ArticlePublisher preview available
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

One long-standing theoretical model of shyness proposes that the origins and maintenance of shyness are associated with an approach-avoidance motivational conflict (Asendorpf, 1990), such that shy individuals are motivated to socially engage (high approach motivation) but are too anxious to do so (high avoidance motivation). However, this model has not been empirically tested in predicting the develop-ment of shyness. In two separate longitudinal studies, we used the Carver and White (1994) Behavioral Inhibition and Activation System (BIS/BAS) scales as a proxy of approach-avoidance motivations and growth curve analyses to examine whether individual differences in these hypothesized motivational tendencies were associated with the development of shyness across 3 years from late childhood to adolescence (Study 1, N = 1284; 49.8% female, Mage = 10.72, SDage = 1.73, M level of parental education fell between associate’s degree/diploma and undergraduate degree) and across nearly a decade from emerging adulthood to young adulthood (Study 2, N = 83; 57.8% females, Mage = 23.56 years, SDage = 1.09 years, 92.8% had at least a high school education). Contrary to the approach-avoidance conflict model of shyness, we found that a combination of high BIS/low BAS, not high BIS/high BAS, was associated with relatively higher shyness contemporaneously and across development in both studies. We discuss the processes that might link individual differences in approach-avoidance motivations to the development of shyness in adolescence and young adulthood.
Approach-Avoidance Conflict and Shyness: A Developmental Investigation
Raha Hassan
1
, Kristie L. Poole
1
, Ayelet Lahat
2
, Teena Willoughby
3
, and Louis A. Schmidt
1
1
Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and Behaviour, McMaster University
2
Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development, Ontario Institute for Studies in Education, University of Toronto
3
Department of Psychology, Brock University
One long-standing theoretical model of shyness proposes that the origins and maintenance of shyness
are associated with an approach-avoidance motivational conict (Asendorpf, 1990), such that shy indi-
viduals are motivated to socially engage (high approach motivation) but are too anxious to do so (high
avoidance motivation). However, this model has not been empirically tested in predicting the develop-
ment of shyness. In two separate longitudinal studies, we used the Carver and White (1994) Behavioral
Inhibition and Activation System (BIS/BAS) scales as a proxy of approach-avoidance motivations and
growth curve analyses to examine whether individual differences in these hypothesized motivational
tendencies were associated with the development of shyness across 3 years from late childhood to ado-
lescence (Study 1, N= 1284; 49.8% female, M
age
=10.72, SD
age
= 1.73, Mlevel of parental education
fell between associates degree/diploma and undergraduate degree) and across nearly a decade from
emerging adulthood to young adulthood (Study 2, N= 83; 57.8% females, M
age
= 23.56 years, SD
age
=
1.09 years, 92.8% had at least a high school education). Contrary to the approach-avoidance conict
model of shyness, we found that a combination of high BIS/low BAS, not high BIS/high BAS, was
associated with relatively higher shyness contemporaneously and across development in both studies.
We discuss the processes that might link individual differences in approach-avoidance motivations to
the development of shyness in adolescence and young adulthood.
Keywords: shyness, personality, BIS/BAS, approach-avoidance conflict, development
Supplemental materials: https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001175.supp
Shyness is characterized by an anxious preoccupation with the self
in response to real or imagined social situations (Melchior & Cheek,
1990) and is a ubiquitous phenomenon with over 90% of the popula-
tion having experienced it at some point in their lives (Zimbardo,
1977). However, a smaller percentage of people are characterized by
dispositional shyness, a stable feature of personality conserved across
time and context (Zimbardo et al., 1974). Although a number of stud-
ies have examined the correlates and consequences of shyness across
development (see Coplan & Bowker, 2014;Schmidt & Schulkin,
1999;Schmidt et al., 2017;Tang et al., 2017;Tang et al., 2020 for
reviews), relatively little attention has been directed toward under-
standing factors underlying the development of shyness beyond late
childhood.
Development of Shyness
The origin of some aspects of shyness are presumed to be rooted in
early temperament (e.g., Kagan, 1994;Poole et al., 2018). In infancy,
early temperamental precursors of shyness emerge, including individ-
ual differences in behavioral and physiological reactivity to novelty
(Calkins et al., 1996;Fox et al., 2001;Kagan, 1994). For example,
infants classied as highly reactive toward novel stimuli are described
as behaviorally inhibited in toddlerhood (Fox et al., 2001;Garcia-Coll
et al., 1984;Kagan, 1994). In early childhood, the development of
self-awareness, emergence of self-conscious emotions, and ability to
take on the perspective of others all become salient factors in the de-
velopment of shyness in which temperament and now self-concept
begin to play critical roles (see Buss, 1986;Poole et al., 2018;for
reviews). Adolescence is another important developmental period
with respect to shyness, likely due, in part, to the onset of puberty and
the saliency of peer relations (Cheek et al., 1986). These social
changes may lead to an increase in shyness during adolescence rela-
tive to childhood (e.g., Cheek et al., 1986;Tang et al., 2017).
We know, however, comparatively little about the developmental
course of shyness after late childhood and adolescence. This might
reect a general lack of attention historically to the developmental
period now called emerging adulthood(Arnett, 2000). This lack of
attention to shyness is perhaps also surprising given recent cross-
Raha Hassan https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2739-9993
Teena Willoughby https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7516-6895
The present study was supported by a Social Sciences and Humanities
Research Council of Canada (SSHRC) Vanier Doctoral Scholarship
awarded to Raha Hassan, a SSHRC Postdoctoral Fellowship awarded to
Kristie L. Poole, a Canadian Institute for Health Research (CIHR)
operating grant awarded to Teena Willoughby, and operating grants from
the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada
(NSERC) and SSHRC awarded to Louis A. Schmidt. We thank the
participating schools, as well as the children and their families who
participated in this study.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Raha
Hassan, Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and Behaviour,
McMaster University, Room 130, Psychology Building, 1280 Main Street
West, Hamilton, ON L8S 4K1, Canada. Email: hassar@mcmaster.ca
814
Developmental Psychology
©2021 American Psychological Association 2021, Vol. 57, No. 5, 814823
ISSN: 0012-1649 https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001175
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... Consistent with this view, the frequency and enjoyment of solitary activities were negatively related to the desire for social contact (Leary et al., 2003), while. greater preference for solitude is associated with blunted approach motivation (Hassan et al., 2021). Therefore, we hypothesize that solitude, as a state of disconnection from communicating with others, is associated with low approach motivation whatever the reason for solitude. ...
... Some theorists argued that high avoidance motivation is not the reason why unsociable children are less involved with peers, as they might merely be more interested in playing with objects than people (Asendorpf, 1990). Also, some researchers reported that the preference for solitude is associated with elevated avoidance motivation (Hassan et al., 2021). However, the Preference for Solitude Scale used in the latter study contained items conceptually related to social avoidance (e.g., "I prefer spending Friday night alone rather than being with others"). ...
... For this effect size, a previous investigation indicated a moderately sized association between the reason for solitude (i.e., Preference for Solitude) and self-report approach motivation (r = 28 to 0.35; Hassan et al., 2021, Study 1). We elected to recruit a sample sufficient to detect an intermediate effect (r = 0.30 ≈ f 2 = 0.0986) between the lower (r = 0.28) and higher (r = 0.35) effects observed by Hassan et al. (2021). With this effect size, we needed 112 right-handed participants 3 to detect bivariate associations with 90% power. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: What are the motivational underpinnings of solitude? We know from self-report studies that increases in solitude are associated with drops in approach motivation and rises in avoidance motivation, but only when solitude is experienced as non-self-determined (i.e., non-autonomous). However, the extent to which individual differences in solitude relate to neurophysiological markers of approach-avoidance motivation derived from resting-state electroencephalogram (EEG) is unknown. These markers are Frontal Alpha Asymmetry, beta suppression, and midline Posterior versus Frontal EEG Theta Activity. Method: We assessed the relation among individual differences in the reasons for solitude (i.e., preference for solitude, motivation for solitude), approach-avoidance motivation, and resting-state EEG markers of approach-avoidance motivation (N = 115). Results: General preference for solitude was negatively related to approach motivation, observed in both self-reported measures and EEG markers of approach motivation. Self-determined solitude was positively related to both self-reported approach motivation and avoidance motivation in the social domain (i.e., friendship). Non-self-determined solitude was negatively associated with self-reported avoidance motivation. Conclusion: This research was a preliminary attempt to address the neurophysiological underpinnings of solitude in the context of motivation.
... This research gap has led to a current debate about the extent to which subtypes are associated with different measures of (mis)adjustment (see . Moreover, some recent empirical findings challenge Asendorpf's approach-avoidance conceptualization (Bowker et al., 2017;Hassan et al., 2021), proclaiming that "modifications … may be needed" (Bowker et al., 2017, p. 286). In their recent review, point out that one of the main problems of the model is to properly assess SAP and SAV as its two key dimensions. ...
Article
Full-text available
The 2 × 2 model of shyness and sociability is a widely accepted theoretical framework, but to date has not been examined using both adequate measures and appropriate statistical approaches. Therefore, we first review existing literature on the model, outline limitations of previous approaches, and present methodological suggestions on how to adequately test it. Second, by means of an example study with 206 adolescents, we provide support for a revised measure assessing social approach and social avoidance motivation as the two main dimensions of the model. Moreover, when testing the model with hierarchical regression analyses, we could partially replicate previous findings, but also found conflicting results. For instance, when compared to the avoidant-shy subtype (AS; i.e., the combination of low approach and high avoidance), the sociable subtype (SO; i. e., high approach and low avoidance) turned out to be more adaptive in all outcomes (i.e., positive and negative affect, emotional problems, peer problems, conduct problems, oral participation) except for prosocial behavior and nonverbal attentiveness. Furthermore, the comparison of SO to the conflicted-shy subtype (CS; i.e., both high approach and avoidance) and unsociable subtype (UN; i.e., both low approach and avoidance) illustrates that which subtype is more adaptive depends on the outcome.
... When there is a motivational imbalance, the dominant need drives out other needs. As a result, the behavioral limits imposed by the latter are removed, allowing previously avoided actions to occur (Hassan et al., 2021). ...
Article
Previous research has demonstrated shyness plays a critical role in the development and maintenance of smartphone addiction. The present study aimed to examine the mediating roles of maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies and experiential avoidance as well as the moderating role of online social support in the relation between shyness and adolescents' smartphone addiction. A sample of 513 Chinese adolescents ( M age = 16.16 years, range from 14 to 19) was recruited to complete the questionnaires. After sex and age were controlled, shyness was significantly and positively associated with adolescents' smartphone addiction and this relation was partially mediated by both maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies and experiential avoidance. The multiple mediation analysis further indicated that maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies and experiential avoidance exerted a mixed mediation effect on the relation between shyness and adolescents' smartphone addiction. Moreover, online social support significantly moderated the relation between shyness and smartphone addiction and this relation is much weaker for adolescents with high online social support. These results contribute to design the effective interventions of adolescents' smartphone addiction.
Article
Full-text available
During the critical period of personality shaping and self-development, adolescents face unique challenges and opportunities. This study, based on Cognitive-Behavioral Theory, explored the relationship between shyness and self-consistency and congruence (hereinafter referred to as SCC), as well as its underlying mechanisms. Through a questionnaire survey on shyness, social comparison orientation, self-focused attention, and SCC among 984 adolescents, the results revealed that (1) Adolescent shyness negatively predicted SCC. (2) Social comparison orientation partially mediated the relationship between shyness and SCC. (3) Self-focused attention moderated the direct pathway of this mediation process, where a high level of self-focused attention exacerbated the negative impact of shyness on SCC. These findings offered a new perspective on understanding SCC and underscored the importance of addressing the information processing mechanisms of social comparison orientation and self-focused attention among shy adolescents in interventions aimed at promoting their psychological harmony and healthy growth.
Article
Full-text available
Academic procrastination is a common concern among adolescents, but the correlation between shyness and academic procrastination and the internal mechanisms have not yet been thoroughly investigated. Based on a questionnaire survey with 1,279 Chinese middle school students, this study examined the effect of shyness on academic procrastination and its underlying mechanism of self-regulation and self-focused attention. Results revealed that: (1) shyness significantly predicted academic procrastination. (2) Self-regulation mediated the relationship between shyness and academic procrastination. (3) Self-focused attention played a moderating role in the first half of this mediation process. Specifically, higher level of self-focused attention strengthened the predictive effect of shyness on self-regulation. These results underscored the latent risks and protective factors associated with shyness, self-regulation, and self-focused attention in adolescent academic procrastination. In future research and interventions, attention may be directed towards improving individual internal factors to assist adolescents in effectively addressing issues related to academic procrastination.
Article
Full-text available
Few studies have examined the mechanisms linking motivated behavior and reward–punishment stimuli in shy individuals. This study was designed to probe these mechanisms by examining shy and non-shy college student responses to both monetary rewards and penalties in the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT). Specifically, out of the 280 undergraduates surveyed in East China, 45 participants (18 boys) identified as shy and 45 (19 boys) identified as non-shy based on their shyness questionnaire scores were selected to participate in the IGT. Results revealed that shy participants selected favorable low-risk seeking decks (deck C) more frequently and adverse high-risk seeking decks (deck B) less frequently and were more inclined to change deck selection after incurring a net loss. Furthermore, the net score of shy students was higher than that of nonshy students. Results demonstrated that shy people were the winners of IGT games, indicating that they are more likely to exhibit risk-averse behaviors when making decisions. The results are discussed from the perspective of the decision-making style and practical implications of shy individuals.
Article
Background: Since the outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic, university students have been exposed to a heightened vulnerability towards developing psychological issues, such as psychological distress and shyness. Internet-based interventions offer a convenient avenue for scalability, thus prompting the development of a smartphone-based hypnotic intervention aimed at addressing shyness among university students. Objective: We devised an innovative smartphone-based hypnotic intervention called mHypnosis to examine its impact on shyness among undergraduate students. Furthermore, we aimed to investigate whether the apprehension of negative evaluations before treatment could serve as a predictor for the effectiveness of the intervention on shyness. Methods: Eighty students with high shyness scores were randomly assigned to the experimental group and the control group. Another 40 participants with low shyness score were selected as the baseline group. The Shyness Scale (SS), Fear of Negative evaluation scale (FNE), Self-Acceptance Questionnaire (SAQ), and Self-Esteem Scale (SES) were used to evaluate the effect of hypnotic intervention. Results: Before the intervention, the scores of the experimental and control groups on the SS, FNE, SAQ, and SES were higher than those in the baseline group (p < 0.05). There was no significant difference in scores between the experimental and control group (p > 0.05). After the intervention, the scores of the SS, FNE, SAQ, and SES were significantly lower in the experimental group than those in the control group (p < 0.05). The pretest score of FNE could predict the shyness score after hypnotic intervention (B = 0.35, p < 0.05). Conclusion: Smartphone-based hypnotic intervention had a significant effect on ameliorating shyness during the COVID-19 pandemic; fear of negative evaluation can be a target for treating shyness.
Preprint
Full-text available
Although previous research has demonstrated shyness plays a critical role in the development and maintenance of smartphone addiction, the mediating and moderating mechanisms underlying this relationship remains obscure. The present study aimed to examine the mediating roles of maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies and experiential avoidance and the moderating role of online social support in the relationship between shyness and adolescents’ smartphone addiction. A sample of 513 Chinese adolescents (Mage = 16.16 years, range from 14 to 19) was recruited to complete the questionnaires. After sex and age were controlled, shyness was significantly and positively associated with adolescents’ smartphone addiction and this relationship was partially mediated by both maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies and experiential avoidance. The multiple mediation analysis further indicated that maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies and experiential avoidance exerted a mixed mediation effect on the relationship between shyness and adolescents’ smartphone addiction. Moreover, online social support significantly moderated the relationship between shyness and smartphone addiction and this relationship is much weaker for adolescents with high online social support. These results contribute to design the effective interventions of adolescents’ smartphone addiction.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Shy children, who tend to feel anxious around others and withdraw from social interactions, are found to be less prosocial than their not-shy peers in some studies, though not in others. To examine the contexts in which shy children may be more or less likely to engage in prosocial behaviour, we compared children’s willingness and ability to intervene during in-person tasks that differed in socialengagement demands and complexity, factors that have been conflated in past research. Methods We presented 42, 3.5- to 4.5-year-old children with prosocial problems that varied, in a 2 x 2 within-subjects design, by the type of intervention required (i.e., simple helping or complex comforting) and the source of the problem (i.e., social: within the experimenter’s personal space; or object: a target object distanced from her). Results Most of the children acted prosocially, with little prompting, in the two helping tasks and in the object-centered comforting task. In contrast, fewer than half of the children acted prosocially in the social-centered comforting task. Shyer children were not less likely to intervene in any of the four tasks, but they were slower to intervene in the object-centred comforting task, in which the experimenter was upset about a broken toy. Discussion Thus, providing social-centered comfort to a recently-introduced adult is challenging for young children, regardless of shyness, though shy children do show hesitancy with object-centered comforting. Further, these findings provide insights into the methodological challenges of disentangling children’s prosocial motivation and understanding, and we propose solutions to these challenges for future research.
Article
Full-text available
The reactivity-regulation model suggests that the origins and maintenance of shyness results from relatively high levels of reactivity in combination with relatively low levels of regulation. Although this model has received some empirical support, there are still issues regarding directionality of the relations among variables and a dearth of studies examining the joint influence of reactivity and regulation on the prospective development of shyness. Using a longitudinal design, we first examined whether the relations among reactivity, regulation, and shyness were unidirectional or bidirectional in a sample of 1284 children (49.8% female, 84.1% White; mean parental education fell between associate degree/diploma and undergraduate degree) assessed annually across three waves from late childhood and early adolescence (Mage = 10.72 years) to adolescence (Mage = 12.42 years) and then examined whether reactivity and regulation interacted to influence the development of shyness over time. At Wave 1, shyness was related to higher levels of reactivity and lower levels of regulation at Wave 2, but neither reactivity nor regulation at Wave 1 predicted shyness at Wave 2. At Wave 2, shyness predicted greater reactivity at Wave 3, but shyness at Wave 3 was only predicted by lower levels of regulation at Wave 2. Contrary to the reactivity-regulation model of shyness, we found that relatively high levels of reactivity and low levels of regulation predicted a steep decrease in shyness over 3 years. These results are discussed in the context of the socioemotional difficulties experienced by shy individuals and demonstrate the importance of empirically evaluating long-standing models of personality development. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
The relation between shyness and creativity has not been fully elucidated. The aim of present study was to examine if shy individuals express their creativity in different ways, as indexed by the relation between shyness and open-mindedness and its three facets: intellectual curiosity, aesthetic sensitivity, and creative imagination. We analyzed two separate structural equation models: first treating overall open-mindedness as a predictor of shyness, and the second in which we examined the three separate facets of open-mindedness (i.e., intellectual curiosity, aesthetic sensitivity, and creative imagination). Young adults (N = 727, Mage = 22.19 years) self-reported their levels of shyness and the three facets of open-mindedness. Results revealed that although shyness was unrelated to the broader construct of open-mindedness, the differentiation of the open-mindedness facets resulted in a good model fit to the data. Specifically, shyness was negatively associated with creative imagination, but positively associated with aesthetic sensitivity. Our findings illustrate the importance of examining different components of creativity, as these seem to be differentially associated with personality dimensions such as shyness. Our findings also have theoretical implications for understanding how shy individuals may express their creativity.
Article
Full-text available
We examined differential outcomes related to two distinct motivations for withdrawal (preference for solitude and shyness) as well as the possibility that support from important others (mothers, fathers, and best friends) attenuate any such links. Adolescents (159 males, 171 females) reported on their motivations to withdraw, internalizing symptoms, and relationship quality in 8th grade, as well as their anxiety and depression in 9th grade. Using structural equation modeling, we found that maternal support weakened the association between shyness and internalizing problems; friend support weakened the association between preference for solitude and depression; and friend support strengthened the association between shyness and depression. Our results suggest shy adolescents may not derive the same benefits from supportive friendships as their typical peers.
Article
Full-text available
Studies have shown that shy children born in the 1920s and 1950s had delayed marriage and parenthood, less stable careers, and lower occupational attainment as adults than other children. Do these effects still hold true? We examined demographic and social outcomes of children born between 1977 and 1982 in a prospective longitudinal study. We assessed shyness in childhood (age 8), adolescence (age 12–16), young adulthood (age 22–26), and adulthood (age 30–35), and derived three shyness trajectories (i.e., decreasing, increasing, and low-stable). Social and demographic outcomes for shy children who outgrew their shyness (i.e., decreasing trajectory) were indistinguishable from those who were consistently low on shyness measures. However, a shyness trajectory beginning in adolescence and increasing to adulthood was associated with poorer outcomes, similar to previous studies. These findings highlight the importance of multiple assessments in long-term longitudinal studies and the need to consider personality development within a generational context.
Article
Does infant temperament predict adult personality and life-course patterns? To date, there is scant evidence examining relations between child temperament and adult outcomes, and extant research has relied on limited methods for measuring temperament such as maternal report. This prospective longitudinal study followed a cohort of infants ( n = 165) for three decades to examine whether infant behavioral inhibition, a temperament characterized by cautious and fearful behaviors to unfamiliar situations, shapes long-term personality, social relationships, vocational/education, and mental health outcomes in adulthood. At age 14 mo, behavioral inhibition was assessed using an observation paradigm. In adolescence (15 y; n = 115), error monitoring event-related potentials were measured in a flanker task. In adulthood (26 y; n = 109), personality, psychopathology, and sociodemographics were self-reported using questionnaires. We found that infants with higher levels of behavioral inhibition at 14 mo grew up to become more reserved and introverted adults (β = 0.34) with lower social functioning with friends and family (β = −0.23) at age 26. Infant behavioral inhibition was also a specific risk factor for adult internalizing (i.e., anxiety and depression, β = 0.20) psychopathology, rather than a transdiagnostic risk for general and externalizing psychopathology. We identified a neurophysiologic mechanism underlying risk and resilience for later psychopathology. Heightened error monitoring in adolescence moderated higher levels of adult internalizing psychopathology among behaviorally inhibited individuals. These findings suggest meaningful continuity between infant temperament and the development of adult personality. They provide the earliest evidence suggesting that the foundation of long-term well-being is rooted in individual differences in temperament observed in infancy.
Article
This study examined mean-levels of shyness from childhood to senescence. To improve validity in our shyness measure across age, we used two different but comparable shyness scales in adult and child/adolescent datasets that were created separately within a repeated cross-sectional design. In the adult analysis (N¼1,294, ages ¼ 17–86), measurement invariance (through multi-group confirmatory factor analysis and the alignment method) was established among age groups but not between the sexes; only comparisons across age were considered valid. Significant mean differences among adult groups indicated shyness followed an inverted U-shape pattern. In the child/adolescent sample (N¼554, ages ¼ 4–16), measurement invariance was found across sex but not age groups. Mean differences in adult shyness might have been tied to age-related social processes and measurement validity may have limited accurate assessment of shyness across child/adolescence. Issues associated with the validity of measuring shyness across the lifespan are discussed.
Article
OBJECTIVE: The current study investigated (a) how a composite measure of shyness comprising introversion and neuroticism relates to other well-known constructs involving social fears, and (b) whether mean levels of shyness vary for men and women depending on the adoption of various social roles. METHOD: Study 1 used a sample of 211 UK participants aged 17–70 (64% female; Mage = 47.90). Study 2 used data from a large cross-sectional data set with UK participants aged 17–70 (Ntarget = 552,663; 64% female; Mage = 34.19 years). RESULTS: Study 1 showed that shyness measured as a composite of introversion and neuroticism was highly correlated with other constructs involving social fears. Study 2 indicated that, controlling for various sociodemographic variables, females appeared to have higher levels, whereas males appeared to have lower levels of shyness. Males and females who were in employment had the lowest shyness levels, whereas those working in unskilled jobs had the highest levels and people working in sales the lowest levels of shyness. Participants in relationships had lower levels of shyness than those not in relationships, but parenthood was not associated with shyness. CONCLUSIONS: Mean levels of shyness are likely to vary according to adopted social roles, gender, and age.
Article
Informed by past theory and research on social withdrawal, the aims of this study were to investigate whether three subtypes of social withdrawal (shyness, avoidance, unsociability) are related with BIS and BAS, as hypothesized by leading theories. Also of interest was whether these three withdrawal subtypes are related uniquely to different theoretically-indicated outcomes during emerging adulthood, a developmental period that has received very little empirical attention in this area of research. Participants were 295 (Mage = 19.31 years) emerging adults who completed self-report measures assessing different motivations for social withdrawal, aggression, anxiety sensitivity, creativity, social anhedonia, and BIS/BAS. Structural equation modeling revealed findings that challenge theoretical models that assume that specific and varying combinations of BIS and BAS underlie different withdrawal subtypes. The models also revealed new evidence of specific and non-specific associations, including the first evidence of a potential benefit (creativity) associated with unsociability.