ArticlePDF Available

Prosociality during COVID‐19: Globally focussed solidarity brings greater benefits than nationally focussed solidarity

Authors:

Abstract

Many charities are appealing for donations to address problems caused by the COVID‐19 crisis. Two survey studies (total N = 500) tested predictors of donation intentions of British participants to help those suffering from the crisis in Britain (ingroup donations) and overseas (outgroup donations). Perceptions of international, global common fate in our success in managing and overcoming the crisis was positively associated with prosocial intentions towards both national ingroup and outgroup targets. In contrast, a desire to ‘close ranks’ within the national ingroup in the face of the pandemic threat was associated with more prosocial intentions towards national ingroup targets only, but it was associated with fewer prosocial intentions towards outgroup targets. This suggests that a focus on global solidarity (in the form of global common fate and identification with all humans) has positive effects on helping both within and across group boundaries, whereas a focus on national solidarity (in the form of ‘closing ranks’ in the face of the pandemic threat) has positive effects on helping within the national group but negative effects on prosocial tendencies towards outgroup members. The effect of perceived global common fate on both ingroup and outgroup helping was mediated by identification with all of humanity. Findings are discussed in terms of practical implications for managing the COVID‐19 crisis. Please refer to the Supplementary Material section to find this article's Community and Social Impact Statement.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Prosociality during COVID-19: Globally focussed
solidarity brings greater benefits than nationally
focussed solidarity
Hanna Zagefka
Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway,
University of London, Egham, UK
Correspondence
Hanna Zagefka, Department of Psychology,
Royal Holloway, University of London, Egham
TW20 0EX, UK.
Email: Hanna.Zagefka@rhul.ac.uk
Abstract
Many charities are appealing for donations to address prob-
lems caused by the COVID-19 crisis. Two survey studies
(total N=500) tested predictors of donation intentions of
British participants to help those suffering from the crisis in
Britain (ingroup donations) and overseas (outgroup dona-
tions). Perceptions of international, global common fate in
our success in managing and overcoming the crisis was pos-
itively associated with prosocial intentions towards both
national ingroup and outgroup targets. In contrast, a desire
to close rankswithin the national ingroup in the face of
the pandemic threat was associated with more prosocial
intentions towards national ingroup targets only, but it was
associated with fewer prosocial intentions towards outgroup
targets. This suggests that a focus on global solidarity
(in the form of global common fate and identification with
all humans) has positive effects on helping both within and
across group boundaries, whereas a focus on national soli-
darity (in the form of closing ranksin the face of the pan-
demic threat) has positive effects on helping within the
national group but negative effects on prosocial tendencies
towards outgroup members. The effect of perceived global
common fate on both ingroup and outgroup helping was
mediated by identification with all of humanity. Findings are
discussed in terms of practical implications for managing
Received: 15 January 2021 Revised: 24 May 2021 Accepted: 24 May 2021
DOI: 10.1002/casp.2553
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
© 2021 The Author. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
J Community Appl Soc Psychol. 2021;114. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/casp 1
the COVID-19 crisis. Please refer to the Supplementary
Material section to find this article's Community and Social
Impact Statement.
KEYWORDS
common fate, COVID-19, donation, identification with all of
humanity, intergroup helping, prosociality, solidarity
1|INTRODUCTION
The COVID-19 pandemic has been gripping the globe for an extended period of time, and this is posing unprece-
dented challenges for the charity sector. Many people are feeling the impact of the lockdown-induced economic
downturn on their personal finances. Charitable donations have declined (Charities Aid Foundation, 2020), making
competition for funding tougher. At the same time, the need of those suffering from the economic and health
impacts of the pandemic, particularly among those who were already disadvantaged before the crisis, is growing. The
goal of this work was to investigate what motivates people to donate money to help others suffering from the
impacts of the COVID-19 pandemicboth those who are members of the national ingroup and those who are mem-
bers of national outgroups. There is a wealth of fundraising appeals addressing coronavirus-related problems both
domestically and internationally, and this work set out to test predictors of donating to causes at home and abroad.
Considered were the effects of a perception of global common fate in managing the pandemic, potential effects of
this on identification with all of humanity, and effects of a desire to close ranksat the national level to manage the
crisis. As such, a focus on global solidarity was juxtapositioned with a focus on national solidarity, to chart their
effects on prosocial behaviours within and across national group boundaries.
1.1 |Theoretical background: Group identities and helping
The theoretical foundations to this investigation lie in the literature on social identities, group processes, and
prosocial behaviour. There are three ways that are relevant here in which social identities inform prosocial behaviour.
First, in categorical terms, people are more likely to help ingroup members than outgroup members. By and large,
help is more readily forthcoming for others that are part of the social ingroup (Bloom, 2017; Stuermer &
Snyder, 2010; van Leeuwen & Zagefka, 2017). For example, those who incorporate other humans across ethnic, reli-
gious, and national boundaries into their moral ingroupare more likely to offer help, even in the face of potentially
great personal sacrifice, as has been demonstrated in studies of those who assisted Jewish people in Nazi-occupied
Europe (Reykowski, 2002). Ingroup bias in donation decisions has also been demonstrated in the context of
COVID-19 (Li, Li, Jiang, & Liu, 2020). Of course, people do, on occasion, also extend kindness towards outgroup
members (Sierksma, Thijs, & Verkuyten, 2015; Stürmer, Snyder, & Omoto, 2005; Vezzali, Cadamuro, Versari,
Giovannini, & Trifiletti, 2015), but generally ingroup helping is more easily triggered. Thus, whether someone is seen
as an ingroup member or an outgroup member affects the likelihood that this person will be helped when in need.
Second, in correlational terms, the more people identify with and are psychologically engaged in their ingroup,
the more important that group is to their sense of self, and the more they are motivated to help others in that group.
Strength of identification with social categories clearly affects helping decisions, in the form of donations and other
prosocial behaviours (e.g., Chapman, Masser, & Louis, 2020; Drury et al., 2019). Thus, helping decisions are not only
affected by categorical ingroup versus outgroupcategorization of a given target. Rather, once a target has been
categorized as ingroup, the extent of the potential helper's identification with the common identity also matters.
2ZAGEFKA
There is clear evidence that greater perceived oneness between self and other (Cialdini, Brown, Lewis, &
Neuberg, 1997) and greater fusion with the group (Swann, G
omez, Huici, Morales, & Hixon, 2010) increase ingroup
helping. Ingroup identification has been shown to be linked to behaviours designed to protect other ingroup mem-
bers also in the context of COVID-19 (Liekefett & Becker, 2021). Thus, the more strongly someone is identified with
their group, the more likely is it that this person will feel moved to act prosocially towards other ingroup members.
Third, group memberships are malleable and depend on situational salience and context (Jetten, Reicher,
Haslam, & Cruwys, 2020; Turner, 1987). New shared identities can be created and arise from novel circumstances,
such as shared adversity (Ntontis, Drury, Amlôt, Rubin, & Williams, 2020; see also Solnit, 2010). Thus, if identities
are pliable, and if they are related to prosociality, this opens the opportunity to use them as a lever to increase
prosociality and solidarity in the face of humanitarian emergencies such as the COVID-19 pandemic.
To be sure, there are many factors other than those related to identity that affect whether prosocial behaviour
will be displayed. For example, there might be individual differences in values and prosocial tendencies (Politi, Van
Assche, Caprara, & Phalet, 2021) that impact on decisions to help. In this current article, we chose to focus on vari-
ables related to identities and global and national solidarity for two reasons. First, at least in Britain, appeals to
national solidarity were featured frequently in the media and in soundbites by politicians at the start of the pandemic
and throughout, and we were interested in studying the effects of this. For example, citizens were told to comply
with social distancing rules for the sake of Queen and country(The Economist, 2020), to protect the NHS(with
the NHS being the National Health Service, a cherished national institution subject of much national pride, Depart-
ment of Health and Social Care, 2021), and to display national unity(BBC, 2020b).
Second, there was growing concern among health professionals about a lack of global solidarity, for example, in
the form of vaccine nationalism(e.g., The Guardian, 2021), and we were interested in the psychological processes
that might help address this problem. For example, the WHO warned that the world's poorest risk being trampled in
stampede for vaccines(The Telegraph, 2020). Hence, there seemed to be a mismatch between the nationally
focussed rhetoric employed by politicians, and the need for a global and international perspective highlighted as nec-
essary by health experts (e.g., The Guardian, 2020).
For these reasons, the focus of this work was on variables related to global solidarity, in the form of perceived
common fate and identification with all humanity, and national solidarity, in the form of a desire to close rankswith
other national ingroup members in the light of the pandemic threat. In this way, the effects of global solidarity was
juxtapositioned with the effects of national solidarity.
1.2 |Factors related to global solidarity: Perceived global common fate, and
identification with all humanity
There are reasons to propose that perceived global common fate and shared threat will increase identification with others
who share the same fate, and through this with help extended to those others, also in the context of the COVID-19 crisis.
As demonstrated in the context of many disasters, suffering from the same adversity can create new shared identities and
solidarity with others who would formerly have been seen as outgroup members (Bowe et al., 2021; Ntontis et al. 2021;
Solnit, 2010). For example, in the context of a flooded community, common fate was positively associated with an emerg-
ing sense of community and identification with the group, as well as with social support (Ntontis et al., 2020).
Support can also be gleamed from the results of some classic experiments: Schachter (1959) demonstrated that
participants induced to be jointly anxious were more likely to seek out the company of others, confirming the effect
of shared threat on affiliation-seeking behaviour. A number of other theoretical approaches have emphasized joint
threat, uncertainty and anxiety as important motives for seeking group belonging and affiliation (e.g., Baumeister &
Leary, 1995). The fact that perceived common fate can give rise to a sense of shared identity is supported by evi-
dence that suggests that a common threat invokes recategorization, so that those formerly seen as outgroup mem-
bers become part of the common ingroup (Flade, Klar, & Imhoff, 2019), and it is supported by evidence that shows
ZAGEFKA 3
that a common perceived threat increases identification with others exposed to this threat (Schmid &
Muldoon, 2015). The fact that perceived common fate is linked to not only to affiliation-seeking but also to helping
for fellow sufferers is further supported by laboratory studies which find that participants are more likely to help
others who are facing a similar fate to themselves (Dovidio & Morris, 1975). Other work, too, supports the notion
that shared threat motivates ingroup strengthening behaviours (Wohl, Branscombe, & Reysen, 2010).
The findings reviewed above speak to an effect of common fateon identificationand to an effect of common
fateon helping. In addition, as also reviewed above there is evidence that shared group membership and identifica-
tionwith a social category increases helpingtowards other members of that category. The strong previous evidence
for a link between group identification and helping suggests that identification might function as a mediator, so that
shared common fate increases identification, which in turn increases prosocial intentions towards ingroup members.
It was expected that these mechanisms wouldalsoholdtrueinthecontextofthe COVID-19 crisis. One feature that
makes the pandemic interesting in theoretical terms is that it constitutes a rarely precedented situation, where the problems
caused by the COVID-19 pandemic threat are shared not only with co-nationals within one's country, but with all humans
globally. COVID-19 poses a threat to humanity that does not stop at national borders. Because the COVID-19 pandemic
adversely impacts virtually all humans, albeit in unequal measure (see, e.g., BBC, 2020a), this devastating event allows us to
test whether the perceived shared common fate on a global level increases identification with all humans (McFarland,
Webb, & Brown, 2012), and prosocial tendencies towards all humansincluding both national ingroup and outgroup mem-
bers. Identification with all of humanityis a construct that was introduced by McFarland et al. (2012) to capture an orienta-
tion whereby humanity is not perceived in terms of ethnic, religious or other divisions, but where it is perceived as one all-
inclusive group with all members being entitled to fair, non-prejudiced treatment. Given that the pandemic has created a sit-
uation where humans can potentially develop an increased awareness of their mutual interdependence and globally shared
threats, this situation is well suited to assessing the potency of the construct all of humanityfor shaping social behaviour.
Hence, it was expected that a perception that humanity faces a shared global common fate in the face of coronavi-
rus would be related to greater identification with all of humanity, and that this variable would mediate the positive
effects of perceived global common fate on all others suffering from the crisis, including both national ingroup members
and national outgroup members, since regardless of nationality all humans are fellow ingroup members in the overarch-
ing category of humanity. A focus on global solidarity, in the form of global common fate and identification with all of
humanity, was thus proposed to motivate prosocial acts both within but also across national group boundaries.
1.3 |Factors related to national solidarity: National closing ranks
As indicated above, a survey of media coverage of political responses to the pandemic in Britain suggests that
appeals by politicians to national solidarity have been much more frequent than appeals to global solidarity. Political
leaders often use external threats as an opportunity to cement their own leadership by calling for the population to
rally round the flagand close ranks, that is, to move closer together, psychologically speaking, in the face of adver-
sity (Reicher & Hopkins, 2001). Because external threats can be used by politicians to further their own agendas
(Reicher & Hopkins, 2001), it is maybe no surprise that politicians in Britain have capitalized on this narrative, also in
the context of COVID-19. National identities are easily invoked (Billig, 1995), they are flagged by politicians and
in the media through symbols and habits of language. In the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, too, perceptions of
threat are open to manipulation (Lalot, Abrams, & Travaglino, 2021), and they have important behavioural conse-
quences, such as, indeed, increasing support for political leaders (Yam et al., 2020).
In Britain, such sentiments of national closing ranks, standing shoulder to shoulderand demonstrating greater
national unityare often invoked by references to the Dunkirk spiritto this day, memories of Britain's war effort
and success during WWII go a long way to engender feelings of national pride and unity in this country (e.g., The Evening
Express, 2021; The Telegraph, 2021). The phrase, first used sometime in 1940, refers to the evacuation of the British
troops from northern France after German military advances. The evacuation took place under constant air attack and
4ZAGEFKA
using warships but also a flotilla of small civilian pleasure boats and civilian working barges. The successful evacuation was
then referenced by politicians to raise British morale and this gave rise to the notion of Dunkirk spirit. The phrase implies
refusal to surrender when Britain is faced with an external threat. It invokes nationalist sentiments and is hence an image
popular with the political right, but it has also connotations of solidarity and pulling togetherin times of adversity. As seen
above, closer affinity and identification with a social group positively affects willingness to act prosocially towards others in
that group. Because of this, appeals to the idea of closing ranksatthenationallevelinthefaceoftheCOVID-19threat
can be assumed to be related to greater prosocial tendencies towards fellow national ingroup members. Greater solidarity
at the national level implies greater psychological closeness with fellow nationals. Because prosocial behaviour is encour-
aged by greater perceived closeness to, similarity with, and oneness with the target of help, national closing ranksshould
encourage help for national ingroup members suffering due to COVID-19. After all, the idea that the national ingroup
needs to pull togetherto defeat an external threat does not only imply that oneself will be helped by other group mem-
bers, but it also implies that oneself has an obligation to help other ingroup members.
At the same time, an appeal to national solidarity throws into sharp relief group boundaries that exclude those who
are not part of the national ingroup. National solidarity clearly stipulates that certain actions and sacrifices are required
for the benefit of the national ingroup, whereas national outgroups are defined as being outside of the moral sphere of
obligation. Because of this, it is hypothesized that national closing rankswould be positively related to help extended
to fellow nationals, but that it would be negatively related to help extended to national outgroup members.
1.4 |Summary of hypotheses, and the present research
In sum, then, it was proposed that a perception of global common fate in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic would
be positively associated with prosocial helping, in the form of donation intentions to benefit both national ingroup and
outgroup members (Hypothesis 1). In contrast, it was proposed that a desire to close ranksat the national level would
be positively associated with donation intentions to benefit national ingroup members, but that it would decrease dona-
tion tendencies towards national outgroup members (Hypothesis 2). Moreover, it was hypothesized that the effect of
global common fate on donations would be mediated by identification with all of humanity, so that shared fate would
be positively associated with identification with all humans, which would in turn be positively associated with
prosociality towards all humans, including both national ingroup and outgroup members (Hypothesis 3).
To test our predictions, two studies were conducted. The first study took place in October 2020, and the second
study was conducted in November 2020, to replicate and confirm the effects. Predictions were identical across both
studies, but two studies were conducted to meet standards for the demonstration of replicability of results. Study
1 asked British participants about their willingness to help fellow British nationals (ingroup members), and their
willingness to help those suffering from the effects of the pandemic in China and Syria. China was chosen as an out-
group target of interest because the virus first emerged from China, making it an interesting comparison point. Syria
was chosen because at the time of the study there were some actual charitable donation appeals by humanitarian
charities focussing on the devastating impact of the pandemic on refugee populations.
2|STUDY 1
2.1 |Method
2.1.1 | Participants
The sample consisted of 250 people recruited via the Prolific platform in October 2020 (mean age =36 years, age
range 1870, 172 females, 77 males, 1 non-binary person). All participants were required to have British nationality.
ZAGEFKA 5
The sample size was determined by the fact that effects stabilize around N=250 (Schönbrodt & Perugini, 2013),
and that this sample size generated sufficient power to similar effects in previous work on intergroup helping in the
context of COVID-19 (James & Zagefka, 2017; Zagefka, 2021). Moreover, an a priori power analysis with G*Power
(Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007) was conducted, assuming a slope of .16 and α=.05 and aiming for a power
of 0.80. This recommended N=237, and we conservatively recruited N=250 to be sure to achieve at least this
desired level of power.
2.1.2 | Measures
All measures were assessed with Likert scales (1 =strongly disagreeto 5 =strongly agree). To start with, partici-
pants were told that the survey questions all related to the COVID-19 crisis.
Perceived global common fate was measured with four items: We need to address coronavirus as a global com-
munity,Citizens of the world need to address the coronavirus problem together,The British success in addressing
coronavirus depends on global cooperation, and The British fate is interlinked with that of other coun-
tries,α=.77.
National closing rankswas measured with three items: When threatened from outside, the British people must
stick together,In the face of an external threat, we British must rally round the flag, and When facing threats from
outside Britain, we must all do our bit to defend the British people,α=.83.
Identification with all of humanity was measured with two items inspired by McFarland et al.'s scale (2012): I
identify with all humans, and I see myself as a citizen of the world,α=.65.
Donations to ingroup victims was measured with two items previously used by Zagefka, Noor, Randsley de
Moura, Hopthrow, and Brown (2011): I would be willing to donate money to British coronavirus victims, and I
think it is important to donate money to British coronavirus victims,α=.82.
Donations to outgroup victims were measured with items worded like the ones above, but this time asking about
donations to Chinese coronavirus victims and coronavirus victims in Syria, α=.92 for this 4-item measure. The order
in which ingroup and outgroup donations were measured was randomized.
1
All aspects of this and the subsequent studies were in line with British Psychological Society ethics guidelines.
The data for this study and Study 2 can be found here: https://osf.io/m67fa/?view_only=a66ce2de2d3c49c393
99bcecdf1f1959.
2.2 |Results and discussion
Bivariate correlations and means are displayed in Table 1 (values below the diagonal). To test whether perceived
global common fate and national closing rankswould have the predicted effects on ingroup and outgroup helping,
that is, H1 and H2, respectively, two linear regressions were run with these two variables as predictor variables.
Results are displayed in Table 2.
When predicting ingroup donations, the overall model was significant, F(2,247) =3.27, p< .05, R
2
=0.03, and
this was also the case when predicting outgroup donations, F(2,247) =18.18, p< .001, R
2
=0.13. As is apparent, in
line with H1 perceived global common fate was positively related to donations. However, although according to H1
positive associations were expected for both outgroup and ingroup helping, the association with ingroup helping did
not reach significance. In line with H2, national closing rankswas positively related to ingroup donations, but nega-
tively related to outgroup donations.
To test H3, whether the effect of perceived global common fateon outgroup donationswas mediated by
identification with all of humanity, Hayes' (2018) PROCESS macro Model 4 was used. Perceived global common
fate was related to identification with all of humanity, b=0.65, SE =0.07, 95% CI [0.67; 1.85]. Identification with all
6ZAGEFKA
of humanity was, in turn, related to outgroup donations, b=0.40, SE =0.09, 95% CI [0.22; 0.57]. As expected, there
was an indirect effect of global common fate on outgroup donations, b=0.26, SE =0.06, 95% CI [0.14; 0.38].
Results are illustrated in Figure 1.
2
To test the second part of H3, whether the effect of perceived global common fateon ingroup donations
was mediated by identification with all of humanity, again Hayes' (2018) PROCESS macro Model 4 was used. Per-
ceived global common fate was related to identification with all of humanity, b=0.65, SE =0.07, 95% CI [0.52;
0.79]. Identification with all of humanity was, in turn, related to ingroup donations, b=0.17, SE =0.08, 95% CI
[0.007; 0.33]. As expected, there was an indirect effect of global common fate on ingroup donations, b=0.11,
SE =0.06, 95% CI [0.004; 0.24].
3
Taken together, clear evidence was found for all three hypotheses. Perceived global common fate was positively
associated with help for outgroup members and ingroup members, although the latter did not reach significance
(H1). In contrast, a desire to close ranksat the national level was positively associated with help for national ingroup
members but it did also have a strongly negative relationship with help for outgroup members (H2). In other words,
while an emphasis on global solidarity had positive associations with prosociality within and across group boundaries,
national solidarity had positive associations with prosociality within the ingroup but it was negatively related to
prosociality with outgroup members. This suggests that in order to foster maximum benefits in terms of prosociality,
one would be well advised to favour a global perspective over a national perspective.
Mediation analyses confirmed that the effect of perceived global common fate on help for outgroup members
and ingroup members was indeed due to a greater degree of identification with all of humanity (H3). As expected, a
perception of having a common destiny and fortune was positively associated with participants feeling closer to and
more connected with national outgroup members, which in turn was positively linked to prosocial tendencies
towards them. These results support the idea that having a common destiny or facing a common adversity can help
redefine identities in such a way that former outgroup members become included in a common ingroup, and there-
fore become part of one's moral ingroup and deserving of altruistic assistance. Following these encouraging results, a
second study was run to confirm the robustness of the effects.
TABLE 1 Bivariate correlations and means, Studies 1 (N=250) and 2 (N=250)
1. Donations
to ingroup
victims
2. Donations to
outgroup
victims
3. National
closing ranks
4. Global
common fate
5. Identification
with all of
humanity
1. Donations to ingroup victims .55*** .18** .21*** .20***
2. Donations to outgroup victims .52*** .17** .15* .20***
3. National closing ranks .12* .33*** .09 .06
4. Global common fate .10 .14* .001 .31***
5. Identification with all of humanity .16** .30*** .18** .52***
Study 1
Means
3.20 2.81 3.61 4.30 4.08
Study 1
SD
0.89 0.99 0.86 0.62 0.78
Study 2
Means
3.17 2.64 3.38 3.89 3.97
Study 2
SD
0.99 1.00 0.89 0.74 0.76
Note: Study 1 correlations below the diagonal and Study 2 correlations above the diagonal. The scale for both studies
ranged 15 with 5 indicating strong endorsement of the construct.
*p< .05; **p< .01; ***p< .001.
ZAGEFKA 7
TABLE 2 Regression results predicting ingroup and outgroup helping from perceived global common fate and national closing ranks, Studies 1 (N=250) and 2 (N=250)
DV: Donations to ingroup victims DV: Donations to outgroup victims
Standardized
beta Unstandardized B
Lower
confidence
interval
Upper
confidence
interval
Standardized
beta Unstandardized B
Lower
confidence
interval
Upper confidence
interval
Study 1 results
Perceived global common fate .10 .15 .04 .33 .14* .23 .05 .43
National closing ranks.12* .13 .01 .26 .33*** .38 .55 .24
Study 2 results
Perceived global common fate .20** .26 .08 .44 .16* .22 .04 .42
National closing ranks.16* .18 0.20 0.34 .19** .21 .36 .07
*p< .05; **p< .01; ***p< .001.
8ZAGEFKA
3|STUDY 2
3.1 |Method
3.1.1 | Participants
A second sample of 250 people was recruited via the Prolific platform in late November 2020 (mean age =35 years,
age range 1877, 169 females, 80 males, 1 differently identified). All participants were required to have British
nationality. The sample size was designed to match that of Study 1.
3.1.2 | Measures
All measures were assessed with Likert scales (1 =strongly disagreeto 5 =strongly agree). To start with, partici-
pants were told that the survey questions all related to the COVID-19 crisis.
Perceived global common fate,national closing ranks, and identification with all of humanity were measured with
items very similar or identical to those used in study 1, α=.78 for the four item measure of global common fate,
α=.83 for the three item measure of national closing ranks, and α=.69 for the two item measure of identification
with all of humanity.
Donations to ingroup victims was measured with three items, preceded by the question Would you be willing to
donate money to help people suffering from the coronavirus crisis?:I would donate to help British people in trou-
ble,I would want to offer financial support to British people in trouble, and Donating money to British people is
the right thing to do,α=.95.
Donations to outgroup victims were measured with the same three items, but this time tailored to the target of
Chinese people suffering due to the COVID-19 crisis, α=.95. The order in which ingroup and outgroup donations
were measured was randomized. Because the two outgroups in Study 1 yielded very similar responses, to the point
that it was sensible to not treat them separately, in Study 2 a decision was made to only focus on one outgroup.
3.2 |Results and discussion
Bivariate correlations and means are displayed in Table 1 (values above the diagonal). To test whether perceived
global common fate and national closing rankswould have the predicted effects on ingroup and outgroup helping,
FIGURE 1 Identification with all of humanity mediates the effect of perceived global common fate on ingroup
and outgroup helping. Unstandardized path coefficients for Study 1 (N=250) above the arrows and for Study 2
(N=250) below the arrows. ***p< .001
ZAGEFKA 9
respectively, again two linear regressions were run with these two variables as predictor variables. Results are dis-
played in Table 2.
When predicting ingroup donations, the overall model was significant, F(2,247) =9.26, p< .05, R
2
=0.07, and
this was also the case when predicting outgroup donations, F(2,247) =7.35, p< .001, R
2
=0.06. Again, perceived
global common fate was positively related to outgroup donations and ingroup donations (significantly so for both tar-
get groups), whereas national closing rankswas positively related to ingroup donations, but negatively related to
outgroup donations. Taken together, again H1 and H2 were supported.
To test whether the effect of perceived global common fate on outgroup donationswas mediated by identifi-
cation with all of humanity (H3), again Hayes' (2018) PROCESS macro Model 4 was used. Mirroring the results of
the previous study, perceived global common fate was again related to identification with all of humanity, b=0.32,
SE =0.06, 95% CI [0.20; 0.44]. Identification with all of humanity was, in turn, related to outgroup donations,
b=0.24, SE =0.09, 95% CI [0.07; 0.41]. As expected, again there was an indirect effect of global common fate on
outgroup donations, b=0.08, SE =0.03, 95% CI [0.02; 0.15]. Results are illustrated in Figure 1.
Testing, with Hayes' Model 4, whether the effect of perceived global common fate on ingroup donationswas
mediated by identification with all of humanity (H3), perceived global common fate was related to identification with
all of humanity, b=0.32, SE =0.06, 95% CI [0.20; 0.44]. Identification with all of humanity was, in turn, related to
ingroup donations, b=0.20, SE =0.08, 95% CI [0.03; 0.36]. As expected, there was an indirect effect of global com-
mon fate on ingroup donations, b=0.06, SE =0.03, 95% CI [0.01; 0.13].
4
Taken together, again there was clear support for all three hypotheses. Perceived global common fate was posi-
tively associated with prosociality towards both ingroup and outgroup targets (H1), whereas national closing ranks
was positively associated only with prosociality towards ingroup targets, but it was negatively associated with
prosociality with outgroup targets (H2). Once more, the effects of global common fate on both ingroup and outgroup
helping were mediated by identification with all of humanity (H3).
4|OVERALL DISCUSSION
Results are in line with the notion that a perception of global common fate increases donation intentions that benefit
both national ingroup and national outgroup members and that it does so via enhanced identification with all of
humanity, whereas a desire to close ranksat the national level increases donations intentions that benefit national
ingroup members but decreases helping extended across national group boundaries. These findings have some impor-
tant theoretical but also practical implications.
In terms of theoretical implications, the findings confirm the usefulness of the category all of humanity. This
construct has previously been introduced and measured to explain a range of intergroup phenomena, for example,
the actions of those who helped Jews during the Nazi regime in Europe (McFarland, Brown, & Webb, 2013). How-
ever, there has rarely been a situation where all of humanity is facing the same immediate threat as is the case with
the current pandemic. This article capitalized on the opportunity to demonstrate that identification with all of
humanity can be triggered by perceived global common fate, and that it might be particularly important for psycho-
logical responses to events that can be construed as a threat at either the national or global level. Another example
of such a threat could be climate change.
A further theoretical implication is that the findings confirm the importance of identity processes in informing
people's helping decisions. The findings are in line with what would be expected on the grounds of social identity
theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1986): if the ingroup is perceived as facing a threat, this often increases identification with
the group and also prosociality towards ingroup members. COVID-19 is undoubtedly one such threat that can be
expected to generate such processes (Jetten et al., 2020).
Relatedly, another theoretical implication is that the same theoretical predictors of prosociality can have conflu-
ent or divergent effects on ingroup and outgroup helping. In addition to once again underscoring the importance of
10 ZAGEFKA
social identities for the study of prosociality, this means that group memberships might not only have direct, main
effects on the extent of help, but that they might also moderate the effects of variables associated with prosociality
(on the idea of group memberships as moderators, see also Stürmer et al., 2005). In other words, the findings suggest
that considering group memberships in the context of prosociality can lead to a more nuanced picture than
approaches that focus merely on individual differences and intrapersonal processes.
Last but not least, the present work goes beyond the existing literature in one important way. It tests, simulta-
neously, the concurrent roles of (a) threat external to the ingroup and (b) threat external to both the ingroup and the
outgroup, as predictors of ingroup and outgroup helping. This dual focus is novel, and the findings present some ini-
tial insights which speak to the implications for prosociality when considered within a framework that incorporates
both threat and multiple levels of categorization.
Having said this, maybe even more important than the theoretical implications are practical implications of these
findings. These are twofold: insights for the generation of charitable donations more generally, and insights for
behaviour change in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic.
In general terms, charitable organizations need to understand what motivates people to donate to charitable
causes. Psychological factors that drive donations can be used to design more effective fundraising appeals. The pre-
sent findings confirm that highlighting commonalities between potential donors and recipients of donations will be
beneficial. Any message that highlights camaraderie and unity between the two entities should positively affect
donation proclivity. Appeals to solidarity, and the idea of unity in the face of common threat and fate, are likely to
increase donations. Moreover, of course different fundraising appeals are often in stiff competition with one another,
and the findings highlight that measures that elicit donations at one level of categorization (e.g., the national ingroup)
will often have detrimental effects at other levels of categorization (e.g., national outgroups). Careful consideration
of funding priorities that takes into account these interdependent effects is therefore advisable.
Having said this, maybe the most important practical implications of the present findings are specific to the
COVID-19 situation. In agreement with Drury, Carter, Ntontis, and Guven (2021), the findings confirm that group
processes can be harnessed to bring about behaviour change in the context of this pandemic, and this present work
applied this general principle to the realm of prosociality and donations designed to mitigate the impacts of the pan-
demic. Appeals that emphasize certain unique features of the COVID-19 situation, for example, goal
interdependence between different countries and the fact that all humans are united in facing the same adversity,
are likely to increase willingness to donate to the COVID-19 response both at home and abroad.
Clearly, there is a mismatch between the prevailing political rhetoric regarding the pandemic, and the messaging
that can be expected to bring the greatest benefits in the context of this global pandemic. British politicians are empha-
sizing national unity, which was shown here to have detrimental effects on willingness to extend help across national
boundaries. Yet, as seen above, the only feasible way out of the pandemic is international collaboration. Solidarity with
those suffering the impacts of the coronavirus in the world's poorest countries is not only vital because of principles of
fairness and human decency, but it is also in the self-interest of the rich nations: as repeatedly emphasized by health
experts, vaccinating an entire wealthy nation is not going to bring long-term solutions when the virus is allowed to
spread freely in other parts of the world, mutilate there, and boomerang back (The Guardian, 2020). In order to foster
support for the idea of global cooperation and mutual international help, messages that focus on global solidarity are
needed, rather than the messages that focus on national solidarity. The implications of the present results are that in
order to generate support for measures that are needed to overcome the crisis, the political rhetoric needs to move
away from a focus on national unity, and put more emphasis on the need for global solidarity. National protectionism is
not going to be a viable solution for managing this global pandemic, which requires a global solution.
The present work has, of course, some important limitations, which at the same time point to avenues for fur-
ther research and improvement. Most importantly, the data are correlational, which means that they cannot support
any claims about the causal direction of observed effects. Further, experimental data would be needed for that.
Moreover, the present research only studied self-reported intentions to donate, rather than actual donation behav-
iour. Therefore, a degree of uncertainty remains about whether the processes identified here would also affect
ZAGEFKA 11
donation behaviour. Having said this, good evidence exists that often self-reports are a valid proxy for actual behav-
iour. Although people tend to over-report helping compared to actual behaviour in terms of mean levels, self-reports
and actual behaviour are quite highly correlated (Bekkers & Wiepking, 2011; Zagefka et al., 2011). Therefore, self-
reports are likely to be a good proxy for actual behaviour in studies like the present which assess correlational pat-
terns. Nonetheless, future work could expand on the present design by incorporating a measure of actual behaviour.
In addition to addressing the methodological limitations acknowledged above, future work could also address some
interesting conceptual issues. Most importantly, it is not clear from this study whether the processes found here are spe-
cific to COVID-19, or whether they would emerge for other challenges that can be perceived as being shared by national
ingroup members or indeed all of humanity. Other such challenges exist, with climate change being a prime example.
Future work would be needed to establish in how far the processes generalize to other global problems.
In sum, the findings suggest that a focus on global solidarity is preferable over national solidarity, not only on the
ground of humanitarian principles, but also because only a global focus is well aligned with an international solution to
the crisis, which is the only feasible solution according to health experts. Given that the pandemic is, at present, causing
simultaneously an increased need for funds to address COVID-19-related problems and a reduction in available funds
due to a global economic downturn, it is hoped that the psychological mechanisms described here can be useful to
increase the effectiveness of fundraising appeal design, and through this the management of the global pandemic.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The data for the study can be found here: https://osf.io/m67fa/?view_only=a66ce2de2d3c49c39399bcecdf1f1959
ORCID
Hanna Zagefka https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1598-0059
ENDNOTES
1
The questionnaires for both studies also included questions such as about blame attributions for the crisis and anxiety
related to the crisis. The study was designed to test both the processes outlined here and, separately, a hypothesis about
blame attributions and how they relate to helping. Blame attributions are not relevant for the present hypotheses and will
not be discussed further here.
2
In theory, it would be possible to test both hypotheses jointly using Structural Equation Modelling (SEM). However, ingroup
donations and outgroup donations tend to be very highly correlated, as indeed they should be, given that both are strongly
driven by dispositional tendencies for altruism and empathy (see, e.g., Politi et al., 2021). Therefore, including them simulta-
neously as unrelated outcome variables in one model is not feasible. Although it would be possible to estimate two SEMs,
one for each outcome variable, this would add nothing over and above the regression and mediation analyses presented here.
3
Two structural equation models (SEMs) were also estimated, one predicting ingroup help and the other outgroup help.
Given that across the three hypotheses there are two main predictors and one mediator, the goal was to consider both
exogenous variables simultaneously when testing for mediation, to avoid risking inflation of the mediation hypothesis. In
the SEMs, global common fateand national closing rankswere the exogenous variables which were allowed to covary.
The model specified an indirect effect of global common fate on helping via identification with all of humanity, and it also
allowed for both an indirect effect (via identification with all of humanity) as well as a direct effect of national closing
rankson helping. Both models fitted the data well (χ
2
s5.57, CFIs 0.90, RMSEAs 0.001). Crucially, the indirect
effects of global common fatewere significant in both instances, with the standardized effect being 0.10, p< .02 when
predicting ingroup helping, and 0.13, p< .02 when predicting outgroup helping.
4
Again, SEMs tested, as for the previous study, if the indirect effects of global common fate on helping would still be signif-
icant in a model that also simultaneously included national closing ranks. Again, this was the case, with the standardized
indirect effect being 0.06, p< .004 when predicting ingroup helping, and 0.07, p< .008 when predicting outgroup helping.
REFERENCES
Baumeister, R., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human
motivation. Psychological Bulletin,117, 497529. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497
BBC (2020a). Coronavirus: Ethnic minorities are a thirdof patients. Retrieved from https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/uk-52255863
12 ZAGEFKA
BBC (2020b). Covid: PM calls for unityas he agrees to publish data behind new tiers. Retrieved from https://www.bbc.co.
uk/news/uk-55125712
Bekkers, R., & Wiepking, P. (2011). Accuracy of self-reports on donations to charitable organizations. Quality & Quantity:
International Journal of Methodology,45, 13691383. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-010-9341-9
Billig, M. (1995). Banal nationalism. London: Sage.
Bloom, P. (2017). Against empathy. The case for rationale compassion. New York: Ecco.
Bowe, M., Wakefield, J. R. H., Kellezi, B., Stevenson, C., McNamara, N., Jones, B. A., Heym, N. (2021). The mental health
benefits of community helping during crisis: Coordinated helping, community identification and sense of unity during
the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology,115. https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.2520
Chapman, C. M., Masser, B. M., & Louis, W. R. (2020). Identity motives in charitable giving: Explanations for charity prefer-
ences from a global donor survey. Psychology & Marketing,37, 12771291. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.21362
Charities Aid Foundation (2020). Retrieved from https://www.cafonline.org/docs/default-source/about-us-publications/
caf-charity-coronavirus-briefing-june-2020.pdf
Cialdini, R. B., Brown, S. L., Lewis, C. L., & Neuberg, S. L. (1997). Reinterpreting the empathy-altruism relationship: When
one into one equals oneness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,73, 481494.
Department of Health and Social Care (2021). Press release: New TV advert urges public to stay at home to protect the
NHS and save lives. Retrieved from https://www.gov.uk/government/news/new-tv-advert-urges-public-to-stay-at-
home-to-protect-the-nhs-and-save-lives
Dovidio, J. F., & Morris, W. N. (1975). Effects of stress and commonality of fate on helping behavior. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology,31, 145149. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0076236
Drury, J., Carter, H., Cocking, C., Ntontis, E., Tekin Guven, S., & Amlôt, R. (2019). Facilitating collective psychosocial resil-
ience in the public in emergencies: Twelve recommendations based on the social identity approach. Frontiers in Public
Health,7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2019.00141
Drury, J., Carter, H., Ntontis, E., & Guven, S. T. (2021). Public behaviour in response to the COVID-19 pandemic: Under-
standing the role of group processes. BJPsych Open,7, e11. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2020.139
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the
social behavioral and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods,39, 175191.
Flade, F., Klar, Y., & Imhoff, R. (2019). Unite against: A common threat invokes spontaneous decategorization between social
categories. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,85, 103890. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103890
Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach (2nd
ed.). New York: The Guilford Press.
James, T. K., & Zagefka, H. (2017). The effects of group memberships of victims and perpetrators in humanly caused disasters
on charitable donations to victims. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,47,446458. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12452
Jetten, J., Reicher, S. D., Haslam, S. A., & Cruwys, T. (Eds.). (2020). Together apart. The psychology of Covid-19. London: Sage.
Lalot, F., Abrams, D., & Travaglino, G. A. (2021). Aversion amplification in the emerging COVID-19 pandemic: The impact of
political trust and subjective uncertainty on perceived threat. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology,31, 213
222. https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.2490
Li, W. Q., Li, L., Jiang, D., & Liu, S. (2020). Fate control and ingroup bias in donation for the fight with the coronavirus pan-
demic: The mediating role of risk perception of COVID-19. Personality and Individual Differences,171, 110456. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110456
Liekefett, L., & Becker, J. (2021). Compliance with governmental restrictions during the coronavirus pandemic: A matter of
personal self-protection or solidarity with people in risk groups? British Journal of Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.
1111/bjso.12439
McFarland, S., Brown, D., & Webb, M. (2013). Identification with all humanity as a moral concept and psychological con-
struct. Current Directions in Psychological Science,22, 194198. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412471346
McFarland, S., Webb, M., & Brown, D. (2012). All humanity is my ingroup: A measure and studies of identification with all
humanity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,103, 830853. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028724
Ntontis, E., Drury, J., Amlôt, R., Rubin, G. J., & Williams, R. (2020). Endurance or decline of emergent groups following a flood
disaster: Implications for community resilience. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction,45, 101493. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.ijdrr.2020.101493
Ntontis, E., Drury, J., Amlôt, R., Rubin, G. J., Williams, R., & Saavedra, P. (2021). Collective resilience in the disaster recovery
period: Emergent social identity and observed social support are associated with collective efficacy, well-being, and the
provision of social support. British Journal of Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12434
Politi, E., Van Assche, J., Caprara, G. V., & Phalet, K. (2021). No man is an Island: Psychological underpinnings of prosociality
in the midst of the COVID-19 outbreak. Personality and Individual Differences,171, 110534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
paid.2020.110534
Reicher, S., & Hopkins, N. (2001). Self and nation. London: Sage.
ZAGEFKA 13
Reykowski, J. (2002). The justice motive and altruistic helping: Rescuers of Jews in Nazi-occupied Europe. In M. Ross & D. T.
Miller (Eds.), The justice motive in everyday life (pp. 251270). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation: Experimental studies of the sources of gregariousness. Stanford, CA: Stanford
University Press.
Schmid, K., & Muldoon, O. T. (2015). Perceived threat, social identification, and psychological well-being: The effects of
political conflict exposure. Political Psychology,36,7592. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12073
Schönbrodt, F. D., & Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample size do correlations stabilize? Journal of Research in Personality,47,
609612.
Sierksma, J., Thijs, J., & Verkuyten, M. (2015). In-group bias in children's intention to help can be overpowered by inducing
empathy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,33,4556. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12065
Solnit, R. (2010). Built in hell: The extraordinary communities that arise in disaster. London, England: Penguin.
Stuermer, S., & Snyder, M. (Eds.). (2010). The psychology of prosocial behaviour. Chichester, England: Blackwell.
Stürmer, S., Snyder, M., & Omoto, A. M. (2005). Prosocial emotions and helping: The moderating role of group membership.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,88, 532546.
Swann, W. B., Jr., G
omez,
A., Huici, C., Morales, J. F., & Hixon, J. G. (2010). Identity fusion and self-sacrifice: Arousal as a cat-
alyst of pro-group fighting, dying, and helping behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,99, 824841.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020014
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psy-
chology of intergroup relations (pp. 724). Chicago: Nelson Hall.
The Economist. (2020, April 11). For queen and country (p. 11). London.
The Evening Express (2021). Retrieved from https://www.eveningexpress.co.uk/news/uk/evoke-dunkirk-spirit-to-speed-
up-covid-19-vaccination-effort-ministers-told/
The Guardian (2020). Retrieved from https://www.theguardian.com/lifeandstyle/2020/aug/14/jeremy-farrar-viruses-
know-no-borders-until-every-country-is-protected-we-are-all-at-risk
The Guardian (2021). Retrieved from https://www.theguardian.com/world/2021/jan/10/salim-abdool-karim-none-of-us-
are-safe-from-covid-if-one-of-us-is-not-we-have-mutual-interdependence
The Telegraph (2020). Retrieved from https://www.telegraph.co.uk/global-health/science-and-disease/worlds-poorest-risk-
trampled-stampede-vaccines-warns/
The Telegraph (2021). Retrieved from https://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/2021/01/04/war-against-covid-governments-
dunkirk-spirit/
Turner, J. C. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorisation theory. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
van Leeuwen, E., & Zagefka, H. (Eds.). (2017). Intergroup helping. Cham, Switzerland: Springer.
Vezzali, L., Cadamuro, A., Versari, A., Giovannini, D., & Trifiletti, E. (2015). Feeling like a group after a natural disaster: Com-
mon ingroup identity and relations with outgroup victims among majority and minority young children. British Journal of
Social Psychology,54, 519538. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12091
Wohl, M. J. A., Branscombe, N. R., & Reysen, S. (2010). Perceiving your group's future to be in jeopardy: Extinction threat
induces collective angst and the desire to strengthen the ingroup. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,36, 898
910. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167210372505
Yam, K. C., Jackson, J. C., Barnes, C. M., Lau, J., Qin, X., & Lee, H. Y. (2020). The rise of COVID-19 cases is associated with
support for world leaders. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,117, 2542925433. https://doi.org/10.
1073/pnas.2009252117
Zagefka, H. (2021). Intergroup helping during the coronavirus crisis: Effects of group identification, ingroup blame and 3rd
party outgroup blame. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology,31,8393. https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.2487
Zagefka, H., Noor, M., Randsley de Moura, G., Hopthrow, T., & Brown, R. (2011). Donating to disaster victims: Responses to
natural and humanly caused events. European Journal of Social Psychology,41,353363. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.781
SUPPORTING INFORMATION
Additional supporting information may be found online in the Supporting Information section at the end of this
article.
How to cite this article: Zagefka, H. (2021). Prosociality during COVID-19: Globally focussed solidarity brings
greater benefits than nationally focussed solidarity. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology,114.
https://doi.org/10.1002/casp.2553
14 ZAGEFKA
... Internalized social norms such as reciprocity also may be crucial when understanding helping behavior (Burger et al., 2009;Gouldner, 1960). Regarding prosocial behavior specifically during the COVID-19 crisis, Zagefka (2022) showed that focusing on global solidarity had positive effects. A recent survey clustering multiple countries found that high levels of perceived social support were most strongly associated with prosocial behavior, followed by high levels of positive affect, perceived stress, and psychological flexibility (Haller et al., 2022). ...
... The social identity approach helps to understand social fragmentation and unity during the pandemic (Abrams et al., 2021;Drury et al., 2021;Templeton et al., 2020). Since the emergence of COVID-19, studies have explored crowd behavior and social identity processes in the context of the pandemic (e.g., Bavel et al., 2020;Drury et al., 2020Drury et al., , 2021Zagefka, 2022). Fatmawati and Dewantara (2022) went further and analyzed how specific traditional rituals can bolster social resilience within indigenous communities and help deal with COVID-19. ...
... The role of social identification and a sense of "being in the same boat" as a motivation to support the ingroup is well established (Drury & Winter, 2003), and our results confirm it in a conflict-affected area. Cooperation, helping behaviors, and coordination are positive consequences of shared identity (Drury, 2012;McLeish & Oxoby, 2011;Zagefka, 2022). Individuals who identify with a group display better coordination and collaboration toward fellow members. ...
... A large number of studies conducted during the COVID-19 outbreak have outlined the importance of individual predispositions and worldviews in regulating people's responses to pandemic challenges (e.g., Bonetto et al., 2021;Kruglanski et al., 2021;Tu et al., 2021;Wolf et al., 2020). Increasing attention has been dedicated to prosociality and its predictors, conceived as a collective remedy to protect individual wellbeing, safeguard communities, and recover societies from the negative impacts of the pandemic (Jordan et al., 2021;Ramkissoon, 2022;Zagefka, 2022). The present research not only corroborates previously established links between basic individual values and prosociality (e.g., Politi, et al., 2021;Schwartz et al., 2010), but also offers an innovative understanding of the underlying threat-regulatory processes involved (e.g., Reiss et al., 2020). ...
... Furthermore, this indirect relationship was particularly accentuated for bridging types of prosocial helping intentions, wherein threats for vulnerable groups were more strongly related to helping distant others (e.g., donating to an organisation) than close others (e.g., helping a neighbours). In line with previous findings, empathic concerns for others in need seem particularly effective in bridging group cleavages and promoting intergroup helping (e.g., Graziano et al., 2007;Van Leeuwen & Zagefka, 2017; see also Zagefka, 2022). the two countries, especially because September 2020 was one of the worst months for India in terms of contagion and deaths (TNN, 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Unlabelled: Prosociality is often considered as quintessential in coping with the threats of health emergencies. As previous research has suggested, prosocial behaviors are shaped by both dispositional factors and situational cues about the helping situation. In the present research, we investigated whether "bonding" types of prosociality, helping directed towards close others within one's social network, and "bridging" types of prosociality, helping directed towards vulnerable people across group boundaries, are predicted by basic individual values and threat appraisals concerning COVID-19. During the pandemic, we conducted a cross-sectional study in the US and India (Ntotal = 954), using the Schwartz value inventory and a multifaceted measure of threat assessment to predict prosocial helping intentions. After controlling for other value and threat facets, self-transcendence values and threat for vulnerable groups uniquely predicted both bonding and bridging types of prosociality. Furthermore, threat for vulnerable groups partially mediated the effect of self-transcendence on prosocial helping intentions: People who endorsed self-transcendent values were particularly concerned by the effect of the pandemic on vulnerable groups, and thus willing to engage in prosocial behaviours to help those in need. Our findings support the idea that prosociality is stimulated by empathic concerns towards others in need and underline the importance for future research to consider the broad spectrum of threats appraised by people during health emergencies. Supplementary information: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s12144-023-04829-1.
... Psychology plays a considerable role in human health (Taylor et al., 1997;Fredrickson, 2001), and the present research shows peoples' identification with all humanity is key to predicting their willingness to contribute to the health of others. That is, while much of social and personality psychology examines the nature and course of prosocial interactions within families, cultures, and societies (Keltner et al., 2014), the work on identification with all humanity is consistently suggesting that there is a portion of the population, across multiple cultures, that strives for showing concern not only for their kin, community, or country, but for all humans Wang et al., 2021;Feng et al., 2022;Zagefka, 2022). ...
... Prosociality refers to an attitude or a set of voluntary actions that an individual may adopt to help, care for, or comfort others [27]. As suggested by the Social Identity Theory [28], the sense of belonging, identity and prosociality would often increase within a group in supporting one another if group members perceive themselves as facing crises [29]. Prosociality requires people to think and act collectively with kindness, cooperation and sensitivity to others' welfare (e.g., protect others from COVID-19). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Identifying common factors that affect public adherence to COVID-19 containment measures can directly inform the development of official public health communication strategies. The present international longitudinal study aimed to examine whether prosociality, together with other theoretically derived motivating factors (self-efficacy, perceived susceptibility and severity of COVID-19, perceived social support) predict the change in adherence to COVID-19 containment strategies. Method In wave 1 of data collection, adults from eight geographical regions completed online surveys beginning in April 2020, and wave 2 began in June and ended in September 2020. Hypothesized predictors included prosociality, self-efficacy in following COVID-19 containment measures, perceived susceptibility to COVID-19, perceived severity of COVID-19 and perceived social support. Baseline covariates included age, sex, history of COVID-19 infection and geographical regions. Participants who reported adhering to specific containment measures, including physical distancing, avoidance of non-essential travel and hand hygiene, were classified as adherence. The dependent variable was the category of adherence, which was constructed based on changes in adherence across the survey period and included four categories: non-adherence, less adherence, greater adherence and sustained adherence (which was designated as the reference category). Results In total, 2189 adult participants (82% female, 57.2% aged 31–59 years) from East Asia (217 [9.7%]), West Asia (246 [11.2%]), North and South America (131 [6.0%]), Northern Europe (600 [27.4%]), Western Europe (322 [14.7%]), Southern Europe (433 [19.8%]), Eastern Europe (148 [6.8%]) and other regions (96 [4.4%]) were analyzed. Adjusted multinomial logistic regression analyses showed that prosociality, self-efficacy, perceived susceptibility and severity of COVID-19 were significant factors affecting adherence. Participants with greater self-efficacy at wave 1 were less likely to become non-adherence at wave 2 by 26% (adjusted odds ratio [aOR], 0.74; 95% CI, 0.71 to 0.77; P < .001), while those with greater prosociality at wave 1 were less likely to become less adherence at wave 2 by 23% (aOR, 0.77; 95% CI, 0.75 to 0.79; P = .04). Conclusions This study provides evidence that in addition to emphasizing the potential severity of COVID-19 and the potential susceptibility to contact with the virus, fostering self-efficacy in following containment strategies and prosociality appears to be a viable public health education or communication strategy to combat COVID-19.
... Studies conducted in the early phases of the pandemic, as well as during and after lockdown measures, particularly referred to solidarity as a crucial concept to overcome the pandemic (e.g., Marzana et al., 2022;McCormick, 2020;Moss & Sandbakken, 2021;Peng & Berry, 2021;van Breen et al., 2021). Relatedly, there were repeated calls for global solidarity to overcome regional resource bottlenecks and for pandemic containment (Jecker & Atuire, 2021;Zagefka, 2022). Many scholars linked global solidarity with equity and justice principles, stating that global solidarity is needed to achieve equity in the context of COVID-19 (Ebrahim et al., 2020;Killeen & Kiware, 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Solidarity and personal responsibility have been repeatedly called upon during the COVID-19 pandemic. This study quantifies and contextualizes the use of these terms in newspaper coverage in Germany and German-speaking Switzerland based on n = 640 articles from six functionally equivalent newspapers. The term solidarity in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic was mentioned in 541/640 articles (84.5%) and was primarily used during phases with high death rates and comparatively stringent policies in place, supporting the idea that solidarity was used to explain restrictive measures to the population and motivate people to comply with these measures. German newspapers published more articles on solidarity than Swiss-German newspapers, consistent with more stringent COVID-19 policies in Germany. Personal responsibility was mentioned in 133/640 articles (20.8%), meaning that the term was less frequently discussed than solidarity. Articles covering personal responsibility included more negative evaluations during phases of high infection rates as compared to phases of low infection rates. Findings indicate that the two terms were, at least to some extent, used in newspaper reporting to contextualize and justify COVID-19 policy during phases of high infection rates. Moreover, the term solidarity was used in a high variety of different contexts and the inherent limits of solidarity were rarely mentioned. Policymakers and journalists need to take this into account for future crises to not jeopardize the positive effects of solidarity.
... For example, those more strongly identified with their community were found to be more willing to act in ways that protect other community members (e.g., by receiving a vaccine; Wakefield & Khauser, 2021; for other examples for how identity processes affect pandemic responses, see Jetten et al., 2020;Stevenson et al., 2021;Vignoles et al., 2021). Other work in the context of Covid has shown that although national identification was found to be linked to willingness to help national ingroup members cope with the pandemic, national identification was also found to reduce willingness to act in ways that protect national outgroup members (e.g., Zagefka, 2021aZagefka, , 2021bZagefka, , 2022. What this body of research demonstrates is that group memberships matter in helping decisions and in how helping is perceived (e.g., Ellithorpe et al., 2022), also in the context of global pandemics and decisions to share health resources. ...
Article
Full-text available
Decisions to give and receive help are often influenced by group memberships, social identities, and intergroup relations. Two studies were conducted to test how perceived political relations between countries are related to willingness to accept offers of intergroup help. Respondents in two low‐income countries, the Philippines (N = 289) and Pakistan (N = 275), indicated their willingness to accept (or not) Covid‐19 vaccine donations from two higher‐status countries (China and the United States) during the Covid pandemic. Results showed that the perceived motivation of the outgroup nation for providing help was associated with rejection or acceptance of help, mediated by emotional reactions to the help. A perception that outgroup nations donate vaccines to demonstrate and assert their superiority and power, that they donate vaccines to keep the outgroup dependent on the ingroup, and a perception that they donate vaccines out of self‐interest, were all associated with rejection of vaccine donations. A perception that donations by the outgroup are motivated by genuine concern for the ingroup was associated with acceptance of help. Findings confirm that political intergroup relations are related to attitudes about whether the ingroup nation should accept intergroup help or not. Theoretical and applied implications are discussed.
Article
Social psychological scholarship highlights the value of leadership for mounting effective responses to the COVID‐19 pandemic. Yet considerably less is known about whether leaders and their respective communities talk about the social crisis in the same way. Through a narrative congruence framework, we interrogate how public storylines of leaders and their communities align or misalign. Utilizing a mixed methods approach, we analyse Facebook posts and comments by Metro Manila mayors and their online followers during the early months of the COVID‐19 pandemic. We uncover a narrative incongruence between (a) leaders who perform responsibility and (b) communities that demand responsiveness . Mayors prioritize equity to give the poor more relief aid, assure efficient cash disbursement, attribute higher infection rates to sufficient testing and blame noncompliant citizens for worsening outbreaks. On the other hand, communities seek equality in relief distribution, decry ambiguous cash disbursement, criticize testing failures and fault weak quarantine protocols for crisis escalation. We conclude with pathways for meaningful engagement between leaders and communities towards effective crisis response especially in the Global South.
Article
Collective memories support collective identities and provide lessons for group members to manage present challenges. Three surveys (total N = 1,137) tested whether U.S. nationals who strongly identify with all humanity (IWAH) were more interested in learning about pandemics in history as events that affected the whole world (Study 1) and more knowledgeable about past pandemics (Studies 2a and 2b). Stronger IWAH predicted greater interest in learning about past pandemics, but not greater knowledge of these events. Further, while IWAH was a consistent predictor of support for global cooperation for pandemic preparedness and, to a lesser extent, of compliance with COVID-19 health-protective behaviors, knowledge of past pandemics contributed modestly to these outcomes. These findings suggest that while past pandemics have the potential to become global memories, they are not yet actual global memories that inform a sense of global human identity and offer courses of action for the present.
Article
Full-text available
Prosociality is of greater interest in recent decades due to events that have questioned the socially responsible behavior of decision-makers in various organizations. The objective of this research is to describe statistically significant differences in prosocial attitudes through sociodemographic characteristics among students at a confessional university in Chile. A quantitative, descriptive, and non-experimental cross-sectional study was carried out. A non-probabilistic convenience sample of students of the last three years of studies at a confessional university located in south-central Chile was considered. The short scale of prosocial behavior was used, and a descriptive analysis, exploratory factor analysis, and inferential analysis were performed. Prosocial attitudes are composed of three new dimensions, which are grouped according to the variables scale of prosocial behavior: comfort and commitment to others, empathy and detachment, and solidarity and help. Gender and religion stand out as statistically significant variables. Future research should consider a more diverse study group, including both denominational and non-denominational universities. Likewise, it would be convenient to study the stability of prosocial attitudes throughout the life cycles and their relationship with other transcendent variables such as personality and self-efficacy.
Article
Factors related to compliance with and perceptions of public health measures concerning COVID-19 were examined in 2 studies. Study 1 was administered in spring 2020 via a university list serve and social media (n = 512, Mage = 34.51). Study 2 was administered in fall 2020 via Amazon Mechanical Turk, a university list serve, and social media (n = 523, Mage = 31.55). In both studies, participants completed measures of identification with all humanity (IWAH), the 10-item personality inventory (TIPI), social dominance orientation (SDO), right-wing authoritarianism (RWA), political orientation, and religion. In Study 1, participants also completed measures regarding how concerned they were about the effects of COVID-19 on society, how much they supported canceling events to promote social distancing, and how much they altered their behavior to avoid gatherings. In Study 2, participants were asked about the extent to which they engaged in recommended behaviors intended to reduce the spread of COVID-19 and their compliance with mask guidelines. Over and beyond the influence of any other factors, IWAH was associated with greater concern about the health of society with regard to COVID-19 and more altering of behavior to avoid gatherings in Study 1 and with more mask compliance in Study 2. These data suggest that a fruitful tactic for increasing compliance with public health measures for COVID-19 might be framing the issue as a way to contribute to welfare of broader society.
Preprint
Full-text available
Communities are vital sources of support during crisis, providing collective contexts for shared identity and solidarity that predict supportive, prosocial responses. The COVID-19 pandemic has presented a global health crisis capable of exerting a heavy toll on the mental health of community members while inducing unwelcome levels of social disconnection. Simultaneously, lockdown restrictions have forced vulnerable community members to depend upon the support of fellow residents. Fortunately, voluntary helping can be beneficial to the well-being of the helper as well as the recipient, offering beneficial collective solutions. Using insights from social identity approaches to volunteering and disaster responses, this study explored whether the opportunity to engage in helping fellow community members may be both unifying and beneficial for those engaging in coordinated community helping. Survey data collected in the UK during June 2020 showed that coordinated community helping predicted the psychological bonding of community members by building a sense of community identification and unity during the pandemic, which predicted increased well-being and reduced depression and anxiety. Implications for the promotion and support of voluntary helping initiatives in the context of longer-term responses to the COVID-19 pandemic are provided. Please refer to the Supplementary Material section to find this article’s Community and Social Impact Statement.
Article
Full-text available
During the coronavirus pandemic, governments across the globe ordered physical-distancing and hygiene restrictions to slow down the spread of COVID-19. The present work was conducted during the peak of restrictions in Germany (April/May 2020). In a convenient (N = 218) and representative sample (N = 715), we examined people's motivations to comply with these restrictions during the lockdown: Were they motivated by personal self-protection, or rather by solidarity with people in risk groups? Specifically, we investigated predictors of personal self-protection (compliance to protect the self against infection) and group-protection behaviours (compliance for reasons of solidarity in protecting people in risk groups). Results indicate that self- and group-protection result from different psychological processes: Whereas personal self-protection seems to be a form of coping with personal anxieties (epistemic and existential needs, personal threat), group protection is an intergroup phenomenon that is enabled by identification with a collective goal (common identity), the perception that society is capable of dealing with the virus (group efficacy), and concern for people in risk groups. We discuss potential implications for behavioural change in pandemics.
Article
Full-text available
Background In the absence of a vaccine, behaviour by the public is key to the response to the COVID-19 pandemic. Yet, as with other types of crises and emergencies, there have been doubts about the extent to which the public are able to engage effectively with the required behaviour. These doubts are based on outdated models of group psychology. Aims and argument We analyse the role of group processes in the COVID-19 pandemic in three domains: recognition of threat, adherence by the public to the required public health behaviours (and the factors that increase such adherence) and actions of the many community mutual aid groups that arose during lockdown. In each case, we draw upon the accumulated research on behaviour in emergencies and disasters, as well as the latest findings in relation to the COVID-19 pandemic, to show that explanations in terms of social identity processes make better sense of the patterns of evidence than alternative explanations. Conclusions If behaviour in the pandemic is a function of mutable group processes rather than fixed tendencies, then behavioural change is possible. There was evidence of significant change in behaviour from the public, particularly in the early days of the pandemic. Understanding the role of group processes means we can help design more effective interventions to support collective resilience in the public in the face of the pandemic and other threats. We draw out from the evidence a set of recommendations on facilitating the public response to COVID-19 by harnessing group processes.
Article
Full-text available
Social support and an emerging sense of community are common in flooding, but post-flood group dynamics have not been fully addressed. In the context of a flooded community, we explore how social identification with one’s community emerges and affects wellbeing, collective efficacy, and social support. Results from a quantitative survey show that social identification was positively associated with common fate, collective efficacy, and wellbeing through residents’ expectations of support and shared goals. Importantly, social identification and disaster exposure interacted: For flooded residents, observing support was associated with providing support regardless of levels of social identification. For unaffected residents there was no association between observed and provided support, regardless of levels of social identification. However, for indirectly affected residents observing support was associated to providing support but only when they highly identified with the community. We argue that structural factors should also be considered when exploring the effects of group membership.
Article
Full-text available
Health psychology shows that responses to risk and threat depend on perceptions as much as objective factors. The present study focuses on the precursors of perceived threat of COVID-19. We draw on political and social psychology and use the aversion amplification hypothesis to propose that subjective uncertainty and political trust should interactively impact perceived threat. We conducted a cross-sectional survey amongst the general population of Scotland (N = 188) in the early period of the COVID-19 pandemic in the UK. We hypothesised that high political trust should ameliorate the threat-elevating impact of uncertainty, thereby reducing perceived threat from high to moderate level. This hypothesis was supported, even after accounting for demographic differences. The discussion addresses the implications of the interactive role of trust and uncertainty for strategies to manage public behaviour as the pandemic progresses.
Article
Full-text available
Significance Amid the present COVID-19 pandemic, we find that many citizens around the world “rally ‘round the flag” and increase their support for their respective political leaders. We observe these findings among countries that are culturally and geographically diverse, and even among leaders who are strongly disliked by citizens prior to the pandemic. Our findings could have important voting implications during or immediately after the pandemic. As an example, the Korean ruling party won the most seats in the house by any party since 1960 in an election held during the pandemic in April 2020. COVID-19 might thus serve as a catalyst to help some incumbent governments.
Article
Two studies tested predictors of helping across national boundaries. British participants reported blame attributions for the coronavirus crisis, either to the British government (ingroup blame), or to the Chinese government (third party outgroup blame), and it was tested whether this was associated with intentions to donate money to help outgroup members suffering from effects of the coronavirus crisis in the world's poorer countries. It was hypothesized that strength of identification with the national ingroup would be negatively associated with blame attributions to the ingroup, and that it would be positively associated with blame attributions to a third party outgroup. Blame attributions were predicted in turn to be related to outgroup helping, with ingroup blame being positively associated with helping intentions, and third party outgroup blame being negatively associated with helping intentions. Support for these predictions were found in one exploratory (N = 100) and one confirmatory (N = 250) study.
Chapter
This book contains essays in honour of Melvin J. Lerner, a pioneer in the psychological study of justice. The contributors to this volume are internationally renowned scholars from psychology, business, and law. They examine the role of justice motivation in a wide variety of contexts, including workplace violence, affirmative action programs, helping or harming innocent victims and how people react to their own fate. Contributors explore fundamental issues such as whether people's interest in justice is motivated by self-interest or a genuine concern for the welfare of others, when and why people feel a need to punish transgressors, how a concern for justice emerges during the development of societies and individuals, and the relation of justice motivation to moral motivation. How an understanding of justice motivation can contribute to the amelioration of major social problems is also examined.
Article
Ingroup bias could be a significant hindrance in a context where intergroup collaboration is crucial, which makes it essential to investigate ingroup bias during pandemics. This research investigated the influence of individuals' belief in fate control on ingroup bias in helping with COVID-19, and the mediating role of risk perception of COVID-19. To test our hypothesis, we analyzed the data from a community sample (n = 318) collected at the initial stage of the COVID-19 outbreak in China. We found that fate control was positively associated with ingroup bias in donation to the patients with COVID-19 and the frontline healthcare professionals. Moreover, the mediating role of risk perception of COVID-19 was significant. A higher level of fate control was associated with higher risk perception of COVID-19, which was, in turn, related to stronger ingroup bias in donation across individuals. These findings highlight the substantial role of general worldview in shaping individuals' responses to pandemics.
Article
In response to the COVID-19 outbreak, sustainable forms of collective resilience help societies coping cohesively with unprecedented challenges. In our empirical contribution, we framed collective resilience and cohesion in terms of prosociality. A study carried out in the midst of the COVID-19 outbreak in the UK (N = 399) articulated basic individual values, ideological orientations (i.e., authoritarianism and social dominance orientation), and core political values in a comprehensive framework to predict bonding and bridging forms of prosocial intentions, and prosocial behaviors directed towards vulnerable groups. According to our findings, people whose worldview incorporates collective and collaborative principles cared more about others' welfare. Jointly, self-transcendence, equality, and accepting immigrants predicted more prosociality, whereas social dominance orientation predicted less prosociality. Over and beyond all other predictors, self-transcendence uniquely predicted prosocial intentions and behaviors alike. To conclude, we suggest interventions to promote and sustain prosociality among people motivated by a larger array of life goals and worldviews.