Content uploaded by Dr. Snežana Stupar-Rutenfrans
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Dr. Snežana Stupar-Rutenfrans on Jul 22, 2021
Content may be subject to copyright.
International Journal of Intercultural Relations 84 (2021) 79–85
Available online 20 July 2021
0147-1767/© 2021 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Ethnic outgroup aggression: A pilot study on the importance of
emotion regulation, nationalism and susceptibility to persuasion
Sneˇ
zana Stupar-Rutenfrans
a
,
*, Petrouschka C.D. Verdouw
a
, Jedidja van Boven
a
,
Olga Aleksandrovna Ryzhkina
b
, Anastasia Batkhina
c
, Idil Aksoz-Efe
d
,
Oriola Hamzallari
e
, Penny Papageorgopoulou
f
, Fitim Uka
g
, Nebojˇ
sa Petrovi´
c
h
,
Arta Statovci
i
, Miranda Rutenfrans-Stupar
j
, Daniela Garbin Praniˇ
cevi´
c
i
,
Skerdi Zahaj
k
, Eric Mijts
l
a
University College Roosevelt (Utrecht University), Lange Noordstraat 1, 4331 CB, Middelburg, The Netherlands
b
Novosibirsk State University, 1 Pirogova Street, Novosibirsk, 630090, Russia
c
National Research University Higher School of Economics, 20 Myasnitskaya Ulitsa, Moscow, 101000, Russia
d
Ataturk University, 25240, Erzurum, Turkey
e
Aleksander Moisiu University, Durr¨
es L. 1. Stream Street Durr¨
es, Durr¨
es, 2000, Albania
f
National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Athens, 157 72, Greece
g
University of Prishtina "Hasan Prishtina", Rr. "George Bush", Nr. 31, 10 000, Prishtin¨
e, Kosovo
h
University of Belgrade, 1 Studentski trg, 11000, Belgrade, Serbia
i
University of Split, Poljiˇ
cka cesta 35, 21000, Split, Croatia
j
Tilburg University, Warandelaan 2, 5037 AB, Tilburg, The Netherlands
k
Tirana University, Kutia Postare Nr 183 / D¨
eshmor¨
et e Kombit Street, Tirana, Albania
l
University of Aruba, J. Irausquinplein 4, Aruba
ARTICLE INFO
Keywords:
Emotion regulation
Outgroup aggression
Intergroup anxiety
Nationalism
ABSTRACT
The current pilot study investigated the psychological mechanisms behind ethnic outgroup
aggression, a signicant outcome of intergroup conicts. While previous research suggested
several impactful predictors of ethnic outgroup aggression, such as intergroup contact and
nationalism, no attempt has been made to synthesize all these constructs into a single cross-
cultural study. Building on existing research, this pilot study is the rst to assess a rened
framework where we tested a proposed mediation model according to nationalism and emotion
regulation mediate the relationship between intergroup contact, susceptibility to persuasion, and
intergroup anxiety on the one hand and ethnic outgroup aggression on the other hand within a
cross-cultural sample. An online questionnaire was distributed using convenience sampling
among 2482 students with an ethnic majority background living and studying in ten (European)
countries. Multigroup path analysis supported the larger part of the hypothesized model where
we found that emotion regulation partially mediated the relationship between susceptibility to
persuasion as a predictor and aggression as an outcome. As expected, we found that the higher the
susceptibility to persuasion, the higher the emotion regulation, and the higher the regulation, the
lower the aggression in all countries. Our pilot study provided preliminary evidence that emotion
regulation, nationalism and susceptibility to persuasion are critical for the understanding of
ethnic outgroup aggression in ethnically diverse societies. Future research needs to be carried out
* Corresponding author at: University College Roosevelt, Utrecht University, The Netherlands.
E-mail address: s.stuparrutenfrans@ucr.nl (S. Stupar-Rutenfrans).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
International Journal of Intercultural Relations
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ijintrel
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2021.07.004
Received 25 August 2020; Received in revised form 7 July 2021; Accepted 9 July 2021
International Journal of Intercultural Relations 84 (2021) 79–85
80
focusing on the development of an intergroup anxiety assessment in which possible gender dif-
ferences in assessed constructs are considered.
Introduction
Ethnic outgroup aggression, dened as negative thoughts or behaviors that are explicitly directed towards the outgroup’s ethnic
background (Smith & Mackie, 2005), is an important outcome of intergroup conicts that are widely present in today’s society.
Previous research conrmed that ethnic outgroup aggression is predicted by low intergroup contact (Schmid, Hewstone, Küpper, Zick,
& Tausch, 2014), high intergroup anxiety (Brown, 2011), one’s high susceptibility to persuasion (Kaptein, Markopoulos, de Ruyter, &
Aarts, 2009), emotion regulation (Spanovic, Lickel, Denson, & Petrovic, 2010), and high nationalism (Mummendey, Klink, & Brown,
2001). To the best of our knowledge, the attempt to synthesize and explain the relationships among all these predictors and ethnic
outgroup aggression in one single study within a larger cross-cultural sample has not yet been undertaken. We seek to address this
knowledge gap by proposing and examining a model (see Fig. 1) in ten countries in which nationalism and emotion regulation (ER)
mediate the relationship between intergroup contact, susceptibility to persuasion, and intergroup anxiety on the one hand and (ethnic
outgroup) aggression on the other hand.
Traditionally, ethnic outgroup aggression is predicted by intergroup contact (direct contact between members of different groups;
Schmid et al., 2014), intergroup anxiety (concerns that intergroup contact will have adverse outcomes; Brown, 2011), and suscep-
tibility to persuasion (an individual’s behavioural compliance to persuasive cues; Kaptein et al., 2009). Increased intergroup contact
limits the polarization of intergroup relations and improves attitudes towards outgroups (Schmid et al., 2014). Furthermore, inter-
group contact promotes outgroup acceptance (as it reduces prejudice whereas both in- and outgroups become aware of the similarities
and collectiveness between the groups rather than characterizing the differences; Capozza, Di Bernardo, & Falvo, 2017), improves
attitudes toward outgroups (contact hypothesis; Allport, 1954), lowers nationalistic attitudes (attitudes towards the importance of
one’s country; Boin, Fuochi, & Voci, 2020; Coryn, Beale, & Myers, 2004), and limits outgroup aggression (Saab, Harb, & Moughalian,
2017). Intergroup anxiety, on the other hand, has substantial adverse effects, such as higher cognitive biases towards the outgroup, as
well as polarized group judgements and prejudices; the resulting feelings of threat can lead to higher nationalism (Halperin et al., 2012;
Viki & Calitri, 2008) and outgroup aggression as fear causes an increase in ingroup members’ support of the use of violence (Brown,
2011). This is not surprising as fear is linked to defensive aggression to avoid being attacked (Mifune, Simunovic, & Yamagishi, 2017).
Empirical studies show that fear can lead to defensive aggression if the costs of engaging in conict are low enough (Iyer, Hornsey,
Vanman, Esposo, & Ale, 2015). Intergroup anxiety, therefore, plays a signicant role in the justication of these aggressive acts to-
wards outgroups (Schaller & Neuberg, 2012). Susceptibility to persuasion is also essential for the expression of aggression (Carlson,
Marcus-Newhall, & Miller, 1990); however, the exact relationship between susceptibility to persuasion and (outgroup) aggression is
still unclear. Individuals who already experience higher levels of prejudice towards an outgroup are more susceptible to persuasion,
which creates stronger negative feelings towards the outgroup in question (Middleton, 1960). Because of the increasing pervasiveness
of social media in determining public opinion, we can speculate that political propaganda can increase the effectiveness of persuasion
when popular predispositions towards ethnic outgroups are used in this propaganda (Petrova & Yanagizawa-Drott, 2016). In addition,
ethnic conicts, which can take extreme forms like genocide, have been linked to attitudes towards authority and social hierarchy:
increased authoritarianism in one’s personality increases the likelihood of endorsing social hierarchies and suppressing groups below
one’s own (Green & Seher, 2003). These traits contribute to an individual’s susceptibility to persuasion by authorities in particular and
make them more likely to develop negative attitudes towards outgroups. These negative attitudes then lead to outgroup aggression
(DeWall, Anderson, & Bushman, 2011). Further exploration of the susceptibility to persuasion-aggression relationship, therefore,
contributes signicantly to the existing body of literature.
We propose that ER (ability to manage one’s emotions through activation of strategies such as expressive suppression and cognitive
reappraisal; Gross, 2015), has a mediating effect on the relationship between intergroup anxiety and outgroup aggression (Trawalter,
Fig. 1. Hypothesized model of intergroup anxiety, nationalism, emotion regulation and ethnic outgroup aggression.
S. Stupar-Rutenfrans et al.
International Journal of Intercultural Relations 84 (2021) 79–85
81
Richeson, & Shelton, 2009), and the relationship between susceptibility to persuasion and outgroup aggression (Mackie, Smith, & Ray,
2008). Intergroup anxiety has been commonly operationalized as a degree to which people experience anxiety-related emotions/states
(such as being anxious, nervous, tense, upset), as assessed during the hypothetical interaction with ethnic outgroups (Intercultural
Anxiety Scale, IAS; Stephan & Stephan, 1992). In that sense, intergroup anxiety is a broader emotional experience similar to stress and
negative affect (Stephan, 2014). Previous research shows that ER can in itself be subject to modication in a stressful situation where
experienced stress diminishes ER due to the cognitive demand it requires (Kinner, Het, & Wolf, 2014). As this study focusses on ER
strategies that are cognitively demanding (e.g. expressive suppression; Gross & John, 2003), it is expected that participants who
experience high levels of intergroup anxiety will apply ER to a lesser extent. Subsequently, low ER, especially in response to strong
negative emotions like (intergroup) fear, fuels intergroup aggression (Spanovic et al., 2010) while greater ER is negatively related to
racial prejudice, implying that higher ER could help to reduce negative behaviours toward outgroups (Mackie et al., 2008). Unsur-
prisingly, interracial interactions are appraised as stressful and thus facilitate intergroup anxiety; this can negatively affect ER as
increased anxiety lowers the ability to suppress negative emotions (Trawalter et al., 2009). Building upon previous research indicating
that ER is a mediating variable in the relationship between variables similar to constructs of our interests (e.g. ingroup identication
and the generation of group emotions, such as pride; Goldenberg, Halperin, van Zomeren, & Gross, 2016), we hypothesize that
intergroup anxiety predicts outgroup aggression via ER.
We also propose that nationalism has a mediating effect on the relationship between intergroup anxiety and outgroup aggression
(Brown, 2011) and the relationship between intergroup contact and outgroup aggression (Tausch et al., 2010). Nationalism has
commonly been operationalized as nationalistic attitudes (attitudes towards the importance of one’s country; Coryn et al., 2004) and
nationalistic identity (strong feelings of group identication; Viki & Calitri, 2008). Both of these notions have been associated with
higher outgroup aggression (Mummendey et al., 2001). The hypothesized mediation relationships are consistent with previous
research, such as Spanovic’ model for the relationship between fear and outgroup aggression (2010).
The current pilot study
We proposed and tested a mediation model in which intergroup anxiety, susceptibility to persuasion, and intergroup contact are
associated with ethnic outgroup aggression through nationalism and ER (Fig. 1). Thereby, we suggested that intergroup anxiety is
positively correlated with nationalism (Viki & Calitri, 2008); that lower intergroup contact creates stronger nationalistic tendencies
(Schmid et al., 2014); and that individuals susceptible for persuasion score higher on nationalism (Snyder & Ballentine, 1996). We also
propose that intergroup anxiety leads to lower ER (Trawalter et al., 2009). Although susceptibility to persuasion is commonly asso-
ciated with ER as well, the direction of this relationship is still unclear (Lewinski, Fransen, & Tan, 2014). Finally, we expect that low ER
and high nationalism will be associated with high aggression (Mummendey et al., 2001; Spanovic et al., 2010). We tested the model in
ten (European) countries and we expected the hypothesized model to be valid in all ethnic groups.
Method
Participants
The convenience sample consisted of 2482 university students with ethnic majority background situated in large cities in 10
countries: Russia, Albania, Turkey, Greece, Serbia, Montenegro, Kosovo, Croatia, Aruba, and The Netherlands. No ethnic minorities
were included in the study. To obtain majorities, demographics of the participants were assessed with questions on country of birth (of
student and their parents) and country of residence, a 10 point Likert scale asking the extent to which someone identies as a
mainstreamer of the country, a question asking whether students have the nationality of the country of residence, and an open question
where students could enter if they have another (additional) nationality. Online (back-translated) questionnaires were offered in the
country’s ofcial languages. The participants were (77.5 % women) aged 16–40 (M =22, SD =1.75), with 398 Albanian (85.7 %
women), 84 Aruban (73.8 % women), 174 Croatian (94.3 % women), 188 Dutch (66.0 % women), 168 Greek (48.8 % women), 394
Kosovars (88.1 % women), 205 Montenegrin (74.1 % women), 458 Russian (78.6 % women), 202 Serbs (75.2 % women), and 211
Turks (66.4 % women).
Measures
ER strategies, expressive suppression (four items) and cognitive reappraisal (six items), were assessed using the 10-item Emotion
Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross & John, 2003). A 7-point Likert scale was used ranging from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 7 (Strongly
agree). An example of an item is “When I want to feel more positive emotions, I change the way I am thinking about the situation”.
Nationalism was assessed using two separate scales, nationalistic attitudes that consisted of ve items related to attitudes toward the
importance of one’s own country, adopted from an empirical study in 22 countries (Coenders, 2001), and nationalistic identity, a 5-item
National Identication Scale (NIS; Verlegh, 2001). Items were rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 5
(Strongly agree). An example of an item is: “I am proud to be [Dutch]”.
Intergroup anxiety was assessed using the 15 items of the Intercultural Anxiety Scale (IAS; Stephan & Stephan, 1992). A 10-point
Likert scale ranging from 1 (Not at all) to 10 (Extremely) was used when participants were asked to report the degree to which they
would experience 15 anxiety-related emotions/states during the hypothetical interaction with foreigners (people with a different
ethnic background than one’s own) such as anxious, nervous, tense, and upset. A sample item reads: “Meeting strangers and introducing
S. Stupar-Rutenfrans et al.
International Journal of Intercultural Relations 84 (2021) 79–85
82
yourself”, with a response ranging from 1 (not at all nervous) to 10 (extremely nervous).
Contact with ethnic outgroups was assessed with the question “How often do you have contact with foreigners (people from a different
ethnic background than yours)?” on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (every day).
Susceptibility to persuasion was assessed by the 12-item Susceptibility Questionnaire (Kaptein et al., 2009). Items were rated on a
7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The scales covered six dimensions of susceptibility (two items
per dimension), namely reciprocation (an example of an item: “When I am in a new situation, I look at others to see what I should do.”),
scarcity, authority, commitment, consensus and liking.
Aggression was measured by 24 items of The Normative Beliefs about Aggression Scale (NOBAGS; Huesmann, Guerra, Zelli, &
Miller, 1992), which assessed the acceptance of aggressive behaviour in two situations; 1) under varying specic conditions of
provocation, namely retaliation belief questions (16 items) and 2) when no conditions are specied (the general aggression belief
questions - eight items). The scale is also validated for older adults and various ethnic groups (Huesmann et al., 1992). The NOBAGS
has been revised to assess interactions between ethnic groups (Shechtman & Basheer, 2005). Ethnic outgroup aggression was assessed
during different hypothetical situations: native versus native, foreigner (a person with a different ethnic background than one’s own)
versus foreigner, native versus foreigner, and foreigner versus native. Terms related to verbal (screaming and verbally hurting others)
and relational (cutting off the contact and avoiding other) aggression responses were included. The nal version of the aggression scale
used in this study consisted of 24 items; ethnic outgroup aggression contained 16 items divided across four interaction situations as
previously indicated, and the general aggression belief that contains eight items divided across the two subscales: general approval of
verbal aggression and general approval of behavioural aggression, consisting of four items each. With the analysis, a 4-point Likert
scale ranging from 1 (It’s perfectly OK) to 4 (It’s really wrong) was recoded into 1 (It’s really wrong) to 4 (It’s perfectly OK) to make the
scores and extent of aggression proportional. An example of an item is: “Do you think it’s wrong for the [Dutch] to say something hurtful to
the foreigner (person who has different ethnic background than one’s own)?”.
Internal consistency and factor analysis
Internal consistencies of all scales used in the current study were satisfactory (Cronbach’s alpha range: .67–.91) in all samples from
all countries. Multi-group conrmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted in AMOS (Arbuckle, 2006) to explore dimensionality and
to test for measurement invariance (Byrne, 2004) across all ethnic groups. Results related to measurement invariance and dimen-
sionality are both promising and in line with previous literature. However, due to varying sample sizes from different countries (e.g.
few samples are rather small to identify valid factor structure and measurement invariance), we were not able to examine whether
respondents from different countries interpret the same measure in a conceptually similar way, and whether factorial structure is fully
applicable for all samples.
Results
Emotion regulation, nationalism and outgroup aggression: the mediation model
First, a hypothesized model (Fig. 1) without mediators was tested in a multigroup path analysis using AMOS (Arbuckle, 2006).
Aggression was constructed based on two variables, general approval of (verbal and behavioural) aggression, and approval of ethnic
outgroup aggression. The structural weights model was the most restrictive model with a good t,
χ
2
(282, N =2482) =492.678,
p<.001;
χ
2
/df =1.747, the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) being .975 and the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) being
.017. Higher scores on susceptibility to persuasion were signicantly associated with lower aggression scores (general and ethnic
outgroup related) in all groups. However, the hypothesized direct effects of intergroup anxiety and contact with ethnic outgroups on
aggression were not conrmed as they were both non-signicant.
Fig. 2. The nal model of susceptibility to persuasion, emotion regulation, nationalism and aggression.
Note: Standardized regression coefcients are provided next to the arrows. The numbers inside the latent variable circles (emotion regulation and
aggression) represent the proportions of variance explained.
***p <.001.
S. Stupar-Rutenfrans et al.
International Journal of Intercultural Relations 84 (2021) 79–85
83
Second, a hypothesized mediation model was tested in which two predictors, namely intergroup anxiety and contact with ethnic
outgroups, were excluded. ER and aggression were categorized as latent variables. Indicators of ER were the two subscales, reappraisal
and suppression. The structural weights model was the most restrictive model with a good t,
χ
2
(460, N=2482) =886.251, p <.001;
χ
2
/df =1.927, CFI =.958 and RMSEA =.019. Additionally, the signicance of the indirect effect of susceptibility to persuasion on
aggression variable using bootstrapping was computed and was signicant (β= − .04; 95 % CI: −.09 to −.01; p<.01). Moreover, the
direct effect of susceptibility to persuasion on aggression was weaker yet signicant in the model with a mediator (β = − .22, p<.001)
compared to the model without a mediator (β = − .30, p <.001). Therefore, it can be concluded that ER can be treated as a partial
mediator in the susceptibility-aggression relationship.
Consequently, the nal model with ER as a partial mediator was tested (see Fig. 2). The structural weights model was the most
restrictive model with a good t,
χ
2
(459, N =2482) =879.556, p <.001;
χ
2
/df =1.916, CFI =.958 and RMSEA =.019. A model in
which ER partially mediated the relationship between susceptibility to persuasion as a predictor and (general and ethnic outgroup)
aggression as an outcome was supported. The higher the susceptibility to persuasion, the higher the ER, and the higher the ER, the
lower the aggression. Additionally, this model showed that the higher the nationalistic attitudes and the lower the nationalistic
identity, the higher the (general and ethnic outgroup) aggression. Results from multigroup path analyses conrmed similarities be-
tween structural parameters indicating no differences in the relationships between samples from different countries.
Discussion
We investigated a mediation model in which intergroup anxiety, susceptibility to persuasion, and intergroup contact are associated
with ethnic outgroup aggression through nationalism and ER. We found support for a model (in all countries) in which ER partially
mediates the relationship between susceptibility to persuasion as a predictor and aggression as an outcome. As expected, we found that
the higher the susceptibility to persuasion, the higher the ER (Lewinski et al., 2014), and the higher the ER, the lower the aggression
(DeWall et al., 2011).
Surprisingly, we found that the higher the nationalistic attitudes and the lower the nationalistic identity, the higher the aggression
in all countries (Mummendey et al., 2001). Our ndings suggest that the two different operationalizations of nationalism may lead to
different relationships to aggression; high scores on nationalistic identity may indicate higher interpersonal stability that further leads
to lower aggression in our participants. Being stable in terms of national identity could be predicted by coexisting high scores on
personality traits such as openness that is typically related to higher multicultural attitudes and lower ingroup-outgroup distance
(Stupar, Van de Vijver, Te Lindert, & Fontaine, 2014) which should be protective factors against outgroup aggression. When inter-
preting our ndings we should also take into account recent transformations that many of sampled countries undergone last decades
(e.g. collapse of USSR, Croatia gaining autonomy, socio-political changes in Turkey) which could have induced weakening of social
bonds within society (Social Bond Theory; Hirschi, 1969) and the sense of nationalistic identity (strong feelings of group identication;
Viki & Calitri, 2008) resulting in more aggression toward outgroups (Hirschi, 1969).
We could not conrm the mediating role of nationalism as hypothesized in the proposed model and the hypothesized direct effects
of intergroup anxiety and contact with ethnic outgroups on (general and ethnic outgroup) aggression. A plausible explanation for this
may be the lower quality of the measures. Specically, contact with ethnic outgroups was measured by one single item assessing only
the frequency of contact with people from a different ethnic background than one’s own, but we did not specify how and in which
situations this contact took place. In previous research on intergroup contact, this construct has been assessed more explicitly utilizing
information related to the type of ethnic outgroups, the type of contact (online versus face-to-face), and the quantity and quality of
contact (Schmid et al., 2014). An additional limitation of this study may be the overgeneralization of the contact questions as different
types of contacts have not been explored, subsequently, this may have led to a lower understanding of the item and therefore generated
more invalid responses. Assessment of intergroup anxiety may also have been biased as we may have measured general arousal during
the intergroup interaction instead of intergroup anxiety, which does not necessarily occur only during the interaction with ethnic
outgroups. To our knowledge, there is no existing validated intergroup anxiety scale that specically measures anxiety purely related
to ethnic outgroup members, this limits the internal validity of this concept (Spanovic et al., 2010); one could also question whether
the development of such scale is valid as the more experimental (exposure) approach may be more reliable in capturing objective
aspects of experienced anxiety. The same applies for the assessment of ethnic outgroup aggression; we adapted NOBAGS in order to
assess aggression during interactions between ethnic outgroups while no validated scale exists that assesses this construct. Finally, the
current cross-sectional design can be seen as an important limitation for testing the causal relationships within our proposed mediation
model, hence an experimental or longitudinal design would provide stronger evidence for the theoretically proposed model.
Conclusion
In conclusion, we found support for a model in which ER partially mediates the relationship between susceptibility to persuasion
and aggression. We also made some surprising observations regarding the nationalism – ethnic outgroup aggression relationship that
may warrant further exploration. Moreover, further development of the intergroup anxiety assessment and aggression related to ethnic
outgroups is needed. Future research should focus on the development of an intergroup anxiety and ethnic outgroup aggression
assessment tool in which only anxiety and specic ethnic aggression will be measured, ideally before, during, and after intergroup
contact/conict. Thereby, researchers should consider taking into account several other variables such as gender, age and personality.
There may be gender differences in susceptibility to persuasion and aggression favoured by gender-related socialization; some studies
have shown that men are more aggressive than women in interpersonal and intergroup relationships (Yokota, 2018). Nevertheless,
S. Stupar-Rutenfrans et al.
International Journal of Intercultural Relations 84 (2021) 79–85
84
given the contemporary research on gender identity differentiation (e.g. cis versus non-cis genders), more research is needed in this
area. Age may also be relevant for the formation of the social attitude with young adults being more exposed to media globalization
possibly resulting in holding stronger multicultural attitudes and lower ingroup-outgroup distance (Stupar et al., 2014). Personality
traits may also play an important role in the proposed mediation model as, openness that is related to higher multicultural attitudes
and lower ingroup-outgroup distance (Stupar et al., 2014) may serve as a protective factor against outgroup aggression. Finally, it is
recommended to take into account not only the perspective of ethnic majorities (as we did in the current study), but also of ethnic
minorities when analysing data on intergroup relations, due to possible differences in contributions aimed at subordinating out-groups
(out-group aggression) from those aimed at defending the in-group against possible out-group aggression (in-group defence) (Carsten
et al., 2016).
Author’s contributions
Sneˇ
zana Stupar-Rutenfrans: design; collection, analysis and interpretation of data; drafting the article and revising it critically
Petrouschka C. D. Verdouw: collection and analysis of data; drafting and revising the article
Jedidja van Boven: revising the article
Olga Aleksandrovna Ryzhkina: collection of data
Anastasia Batkhina: collection of data
Idil Aksoz-Efe: collection of data
Oriola Hamzallari: collection of data
Penny Papageorgopoulou: collection of data
Fitim Uka: collection of data
Nebojˇ
sa Petrovi´
c: collection of data
Arta Statovci: collection of data
Miranda Rutenfrans-Stupar: collection of data
Daniela Garbin Praniˇ
cevi´
c: collection of data
Skerdi Zahaj: collection of data
Eric Mijts: collection of data.
Ethical compliance statement
The authors have no funding to disclose. All procedures performed in the current study were in accordance with the ethical
standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable
ethical standards. We declare that we have no conict of interest. Informed consent was obtained from all individual adult participants
included in the study.
Acknowledgements
We thank several collaborators who helped us to collect data in different countries: Prof. dr. Anna Solomonovskaya (Novosibirsk
State University, Russia), Prof. dr. Nadezhda Lebedeva (National Research University Higher School of Economics, Russia), Prof. dr.
Igor Kolesov (Altai State Pedagogical University, Russia), Prof. dr. Vladimir Takˇ
si´
c (University of Rijeka, Croatia), dr. Jovana ˇ
Skori´
c
(University of Novi Sad, Serbia), dr. Mirjam Keteels and dr. Loes Keijsers (Tilburg University, the Netherlands), dr. Gero Lange
(Radboud Nijmegen, the Netherlands), Drs. Arnoud Versluis (Breda University of Applied Sciences, the Netherlands), Suzanne van
Kuijk MSc (Fontys, the Netherlands), and dr. Dimitris Charitos (National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Greece). We thank all
student participants for their contribution to our research, all translators that helped in back-to-translation of the questionnaires and
all UCR students that contributed to the data collection and analysis, especially Nineke Wilts. We also want to give special thanks to dr.
Katelyn E. Poelker (Saint Louis University) who generously gave us her input around the use of language in the current article. We are
also grateful for other resources obtained from Catharine van Tussenbroek Funds and the UCR research director, Prof. dr. Bert van den
Brink, which made possible discussing and sharing of (preliminary) results of the current study during several international occasions.
References
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison Wesley.
Arbuckle, J. L. (2006). Amos (Version 7.0) [Computer Program]. Chicago, IL: SPSS.
Boin, J., Fuochi, G., & Voci, A. (2020). Deprovincialization as a key correlate of ideology, prejudice, and intergroup contact. Personality and Individual Differences, 157,
Article 109799. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.109799.
Brown, R. (2011). Prejudice: Its social psychology. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Byrne, B. M. (2004). Testing for multigroup invariance using AMOS graphics: A road less travelled. Structural Equation Modeling, 11(2), 272–300. https://doi.org/
10.1207/s15328007sem1102_8.
Capozza, D., Di Bernardo, G. A., & Falvo, R. (2017). Intergroup contact and outgroup humanization: Is the causal relationship uni- or bidirectional? PloS One, 12(1),
Article e0170554. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0170554.
Carlson, M., Marcus-Newhall, A., & Miller, N. (1990). Effects of situational aggression cues: A quantitative review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 622.
https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.58.4.622.
Carsten, K. W., De Dreu, J. G., Zsombor, M., Michael, G., Eliska, P., & Jonathan, K. S. (2016). Columbus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(38),
10524–10529. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1605115113.
S. Stupar-Rutenfrans et al.
International Journal of Intercultural Relations 84 (2021) 79–85
85
Coenders, M. T. A. (2001). Nationalistic attitudes and ethnic exclusionism in a comparative perspective: An empirical study of attitudes toward the country and ethnic
immigrants in 22 countries. Doctoral dissertation. Radboud University Nijmegen.
Coryn, C. L., Beale, J. M., & Myers, K. M. (2004). Response to September 11: Anxiety, patriotism, and prejudice in the aftermath of terror. Current Research in Social
Psychology, 9(12), 165–183.
DeWall, C. N., Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2011). The general aggression model: Theoretical extensions to violence. Psychology of Violence, 1(3), 245. https://
doi.org/10.1037/a0023842.
Goldenberg, A., Halperin, E., van Zomeren, M., & Gross, J. J. (2016). The process model of group-based emotion: Integrating intergroup emotion and emotion
regulation perspectives. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20(2), 118–141. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868315581263.
Green, D. P., & Seher, R. L. (2003). What role does prejudice play in ethnic conict? Annual Review of Political Science, 6, 509–531. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.
polisci.6.121901.085642.
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840x.2014.940781.
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348.
Halperin, E., Crisp, R. J., Husnu, S., Trzesniewski, K. H., Dweck, C. S., & Gross, J. J. (2012). Promoting intergroup contact by changing beliefs: Group malleability,
intergroup anxiety, and contact motivation. Emotion, 12, 1192. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028620.
Hirschi, T. (1969). Causes of delinquency. Berkeley, California: University of California Press.
Huesmann, L. R., Guerra, N. G., Zelli, A., & Miller, L. (1992). Differing normative beliefs about aggression for boys and girls. In K. Bj¨
orkqvist, & P. Niemel¨
a (Eds.), Of
mice and women: Aspects of female aggression (pp. 77–87). Cambridge, MA: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-102590-8.50012-X.
Iyer, A., Hornsey, M. J., Vanman, E. J., Esposo, S., & Ale, S. (2015). Fight and ight: Evidence of aggressive capitulation in the face of fear messages from terrorists.
Political Psychology, 36, 631–648. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12182.
Kaptein, M., Markopoulos, P., de Ruyter, B., & Aarts, E. (2009). Can you be persuaded? Individual differences in susceptibility to persuasion. In IFIP Conference on
Human-Computer Interaction (pp. 115–118).
Kinner, V. L., Het, S., & Wolf, O. T. (2014). Emotion regulation: Exploring the impact of stress and sex. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 1–8. https://doi.org/
10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00397.
Lewinski, P., Fransen, M. L., & Tan, E. S. (2014). Predicting advertising effectiveness by facial expressions in response to amusing persuasive stimuli. Journal of
Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 7(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.1037/npe0000012.
Mackie, D. M., Smith, E. R., & Ray, D. G. (2008). Intergroup emotions and intergroup relations. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(5), 1866–1880. https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00130.x.
Middleton, R. (1960). Ethnic prejudice and susceptibility to persuasion. American Sociological Review, 679–686. https://doi.org/10.2307/2090140.
Mifune, N., Simunovic, D., & Yamagishi, T. (2017). Intergroup biases in fear-induced aggression. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 49. https://doi.org/10.3389/
fpsyg.2017.00049.
Mummendey, A., Klink, A., & Brown, R. (2001). Nationalism and patriotism: National identication and out-group rejection. The British Journal of Social Psychology,
40(2), 159–172. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466601164740.
Petrova, M., & Yanagizawa-Drott, D. (2016). Media persuasion, ethnic hatred, and mass violence. Economic Aspects of Genocides, Other Mass Atrocities, and Their
Prevention, 274. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199378296.003.0012.
Saab, R., Harb, C., & Moughalian, C. (2017). Intergroup contact as a predictor of violent and nonviolent collective action: Evidence from Syrian refugees and Lebanese
nationals. Peace and Conict Journal of Peace Psychology, 23(3), 297. https://doi.org/10.1037/pac0000234.
Schaller, M., & Neuberg, S. L. (2012). Danger, disease, and the nature of prejudice (s). In Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 46, pp. 1–54). Academic Press.
Schmid, K., Hewstone, M., Küpper, B., Zick, A., & Tausch, N. (2014). Reducing aggressive intergroup action tendencies: Effects of intergroup contact via perceived
intergroup threat. Aggressive Behavior, 40(3), 250–262. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21516.
Shechtman, Z., & Basheer, O. (2005). Normative beliefs supporting aggression of Arab children in an intergroup conict. Aggressive Behavior: Ofcial Journal of the
International Society for Research on Aggression, 31(4), 324–335. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20069.
Smith, E. R., & Mackie, D. M. (2005). Aggression, hatred, and other emotions. In J. Dovidio, P. Glick, & L. Rudman (Eds.), On the nature of prejudice: Fifty years after
allport (pp. 361–376). https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470773963.ch22.
Snyder, J., & Ballentine, K. (1996). Nationalism and the marketplace of ideas. International Security, 21(2), 5–40. https://doi.org/10.2307/2539069.
Spanovic, M., Lickel, B., Denson, T. F., & Petrovic, N. (2010). Fear and anger as predictors of motivation for intergroup aggression: Evidence from Serbia and
Republika Srpska. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 13(6), 725–739. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430210374483.
Stephan, W. G. (2014). Intergroup anxiety: Theory, research, and practice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 18(3), 239–255. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1088868314530518.
Stephan, C. W., & Stephan, W. G. (1992). Reducing intercultural anxiety through intercultural contact. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 16(1), 89–106.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0147-1767(92)90007-h.
Stupar, S., Van de Vijver, A. J. R., Te Lindert, A., & Fontaine, J. R. J. (2014). Multicultural attitudes mediate the relation between personality and perceived ethnic
outgroup distance in the Netherlands. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 38, 24–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2013.05.002.
Tausch, N., Hewstone, M., Kenworthy, J. B., Psaltis, C., Schmid, K., Popan, J. R., … Hughes, J. (2010). Secondary transfer effects of intergroup contact: Alternative
accounts and underlying processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(2), 282. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018553.
Trawalter, S., Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2009). Predicting behavior during interracial interactions: A stress and coping approach. Personality and Social
Psychology Review, 13(4), 243–268. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868309345850.
Verlegh, P. W. J. (2001). Country-of-Origin effects on consumer product evaluations. Doctoral dissertation. Wageningen, The Netherlands: Wageningen University.
Viki, G. T., & Calitri, R. (2008). Infrahuman outgroup or suprahuman ingroup: The role of nationalism and patriotism in the infrahumanization of outgroups. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 38(6), 1054–1061. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.495.
Yokota, K. (2018). The evolutionary roots of gender differences in aggressive behavior. Japanese Psychological Review, 60(1), 15–22. https://doi.org/10.24602/
sjpr.60.1_15.
S. Stupar-Rutenfrans et al.