ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Note-taking is a commonly applied pedagogical strategy across all areas of education. In higher education specifically, there has been an increasing push to get students involved in collaborative note-taking in order to increase their engagement with the contents and to inspire deeper and more meaningful learning. However, there is a lack of clarity as to whether collaborative note-taking positively influences student performance. For this reason, the present study (n = 189) compares the learning performances of students in a collaborative note-taking condition to those of students in an individual note-taking condition. The students were compared in regards to their retention of information and their performance on academic writing. The study found that students from the collaborative note-taking group performed better on measures of retention, while the individual note-taking group performed better on measures of academic writing. These results suggest that while the collaborative processes of group note-taking lead students to retain more information, these processes do not lead to better performance in academic writing. The present study fills a gap in the research by showing how the effectiveness of collaborative note-taking might depend on the learning context or on the desired result of the class. ARTICLE HISTORY
Content may be subject to copyright.
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=nile20
Interactive Learning Environments
ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/nile20
How collaboration influences the effect of note-
taking on writing performance and recall of
contents
Mik Fanguy, Matthew Baldwin, Evgeniia Shmeleva, Kyungmee Lee & Jamie
Costley
To cite this article: Mik Fanguy, Matthew Baldwin, Evgeniia Shmeleva, Kyungmee Lee & Jamie
Costley (2021): How collaboration influences the effect of note-taking on writing performance and
recall of contents, Interactive Learning Environments, DOI: 10.1080/10494820.2021.1950772
To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/10494820.2021.1950772
© 2021 The Author(s). Published by Informa
UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis
Group
Published online: 12 Jul 2021.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 23
View related articles
View Crossmark data
How collaboration inuences the eect of note-taking on writing
performance and recall of contents
Mik Fanguy
a
, Matthew Baldwin
b
, Evgeniia Shmeleva
c
, Kyungmee Lee
d
and
Jamie Costley
c
a
EFL Department, Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology (KAIST);
b
EFL Program, School of
Humanities and Social Sciences, KAIST;
c
National Research University Higher School of Economics, Institute of
Education;
d
Lancaster University, The Department of Educational Research
ABSTRACT
Note-taking is a commonly applied pedagogical strategy across all areas
of education. In higher education specically, there has been an
increasing push to get students involved in collaborative note-taking in
order to increase their engagement with the contents and to inspire
deeper and more meaningful learning. However, there is a lack of
clarity as to whether collaborative note-taking positively inuences
student performance. For this reason, the present study (n = 189)
compares the learning performances of students in a collaborative
note-taking condition to those of students in an individual note-taking
condition. The students were compared in regards to their retention of
information and their performance on academic writing. The study
found that students from the collaborative note-taking group
performed better on measures of retention, while the individual note-
taking group performed better on measures of academic writing. These
results suggest that while the collaborative processes of group note-
taking lead students to retain more information, these processes do not
lead to better performance in academic writing. The present study lls a
gap in the research by showing how the eectiveness of collaborative
note-taking might depend on the learning context or on the desired
result of the class.
ARTICLE HISTORY
Received 8 May 2021
Accepted 28 June 2021
KEYWORDS
Collaborative note-taking;
collaborative writing; higher
education; retention
Introduction
As collaborative learning has been a prevalent pedagogical approach since the 1980s, the current
generation of instructors may see collaboration as a positive pedagogical approach suitable for
most, if not all, learning contexts (Menekse & Chi, 2019;ODonnell, 2006). More specically, collab-
oration is a commonly-used practice in higher education, where it has been integrated into curricula
across disciplines within both online and traditional on-campus classrooms (Nokes-Malach et al.,
2015). This is because much research into collaboration in higher learning environments suggests
it is of benet to learners (Johnson et al., 2014). However, in spite of its ubiquitous implementation
and a body of extant literature in its favor, questions remain regarding the inconsistent eectiveness
of collaboration on learning (Kester & Paas, 2005; Zambrano et al., 2019). For this reason, much
© 2021 The Author(s). Published by Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License (http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the
original work is properly cited, and is not altered, transformed, or built upon in any way.
CONTACT Jamie Costley jcostley@hse.ru National Research University Higher School of Economics, Institute of Education
This article has been corrected with minor changes. These changes do not impact the academic content of the article.
INTERACTIVE LEARNING ENVIRONMENTS
https://doi.org/10.1080/10494820.2021.1950772
research in the eld of education is focused on investigating the diering topics, contexts, and edu-
cational modalities that might be more or less appropriate for collaboration (Retnowati et al., 2017).
The act of individuals taking notes in lectures is a proven strategy that is deemed an essential
approach to learning (van de Sande et al., 2017). This importance manifests itself in improved
student learning and performance in class (Luo et al., 2018). Furthermore, engaging in note-
taking correlates with achievement (Fisher & Harris, 1973; van de Sande et al., 2017) because of
better retention and recall (Fisher & Harris, 1973; Rickards & Friedman, 1978), increased attention
to material (Kane et al., 2017; Kiewra, 1987), and the memory benets, i.e. storage and encoding,
that come from recording the notes (Peverly & Wolf, 2019). For these reasons, note-taking is preva-
lent with students participating in oine and online classes (Liu et al., 2019; Veletsianos et al., 2015).
Some researchers have suggested that collaborative note-taking in particular might be more
eective than taking notes individually (Harbin, 2020). This is because it may be the case that
note-taking places a cognitive burden on learners, which might be better resolved when working
in a group rather than individually (Chen et al., 2021). The cognitive challenge of trying to compre-
hend, process, and record information may be divided among group members, allowing students to
retain more of the information from collaborative note-taking (Orndor,2015; Shi et al., 2020). If lear-
ners share the workload of note-taking among a group, individual memberscognitive capacities will
be freed up, leading to higher levels of learning (Kirschner et al., 2018).
Because of the potential benets of collaborative note-taking, some universities actively encou-
rage instructors to promote collaborative note-taking (Laudari, 2019). Therefore, researchers have
started to investigate how eective collaborative note-taking is and in what contexts it can be
most readily applied (Veletsianos et al., 2016). Much of the research into the area of collaborative
note-taking is based on perceived learning as self-reported by students and instructors, not grounded
in class assessment or direct measures of performance. Regardless, such research has shaped the
discussion on collaborative learningsecacy and led to its widespread use. Consequently, more
needs to be known about whether collaborative learning is suitable in the context of note-taking,
and the present study seeks to develop a more nuanced empirical discussion of the topic.
Literature review
Student note-taking in the context of higher education is seen to be an eective strategy to improve
student learning (Wu, 2020). Aside from the benets of taking notes for oneself, there has been
research that suggests sharing notes, and taking or reviewing notes in groups is also benecial.
Kiewra (1989) found that those who borrowed notes from attendees that did not attend the
lecture themselves performed similarly on assessment measures to those who originally took and
reviewed the notes. Luo et al. (2016) found that students who collaborated with a partner to
revise their notes, recorded more original and complete information. In the case of an oine syn-
chronous class, students who participated in a collaborative note-taking condition using shared
Google Documents achieved on average a letter grade higher than their peers in the control
group who took the same course (Orndor,2015). In an asynchronous online learning environment,
Balwin et al. (2019) saw better learning outcomes for group note-takers than those in the control
group, who were advised to take notes individually. However, a key limitation in both of the
studies (Baldwin et al., 2019; Orndor,2015) is that the researchers only monitored and examined
the collaborative note-taking documents. Consequently, it is unknown how much note-taking
members of the control group actually did. In these cases, it is possible that the learning eects
of collaborative note-taking were compared to the eects when no notes were taken at all.
In regards to the amount students write when they take notes and their learning performance,
there is a large corpus of research literature showing a positive relationship between the quantity
of words in studentswritten notes and their learning outcomes (Haynes et al., 2015; Kiewra,
1987). Mueller and Oppenheimer (2014) found that the number of words in studentsnotes was
positively correlated with their ability to recall concepts from lectures they attended. Consequently,
2M. FANGUY ET AL.
volume has been regarded in the literature as an important measure of the quality of the notes stu-
dents take. Research has shown that collaborative note-takers take a larger volume of notes than
individual note-takers and tend to perform better on related exams (Kam et al., 2005), and it has
been suggested that increased volume in collaborative notes may help students generate more
ideas on the topics being focused on (Adeniran et al., 2019; Doberstein et al., 2019). However,
more voluminous notes may not always be best, as Mueller and Oppenheimer (2014) also found
that increased word counts correlated with reduced learning in cases where notes were written
as word-for-word transcriptions of the lectures. In such cases, students may become overburdened
with trying to copy down every word being spoken rather than thinking critically about the lecture
concepts and encoding those ideas to their working memories.
Collaboration has been shown to have a variety of eects depending on learning contexts. Col-
laboration in small groups has enabled greater academic achievement (Menekse & Chi, 2019;ODon-
nell, 2006) and better learning outcomes (Le et al., 2013). However, while reviewing group versus
individual work in a classroom setting, evidence for collaborative learningsecacy is mixed and
provides some evidence that those in a group perform worse than they would have alone
(Nokes-Malach et al., 2015; Retnowati et al., 2017). Included in that review is evidence of students
positive attitudes toward working in groups and the belief that their learning was of a higher stan-
dard than when they worked alone. Crucially though, the group members did not perform as well as
those who studied individually (Leidner & Fuller, 1997). The outcome of the eect, therefore, does
not always equate to what it is perceived to be.
Retention of learned information is an important aspect of education as a student needs to store
course material in long-term memory in a manner that allows it to be called upon at a future time
(Roediger & Karpicke, 2018). There is evidence that working together can aid retention of learning
material (Johnson et al., 2014). Collaboration has been seen to help those within a group retain
more class material when the individuals divide up a task and concentrate on dierent parts
(Tindale & Winget, 2017), whereas a meta-analysis by Marion and Thorley (2016) found that
working together in groups to memorize can help individuals recall information by themselves
later. It is worth noting that according to the encoding hypothesis, which relates to note-taking
directly, the act of note-taking in itself assists learning and the remembering of information.
However, dividing the task of note-taking among members of a group may reduce this positive
eect for each individual. This leads to two key questions: (1) How important is encoding? and (2)
If during collaboration, encoding is diminished, can this be countered by a potentially improved
product (storage) for the group members to review from?
Conversely, there are occasions when working together is detrimental to recall. The Retrieval
Strategy Disruption Hypothesis (Basden et al., 1997), whereby the output of one group member
impedes the retrieval processes of another, is an oft-given reason why collaborative inhibition
occurs (Marion & Thorley, 2016). The group consequently retrieves less information than individuals,
as collaboration may disrupt learnersability to construct their own knowledge (Abel & Bäuml, 2017).
Further disruption to retention has been noted in the form of cognitive transactional costs (Kirschner
et al., 2009) - the mental time and eort spent assisting or listening to others in the group - expend-
able resources that might be better employed in learning the material by oneself.
Despite the cognitive transactional cost as well as retrieval strategy disruption, the act of working
in groups has the potential to aid practice and performance of a task. Through the activity itself,
participants in a group may recognize gaps in their own learning and seek guidance from their
peers (Doo et al., 2020;Shinetal.,2020). Those more knowledgeable about a subject may give infor-
mation or suggestions, such as ways to approach a goal or an explanation for information another
student nds confusing. In these ways, collaboration facilitates scaoldingthat is, it enables group
members to do that which they could not have done without the assistance of others (Vygotsky, 1978).
Transfer of learning, often seen as the goal of learning, is the application of knowledge one has
acquired in the past, to a new, similar context (Pan & Rickard, 2018). A typical practice in higher edu-
cation is for instructors to convey new theory or knowledge in the classroom and have students
INTERACTIVE LEARNING ENVIRONMENTS 3
practically apply it to a problem or scenario. In this regard, unstructured/structured small-group
learning has been found to have a positive eect on studentslater individual attempts at learning
transfer (Pai et al., 2015). Doing group activities provides an opportunity to practice a skill; however,
how the task is divided between members could restrict that opportunity. An instance where this can
be crucial is second language learning (L2), and in particular when learning to write in an L2. The
intricacies of academic writing dictate that a L2 learner needs to practice the dierent writing
skills through individual experience (Myles, 2002). Observing someone else conducting a writing
skill in a group is not the equivalent of performing it oneself, rst hand. This lack of application
could diminish the positive eect that practice alone has on accuracy in essay writing (Robb
et al., 1986).
The present study
The present study seeks to measure and compare the learning eects of two approaches to note-
taking in a course featuring online video instruction: (1) individual note-taking, wherein each
student is responsible for recording his/her own set of notes, and (2) collaborative note-taking,
wherein students are responsible for taking notes collaboratively in shared online documents in
small groups. To do so, participants were divided into two groups, with one group taking notes indi-
vidually and the other taking notes collaboratively in small groups. As prior experimental studies on
collaborative note-taking (Baldwin et al., 2019; Orndor,2015) have not monitored the amount of
notes that were taken in the control (non-collaborative) condition, in the present study, students
online note-taking documents from the individual and collaborative note-taking conditions were
created and monitored by the course instructor. In this way, the amount of notes taken by students
from each condition could be assessed. Learning outcomes were measured in two ways. Students
ability to recall contents from the online lecture videos were measured through their individual
scores on online quizzes, as quizzes are widely acknowledged as a useful measure of learnerscom-
prehension of learning content (Herold et al., 2012; Kamuche, 2011). Studentswriting ability, which
is the focus of the scientic writing course examined in this study, was assessed by evaluation of their
individual writing assignments. This study seeks to answer the following research questions:
RQ1: Does collaborative note-taking increase studentsrecall of course concepts from lecture videos as com-
pared to individual note-taking?
RQ2: Does collaborative note-taking increase studentswriting ability as compared to individual note-taking?
RQ3: Do individual note-takers write more than collaborative note-takers?
The study has three main hypotheses:
H1: Students in the collaborative group will earn higher scores on related quizzes than students taking notes
individually.
H2: Students in the collaborative group will earn higher scores on individual writing assignments than students
in the individual note-taking condition.
H3: The volume of notes taken by individual note-takers will be higher than that of constituent members of col-
laborative note-taking groups.
Methodology
Participants and learning context
There were 186 students engaged in online note-taking in 10 dierent course sections of a graduate
scientic writing course at a Korean university. All students who enrolled in the course were majoring
in STEM (science, technology, engineering, and math) elds. There were 825 students in each
4M. FANGUY ET AL.
course section. Each of the 189 students joined sections (classes) that were designated as either indi-
vidual or group note-taking. There were 6 sections designated as collaborative and 4 as individual
note-taking. Within the group note-taking condition, there were 27 groups with 3, 4, or 5
members. 128 subjects were masters students, and 58 were in a doctoral program. There were 48
females and 138 males. The average age of the students was 25.5 (SD = 2.5), with a minimum
value 22 and a maximum value 36.
The participants were divided into two groups, with one being a collaborative note-taking group
(64 students) and the other being an individual note-taking group (123 students) with a similar com-
position of gender and age. The group participants did not dier signicantly by gender (Χ2(1)
= .639, p= .424), age (t = 0.907, p= .365), or educational level as approximated by the pre-test
quizzes (t = .998, p= .319). The pre-test was a 10-item quiz given at the start of the semester. This
pre-test consisted of items from each of the weekly topics that the course covered to see the stu-
dentslevel of knowledge of the information covered in the course.
In the scientic writing course that was the focus of this study, graduate students learn to write a
manuscript on their research ndings for publication in an academic journal (Fanguy et al., 2021).
Course lectures were provided as online videos on the course learning management system in
streaming format. The course comprises 10 instructional weeks, and each week includes 48
lecture videos, with a total of 56 videos for the course. The average duration of the videos was
approximately 12 min, with the shortest video being 4:56 and the longest video lasting 24:50.
During each instructional week, learners were also requested to take notes on the video contents.
Students in the individual note-taking group were asked to take notes individually, while students in
the collaborative note-taking group were asked to do so in small groups of 35 students, which stu-
dents were allowed to self-select into (with instructions to try to keep groups to 4 or 5 students). The
notes that were taken by students in each treatment condition were taken using Google Documents
that were created and monitored by the professor teaching the course. Therefore, in the individual
note-taking group, each student in the course took notes in 10 individual Google Documents that
corresponded to the 10 weeks of course instruction. Similarly, in collaborative note-taking groups,
each group took notes in 10 shared Google Documents that corresponded to the 10 weeks of
course instruction. As the course videos were provided on the course learning management
system, students in both treatment groups could access the videos as often as they desired and
could rewind, pause, or fast-forward while note-taking. At the end of each week of instruction, all
learners were given an online quiz covering the learning content covered in the course videos of
that week. The instructor of the course encouraged all students from both conditions to refer to
the notes they had written on the online lecture videos when taking the related quiz. The quizzes
covered a variety of topics discussed in the lecture videos including academic writing conventions,
ethical issues related to the communication of scientic research, and navigation of the submission
and peer review process of academic journals. Such topics were deemed by the course instructor to
be appropriate to be assessed with quizzes. From the notes that students wrote in each of the two
treatment conditions, data was mined for the volume of words written.
Measures
Volume. The number of words contributed by each student to the nal version of each of 10 note-
taking documents during the semester was tallied, and this sum served as the volume variable in this
study. In the individual note-taking group, the total number of words written in all note-taking docu-
ments was used as the volume variable. In the collaborative note-taking group, the total number of
words contributed to all of the collaborative note-taking documents by each constituent group
member served as the volume variable. In order to operationalize the volume variable, the total
word count of each document was assessed using a program written in Python language (URL
removed for blinding).
INTERACTIVE LEARNING ENVIRONMENTS 5
Quiz scores. A total of 10 quizzes were given online during the semester in order to measure stu-
dentsrecall and understanding the content from video lectures during each of the 10 instructional
weeks. Each quiz consisted of 830 multiple-choice items based on the concepts of the online videos
from the corresponding instructional week. Students were given only one attempt to take each quiz,
and quiz attempts were timed with two minutes allowed for each question. Students were required
to take each quiz by 6 pm on the Friday of each instructional week. The quiz items were designed to
allow for more than one answer choice to be selected, and students were awarded partial credit
when fewer than the total number of correct options were selected. However, if an incorrect
answer option was selected, a score of 0 was given for the quiz item in order to discourage students
from indiscriminately guessing when they did not understand the learning content being measured.
The scores of each quiz were weighted equally in order to account for 3% of the course grade point
total. Therefore, the total 10 quiz scores, each of which was worth 3% of the grade total, were com-
bined to account for 30% of the grade point total for the course. The Cronbachs(1951) alpha coe-
cients for the respective weekly quiz scores were as follows: a= .68, .62, .60, .69, .81, .64, .78, .58, .65,
and .85. These results indicate that the quizzes provided a moderately reliable measure of the learn-
ing content of each instructional week. More information about each quiz item and its relationship to
the instruction can be viewed at the following URL under the label of quiz items and video list: URL
removed for blinding.
Individual writing assignments
The scientic writing course examined in the present study required students to submit ve individ-
ual writing assignments corresponding to six major sections of a research article: Introduction, Meth-
odology, Results, Discussion & Conclusion, Abstract, and References. Each of these assignments were
evaluated using rubrics (Appendix A) that were adapted from those proposed by Clabough and Cla-
bough (2016) and were scored by the course instructor on a scale of 0-10, with each assignment
accounting for 10% and all six assignments accounting for 60% of the course grade point total.
The summed scores of these six assignments were used as the individual writing variable in the
present study. To ensure reliability of rating, two instructors of the scientic writing course separately
rated 10 randomly selected writing samples as part of a norming session and discussed instances
where dierences in scoring occurred. After acceptable scoring calibration was achieved, the two
instructors simultaneously scored 20% of all writing samples submitted from all 6 sections of the
course being examined.
Survey. Furthermore, the students took a survey at the completion of the course. The survey was
administered to both treatment groups in the present study. Survey items covered various topics
including, student demographics, the usefulness of notes, the eectiveness of the Google Docu-
ments platform, and video viewing practices and habits. Two survey items of interest to the
present study were analyzed to see how they diered between the individual and group note-
taking condition: I always watched all the course videos, and Taking notes made me more likely to
rewatch parts or all of a given course video. These were Likert-like items from 1 - 7 which asked stu-
dents to respond to either very true of mewhich was a 7, to not at all true of me,which was a
1. These items both represent the way students interact with the materials in the course in relation to
their note-taking and therefore will help add to our understanding of the diering note-taking beha-
viors of the participants in the individual and collaborative note-taking conditions.
Results
Before comparing the treatment groups by learning outcomes, we used the ShapiroWilk test to test
the assumption of normality of data distribution and Levenes test to test the assumption of equal
variances (homoscedasticity). The results indicated that both assumptions hold for weekly quizzes
scores (F = .31, p= .579 for Levenes test, and W = 0.99, p= .304 for the ShapiroWilk test), but do
6M. FANGUY ET AL.
not hold for individual writing assignments (F = 4.85, p= .028 and W = 0.95, p< .000, respectively).
Therefore, we used a parametric t-test to check the signicance of the dierences in quizzes
scores and a nonparametric MannWhitney Utest to check the signicance of the dierences in indi-
vidual assignment scores.
The results show (Tables 12) that the collaborative note-taking group had a 1.38 higher average
weekly quiz scores than the individual note-taking group (hypothesis 1 is held), and this dierence
was statistically signicant (t = 3.67; p< .000). A comparison of the individual written assessment
scores reveals that the collaborative note-taking group had a 3.08 lower writing scores than the indi-
vidual note-taking group, and this dierence was statistically signicant (z = 5.25; p< .000). This pro-
vides evidence for the rejection of hypothesis 2.
We use the information about how students interacted with the materials to explain the dier-
ences in learning outcomes in the individual and group note-taking conditions (1) volume of
notes, (2) watching all the videos during the course, and (3) rewatching parts of the videos during
the course. All the variables fail the ShapiroWilk test for normality (W = .83, p= .000 for the rst vari-
able, W = 0.81, p= .000 for the second one, and W = .93, p= .000 for the third one). Only the variable
indicating watching all the videos during the course passes the test for homoscedasticity (F = 2.76, p
= 0.097). The assumptions for homoscedasticity for the rst and the third variables do not hold (F =
55.70, p= .000 and F = 7.50, p= .006, respectively). Therefore, we use a nonparametric Mann
Whitney Utest to check the signicance of the dierences in individual assignment scores.
As detailed in Table 3, the individual note-taking group had a 3582.97 signicantly higher volume
of notes (z = 7.82; p< .000; hypothesis 3 is held), a .34 higher score on the variable indicating watch-
ing all the course videos (z = 2.11; p= .034), and a .67 higher score on the variable indicating rewatch-
ing the videos (z = 2.41; p= .015) than the individual note-taking group. These results suggest
students from individual note-taking groups interacted with the course materials more than stu-
dents from the other treatment group.
Discussion
The students who were in the collaborative condition in the present study performed better on the
weekly quizzes than those in the individual condition. These quizzes provided a measure of the stu-
dents retention of materials from the courses online video lectures (Roediger & Karpicke, 2018). The
present study falls in line with other research that suggests that collaboration benets students
learning generally (Johnson et al., 2014), as well as retention of information specically (Marion &
Thorley, 2016). On the other hand, previous studies have suggested that collaboration may interrupt
a learners ability to retain and recall information and may also introduce a cognitive transaction cost
to completing the task as a group (Marsh & Rajaram, 2019), as learners must spend time and mental
eort in order to share information with one another. Moreover, when an individual group member
contributes dominantly to a learning task, other members may not get ample opportunities to
engage, which may hinder their learning (Hew & Brush, 2007). This may also have occurred in the
present study, as the amount of notes contributed was not always evenly balanced among group
members in the collaborative note-taking condition. However, while there may be some collabora-
tive inhibition, transaction costs, and unequal participation from the learner-to-learner interaction,
the present ndings suggest that the benets of reducing studentscognitive burden as well as
Table 1. The gender, age, and pre-tests results of the participants, N= 186
Gender Frequency Percent Age (Mean / SD) Pre-test quiz results (Mean / SD)
Individual note-taking group (N=63) Female 14 22.22 25.14 / 2.79 5.15 / 1.20
Male 49 77.78 25.36 / 2.37 5.23 / 1.27
Collaborative note-taking group
(N=123)
Female 34 27.64 25.11 / 1.90 5.36 / 1.38
Male 89 72.36 25.88 / 2.78 4.87 / 1.62
INTERACTIVE LEARNING ENVIRONMENTS 7
the increased amount of focus collaboration brings outweigh the aforementioned disadvantages of
collaboration.
The students who took notes individually outperformed the students who took collaborative
notes in regards to their performance on academic writing. These pieces of writing were the main
focus of the course and involved students completing an individual paper related to their own
area of research interest. The present studys results do not support research that shows that collab-
oration can help students notice gaps in their knowledge and allows scaolding and feedback from
other learners (Doo et al., 2020). However, the present studys results do correspond to those of
Leidner and Fuller (1997), who found that although students who worked in collaborative groups
expressed more interest in the learning content and perceived learning, while students who
worked individually exhibited better learning performance. Leidner and Fuller explained this
result by surmising that learning activities done by oneself allow the learner to process information
in a way that allows them to understand the information more deeply and apply skills from that pro-
cessing at a later time. Similarly, students in the individual note-taking condition of the present study
may have beneted from processing information on their own rather than in groups.
Several other variables were considered after the main hypotheses were investigated. The volume
of the individual note-taking and collaborative note-taking conditions were compared, and this
showed that individual note-takers took more than twice the amount of notes (in terms of word
Table 2. Student outcomes, t-test, and Mann-Whitney U test for the individual note-taking group and the collaborative note-
taking group
Weekly quiz scores Individual writing assignment scores
Individual note-taking group (N=63)
Mean 20.51 44.63
SD 2.44 3.16
Collaborative note-taking group (N=123)
Mean 21.89 41.55
SD 2.42 4.12
Test for dierences
Mean dierence 1.38 3.08
tvalue
a
3.67
zvalue
b
5.25
pvalue 0.00 0.00
Total (N=186)
Mean 21.43 42.59
SD 2.51 4.08
Notes: a t-test; bMann-Whitney Utest
Table 3. The means of the parameters of the course participation and Mann-Whitney U test for the individual note-taking group
and the collaborative note-taking group
Volume of
notes
Watching all the course
videos
Rewatching parts or all of a given course
video.
Individual note-taking group
(N=63)
Mean 6269.41 6.49 5.61
SD 3321.39 0.89 1.18
Collaborative note-taking group
(N=123)
Mean 2686.43 6.15 4.94
SD 1417.20 1.18 1.72
Test for dierences
Mean dierence 3582.97 0.34 0.67
zvalue 7.82 2.11 2.41
pvalue 0.000 0.034 0.015
Total (N=186)
Mean 3900.02 6.26 5.17
SD 2812.93 1.10 1.59
8M. FANGUY ET AL.
count) than collaborative note-takers. This nding seems intuitive as the collaborative note-takers
are sharing the amount of notes required for each class, leading them to write less individually.
This is of pedagogical importance because, if students practice a skill less, they will not perform
as well as those who have practiced the skill more (Robb et al., 1986). The present study supports
this and suggests that even though collaboration is often used as an instructional activity to encou-
rage studentspractice, it may in fact lead to less individual practice of the skill being applied. This is
particularly salient in the case where the skill being practiced either in a group or individually is close
in kind to the outcome variable of interest. In the case of the present study, academic writing and
note-taking are similar in that they are both writing. It has been demonstrated that when learners
are trying to improve their second language, practice is of great importance (Nalliveettil & Mahasneh,
2017). This suggests that the better results in regards to writing found for the individual note-takers
may be caused by those learners having more practice of the skills that the class is focused on as
compared to the collaborative note-takers.
Also, to further understand the dierences found in the main variables of interest, two survey
items of interest were assessed. The students were asked how true it was of themthat they
always watched all the course videos.In this regard, students in the individual note-taking con-
dition had higher average scores, meaning that they were more likely to watch all the videos. Fur-
thermore, the students were asked how true it was of themthat Taking notes made me more
likely to rewatch parts or all of a given course video.In this case, the individual note-takers also
scored higher on average than the collaborative note-takers. There are two possible explanations
for this: (1) that students who took individual notes were more focused because they were respon-
sible for taking notes for all the course videos without help from others, or (2) students in the col-
laborative condition felt that they did not need to watch all the videos as they had access to
peer-created notes. This shows that what is seen as an advantage in collaborative learning (distri-
bution of workload) may cause potential issues as it may lead to students being less engaged
with the course materials.
It should be noted that the issues with collaboration discovered in this studys results are a little
dierent from the free-rider eectnoted in the literature (Strijbos & De Laat, 2010). In the free-
rider eect, students rely on others within a group to help complete a group activity. This may or
may not have occurred in the present study; however, it may be the case that regardless of stu-
dentsperformance in collaborative activities, students will interact less with the course materials
because the nature of collaboration leads to fewer requirements for them to engage with the con-
tents. So while prior work on the free-rider eect describes it as an individual member gaining
benets from group labor with minimal contribution (Strijbos & De Laat, 2010), the present
study suggests that such an approach may come at the expense of the individuals own learning
outcomes. While prior studies on free-riding have tended to focus on self-reported perceived levels
of learning and satisfaction with collaboration among group members, the present study has
measured the individual learning performances of the members, providing useful insights into
the eects of free-riding. This shows how the present study distinguishes itself from previous
research and gives a more focused, in-depth understanding of collaborationseect on student
performance.
Pedagogical recommendations
This study nds that more nuance is required when applying collaborative learning in an online
setting. Generally speaking, there is a tendency to consider that more collaboration is always
better (Menekse & Chi, 2019;ODonnell, 2006). However, the present study nds this is not the
case. As can be seen from the results, while collaboration was benecial for the studentsretention
of information, it was better for the students academic writing to write notes individually. The over-
arching pedagogical recommendation that can be drawn from this is that context and objectives
play a large part in determining if collaborative learning should be implemented. Therefore, there
INTERACTIVE LEARNING ENVIRONMENTS 9
are four more specic pedagogical recommendations: (1) When retention is important, collaborative
note-taking is eective; (2) academic writing will show greater improvement if students work indi-
vidually on note-taking; (3) more writing practice will help with student writing performance; and
(4) group activities should be systematically designed to promote eective collaboration.
The rst recommendation is that collaboration has great benet in instances where the goal of
instruction is to help students better understand and recall concepts and information. Collabora-
tive activities where group members attempt to collectively record and build knowledge will
help to deepen their understanding of course concepts and improve retention. This occurs
because students can share the burden of recording information, which can free cognitive
resources to make deeper connections with the content (Costley & Fanguy, 2021). Collaborative
note-taking from this perspective is situational. When the course goals are focused around building
knowledge as opposed to practicing a particular skill then collaborative note-taking can be a good
pedagogical practice.
The second recommendation is that academic writing is a skill that requires practice from stu-
dents in order to improve and gain mastery (Johari, 2018; Myles, 2002; Nalliveettil & Mahasneh,
2017; Silliman et al., 2020). Therefore, academic writing courses should include substantial writing
assignments that require students to invest time and eort engaging in the writing process. The
third and perhaps more surprising recommendation of the present study is that such assignments
should be completed individually rather than in collaborative groups because students will gain
more practice when the practice aorded by the task is not shared among several members, but
is instead assigned to a single learner.
The fourth recommendation of the present study is that when instructors include collaborative
learning activities into their courses, care must be taken to ensure that the proper conditions
exist for meaningful collaboration to occur (Ellis & Han, 2020). For example, a prior study found
that members of a group that collaborated through a shared online instructional interface exhibited
higher levels of recall on a test than students in the control condition who studied oine in a non-
collaborative manner (Szewkis et al., 2011). The authors noted that for successful collaborative learn-
ing to occur, there are a number of necessary conditions: sharing a common goal, positive interde-
pendence among members, coordination and communication, individual accountability, awareness
of peerswork, and joint rewards. The fact that the online collaborative note-taking condition in the
present study contained all of these conditions may help to explain the similarity of the present
ndings to those of Szewkis et al. Because students were able to use the notes they created in
their groups in order to study for the weekly quizzes, they had a clear common goal, which also
helped to create a sense of interdependence or reliance on one another. Groups had to decide
amongst themselves how to divide the work and create the notes, but individuals could be held
accountable for their contribution since each group member, as well as the course instructor,
could clearly see who wrote each part of the notes. Therefore, members were aware of the contri-
butions or lack thereof of each member. Lastly, student groups who took high-quality notes
shared in the joint reward or benet of having a highly complete set of notes with which to
study for quizzes.
Aside from the specic case of note-taking and academic writing, the study results also emphasize
a more generalizable concept at play in collaborative learning situations. In many cases, collabor-
ation may lead to students engaging in less practice of a skill than they would if they were to
apply the skill on their own. While the underlying processes of collaborative learning might lead
them to enjoy the processes more and perhaps to retain the information better if they collaborate,
they will not have the ability to actively engage in the skill they are trying to develop. A simple
analogy may help to illustrate. Imagine students are learning how to do Cardiopulmonary Resuscita-
tion (CPR) on a practice dummy. In one case, there is a group of four learning this skill while sharing
one dummy, and in another case, an individual is practicing alone on one dummy. The group may
enjoy the process of learning how to do CPR due to interactions with their partners, and the pro-
cesses of collaboration may lead them to remember the steps of procedure better than the
10 M. FANGUY ET AL.
individual. However, the individual has four times the amount of time on task to practice on the
dummy. This analogy may help to demonstrate what is happening in the present study and in
other cases of collaboration: collaborative groups may have better understood and remembered
many of the concepts from the course due to their interactions with one another, but individual
note-takers did ve times as much writing to complete their notes and therefore gained valuable
writing practice. For this reason, careful consideration must be given to the type of contents that
the students are learning and whether the relative value of collaboration is outweighed by the les-
sening amount of practice the students will engage in.
Conclusion
This study compared the studentsperformances in online classes in both weekly quizzes and aca-
demic writing. The students were divided into a condition where they took notes individually, or in
collaborative groups. The results show that students in the collaborative condition performed better
on the quizzes, while the students in the individual note-taking condition performed better on the
writing tasks. This study provides new insights into how note-taking aects student performance
from a collaborative perspective. Previous studies have tended to look at either collaborative
note-taking or individual note-taking by themselves, and have not compared them as our study
has done.
The present study also brings a more balanced narrative into the recent conversations on the
eectiveness of collaborative learning activities for individual student learning performance.
Advanced communication technologies have enabled researchers and educators to realize the
social constructivist ideal of student learning as a collaborative knowledge construction process
in dierent pedagogical settings. A growing volume of literature has documented the positive
sides of computer-supported collaborative learning and associated challenges to implementing
those activities in classrooms. As the scholarship has been established and mature, we argue it
has reached its tipping point where more research eorts need to be exerted to develop a com-
prehensive account of the eects of collaborative learning activities across dierent subject matters
and intended learning outcomes. Learning is multi-focal endeavors, strongly inuenced by the
nature of a focused set of knowledge and skills. When it comes to the question of eective ped-
agogical approaches, therefore, the answers vary across disciplines and dierent stages of learner
development. In that regard, this particular study contributes to adding nuance to the literature by
showing that while collaboration may be an eective tool at improving the retention of lecture
contents, it reduces the amount of academic writing practice a student might engage in. That
is, those in the individual note-taking condition wrote twice as much as those in the collaborative
condition.
Despite these contributions, the present study has a number of limitations that must be
addressed. The rst is that the only aspect of the notes that was assessed was volume. However,
the quality of the notes, for example how many of the main concepts from the lectures are
written down, is another important aspect of note-taking quality that was not assessed. It is possible
that the improved retention of course concepts by the collaborative note-takers could have been
due to creating and having access to higher quality notes than those of individual note-takers;
however, as this study did not measure quality, such a relationship cannot be examined. Therefore,
future research should evaluate and compare the quality of notes taken between individual and col-
laborative note-takers. A second limitation is that this study used self-reported information from
survey items in order to assess studentsvideo viewing habits, and clickstream behavior would
have been a more eective method of doing so. As clickstream data was unavailable from the uni-
versity learning management system, future research should examine the relationship between col-
laborative note-taking and the tendency to view videos. Considering the ubiquitousness of note-
taking and benets to be gained from collaboration, this is an area that is rich for further potential
investigation.
INTERACTIVE LEARNING ENVIRONMENTS 11
Disclosure statement
No potential conict of interest was reported by the author(s).
Notes on contributors
Mik Fanguy is a visiting professor in the English as a Foreign Language Program at the Korea Advanced Institute of
Science and Technology (KAIST) in South Korea. His research interests include online collaborative writing and notetak-
ing and online and blended education.
Matthew Baldwin is a visiting professor in the English as a Foreign Language Program at the Korea Advanced Institute
of Science and Technology (KAIST). He holds an MA in TESOL and a BA in English language and literature. His research
interests include Content and Language Integrated Learning (CLIL), international education, online learning and ipped
class instruction.
Evgeniia Shmeleva is a Ph.D. candidate and a Research Fellow at the Centre of Sociology of Higher Education, Institute
of Education, National Research University Higher School of Economics. Her major research interests lie in the area of
student academic dishonesty, student attrition, online and distance learning, and integration of educational technol-
ogies at the secondary level of education.
Kyungmee Lee is a Lecturer in the Department of Educational Research, Lancaster University, and co-Director of the
Centre for Technology Enhanced Learning. Her research interests include understanding and supporting academic
and social experiences of non-traditional student groups in online higher education, including international students,
adult students, doctoral students, teachers and educational professionals.
Jamie Costley is an assistant professor in the Center for Sociology of Higher Education, Institute of Education at the
Moscow Higher School of Economics. He is interested in a variety of topics related to how to improve learning in
online environments, specically in the areas of collaborative learning, cognitive load, and instructional design.
ORCID
Mik Fanguy http://orcid.org/0000-0002-9383-1510
Matthew Baldwin http://orcid.org/0000-0001-9863-8544
Evgeniia Shmeleva http://orcid.org/0000-0001-8004-3315
Kyungmee Lee http://orcid.org/0000-0002-9580-9026
Jamie Costley http://orcid.org/0000-0002-1685-3863
References
Abel, M., & Bäuml, K. H. T. (2017). Collaborative remembering revisited: Study context access modulates collaborative
inhibition and later benets for individual memory. Memory & Cognition,45(8), 13191334. https://doi.org/10.
3758/s13421-017-0737-9
Adeniran, A., Mastho, J., & Beacham, N. (2019). Model-based characterization of text discourse content to evaluate
online group collaboration. In S. Isotani, E. Millán, A. Ogan, P. Hastings, B. McLaren, & R. Luckin (Eds.), Articial intelli-
gence in education. AIED 2019. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Vol. 11626. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.
1007/978-3-030-23207-8_1
Basden, B. H., Basden, D. R., Bryner, S., & Thomas, R. L. III. (1997). A comparison of group and individual remembering:
Does collaboration disrupt retrieval strategies? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
23(5), 11761189. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.23.5.1176
Baldwin, M. P., Fanguy, M., & Costley, J. H. (2019). The eects of collaborative note-taking in ipped learning contexts.
Journal of Language & Education Volume, 5(4). https://doi.org/10.17323/jle.2019.9726
Chen, S., Wang, D., & Huang, Y. (2021, May 813). Exploring the complementary features of audio and text notes for video-
based learning in mobile settings. Extended Abstracts of the 2021 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing
systems. (pp. 17). https://doi.org/10.1145/3411763.3451801
Clabough, E. B., & Clabough, S. W. (2016). Using rubrics as a scientic writing instructional method in early stage under-
graduate neuroscience study. Journal of Undergraduate Neuroscience Education : JUNE : A Publication of FUN, Faculty
for Undergraduate Neuroscience,15(1), A85A93. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5105970/
Costley, J., & Fanguy, M. (2021). Collaborative note-taking aects cognitive load: the interplay of completeness and
interaction. Educational Technology Research and Development,69(2), 655671. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-
021-09979-2
12 M. FANGUY ET AL.
Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coecient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika,16(3), 297334. https://doi.
org/10.1007/BF02310555
Doberstein, D., Hecking, T., & Hoppe, H. U. (2019). What can interaction sequences tell us about collaboration quality in
small learning groups? In M. Herzog, Z. Kubincová, P. Han, & M. Temperini (Eds.), Advances in web-based learning
ICWL 2019. ICWL 2019. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Vol. 11841. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/
978-3-030-35758-0_6
Doo, M. Y., Bonk, C., & Heo, H. (2020). A meta-analysis of scaolding eects in online learning in higher education.
International Review of Research in Open and Distributed Learning,21(3), 6080. http://www.irrodl.org/index.php/
irrodl/article/view/4638
Ellis, R., & Han, F. (2020). Assessing university student collaboration in new ways. Assessment & Evaluation in Higher
Education,116. https://doi.org/10.1080/02602938.2020.1788504
Fanguy, M., Lee, S. Y., & Churchill, D. G. (2021). Adapting educational experiences for the chemists of tomorrow. Nature
Reviews Chemistry,5(3), 141142.
Fisher, J. L., & Harris, M. B. (1973). Eect of note taking and review on recall. Journal of Educational Psychology, 65(3), 321
325. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035640
Harbin, M. B. (2020). Collaborative note-taking: A tool for creating a more inclusive college classroom. College Teaching,
17. https://doi.org/10.1080/87567555.2020.1786664
Haynes, J. M., McCarley, N. G., & Williams, J. L. (2015). An analysis of notes taken during and after a lecture presentation.
North American Journal of Psychology,17(1), 175186. https://www.researchgate.net/prole/Joshua_Williams4/
publication/272417797_An_Analysis_of_Notes_Taken_During_and_After_a_Lecture_Presentation/links/
54e3a2000cf2dbf60693a790.pdf
Herold, M. J., Lynch, T. D., Ramnath, R., & Ramanathan, J. (2012, October). Student and instructor experiences in the
inverted classroom. 2012 Frontiers in Education Conference Proceedings.16. IEEE. https://doi.org/10.1109/FIE.2012.
6462428
Hew, K. F., & Brush, T. (2007). Integrating technology into K-12 teaching and learning: Current knowledge gaps and rec-
ommendations for future research. Educational Technology Research and Development,55(3), 223252. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s11423-006-9022-5
Johari, S. K. (2018). The eects of task-based process writing approach on the academic writing skills among second
language tertiary learners. Journal of ELT Research: The Academic Journal of Studies in English Language Teaching
and Learning,120. https://doi.org/10.22236/JER_Vol3Issue1pp1-20
Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R. T., & Smith, K. A. (2014). Cooperative learning: Improving university instruction by basing
practice on validated theory. Journal on Excellence in University Teaching,25(4), 126. Retried from http://personal.
cege.umn.edu/~smith/docs/Johnson-Johnson-Smith-Cooperative_Learning-JECT-Small_Group_Learning-draft.pdf
Kam, M., Wang, J., Iles, A., Tse, E., Chiu, J., Glaser, D., Canny, J. (2005, April 27). Livenotes: A system for cooperative and
augmented note-taking in lectures. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing systems.
(pp. 531540). https://doi.org/10.1145/1054972.1055046
Kamuche, F. U. (2011). The eects of unannounced quizzes on student performance: Further evidence. College Teaching
Methods & Styles Journal (CTMS),3(2), 2126. https://doi.org/10.19030/ctms.v3i2.5277
Kane, M. J., Smeekens, B. A., von Bastian, C. C., Lurquin, J. H., Carruth, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2017). A combined experimental
and individual-dierences investigation into mind wandering during a video lecture. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General,146(11), 16491674. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/xge0000362 https://doi.org/10.1037/
xge0000362
Kester, L., & Paas, F. (2005). Instructional interventions to enhance collaboration in powerful learning environments.
Computers in Human Behavior,21(4), 689696. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2004.11.008
Kiewra, K. A. (1987). Notetaking and review: The research and its implications. Instructional Science,16(3), 233249.
https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/BF00120252.pdf https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00120252
Kiewra, K. A. (1989). A review of note-taking: The encoding-storage paradigm and beyond. Educational Psychology
Review,1(2), 147172. https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/BF01326640.pdf https://doi.org/10.1007/
BF01326640
Kirschner, F., Paas, F., & Kirschner, P. A. (2009). A cognitive load approach to collaborative learning: United brains for
complex tasks. Educational Psychology Review,21(1), 3142. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-008-9095-2
Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., Kirschner, F., & Zambrano, J. (2018). From cognitive load theory to collaborative cognitive
load theory. International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning,13(2), 213233. https://doi.org/10.
1007/s11412-018-9277-y
Laudari, S. (2019). Collaborative note-takingin adaptable resources for teaching with technology. LX.Lab, Institute for
Interactive Media & Learning, University of Technology, Sydney. https://lx.uts.edu.au/collections/adaptable-
resources/resources/collaborative-note-taking/
Le, N. T., Loll, F., & Pinkwart, N. (2013). Operationalizing the continuum between well-dened and ill-dened problems
for educational technology. IEEE Transactions on Learning Technologies,6(3), 258270. https://doi.org/10.1109/TLT.
2013.16
INTERACTIVE LEARNING ENVIRONMENTS 13
Leidner, D. E., & Fuller, M. (1997). Improving student learning of conceptual information: GSS supported collaborative
learning vs. individual constructive learning. Decision Support Systems, 20(2), 149163. https://doi.org/10.1016/
S0167-9236(97)00004-3
Liu, C., Yang, C. L., Williams, J. J., & Wang, H. C. (2019, May 49). Notestruct: Scaolding note-taking while learning from
online videos.InExtended Abstracts of the 2019 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing systems. (pp. 16).
https://doi.org/10.1145/3290607.3312878
Luo, L., Kiewra, K. A., Flanigan, A. E., & Peteranetz, M. S. (2018). Laptop versus longhand note taking: Eects on lecture
notes and achievement. Instructional Science,46(6), 947971. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-018-9458-0
Luo, L., Kiewra, K. A., & Samuelson, L. (2016). Revising lecture notes: How revision, pauses, and partners aect note taking
and achievement. Instructional Science,44(1), 4567. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-016-9370-4
Marion, S. B., & Thorley, C. (2016). A meta-analytic review of collaborative inhibition and postcollaborative memory:
Testing the predictions of the retrieval strategy disruption hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin,142(11), 11411164.
https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000071 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/27618544/ https://doi.org/10.1037/
bul0000071
Marsh, E. J., & Rajaram, S. (2019). The digital expansion of the mind: Implications of internet usage for memory and cog-
nition. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition,8(1), 114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.11.001
Menekse, M., & Chi, M. T. (2019). The role of collaborative interactions versus individual construction on studentslearn-
ing of engineering concepts. European Journal of Engineering Education,44(5), 702725. https://doi.org/10.1080/
03043797.2018.1538324
Mueller, P. A., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2014). The pen is mightier than the keyboard: Advantages of longhand over laptop
note taking. Psychological Science,25(6), 11591168. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614524581
Myles, J. (2002). Second language writing and research: The writing process and error analysis in student texts. Tesl-Ej,6
(2), 120. http://www. tesl.ej.org/wordpress/issues/volume6/ej22al/
Nalliveettil, G. M., & Mahasneh, A. (2017). Developing competence in basic writing skills: Perceptions of EFL undergradu-
ates. International Journal of Applied Linguistics and English Literature,6(7), 323341. https://doi.org/10.7575/aiac.
ijalel.v.6n.7p.332
Nokes-Malach, T. J., Richey, J. E., & Gadgil, S. (2015). When is it better to learn together? Insights from research on col-
laborative learning. Educational Psychology Review,27(4), 645656. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9312-8
ODonnell, A. M. (2006). The role of peers and group learning. In P. A. Alexander, & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of edu-
cational psychology (pp. 781802). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
OrndorIIIH. N. (2015). Collaborative note-taking: The impact of cloud computing on classroom performance.
International Journal of Teaching and Learning in Higher Education,27(3), 340351. https://les.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/
EJ1093744.pdf
Pai, H. H., Sears, D. A., & Maeda, Y. (2015). Eects of small-group learning on transfer: A meta-analysis. Educational
Psychology Review,27(1), 79102. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-014-9260-8
Pan, S. C., & Rickard, T. C. (2018). Transfer of test-enhanced learning: Meta-analytic review and synthesis. Psychological
Bulletin, 144(7), 710756. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000151
Peverly, S. T., & Wolf, A. D. (2019). Note-taking. In J. Dunlosky, & K. A. Rawson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of cognition
and education (pp. 320355). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108235631.014
Retnowati, E., Ayres, P., & Sweller, J. (2017). Can collaborative learning improve the eectiveness of worked examples in
learning mathematics? Journal of Educational Psychology,109(5), 666679. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000167
Rickards, J. P., & Friedman, F. (1978). The encoding versus the external storage hypothesis in note taking. Contemporary
Educational Psychology,3(2), 136143. https://doi.org/10.1016/0361-476X(78)90020-6
Robb, T., Ross, S., & Shortreed, I. (1986). Salience of feedback on error and its eect on EFL writing quality. TESOL
Quarterly,20(1), 8396. https://doi.org/10.2307/3586390
Roediger IIIH. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2018). Reections on the resurgence of interest in the testing eect. Perspectives on
Psychological Science,13(2), 236241. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617718873
Shi Y., Yang H., Yang Z., Liu W., & Yang H. H. (2020). The eects of a collaborative learning approach with digital note-
taking on college studentslearning achievement and cognitive Load. In S. Cheung, R. Li, K. Phusavat, N. Paoprasert,
& L. Kwok (Eds), Blended learning. Education in a smart learning environment. ICBL 2020. Lecture Notes in Computer
Science, Vol. 12218. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-51968-1_16
Shin, S., Brush, T. A., & Glazewski, K. D. (2020). Examining the hard, peer, and teacher scaolding framework in inquiry-
based technology-enhanced learning environments: Impact on academic achievement and group performance.
Educational Technology Research and Development,125. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-020-09763-8
Silliman, E. R., Bahr, R. H., & Wilkinson, L. C. (2020). Writing across the academic languages: Introduction. Reading and
Writing,33(1), 111. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09993-0
Strijbos, J. W., & De Laat, M. F. (2010). Developing the role concept for computer-supported collaborative learning: An
explorative synthesis. Computers in Human Behavior,26(4), 495505. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2009.08.014
Szewkis, E., Nussbaum, M., Rosen, T., Abalos, J., Denardin, F., Caballero, D., Alcoholado, C. (2011). Collaboration within
large groups in the classroom. International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning,6(4), 561575.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11412-011-9123-y
14 M. FANGUY ET AL.
Tindale, R. S., & Winget, J. R. (2017). Learning While Deciding in Groups. The Oxford handbook of group and organiz-
ational learning.https://psyarxiv.com/8ufgh/download?format=pdf
van de Sande, C., Abramson, J., & Judson-Garcia, J. (2017). An exploration of note-taking in an online calculus course.
Journal of Computers in Mathematics and Science Teaching,36(1), 7599. https://www.learntechlib.org/primary/p/
174372/
Veletsianos, G., Collier, A., & Schneider, E. (2015). Digging deeper into learnersexperiences in MOOC s: Participation in
social networks outside of MOOC s, notetaking and contexts surrounding content consumption. British Journal of
Educational Technology,46(3), 570587. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjet.12297
Veletsianos, G., Reich, J., & Pasquini, L. A. (2016). The life between big data log events: Learnersstrategies to overcome
challenges in MOOCs. AERA Open,2(3), 2332858416657002. https://doi.org/10.1177/2332858416657002
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Socio-cultural theory. Mind in Society,5258.
Wu, J. Y. (2020). The predictive validities of individual working-memory capacity proles and note-taking strategies on
online search performance. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning,36(6), 876889. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcal.12441
Zambrano, J., Kirschner, F., Sweller, J., & Kirschner, P. A. (2019). Eects of group experience and information distribution
on collaborative learning. Instructional Science,47(5), 531550. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-019-09495-0
INTERACTIVE LEARNING ENVIRONMENTS 15
... Conversely, Shi and Yu (2024) found that laptop note-taking can improve learning performance and satisfaction when students are trained to manage these distractions effectively. Fanguy et al. (2023) examined the collaborative aspects of note-taking, showing that electronic platforms can enhance collaborative learning and content recall, though they may not always translate to improved writing performance. This highlights a critical consideration in choosing the appropriate method based on the learning context and objectives. ...
... This suggests that peer comments can be an effective way to support students' academic writing skills, as well as their note-taking accuracy. Similarly, Fanguy et al. (2023) investigated the impact of collaboration on note-taking and writing performance, specifically comparing individual note-taking to collaborative note-taking. Contrary to the previously stated summary, their study revealed that while collaborative note-takers did better on retention measures, they performed worse on writing assessments compared to individual note-takers. ...
... Analytical results from quantitative assessments align with the prevailing literature, highlighting the advantages of individual electronic note-taking in promoting academic writing abilities over collaborative methods, as supported by Kiewra (2016) and Shi et al. (2022) Additionally, the effect sizes identified between the control group and experimental groups 1 and 3 reinforce the notion that collaborative electronic note-taking exerts a minimal impact on academic writing proficiency. Building upon the work of Fanguy et al. (2023), this investigation explores the dynamics of collaboration in note-taking and its effect on writing efficiency. Unlike Fanguy et al., this study is firmly situated within a CALL framework, examining the relative effectiveness of collaborative versus individual electronic notetaking strategies in enhancing the academic writing skills of graduate students. ...
Article
Full-text available
This investigation sought to discern the impacts of different electronic note-taking strategies—collaborative, individual, and a hybrid of both—on the academic writing skills of Chinese EFL graduate students. The study engaged 185 participants, systematically assigned to one of four groups: those practicing collaborative electronic note-taking, those employing individual strategies, a group combining both approaches, and a control group receiving no specific electronic note-taking intervention. The evaluation of academic writing skills was conducted through pre- and post-test assessments. In addition, participants’ perceptions regarding the efficacy of electronic note-taking were collected through questionnaires and interviews. Analysis of the academic writing post-test scores unveiled a statistically significant influence of the note-taking strategy on the enhancement of academic writing skills, with the metrics revealing (F(3, 81) = 15.055, p < 0.001, η^2 = 0.358). This indicates a pronounced improvement in the writing capabilities of students engaged in electronic note-taking activities compared to those in the control group. Noteworthy is the performance of the individual note-taking group, which outstripped the control group’s outcomes. However, comparisons within the electronic note-taking strategies—between collaborative and individual approaches or against the combined strategy—did not yield significant disparities. Feedback from the questionnaire and interviews painted a positive picture of electronic note-taking’s role in academic writing skill development. Participants acknowledged several advantages, including but not limited to, the facilitation of idea diversity, the promotion of interactive learning environments, and the enhancement of writing strategies. Such qualitative insights corroborate the quantitative findings, underscoring the efficacy of electronic note-taking, in its various forms, as a potent tool for advancing the academic writing skills of Chinese EFL graduate students. This body of evidence advocates for the integration of electronic note-taking methodologies into educational curricula aimed at improving note-taking and academic writing proficiencies. It also signals the necessity for further explorations into the longitudinal impacts of these strategies on academic achievements and their adaptability across diverse educational settings. The collective data from this study enrich our understanding of electronic note-taking’s potential to transform learning outcomes in higher education.
... Regarding the impact of collaborative learning on academic performance, various empirical studies were conducted (Alalwan et al., 2019;Al-Rahmi et al., 2018;Manickam et al., 2020). Based on the literature carried out by the researchers, some of them determined the role of collaborative learning in the context of students' performance (Amadu et al., 2018;İlçin et al., 2018), which includes writing performance (Fanguy et al., 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to identify the mediating effect of collaborative learning on the relationship between Learning Management System (LMS) usage and academic performance among undergraduate students in a public university through the constructivism theory. The study employed a purposive sampling survey method, and 381 valid respondents were collected. Data analysis was conducted based on the Partial Least Square Structural Equation Model (PLS-SEM). As a result, LMS usage positively impacts the academic performance of the students. Besides, LMS usage significantly impacts collaborative learning, and collaborative learning positively influences academic performance. Lastly, collaborative learning was found to exert a partial mediating effect on the relationship between LMS usage and academic performance. This research contributes to the existing literature and illustrates that collaborative learning through LMS can enrich learning activities and promote group discussions. Conclusion, implications, and avenues for future research are discussed.
... Writing in groups also help improve social skills and self-confidence (Mutwarasibo, 2014) in providing peer-feedback at discussion stage (Ismail et al., 2020). However, individual learning is more suitable for developing academic writing skills that require focus and independence (Fanguy et al., 2023). Learning inside and outside the classroom complements each other, such as reinforcing the identity of a confident writer, and influencing students' engagement in academic tasks (Bankier, 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
Proficiency in multimodal writing is becoming progressively crucial for students, particularly in the realm of academic writing. Nevertheless, the existing learning materials in higher education fail to adequately meet those requirements. Hence, the goal of this study is to create a workbook that will assist students in improving their ability to write using various modes in academic writing classes. This study employs Research and Development, with a specific focus on the Analysis and Design phases of the ADDIE approach. The investigation had a total of 114 participants. We employed triangulation techniques to develop a questionnaire and gathered data in July 2024. Subsequently, we analyzed and displayed the data using descriptive statistics, elucidating its meaning in accordance with the pre-established categories. The requirements assessment revealed that students necessitate support in academic writing, including obtaining credible sources, employing technological tools, completing plagiarism checks, and maintaining citations. Students desire a customizable learning structure that allows them to produce multimodal (digital) texts and engage in activities both individually and collaboratively. The needs analysis determines the structure of the multimodal project workbook, which consists of five sections arranged according to the syntax of project-based learning. To achieve more effective and practical outcomes, it is critical to proceed with the research by carrying out the Development and Implementation stages.
... Other research shows how language changes from source text to notes to a summary text (Hood, 2008). Still others revealed that collaborative notetaking does not improve academic writing performance, though this process leads to the retention of more information among the students (Fanguy et al., 2021), which suggests that social interaction may affect the outcomes of note-taking in different ways. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Note-taking is prevalent in academia—it is the basis of scholarly work, i.e. searching for information, collecting and reading literature, writing and collaborating, referred to as a “primitive” that assists these information activities (e.g., Palmer, C. L., Teffeau, L. C., & Pirmann, C. M. (2009). Scholarly information practices in the online environment: Themes from the literature and implications for library service development. OCLC Research. https://accesson.kisti.re.kr/upload2/i_report/1239602399570.pdf ). Researchers and higher education students take notes throughout the inquiry cycle, i.e. while designing research, collecting data, analysing data, and writing the report. In addition, with written assignments being a considerable part of student academic work, notes are taken in the writing process, from generating ideas for writing tasks, through text planning and drafting to its editing. As this process may be challenging, digital note-taking has the potential to facilitate writing in academic contexts (Matysek, A., & Tomaszczyk, J. (2020). Digital wisdom in research work. Zagadnienia Informacji Naukowej—Studia Informacyjne, 58(2A(116A)), 98–113. https://doi.org/10.36702/zin.705 ). Yet, despite the availability of literature concerning formal requirements of writing, such as style, structure, referencing, etc., relatively little literature deals with the note-taking activity that assists academic writing, and even less with digital note-taking. In order to bridge this gap, this chapter focuses on the note-taking activity and shows how digital tools can support note takers in the academic writing context.
Article
Full-text available
With the recent data revealing the performance status of Filipino learners in science aggravated by the negative effects of the pandemic, a concerted effort is directed toward exploring how learners learn science concepts in the current educational landscape to address issues of poor conceptual understanding and a learning gap. This qualitative study explored the perceptions of STEM learners on different ways of learning science concepts and how teachers developed the habits and attitudes of learning science among their students to develop conceptual understanding. Using a descriptive phenomenological approach, a semi-structured interview guide was conducted among 15 teachers with their corresponding students (n = 60) to explore their strategies and purposeful ways of learning and understanding concepts in science. Thematic analysis revealed five themes that describe STEM learners’ science learning process: extended reading, recalling, and rewriting; seeking help from MKO (more knowledgeable others); self-directed learning; acquisition from learning resources; and experiential learning. Both learners’ and teachers’ narratives emphasized the need for diverse approaches catering to the needs, interests, and contexts of every individual to promote the science of learning among students. While learners are struggling to concretize abstract concepts in science, their varied intentional science of learning enables them to navigate in the learning process.
Article
This study explores the impact of an innovative approach that combines artificial intelligence (AI) chatbot support with collaborative note-taking (CNT) in the comprehension of semantic terms among English as a Foreign Language (EFL) learners. Given the significance of semantics in English language learning, traditional didactic methods often present challenges for EFL learners. The proposed AI chatbot-supported approach aims to foster learner interaction, while the CNT strategy focuses on enhancing knowledge retention and engagement with learning materials. Conducted as a quasi-experimental pre-test-post-test design, the study involved 60 English language and literature majors from a non-English-speaking area enrolled at a private university. Participants were divided into the AI chatbot-supported and CNT (AI-CNT) group and the conventional CNT (cCNT) group. Results indicated that the AI-CNT group outperformed the cCNT group across various dimensions of semantic learning outcomes, including performance, achievement, self-efficacy, metacognition, and anxiety reduction. This study highlights the potential of integrating AI chatbot support and the CNT strategy to significantly enhance the EFL semantic learning experience. The personalized and interaction-based linguistic practices, enriched with feedback and emotional support, offer a promising avenue for advancing language learning outcomes in the digital age. Language(s) Learned in This Study: English APA Citation: Chen, M. R. A. (2024). The AI chatbot interaction for semantic learning: A collaborative note-taking approach with EFL students. Language Learning & Technology, 28(1), 1-25. https://hdl.handle.net/10125/73586
Article
Collaboration has an essential role in memory, and how to appropriately use it to affect individual memory positively is a matter of concern. The meta‐analysis generally assessed the effect of collaboration on subsequent individual retrieval, registered on the PROSPERO platform and adhering to the PRISMA guidelines, using the Web of Science, Science Direct, CNKI and WanFang databases with post‐collaborative memory as the main subject, screened studies published up to December 31, 2023, a total of 64 studies with 101 effect sizes, including 13,398 participants from 11 countries. Heterogeneity test, sensitivity analysis, subgroup analysis and meta‐regression analysis were performed on the included studies, while publication bias was assessed. The results found that collaboration improves subsequent individual retrieval memory more than individuals, and collaboration has a moderate facilitating effect on subsequent individual retrieval. Group size, material category, category size, collaboration phase, collaboration approach, task process and test method were among the moderating variables. The study emphasizes the role of collaboration in cognition and demonstrates the post‐collaborative benefits. The conclusions are of value for developing methods to improve individual memory.
Article
While videoconferencing is prevalent, concurrent participation channels are limited. People experience challenges keeping up with the discussion, and misunderstanding frequently occurs. Through a formative study, we probed into the design space of providing real-time transcripts as an extra communication space for video meeting attendees. We then present MeetScript, a system that provides parallel participation channels through real-time interactive transcripts. MeetScript visualizes the discussion through a chat-alike interface and allows meeting attendees to make real-time collaborative annotations. Over time, MeetScript gradually hides extraneous content to retain the most essential information on the transcript, with the goal of reducing the cognitive load required on users to process the information in real time. In an experiment with 80 users in 22 teams, we compared MeetScript with two baseline conditions where participants used Zoom alone (business-as-usual), or Zoom with an adds-on transcription service (Otter.ai). We found that MeetScript significantly enhanced people's non-verbal participation and recollection of their teams' decision-making processes compared to the baselines. Users liked that MeetScript allowed them to easily navigate the transcript and contextualize feedback and new ideas with existing ones.
Article
Full-text available
Studies showing improved learning performances for students who take notes collaboratively have speculated that sharing this task among group members may reduce the extraneous cognitive burden placed on each member. Therefore, a study (n = 171) was conducted in the context of a flipped scientific writing course to examine the effects of collaborative note-taking on student’s levels of cognitive load. Students in the course were divided into two groups, with members of the treatment group being directed to take collaborative notes in a shared online document and members of the control group receiving no such instructions. The study also measured the level of collaboration the collaborative note-takers engaged in, as well as the level of completeness of the notes that they produced. The results showed that, firstly, the treatment group reported higher levels of both germane and extraneous cognitive load compared to those of the control group, meaning that collaborative note-takers experienced higher levels of understanding of course content as well as increased confusion. Secondly, the level of collaboration was positively and significantly correlated with levels of germane load (understanding), but not with extraneous load (confusion). Thirdly, no correlation was found between completeness of notes and cognitive load. Accordingly, the authors suggest that collaborative note-taking is worthwhile, as the gains to students’ understanding of course content outweigh the disadvantages of increased confusion.
Article
Full-text available
Technical universities are constantly experimenting with innovative student-centred classroom approaches. We describe flipped and linked classroom approaches in the context of our ongoing chemical education theme of catalysis.
Article
Full-text available
Taking notes is a popular activity for students attending instructional lectures and has been linked to achievement. Research on note-taking has shown that the contents and usefulness of student notes depends on several factors, including the pace of the lecture and the presence of organizational and selection cues. However, this research has focused on note-taking from live lectures, often in a laboratory setting. The study presented here explored note-taking from video lectures by students who were genuinely invested in doing well in an online course. Of particular interest were participation patterns, whether the placement and completeness of content within a video was correlated with its inclusion in student notes, and whether the regularity and quality of note-taking was correlated with exam performance in a problem-solving course. Results showed that student participation was consistent over time, that leading worked examples within a video were salient, but that example completeness was not a factor in inclusion, and that note-taking participation and quality was not positively correlated with exam performance. This observational study sets the stage for future experimental research to adapt what we know about traditional note-taking and its relationship to achievement to online educational environments .
Article
Full-text available
The significance of scaffolding in education has received considerable attention. Many studies have examined the effects of scaffolding with diverse groups of participants, purposes, learning outcomes, and learning environments. The purpose of this research was to conduct a meta-analysis of the effects of scaffolding on learning outcomes in an online learning environment in higher education. This meta-analysis included studies with 64 effect sizes from 18 journal articles published in English, in eight countries, from 2010 to 2019. The meta-analysis revealed that scaffolding in an online learning environment has a large and statistically significant effect on learning outcomes. The meta-cognitive domain yielded a larger effect size than did the affective and cognitive domains. In terms of types of scaffolding activities, meta-cognitive scaffolding outnumbered other types of scaffolding. Computers as a scaffolding source in an online learning environment were also more prevalent than were human instructors. In addition, scholars in the United States have produced a large portion of the scaffolding research. Finally, the academic area of language and literature has adopted scaffolding most widely. Given that effective scaffolding can improve the quality of learning in an online environment, the current research is expected to contribute to online learning outcomes and learning experiences.
Article
Full-text available
Lay Description What is already known about this topic Working memory capacity is fundamental to multitasking ability. Online search is a typical multitasking behavior that requires constant switching between different information sources. Students who take note perform better than those who do not while reading materials with seductive but irrelevant information. What this paper adds Students working memory capacity is examined under silent and irrelevant speech conditions. Students exhibited different profile patterns in their working memory capacity under silent and irrelevant speech conditions. Taking note, either in matrix form or free form, can significantly improve students' online search performance for those with high WMC ONLY in irrelevant speech or for those with low WMC in both silent and irrelevant speech conditions. Implications for practice Note‐taking as auxiliary support may facilitate students' integration and evaluation of the online information across multiple sources during their online search. Instructions on how to self‐construct matrix note are needed to help students develop advanced note‐taking skills for their learning involving online search.
Article
Groups are used to make many important societal decisions. Similar to individuals, by paying attention to the information available during the decision processes and the consequences of the decisions, groups can learn from their decisions as well. In addition, group members can learn from each other by exchanging information and being exposed to different perspectives. However, groups make decisions in many different ways and the potential and actual learning that takes place will vary as a function of the manner in which groups reach consensus. This chapter reviews the literature on group decision making with a special emphasis on how and when group decision making leads to learning. We argue that learning is possible in virtually any group decision-making environment, but freely interacting groups create the greatest potential for learning. We also discuss when and why groups may not always take advantage of the learning potential.
Chapter
The purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of a collaborative learning approach with a digital note-taking method on college students’ learning achievement and cognitive load in computer network courses. An experiment was conducted using a sample of 42 students from a class of a university in central China. Students were randomly divided into three groups, with each group consists of 14 students. The experimental group A employed a collaborative learning approach with a digital note-taking method and the experimental group B that employed a conventional lecturing approach with a digital note-taking method, while the control group that employed a conventional lecturing approach and traditional note-taking with pen and paper. Students in the classes studied computer network courses for 6 weeks. The pre- and post-tests showed that the students with a collaborative problem-solving approach and employ the digital note-taking method in classroom instruction have a significantly higher learning achievement while significantly lower extraneous load than that of students with the conventional lecturing approach and traditional note-taking with pen and paper.
Article
This study argues for the importance of using the different evidence to assess and evaluate a key graduate skill – collaboration. To do so, it investigates the experience of 356 first-year students in a blended course design and measures their collaborative patterns. Combining research methodologies from student approaches to learning and social network analysis, the results reveal evidence of different collaborative patterns across the population sample. The investigation uncovers contrasting groupings of students with deep and surface approaches to inquiry and to online learning technologies, positive and negative conceptions of the learning environment, and relatively higher or lower academic outcomes. These are discovered to logically relate to different collaborative patterns. The most effective collaboration strategies involve collaborating only as much as tasks needed, in smaller groups, and being reciprocal by accepting and inviting peers to work together. Effective collaboration strategies also include students positioning themselves to gather information easily in their collaboration networks and to develop closely knit collaborative groups. The results offer an evidence-base to identify different experiences of student learning and collaboration to improve program design and the attribute of collaboration, and to improve the concepts underpinning policy development for quality improvement of university graduates.
Article
Collaborative note-taking is a pedagogical technique that asks students to rotate note-taking responsibilities during class meetings in a shared document. Implementing this technique helped me, as an instructor, better leverage my students’ strengths and weaknesses in an introductory American government class—though the benefits should extend to a wide range of disciplinary contexts. In particular, collaborative note-taking helped level the playing field for students entering my classroom with wide-ranging levels of prior preparation. It also provided a consistent access point for evaluating student comprehension and learning and improved the quality of classroom discussion. As a result, implementing this pedagogical approach can help foster a more collaborative, inclusive, and equitable learning environment in university classrooms.