PreprintPDF Available

Stunting, age at school entry and academic performance in developing countries: A systematic review and meta-analysis

Authors:
  • University Joseph KI-ZERBO
Preprints and early-stage research may not have been peer reviewed yet.

Abstract and Figures

IntroductionAlthough many studies have examined the associations between growth problems in infancy and age at school entry, grade repetition, school dropout and schooling level in developing country, no synthesis of the evidence has been conducted. We aim to review evidence of the effects of stunting, or height-for-age, on schooling level and schooling trajectories, defined as the combination of school entry age, grade repetition, and school dropouts.Methods We conducted a systematic review of studies (last update March 20, 2021) estimating that estimate the association between stunting, or height-for-age, and at least one component of the school trajectory, or schooling level, using five databases (PubMed, Embase, Education Resources Information Center (ERIC), Web of Science and PsycINFO). Study selection and data extraction were performed by two independent reviewers. Pooled effects were calculated using the generic inverse variance weighting random effect model. The studies’ risk of bias was assessed using the ROBINS-I tool for non-randomized studies.ResultsWe screened 3944 records by titles and abstracts and retained 16 for inclusion in the qualitative and meta-analysis. Meta-analysis showed that an increase in height-for-age leads to an increase in early enrollment [OR: 1.34 (95% CI: 1.07; 1.67)], a reduction in late enrollment [OR: 0.63 (95% CI: 0.51; 0.78)], an increase in schooling level [MD: 0.24 (95% CI: 0.14; 0.34)], and a reduction of school overage [OR: 0.79 (95% CI: 0.70; 0.90)]. The odds of grade repetition increased by 59% (OR = 1.59; 95% CI: 1.18; 2.14) for stunted children compared to those with no stunting.Conclusions This review suggests that stunting in childhood might lead to a delay in school enrollment, grade repetition, school dropout, and low schooling levels in developing countries. Future research should evaluate the effect of stunting on academic trajectories in the same population and explore the potential modification effect of socioeconomic status. The current findings suggest that policy makers need to work more to prevent stunting and to include health issues in educational policies. Systematic review registration : PROSPERO CRD42020198346
Content may be subject to copyright.
Page 1/18
Stunting, age at school entry and academic performance in developing
countries: A systematic review and meta-analysis
Rabi Joël Gansaonré ( rabi-joel.gansaonre.1@ulaval.ca )
Universite Laval https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6199-1771
Lynne Moore
Universite Laval Faculte de medecine
Louis-Philippe Bleau
Université Laval: Universite Laval
Jean-François Kobiané
Université Joseph Ki-Zerbo: Universite Joseph Ki-Zerbo
Slim Haddad
Universite Laval Faculte de medecine
Research
Keywords: stunting, height-for-age, children, school entry, grade repetition, school dropout, schooling level
DOI: https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-606866/v1
License: This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.Read Full License
Page 2/18
Abstract
Introduction
Although many studies have examined the associations between growth problems in infancy and age at school entry, grade repetition, school
dropout and schooling level in developing country, no synthesis of the evidence has been conducted. We aim to review evidence of the effects
of stunting, or height-for-age, on schooling level and schooling trajectories, dened as the combination of school entry age, grade repetition,
and school dropouts.
Methods
We conducted a systematic review of studies (last update March 20, 2021) estimating that estimate the association between stunting, or
height-for-age, and at least one component of the school trajectory, or schooling level, using ve databases (PubMed, Embase, Education
Resources Information Center (ERIC), Web of Science and PsycINFO). Study selection and data extraction were performed by two independent
reviewers. Pooled effects were calculated using the generic inverse variance weighting random effect model. The studies’ risk of bias was
assessed using the ROBINS-I tool for non-randomized studies.
Results
We screened 3944 records by titles and abstracts and retained 16 for inclusion in the qualitative and meta-analysis. Meta-analysis showed that
an increase in height-for-age leads to an increase in early enrollment [OR: 1.34 (95% CI: 1.07; 1.67)], a reduction in late enrollment [OR: 0.63
(95% CI: 0.51; 0.78)], an increase in schooling level [MD: 0.24 (95% CI: 0.14; 0.34)], and a reduction of school overage [OR: 0.79 (95% CI: 0.70;
0.90)]. The odds of grade repetition increased by 59% (OR = 1.59; 95% CI: 1.18; 2.14) for stunted children compared to those with no stunting.
Conclusions
This review suggests that stunting in childhood might lead to a delay in school enrollment, grade repetition, school dropout, and low schooling
levels in developing countries. Future research should evaluate the effect of stunting on academic trajectories in the same population and
explore the potential modication effect of socioeconomic status. The current ndings suggest that policy makers need to work more to
prevent stunting and to include health issues in educational policies.
Systematic review registration
: PROSPERO CRD42020198346
Introduction
In many countries, only a minority of children grow up healthy[1]. The 2018 World Nutrition Report indicates that stunting affects 150.8million
children under ve years of age, which represents 22.2% of the world’s children[2, 3]. The vast majority of stunted children come from
developing countries (148.0 of 150.8million)[3]. These countries also have more of out-of-school children or people with low academic
achievement than the global average. The UNESCO Institute for Statistics reports that, in 2018, 17.7% of children of primary school age were
out of school in the least developed countries, compared to only 8.2% globally[4]. In the same year, only 54.0% reached the last grade of
primary education in developing countries compared to 81.7% globally[4].
In this context, many studies have been carried out in developing countries on the effects of early childhood development on future academic
achievement. These studies have shown that stunting in the rst ve years of life leads to cognitive impairment in children[5–8], poor school
performance, fewer years of schooling, and low productivity in adulthood[7, 8]. Children who have been stunted in childhood are therefore more
likely to delay school enrollment, perform poorly in school, repeat a grade, and drop out of school than those who have not been stunted[9, 10].
However, some studies observed no signicant association between childhood stunting and academic performance[11, 12], grade repetition[10,
13], and school dropout[14].
Systematic reviews have been undertaken in this eld in developing countries. However, most of them[15–20] are qualitative reviews. To our
knowledge, only one review[21] carried out a meta-analysis on the effect of linear growth or stunting on child development, but it does not
include outcomes on age at school entry, grade repetition, and school dropouts. We aim to review evidence of the effect of stunting or height-
for-age on schooling level and schooling trajectories, dened as the combination of school entry age, grade repetition, and school dropouts.
Methodology
The protocol of the review was designed according to the “Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis Protocols
(PRISMA-P) 2015 statement”[22] and registered on the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO) on September
Page 3/18
20, 2020 (#CRD42020198346). This review was conducted according to the “Cochrane collaborative guidelines for systematic reviews”[23]
and is reported according to “The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate healthcare
interventions”[24] (see PRISMA check list in Additional le 1).
Eligibility Criteria
The PICOS (Population, Intervention/exposure, Comparator, Outcome and Study design) approach was used to dene inclusion criteria. Our
population of interest was primary school aged children in developing countries. Generally, the legal age of admission to primary school is
between ve and seven years old[25]. Primary school usually lasts six years, although it can range from four to seven years[25], and it usually
ends between ages 10 and 12 years[25]. We considered studies that include children aged between 5 and 12 years from developing countries.
We also included studies on poor child development that resulted in stunting when children were less than ve years old. Studies that use
standardized height-for-age ratio (height-for-age z-scores) as a measure of child growth during infancy were included. We also considered
studies that used height as a marker of stunting. We considered four outcomes: age at school entry, grade repetition, school dropout, and
schooling level. Schooling level was dened as the highest level of education attained by an individual at the time of study. Eligible studies
were observational studies (prospective, retrospective, case-control, case series, and cross-sectional studies). Studies on diverse populations
were included if data on the subgroup of primary school age children related to stunting and outcomes could be extracted or if primary school
aged children constituted more than 80% of the study population.
Research Strategy
We conducted a comprehensive systematic literature search via Pubmed, Embase, ERIC (via Ovid), Web of Science and PsycINFO (via Ovid)
(last updated March 20, 2021). We developed a rigorous search strategy using relevant keywords related to stunting, schooling trajectory, and
geographic area. The search strategy was designed using both free and controlled vocabularies in PubMed, and then translated into other
databases. No restriction was applied on language or date of publication. We consulted an information specialist to validate our search
strategy, and the sensitivity of the strategy was evaluated by verifying the inclusion of ve relevant studies.
Studies management and selection
Data management
The bibliographic reference management software package EndNote was used for citation management. We imported references from
databases into EndNote and then removed duplicates using both automatic and manual screening based on study titles. Then, citations were
transferred to Covidence for selection.
Study Selection process
Two reviewers (JG and LPB) evaluated all studies independently by screening titles, abstracts, and full texts to identify studies that met the
inclusion criteria. We rst evaluated inter-reviewer agreement (Kappa) on eligibility using the rst 300 citations to ensure that reviewers had a
good understanding of inclusion criteria. Inter-reviewer agreements were assessed after each step of selection. Disagreements between JG and
LPB were resolved by consensus or by consulting a third reviewer. At the full text stage, reasons for exclusions were recorded.
Data extraction
An Excel data extraction form and a detailed instruction manual was developed and piloted with a sample of three studies. The same two
reviewers (JG and LPB) extracted data independently from the selected studies. Data extracted include study characteristics (rst author, year
of publication, country), population characteristics (sample size, proportion of girls, age), study design, follow-up duration, exposure (type of
exposure, age at exposition measurement), outcomes (age at school entry, level of education attained, repetition, dropouts), effect measures,
and condence intervals (adjusted measure of effect, condence interval, p-value, standard errors, confounding variables). Study authors were
contacted with up to three email attempts in case of missing information or unclear data. All extracted data from the two reviewers were cross-
checked and disagreements were discussed to reach a consensus or by involving a third reviewer.
Risk of bias
The risk of bias of the included studies was assessed using the ROBINS-I tool for non-randomized studies [26]. The tool covers confounding
bias, selection bias, classication bias, bias due to missing data, and bias due to measurement of the outcome. Studies were classied as low,
moderate, or high risk. To assess confounding bias, we considered a model well adjusted if it included demographic characteristics (e.g. child
sex, child age), and household characteristics (e.g. socioeconomic status, household size, place of residence), and characteristics of the
mother and father (e.g. education, size, ethnic group). The same two reviewers (JG and LPB) extracted data on risk of bias evaluation
independently. Disagreements were resolved by discussion or by involvement of a third reviewer.
Page 4/18
Statistical analysis and data synthesis
Eligible studies were described in detail according to PICOS parameters. We conducted a meta-analysis to estimate the pooled effect of
stunting on the different outcomes and their 95% condence intervals. Then, all studies with sucient information to estimate the pooled
effect were included in the meta-analysis. Pooled effects were calculated according to the domain of the outcome, study design, and effect
measured. Thus, for a given outcome domain, more than one pooled effect was estimated if the outcomes were measured in different ways or
if different types of effect measures were extracted.
Pooled effects were estimated using generic inverse variance weighting random effect models with Review Manager (RevMan) software [27–
29]. Heterogeneity was evaluated by the Higgins I² statistic which is the proportion of total variation in the pooled effect size attributable to
heterogeneity between studies [30]. Heterogeneity was considered very low, low, moderate, or high if I² were, respectively, 25 %, > 25 % and
50 %, > 50% and  75 %, or > 75 % [31, 32]. Sensitivity analyses were performed if heterogeneity was high (I²>50%).
Sensitivity and subgroup analysis
To understand the source of heterogeneity, a sensitivity analysis was performed by removing one study at a time from the pooled effect size
estimation. This allowed us to measure the effect of each selected study on the pooled effect heterogeneity. We were not able to conduct
sensitivity analysis on studies at low risk of bias or subgroup analysis based on age of child stunting assessment ( 2 years of age and > 2
years of age) due to the insucient number of studies.
Results
Study selection and characteristics
We identied 4981 studies, of which 3944 were screened by title and abstract after removing duplicates (Fig.1). Eighty-seven (87) studies were
assessed by full text and 16 were considered eligible for the review. From these studies, six were included in the meta-analysis. The inter-
reviewer agreements (Kappa statistics) were 97.5% and 83.5% respectively, for title and abstract screening and full-text selection. All studies
were in English.
Out of the 16 eligible studies, 7 were published before 2006 [14, 33–38], and 7 others between 2006 and 2015 [6, 7, 12, 39–42] (Table1 and
Table2). Only two studies were published after 2015 [10, 43]. Most of the studies (56.2 %) used a prospective observational design [6, 7, 10, 12,
14, 36, 37, 41, 42], while 43.8 % were cross-sectional studies [33–35, 38–40, 43]. The most commonly studied exposure was standardized
height-for-age (56.2 %) [6, 12, 34, 37, 38, 40, 41] followed by stunting (37.4%) [10, 14, 35, 36, 39]. Two studies [42, 43] used both stunting and
height-for-age z-score. Most of the studies analyzed age at school entry [7, 10, 36–42], and schooling level [6, 10, 12, 34, 35, 39, 43]. Relatively
few studies analyzed grade repetition [10, 12, 14, 37, 42] and dropout [14, 33, 38]. One study included four countries [6] and another involved
two countries [35]. One study presented results only by sex [36]. In Sunny, DeStavola (10), the exposition was measured at three time points
(between 0 and 4 months, between 11 and 16 months, and between 4 and 8 years). Sample sizes of included studies ranged from 325 to 2711.
Participants were between 6.2 and 18.0 years old at the time of study (Table2).
Page 5/18
Table 1
Characteristics of studies included in the review and meta-analysis
Characteristics N %
Characteristics of studies  
Year of publication
 < 2006 7 43.8
 2006–2015 7 43.8
 > 2015 2 12.4
Study design
 Cross-sectional 7 43.8
 Prospective 9 56.2
 Retrospective
Type of exposure*  
Height-for-age z-score 9 56.2
 Height 2 12.4
 Stunting 6 37.4
Type of outcome domain*  
Age at school entry 9 56.2
Schooling level 7 43.8
Grade repetition 5 31.3
School dropout 3 18.8
*Some studies were counted more than once because they use more than one type of exposition or outcome leading to a number of
studies greater than 16 and a total of percentages greater than 100%.
Page 6/18
Table 2
Description of included studies and authors’ conclusions
Author, year
Study design
Setting Sample
size Age
(mean) Sex
(% of
girls)
Follow-
up
duration
Exposure Outcome
domain Potential
confounders
considered
Authors’
conclusion
Acharya,
2019 [43]
Cross-
sectional
India 1194 NR 48.6 - Stunting
and
Height-for-
age
between 8
and 11
years
Schooling
level Gender, child’s age,
child had any
chronic illness or
congenital or
perinatal disorder,
mother’s
education, father’s
education, child
lived with both
biological parents
in household,
household income,
scheduled
caste/tribe, English
medium school,
worried about not
having sucient
food during the
past month
Acute
nutritional
status was
associated
with lower
math scores
and lower
educational
attainment,
suggesting
that current
adverse
conditions
are also
important
determinants
of cognitive
achievement
and
educational
attainment.
Alderman,
2006 [41]
Prospective
cohort
Zimbabwe 665 17.7
years 50.8  14.4
years Height-for-
age at 39.9
months
Age at
school
entry
Sex, current age,
maternal age,
maternal
education,
maternal height,
year of rst
measurement
(instrumental
variable) exposure
to civil a war,
drought shock
Improved
preschooler
nutritional
status, as
measured by
height given
age, is
associated
with
increased
height as a
young adult,
a greater
number of
grades of
schooling
completed,
and an earlier
age at which
the child
starts school
Alderman,
2009 [7]
Prospective
cohort
Tanzania  1147 15.7
years 49.0 10
years Percentage
of median
reference
height at
10 years or
less
Age at
school
entry
Residuals %
median, female,
age of child in
years, mothers
height, father’s
height, parent had
secondary
schooling, number
of teachers per
class, number of
blackboards per
class, urban,
maximum
education in
household, (log)
per capita
household
expenditure,
electricity in the
household,
(instrumental
variables) crop
loss in 2004, and
ood or drought in
2004
Children who
are
malnourished
have lower
schooling
and delay
their school
entry.
NR: Not reported
Page 7/18
Author, year
Study design
Setting Sample
size Age
(mean) Sex
(% of
girls)
Follow-
up
duration
Exposure Outcome
domain Potential
confounders
considered
Authors’
conclusion
Bogin, 1987
[33]
Cross-
sectional
Guatemala 514 Range:
7-
13.99
NR - Height
between 7
and 13.99
years
School
dropout Grade, child’s age,
weight, arm
circumference,
triceps skinfold,
subscapular
skinfold, muscle
area, and fat area
School
continuation
or drop-out
was not
inuenced by
the health or
nutritional
environment.
Crookston,
2013 [6]
Prospective
cohort
Ethiopia
India
Peru
Vietnam
1757
1815
1845
1829
8.1
8.0
7.9
8.1
46.6
46.3
49.8
48.8
7 years Height-for-
age at 1
year
Schooling
level Sex, age of the
mother, years of
schooling of the
mother, years of
schooling of the
father, asset index,
urban residence,
community
population,
community wealth,
and presence of a
community
hospital
Improving
growth in
children who
are stunted in
infancy and
maintaining
nutrition in
children who
otherwise
might falter
may have
signicant
benet for
schooling
and cognitive
achievement.
Daniels, 2004
[36]
Prospective
cohort
Philippines 2198 18.0
years 47,1  16
years Stunting at
2 years Age at
school
entry
Parity, maternal
and paternal
education,
maternal height,
index of assets,
index of
environmental
cleanliness,
presence of
electricity, and
deated household
income
Boys and
girls who
were taller at
2 y were
markedly less
likely to drop
out in grade
school or to
be behind in
school and
were
therefore
more likely to
graduate
from high
school on
time.
Gandhi, 2011
[12]
Prospective
cohort
Malawi 325 12.0 51.0  12
years Height-for-
age at 1
month,
residuals
height-for-
age at 6,
18, and 60
months
Schooling
level
Grade
repetition
Gender, gestational
duration, father’s
occupation,
father’s literacy,
mother’s literacy,
and wealth index
Height-for-
age at 1
month and
conditional
height gain
prior to 6
months did
not show
signicant
association
with the
outcome
measures.
NR: Not reported
Page 8/18
Author, year
Study design
Setting Sample
size Age
(mean) Sex
(% of
girls)
Follow-
up
duration
Exposure Outcome
domain Potential
confounders
considered
Authors’
conclusion
Glewwe,
1995 [38]
Cross-
sectional
Ghana 1757 11.0
years 48.2 - Height-for-
age
between 6
and 15
years
Age at
school
entry
School
dropout
Sex, age, zscore
residual, log
expenditure/capita,
expenditure
residual, mother's
schooling, father's
schooling, number
of siblings, ethnic
group, residence
area, travel time to
middle school,
travel time to
primary school,
average teacher
experience,
average teacher
schooling, average
teacher training,
fraction
classrooms with
blackboard,
books/classroom,
fraction
classrooms
leaking, fraction
classrooms
unusable, fraction
classrooms shed
construction, some
children lack
desks, private
school, school has
all six grades,
enrollment fee, and
school denies
admission
Delayed
primary
school
enrollment is
caused by
nutritional
deciencies
in early
childhood,
evidence
which
survives
numerous
robustness
checks.
Glewwe,
2001 [37]
Prospective
cohort
Philippines 1016 11
years 48.0 11
years Height-for-
age Age at
school
entry
Grade
repetition
Sex, age enrolled,
month of birth
effects,
(instruments)
month of birth, the
sibling age
difference, and the
sibling age and sex
differences
interacted with the
barangay level
average grade
repetition rate.
Better
nourished
children
perform
signicantly
better in
school, partly
because they
enter school
earlier and
thus have
more time to
learn but
mostly
because of
greater
learning
productivity
per year of
schooling.
NR: Not reported
Page 9/18
Author, year
Study design
Setting Sample
size Age
(mean) Sex
(% of
girls)
Follow-
up
duration
Exposure Outcome
domain Potential
confounders
considered
Authors’
conclusion
Grira, 2004
[34]
Cross-
sectional
Bangladesh 1338 12.4
years 47.0 - Height-for-
age at 12.4
years
Schooling
level Age, sex, mother’s
schooling, father's
schooling, family
size, agricultural
land, log
income/capita,
school ooring
type, and school
expenses/income
Child health
is not
signicantly
determinant
to the school
enrollment
decision, but,
once enrolled,
nutritional
deciencies
substantially
retard school
progress as
they can
affect a
child's
cognitive
achievement.
Khanam,
2011 [39]
Cross-
sectional
Bangladesh 1441 11.2
years 39.0 - Stunting
between 5
and 17
years
Age at
school
entry,
Schooling
level
Child’s age, gender
of child, total
household
members, log
household
expenditure, father
can read and write,
mother can read
and write, clean
housing condition,
sanitary latrine,
hand washing,
primary school,
secondary girls’
school, secondary
mixed school,
distance to doctor,
and availability of
electricity
Malnourished
children are
signicantly
more likely to
enroll in
school later
than the due
age. It is also
found that,
after
adjusted for
actual
enrolled age,
the grade
attainment of
children is
not affected
by stunting
condition.
McCoy, 2015
[40]
Cross-
sectional
Zambia 2711 6.2
years 48.4 NR Height-for-
age at 6.2
years
Age at
school
entry
Gender, child age,
household size,
regional income,
and urbanicity
Childrens
height-for-
age at age 6
was
signicantly
predictive of
enrollment
both
concurrently
and one year
later,
suggesting
that
caregivers (or
primary
school
teachers)
may be
making
decisions
about school
readiness
largely based
on their
childrens
physical size.
NR: Not reported
Page 10/18
Author, year
Study design
Setting Sample
size Age
(mean) Sex
(% of
girls)
Follow-
up
duration
Exposure Outcome
domain Potential
confounders
considered
Authors’
conclusion
Mendez,
1999 [14]
Prospective
cohort
Philippines 2131 11.0
years NR 10
years Stunting at
2 years Grade
repetition
School
dropout
NR Children
stunted at
age 2 y had a
marked delay
in initial
school
enrollment
and were
much more
likely to
experience
absences
and to drop
out of school
than non-
stunted
children.
Sunny, 2018
[10]
Prospective
cohort
Malawi 1044 NR 47.9 11
years Stunting
between, 0
and 4
months, 11
and 16
months, 4
and 8
years
Age at
school
entry
Schooling
level
Grade
repetition
Sex, father’s
education,
mother’s
education,
household asset
index at birth
Stunting in
early and late
childhood
was
associated
with poor
school
outcomes
(late
enrollment
and poor
progression
through
school).
The
Partnership
for Child
Development,
1999 [35]
Cross-
sectional
Ghana
Tanzania
1566
1390
10.9
years
11.1
years
49.9
52.9
- Ghana:
stunting at
10.9 years
Tanzania:
stunting at
11.1 years
Schooling
level Ghana: age,
Schistosoma
haematobium
(egg/10 ml)
Tanzania: age, sex,
socioeconomic
status score,
school travel time
Children who
are stunted
are more
likely to delay
enrollment in
school.
Delayed
enrollment
may lead to
fewer years
of schooling,
poor
educational
achievement
and poor
employment
prospects.
NR: Not reported
Page 11/18
Author, year
Study design
Setting Sample
size Age
(mean) Sex
(% of
girls)
Follow-
up
duration
Exposure Outcome
domain Potential
confounders
considered
Authors’
conclusion
Satriawan,
2009 [42]
Prospective
cohort
Indonesia 1944 Range:
7–9
years
NR  7
years Height-for-
age and
non-
stunted
Age at
school
entry
Grade
repetition
Time, mother’s
education, father’s
education, age of
household head,
number of 6 to 14
year old children in
the household,
number of females
adult in the
household, number
of male adults in
the household, per-
capita expenditure,
price of rice, price
of sugar, price of
cooking oil, price
of condensed milk,
community xed
effect.
Reducing
incidence of
poor
childhood
nutrition
reduces also
the
probability of
delayed
enrollment,
but not the
probability of
repeating a
grade. More
importantly,
the estimated
effects when
taking into
account the
endogeneity
of childhood
nutrition are
5 to 7 times
stronger than
when
ignoring the
endogeneity
of childhood
nutrition.
NR: Not reported
Risk of bias
Almost all studies had a moderate (13) or high (2) risk of confounding bias (Table3 in Additional le 2). More than one-fth (4, 25%) of studies
have a high risk of bias due to missing data. Missing information was not reported in ve (31%) studies (Table3 in Additional le 2). Risks of
bias in other domains of bias were low for all studies. Assessment of publication bias by funnel plot was not possible because of low number
of studies by outcome of interest and variation in measure of effects estimated.
Stunting and age at school entry
Nine studies presented associations between height-for-age or stunting and an outcome related to age at school entry[7, 10, 36–42]. These
studies could not be combined because exposures, outcomes or effect measures differed. Among these studies, one estimated the association
between height-for-age at two years and early or late enrollment by sex [36]. The meta-analysis from this study (Fig.2) suggests that one unit
increase in height-for-age is associated with a 34% increase in the odds of early enrollment [OR: 1.33 (95% CI: 1.07; 1.67), I²=0%] and a
reduction of 37% in the odds of late enrollment [OR: 0.63 (95% CI: 0.51; 0.78), I²=0%]. All studies reported an association between height-for-age
or stunting and the age at school entry [7, 10, 36–42] (Table2 and Table4 in Additional le 3).
Stunting and schooling level
Two of the four cross-sectional studies[34, 35] assessing the association between height-for-age and school overage by mean difference
observed that an increase of one unit of height-for-age in a child was associated with an increase in schooling level for their age. The pooled
effect of the studies led to similar results [MD: 0.24 (95% CI: 0.14; 0.34), I²=92%] (Fig.3), but was characterized by high heterogeneity. Two
other cross-sectional studies were not used in this pooled effect estimation [39, 43]. One [39] found that stunted children were more likely to be
overage, and the other reported that stunted children were more likely to be in a low grade; a one unit increase in height-for-age was associated
with an increase in grade attainment [43]. In meta-analysis of longitudinal studies, an increase in height-for-age was associated with a
reduction in the odds of school overage [OR: 0.79 (95% CI: 0.70; 0.90), I²=76%] with high heterogeneity. Gandhi, Ashorn (12) and Sunny,
DeStavola (10) were not included in the pooled effect estimation because of the analysis methods they used and their outcome
measurements. Gandhi, Ashorn (12) reported a non-signicant association between height-for-age and schooling level, and Sunny, DeStavola
(10) found a signicant association (Table2 and Table4 in Additional le 3).
Stunting and grade repetition
Figure 4 shows that grade repetition is associated with stunting or height-for-age. All included studies used a longitudinal design. The pooled
estimates suggests that the odds of grade repetition increase by 59 % [OR: 1.59 (95% CI: 1.18; 2.14), I²=51%) for stunted children compared to
non-stunted children with moderate heterogeneity. Two studies, which are not include in the meta-analysis, report an association between
stunting and grade repetition[12, 37], and one other study did not nd an association[42] (see Table2 and Table4 in Additional le 3).
Stunting and school dropout
Page 12/18
Discussion
This systematic review suggests that stunting determines age at school entry, schooling level, and grade repetition. An increase one unit in
standard deviation in height-for-age is associated with an increase in the odds of early enrollment and delayed enrollment. Children with
greater height-for-age were less likely to be overage for their grade. We also found that stunted children were more likely to repeat a grade than
nonstunted children. Results from this study do not allow conclusions to be drawn regarding the relationship between stunting or height-for-
age and dropping out of school.
Childhood stunting can be associated with diculties learning the school curriculum. Children with high height-for-age z-scores, or those who
were non-stunted started school earlier than those with low height-for-age z-score or those who were stunted. The latter are considered unready
to start school at the minimum enrollment age [7, 37, 44]. Delayed enrollment could also reect a lter imposed by schools if administrators
use height as a sign of school readiness [7]. The high probability of grade repetition for stunted children is due to low school performance.
Grade repetition occurs when children’s academic performance is deemed unsatisfactory. Schooling levels can be seen as the reection of age
at school enrollment and grade repetition, and are thus dependent on academic performance. Several studies have highlighted that stunted
growth and height-for-age are associated, respectively, negatively and positively with test results in mathematics, reading, communication and
motor development [6, 21, 45–49]. Results showed that impaired growth and development in infancy negatively affects later academic
performance and therefore academic trajectory, which leads, overall to low school levels. This may explain why stunted children are more likely
to be unemployed, less productive, and to have low social status than non-stunted children[50–53].
Stunting could lead to a delay in the development of cognitive functions and permanent cognitive impairments, which improve little with
age[54]. This relationship between stunting and cognitive abilities is particularly important in the rst years of life when vital human
development occurs in all domains, including the brain formation[16, 55]. When stunting occurs in this early stage of life, it severely affects
attention development, executive functions such as cognitive exibility, working memory, and visuospatial functions like visual
construction[54]. Experimental research on animals has also shown that nutrition deciencies negatively affect brain development and
measure of performance[55–58], but it is dicult to extrapolate this to human cognition[57]. Thus, to establish causality between nutritional
status and performances, intervention studies has been undertaken, and they have shown that early intervention on health and nutrition
increase child probability to be enrolled on time in primary school, and improve cognitive development [15, 59].
Strengths and limitations
This review is one step towards better understanding the effects of growth in early childhood on subsequent school trajectories. It is the rst
review to highlight the components of the academic trajectory that are inuenced by stunting. This review does have some weaknesses,
however. First, almost all studies were identied as having moderate risk of confounding bias, even though some of them used advanced
methods to control for confusion. This is due to the tool of bias assessment. Second, outcomes and measures of effect varied widely across
studies, which limited our ability to estimate pooled effects. However, this diversity allowed us to explore multiple facets of academic
performance. Third, we did not obtain sucient data to estimate pooled effects of stunting or height-for-age on dropouts, which suggests that
this outcome has not been suciently studied in the literature. Fourth, due to the low number of studies, we were not able to perform subgroup
analysis which may have shown an effect of the timing of stunting (e.g., stunting before 2 years vs stunting after 2 years).
Conclusion
The results show that stunting in childhood might lead to a delay school enrollment, grade repetition, school dropout and low schooling levels.
This study is a step towards understanding the overall effect of stunting or height-for-age on academic trajectory. Results showed that
impaired growth and development in infancy is associated with a delay of school age entry, an increased risk of grade repetition, and increased
school dropout, which, in turn, lead to childrens low levels of education. Although this review provides an overall picture of the educational
trajectory of children from developing countries who experienced stunting in childhood, further research is needed on the effect of stunting on
educational trajectories among this population. Since stunting affects more children from poor communities than from wealthy communities,
Pooled effects were not estimated for school dropout because no two studies used the same effect measures. Nevertheless, results from these
studies were mitigated. Mendez and Adair (14) reported that stunted children were more likely to drop out of school than non-stunted children.
But Glewwe and Jacoby (38) found that taller children tended to leave school earlier, while Bogin and MacVean (33) reported that school
continuation or dropout was not inuenced by health or nutritional environment (Table2 and Table4 in Additional le 3).
Sensitivity analysis
The number of studies was not sucient to conduct sensitivity analyses according to risk of bias or subgroup analyses by age. We performed
a sensitivity analysis on schooling level by removing one study or estimated effect at a time from the pooled effect size. When we removed the
effect size of Tanzania from The Partnership for Child Development (35) study, the Higgin’s I² decreased from 90–0% and the magnitude of the
pooled effect increased from 0.24 (Fig.3) to 0.29 (Fig.5 in Additional le 4).
Page 13/18
future research should also explore the effect modication of socioeconomic status on the relationship between stunting and school
trajectories to inform the development of effective interventions. The current results imply the need for leaders of developing countries to work
more for the prevention of stunting through programs and projects focused on nutrition and health problems in childhood. Similarly, health
issues should be integrated into education policies to allow for specic care of stunted children in order to improve their school performance.
Abbreviations
PROSPERO
International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews
PRISMA-P
Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis Protocols
OR
Odds Ratio
MD
Mean Difference
RevMan
Review Manager
PICOS
Population, Intervention/exposure, Comparator, Outcome and Study Design
Declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Not Applicable
Consent for publication
Not Applicable
Availability of data and materials
Not Applicable
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing of interests
Funding
This research has no source of funding.
Authors' contributions
RJG, SH, and LM conceived the study. RJG drafted the protocol and LM revised it. RJG and LPB selected studied and extracted data. SH
intervened to settle disagreements. RJG wrote the rst draft of the manuscript, which was revised by LM and JFK. The nal manuscript was
read and approved by all authors.
Acknowledgements
Not Applicable
References
Page 14/18
1. Institut international de recherche sur les politiques alimentaires. Rapport 2015 sur la nutrition mondiale: mesures et redevabilité en vue
d’accélérer les progrès mondiaux en matière de nutrition et de développement durable. Washington, DC; 2015.
2. Development Initiatives. 2018 Global Nutrition Report: Shining a light to spur action on nutrition. Bristol, UK: Development Initiatives; 2018.
3. United Nations Childrens Fund, World Health Organization, World Bank Group. Levels and trends in chlid malnutrition: Key ndings of the
2018 Edition of the Joint Child Malnutrition Estimates. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2018.
4. UNESCO Institut for Statistics (UIS). http://data.uis.unesco.org/ [cited 2020 09 July].
5. Berkman DS, Lescano AG, Gilman RH, Lopez SL, Black MM. Effects of stunting, diarrhoeal disease, and parasitic infection during infancy
on cognition in late childhood: a follow-up study. The Lancet. 2002;359(9306):564–71.
. Crookston BT, Schott W, Cueto S, Dearden KA, Engle P, Georgiadis A, et al. Postinfancy growth, schooling, and cognitive achievement:
Young Lives. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2013;98(6):1555–63.
7. Alderman H, Hoogeveen H, Rossi M. Preschool nutrition and subsequent schooling attainment: longitudinal evidence from Tanzania. Econ
Dev Cult Change. 2009;57(2):239–60.
. Humphrey JH. Child undernutrition, tropical enteropathy, toilets, and handwashing. The Lancet. 2009;374(9694):1032–5.
9. Yamauchi F. Early Childhood Nutrition, Schooling, and Sibling Inequality in a Dynamic Context: Evidence from South Africa. Econ Dev Cult
Change. 2008;56(3):657–82.
10. Sunny BS, DeStavola B, Dube A, Kondowe S, Crampin AC, Glynn JR. Does early linear growth failure inuence later school performance? A
cohort study in Karonga district, northern Malawi. PLoS One. 2018;13(11):1–15.
11. Wolde T, Belachew T. Chronic undernutrition (stunting) is detrimental to academic performance among primary schools of adolescent
children: a randomized cross sectional survey in Southern Ethiopia. BMC Research Notes. 2019;12(1):142.
12. Gandhi M, Ashorn P, Maleta K, Teivaanmaki T, Duan X, Cheung YB. Height gain during early childhood is an important predictor of
schooling and mathematics ability outcomes. Acta Paediatr. 2011;100(8):1113–8.
13. Gomes-Neto JB, Hanushek EA, Leite RH, Frota-Bezzera RC. Health and schooling: Evidence and policy implications for developing
countries. Economics of Education Review. 1997;16(3):271–82.
14. Mendez MA, Adair LS. Severity and timing of stunting in the rst two years of life affect performance on cognitive tests in late childhood. J
Nutr. 1999;129(8):1555–62.
15. Perkins JM, Kim R, Krishna A, McGovern M, Aguayo VM, Subramanian SV. Understanding the association between stunting and child
development in low- and middle-income countries: Next steps for research and intervention. Soc Sci Med. 2017;193:101–9.
1. Grantham-McGregor S, Cheung YB, Cueto S, Glewwe P, Richter L, Strupp B. Developmental potential in the rst 5 years for children in
developing countries. The Lancet. 2007;369(9555):60–70.
17. Walker SP, Wachs TD, Meeks Gardner J, Lozoff B, Wasserman GA, Pollitt E, et al. Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in
developing countries. The Lancet. 2007;369(9556):145–57.
1. Walker SP, Wachs TD, Grantham-McGregor S, Black MM, Nelson CA, Huffman SL, et al. Inequality in early childhood: risk and protective
factors for early child development. The Lancet. 2011;378(9799):1325–38.
19. Gorman KS. Malnutrition and Cognitive Development: Evidence from Experimental/Quasi-Experimental Studies among the Mild-to-
Moderately Malnourished. The Journal of Nutrition. 1995;125(suppl_8):2239S-44S.
20. Grantham-McGregor S. A Review of Studies of the Effect of Severe Malnutrition on Mental Development. The Journal of Nutrition.
1995;125(suppl_8):2233S-8S.
21. Sudfeld CR, McCoy DC, Fink G, Muhihi A, Bellinger DC, Masanja H, et al. Malnutrition and its determinants are associated with suboptimal
cognitive, communication, and motor development in Tanzanian children. J Nutr. 2015;145(12):2705–14.
22. Moher D, Shamseer L, Clarke M, Ghersi D, Liberati A, Petticrew M, et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis
protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Systematic Reviews. 2015;4(1):1.
23. Shamseer L, Moher D, Clarke M, Ghersi D, Liberati A, Petticrew M, et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis
protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015: elaboration and explanation. BMJ: British Medical Journal. 2015;349:g7647.
24. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gøtzsche PC, Ioannidis JPA, et al. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and
meta-analyses of studies that evaluate healthcare interventions: explanation and elaboration. The BMJ. 2009;339.
25. Institut de statistique de l'UNESCO. Classication internationale type de l’Éducation (CITE) 2011. Québec: Montréal; 2013.
2. Sterne JA, Hernán MA, Reeves BC, Savović J, Berkman ND, Viswanathan M, et al. ROBINS-I: a tool for assessing risk of bias in non-
randomised studies of interventions. Bmj. 2016;355:i4919.
27. Collaboration C. Review Manager (Revman). Version 5.2. Copenhagen: The Nordic Cochrane Centre; 2012.
2. Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JP, Rothstein HR. Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons; 2009.
Page 15/18
29. Higgins JPT, Thomas J, Chandler J, Cumpston M, Li T, Page MJ, et al. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions.
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell; 2019. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119536604.
30. Higgins JPT, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med. 2002;21(11):1539–58.
31. Higgins JPT, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. 2003;327(7414):557–60.
32. Zomahoun HTV, Guénette L, Lauzier S, Lawani AM, Ferdynus C, Huiart L, et al. Effectiveness of motivational interviewing interventions on
medication adherence in adults with chronic diseases: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Epidemiol. 2017;46(2):589–602.
33. Bogin B, MacVean RB. Growth status, age, and grade as predictors of school continuation for Guatemalan Indian children. Am J Phys
Anthropol. 1987;73(4):507–13.
34. Grira H. The determinants of grade attainment in low-income countries: Evidence from rural Bangladesh. Dev Econ. 2004;42(4):494–509.
35. The Partnership for Child Development. Short stature and the age of enrolment in Primary School: studies in two African countries. The
Partnership for Child Development. Soc Sci Med. 1999;48(5):675–82.
3. Daniels MC, Adair LS. Growth in young Filipino children predicts schooling trajectories through high school. J Nutr. 2004;134(6):1439–46.
37. Glewwe P, Jacoby HG, King EM. Early childhood nutrition and academic achievement: A longitudinal analysis. J Public Econ.
2001;81(3):345–68.
3. Glewwe P, Jacoby HG. An economic analysis of delayed primary school enrollment in a low income country: the role of early childhood
nutrition. The review of Economics and Statistics. 1995:156–69.
39. Khanam R, Nghiem HS, Rahman MM. The impact of childhood malnutrition on schooling: Evidence from Bangladesh. J Biosoc Sci.
2011;43(4):437–51.
40. McCoy DC, Zuilkowski SS, Fink G. Poverty, physical stature, and cognitive skills: Mechanisms underlying children's school enrollment in
Zambia. Dev Psychol. 2015;51(5):600–14.
41. Alderman H, Hoddinott J, Kinsey B. Long term consequences of early childhood malnutrition. Oxford Economic Papers-New Series.
2006;58(3):450–74.
42. Satriawan E. Essays on evaluation of the effectiveness of public health programs on child's well-being: Evidence from Indonesia family
life survey [Dissertation]. United States: Michigan State University; 2009.
43. Acharya Y, Luke N, Haro MF, Rose W, Russell PSS, Oommen AM, et al. Nutritional status, cognitive achievement, and educational
attainment of children aged 8–11 in rural South India. PLoS One. 2019;14(10):e0223001.
44. Fentiman A, Hall A, Bundy D. School enrolment patterns in rural Ghana: a comparative study of the impact of location, gender, age and
health on children's access to basic schooling. Comparative education. 1999;35(3):331–49.
45. Wisniewski SLW. Child nutrition, health problems, and school achievement in Sri Lanka. World Dev. 2010;38(3):315–32.
4. Themane M, Monyeki K, Nthangeni M, Kemper H, Twisk J. The relationship between health (malnutrition) and educational achievements
(Maths and English) in the rural children of South Africa. International Journal of Educational Development. 2003;23(6):637–43.
47. Chang SM, Walker SP, Grantham-McGregor S, Powell CA. Early childhood stunting and later behaviour and school achievement. J Child
Psychol Psychiatry. 2002;43(6):775–83.
4. Asmare B, Taddele M, Berihun S, Wagnew F. Nutritional status and correlation with academic performance among primary school children,
northwest Ethiopia. BMC Res Notes. 2018;11(1):805.
49. Ayalew M, Bayray A, Bekele A, Handebo S. Nutritional Status and Educational Performance of School-Aged Children in Lalibela Town
Primary Schools, Northern Ethiopia. International Journal of Pediatrics. 2020;2020:9.
50. Case A, Fertig A, Paxson C. The lasting impact of childhood health and circumstance. J Health Econ. 2005;24(2):365–89.
51. Case A, Paxson C. Stature and status: Height, ability, and labor market outcomes. The journal of political economy. 2008;116(3):499–532.
52. Dewey KG, Begum K. Long-term consequences of stunting in early life. Maternal Child Nutrition. 2011;7(s3):5–18.
53. Smith JP. The impact of childhood health on adult labor market outcomes. Rev Econ Stat. 2009;91(3):478–89.
54. Kar BR, Rao SL, Chandramouli BA. Cognitive development in children with chronic protein energy malnutrition. Behav Brain Funct.
2008;4(1):31.
55. Prado EL, Dewey KG. Nutrition and brain development in early life. Nutr Rev. 2014;72(4):267–84.
5. Fish I, Winick M. Effect of malnutrition on regional growth of the developing rat brain. Exp Neurol. 1969;25(4):534–40.
57. Lucas A, Morley R, Isaacs E. Nutrition and Mental Development. Nutr Rev. 2001;59(8):24–33.
5. Smart JL. ‘Malnutrition, Learning and Behavior’: 25 years on from the MIT Symposium. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society.
2007;52(1):189 – 99.
Page 16/18
59. Todd JE, Winters P. The effect of early interventions in health and nutrition on on-time school enrollment: Evidence from the oportunidades
program in rural Mexico. Economic development cultural change. 2011;59(3):549–81.
Figures
Figure 1
Study selection process for the review * Kappa for screening by title and abstract = 97.5% ** Kappa for selection by full text = 83.5%
Page 17/18
Figure 2
Meta-analysis of the association between height-for-age and early or late enrollment
Figure 3
Meta-analysis of the association between height-for-age and schooling level
Figure 4
Meta-analysis of the association between height-for-age and grade repetition
Supplementary Files
This is a list of supplementary les associated with this preprint. Click to download.
Page 18/18
Additionalle1.docx
Additionalle2.docx
Additionalle3.docx
Additionalle4.docx
Additionalle5.docx
Article
Full-text available
Background: The association between school and home is fundamental to sustainable education: parents' understanding of the school's priorities and teachers' understanding of their pupils' home environment are both vital for children to remain in school and succeed academically. The relationship between parents and teachers is closest in preschool settings, providing a valuable opportunity to build bridges between home and school. In this protocol paper, we outline our planned methods for identifying beneficial home and school behaviors. Objective: Our project aims to identify culture-specific structures and behaviors in home and school settings, which influence the quantity and quality of child-directed speech and identify positive experiences that can help improve children's linguistic development and nutrition. Methods: Using a mixed methods approach and focusing on early language learning, nutrition, and responsive caregiving, we will video-record and analyze mealtime language and eating behaviors at home and in school, targeting 80 preschool children and their families in rural Kenya and Zambia. In addition, we will assess children's language skills through audio recordings and use questionnaire-based interviews to collect extensive sociodemographic and dietary data. Results: Between the start of our project in January 2020 and the end of December 2021, we had collected complete sets of sociodemographic, observational, and food recall data for 40 children in Kenya and 16 children in Zambia. By the end of May 2022, we had started data collection for an additional 24 children in Zambia and transcribed and coded approximately 85% of the data. By the end of September, 2022, we plan to complete data collection, transcription, and coding for the entire sample of 80 children across both countries. From September 2022 onwards, we will focus on analyzing our language data, and we hope to have results ready for publication in early 2023. By relating children's language outcomes and nutritional intake to the observed mealtime behaviors, we hope to identify practices that increase the quantity and quality of child-directed speech and improve children's nutritional intake. Conclusions: Good nutrition and the promotion of language learning are key issues in early childhood development. By using a cross-cultural approach, combining a variety of methods, and working closely with stakeholders and policy makers throughout the project, we hope to find and share best practices for improving children's linguistic outcomes and nutrition and lay the foundation for the development of practitioner networks and parent outreach programs. International registered report identifier (irrid): DERR1-10.2196/36925.
Article
Full-text available
Background. Every student has the potential to do well in school. Failing to provide good nutrition puts them at risk for missing out on meeting that potential and leads to long-term irreversible damage to cognitive development. However, taking action today to provide healthier choices in schools can help set students up for a successful future full of possibilities. So, this study is aimed at assessing the effect of nutritional status on academic performance of school-aged children. Methods. A school-based cross-sectional study was conducted from Feb 05- March 30, 2017 in Lalibela town. A total of 505 primary school students were included in the study. The child anthropometric measures were calculated using AnthroPlus software version 1.0.4 WHO 2007 standards. Bivariable and multivariable ordinal logistic regression were fitted. The proportional odds ratio (POR) with a 95% CI and value < 0.05 were considered statistically significant. Results. The overall prevalence of stunting, underweight, and thinness was 29.5%, 35.7%, and 29.5%, respectively. In this study, 14.7% of the study participants had low academic achievement in the semester. The multivariable analysis showed that rural residence (; 95% CI: (0.21, 0.75)), not studying regularly (; 95% CI: (0.29, 0.82)), severe underweight (; 95% CI: (0.09, 0.71)), and severe stunting (; 95% CI: (0.01, 0.37)) were associated with decreased odds of high average semester score achievement of school-aged children. Additionally, higher maternal educational status (; 95% CI: (1.10, 4.07)) and family income from 550 to 2999 ETB (; 95% CI: (1.04, 2.81)) were associated with increased odds of high semester average score achievement. Conclusion. Nearly one-third of school-aged children in Lalibela town were stunted, thin, and underweight. Rural place of residence, not studying regularly study, underweight, and stunting were associated with decreased academic achievement. While, family monthly income from 550 to 2999 ETB and higher maternal education status were associated with increased academic achievement. 1. Background Globally, the magnitude and severity of malnutrition are still a public health problem. Malnourishment accounts for 2.6 million of deaths per year and more than 450 million children’s failure in somatic and mental development [1]. Child undernutrition is a challenge of global health, especially in low- and middle-income countries [2]. Current evidences on children in Africa, Asia, and Latin America reported that the mortality of stunted and underweight children was greater than three times that of well-nourished children [3]. Anthropometric indices are commonly used to assess nutritional status, health, and development of children as well as the whole population [4, 5]. Nutritional status depends on dietary intake of food, nutrients, diseases and overall health status, and health care practices, which indirectly affects the educational performances [6]. Academic performance, intellectual development, and school attendance of the children and adolescents were adversely affected by undernutrition. On the other hand, good nutrition was associated with progress of perceptive and behavioral talents and improved school attendance [7, 8]. Empirical evidences witnessed the influence of nutritional status on academic achievement of the students. For example, in Indonesia, the cognitive development of preschool children was significantly affected by stunting, underweight, and severe wasting [9]. Additionally, low level of educational performance (%) in Tamil (a language in Nuwara Eliya Educational Zone), mathematics, and overall subject average was significantly associated with underweight [10]. On the other hand, stunting is associated with decreased school-age enrolment and reduced achievement equivalent to three years of schooling as well as productivity deficit of greater than 20% [8, 11]. Though there are different issues raised for poor educational performances, 16% of the total repetition rate in primary education is highly associated with stunting in Ethiopia [12]. The participation rate of primary education is less than 22% of the relevant age cohort [13]. Similarly, realities in the country indicates that low educational performance rate of children with stunting is 15.1% and 11.2% with nonstunted children [14]. So, undernutrition was pointed to be associated with low educational achievements in primary education [15]. Higher risk of grade repetition and dropout of schools among age category of 6-18 years is still because of stunting [12]. Although there are several reports of malnutrition in Ethiopia, the relationship of nutritional status and educational performance of school-aged children was not yet explored in different areas of the country. Thus, this study was aimed at assessing nutritional status and academic achievements of primary school children in Lalibela, Northern Ethiopia. 2. Methods 2.1. Study Setting and Design A school-based cross-sectional study was carried out from February 5 to March 30, 2017 in Lalibela town. Lalibela town is located in North Wollo Administrative Zone, which is 701 km from Addis Ababa, the capital of Ethiopia. According to the town health bureau population profile, the total population of the town is estimated to be 32,668. One district hospital and a health center provide health service in the town. With regard to education, one College, one preparatory school, one high school, nine primary schools, and two kindergarten schools are available in the town. There are about 2,992 total children in primary schools in the town [16]. 2.2. Study Population All school-aged children in Lalibela primary schools and those who lived in the town for at least six months were recruited in the study. The sample size was computed using Epi Info version 7, by considering the following assumptions: proportion of low academic performance in unexposed 34.9% [10], %, %, , and . By adding 10% nonresponse rate, the final sample size was 542. Four public schools were selected randomly from nine primary schools in Lalibela town. The sample size was distributed proportionally to the selected schools. Then, simple random sampling method was employed to select each study participant. 2.3. Data Collection Procedures Structured, pretested, and interviewer-administered questionnaires were used for data collection. First, the questionnaire was prepared in English and translated into the local language, Amharic, and then translated back to English to check the consistency. Mothers or caregivers were interviewed on socioeconomic and other demographic data. Anthropometric data (height and weight) were collected from the school-aged children using standardized procedures. Weight was measured to the nearest 0.1 kg with an electronic scale. Child height was measured to the nearest 0.1 cm using a wooden stadiometer placed on a flat surface. Regarding educational performance, 2017-year first semester scores on Amharic (local language), English, Mathematics, and Science courses as well as semester average score were extracted from the roster book of students in each school. Three supervisors and five clinical nurse data collectors were recruited for data collection. Intensive training was given for the data collectors and supervisors by the primary investigator. 2.4. Study Variables Academic performance was defined as low score if less than 50%, medium score if 50%-75%, and high score if ≥75% [15]. Undernutrition was assessed according to the new WHO recommendation using stunting (height-for-age score (SD), underweight is a weight for age (SD), and thinness (body mass index for SD. The four categories were defined as normal (+1 to -1SD -score), mild (between -1 and -2SD -score), moderate (between -2 and -3SD -score), and severe (less than -3SD -score) [17]. 2.5. Data Processing and Analysis Data were coded and entered in SPSS version 22 and exported to Stata version 14 statistical software for analysis. HAZ, WAZ, and BAZ were calculated using AnthroPlus software version 1.0.4 by the WHO 2007 growth reference standard. Descriptive statistics were used to present the findings. A bivariable analysis was done for each subject and total average score separately. Variables with values of < 0.2 in the bivariable analysis were entered into a multivariable ordinal logistic regression analysis to identify the independent predictors associated with academic achievement. In the multivariable ordinal regression, the statistical significance of a variable was declared with a value of < 0.05. The proportional odds assumption was checked using the Pearson chi-squared goodness-of-fit test, and data were consistent with the fitted model. 2.6. Ethical Consideration The procedure of this study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Mekelle University College of Health Sciences. Formal letters of cooperation were written to all selected public primary schools in Lalibela Town Administration. Written consent was obtained from each parent, caregivers, and directly from the school directors of all study participants. Parents and caregivers were kindly asked by formal letter to get permission before any procedure. Then, each study participant was asked for their willingness to participate in the study. Furthermore, we gave all the participants a full right to discontinue the procedures when they assumed that things were going bad with them. 3. Results 3.1. Sociodemographic Characteristics A total of 505 school-aged children were included in the study which gives the response rate of 93%. The mean age of the participant was year. About 89.7% and 78.6% of the participants were living with their mothers and live in urban setting, respectively. Majority of them (98.6%) were Orthodox Christian. With regard to maternal characteristics, 79.3% and 46.9% of the mothers were married and illiterate on educational status, see Table 1. Variables Frequencies Percent (%) Gender Male 252 49.90 Female 253 50.10 Is a child living with his mother? Yes 453 89.70 No 52 10.30 Maternal age (year) <20 3 0.59 20-25 44 8.71 26-29 90 17.82 30-35 140 27.72 >35 228 45.15 Residence Urban 397 78.61 Rural 108 21.39 Maternal marital status: Married 410 81.19 Divorced 50 9.90 Separated 33 6.53 Widowed 12 2.38 Maternal religion Orthodox 498 98.61 Muslim 7 1.39 Family income per month (ETB) <1000 251 49.70 1001-2500 189 37.43 2501 and above 65 12.87 Number of family living together 2 46 9.1 3-5 307 60.8 >5 152 30.1
Article
Full-text available
Background Malnutrition among children is one of the most pressing health concerns middle- and low-income countries face today, particularly those in Sub-Saharan Africa and South Asia. Early-life malnutrition has been shown to affect long-term health and income. One hypothesized channel linking early-life malnutrition and long-term outcomes is cognitive development. However, there is limited empirical evidence on the relationship between nutritional status and cognitive achievement in middle childhood. Study design As part of the South India Community Health Study (SICHS), we collected educational attainment and anthropometric data from 1,194 children in rural Vellore district of Tamil Nadu, India, and assessed their math and reading skills. We analyzed the relationship between continuous and binary anthropometric measures of nutritional status and three measures of cognitive achievement (reading, math, and grade level), adjusting for potential confounders, using a regression framework. Results Lower height-for-age and weight-for-age and their corresponding binary measures (stunting, underweight) were associated with lower reading scores, lower math scores, and lower grade level, with the exception of the association between weight-for-age and reading, which was marginally significant. A stunted child had one-third of a grade disadvantage compared to a non-stunted counterpart, whereas an underweight child had one-fourth of a grade disadvantage compared to a non-underweight counterpart. Lower BMI-for-age was associated with grade level and marginally associated with lower math scores, and its binary measure (thinness) was marginally associated with lower math scores. Conclusions Acute and chronic malnutrition in middle childhood were negatively associated with math scores, reading scores, and educational attainment. Our study provides new evidence that cognitive achievement during middle childhood could be an important mechanism underlying the association between early-life malnutrition and long-term wellbeing.
Article
Full-text available
Objective Despite several decades of work in improving childhood nutrition in Ethiopia, stunting remains a major public health concern with lack of evidence on its effect on school performance. Therefore, this study aimed at determining prevalence of stunting and the impact on academic performance among schoolchildren attending primary schools in Meskan District of Southern Ethiopia. Results During October 2016, we interviewed primary school children and their parents, collected anthropometric measurements of children, and conducted school record reviews among 408 randomly selected children attending ten primary schools. Univariate and multivariable linear regression were performed to assess stunting associated with academic performance among schoolchildren. A total of 378 primary schoolchildren were involved in the study giving a response rate of 93%. The prevalence of stunting was 16.9%. Grade repetition, place of residence, class rank of students and absenteeism from the class was negatively associated with the academic performance. In general, the present study children had relatively high prevalence of chronic undernutrition. We found that school underperformance was significantly high in the study area. The study underlines the need for nutrition interventions targeting rural areas to improve children’s academic performance.
Article
Full-text available
Objective This study aimed to determine the association between nutritional status and academic performance among primary school children in Debre Markos Town, northwest Ethiopia, 2017. Results The prevalence of stunting, underweight and wasting were 27.5% (95% CI 23.2–31.9%), 20.4% (95% CI 16.5–24.3%) and 8.7% (95% CI 6.2–11.5%), correspondingly. The low level of educational performance was significantly higher (p < 0.05) among the stunted, underweight and wasted children than that of the normal children. In multivariable logistic regression, age of the child (Adjusted Odds Ratio (AOR) = 0.177, 95% CI 0.07, 0.4), monthly income less < 1000.00 birr (AOR = 0.05, 95% Cl 0.02, 0.15), stunted children (AOR = 0.21, 95% CI 0.10, 0.43) and under-weight (AOR = 0.63, 95% CI 0.26, 0.84) were associated with academic performance. This study revealed that indicators of undernutrition were prevalent among school-age children. Thus, collaboration between the health and education sectors is required to alleviate the problem. Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (10.1186/s13104-018-3909-1) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Stunting or linear growth retardation in childhood is associated with delayed cognitive development due to related causes (malnutrition, illness, poor stimulation), which leads to poor school outcomes at later ages, although evidence of the association between the timing and persistence of stunting and school outcomes within the sub-Saharan African context is limited. Methods Anthropometric data around birth (0–4 months), early (11–16 months) and late childhood (ages 4–8 years) along with school outcomes up until the age of 11 were analysed for a cohort of 1,044 respondents, born between 2002–2004 in Karonga district, northern Malawi. The schooling outcomes were age at school enrolment, grade repetition in Standard 1 and age-for-grade by age 11. Height-for-Age Z-scores (HAZ) and growth trajectories were examined as predictors, based on stunting (<-2SD HAZ) and on trajectories between early and late childhood (never stunted, improvers, decliners or persistently stunted). Multinomial and logistic regression were used to estimate the association between stunting/trajectories and schooling, adjusted for socioeconomic confounders. Results The effects of stunting on schooling were evident in early childhood but were more pronounced in late childhood. Children who were stunted in early childhood (9.3%) were less likely to be underage at enrolment, more likely to repeat Standard 1 and were 2–3 times more likely to be overage for their grade by the age of 11, compared to their non-stunted peers. Those persistently stunted between early and late childhood (7.3%) faced the worst consequences on schooling, being three times as likely to enrol late and 3–5 times more likely to be overage for their grade by the age of 11, compared to those never stunted. Compared to improvers, those persistently stunted were three times as likely to be overage by two or more years by the age of 11, with no effect on enrolment or repetition. Conclusion Our findings confirm the importance of early childhood stunting on schooling outcomes and suggest some mitigation by improvements in growth by the age of starting school. The nutritional and learning needs of those persistently stunted may need to be prioritised in future interventions.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Medication adherence is frequently suboptimal in adults with chronic diseases, resulting in negative consequences. Motivational interviewing (MI) is a collaborative conversational style for strengthening a person's motivation and commitment to change. We aimed to assess whether MI interventions are effective to enhance medication adherence in adults with chronic diseases and to explore the effect of individual MI intervention characteristics. Methods: We searched electronic databases and reference lists of relevant articles to find randomized controlled trials (RCTs) that assessed MI intervention effectiveness on medication adherence in adults with chronic diseases. A random-effects model was used to estimate a pooled MI intervention effect size and its heterogeneity (I(2)). We also explored the effects of individual MI characteristics on MI intervention effect size using a meta-regression with linear mixed model. Results: Nineteen RCTs were identified, and 16 were included in the meta-analysis. The pooled MI intervention effect size was 0.12 [95% confidence interval (CI) = (0.05, 0.20), I(2) = 1%]. Interventions that were based on MI only [β = 0.183, 95% CI = (0.004, 0.362)] or those in which interventionists were coached during intervention implementation [β = 0.465, 95% CI = (0.028, 0.902)] were the most effective. MI interventions that were delivered solely face to face were more effective than those that were delivered solely by phone [β = 0.270, 95% CI = (0.041, 0.498)]. Conclusions: This synthesis of RCTs suggests that MI interventions might be effective at enhancing of medication adherence in adults treated for chronic diseases. Further research is however warranted, as the observed intervention effect size was small.
Article
Full-text available
Non-randomised studies of the effects of interventions are critical to many areas of healthcare evaluation, but their results may be biased. It is therefore important to understand and appraise their strengths and weaknesses. We developed ROBINS-I ("Risk Of Bias In Non-randomised Studies-of Interventions"), a new tool for evaluating risk of bias in estimates of the comparative effectiveness (harm or benefit) of interventions from studies that did not use randomisation to allocate units (individuals or clusters of individuals) to comparison groups. The tool will be particularly useful to those undertaking systematic reviews that include non-randomised studies.
Article
The revised edition of the Handbook offers the only guide on how to conduct, report and maintain a Cochrane Review ? The second edition of The Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions contains essential guidance for preparing and maintaining Cochrane Reviews of the effects of health interventions. Designed to be an accessible resource, the Handbook will also be of interest to anyone undertaking systematic reviews of interventions outside Cochrane, and many of the principles and methods presented are appropriate for systematic reviews addressing research questions other than effects of interventions. This fully updated edition contains extensive new material on systematic review methods addressing a wide-range of topics including network meta-analysis, equity, complex interventions, narrative synthesis, and automation. Also new to this edition, integrated throughout the Handbook, is the set of standards Cochrane expects its reviews to meet. Written for review authors, editors, trainers and others with an interest in Cochrane Reviews, the second edition of The Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions continues to offer an invaluable resource for understanding the role of systematic reviews, critically appraising health research studies and conducting reviews.
Article
Stunting, caused by experiences of chronic nutritional deprivation, affects approximately 25% of children under age five globally (i.e., 156 million children). In this review, evidence of a relationship between stunting and child development in low- and middle-income countries is summarized, and issues for further research are discussed. We focus on studies that measured low height-for-age among children less than 5 years old as the exposure and gross/fine motor skills, psychosocial competencies, cognitive abilities, or schooling and learning milestones as the outcomes. This review highlights three key findings. First, the variability in child development tools and metrics used among studies and the differences in the timing and frequency of the assessments complicate comparisons across study findings. Second, considerable evidence from across many countries supports an association between stunting and poor child development despite methodological differences and heterogeneity in the magnitude of associations. Further, effect sizes differ by developmental domain with greater associations shown for cognitive/schooling outcomes. How stunting influences child development, which domains of child development are more affected, and how the various domains of child development influence one another require further experimental research to test causal pathways. Finally, there is mixed evidence of the additive effect of nutrition + stimulation interventions on child development. However, understanding best methods for improving child developmental outcomes - either through nutrition programs or through integrated nutrition + psychosocial stimulation programs (or nutrition + other program interventions) - is a key area of further inquiry. Given that nearly 40% of children under age five suffer from loss of developmental potential - for which stunting is likely one of the key risk factors - reductions in stunting could have tremendous implications for child development and human capital formation, particularly in low- and middle-income countries.