individuals. Erotic target identity inversions (ETIIs) refer to some men’s sexual arousal by the fantasy of being the same kinds of individuals to whom they are sexually attracted. Thus, ETIIs reflect the movement from external attraction to internal attraction on the dimension of location. ETIIs can motivate men to change their appearance and behavior to become more like the individuals to whom they are sexually attracted. ETIIs also provide a compelling theoretical explanation for otherwise puzzling phenomena, such as cross-dressing among heterosexual men, desire for limb amputation, and the furry phenomenon. Despite its scientific and clinical value, the concept of ETIIs has been underappreciated and understudied. This chapter reviews the ETIIs that have been previously identified in the literature, addresses important issues related to ETIIs, discusses the causes and development of ETIIs, and proposes future directions for research.KeywordsErotic target identity inversionAutogynephiliaApotemnophiliaAutopedophiliaParaphiliaSexual orientation
Chapter 20
Erotic Target Identity Inversions
Kevin J. Hsu and J. Michael Bailey
Abstract Sexual orientation is conventionally understood as relative attraction to
men versus women. It has recently been argued that male sexual orientation in
particular can be extended to include other dimensions of sexual attraction besides
gender. One such dimension is sexual maturity, or relative attraction to children
versus adults. A less familiar dimension is location, or relative attraction to other
individuals versus sexual arousal by the fantasy of being one of those individuals.
Erotic target identity inversions (ETIIs) refer to some mens sexual arousal by the
fantasy of being the same kinds of individuals to whom they are sexually attracted.
Thus, ETIIs reect the movement from external attraction to internal attraction on
the dimension of location. ETIIs can motivate men to change their appearance and
behavior to become more like the individuals to whom they are sexually attracted.
ETIIs also provide a compelling theoretical explanation for otherwise puzzling
phenomena, such as cross-dressing among heterosexual men, desire for limb ampu-
tation, and the furry phenomenon. Despite its scientic and clinical value, the
concept of ETIIs has been underappreciated and understudied. This chapter reviews
the ETIIs that have been previously identied in the literature, addresses important
issues related to ETIIs, discusses the causes and development of ETIIs, and proposes
future directions for research.
Keywords Erotic target identity inversion · Autogynephilia · Apotemnophilia ·
Autopedophilia · Paraphilia · Sexual orientation
K. J. Hsu (*)
Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA
Department of Psychological and Social Sciences, Pennsylvania State University, Abington,
Abington, PA, USA
e-mail: khsu@psu.edu
J. M. Bailey
Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA
©The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022
D. P. VanderLaan, W. I. Wong (eds.), Gender and Sexuality Development, Focus on
Sexuality Research, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-84273-4_20
589
Most people understand sexual orientation to mean relative attraction to men versus
women. Some researchers (Blanchard, 2015; Hsu & Bailey, 2017; Seto, 2017) have
recently argued that the meaning of sexual orientation can be extended to include
other dimensions of sexual attraction besides gender, at least in men. For example,
men vary in the extent to which they are attracted to sexually mature adults
(teleiophilia) compared with prepubescent or pubescent children (pedohebephilia)
(Bailey et al., 2016; Seto, 2012,2017). It has been argued that important aspects of
this dimension of sexual attraction––early onset, strong arousal by some but not all
gender-age categories, and stability across time––are identical to the reasons why we
refer to relative attraction to men versus women as sexual orientation.
In addition to gender and sexual maturity, another dimension of mens sexual
attraction may comprise sexual orientation. This dimension is likely to be less
familiar and more puzzling: the degree to which men are sexually attracted to
other individuals or instead sexually aroused by the fantasy of being one of those
individuals (Blanchard, 1991; Freund & Blanchard, 1993; Lawrence, 2009b).
Although most men are sexually attracted to other individuals (e.g., women) and
thus have external erotic targets, some men are sexually aroused by the fantasy that
they are one of those individuals and thus locate an erotic target within their own
body. In the latter case, their sexuality comprises an erotic target identity inversion
(ETII; Freund & Blanchard, 1993).
Men with ETIIs vary in the extent to which they retain attraction to other
individuals (Blanchard, 1989b,1992). Indeed, some men with ETIIs are
analloerotic, or exclusively sexually aroused by fantasizing about themselves as
another individual. Because they do not experience sexual attraction to others,
analloerotic men with ETIIs may report that they are asexual (Bogaert, 2012; Brotto
& Yule, 2017). Those unfamiliar with ETIIs may also assume that they lack any kind
of sexual attraction at all. Among men with ETIIs who retain sexual attraction to
others, the external and internal erotic targets are similar. For example, men sexually
aroused by the fantasy of being a woman are sexually attracted to actual women, if
they are attracted to other individuals at all. Thus, ETIIs reect the extent to which
mens sexual attraction is external versus internal on a dimension that may be
considered an erotic targets location (Blanchard, 2015; Hsu & Bailey, 2017).
ETIIs can motivate men to change their appearance and behavior to become more
like their internalized erotic target (Bailey, 2003; Blanchard, 1989a,1991; Freund &
Blanchard, 1993; Lawrence, 2006,2009b,2013). For example, men sexually
aroused by the fantasy of being a woman are often motivated to cross-dress and to
act like women (Bailey, 2003; Blanchard, 1991; Blanchard et al., 1986; Brown et al.,
2020; Hsu et al., 2015; Hsu et al., 2017; Lawrence, 2013). Some men are so strongly
motivated by ETIIs that they seek expensive and irreversible medical procedures to
make their bodies more like their internalized erotic target. For example, some natal
males sexually aroused by the fantasy of being a woman are motivated to seek
gender conrmation surgery in order to make their bodies more like those of women
(Bailey, 2003; Blanchard, 1991; Freund & Blanchard, 1993; Lawrence, 2009b,
2013). Recently in Western countries, most natal males who have completed gender
conrmation surgery appear to be motivated by ETIIs (Lawrence, 2010a).
590 K. J. Hsu and J. M. Bailey
In the following sections, we review the ETIIs that have received scienticor
clinical attention thus far. We then address ETIIs as paraphilias, ETIIs in natal
females, and sexual masochism as an alternative explanation to ETIIs. Finally, we
discuss the causes and development of ETIIs and propose future directions for
research. In our proposal for future directions, we introduce a few more potential
ETIIs for consideration.
20.1 Autogynephilia
Autogynephilia is a natal males propensity to be sexually aroused by the thought or
image of being a woman (Blanchard, 1989a), and it is the best studied example of a
possible ETII (Blanchard, 2005; Lawrence, 2013). Autogynephilia occurs in natal
males who are sexually attracted to women (Blanchard, 1991,1993a; Freund &
Blanchard, 1993; Lawrence, 2007). However, autogynephilic malesattraction to
women has been internalized to some degree, such that they are also sexually
aroused by the fantasy of being a woman. Although autogynephilia usually coexists
with sexual attraction to women, sometimes the internalization is so complete that
the autogynephilic male does not experience sexual attraction to women. Because
women are the preferred erotic targets of most men, autogynephilia is likely to be the
most common form of ETII (Blanchard, 1993b).
Most commonly, autogynephilia manifests as erotic cross-dressing, usually by
adolescence (Bailey, 2003; Blanchard, 1991; Blanchard et al., 1986; Brown et al.,
2020; Hsu et al., 2015,2017; Lawrence, 2013). The specic erotic interest in cross-
dressing is called transvestic fetishism (sometimes shortened to transvestism). Cross-
dressing is a relatively easy, impermanent, and inexpensive way for a man to make
himself look more like a woman. Thus, autogynephilic males often cross-dress to
satisfy their sexual fantasy of being a woman. Some nd it erotic to fantasize about
having female-typical anatomy like breasts or a vagina. These anatomically
autogynephilic males are more likely to experience gender dysphoria, or recurrent
and intense feelings of discontent with being male (Blanchard, 1993a,1993c). For
some autogynephilic males, gender dysphoria and motivation to become a woman
are so strong that they pursue gender conrmation surgery (Blanchard, 1991;
Lawrence, 2004). The rst row of Table 20.1 lists autogynephilia and its related
phenomena as they pertain to the concept of ETIIs.
Studies that have examined the co-occurrence of autogynephilia and sexual
attraction to women among gender dysphoric natal males or male-to-female trans-
gender women provide indirect evidence supporting the idea that autogynephilia is
an ETII. These studies have converged on the general nding that gender dysphoric
natal males (Blanchard, 1985,1989b,1992; Blanchard et al., 1987; Freund et al.,
1982; Zucker et al., 2012) and transgender women (Lawrence, 2005; Nuttbrock
et al., 2011; Smith et al., 2005; Veale et al., 2008) who are sexually attracted to
women report higher levels of autogynephilia or erotic cross-dressing, compared
with those who are exclusively sexually attracted to men. Conversely, men with
20 Erotic Target Identity Inversions 591
Table 20.1 Sexual attraction to erotic targets, erotic target identity inversions, and their related phenomena in men
Erotic targets
Sexual attraction to erotic
targets Erotic target identity inversion
Manifestation of erotic target identity
inversion
Related identity
dysphoria
Women Gynephilia Autogynephilia Cross-dressing (Transvestism) Gender dysphoria
Amputees Acrotomophilia Apotemnophilia Pretending Body integrity
dysphoria
Children Pedohebephilia Autopedophilia Dressing in childrens clothing
(Pedovestism)
Age dysphoria
Anthropomorphic
animals
Anthropomorphozoophilia Autoanthropomorphozoophilia Fursuiting Species dysphoria
Real animals Zoophilia Autozoophilia Unknown Species dysphoria
Men Androphilia Autoandrophilia Homeovestism Masculinity dysphoria
Corpses Necrophilia Autonecrophilia Unknown Unknown
592 K. J. Hsu and J. M. Bailey
transvestic fetishism overwhelmingly report at least some attraction to women, with
the majority identifying as heterosexual (Docter & Fleming, 2001; Docter & Prince,
1997; Långström & Zucker, 2005; Zucker et al., 2012).
An online study of 149 men who endorsed items explicitly assessing
autogynephilia found that 81% identied as heterosexual and 15% as bisexual,
further suggesting that this population is near exclusively comprised of men sexually
attracted to women (Hsu et al., 2015). A more recent online study, using a much
larger sample of 522 men who endorsed an item assessing autogynephilia, found that
63% identied as heterosexual and 30% as bisexual (Brown et al., 2020). Such
ndings would be predicted if autogynephilia is an ETII involving the internalization
of sexual attraction to women. Although Brown et al.s(2020) study required that
autogynephilic men also reported sexual arousal by the thought of having sex with a
woman, it did not appear to have inated the combined prevalence of heterosexual
and bisexual identities, which was similar to that in Hsu et al.s(2015) study.
Consistent with the concept of ETIIs, gender dysphoric natal males and trans-
gender women sexually attracted to women tend to report much more autogynephilia
than do those not attracted to women (e.g., Blanchard, 1992). There is also a strong
tendency for autogynephilic men to report sexual attraction to women (e.g., Hsu
et al., 2015). Because men have such a high base rate of sexual attraction to women,
these ndings are neither entirely surprising nor strong evidence for the concept of
ETIIs. Indeed, some transgender women insist that they are lesbians trapped in a
mans bodyand vehemently oppose the idea that their sexual attraction to women is
related to autogynephilia (Dreger, 2008). If these individuals admit to experiencing
autogynephilia, they argue that it is an expression of their latent feelings of being
female, rather than an ETII. There are other, less common but more perplexing
phenomena that provide more persuasive evidence for the concept of ETIIs. We
review the most compelling example next.
20.2 Apotemnophilia
Lawrence (2006) argued that men who desire limb amputation are motivated by an
ETII. Their desire is, according to Lawrence, analogous to some autogynephilic
mens desire for gender conrmation surgery. For men who desire limb amputation,
the motivation is apotemnophilia, or sexual arousal by the thought or image of being
an amputee (Money et al., 1977), which can be conceptualized as the ETII that
occurs in men with acrotomophilia, or sexual attraction to amputees (Money, 1986).
In this case, their attraction to amputees has been internalized to some degree, such
that they are sexually aroused by the fantasy of being an amputee.
In a study conducted by First (2005), 67% of 52 participants who desired limb
amputation reported sexual arousal from the fantasy of being an amputee, and 87%
reported sexual attraction to amputees. More recent surveys of primarily men who
desire limb amputation have also found large percentages reporting sexual arousal
from the fantasy of being an amputee and sexual attraction to amputees (Blom et al.,
20 Erotic Target Identity Inversions 593
2012,2017; Pregartbauer et al., 2014). Thus, following the concept of ETIIs, men
who desire limb amputation tend to report both apotemnophilia and acrotomophilia.
The tendency for apotemnophilic men to be especially attracted to amputees cannot
plausibly be explained by high base rates, in contrast to the tendency for
autogynephilic males to be especially attracted to women.
There are other parallels between those with apotemnophilia and those with
autogynephilia (Lawrence, 2006). For example, in Firsts(2005) study, 92% of
participants reported that they engaged in pretending, which is temporarily
presenting as an amputee through prosthetics or other means. Pretending may
involve binding or otherwise concealing a limb, and using crutches or a wheelchair
to impersonate an amputee. Similar to cross-dressing in autogynephilic males,
pretending is a relatively easy, impermanent, and inexpensive way for
apotemnophilic men to look more like amputees and to thus satisfy their sexual
fantasy of being an amputee. Some apotemnophilic men also experience body
integrity dysphoria,
1
or distress over having an intact body, which is often accom-
panied by a desire for limb amputation. Among participants in Firsts study, 17%
successfully had a limb amputated. Similarly, some autogynephilic males experience
gender dysphoria and the desire for gender conrmation surgery. Taken together, the
parallels between apotemnophilia and autogynephilia provide evidence that both
belong to the same dimension of sexual orientation, one best explained by the
concept of ETIIs. The second row of Table 20.1 lists apotemnophilia and its related
phenomena as they pertain to the concept of ETIIs.
Most of the participants in Firsts(2005) study were heterosexual men. The
concept of ETIIs raises the question of whether apotemnophilic men sexually
attracted to female amputees are also likely to have autogynephilia (Lawrence,
2006). After all, their preferred erotic targets are women as well as amputees. First
found elevated rates of various manifestations of autogynephilia in his sample,
including cross-dressing (29%), transvestic fetishism (15%), and desire for gender
conrmation surgery (12%). The rate of transvestic fetishism in Firsts sample is ve
times the rate of only 3% reported in the general male population by Långström and
Zucker (2005).
20.3 Autopedophilia
Until recently, no other putative ETII besides autogynephilia and apotemnophilia
had received systematic empirical study. Freund and Blanchard (1993) provided at
least one good lead by presenting several cases of pedohebephilic men who were
sexually aroused by fantasizing about being a child or dressing in childrens cloth-
ing. These men appear to have been motivated by an ETII involving sexual arousal
1
The condition has also been called body integrity identity disorder (First, 2005), but we refer to it
here as body integrity dysphoria to be consistent with gender dysphoria.
594 K. J. Hsu and J. M. Bailey
by the thought or image of being a child, which Lawrence (2006,2009b) has called
autopedophilia.
2
In this case, pedohebephilic mens attraction to children has been
internalized to some degree, such that they are sexually aroused by the fantasy of
being a child.
Following the seminal publication by Freund and Blanchard (1993) and two other
case reports on autopedophilia (Dickey, 2007; Howitt, 1995), Hsu and Bailey (2017)
examined whether autopedophilia exists and can be conceptualized as an ETII in a
non-forensic, non-clinical sample of pedohebephilic men recruited anonymously
from the Internet. They found that a greater percentage of 475 pedohebephilic
participants reported some degree of autopedophilia (49%) than some degree of
autogynephilia (32%). Furthermore, pedohebephilic participants were more
intensely autopedophilic than they were autogynephilic, d¼1.50, based on contin-
uous measures that assessed both autopedophilia and autogynephilia. These results
were consistent with the concept of ETIIs: Because pedohebephilic men are prefer-
entially attracted to children rather than to women, they should be more
autopedophilic in prevalence and degree than autogynephilic, despite the fact that
autogynephilia is likely the more common ETII in general. Like the tendency for
apotemnophilic men to be especially attracted to amputees, the tendency for
pedohebephilic men to be especially autopedophilic cannot be explained by high
base rates.
Hsu and Bailey (2017) presented additional results that suggest parallels between
autopedophilia and autogynephilia. For example, the degree of autopedophilia was
related to the frequency of dressing in childrens clothing in their sample, and 13% of
their autopedophilic participants reported that they had dressed in childrens clothing
as an adult. The specic erotic interest in wearing childrens clothing is called
pedovestism (Lawrence, 2006,2009b). Dressing in childrens clothing is a relatively
easy, impermanent, and inexpensive way for autopedophilic men to look more like
children, and it appears analogous to both cross-dressing in autogynephilic males
and pretending in apotemnophilic men. In addition, just as some autogynephilic
males experience gender dysphoria and the desire for gender conrmation surgery,
and some apotemnophilic men experience body integrity dysphoria and the desire
for limb amputation, some autopedophilic men experience age dysphoria (distress
over being an adult) and the desire to physically become a child (Freund &
Blanchard, 1993; Hsu & Bailey, 2017). Although the desire to physically become
a child cannot be actualized, Freund and Blanchard described a pedohebephilic man
who wished to have his foreskin reconstructed to look like a young boys. The
parallels between autopedophilia and autogynephilia, in addition to those between
2
The term autopedohebephilia would be more accurate, but it is cumbersome and inconsistent with
existing terminology. Thus, we use the term autopedophilia to mean sexual arousal by the thought
or image of being either a prepubescent or a pubescent child.
20 Erotic Target Identity Inversions 595
both phenomena and apotemnophilia, suggest that all three share a common expla-
nation, and the concept of ETIIs is the best candidate. The third row of Table 20.1
lists autopedophilia and its related phenomena as they pertain to the concept of
ETIIs.
Consistent with the concept of ETIIs, autopedophilic men in the study by Hsu and
Bailey (2017) tended to be sexually aroused by the fantasy of being the same kinds
of children to whom they were sexually attracted, with respect to gender and age. For
instance, autopedophilic men sexually attracted to girls more commonly found it
sexually arousing to imagine themselves as a girl (58%), and autopedophilic men
sexually attracted to boys overwhelmingly found it sexually arousing to imagine
themselves as a boy (96%). In addition, sexual attraction to children of a particular
gender-age category (e.g., girls 410 years old) among autopedophilic men was most
strongly correlated with sexual arousal by the fantasy of being a child of the same
gender-age category.
The following narrative not only exemplies autopedophilia as an ETII, but also
three dimensions of male sexual orientation––gender (attraction to females), sexual
maturity (attraction to children), and location (attraction to the fantasy of being
another individual):
Sometimes I fantasize about being with a child myself, but then I end up thinking about it
from the childs perspective, being sexually active with an adult. Sometimes I fantasize
about spanking a child, and then I would think about being the child getting spanked. Other
times, I fantasize about being a little girl being sexually active with another little girl. (Hsu &
Bailey, 2017, p. 121).
20.4 Autoanthropomorphozoophilia
Freund and Blanchard (1993) presented a case report of a man who was sexually aroused
by imagining himself as an anthropomorphic dog named Puppy Smith. This man also
masturbated with plush animals that represented Puppy Smith and other anthropomor-
phic animals. Thus, this man appeared to have both sexual attraction to anthropomorphic
animals (anthropomorphozoophilia) and sexual arousal by the thought or image of being
an anthropomorphic animal (autoanthropomorphozoophilia)(Hsu&Bailey,2019).
In conceptualizing autoanthropomorphozoophilia as an ETII, mens attraction to
anthropomorphic animals has been internalized to some degree, such that they are
sexually aroused by the fantasy of being an anthropomorphic animal. Because plush
animals are often representations of anthropomorphic animals, Lawrence (2009b)
suggested that sexual attraction to plush animals might actually reect sexual
attraction to anthropomorphic animals.
Furries are individuals who are especially interested in anthropomorphic animals
(Gerbasi et al., 2008). Although furries have attracted the most media attention from their
large conventions where they socialize and share art, they tend to interact with each other
online rather than in person. For at least four reasons, furries are an increasingly visible
phenomenon relevant to the question of whether autoanthropomorphozoophilia is an
596 K. J. Hsu and J. M. Bailey
ETII. First, furries have been controversially portrayed as sexually motivated in the
media and popular culture (e.g., Gurley, 2001). Although their exact estimates differ,
recent surveys of furries have found that some degree of sexual motivation is not
uncommon, reported by at least 50% of their samples (Hsu & Bailey, 2019;Plante
et al., 2011;Schroyetal.,2016). Second, many furries identify with anthropomorphic
animals and create fursonas, versions of themselves as anthropomorphic animals
(Gerbasi et al., 2008). Third, some furries wear costumes resembling their fursonas,
which look similar to the costumes worn by mascots of athletic teams or theme parks.
This practice is called fursuiting, and it appears analogous to cross-dressing in
autogynephilic males and pretending in apotemnophilic men (Lawrence, 2009b).
Finally, some furries report species dysphoria, or distress over being a human (Gerbasi
et al., 2008). In their sample of 217 furries, Gerbasi et al. found that 24% indicated a
persistent feeling of discomfort with their human body, and 29% indicated a feeling that
they are a non-human species trapped in a human body.The species dysphoria
reported by some furries seems to parallel gender dysphoria in autogynephilic males,
and body integrity dysphoria in apotemnophilic men. Because furries tend to be sexually
motivated and change their appearance and behavior to become more like anthropomor-
phic animals, they may be especially likely to have ETIIs. The fourth row of Table 20.1
lists autoanthropomorphozoophilia and its related phenomena as they pertain to the
concept of ETIIs.
A recent study examined the extent to which 334 male furries were sexually
motivated, and if so, whether they were motivated by the ETII of
autoanthropomorphozoophilia (Hsu & Bailey, 2019). The vast majority (99%) of
the sample endorsed some degree of sexual motivation for being furries. As
predicted by the concept of ETIIs, more furry participants reported some degree of
sexual attraction to anthropomorphic animals (99%) than to real animals (47%) or
women (73%). Furry participants were also more likely to report some degree of
sexual arousal by the fantasy of being an anthropomorphic animal (93%) than by the
fantasy of being a real animal (44%) or a woman (37%), ds > 1.23. Furthermore,
autoanthropomorphozoophilic participants tended to be sexually aroused by the
fantasy of being the same kinds of anthropomorphic animals to whom they were
sexually attracted, with respect to gender and species. For instance, sexual attraction
to anthropomorphic animals of a particular species (e.g., wolves) was most strongly
correlated with sexual arousal by the fantasy of being an anthropomorphic animal of
the same species.
The following narrative exemplies autoanthropomorphozoophilia as an ETII
and also suggests its conuence with another ETII involving sexual arousal by the
fantasy of being fat:
It was the early nineties. Gareld was still cool, and I was very interested in both fat people
and the idea of being fat. I imagined that I was Gareld, a tubby cat, and I became aroused. I
liked the idea of being a cartoon cat that was fat and uffy. Later in my teenage years, I
would seek out other overweight cartoon animals and become aroused imagining that I was
them. (Hsu & Bailey, 2019, p. 1365).
20 Erotic Target Identity Inversions 597
20.5 Autozoophilia
There has been limited evidence for an ETII specic to men with zoophilia, or sexual
attraction to real animals as opposed to anthropomorphic animals. If there were such
an ETII, it would involve sexual arousal by the thought or image of being a real
animal, or autozoophilia (Lawrence, 2009b). In this case, zoophilic mens attraction
to real animals would be internalized to some degree, such that they are sexually
aroused by the fantasy of being a real animal.
In a study of 82 zoophilic men, Miletski (2002) found that 40% reported at least
some identication with animals of their own gender. Similarly, Williams and
Weinberg (2003) reported that some zoophilic men in their study believed that
they had animal characteristics or felt like animals. Beetz (2004) also described
zoophilic men with species dysphoria and the desire to become an animal, usually of
the same species to which they were sexually attracted. As previously mentioned
with autoanthropomorphozoophilia, species dysphoria and the desire to become an
animal appear analogous to gender dysphoria and the desire to become a woman in
autogynephilic males (Lawrence, 2009b). However, like the desire to become a child
among autopedophilic men, the desire to become an animal cannot be actualized.
Casavant (2005) reported the case of a man who identied as a tiger and had
extensive body modications in an attempt to achieve the physical appearance of
one. The fth row of Table 20.1 lists autozoophilia and its related phenomena as they
pertain to the concept of ETIIs.
It is not clear whether zoophilic men are manifesting autozoophilia when they identify
with animals or experience species dysphoria, because the relevant studies have not asked
about sexual arousal by the fantasy of being an animal. Although these studies have been
unable to shed light on whether autozoophilia can be conceptualized as an ETII, they have
provided some important groundwork for future inquiry. In Hsu and Baileys(2019)
recent study of male furries, for example, participants who were sexually aroused by the
fantasy of being a real animal tended to also be sexually attracted to animals, consistent
with autozoophilia as an ETII. In addition, Brown et al. (2020) collected data on
286 participants who reported sexual arousal by the fantasy of being an animal, which
was erroneously called autoanthropomorphozoophilia instead of autozoophilia. In this
sample, autozoophilia was associated with dressing or behaving as an animal, feeling that
life would be better as an animal, and considering body modications to look more like an
animal. Because participants were not asked about their sexual attraction to animals, it
could not be determined the extent to which this autozoophilic sample t with the concept
of ETIIs.
598 K. J. Hsu and J. M. Bailey
20.6 Autoandrophilia
Freund and Blanchard (1993) speculated that there could be an ETII specic to men
sexually attracted to other men. If this ETII exists, it would involve sexual arousal by
the thought or image of being another man, which has been called autoandrophilia
(Dickey & Stephens, 1995; Lawrence, 2006,2009b). In this case, mens attraction to
other men would be internalized to some degree, such that they are sexually aroused
by the fantasy of being another man.
No systematic research has examined the extent to which autoandrophilia exists
in men sexually attracted to other men, and whether autoandrophilia can be concep-
tualized as an ETII. A few case studies, however, support the idea that
autoandrophilia is an ETII. For instance, Zavitzianos (1972) described a gay man
who appeared to be sexually attracted to male athletes with large penises, and who
masturbated wearing a jockstrap to help him imagine that he had a large penis
himself. Zavitzianos (1977) later summarized two other cases of gay men with
possible autoandrophilia. In the rst case, a gay man reported sexual attraction to
men who wear boots and gabardine clothes, and he also reported masturbating in
front of a mirror while wearing boots and gabardine clothes himself. In the second
case, a gay man reported masturbating to the fantasy of being with an army man, as
well as masturbating to the fantasy of being an army man himself while wearing a
military uniform. Zavitzianos (1972) referred to the sexual interest in wearing
specic kinds of same-gender clothing (e.g., jockstrap, military uniform) as
homeovestism, and it seems analogous to transvestic fetishism in autogynephilic
males and pedovestism in autopedophilic men (Lawrence, 2006,2009b). Indeed, for
the men in these cases, their homeovestism was consistent with autoandrophilia.
Because these men masturbated wearing the same kinds of clothing worn by the men
they nd attractive, their autoandrophilia was consistent with the concept of ETIIs.
They have internalized their sexual attraction to these other men, resulting in the
sexual desire to become more like them.
It may be difcult to detect autoandrophilia in men, because autoandrophilic men
are sexually aroused by the fantasy of being another man but are already men
themselves. The cases of autoandrophilic men reported by Zavitzianos (1972,
1977) might have been easier to detect because those men were attracted to the
more salient or stereotypical features of masculinity in other men (e.g., being in the
army), which they then found arousing to fantasize about having themselves. Thus,
if an autoandrophilic man became (or was already) as masculine as the men to whom
he is attracted, it would mean that he has actualized his desire to become another
man, much like autogynephilic males and apotemnophilic men have actualized their
desires to become a woman or an amputee through gender conrmation surgery or
limb amputation, respectively. Lawrence (2006) suggested that some gay men who
engage in bodybuilding might be attempting to make their bodies more like those of
the more muscular and thus more masculine men to whom they are attracted.
20 Erotic Target Identity Inversions 599
However, in gay men, it would be difcult to differentiate between bodybuilding that
is motivated by an ETII such as autoandrophilia and bodybuilding that is motivated
by the desire to look more attractive to potential sexual partners.
Lawrence (2009a) described a man who was both extremely dissatised with his
own male body and desired to have a more muscular, masculine body like those of
the men he found sexually attractive. This mans most arousing sexual fantasy
involved having a muscular and attractive male body, being part of a group of
masculine heterosexual men who accept him as one of them, and masturbating
together with these other men. Consistent with conceptualizing his autoandrophilia
as an ETII, this man reported frequent sexual arousal by the fantasy of wearing the
bodies of other men and being masculine like them. In the absence of a term to
describe the distress that some autoandrophilic men experience from not being
muscular or masculine enough, we propose masculinity dysphoria, which would
be analogous to gender dysphoria in autogynephilic males. The sixth row of
Table 20.1 lists autoandrophilia and its related phenomena as they pertain to the
concept of ETIIs.
20.7 Autonecrophilia
Autonecrophilia refers to sexual arousal by the thought or image of being a corpse
(Aggrawal, 2009), and it might be conceptualized as the ETII that occurs in men
sexually attracted to corpses. In this case, their sexual attraction to corpses would be
internalized to some degree, such that they are sexually aroused by the fantasy of
being a corpse. Because it is likely to be extremely rare, autonecrophilia has not been
the subject of empirical research.
The infamous case of Dennis Rader (BTK) might be a vivid example of a
sexually sadistic serial killer with autonecrophilia. A large amount of evidence was
recovered after his arrest, including photographs that he took of his dead victims
placed in specic positions with their hands bound and with a cloth or mask covering
their faces (Knoll & Hazelwood, 2009). Photographs were also included that Rader
took of himself placed in specic positions with his hands bound and with a cloth or
mask covering his face, exactly as his dead victims were. He was cross-dressed in his
victimsclothing, and one photograph shows Rader lying on his back in a grave that
was meant for one of his victims. Based on this evidence, it seems that Rader
experienced necrophilia and autonecrophilia, and the similarity between the photo-
graphs of his victims and himself suggests that his autonecrophilia was an ETII. The
seventh row of Table 20.1 lists autonecrophilia and its related phenomena as they
pertain to the concept of ETIIs.
600 K. J. Hsu and J. M. Bailey
20.8 ETIIs as Paraphilias
Freund and Blanchard (1993) suggested that ETIIs may be considered paraphilias,
an ill-dened set of unusual sexual interests that include pedohebephilia,
acrotomophilia, zoophilia, exhibitionism, voyeurism, masochism, and sadism,
among others. Although paraphilias are not pathological by nature, they may
cause signicant distress or impairment in an individual, or they may cause an
individual to harm others. Such instances of paraphilias have been called paraphilic
disorders in the most recent edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders (fth ed. [DSM-5]; American Psychiatric Association, 2013).
Because they have not been clearly dened or understood, paraphilias have been
criticized as a scientically vacuous concept that is based on value judgments
regarding which sexual interests are more or less socially desirable (e.g., Moser &
Kleinplatz, 2006). Two important observations, however, support the scientic
validity of paraphilias as a concept with limited but promising support: Paraphilias
are much more commonly found in natal males than in natal females, and paraphilias
tend to co-occur within the same individual (Abel & Osborn, 1992; Baur et al., 2016;
Dawson et al., 2016; Joyal & Carpentier, 2017; Långström & Seto, 2006; Långström
& Zucker, 2005). Both of these general ndings suggest that classifying a sexual
interest as a paraphilia is more scientically meaningful than a mere value judgment.
Paraphilias have typically been classied as sexual interests in either
non-normative erotic targets (e.g., pedohebephilia, zoophilia) or non-normative
sexual activities (e.g., masochism, sadism) (Freund et al., 1996). ETIIs may repre-
sent another class of paraphilias that consist of sexual interests in an erotic targets
non-normative location within ones own body (e.g., autogynephilia), resulting in
sexual arousal by the fantasy of being the erotic target (Freund & Blanchard, 1993).
Consistent with their conceptualization as paraphilias, ETIIs have thus far been
observed almost exclusively in natal males rather than in natal females. For instance,
Firsts(2005) study included only four women out of 52 participants who desired
limb amputation. We defer further discussion about the existence of ETIIs in natal
females to the next section. Also consistent with their conceptualization as
paraphilias, ETIIs tend to co-occur, such as autogynephilia most notably with
apotemnophilia (29%; First, 2005; Lawrence, 2006), autopedophilia (r¼0.48;
Hsu & Bailey, 2017), and autoanthropomorphozoophilia (r¼0.20; Hsu & Bailey,
2019). A recent study found that men who endorsed any degree of autogynephilia
were approximately three times more likely to have endorsed any degree of
autopedophilia and autozoophilia, compared with men who did not endorse any
degree of autogynephilia (Brown et al., 2020). In addition, at least two other unusual
sexual interests that might be classied as paraphilias but not ETIIs have been shown to
co-occur with autogynephilia: masochism (Blanchard & Hucker, 1991; Hsu et al.,
2015;Långström&Zucker,2005;Lawrence,2006,2013) and gynandromorphophilia
(sexual interest in transgender women with a penis; Blanchard & Collins, 1993;Hsu
et al., 2016; Rosenthal et al., 2017).
20 Erotic Target Identity Inversions 601
20.9 ETIIs in Natal Females
Conditions resembling ETIIs have been rarely observed in natal females, compared
with natal males. Indeed, female paraphilias are not well studied, and it is unclear
whether paraphilias occur in natal females at all. Although supercial similarities
seem to exist between some female sexual interests and male paraphilias, especially
masochism, no evidence has convincingly shown that the same phenomena in natal
females reect paraphilias as in natal males. Evidence for female paraphilias would
require, at the very least, that they tend to co-occur in the same individual.
Because men are the preferred erotic targets of most women, autoandrophilia is
the most likely candidate for an ETII in natal females. In contrast to autogynephilia
in men, however, fewer cases of autoandrophilia in women have been reported.
Stoller (1982) reported on three women with transvestic fetishism, and several
population studies of paraphilias have found small numbers of women reporting
transvestic fetishism (Baur et al., 2016; Dawson et al., 2016; Joyal & Carpentier,
2017; Långström & Zucker, 2005). Despite these reports, it remains unclear whether
transvestic fetishism in women reects autoandrophilia and a paraphilia, much less
an ETII. Some women and female-to-male transgender men have reported both
sexual attraction to men and autoandrophilia (Coleman et al., 1993; Dickey &
Stephens, 1995;OKeefe, 2007; Stoller, 1982), consistent with the concept of
ETIIs. Notably, one recent study by Brown et al. (2020) reported on 328 women
who indicated sexual arousal by the fantasy of being a man, with a majority
identifying as heterosexual or bisexual (82%) and reporting sexual attraction to
men (84%). Although this pattern of sexual interests might appear to be consistent
with the concept of ETIIs, the authors also showed that women reported
autoandrophilia at a lower prevalence and intensity than men reported
autogynephilia.
More research is needed to clarify whether autoandrophilia in natal females is an
ETII. Autoandrophilia in natal females might instead be sexual arousal by the
specic fantasy of being a gay man and participating in gay male sex, rather than
internalization of sexual attraction to men. Bailey and Blanchard (2017) argued that
this kind of sexual arousal is responsible for a rare type of gender dysphoria in natal
females, called autohomoerotic gender dysphoria. In one of Stollers(1982) case
reports, a woman with transvestic fetishism described her autoandrophilia in this
manner:
Today my sex life is mostly satised by masturbation, with transvestite episodes occasion-
ally providing a pleasant stimulus to masturbation. Ive dressed as a man, replete with
moustache, and had my partner call me by a mans name. I take pleasure in being called by a
mans name. Dressed as a man, Ive sucked my partners penis. I felt myself, during the
experience, to be a gay male. (p. 103).
Besides autoandrophilia, no other putative ETII in natal females has received
much scientic or clinical attention. Firsts(2005) study included only four women
with the desire for limb amputation, and Blom et al.s(2012,2017) more recent
surveys included a total of 20 such women. Information about apotemnophilia
602 K. J. Hsu and J. M. Bailey
specic to these women was not presented, however. Money (1991) described a
woman who reported sexual attraction to amputees, pretending, and the desire for
limb amputation, suggesting an ETII presentation of apotemnophilia. In a study
attempting to examine autogynephilia in natal females, 93% of 29 women appeared
to report autogynephilia (Moser, 2009). Lawrence (2010b), however, raised serious
criticisms about its methodology and interpretation, especially the fact that many
items measured not autogynephilia but instead sexual arousal from the anticipation
of a romantic evening or sexual encounter.
Brown et al. (2020) recently studied ETIIs in natal female participants and found
that they were about half as likely as natal male participants to endorse more than one
of the four ETIIs assessed: autogynephilia (natal males), autoandrophilia (natal
females), autopedophilia, and autozoophilia. However, natal female participants
who endorsed one ETII were more than twice as likely to have endorsed another.
Both of these ndings further support the conceptualization of ETIIs as paraphilias,
which are much less common in natal females than in natal males and also tend to
co-occur within the same individual. We previously summarized two recent studies
that examined whether autopedophilia and autoanthropomorphozoophilia can be
conceptualized as ETIIs among men (Hsu & Bailey, 2017,2019). These studies
also included a very small minority of natal female participants, whose results will be
detailed in a future article and compared with those from Brown et al.s(2020) study.
20.10 Sexual Masochism as an Alternative to ETIIs
One plausible alternative to conceptualizing some phenomena as ETIIs is sexual
masochism (Freund & Blanchard, 1993). In particular, autogynephilic,
autopedophilic, and autozoophilic fantasies and behaviors can sometimes be viewed
as masochistic. Specically, the idea of becoming a woman, a child, or an animal
may be humiliating or submissive to some men, as may cross-dressing, dressing in
childrens clothing, or pretending to be an animal. Thus, sexual arousal in these types
of fantasies and behaviors may be related to masochism instead of an ETII or the
internalization of sexual attraction to women, children, or animals. In support of this
alternative, autogynephilia tends to co-occur with masochism (Blanchard & Hucker,
1991; Hsu et al., 2015; Långström & Zucker, 2005; Lawrence, 2006,2013), and
some forms of animal role-play (e.g., pony play, pup play) have been associated with
masochism (Aggrawal, 2011; Wignall & McCormack, 2017). Fantasies and behav-
iors motivated by sexual masochism exaggerate differences between men and their
preferred erotic targets (e.g., women), increasing their feelings of submissiveness. In
contrast, fantasies and behaviors motivated by an ETII increase similarity between
men and their preferred erotic targets.
Freund and Blanchard (1993) presented three cases of men who supercially
appeared to be motivated by ETIIs based on their fantasies and behaviors, which
included fantasizing about being either a baby or a young boy and wearing diapers.
One man described his sexual fantasy of being a baby put into the washing machine
20 Erotic Target Identity Inversions 603
by his mother, and he also reported a history of cross-dressing in order to feel
powerless or degraded. The men in all three cases reported that they were most
sexually attracted to adult women and fantasized about adult women spanking or
degrading them. Their fantasies thus exaggerated differences between them and their
preferred erotic targets (i.e., adult women) and did not increase similarity. As a
result, Freund and Blanchard concluded that these men were motivated by masoch-
istic fantasies of being a baby or child and wearing diapers, rather than by an ETII.
Among autopedophilic and autoanthropomorphozoophilic men, the correspon-
dence between their external and internal erotic targets with respect to gender and
either age or species suggests that they are more likely motivated by ETIIs than by
sexual masochism (Hsu & Bailey, 2017,2019). The nding that these men tended to
be sexually aroused by the fantasy of being the same kinds of children or anthropo-
morphic animals to whom they were sexually attracted is more consistent with the
concept of ETIIs, because there was increased similarity rather than differences
between the men and their preferred erotic targets. Future work to distinguish
ETIIs and sexual masochism may continue to benet from examining the specicity
of a potential ETII at multiple levels, or whether sexual attraction to other individuals
matches sexual arousal by the fantasy of being one of those individuals in more than
one dimension of sexual attraction (e.g., gender and age). Lawrence (2013)
presented an especially compelling example of this specicity in an autogynephilic
individual: In adolescence, I had a strong attraction to certain actresses (e.g., Dianna
Rigg from The Avengers and Mary Tyler Moore). I would get aroused by fantasies
involving having my appearance changed to match that of these actresses(p. 113).
Further complicating the distinction between them, ETIIs and sexual masochism
are both paraphilias, and paraphilias tend to co-occur (Abel & Osborn, 1992; Baur
et al., 2016; Dawson et al., 2016; Joyal & Carpentier, 2017; Långström & Seto,
2006; Långström & Zucker, 2005). Thus, it is likely that the same individual might
fantasize or behave in ways that reect an ETII, sexual masochism, or perhaps even
both simultaneously. Autogynephilic males, for example, are especially likely to be
masochistic (Blanchard & Hucker, 1991; Hsu et al., 2015; Långström & Zucker,
2005; Lawrence, 2006,2013). In theory, this might mean that autogynephilia is an
ETII that tends to co-occur with sexual masochism, that it is a form of sexual
masochism, or that it reects both an ETII and sexual masochism. It seems possible
that these three interpretations of autogynephilia can be true to different degrees,
depending on the autogynephilic male.
20.11 Causes and Development of ETIIs
Little is known about the causes and development of ETIIs. Because paraphilias are
poorly understood in general, it is not surprising that most ETIIs are even less
so. Family co-occurrence of transvestic fetishism and gender dysphoria, both closely
related to autogynephilia, has been reported in several natal males, including a pair
of monozygotic twins (Green, 2000). Although this nding tentatively suggests a
604 K. J. Hsu and J. M. Bailey
familial contribution to autogynephilia, it is not clear the extent to which the
contribution is due to shared genes or shared environment. If there is a role for
environment in the causes and development of ETIIs, we suspect that it is a minor
one. Without knowledge of anthropomorphic animals and exposure to them, some-
one is unlikely to become a furry or to develop autoanthropomorphozoophilia.
Indeed, some furries recall that they became furries after discovering online pornog-
raphy of anthropomorphic animals, and others recall that they developed
autoanthropomorphozoophilia after watching cartoons featuring anthropomorphic
animals (Hsu & Bailey, 2019). Most people encounter anthropomorphic animals at
some point in their lives, however, and do not become furries or develop
autoanthropomorphozoophilia. Thus, environmental or learning factors may be
relevant to the causes and development of ETIIs, but they cannot explain why
some individuals develop ETIIs and others do not. Individual factors are more likely
to be relevant, especially in explaining the development within the same individual
of seemingly different but co-occurring ETIIs, such as autogynephilia and
apotemnophilia (First, 2005; Lawrence, 2006), or autogynephilia and autopedophilia
(Hsu & Bailey, 2017).
An underlying predisposition, perhaps genetic or inborn, could be one such
individual factor that leads some individuals and not others to develop ETIIs. Two
case reports have described boys younger than 3 years old who expressed a desire to
cross-dress and displayed penile erections when they did (Stoller, 1985; Zucker &
Blanchard, 1997). These cases suggest that individual factors relevant to the devel-
opment of autogynephilia and other ETIIs, if not genetic or inborn, have an early
onset. Given the overlap between autogynephilia and paraphilias that are not con-
sidered ETIIs, including masochism (Blanchard & Hucker, 1991; Hsu et al., 2015;
Långström & Zucker, 2005; Lawrence, 2006,2013) and gynandromorphophilia
(Blanchard & Collins, 1993; Hsu et al., 2016; Rosenthal et al., 2017), ETIIs might
even share the same causes as other paraphilias, which would suggest an underlying
predisposition to develop paraphilias in general and not ETIIs specically.
A tendency to gravitate toward male-typical and nerdyoccupations and
hobbies, especially those involving computers and technology, has been observed
in those with ETIIs, including autogynephilic individuals (Bailey, 2003; Lawrence,
2007,2013) and furries (Gerbasi et al., 2008). Consistent with this observation, a
former phone sex operator remarked that many who called her to role-play their
autogynephilic fantasies were nerdyand worked in Silicon Valley, a high-tech
sector in San Francisco (Culturally Bound Gender, 2013). According to Lawrence
and Bailey (2009), a study by Veale et al. (2008) showed that transgender women
who are active on the Internet are overwhelmingly autogynephilic. Furthermore, two
studies have found that autogynephilic transgender women score higher than
non-autogynephilic transgender women do on a measure of autistic traits (Jones
et al., 2012; Pasterski et al., 2014), which are closely associated with male-typical
and nerdyinterests (Baron-Cohen, 2002). Consistent with autogynephilia being
correlated with both increased nerdyinterests and autistic traits, an autogynephilic
individual who experienced gender dysphoria in one case study was described as
having strong interests in model building and anime, as well as Aspergers syndrome
20 Erotic Target Identity Inversions 605
(Gallucci et al., 2005). Another study showed that furries are more likely to report
having been diagnosed with an autism spectrum disorder, compared with the general
population (Reysen et al., 2018). Finally, a recent study examined whether autistic
traits were related to four putative ETIIs (autogynephilia in men, autoandrophilia in
women, autopedophilia, and autozoophilia), nding limited evidence in this regard
(Brown et al., 2020).
Future research is necessary to clarify the ways in which autogynephilia,
autoanthropomorphozoophilia, and other ETIIs are related to nerdyinterests and
autistic traits, which have the potential to reveal more about the causes and devel-
opment of ETIIs. For instance, if ETIIs are correlated with increased autistic traits, it
might be that individuals with ETIIs have difculty with distinguishing their own
mental states and experiences from those of others, which tends to show in the
cognitive representations and neural systems of individuals with autistic traits
(Gallese et al., 2013; Lombardo & Baron-Cohen, 2010). ETIIs might then reect
the specic difculty with distinguishing oneself from other individuals as an erotic
target, resulting in sexual arousal by the fantasy of being another individual. Several
neuroimaging studies have examined differences in brain structure and activity
between men who desire limb amputation and men who do not (see Brugger et al.,
2016, for a review). The results have implicated brain regions relevant to body
representation and the sensorimotor system in the desire for limb amputation.
We speculate that ETIIs and autistic traits might be related because both are
caused by difculty with understanding mental representations of oneself versus
others, and ETIIs specically represent this difculty as it pertains to understanding
an erotic target as oneself versus others. But there is at least one other developmental
pathway through which ETIIs might be related to autistic traits, and it involves the
dysphoria that sometimes accompanies ETIIs. It is possible that increased autistic
traits cause some individuals with ETIIs to develop an intense and obsessional focus
on becoming more like their preferred erotic targets (e.g., women, anthropomorphic
animals), resulting in dysphoria related to their current identity (e.g., man, human).
In this scenario, ETIIs are related to autistic traits not because there is a direct
association between them, but rather because autistic traits cause dysphoria in
those with ETIIs.
Consistent with this explanation, the link between autogynephilia and nerdy
interests or autistic traits has mainly been observed in autogynephilic individuals
who were either experiencing gender dysphoria or transgender with a presumed
history of gender dysphoria (Bailey, 2003; Culturally Bound Gender, 2013; Gallucci
et al., 2005; Jones et al., 2012; Lawrence, 2007,2013; Pasterski et al., 2014). No
study has yet reported such a link controlling for gender dysphoria or in a sample of
autogynephilic males without gender dysphoria. Instead, recent studies have indi-
cated that prepubescent children with gender dysphoria are also elevated in autistic
traits (VanderLaan et al., 2015a,2015b; Zucker et al., 2017). Natal males who
experienced gender dysphoria as prepubescent children tend overwhelmingly to
become gay rather than autogynephilic men in adulthood (Zucker, 2014). Because
the evidence suggests increased autistic traits in both autogynephilic individuals and
prepubescent children with gender dysphoria, autistic traits might be related to the
606 K. J. Hsu and J. M. Bailey
dysphoria that these two different groups share and not autogynephilia or ETIIs more
generally. This developmental pathway might implicate autistic traits as causally
related to dysphoria in those with ETIIs, in contrast to a developmental pathway in
which ETIIs and autistic traits are both caused by difculty with distinguishing
between mental representations of oneself and others.
20.12 Future Directions
It is unlikely that ETIIs are limited to those that were reviewed in this chapter. Two
promising approaches to studying ETIIs have been employed sparingly so far. The
rst, used by Hsu and Bailey (2017) in their study of autopedophilia, is to recruit
participants sexually attracted to specic kinds of individuals (e.g., pedohebephilic
men) and examine whether a subset of them report sexual arousal by the fantasy of
being one of those individuals, consistent with the concept of ETIIs. The second,
used by Lawrence (2006) in her work on apotemnophilia and Hsu and Bailey (2019)
in their study of autoanthropomorphozoophilia, is to recruit participants who desire
to change their appearance or behavior to become more like a specic kind of
individual (e.g., men who desire limb amputation, furries), and to examine the extent
to which they are motivated by ETIIs. Individuals who might be sexually motivated
to change their appearance or behavior are especially promising candidates for this
second approach.
Regarding the rst approach to studying ETIIs, there exist men sexually attracted
to overweight or obese individuals (Swami & Furnham, 2009) and to individuals of a
particular race/ethnicity that is different than their own (Phua & Kaufman, 2003).
Future research could investigate the putative ETIIs specic to these two populations
of men, which would involve sexual arousal by the fantasy of being overweight or
obese and by the fantasy of being a different race/ethnicity, respectively. Regarding
the second approach to studying ETIIs, there exist individuals known as adult baby/
diaper lovers, who are predominantly male and enjoy role-playing as babies and
wearing diapers (Hawkinson & Zamboni, 2014). It is possible that adult baby/diaper
lovers are motivated by an ETII involving sexual arousal by the fantasy of being a
baby, because they change their appearance and behavior to become more like
babies and appear to do so for sexual reasons. However, this interpretation would
require that they tend to experience both sexual attraction to babies and sexual
arousal by the fantasy of being a baby. Two recent studies found little evidence
that adult baby/diaper lovers tend to be sexually attracted to babies and thus
motivated by an ETII (Fuss et al., 2019; Hsu, 2019). Not all individuals who change
their appearance or behavior to become more like another kind of individual are
motivated by ETIIs, but we must study that possibility in order to know.
The concept of ETIIs has been controversial and difcult for many people to
appreciate and accept, despite its increasing empirical support and potential for
explaining otherwise puzzling phenomena, such as cross-dressing among heterosex-
ual men, desire for limb amputation, and the furry phenomenon. Some individuals
20 Erotic Target Identity Inversions 607
who might be characterized by ETIIs dislike and reject the notion that their changes
in appearance and behavior are sexually motivated by a paraphilia. For example,
some transgender women have been especially hostile to the idea that autogynephilia
can be a fundamental motivation for gender conrmation surgery in natal males
(Dreger, 2008). We hope this chapter helps researchers, clinicians, and other inter-
ested readers to better understand ETIIs. We also hope this chapter stimulates much
needed research on ETIIs, which may reect an understudied but important dimen-
sion of sexual orientation.
References
Abel, G. G., & Osborn, C. (1992). The paraphilias: The extent and nature of sexually deviant and
criminal behavior. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 15, 675687.
Aggrawal, A. (2009). A new classication of necrophilia. Journal of Forensic and Legal Medicine,
16, 316320.
Aggrawal, A. (2011). A new classication of zoophilia. Journal of Forensic and Legal Medicine,
18,7378.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
(5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Bailey, J. M. (2003). The man who would be queen: The science of gender-bending and transsex-
ualism. Washington, DC: Joseph Henry Press.
Bailey, J. M., & Blanchard, R. (2017, December 7). Gender dysphoria is not one thing [Blog post].
Retrieved from http://4thwavenow.com/2017/12/07/gender-dysphoria-is-not-one-thing
Bailey, J. M., Hsu, K. J., & Bernhard, P. A. (2016). An internet study of men sexually attracted to
children: Sexual attraction patterns. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125, 976988.
Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6,
248254.
Baur, E., Forsman, M., Santtila, P., Johansson, A., Sandnabba, K., & Långström, N. (2016).
Paraphilic sexual interests and sexually coercive behavior: A population-based twin study.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45, 11631172.
Beetz, A. M. (2004). Bestiality/zoophilia: A scarcely investigated phenomenon between crime,
paraphilia, and love. Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice, 4,136.
Blanchard, R. (1985). Typology of male-to-female transsexualism. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
14, 247261.
Blanchard, R. (1989a). The classication and labeling of nonhomosexual gender dysphorias.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 18, 315334.
Blanchard, R. (1989b). The concept of autogynephilia and the typology of male gender dysphoria.
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 177, 616623.
Blanchard, R. (1991). Clinical observations and systematic studies of autogynephilia. Journal of
Sex and Marital Therapy, 17, 235251.
Blanchard, R. (1992). Nonmonotonic relation of autogynephilia and heterosexual attraction. Jour-
nal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 271276.
Blanchard, R. (1993a). Partial versus complete autogynephilia and gender dysphoria. Journal of Sex
and Marital Therapy, 19, 301307.
Blanchard, R. (1993b). The she-male phenomenon and the concept of partial autogynephilia.
Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 19,6976.
Blanchard, R. (1993c). Varieties of autogynephilia and their relationship to gender dysphoria.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 22, 241251.
608 K. J. Hsu and J. M. Bailey
Blanchard, R. (2005). Early history of the concept of autogynephilia. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
34, 439446.
Blanchard, R. (2015, August). Departures from alloerotic heterosexual teleiophilia: Research on the
less examined types. In J. M. Bailey (Chair), Erotic target identity inversions. Symposium
conducted at the meeting of the International Academy of Sex Research, Toronto, ON.
Blanchard, R., Clemmensen, L. H., & Steiner, B. W. (1987). Heterosexual and homosexual gender
dysphoria. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 16, 139152.
Blanchard, R., & Collins, P. I. (1993). Men with sexual interest in transvestites, transsexuals, and
she-males. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 181, 570575.
Blanchard, R., & Hucker, S. J. (1991). Age, transvestism, bondage, and concurrent paraphilic
activities in 117 fatal cases of autoerotic asphyxia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 371377.
Blanchard, R., Racansky, I. G., & Steiner, B. W. (1986). Phallometric detection of fetishistic
arousal in heterosexual male cross-dressers. Journal of Sex Research, 22, 452462.
Blom, R. M., Hennekam, R. C., & Denys, D. (2012). Body integrity identity disorder. PLoS One, 7,
e34702. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034702
Blom, R. M., van der Wal, S. J., Vulink, N. C., & Denys, D. (2017). Role of sexuality in body
integrity identity disorder (BIID): A cross-sectional internet-based survey study. Journal of
Sexual Medicine, 14, 10281035.
Bogaert, A. F. (2012). Asexuality and autochorissexualism (identity-less sexuality). Archives of
Sexual Behavior, 41, 15131514.
Brotto, L. A., & Yule, M. (2017). Asexuality: Sexual orientation, paraphilia, sexual dysfunction, or
none of the above? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 46, 619627.
Brown, A., Barker, E. D., & Rahman, Q. (2020). Erotic target identity inversions among men and
women in an internet sample. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 17,99110.
Brugger, P., Christen, M., Jellestad, L., & Hänggi, J. (2016). Limb amputation and other disability
desires as a medical condition. Lancet Psychiatry, 3, 1171186.
Casavant, V. R. (2005, August 16). Catmans transformation raises concerns over extreme surgery.
Seattle Times. Retrieved from http://www.seattletimes.com/seattle-news/catmans-transforma
tion-raises-concerns-over-extreme-surgery
Coleman, E., Bockting, W. O., & Gooren, L. (1993). Homosexual and bisexual identity in
sex-reassigned female-to-male transsexuals. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 22,3750.
Culturally Bound Gender (2013, April 27). But theres no such thing as autogynephilia!: Phone
sex, the male gaze, and how Blanchard and trans* activists both get it wrong [Blog post].
Retrieved from http://culturallyboundgender.wordpress.com/2013/04/27/but-theres-no-such-
thing-as-autogynephilia-phone-sex-the-male-gaze-and-how-blanchard-and-trans-activists-both-
get-it-wrong.
Dawson, S. J., Bannerman, B. A., & Lalumière, M. L. (2016). Paraphilic interests: An examination
of sex differences in a nonclinical sample. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment,
28,2045.
Dickey, R. (2007). Commentary on adult baby syndromeby Evcimen and Gratz (2006). Archives
of Sexual Behavior, 36, 131132.
Dickey, R., & Stephens, J. (1995). Female-to-male transsexualism, heterosexual type: Two cases.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 24, 439445.
Docter, R. F., & Fleming, J. S. (2001). Measures of transgender behavior. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 30, 255271.
Docter, R. F., & Prince, V. (1997). Transvestism: A survey of 1032 cross-dressers. Archives of
Sexual Behavior, 26, 589605.
Dreger, A. D. (2008). The controversy surrounding The man who would be queen: A case history of
the politics of science, identity, and sex in the internet age. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37,
366421.
First, M. B. (2005). Desire for amputation of a limb: Paraphilia, psychosis, or a new type of identity
disorder. Psychological Medicine, 35, 919928.
20 Erotic Target Identity Inversions 609
Freund, K., & Blanchard, R. (1993). Erotic target location errors in male gender dysphorics,
paedophiles, and fetishists. British Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 558563.
Freund, K., Seto, M. C., & Kuban, M. (1996). Two types of fetishism. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 34, 687694.
Freund, K., Steiner, B. W., & Chan, S. (1982). Two types of cross-gender identity. Archives of
Sexual Behavior, 11,4963.
Fuss, J., Jais, L., Grey, B. T., Guczka, S. R., Briken, P., & Biedermann, S. V. (2019). Self-reported
childhood maltreatment and erotic target identity inversions among men with paraphilic infan-
tilism. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 45, 781795.
Gallese, V., Rochat, M. J., & Berchio, C. (2013). The mirror mechanism and its potential role in
autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 55,1522.
Gallucci, G., Hackerman, F., & Schmidt, C. W. (2005). Gender identity disorder in an adult male
with Aspergers syndrome. Sexuality and Disability, 23,3540.
Gerbasi, K. C., Paolone, N., Higner, J., Scaletta, L. L., Bernstein, P. L., Conway, S., & Privitera,
A. (2008). Furries from A to Z (anthropomorphism to zoomorphism). Society & Animals:
Journal of Human-Animal Studies, 16, 197222.
Green, R. (2000). Family cooccurrence of gender dysphoria: Ten sibling or parentchild pairs.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 29, 499507.
Gurley, G. (2001, March). Pleasures of the fur. Vanity Fair. Retrieved from http://www.vanityfair.
com/culture/2001/03/furries200103
Hawkinson, K., & Zamboni, B. D. (2014). Adult baby/diaper lovers: An exploratory study of an
online community sample. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43, 863877.
Howitt, D. (1995). Paedophiles and sexual offences against children. New York: Wiley.
Hsu, K. J. (2019). Erotic target identity inversions in male furries, adult baby/diaper lovers, and
eunuchs. (Doctoral dissertation). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and Theses database.
(UMI No. 13878916).
Hsu, K. J., & Bailey, J. M. (2017). Autopedophilia: Erotic-target identity inversions in men sexually
attracted to children. Psychological Science, 28, 115123.
Hsu, K. J., & Bailey, J. M. (2019). The furryphenomenon: Characterizing sexual orientation,
sexual motivation, and erotic target identity inversions in male furries. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 48, 13491369.
Hsu, K. J., Rosenthal, A. M., & Bailey, J. M. (2015). The psychometric structure of items assessing
autogynephilia. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 44, 13011312.
Hsu, K. J., Rosenthal, A. M., Miller, D. I., & Bailey, J. M. (2016). Who are
gynandromorphophilic men? Characterizing men with sexual interest in transgender women.
Psychological Medicine, 46, 819827.
Hsu, K. J., Rosenthal, A. M., Miller, D. I., & Bailey, J. M. (2017). Sexual arousal patterns of
autogynephilic male cross-dressers. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 46, 247253.
Jones, R. M., Wheelwright, S., Farrell, K., Martin, E., Green, R., Di Ceglie, D., & Baron-Cohen,
S. (2012). Brief report: Female-to-male transsexual people and autistic traits. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 42, 301306.
Joyal, C. C., & Carpentier, J. (2017). The prevalence of paraphilic interests and behaviors in the
general population: A provincial survey. Journal of Sex Research, 54, 161171.
Knoll, J. L., & Hazelwood, R. R. (2009). Becoming the victim: Beyond sadism in serial sexual
murderers. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 14, 106114.
Långström, N., & Seto, M. C. (2006). Exhibitionistic and voyeuristic behavior in a Swedish
national population survey. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 35, 427435.
Långström, N., & Zucker, K. J. (2005). Transvestic fetishism in the general population: Prevalence
and correlates. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 31,8795.
Lawrence, A. A. (2004). Autogynephilia: A paraphilic model of gender identity disorder. Journal of
Gay & Lesbian Psychotherapy, 8,6987.
Lawrence, A. A. (2005). Sexuality before and after male-to-female sex reassignment surgery.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34, 147166.
610 K. J. Hsu and J. M. Bailey
Lawrence, A. A. (2006). Clinical and theoretical parallels between desire for limb amputation and
gender identity disorder. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 35, 263278.
Lawrence, A. A. (2007). Becoming what we love: Autogynephilic transsexualism conceptualized as
an expression of romantic love. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 50, 506520.
Lawrence, A. A. (2009a). Anatomic autoandrophilia in an adult male. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
38, 10501056.
Lawrence, A. A. (2009b). Erotic target location errors: An underappreciated paraphilic dimension.
Journal of Sex Research, 46, 194215.
Lawrence, A. A. (2010a). Societal individualism predicts prevalence of nonhomosexual orientation
in male-to-female transsexualism. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 573583.
Lawrence, A. A. (2010b). Something resembling autogynephilia in women: Comment on Moser
(2009). Journal of Homosexuality, 57,14.
Lawrence, A. A. (2013). Men trapped in mens bodies: Narratives of autogynephilic transsexual-
ism. New York: Springer.
Lawrence, A. A., & Bailey, J. M. (2009). Transsexual groups in Veale et al. (2008) are
autogynephilicand even more autogynephilic.Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 173175.
Lombardo, M. V., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2010). Unraveling the paradox of the autistic self. Wiley
Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 393403.
Miletski, H. (2002). Understanding bestiality and zoophilia. Bethesda, MD: EastWest.
Money, J. (1986). Lovemaps: Clinical concepts of sexual/erotic health and pathology, paraphilia,
and gender transposition in childhood, adolescence, and maturity. New York: Irvington.
Money, J. (1991). Paraphilia in females: Fixation on amputation and lameness; two personal
accounts. Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality, 3, 165172.
Money, J., Jobaris, R., & Furth, G. (1977). Apotemnophilia: Two cases of self-demand amputation
as a paraphilia. Journal of Sex Research, 13, 115125.
Moser, C. (2009). Autogynephilia in women. Journal of Homosexuality, 56, 539547.
Moser, C., & Kleinplatz, P. J. (2006). DSM-IV-TR and the paraphilias: An argument for removal.
Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality, 17,91109.
Nuttbrock, L., Bockting, W., Mason, M., Hwahng, S., Rosenblum, A., Macri, M., & Becker,
J. (2011). A further assessment of Blanchards typology of homosexual versus
non-homosexual or autogynephilic gender dysphoria. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40,
247257.
OKeefe, T. (2007, April). Autogynephilia and autoandrophilia in non-sex and gender dysphoric
persons. Paper presented at the meeting of the World Association for Sexual Health,
Sydney, NSW.
Pasterski, V., Gilligan, L., & Curtis, R. (2014). Traits of autism spectrum disorders in adults with
gender dysphoria. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43, 387393.
Phua, V. C., & Kaufman, G. (2003). The crossroads of race and sexuality: Date selection among
men in internet personalads. Journal of Family Issues, 24, 981994.
Plante, C. N., Mock, S., Reysen, S., & Gerbasi, K. C. (2011). International Anthropomorphic
Research Project: Winter 2011 Online Survey Summary. Retrieved from http://sites.google.
com/site/anthropomorphicresearch/past-results/international-online-furry-survey-2011
Pregartbauer, L., Schnell, T., & Kasten, E. (2014). Body integrity identity disorder and
mancophilia: Similarities and differences. American Journal of Applied Psychology, 3,
116121.
Reysen, S., Plante, C. N., Chadborn, D., Roberts, S. E., Gerbasi, K. C., Miller, J. I., ... Ray,
A. (2018). A brief report on the prevalence of self-reported mood disorders, anxiety disorders,
attention-decit/hyperactivity disorder, and autism spectrum disorder in anime, brony, and furry
fandoms. Phoenix Papers, 3,6475.
Rosenthal, A. M., Hsu, K. J., & Bailey, J. M. (2017). Who are gynandromorphophilic men? An
internet survey of men with sexual interest in transgender women. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
46, 255264.
20 Erotic Target Identity Inversions 611
Schroy, C., Plante, C. N., Reysen, S., Roberts, S. E., & Gerbasi, K. C. (2016). Different motivations
as predictors of psychological connection to fan interest and fan groups in anime, furry, and
fantasy sport fandoms. Phoenix Papers, 2, 148167.
Seto, M. C. (2012). Is pedophilia a sexual orientation? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 231236.
Seto, M. C. (2017). The puzzle of male chronophilias. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 46,322.
Smith, Y. L. S., van Goozen, S., Kupier, A., & Cohen-Kettenis, P. T. (2005). Transsexual subtypes:
Clinical and theoretical signicance. Psychiatry Research, 137, 151160.
Stoller, R. J. (1982). Transvestism in women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 11,99115.
Stoller, R. J. (1985). A child fetishist. In Presentations of gender (pp. 93136). New Haven: Yale
University Press.
Swami, V., & Furnham, A. (2009). Big and beautiful: Attractiveness and health ratings of the
female body by male fat admirers.Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 201208.
VanderLaan, D. P., Leef, J. H., Wood, H., Hughes, S. K., & Zucker, K. J. (2015a). Autism spectrum
disorder risk factors and autistic traits in gender dysphoric children. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 45, 17421750.
VanderLaan, D. P., Postema, L., Wood, H., Singh, D., Fantus, S., Hyun, J., ... Zucker, K. J.
(2015b). Do children with gender dysphoria have intense/obsessional interests? Journal of Sex
Research, 52, 213219.
Veale, J. F., Clarke, D. E., & Lomax, T. C. (2008). Sexuality of male-to-female transsexuals.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 586597.
Wignall, L., & McCormack, M. (2017). An exploratory study of a new kink activity: Pup play.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 46, 801811.
Williams, C. J., & Weinberg, M. S. (2003). Zoophilia in men: A study of sexual interest in animals.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32, 523535.
Zavitzianos, G. (1972). Homeovestism: Perverse form of behaviour involving wearing clothes of
the same sex. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 53, 471477.
Zavitzianos, G. (1977). The object in fetishism, homeovestism and transvestism. International
Journal of Psychoanalysis, 58, 487495.
Zucker, K. J. (2014). Gender dysphoria. In M. Lewis & K. D. Rudolph (Eds.), Handbook of
developmental psychopathology (3rd ed., pp. 683702). New York: Springer.
Zucker, K. J., & Blanchard, R. (1997). Transvestic fetishism: Psychopathology and theory. In D. R.
Laws & W. ODonohue (Eds.), Sexual deviance: Theory, assessment, and treatment
(pp. 253279). New York: Guilford.
Zucker, K. J., Bradley, S. J., Owen-Anderson, A., Kibblewhite, S. J., Wood, H., Singh, D., & Choi,
K. (2012). Demographics, behavior problems, and psychosexual characteristics of adolescents
with gender identity disorder or transvestic fetishism. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 38,
151189.
Zucker, K. J., Nabbijohn, A. N., Santarossa, A., Wood, H., Bradley, S. J., Matthews, J., &
VanderLaan, D. P. (2017). Intense/obsessional interests in children with gender dysphoria: A
cross-validation study using the Teachers Report Form. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and
Mental Health, 11, 51.
612 K. J. Hsu and J. M. Bailey
... Erotic target identity inversions (ETIIs) are paraphilias in which a person's external erotic target is located onto the self, resulting in sexual arousal by the fantasy of being a member of the target class, or by imitating one (Blanchard, 1991;Freund & Blanchard, 1993;Hsu & Bailey, 2022;Lawrence, 2009). The best studied ETII is autogynephilia, the propensity to be sexually aroused by the fantasy of being a woman (Blanchard, 1989a). ...
... In conceptualizing autogynephilia as an ETII, sexual attraction to women is internalized to some degree, such that a person also finds it sexually arousing to be a woman (Blanchard, 1989b(Blanchard, , 1992. ETIIs reflect the extent to which sexual attraction is internally versus externally focused on a dimension of sexual orientation that might be considered an erotic target's location (Blanchard, 2015;, 2022Seto, 2017). ...
... Within each of these three groups of men, a large subset is not only sexually attracted to amputees, children, or anthropomorphic animals, but also find it sexually arousing to be an amputee (apotemnophilia), a child (autopedophilia), or an anthropomorphic animal (autoanthropomorphozoophilia), respectively. The correspondence between external and internal Archives of Sexual Behavior 1 3 erotic targets is crucial (see Hsu & Bailey, 2022 for a recent review of ETIIs). ...
Article
Full-text available
Adult baby/diaper lovers (AB/DLs) enjoy role-playing as babies and/or wearing diapers. They also engage in other related activities, such as urinating or defecating themselves and having an adult care for them. Previous surveys have revealed that AB/DLs commonly report sexual motivation, a finding that is corroborated by case reports in the psychiatric literature and some media interviews. The fact that AB/DLs change their appearance and behavior to become more like babies raises the possibility that they have an erotic target identity inversion (ETII). In ETIIs, a person’s external erotic target is inverted into the self, resulting in sexual arousal by the fantasy of being a member of the target class or by imitating one. If AB/DLs are sexually motivated by an ETII, they should experience both sexual attraction to babies and sexual arousal by the fantasy of being a baby. We surveyed 207 male AB/DLs recruited from the Internet about their sexual orientation, sexual motivation, and sexual interests for a primarily quantitative analysis. Consistent with past research, a substantial minority of participants reported non-heterosexual identities (42%), and a large majority reported some degree of sexual motivation for being AB/DLs (93%). Wearing diapers and urinating or defecating oneself were rated as especially sexual. Even though 40% of participants reported sexual arousal by the fantasy of being a baby, only 4% reported sexual attraction to babies. This pattern of results is contrary to predictions from the concept of ETIIs. Instead, participants indicated that physical or mental pain, humiliation, and an adult woman were important to their sexual fantasies of being a baby. Masochism appears to be a promising alternative to an ETII for explaining the sexual motivation of AB/DLs.
... There are, however, a number of additional parameters of sexual orientation that have been raised in the literature. For example, these other parameters include: age groups that one is attracted toward (Seto, 2017); the degree to which sexual orientation toward different categories of people is "fluid" or varies over time (Diamond et al., 2020); romantic attractions (e.g., Diamond, 2003;Savin-Williams, 2016); gender role presentation (Maybach & Gold, 1994;Petterson & Vasey, 2021;Schudson et al., 2018); the degree to which attractions are directed toward others versus oneself (Hsu & Bailey, 2021) or toward others versus no one (Brotto & Milani, 2021); and one's interest in monogamous versus nonmonogamous relationships (Hamilton & Winward, 2021). Each of these parameters might be biodevelopmentally dissociable from the dimension of androphilia-gynephilia and, thus, identifying if and how would help carve the nature of sexual orientation at its joints. ...
Article
Full-text available
Sexual orientation is a core aspect of human experience and understanding its development is fundamental to psychology as a scientific discipline. Biological perspectives have played an important role in uncovering the processes that contribute to sexual orientation development. Research in this field has relied on a variety of populations, including community, clinical, and cross-cultural samples, and has commonly focused on female gynephilia (i.e., female sexual attraction to adult females) and male androphilia (i.e., male sexual attraction to adult males). Genetic, hormonal, and immunological processes all appear to influence sexual orientation. Consistent with biological perspectives, there are sexual orientation differences in brain development and evidence indicates that similar biological influences apply across cultures. An outstanding question in the field is whether the hypothesized biological influences are all part of the same process or represent different developmental pathways leading to same-sex sexual orientation. Some studies indicate that same-sex sexually oriented people can be divided into subgroups who likely experienced different biological influences. Consideration of gender expression in addition to sexual orientation might help delineate such subgroups. Thus, future research on the possible existence of such subgroups could prove to be valuable for uncovering the biological development of sexual orientation. Recommendations for such future research are discussed.
... The late-onset form (not arising until puberty at the very earliest) is also not what it may seem, being a sexual self-orientation: that is, it's inwardly directed. Erotic-target identity inversion, as it is labelled by Hsu & Bailey (2021) and Brown, Barker & Rahman (2020), is not an actual changed identification or orientation, but a narcissistic intensification combining existing sexual identification and orientation. In males this is an heterosexual fantasy of being oneself female; ...
Article
Full-text available
Sex is defined functionally, in terms of gamete production, which is strictly binary and immutable. Traits thought sex-derived (or related), in being at some remove may not be sex-specific, utilising systems common across sex; however, apparently sex-overlapping traits serve to reinforce, not compromise sex binarity. Sexual identification and orientation might be expected to show degrees of sex non-separation, but seemingly through their very closeness in derivation from sex are themselves binary and immutable. Sexual orientation is of discrete (one majority and one minority aberrant) form; with bisexuality merely ostensible: male hypersexuality and female non-sexual tension reduction. Notions of sexual identity are chimeric: sex dysphoria is mostly latent homosexuality; the remainder intensified sexual self-orientation or psychopathology-driven social contagion of a condition imaginarily possessed.
Article
Full-text available
Autogynephilia is a natal male’s propensity to be sexually aroused by the thought or fantasy of being a woman. Both male cross-dressers and transfeminine individuals (a broad range of individuals born male with a feminine gender identity; e.g., trans women) have been shown to be motivated or characterized by autogynephilia. Although there is a lack of research on whether other potentially related aspects of sexuality are comparable between them, the conceptual framework of autogynephilia offers several predictions that can be tested empirically. Following these predictions, the present study examined whether 10 diverse aspects of sexuality differed between 519 male cross-dressers and 288 transfeminine individuals recruited from online communities, as well as between both groups and 293 cisgender men and 301 cisgender women recruited as control groups. The overwhelming majority of male cross-dresser and transfeminine participants identified as heterosexual, bisexual, asexual, or pansexual. Compared with transfeminine participants, male cross-dressers reported more core and general autogynephilia, paraphilic interests, sociosexual orientation, sexual compulsivity, and problematic pornography use, but less bisexual attraction. Compared with cisgender men and women, male cross-dressers and transfeminine participants as a combined sample reported more bisexual attraction, sexual orientation ambiguity, core and general autogynephilia, paraphilic interests, analloeroticism, sexual compulsivity, and problematic pornography use, but less perceived desirability as a partner. Differences were larger comparing male cross-dressers and transfeminine participants with either control group than with each other. Results suggest that while autogynephilia is especially important to the sexuality of male cross-dressers, it also figures importantly in the sexuality of transfeminine individuals, even if it is expressed and organized differently.
Article
Full-text available
Is the category paraphilia a natural kind? That is, do different paraphilias share anything scientifically interesting or are they classified together because they are unusual and sometimes problematic? We investigated this question systematically in 11 samples of paraphilic males (N = 4,617) and 11 samples of control males (N = 1,494). Primary data consisted of responses to the 11-item Paraphilic Interests Scale. Contrary to our initial hypothesis, the scale mean was similar for paraphilic and control samples. Using logistic regression and the same items, we derived three highly correlated measures that robustly discriminated paraphilic and control samples (ds ranging from 0.86 to 0.92). These successful measures capitalized on the unanticipated fact that some items (especially those assessing transvestism and masochistic humiliation) were positively associated with membership in paraphilic samples, while others (especially those assessing voyeurism) were negatively associated with such membership. Subsequent analyses focused on one of the measures, the Paraphilic Interests Scale Contrast (PISC). Consistent with prior findings distinguishing paraphilias and homosexual orientation, PISC was not elevated among homosexual males compared with heterosexual males among the control groups. Within four paraphilic samples, PISC was positively associated with additional paraphilic phenomena. Results provide tentative support for both the proposition that paraphilia is a natural kind and the usefulness of PISC as a measure of paraphilia.
Article
Full-text available
Autogynephilic Persuasive Pornography (AGPP) is a type of pornography that is characterised by its unique autogynephilic and persuasive traits. Autogynephilia refers to the sexual pleasure one derives by imagining themselves as women. Known in the subculture as Sissy Hypno, this type of pornography is an altered mishmash of conventional licensed studio/mainstream pornography found on the internet, designed to interact with/persuade the user (by text or audio) and aid in the process of erotic self-feminisation. AGPP is consumed by a wide range of individuals and for a wide variety of reasons. Yet, there is little to no empirical research aimed at describing this novel form of media, the motivations of its consumers and the effects of its consumption. Therefore, this research study qualitatively explores the experiences of 16 participants (N = 16, age > 19), who consumed AGPP at the time of the study, by phenomenologically interpreting self-reported behaviour and self-described inclinations regarding AGPP use. The participants were recruited from Reddit, and the participant sample is an ethnically diverse sample consisting of men and transwomen. The key findings were categorised into the following superordinate themes: (1) Influence of AGPP on personality and individual lifestyle (2) Exploring femininity and submissive sexual behaviour and (3) Influence of AGPP on gender and sexuality. The analysis of these themes (and their subordinate themes) allows for the conceptualisation of AGPP and an analysis of its reported effects, contributes to the limited evidence of cisgender men strongly relating to Autogynephilia or erotic self-feminisation, alongside Autogynephilia being a frequent motivation for consuming AGPP. Some of the positive and negative effects of consuming AGPP (primarily focusing on gender identity, sexual attitudes and sexuality) were also identified through self-report, thereby, necessitating the need for widening the scientific exploration of Autogynephilia amongst AGPP consumers (not limited to gender categories) and the clinical implications of consuming AGPP (e.g., compulsive AGPP consumption).
Article
Full-text available
Some men sexually attracted to types of persons (e.g., women) or things (e.g., animals) also have internalized sexual attractions: sexual arousal by the idea of being the type of person or thing to whom they are attracted. Consequently, some of these men develop erotic target identity inversions, in which they imitate, yearn to be, or identify as an instance of their erotic target. Erotic Target Identity Inversion Theory predicts that for every external erotic target to which men are attracted, a subset of men will develop an internalized sexual attraction, which may cause an erotic target identity inversion. We examined these predictions in Internet surveys of three samples of men with paraphilic sexual interests: 322 men attracted to amputees, 1501 men attracted to animals, and 402 men attracted to severely obese persons. All samples included substantial minorities of men reporting internalized sexual attractions and erotic target identity inversions specific to their external sexual attractions (e.g., men attracted to amputees who are also aroused by the fantasy of being amputees and wish to become amputees). The correlation between degree of each internalized sexual attraction and degree of its corresponding erotic target identity inversion was approximately 1.0 after correction for attenuation. In each sample, participants’ specific internalized sexual attraction was positively correlated with autogynephilia, likely the most common internalized sexual attraction in men. Erotic Target Identity Inversion Theory can potentially explain a variety of otherwise puzzling phenomena, including transgender identity among female-attracted natal males and men seeking amputations of healthy limbs.
Chapter
Sexual orientation is a core aspect of human experience and understanding its development is fundamental to psychology as a scientific discipline. Biological perspectives have played an important role in helping to uncover the processes that contribute to sexual orientation development. Research in this field has relied on a variety of populations, including community, clinical, and cross-cultural samples, and has commonly focused on female gynephilia (i.e., female sexual attraction to adult females) and male androphilia (i.e., male sexual attraction to adult males). Genetic, hormonal, and immunological processes all appear to influence sexual orientation. Consistent with biological perspectives, there are sexual orientation differences in brain development and evidence indicates that similar biological influences apply across cultures. An outstanding question in the field is whether the hypothesized biological influences are all part of the same process or represent different developmental pathways leading to same-sex sexual orientation. Some studies indicate that same-sex sexually oriented people can be divided into subgroups who likely experienced different biological influences. Consideration of gender expression in addition to sexual orientation might help delineate such subgroups. Thus, future research on the possible existence of such subgroups could prove to be valuable for uncovering the biological development of sexual orientation. Recommendations for such future research are discussed.KeywordsSexual orientationDevelopmentGeneticsSex hormonesMaternal immune hypothesisGender expression
Article
Full-text available
Furries are individuals who are especially interested in anthropomorphic or cartoon animals (e.g., Bugs Bunny). They often strongly identify with anthropomorphic animals and create fursonas, identities of themselves as those anthropomorphic animals. Some practice fursuiting, or wearing costumes that resemble anthropomorphic animals. Furries have been portrayed as sexually motivated in the media and popular culture, although little empirical research has addressed this issue. If some furries are sexually motivated, they may be motivated by an erotic target identity inversion (ETII): sexual arousal by the fantasy of being the same kinds of individuals to whom they are sexually attracted. Furries with ETIIs would experience both sexual attraction to anthropomorphic animals and sexual arousal by fantasizing about being anthropomorphic animals, because they often change their appearance and behavior to become more like anthropomorphic animals. We surveyed 334 male furries recruited from the Internet about their sexual orientation, sexual motivation, and sexual interests. A large majority of our sample reported non-heterosexual identities (84%) and some degree of sexual motivation for being furries (99%). Male furries also tended to report a pattern of sexual interests consistent with an ETII involving anthropomorphic animals. Both sexual attraction to anthropomorphic animals and sexual arousal by fantasizing about being anthropomorphic animals were nearly universal. Furthermore, male furries tended to be sexually aroused by fantasizing about being the same kinds of anthropomorphic animals to whom they were sexually attracted, with respect to gender and species. This sexual motivation and these unusual sexual interests do not justify discrimination or stigmatization.
Article
Full-text available
We examined the prevalence rates of mental and neurodevelopmental illness among members of three different fandoms: anime, My Little Pony (brony), and furry. In total, nearly 2,600 fans across these groups self-reported whether or not they had ever been diagnosed with a psychological and/or neurodevelopmental illness. Data revealed that anime fans, bronies, and furries all show higher rates of diagnoses on the autism spectrum than rates observed in studies of the general population. Rates of mood and anxiety disorders were lower than that of the general population. Rates of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder were lower in anime and brony fandoms, but were not different from the general population for furry fans. Taken together, the results provide an exploratory look at mental health issues in fan communities and suggest both commonalities across fan groups and idiosyncrasies within particular fandoms.
Article
Full-text available
Objective This study assessed whether children clinically referred for gender dysphoria (GD) show symptoms that overlap with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Circumscribed preoccupations/intense interests and repetitive behaviors were considered as overlapping symptoms expressed in both GD and ASD. Methods To assess these constructs, we examined Items 9 and 66 on the Teacher’s Report Form (TRF), which measure obsessions and compulsions, respectively. ResultsFor Item 9, gender-referred children (n = 386) were significantly elevated compared to the referred (n = 965) and non-referred children (n = 965) from the TRF standardization sample. For Item 66, gender-referred children were elevated in comparison to the non-referred children, but not the referred children. Conclusions These findings provided cross-validation of a previous study in which the same patterns were found using the Child Behavior Checklist (Vanderlaan et al. in J Sex Res 52:213–19, 2015). We discuss possible developmental pathways between GD and ASD, including a consideration of the principle of equifinality.
Article
Full-text available
The most salient dimension of men’s sexual orientation is gender: attraction to males versus females. A second dimension is sexual maturity: attraction to children versus adults. A less appreciated dimension is location: attraction to other individuals versus the sexual fantasy of being one of those individuals. Men sexually aroused by the idea or fantasy of being the kinds of individuals to whom they are sexually attracted have an erotic-target identity inversion (ETII). We conducted an online survey to investigate the prevalence and phenomenology of ETIIs among 475 men sexually attracted to children. Autopedophilia, or sexual arousal by the idea of being a child, was common. Furthermore, autopedophilic men tended to be sexually aroused by imagining themselves as the kinds of children (with respect to gender and age) to whom they are sexually attracted. Results support the concept of ETIIs and exemplify the simultaneous importance of three dimensions of male sexual orientation.
Article
Full-text available
Gynandromorphophilia (GAMP) is attraction to gynandromorphs (GAMs), who are natal males with both breasts and a penis, colloquially known as "she-males." Men with GAMP (n = 314) and heterosexual men not attracted to GAMs (n = 211) completed an Internet survey regarding their sexual attraction patterns, relationship history, and potential correlates of GAMP. Men with GAMP reported much higher attraction to natal women than to men, although they also reported slightly higher levels of bisexual feelings compared with controls. Men with GAMP were equally attracted to natal women and GAMs, on average. Thus, GAMP is best considered an unusual form of heterosexuality rather than a separate sexual orientation. Indeed, men with GAMP scored much higher than controls on a measure of autogynephilia, or sexual arousal by the idea or fantasy of being a woman, which is also considered a variant of heterosexual attraction.
Article
Full-text available
To our knowledge, this is the first large study of the attractions of child-attracted men recruited in any manner other than their being charged with legal offenses. We recruited 1,189 men from websites for adults attracted to children. Men in our sample were highly attracted to children, and they were much less attracted to adults, especially to adult men. However, men varied with respect to which combination of gender and age they found most attractive. Men in our sample were especially attracted to pubescent boys and prepubescent girls. Their self-reported attraction patterns closely tracked the age/gender gradient of sexual arousal established in prior research. Consistent with the gradient, men most attracted to prepubescent children were especially likely to have bisexual attractions to children. Pedohebephilia—attraction to sexually immature children—is best considered a collection of related if distinct sexual orientations, which vary in the particular combination of gender and sexual maturity that elicits greatest sexual attraction. Finally, our study reveals the potential power and efficiency of studying highly cooperative child-attracted men recruited via the Internet.
Article
Introduction: Erotic target identity inversions (ETIIs) are poorly studied paraphilias that involve sexual arousal by the idea or fantasy of being the object of one's sexual desires. Aim: To conduct a large non-clinical online survey to investigate self-reported sexual arousal, behavioral expression, and psychological correlates of 4 proposed ETIIs. Methods: A total of 736 natal males and 549 natal females responded to items about self-reported sexual arousal to the idea of acting as an animal (autoanthropomorphozoophilia) or the idea of acting as a child or infant (autonepiophilia), natal males reporting arousal to the idea of acting as a woman (autogynephilia), and natal females reporting arousal to the idea of acting as a man (autoandrophilia). Data pertaining to sexual orientation, childhood gender nonconformity, gender identity discomfort, autism, masochism, and humiliation were also collected. Main outcome measures: The main outcome was a measure of self-reported arousal and expression of the ETIIs being explored using 4 items: arousal level (-3 to 3) when imagining being the erotic target exemplar; frequency of engagement in dressing or behaving like their preferred target (0-4); strength of feeling that they would be better off as the target (0-4); and the frequency of consideration of making physical changes to look or function more like the target (0-4). Results: Mild levels of reported sexual arousal to the idea of being the preferred erotic target were common among the 4 groups, characterizing about half of them. Gender identity discomfort was associated with autogynephilia, autoandrophilia, and autoanthropomorphozoophilia. Greater gender nonconformity was associated with autogynephilia, autoandrophilia, and autonepiophilia. Autism scores were associated with autoandrophilia and autonepiophilia. Masochism was not associated with ETII scores, but humiliation was. Clinical implications: Findings suggest that it may be important to distinguish between subgroups of those with different levels and types of ETII arousal/expression. Strengths & limitations: Strengths of this study include the large, non-clinical sample of men and women for the investigation of ETIIs and the inclusion of measures of psychological correlates. The use of an Internet sample with self-report measures may be unrepresentative, although the Internet has the advantage of allowing recruitment from stigmatized or unusual groups. The cross-sectional nature limits our conclusions, as no causal inferences can be made. Conclusion: The results support the concept of ETIIs as a paraphilic dimension in non-clinical samples and the possible role of gender-related psychological factors. Brown A, Barker ED, Rahman Q. Erotic Target Identity Inversions Among Men and Women in an Internet Sample. J Sex Med 2019;XX:XXX-XXX.
Article
Adults with paraphilic infantilism may desire to wear diapers or engage in infant-like role-play. Erotic target identity inversions (ETII) and childhood maltreatment have been hypothesized to cause regressive interests. To evaluate these two etiological hypotheses, we surveyed the online Adult Baby/Diaper Lover community. Data from 1904 cisgender men revealed a prevalence of pedophilic interests comparable to the general male population, arguing against ETIIs as common in paraphilic infantilism. In contrast, self-reported childhood neglect and abuse were highly prevalent, suggesting that infantilism might be correlated with childhood maltreatment. Additionally, some participants described repetition and/or sexualization of negative childhood experiences.
Article
Background: Body integrity identity disorder (BIID)-a strong desire for amputation or paralysis-is often accompanied by feelings and cognitions of sexual arousal, although this sexual component has been largely neglected in the recent literature. Aim: To examine the presence of BIID-related sexual arousal in subjects with BIID and explore clinical and demographic variables of subjects with BIID who do and do not possess this sexual arousal. Methods: Eighty individuals with BIID responded to an internet-based survey we created. For all subjects, restoring identity was the primary motivation for preferred body modification. We collected data about respondents' demographic, clinical, and sexual characteristics. Based on responses to questions about BIID-specific sexual desires, subjects were assigned to the group with BIID-related sexual feelings (S-BIID; n = 57) or the group without such feelings (NS-BIID; n = 23). Outcomes: Differences in clinical, demographic, and sexual characteristics between S-BIID and NS-BIID groups. Results: Of the respondents, 71.3% endorsed S-BIID. Subjects with S-BIID were significantly more often men, religious, and of a homosexual identity compared with the NS-BIID group. Subjects with S-BIID also significantly more often reported a change in localization and/or intensity of their BIID feelings over time. Furthermore, 66.7% of subjects with S-BIID reported S-BIID as an additional motivation for body modification. Seven of the 57 subjects with S-BIID achieved their preferred body modification through (self)-amputation, whereas none of the subjects with NS-BIID did. Clinical implications: BIID is a heterogeneous disorder in which subjects who self-reported comorbid sexual arousal more often resorted to (self-induced) amputation. Strengths and limitations: This study contains the largest BIID cohort presented in the literature and is the first to genuinely research sexuality in BIID. The first limitation is the lack of face-to-face interviews with the subjects, so no clinical diagnoses could be made. Moreover, there is an ascertainment bias because subjects were collected through the internet and in English, which excluded those who spoke other languages or subjects without an internet connection. Conclusion: The present study provides preliminary evidence for a subpopulation or distinct group of individuals with BIID based on the presence of S-BIID. Blom RM, van der Wal SJ, Vulink NC, Denys D. Role of Sexuality in Body Integrity Identity Disorder (BIID): A Cross-Sectional Internet-Based Survey Study. J Sex Med 2017;XX:XXX-XXX.
Article
Some people have a profound dissatisfaction with what is considered an able-bodied state by most others. These individuals desire to be disabled, by conventional standards. In this Review, we integrate research findings about the desire for a major limb amputation or paralysis (xenomelia). Neuropsychological and neuroimaging explorations of xenomelia show functional and structural abnormalities in predominantly right hemisphere cortical circuits of higher-order bodily representation, including affective and sexual aspects of corporeal awareness. These neural underpinnings of xenomelia do not necessarily imply a neurological cause, and a full understanding of the condition requires consideration of the interface between neural and social contributions to the bodily self and the concept of disability. Irrespective of cause, disability desires are accompanied by a disabling bodily dysphoria, in many respects similar to gender dysphoria, and we suggest that they should be considered a mental disorder.