ArticlePDF Available

Ethnic Outgroup Aggression: A Pilot Study on the Importance of Emotion Regulation, Nationalism and Susceptibility to Persuasion

Authors:
  • University College Roosevelt, Utrecht University

Abstract and Figures

The current pilot study investigated the psychological mechanisms behind ethnic outgroup aggression, a significant outcome of intergroup conflicts. While previous research suggested several impactful predictors of ethnic outgroup aggression, such as intergroup contact and nationalism, no attempt has been made to synthesize all these constructs into a single cross-cultural study. Building on existing research, this pilot study is the first to assess a refined framework where we tested a proposed mediation model according to nationalism and emotion regulation mediate the relationship between intergroup contact, susceptibility to persuasion, and intergroup anxiety on the one hand and ethnic outgroup aggression on the other hand within a cross-cultural sample. An online questionnaire was distributed using convenience sampling among 2,482 students with an ethnic majority background living and studying in ten (European) countries. Multigroup path analysis supported the larger part of the hypothesized model where we found that emotion regulation partially mediated the relationship between susceptibility to persuasion as a predictor and aggression as an outcome. As expected, we found that the higher the susceptibility to persuasion, the higher the emotion regulation, and the higher the regulation, the lower the aggression in all countries. Our pilot study provided preliminary evidence that emotion regulation, nationalism and susceptibility to persuasion are critical for the understanding of ethnic outgroup aggression in ethnically diverse societies. Future research needs to be carried out focusing on the development of an intergroup anxiety assessment in which possible gender differences in assessed constructs are considered.
Content may be subject to copyright.
International Journal of Intercultural Relations 84 (2021) 79–85
Available online 20 July 2021
0147-1767/© 2021 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Ethnic outgroup aggression: A pilot study on the importance of
emotion regulation, nationalism and susceptibility to persuasion
Sneˇ
zana Stupar-Rutenfrans
a
,
*, Petrouschka C.D. Verdouw
a
, Jedidja van Boven
a
,
Olga Aleksandrovna Ryzhkina
b
, Anastasia Batkhina
c
, Idil Aksoz-Efe
d
,
Oriola Hamzallari
e
, Penny Papageorgopoulou
f
, Fitim Uka
g
, Nebojˇ
sa Petrovi´
c
h
,
Arta Statovci
i
, Miranda Rutenfrans-Stupar
j
, Daniela Garbin Praniˇ
cevi´
c
i
,
Skerdi Zahaj
k
, Eric Mijts
l
a
University College Roosevelt (Utrecht University), Lange Noordstraat 1, 4331 CB, Middelburg, The Netherlands
b
Novosibirsk State University, 1 Pirogova Street, Novosibirsk, 630090, Russia
c
National Research University Higher School of Economics, 20 Myasnitskaya Ulitsa, Moscow, 101000, Russia
d
Ataturk University, 25240, Erzurum, Turkey
e
Aleksander Moisiu University, Durr¨
es L. 1. Stream Street Durr¨
es, Durr¨
es, 2000, Albania
f
National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Athens, 157 72, Greece
g
University of Prishtina "Hasan Prishtina", Rr. "George Bush", Nr. 31, 10 000, Prishtin¨
e, Kosovo
h
University of Belgrade, 1 Studentski trg, 11000, Belgrade, Serbia
i
University of Split, Poljiˇ
cka cesta 35, 21000, Split, Croatia
j
Tilburg University, Warandelaan 2, 5037 AB, Tilburg, The Netherlands
k
Tirana University, Kutia Postare Nr 183 / D¨
eshmor¨
et e Kombit Street, Tirana, Albania
l
University of Aruba, J. Irausquinplein 4, Aruba
ARTICLE INFO
Keywords:
Emotion regulation
Outgroup aggression
Intergroup anxiety
Nationalism
ABSTRACT
The current pilot study investigated the psychological mechanisms behind ethnic outgroup
aggression, a signicant outcome of intergroup conicts. While previous research suggested
several impactful predictors of ethnic outgroup aggression, such as intergroup contact and
nationalism, no attempt has been made to synthesize all these constructs into a single cross-
cultural study. Building on existing research, this pilot study is the rst to assess a rened
framework where we tested a proposed mediation model according to nationalism and emotion
regulation mediate the relationship between intergroup contact, susceptibility to persuasion, and
intergroup anxiety on the one hand and ethnic outgroup aggression on the other hand within a
cross-cultural sample. An online questionnaire was distributed using convenience sampling
among 2482 students with an ethnic majority background living and studying in ten (European)
countries. Multigroup path analysis supported the larger part of the hypothesized model where
we found that emotion regulation partially mediated the relationship between susceptibility to
persuasion as a predictor and aggression as an outcome. As expected, we found that the higher the
susceptibility to persuasion, the higher the emotion regulation, and the higher the regulation, the
lower the aggression in all countries. Our pilot study provided preliminary evidence that emotion
regulation, nationalism and susceptibility to persuasion are critical for the understanding of
ethnic outgroup aggression in ethnically diverse societies. Future research needs to be carried out
* Corresponding author at: University College Roosevelt, Utrecht University, The Netherlands.
E-mail address: s.stuparrutenfrans@ucr.nl (S. Stupar-Rutenfrans).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
International Journal of Intercultural Relations
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ijintrel
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2021.07.004
Received 25 August 2020; Received in revised form 7 July 2021; Accepted 9 July 2021
International Journal of Intercultural Relations 84 (2021) 79–85
80
focusing on the development of an intergroup anxiety assessment in which possible gender dif-
ferences in assessed constructs are considered.
Introduction
Ethnic outgroup aggression, dened as negative thoughts or behaviors that are explicitly directed towards the outgroups ethnic
background (Smith & Mackie, 2005), is an important outcome of intergroup conicts that are widely present in todays society.
Previous research conrmed that ethnic outgroup aggression is predicted by low intergroup contact (Schmid, Hewstone, Küpper, Zick,
& Tausch, 2014), high intergroup anxiety (Brown, 2011), ones high susceptibility to persuasion (Kaptein, Markopoulos, de Ruyter, &
Aarts, 2009), emotion regulation (Spanovic, Lickel, Denson, & Petrovic, 2010), and high nationalism (Mummendey, Klink, & Brown,
2001). To the best of our knowledge, the attempt to synthesize and explain the relationships among all these predictors and ethnic
outgroup aggression in one single study within a larger cross-cultural sample has not yet been undertaken. We seek to address this
knowledge gap by proposing and examining a model (see Fig. 1) in ten countries in which nationalism and emotion regulation (ER)
mediate the relationship between intergroup contact, susceptibility to persuasion, and intergroup anxiety on the one hand and (ethnic
outgroup) aggression on the other hand.
Traditionally, ethnic outgroup aggression is predicted by intergroup contact (direct contact between members of different groups;
Schmid et al., 2014), intergroup anxiety (concerns that intergroup contact will have adverse outcomes; Brown, 2011), and suscep-
tibility to persuasion (an individuals behavioural compliance to persuasive cues; Kaptein et al., 2009). Increased intergroup contact
limits the polarization of intergroup relations and improves attitudes towards outgroups (Schmid et al., 2014). Furthermore, inter-
group contact promotes outgroup acceptance (as it reduces prejudice whereas both in- and outgroups become aware of the similarities
and collectiveness between the groups rather than characterizing the differences; Capozza, Di Bernardo, & Falvo, 2017), improves
attitudes toward outgroups (contact hypothesis; Allport, 1954), lowers nationalistic attitudes (attitudes towards the importance of
ones country; Boin, Fuochi, & Voci, 2020; Coryn, Beale, & Myers, 2004), and limits outgroup aggression (Saab, Harb, & Moughalian,
2017). Intergroup anxiety, on the other hand, has substantial adverse effects, such as higher cognitive biases towards the outgroup, as
well as polarized group judgements and prejudices; the resulting feelings of threat can lead to higher nationalism (Halperin et al., 2012;
Viki & Calitri, 2008) and outgroup aggression as fear causes an increase in ingroup memberssupport of the use of violence (Brown,
2011). This is not surprising as fear is linked to defensive aggression to avoid being attacked (Mifune, Simunovic, & Yamagishi, 2017).
Empirical studies show that fear can lead to defensive aggression if the costs of engaging in conict are low enough (Iyer, Hornsey,
Vanman, Esposo, & Ale, 2015). Intergroup anxiety, therefore, plays a signicant role in the justication of these aggressive acts to-
wards outgroups (Schaller & Neuberg, 2012). Susceptibility to persuasion is also essential for the expression of aggression (Carlson,
Marcus-Newhall, & Miller, 1990); however, the exact relationship between susceptibility to persuasion and (outgroup) aggression is
still unclear. Individuals who already experience higher levels of prejudice towards an outgroup are more susceptible to persuasion,
which creates stronger negative feelings towards the outgroup in question (Middleton, 1960). Because of the increasing pervasiveness
of social media in determining public opinion, we can speculate that political propaganda can increase the effectiveness of persuasion
when popular predispositions towards ethnic outgroups are used in this propaganda (Petrova & Yanagizawa-Drott, 2016). In addition,
ethnic conicts, which can take extreme forms like genocide, have been linked to attitudes towards authority and social hierarchy:
increased authoritarianism in ones personality increases the likelihood of endorsing social hierarchies and suppressing groups below
ones own (Green & Seher, 2003). These traits contribute to an individuals susceptibility to persuasion by authorities in particular and
make them more likely to develop negative attitudes towards outgroups. These negative attitudes then lead to outgroup aggression
(DeWall, Anderson, & Bushman, 2011). Further exploration of the susceptibility to persuasion-aggression relationship, therefore,
contributes signicantly to the existing body of literature.
We propose that ER (ability to manage ones emotions through activation of strategies such as expressive suppression and cognitive
reappraisal; Gross, 2015), has a mediating effect on the relationship between intergroup anxiety and outgroup aggression (Trawalter,
Fig. 1. Hypothesized model of intergroup anxiety, nationalism, emotion regulation and ethnic outgroup aggression.
S. Stupar-Rutenfrans et al.
International Journal of Intercultural Relations 84 (2021) 79–85
81
Richeson, & Shelton, 2009), and the relationship between susceptibility to persuasion and outgroup aggression (Mackie, Smith, & Ray,
2008). Intergroup anxiety has been commonly operationalized as a degree to which people experience anxiety-related emotions/states
(such as being anxious, nervous, tense, upset), as assessed during the hypothetical interaction with ethnic outgroups (Intercultural
Anxiety Scale, IAS; Stephan & Stephan, 1992). In that sense, intergroup anxiety is a broader emotional experience similar to stress and
negative affect (Stephan, 2014). Previous research shows that ER can in itself be subject to modication in a stressful situation where
experienced stress diminishes ER due to the cognitive demand it requires (Kinner, Het, & Wolf, 2014). As this study focusses on ER
strategies that are cognitively demanding (e.g. expressive suppression; Gross & John, 2003), it is expected that participants who
experience high levels of intergroup anxiety will apply ER to a lesser extent. Subsequently, low ER, especially in response to strong
negative emotions like (intergroup) fear, fuels intergroup aggression (Spanovic et al., 2010) while greater ER is negatively related to
racial prejudice, implying that higher ER could help to reduce negative behaviours toward outgroups (Mackie et al., 2008). Unsur-
prisingly, interracial interactions are appraised as stressful and thus facilitate intergroup anxiety; this can negatively affect ER as
increased anxiety lowers the ability to suppress negative emotions (Trawalter et al., 2009). Building upon previous research indicating
that ER is a mediating variable in the relationship between variables similar to constructs of our interests (e.g. ingroup identication
and the generation of group emotions, such as pride; Goldenberg, Halperin, van Zomeren, & Gross, 2016), we hypothesize that
intergroup anxiety predicts outgroup aggression via ER.
We also propose that nationalism has a mediating effect on the relationship between intergroup anxiety and outgroup aggression
(Brown, 2011) and the relationship between intergroup contact and outgroup aggression (Tausch et al., 2010). Nationalism has
commonly been operationalized as nationalistic attitudes (attitudes towards the importance of ones country; Coryn et al., 2004) and
nationalistic identity (strong feelings of group identication; Viki & Calitri, 2008). Both of these notions have been associated with
higher outgroup aggression (Mummendey et al., 2001). The hypothesized mediation relationships are consistent with previous
research, such as Spanovicmodel for the relationship between fear and outgroup aggression (2010).
The current pilot study
We proposed and tested a mediation model in which intergroup anxiety, susceptibility to persuasion, and intergroup contact are
associated with ethnic outgroup aggression through nationalism and ER (Fig. 1). Thereby, we suggested that intergroup anxiety is
positively correlated with nationalism (Viki & Calitri, 2008); that lower intergroup contact creates stronger nationalistic tendencies
(Schmid et al., 2014); and that individuals susceptible for persuasion score higher on nationalism (Snyder & Ballentine, 1996). We also
propose that intergroup anxiety leads to lower ER (Trawalter et al., 2009). Although susceptibility to persuasion is commonly asso-
ciated with ER as well, the direction of this relationship is still unclear (Lewinski, Fransen, & Tan, 2014). Finally, we expect that low ER
and high nationalism will be associated with high aggression (Mummendey et al., 2001; Spanovic et al., 2010). We tested the model in
ten (European) countries and we expected the hypothesized model to be valid in all ethnic groups.
Method
Participants
The convenience sample consisted of 2482 university students with ethnic majority background situated in large cities in 10
countries: Russia, Albania, Turkey, Greece, Serbia, Montenegro, Kosovo, Croatia, Aruba, and The Netherlands. No ethnic minorities
were included in the study. To obtain majorities, demographics of the participants were assessed with questions on country of birth (of
student and their parents) and country of residence, a 10 point Likert scale asking the extent to which someone identies as a
mainstreamer of the country, a question asking whether students have the nationality of the country of residence, and an open question
where students could enter if they have another (additional) nationality. Online (back-translated) questionnaires were offered in the
countrys ofcial languages. The participants were (77.5 % women) aged 1640 (M =22, SD =1.75), with 398 Albanian (85.7 %
women), 84 Aruban (73.8 % women), 174 Croatian (94.3 % women), 188 Dutch (66.0 % women), 168 Greek (48.8 % women), 394
Kosovars (88.1 % women), 205 Montenegrin (74.1 % women), 458 Russian (78.6 % women), 202 Serbs (75.2 % women), and 211
Turks (66.4 % women).
Measures
ER strategies, expressive suppression (four items) and cognitive reappraisal (six items), were assessed using the 10-item Emotion
Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross & John, 2003). A 7-point Likert scale was used ranging from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 7 (Strongly
agree). An example of an item is When I want to feel more positive emotions, I change the way I am thinking about the situation.
Nationalism was assessed using two separate scales, nationalistic attitudes that consisted of ve items related to attitudes toward the
importance of ones own country, adopted from an empirical study in 22 countries (Coenders, 2001), and nationalistic identity, a 5-item
National Identication Scale (NIS; Verlegh, 2001). Items were rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 5
(Strongly agree). An example of an item is: I am proud to be [Dutch].
Intergroup anxiety was assessed using the 15 items of the Intercultural Anxiety Scale (IAS; Stephan & Stephan, 1992). A 10-point
Likert scale ranging from 1 (Not at all) to 10 (Extremely) was used when participants were asked to report the degree to which they
would experience 15 anxiety-related emotions/states during the hypothetical interaction with foreigners (people with a different
ethnic background than ones own) such as anxious, nervous, tense, and upset. A sample item reads: Meeting strangers and introducing
S. Stupar-Rutenfrans et al.
International Journal of Intercultural Relations 84 (2021) 79–85
82
yourself, with a response ranging from 1 (not at all nervous) to 10 (extremely nervous).
Contact with ethnic outgroups was assessed with the question How often do you have contact with foreigners (people from a different
ethnic background than yours)?on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (every day).
Susceptibility to persuasion was assessed by the 12-item Susceptibility Questionnaire (Kaptein et al., 2009). Items were rated on a
7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The scales covered six dimensions of susceptibility (two items
per dimension), namely reciprocation (an example of an item: When I am in a new situation, I look at others to see what I should do.),
scarcity, authority, commitment, consensus and liking.
Aggression was measured by 24 items of The Normative Beliefs about Aggression Scale (NOBAGS; Huesmann, Guerra, Zelli, &
Miller, 1992), which assessed the acceptance of aggressive behaviour in two situations; 1) under varying specic conditions of
provocation, namely retaliation belief questions (16 items) and 2) when no conditions are specied (the general aggression belief
questions - eight items). The scale is also validated for older adults and various ethnic groups (Huesmann et al., 1992). The NOBAGS
has been revised to assess interactions between ethnic groups (Shechtman & Basheer, 2005). Ethnic outgroup aggression was assessed
during different hypothetical situations: native versus native, foreigner (a person with a different ethnic background than ones own)
versus foreigner, native versus foreigner, and foreigner versus native. Terms related to verbal (screaming and verbally hurting others)
and relational (cutting off the contact and avoiding other) aggression responses were included. The nal version of the aggression scale
used in this study consisted of 24 items; ethnic outgroup aggression contained 16 items divided across four interaction situations as
previously indicated, and the general aggression belief that contains eight items divided across the two subscales: general approval of
verbal aggression and general approval of behavioural aggression, consisting of four items each. With the analysis, a 4-point Likert
scale ranging from 1 (Its perfectly OK) to 4 (Its really wrong) was recoded into 1 (Its really wrong) to 4 (Its perfectly OK) to make the
scores and extent of aggression proportional. An example of an item is: Do you think its wrong for the [Dutch] to say something hurtful to
the foreigner (person who has different ethnic background than ones own)?.
Internal consistency and factor analysis
Internal consistencies of all scales used in the current study were satisfactory (Cronbachs alpha range: .67.91) in all samples from
all countries. Multi-group conrmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted in AMOS (Arbuckle, 2006) to explore dimensionality and
to test for measurement invariance (Byrne, 2004) across all ethnic groups. Results related to measurement invariance and dimen-
sionality are both promising and in line with previous literature. However, due to varying sample sizes from different countries (e.g.
few samples are rather small to identify valid factor structure and measurement invariance), we were not able to examine whether
respondents from different countries interpret the same measure in a conceptually similar way, and whether factorial structure is fully
applicable for all samples.
Results
Emotion regulation, nationalism and outgroup aggression: the mediation model
First, a hypothesized model (Fig. 1) without mediators was tested in a multigroup path analysis using AMOS (Arbuckle, 2006).
Aggression was constructed based on two variables, general approval of (verbal and behavioural) aggression, and approval of ethnic
outgroup aggression. The structural weights model was the most restrictive model with a good t,
χ
2
(282, N =2482) =492.678,
p<.001;
χ
2
/df =1.747, the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) being .975 and the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) being
.017. Higher scores on susceptibility to persuasion were signicantly associated with lower aggression scores (general and ethnic
outgroup related) in all groups. However, the hypothesized direct effects of intergroup anxiety and contact with ethnic outgroups on
aggression were not conrmed as they were both non-signicant.
Fig. 2. The nal model of susceptibility to persuasion, emotion regulation, nationalism and aggression.
Note: Standardized regression coefcients are provided next to the arrows. The numbers inside the latent variable circles (emotion regulation and
aggression) represent the proportions of variance explained.
***p <.001.
S. Stupar-Rutenfrans et al.
International Journal of Intercultural Relations 84 (2021) 79–85
83
Second, a hypothesized mediation model was tested in which two predictors, namely intergroup anxiety and contact with ethnic
outgroups, were excluded. ER and aggression were categorized as latent variables. Indicators of ER were the two subscales, reappraisal
and suppression. The structural weights model was the most restrictive model with a good t,
χ
2
(460, N=2482) =886.251, p <.001;
χ
2
/df =1.927, CFI =.958 and RMSEA =.019. Additionally, the signicance of the indirect effect of susceptibility to persuasion on
aggression variable using bootstrapping was computed and was signicant (β= − .04; 95 % CI: .09 to .01; p<.01). Moreover, the
direct effect of susceptibility to persuasion on aggression was weaker yet signicant in the model with a mediator (β = − .22, p<.001)
compared to the model without a mediator (β = − .30, p <.001). Therefore, it can be concluded that ER can be treated as a partial
mediator in the susceptibility-aggression relationship.
Consequently, the nal model with ER as a partial mediator was tested (see Fig. 2). The structural weights model was the most
restrictive model with a good t,
χ
2
(459, N =2482) =879.556, p <.001;
χ
2
/df =1.916, CFI =.958 and RMSEA =.019. A model in
which ER partially mediated the relationship between susceptibility to persuasion as a predictor and (general and ethnic outgroup)
aggression as an outcome was supported. The higher the susceptibility to persuasion, the higher the ER, and the higher the ER, the
lower the aggression. Additionally, this model showed that the higher the nationalistic attitudes and the lower the nationalistic
identity, the higher the (general and ethnic outgroup) aggression. Results from multigroup path analyses conrmed similarities be-
tween structural parameters indicating no differences in the relationships between samples from different countries.
Discussion
We investigated a mediation model in which intergroup anxiety, susceptibility to persuasion, and intergroup contact are associated
with ethnic outgroup aggression through nationalism and ER. We found support for a model (in all countries) in which ER partially
mediates the relationship between susceptibility to persuasion as a predictor and aggression as an outcome. As expected, we found that
the higher the susceptibility to persuasion, the higher the ER (Lewinski et al., 2014), and the higher the ER, the lower the aggression
(DeWall et al., 2011).
Surprisingly, we found that the higher the nationalistic attitudes and the lower the nationalistic identity, the higher the aggression
in all countries (Mummendey et al., 2001). Our ndings suggest that the two different operationalizations of nationalism may lead to
different relationships to aggression; high scores on nationalistic identity may indicate higher interpersonal stability that further leads
to lower aggression in our participants. Being stable in terms of national identity could be predicted by coexisting high scores on
personality traits such as openness that is typically related to higher multicultural attitudes and lower ingroup-outgroup distance
(Stupar, Van de Vijver, Te Lindert, & Fontaine, 2014) which should be protective factors against outgroup aggression. When inter-
preting our ndings we should also take into account recent transformations that many of sampled countries undergone last decades
(e.g. collapse of USSR, Croatia gaining autonomy, socio-political changes in Turkey) which could have induced weakening of social
bonds within society (Social Bond Theory; Hirschi, 1969) and the sense of nationalistic identity (strong feelings of group identication;
Viki & Calitri, 2008) resulting in more aggression toward outgroups (Hirschi, 1969).
We could not conrm the mediating role of nationalism as hypothesized in the proposed model and the hypothesized direct effects
of intergroup anxiety and contact with ethnic outgroups on (general and ethnic outgroup) aggression. A plausible explanation for this
may be the lower quality of the measures. Specically, contact with ethnic outgroups was measured by one single item assessing only
the frequency of contact with people from a different ethnic background than ones own, but we did not specify how and in which
situations this contact took place. In previous research on intergroup contact, this construct has been assessed more explicitly utilizing
information related to the type of ethnic outgroups, the type of contact (online versus face-to-face), and the quantity and quality of
contact (Schmid et al., 2014). An additional limitation of this study may be the overgeneralization of the contact questions as different
types of contacts have not been explored, subsequently, this may have led to a lower understanding of the item and therefore generated
more invalid responses. Assessment of intergroup anxiety may also have been biased as we may have measured general arousal during
the intergroup interaction instead of intergroup anxiety, which does not necessarily occur only during the interaction with ethnic
outgroups. To our knowledge, there is no existing validated intergroup anxiety scale that specically measures anxiety purely related
to ethnic outgroup members, this limits the internal validity of this concept (Spanovic et al., 2010); one could also question whether
the development of such scale is valid as the more experimental (exposure) approach may be more reliable in capturing objective
aspects of experienced anxiety. The same applies for the assessment of ethnic outgroup aggression; we adapted NOBAGS in order to
assess aggression during interactions between ethnic outgroups while no validated scale exists that assesses this construct. Finally, the
current cross-sectional design can be seen as an important limitation for testing the causal relationships within our proposed mediation
model, hence an experimental or longitudinal design would provide stronger evidence for the theoretically proposed model.
Conclusion
In conclusion, we found support for a model in which ER partially mediates the relationship between susceptibility to persuasion
and aggression. We also made some surprising observations regarding the nationalism ethnic outgroup aggression relationship that
may warrant further exploration. Moreover, further development of the intergroup anxiety assessment and aggression related to ethnic
outgroups is needed. Future research should focus on the development of an intergroup anxiety and ethnic outgroup aggression
assessment tool in which only anxiety and specic ethnic aggression will be measured, ideally before, during, and after intergroup
contact/conict. Thereby, researchers should consider taking into account several other variables such as gender, age and personality.
There may be gender differences in susceptibility to persuasion and aggression favoured by gender-related socialization; some studies
have shown that men are more aggressive than women in interpersonal and intergroup relationships (Yokota, 2018). Nevertheless,
S. Stupar-Rutenfrans et al.
International Journal of Intercultural Relations 84 (2021) 79–85
84
given the contemporary research on gender identity differentiation (e.g. cis versus non-cis genders), more research is needed in this
area. Age may also be relevant for the formation of the social attitude with young adults being more exposed to media globalization
possibly resulting in holding stronger multicultural attitudes and lower ingroup-outgroup distance (Stupar et al., 2014). Personality
traits may also play an important role in the proposed mediation model as, openness that is related to higher multicultural attitudes
and lower ingroup-outgroup distance (Stupar et al., 2014) may serve as a protective factor against outgroup aggression. Finally, it is
recommended to take into account not only the perspective of ethnic majorities (as we did in the current study), but also of ethnic
minorities when analysing data on intergroup relations, due to possible differences in contributions aimed at subordinating out-groups
(out-group aggression) from those aimed at defending the in-group against possible out-group aggression (in-group defence) (Carsten
et al., 2016).
Authors contributions
Sneˇ
zana Stupar-Rutenfrans: design; collection, analysis and interpretation of data; drafting the article and revising it critically
Petrouschka C. D. Verdouw: collection and analysis of data; drafting and revising the article
Jedidja van Boven: revising the article
Olga Aleksandrovna Ryzhkina: collection of data
Anastasia Batkhina: collection of data
Idil Aksoz-Efe: collection of data
Oriola Hamzallari: collection of data
Penny Papageorgopoulou: collection of data
Fitim Uka: collection of data
Nebojˇ
sa Petrovi´
c: collection of data
Arta Statovci: collection of data
Miranda Rutenfrans-Stupar: collection of data
Daniela Garbin Praniˇ
cevi´
c: collection of data
Skerdi Zahaj: collection of data
Eric Mijts: collection of data.
Ethical compliance statement
The authors have no funding to disclose. All procedures performed in the current study were in accordance with the ethical
standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable
ethical standards. We declare that we have no conict of interest. Informed consent was obtained from all individual adult participants
included in the study.
Acknowledgements
We thank several collaborators who helped us to collect data in different countries: Prof. dr. Anna Solomonovskaya (Novosibirsk
State University, Russia), Prof. dr. Nadezhda Lebedeva (National Research University Higher School of Economics, Russia), Prof. dr.
Igor Kolesov (Altai State Pedagogical University, Russia), Prof. dr. Vladimir Takˇ
si´
c (University of Rijeka, Croatia), dr. Jovana ˇ
Skori´
c
(University of Novi Sad, Serbia), dr. Mirjam Keteels and dr. Loes Keijsers (Tilburg University, the Netherlands), dr. Gero Lange
(Radboud Nijmegen, the Netherlands), Drs. Arnoud Versluis (Breda University of Applied Sciences, the Netherlands), Suzanne van
Kuijk MSc (Fontys, the Netherlands), and dr. Dimitris Charitos (National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Greece). We thank all
student participants for their contribution to our research, all translators that helped in back-to-translation of the questionnaires and
all UCR students that contributed to the data collection and analysis, especially Nineke Wilts. We also want to give special thanks to dr.
Katelyn E. Poelker (Saint Louis University) who generously gave us her input around the use of language in the current article. We are
also grateful for other resources obtained from Catharine van Tussenbroek Funds and the UCR research director, Prof. dr. Bert van den
Brink, which made possible discussing and sharing of (preliminary) results of the current study during several international occasions.
References
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison Wesley.
Arbuckle, J. L. (2006). Amos (Version 7.0) [Computer Program]. Chicago, IL: SPSS.
Boin, J., Fuochi, G., & Voci, A. (2020). Deprovincialization as a key correlate of ideology, prejudice, and intergroup contact. Personality and Individual Differences, 157,
Article 109799. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.109799.
Brown, R. (2011). Prejudice: Its social psychology. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Byrne, B. M. (2004). Testing for multigroup invariance using AMOS graphics: A road less travelled. Structural Equation Modeling, 11(2), 272300. https://doi.org/
10.1207/s15328007sem1102_8.
Capozza, D., Di Bernardo, G. A., & Falvo, R. (2017). Intergroup contact and outgroup humanization: Is the causal relationship uni- or bidirectional? PloS One, 12(1),
Article e0170554. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0170554.
Carlson, M., Marcus-Newhall, A., & Miller, N. (1990). Effects of situational aggression cues: A quantitative review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 622.
https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.58.4.622.
Carsten, K. W., De Dreu, J. G., Zsombor, M., Michael, G., Eliska, P., & Jonathan, K. S. (2016). Columbus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(38),
1052410529. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1605115113.
S. Stupar-Rutenfrans et al.
International Journal of Intercultural Relations 84 (2021) 79–85
85
Coenders, M. T. A. (2001). Nationalistic attitudes and ethnic exclusionism in a comparative perspective: An empirical study of attitudes toward the country and ethnic
immigrants in 22 countries. Doctoral dissertation. Radboud University Nijmegen.
Coryn, C. L., Beale, J. M., & Myers, K. M. (2004). Response to September 11: Anxiety, patriotism, and prejudice in the aftermath of terror. Current Research in Social
Psychology, 9(12), 165183.
DeWall, C. N., Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2011). The general aggression model: Theoretical extensions to violence. Psychology of Violence, 1(3), 245. https://
doi.org/10.1037/a0023842.
Goldenberg, A., Halperin, E., van Zomeren, M., & Gross, J. J. (2016). The process model of group-based emotion: Integrating intergroup emotion and emotion
regulation perspectives. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20(2), 118141. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868315581263.
Green, D. P., & Seher, R. L. (2003). What role does prejudice play in ethnic conict? Annual Review of Political Science, 6, 509531. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.
polisci.6.121901.085642.
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 126. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840x.2014.940781.
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348.
Halperin, E., Crisp, R. J., Husnu, S., Trzesniewski, K. H., Dweck, C. S., & Gross, J. J. (2012). Promoting intergroup contact by changing beliefs: Group malleability,
intergroup anxiety, and contact motivation. Emotion, 12, 1192. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028620.
Hirschi, T. (1969). Causes of delinquency. Berkeley, California: University of California Press.
Huesmann, L. R., Guerra, N. G., Zelli, A., & Miller, L. (1992). Differing normative beliefs about aggression for boys and girls. In K. Bj¨
orkqvist, & P. Niemel¨
a (Eds.), Of
mice and women: Aspects of female aggression (pp. 7787). Cambridge, MA: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-102590-8.50012-X.
Iyer, A., Hornsey, M. J., Vanman, E. J., Esposo, S., & Ale, S. (2015). Fight and ight: Evidence of aggressive capitulation in the face of fear messages from terrorists.
Political Psychology, 36, 631648. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12182.
Kaptein, M., Markopoulos, P., de Ruyter, B., & Aarts, E. (2009). Can you be persuaded? Individual differences in susceptibility to persuasion. In IFIP Conference on
Human-Computer Interaction (pp. 115118).
Kinner, V. L., Het, S., & Wolf, O. T. (2014). Emotion regulation: Exploring the impact of stress and sex. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 18. https://doi.org/
10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00397.
Lewinski, P., Fransen, M. L., & Tan, E. S. (2014). Predicting advertising effectiveness by facial expressions in response to amusing persuasive stimuli. Journal of
Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 7(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.1037/npe0000012.
Mackie, D. M., Smith, E. R., & Ray, D. G. (2008). Intergroup emotions and intergroup relations. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(5), 18661880. https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00130.x.
Middleton, R. (1960). Ethnic prejudice and susceptibility to persuasion. American Sociological Review, 679686. https://doi.org/10.2307/2090140.
Mifune, N., Simunovic, D., & Yamagishi, T. (2017). Intergroup biases in fear-induced aggression. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 49. https://doi.org/10.3389/
fpsyg.2017.00049.
Mummendey, A., Klink, A., & Brown, R. (2001). Nationalism and patriotism: National identication and out-group rejection. The British Journal of Social Psychology,
40(2), 159172. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466601164740.
Petrova, M., & Yanagizawa-Drott, D. (2016). Media persuasion, ethnic hatred, and mass violence. Economic Aspects of Genocides, Other Mass Atrocities, and Their
Prevention, 274. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199378296.003.0012.
Saab, R., Harb, C., & Moughalian, C. (2017). Intergroup contact as a predictor of violent and nonviolent collective action: Evidence from Syrian refugees and Lebanese
nationals. Peace and Conict Journal of Peace Psychology, 23(3), 297. https://doi.org/10.1037/pac0000234.
Schaller, M., & Neuberg, S. L. (2012). Danger, disease, and the nature of prejudice (s). In Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 46, pp. 154). Academic Press.
Schmid, K., Hewstone, M., Küpper, B., Zick, A., & Tausch, N. (2014). Reducing aggressive intergroup action tendencies: Effects of intergroup contact via perceived
intergroup threat. Aggressive Behavior, 40(3), 250262. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21516.
Shechtman, Z., & Basheer, O. (2005). Normative beliefs supporting aggression of Arab children in an intergroup conict. Aggressive Behavior: Ofcial Journal of the
International Society for Research on Aggression, 31(4), 324335. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20069.
Smith, E. R., & Mackie, D. M. (2005). Aggression, hatred, and other emotions. In J. Dovidio, P. Glick, & L. Rudman (Eds.), On the nature of prejudice: Fifty years after
allport (pp. 361376). https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470773963.ch22.
Snyder, J., & Ballentine, K. (1996). Nationalism and the marketplace of ideas. International Security, 21(2), 540. https://doi.org/10.2307/2539069.
Spanovic, M., Lickel, B., Denson, T. F., & Petrovic, N. (2010). Fear and anger as predictors of motivation for intergroup aggression: Evidence from Serbia and
Republika Srpska. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 13(6), 725739. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430210374483.
Stephan, W. G. (2014). Intergroup anxiety: Theory, research, and practice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 18(3), 239255. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1088868314530518.
Stephan, C. W., & Stephan, W. G. (1992). Reducing intercultural anxiety through intercultural contact. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 16(1), 89106.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0147-1767(92)90007-h.
Stupar, S., Van de Vijver, A. J. R., Te Lindert, A., & Fontaine, J. R. J. (2014). Multicultural attitudes mediate the relation between personality and perceived ethnic
outgroup distance in the Netherlands. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 38, 2435. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2013.05.002.
Tausch, N., Hewstone, M., Kenworthy, J. B., Psaltis, C., Schmid, K., Popan, J. R., Hughes, J. (2010). Secondary transfer effects of intergroup contact: Alternative
accounts and underlying processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(2), 282. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018553.
Trawalter, S., Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. N. (2009). Predicting behavior during interracial interactions: A stress and coping approach. Personality and Social
Psychology Review, 13(4), 243268. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868309345850.
Verlegh, P. W. J. (2001). Country-of-Origin effects on consumer product evaluations. Doctoral dissertation. Wageningen, The Netherlands: Wageningen University.
Viki, G. T., & Calitri, R. (2008). Infrahuman outgroup or suprahuman ingroup: The role of nationalism and patriotism in the infrahumanization of outgroups. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 38(6), 10541061. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.495.
Yokota, K. (2018). The evolutionary roots of gender differences in aggressive behavior. Japanese Psychological Review, 60(1), 1522. https://doi.org/10.24602/
sjpr.60.1_15.
S. Stupar-Rutenfrans et al.
... To address our research questions, we performed descriptive analysis, measurement invariance analysis, path analysis, and multi-group path analysis. Results of simulation studies (e.g., Kim et al., 2017), as well as other studies with similar analyses (e.g., Byrne & van de Vijver, 2017;Klassen et al., 2010;Stupar-Rutenfrans et al., 2021), suggest that our sample size for each country was adequate for the planned analyses. Descriptive analyses were conducted in SPSS 26 to provide an overview of the key variables, and the other analyses were performed in Mplus 8.4. ...
Article
Full-text available
Young adulthood (18–30 years old) is a crucial period due to its developmental tasks such as career establishment and financial independence. However, young adults’ relative lack of resources makes them vulnerable to employment disruptions (job loss and income loss), which may have both immediate and long-term effects on their financial wellbeing and mental health. The economic impact of COVID-19 restrictions resulted in an increase in unemployment and a decrease in income worldwide, especially for young adults. This study examined to what extent and how job loss and income loss due to the pandemic influenced young adults’ perception of their present financial wellbeing, future financial wellbeing, and psychological wellbeing by using cross-sectional survey data collected from six countries (China, Italy, Lithuania, Portugal, Slovenia, and the United States). Results showed that the impact of income loss and job loss on all three types of wellbeing were mediated by young adults’ negative perception of the COVID-19 lockdown restriction (i.e., perceived as a misfortune). Cross-country differences existed in the key variables. The association between employment disruptions, young adults’ perception of the COVID-19 lockdown restriction, and wellbeing were equivalent across countries except China. Implications for policy and practice are discussed.
... In addition, a strong and positive proximate identification (in-group: community and country) presupposes feelings of belonging, satisfaction and pride, as well as engagement and participation in one's own social and cultural practices. In this sense, strong proximate identities may imply a reactionary position that, in the long run, can produce an incubation of contradictions and resentments that at a certain point might provoke unfavorable reactions toward the outgroup (Stupar-Rutenfrans et al., 2021;Zhai and Yan, 2022), for example, to people from other countries or cultures. However, it also may represent an element of union and cohesion in the face of external aggression (from another country or the dominant culture against the minority culture or a natural catastrophe like COVID-19 pandemics). ...
Article
Full-text available
Recent studies suggest that identification with all humanity (IWAH), apart from being related to universalistic values, could also be related to self-transcendent emotions (STE). In this scenario, the general objective of this cross-cultural longitudinal study is to examine the relationship between identification with proximate categories (i.e., community and country) and superordinate one (all humanity), and their association with positive self-oriented and STEs during a traumatic global phenomenon such as COVID-19 pandemics. Additionally, we explore variations regarding the patterns of those associations in different cultural contexts (Chile and Spain) and examine whether they change among two different time points (T1–T2). The total sample was composed of 403 participants, of whom 224 were residents in Chile (M = 39.25, SD = 12.56; range 18–71 years; 49.6% women) and 179 were residents in Spain (M = 36.35, SD = 12.12; range 18–68 years; 59.8% women). Data collection was carried out in September (T1) and November (T2) 2020, through online surveys administered via Survey Monkey® platform. Overall, results show, as expected, greater identification with proximate categories rather than superordinate ones, and an association between STEs and IWAH, but also with national and community identification. IWAH, but not STEs decreased significantly (T1–T2) in both countries. Thereafter, these emotional and behavioral responses decline as a symptom of growing fatigue with the pandemic situation, and also reflect a shift from broader to more local concerns. Analysis regarding comparisons between countries indicated higher levels of identification with community and with all humanity in Spain and with country in Chile. The results are discussed in the context of new developments in studies on IWAH.
... Secondly, the study sought to examine if there would be a ripple effect of the intergroup hostility between Arabs and Jews that would affect attitudes between other groups; consequently, the second hypothesis was that there would also be a decrease in support for secular teachers in ultra-Orthodox schools and for ultra-Orthodox teachers in secular schools. Based on a longitudinal survey conducted in Israel, the study allowed for a rare opportunity to inspect the effects of real life pre and post social-political conflict situation, that has been called for by other researchers (Stupar-Rutenfrans et al., 2021). This can allow for better evaluation of the effects of intergroup anxiety, hostility and aggression and can aid in creating more suited research tools and interventions for similar conflict situations around the globe. ...
Article
Full-text available
Initial disruptions to a system can spread out to interrupt increasingly larger parts of that system, known as a ripple effect. The present study sought to examine whether intergroup violence affected attitudes toward tolerance as exemplified by attitudes toward boundary-crossing teachers. The main hypothesis was that after the May 2021 violence between Jews and Arabs in Israel, there would be a decrease in support for Jewish teachers in Arab schools and Arab teachers in Jewish schools. The study further sought to examine if there would be a decrease in support for secular teachers in ultra-Orthodox schools and vice versa (a ripple effect). 402 Jewish Israeli high-school students (grades 10-12) responded to a questionnaire in March 2021, and then again, a month after the May violence. The main hypothesis was partially supported, in that there was a decrease in the support for Arab teachers in Jewish schools across the board, and a decrease in the support for Jewish teachers in Arab schools among religious youth. The study provides modest support for a ripple effect demonstrated by a decrease in support for secular teachers in ultra-Orthodox schools among religious youth. In accordance with integrated threat theory, we suggest a phenomenon of retreating into one’s ingroup following violence, in this case rejecting the notion of interaction between Arabs and Jews, and then extending these walls to discard other forms of intergroup contact. We argue that such crises should be countered with comprehensive intercultural interventions rather than a circumscribed appeasement between groups.
Article
Full-text available
Recent research has shown that positive intergroup contact can undermine social change efforts among disadvantaged group members. We extend past this finding by investigating how frequent positive contact relates to both violent and nonviolent collective action, among both advantaged and disadvantaged groups, over and above outgroup attitudes. We examined these links in a survey of Lebanese nationals (advantaged group) and Syrian refugees (disadvantaged group) in the context of a severe economic crisis affecting both groups. We replicated previous research showing that contact predicts outgroup attitudes positively and more strongly among advantaged than among disadvantaged group members. Of importance, we also found evidence suggesting that the relationship between positive contact and collective action may depend on group status and on collective action type. Frequent positive contact reduced nonviolent collective action tendencies among disadvantaged group members but not among advantaged group members. However, more positive frequent contact was linked to lower violence support among both advantaged and disadvantaged group members. These results help shed new light on the potential benefits and limits of intergroup contact as a social change tool.
Article
Full-text available
The attribution of uniquely human characteristics to the outgroup may favor the search for contact with outgroup members and, vice versa, contact experiences may improve humanity attributions to the outgroup. To explore this bidirectional relationship, two studies were performed. In Study 1, humanity perceptions were manipulated using subliminal conditioning. Two experimental conditions were created. In the humanization condition, the unconditioned stimuli (US) were uniquely human words; in the dehumanization condition, the US were non-uniquely human and animal words. In both conditions, conditioned stimuli were typical outgroup faces. An approach/avoidance technique (the manikin task) was used to measure the willingness to have contact with outgroup members. Findings showed that in the humanization condition participants were faster in approaching than in avoiding outgroup members: closeness to the outgroup was preferred to distance. Latencies of approach and avoidance movements were not different in the dehumanization condition. In Study 2, contact was manipulated using the manikin task. One approach (contact) condition and two control conditions were created. The attribution of uniquely human traits to the outgroup was stronger in the contact than in the no-contact conditions. Furthermore, the effect of contact on humanity attributions was mediated by increased trust toward the outgroup. Thus, findings demonstrate the bidirectionality of the relationship between contact and humanity attributions. Practical implications of findings are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Using a recently created preemptive strike game (PSG) with 176 participants, we investigated if the motivations of spite and/or fear promotes aggression that requires a small cost to the aggressor and imposes a larger cost on the opponent, and confirmed the earlier finding that fear does but spite does not promote intergroup aggression when the groups are characterized as minimal groups; additionally, the rate of intergroup aggression did not vary according to the group membership of the opponent. The PSG represents a situation in which both the motivations of spite and of fear can logically drive players to choose an option of aggression against an opponent. Participants decide whether or not to attack another participant, who also has the same capability. The decision is made in real time, using a computer. We discuss theoretical implications of our findings on the evolutionary foundations of intragroup cooperation and intergroup aggression. The evolutionary model of intergroup aggression, or the parochial altruism model, posits that intragroup cooperation and intergroup aggression have co-evolved, and thus it predicts both intragroup cooperation and intergroup aggression to emerge even in a minimal group devoid of a history of intergroup relationships. The finding that only intragroup cooperation but not intergroup aggression emerged in the minimal group experiments strongly suggests that intergroup aggression involves a psychological mechanism that is independent from that of intragroup cooperation. We further discuss the implications of these findings on real-world politics and military strategy.
Article
In this paper, the evolutionary roots of gender differences in aggressive behavior are presented. Previous studies in the field of social psychology have shown that men are more aggressive than women not only in interpersonal, but also in intergroup relationships. From an evolutionary psychological view, it is predicted that outgroup aggression is triggered by the psychological mechanisms adapted to intergroup conflict specified for males. However, social psychologists demonstrated that ingroup cooperation, but not outgroup aggression, was dominant in intergroup conflict situations in a laboratory experiment. On the other hand, in these days, some evidence in the field of cultural anthropology, ethnography, and bioarcheology have clearly shown that hunter-gatherer and forager males frequently engaged in war. I discuss whether intergroup conflict influences selection pressure on male aggressive behavior as a reproductive strategy to enhance fitness.
Article
This paper aimed to examine deprovincialization, defined as a less ingroup-centric worldview fostering openness to other cultures and outgroups, and to test its relationships with personality, cognitive styles, values, political ideologies, intergroup contact, and prejudice. In Study 1, we proved the factorial validity of the Cultural Deprovincialization Scale (CDS), an instrument specifically designed to assess a growing acceptance of other peoples and cultures following intergroup encounters, and we provided evidence for its construct validity by exploring its nomological net. People scoring higher on the CDS were more extraverted, agreeable, open to experience, cognitively flexible, and scored lower on social dominance orientation, right-wing authoritarianism, and nationalism. Moreover, deprovincialized people reported higher scores on positive contact, lower scores on negative contact, better outgroup attitudes, and lower levels of prejudice. In Study 2, using a longitudinal design, we tested the within- and between-individual associations that deprovincialization had with positive and negative intergroup contact, outgroup attitudes, and prejudice. Both between-individual and within-individual variations in positive and negative contact predicted time-varying deprovincialization, whereas between-individual and within-individual variations in deprovincialization predicted time-varying outgroup attitudes and prejudice, controlling for age, gender, and social dominance orientation, and also for contact when deprovincialization was a predictor.
Chapter
This chapter outlines the fundamental empirical challenges when studying media effects on conflict and discusses some recent methodological advances designed to overcome them. The evidence in this emerging literature indicates that mass media can be an effective tool for political elites to orchestrate mass violence. Both direct and indirect persuasion matter. The emerging evidence indicates that direct persuasion is stronger when propaganda targeting ethnic minorities is aligned with political predispositions of the ethnic majority, and indirect persuasion may occur from coordination of violence and from spillovers arising from social interactions.
Article
Significance Across a range of domains, from group-hunting predators to laboratory groups, companies, and nation states, we find that out-group aggression is less successful because it is more difficult to coordinate than in-group defense. This finding explains why appeals for defending the in-group may be more persuasive than appeals to aggress a rivaling out-group and suggests that (third) parties seeking to regulate intergroup conflict should, in addition to reducing willingness to contribute to one’s group’s fighting capacity, undermine arrangements for coordinating out-group aggression, such as leadership, communication, and infrastructure.
Article
In this study our P211 Methods of Experimental Psychology students and research team specifically examined feelings of personal anxiety created by terrorist attacks and ongoing conflict with the Middle East, patriotic attachment towards the United States, and subtle and blatant prejudicial attitudes toward Arabic people following the events of September 11, 2001. The design, hypotheses, instrument selection, data collection, and analyses for this study were conducted by our students as a course learning tool. Our students developed three distinct hypotheses and conducted analysis of these hypotheses, with minimal assistance from our research team. Three hundred-one (174 female, 127 male) students at Indiana University South Bend completed questionnaires for our study, measuring levels of anxiety, patriotism, prejudice, and a variety of sociodemographic factors. Four periods of data collection were completed during a period of 19 months following the events of September 11, 2001. Hypotheses developed by our students were tested using multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) techniques. As predicted, anxiety producing events (periods 1 and 4 combined) coincided with greater patriotic attachment toward the United States and amplified prejudicial attitudes toward the target group; Arabic people. No significant differences were found for either gender or our experimental condition (support versus protest).
Chapter
An evolutionary perspective on human cognition provides a foundation for research programs that identify unique linkages between specific threats and specific prejudices directed against specific categories of people. It also provides a set of logical tools that help identify conditions under which these prejudices are exaggerated or inhibited. We focus here on two kinds of threats: The threat of interpersonal violence and the threat of infectious disease. The inferred threat of interpersonal violence leads to a fear prejudice against members of coalitional outgroups. This prejudice (along with a set of cognitive consequences) emerges especially under conditions that connote vulnerability to interpersonal harm. The inferred threat of infectious disease leads to a disgust prejudice against individuals whose morphological appearance or behavior deviates from normative standards. This prejudice emerges especially under conditions that connote vulnerability to infection. Together, these lines of research yield insights about the origins of prejudices directed against many different categories of people (many of whom pose no real threat whatsoever) and also have useful implications for prejudice-reducing interventions. The results also indicate that the psychology of prejudice is best conceptualized as the psychology of prejudices (plural).