ArticlePDF Available

National and Ethnic Identities and Differences Today: A Psychological Perspective

Authors:

Abstract

This paper discusses national and ethnic identities and differences as they currently exist in the world today. The perspective adopted is that of the social sciences in general and psychology in particular. The main focus is on how social and psychological processes shape people’s identities and perceptions of difference, and how people’s national and ethnic identifications and attitudes are acquired and develop through the course of childhood and adolescence.
1
National and Ethnic Identities and Differences Today: A Psychological
Perspective0F
1
Martyn Barrett
Centre for Research on Nationalism, Ethnicity and Multiculturalism (CRONEM),
University of Surrey, UK
Introduction
1.1
Several of the papers within this thematic session of the Athens Dialogues have addressed issues concerning
identity and difference within the contexts of Classical Greek civilisation, the Hellenistic-Roman world, Byzantium,
and the Islamic world. However, identity and difference do not only represent major issues in the historic past.
They also represent major issues in the world of today. Indeed, many of the most significant and profound events
which are taking place in today’s globalised world are driven precisely by issues of identity and difference. The
politicization of national, ethnic and religious identities within contemporary affairs, coupled to the prejudice and
hostility which many people direct at national, ethnic, and religious groups other than their own, and the global
scale of the consequences which can potentially flow from these antagonisms, have made the task of
understanding the construction of human identities and cultural differences especially urgent.
1.2
In this paper, I will comment on national and ethnic identities and differences as they currently exist in the world
today. Because I am a psychologist, my perspective will be that of the social sciences in general and psychology
in particular. My main focus will be on how social and psychological processes shape people’s identities and
perceptions of difference, and how people’s national and ethnic identifications and attitudes are acquired and
develop through the course of childhood and adolescence.
On Multiple Identities
2.1
It is necessary to begin by observing that national and ethnic identities are not our only identities. Instead, we all
have a large number of different identities. These various identities are rooted in different aspects of our self-
perceptions. Some of our identities are what may be called ‘social identities.’ Social identities are those identities
which are derived from our membership of social groups (such as our nation, our ethnic group, our religious
group, our gender group, our occupational group, the organisation that we work for, the sporting clubs or teams
that we support, etc.). When our membership of one of these social groups forms a salient part of our own self-
concept in such a way that we attribute emotional significance and value to that group membership, we may be
said to have acquired a subjective identification with the group (Tajfel 1978). However, social identities are not
our only identities. We can also use our personal attributes (e.g. caring, studious, tolerant, fun-loving, etc.), our
interpersonal relationships and roles (e.g. mother, friend, colleague, etc.) and autobiographical narratives about
our lives (e.g. born in London to middle class parents, educated at a state school, etc.) to define ourselves and
our own uniqueness further (Stryker 1980; Deaux 1992, 1996). These multiple identifications with different social
groups, attributes, relationships, roles and narratives help us to define, position and orientate ourselves within the
social world relative to other people. Because the primary focus of this paper is on national and ethnic identities,
most of the following comments relate primarily to social identities. However, it should be borne in mind
throughout that these identities constitute only one part of the multiplicity of identities which constitute our self-
concepts.
1 Martyn Barrett (2010). National and ethnic identities and differences today: A psychological perspective. Invited paper
presented at the Athens Dialogues 2010, Onassis Cultural Centre, Athens, Greece, 24-27 November 2010.
2
On Individual Variability in Patterns of Social Identification
3.1
Psychological research has revealed that people’s subjective representations of the relationships which exist
between their various social identifications can differ from the objective relationships which actually exist between
the social categories which form the basis of their identifications (Roccas and Brewer 2002). For example, in the
case of an Italian person who is a Catholic, the objective relationship between being Italian and being Catholic is
one of partial overlap: some but not all Italian people are Catholics, and some but not all Catholics are Italian.
However, in subjectively representing the relationship between these two categories, an Italian Catholic may
employ any one of a number of different patterns. One possible pattern is to keep the two identities completely
separate cognitively, and to identify with only one of the categories at any one time according to context (e.g.
Italian when at work, but Catholic when in church). Another possible response is to allow one identity to dominate
and to regard the other identity as a subcategory within that dominating identity (e.g. to regard Catholics as being
just one subgroup within the larger group of Italians, others subgroups being Protestants, Muslims, Jews, etc.). A
third possible cognitive strategy is to regard one of the identities as being essential for membership of the other
category (e.g. to regard Catholics as being the only ‘real’ Italian people), while a fourth strategy might be to
regard one’s identity as being formed by the intersection of the two component identities (i.e. to identify oneself
specifically as an Italian Catholic, a category exhibiting its own unique emergent characteristics which differ from
those associated with being either just Italian or just Catholic on its own).
3.2
Hence, there are several ways in which the same set of multiple social identities can be represented
psychologically, and different individuals with similar sets of identifications may represent the relationships
between their component identities in very different ways from one another. Additional variability in patterns of
identification can arise between individuals because the meanings, symbolic contents, evaluations and feelings
which any individual attaches to a particular identity (such as their national or ethnic identity) are personalised as
a consequence of that individual’s own life history, experiences, personality and cognitive and affective biases
(Breakwell 2001). Hence, there may also be wide discrepancies between the subjective connotations which are
ascribed to the same identities by different individuals.
3.3
Further variability between individuals can also arise because there are different ways in which an individual can
identify with a social group (Leach et al. 2008; Roccas et al. 2008). One form of identification is in terms of how
central or important the social group is to one’s own self-definition and self-concept. A second form is less
cognitive and more affective in nature, being linked to the amount of pride, happiness and satisfaction which one
derives from being a member of the group. A third form of identification concerns the extent to which one
embraces and internalises the norms and values of the group, while a fourth form is more focused on
commitment to and solidarity with the group as indexed by one’s willingness to do things for the sake of other
group members. A fifth form of identification is in terms of the extent to which one views the social group as being
superior to other groups. Research has revealed that these various ways in which individuals may identify with
social groups are empirically distinguishable, so that one person can, for example, be high on group superiority
and on adherence to group values but low on commitment to other group members, whereas another person
may be high on commitment to other members of the group but low on superiority and adherence to group
values. These patterns represent very different ways in which to identify with the group. For example, in the case
of identification with a national group, the former individual would probably regard the defence of national values
as being one of the most crucial aspects of citizenship, while the latter might regard concern for the welfare of
fellow citizens as being of far greater importance. Both individuals may be highly identified with the nation, but the
cognitive, affective and behavioural consequences of their identifications are likely to be very different.
On the Context-dependent Nature of Identifications
4.1
A further complexity is that the salience of identifications varies as a function of context. Indeed, there is very
good evidence that a person’s multiple identifications are never all activated simultaneously; instead, the
subjective salience of particular identifications tends to fluctuate in a fluid and dynamic manner as a person
moves from one situation to another (Oakes, Haslam, and Turner 1994). These fluctuations are caused, in part,
3
by the available contrasts which are highlighted within each situation (e.g. as an English person in Greece, my
Englishness is made very salient to me; however, when I am teaching students at my university in England, my
identity as a teacher is instead rendered more salient). In other words, particular identifications often become
most salient to us when we are confronted with other people who differ from us in a way that is relevant to those
identifications.
4.2
Fluctuations in the way in which we think about our various identifications are also linked to the changes which
occur to our interests, needs, motivations, goals, and expectations as we move from one situation to another
(Turner and Onerato 1999). The work of Baumann (1996) is especially revealing in this regard. He draws a
conceptual distinction between ‘dominant’ and ‘demotic’ discourse about identity and culture. Dominant discourse
treats identity, culture, and community in a reified manner as if these are clearly identifiable entities which are
intrinsically linked to one another. This type of discourse enables simplistic judgements to be made about
whether someone is, for example, ‘truly’ English or Greek (i.e. because they participate in either English or Greek
culture, and belong to either the English or the Greek national community). Dominant discourse therefore permits
a straightforward equation between identity, culture, and community, and permits people to talk about ‘Greek
identity,’ ‘the Greek national community,’ and ‘Greek culture.’ By contrast, non-dominant or demotic discourse
treats identities, cultures, and communities as multifaceted, internally diverse, contested, constantly changing,
and subject to personal negotiation and choice. Identifications and culture are therefore regarded in demotic
discourse as dynamic processes through which meanings and the boundaries of groups and communities are
constantly being redefined and renegotiated by individuals according to their own current needs.
4.3
Baumann’s empirical work reveals how individuals often adopt both discourses about identity and culture in their
everyday lives. If, in a particular context, there is a strategic social or political advantage to be gained by
representing themselves and their group in reified terms (especially to an external audience), the goal being to
achieve particular benefits for themselves or for the group, they are likely to use the dominant discourse for this
purpose. However, in other contexts, exactly the same individuals may switch to a demotic discourse if it suits
their purpose for facilitating other goals (such as challenging group norms, or questioning the attribution of
characteristics to the group by individuals who are not group members). In other words, the way in which
individuals represent the social groups with which they identify shifts according to their own goals, needs and
motivations.
On Identity Threat
5.1
While the discussion up to this point has emphasised the complexity and context-dependency of identifications,
there are nevertheless also cases where particular identifications can become chronically salient to individuals,
dominating their perceptions across many different situations. These cases typically involve what has been called
‘identity threat’ (Branscombe, Ellemers, Spears and Doosje 1999). Identity threat can take many different forms,
including: being categorised by other people against one’s will and subjected to prejudice on that basis (e.g.
being judged solely on the basis of one’s race or ethnicity); when the group to which one belongs is directly
attacked either verbally or physically by members of another group (e.g. when members of a minority ethnic
group are insulted or physically assaulted); when external factors prevent a group to which one belongs from
becoming a distinct and meaningful category from other categories (e.g. when an indigenous minority national
group is denied political recognition or rights); when the value of a group to which one belongs is undermined in
some way (e.g. when a minority ethnic group has a low social status within a country); or when other members of
a group refuse to accept an individual as a full member of that group (e.g. when migrants or ethnic minority
individuals are denied full citizenship rights within a country).
5.2
It has been found that individuals engage in a variety of psychological responses to identity threat (Branscombe
et al. 1999). First, the identity which is the target of the threat can become chronically salient to the individual who
experiences the threat, remaining salient across all contexts and situations. Second, the social group which is
under threat may be perceived to exhibit greater internal cohesion and homogeneity with little internal variation
(i.e. the group comes to be perceived in more stereotypical terms). A third type of response which can occur is for
4
members of the threatened social group to engage in collective political or civic action to try to mitigate the
perceived threat. A fourth possible response is to engage in either strong ingroup favouritism or strong outgroup
denigration (i.e. to psychologically enhance the positive evaluation of the threatened social group to which one
belongs, or to enhance the negative evaluation of other groups to which one does not belong). Fifth, if the
individual under threat simultaneously has other identities deriving from membership of other higher status social
groups, then that individual may reduce the centrality or importance of the threatened group membership to the
self-concept, and instead enhance the centrality or importance of their membership of these higher status groups.
Which of these responses occurs can depend on the specific type of identity threat which is involved and on
whether the individual has a strong or a weak identification with the threatened group. For example, sometimes
low identifiers will simply disidentify still further with the threatened group, with only high identifiers exhibiting one
or more of the above responses.
5.3
Social identity threats which arise from the way in which other people categorize the self can be particularly
marked in the case of ethnic minority individuals, especially when the other-ascribed categorizations differ from
minority individuals’ own self-ascribed identifications. In some cases, minority individuals may strategically
choose to use the ascribed identity to engage in collective action aimed at ameliorating the social position of all
the people who fall within the invoked social group. For example, within the USA, people of Asian origin
commonly reject the use of the terms ‘Asian’ or ‘Asiatic’ to describe themselves, and instead tend to identify
subjectively as Chinese, Japanese, Korean, Vietnamese, etc. However, they may nevertheless unite behind an
Asian pan-ethnicity for political purposes in order to acquire greater effectiveness in challenging existing power
hierarchies and institutions (Espiritu 1992).
5.4
That said, discrepancies between the identities which are ascribed by other people vs. the subjective
identifications which minority individuals ascribe to themselves can sometimes be a significant source of stress,
impacting on minority individuals’ psychological well-being and sense of social inclusion. These outcomes may
also be influenced by whether there is congruence or incongruence between the way in which minority individuals
perceive the relationships between their own multiple identities and the way in which other people perceive those
relationships. For example, within the UK, there has been an intense debate in recent years about whether being
British and being Muslim are compatible identities. Many white British people currently deny that these are
compatible identities. By contrast, many British Muslims feel that these are fully compatible identities from which
no conflict in either values or duties arises; however, they feel that they are constantly being required to display
their loyalty towards Britain and being asked to choose between their Muslim and British identities. Hence, while
many white British people view Muslim people’s self-segregation and refusal to assimilate as a barrier to their
effective integration within British society, many Muslims themselves view the ‘policing’ of the boundary of the
British nation by the white British majority as the principal barrier to their inclusion (ETHNOS 2006).
On the Psychological Construction of Difference
6.1
An inevitable concomitant to the process of psychological identification is the parallel process of constructing
patterns of difference in order to differentiate the group with which one identifies at a psychological level from
other groups to which one does not belong (i.e. to differentiate the psychological ingroup from outgroups). Over
the years, there has been a substantial body of research into the way in which people construct boundaries
between categories. This research suggests that boundaries between categories are not positioned arbitrarily.
Instead, boundaries tend to be located on the basis of patterns of perceived similarities and differences (Tversky
1977; Rosch 1978; Oakes et al. 1994). In particular, the likelihood that a group of items will be placed into the
same category is driven by the extent to which the differences within that set of items are judged to be less than
the differences which exist between that set of items and other items. As the number of common features within
the set of items increases, and the number of distinctive features which differentiate those items from other items
also increases, the greater the likelihood that the set of items will be categorised together. Hence, category
formation is based on both characteristic features which tend to be shared by category items, and distinctive
features which tend to differentiate those items from other non-category items.
6.2
5
For this reason, insofar as human beings exhibit both characteristic and distinctive features, they are likely to be
perceived as falling into categories. However, it has also been found that perceptions of similarities and
differences are subject to social influence. For example, merely attaching verbal labels to items causes
individuals to attenuate the differences between items which have been given the same label, and to accentuate
the differences between items which have been given different labels (Tajfel and Wilkes 1963). Thus, simply
giving names to social groups can enhance the perception that there are differences between those groups and
reduce the perception of differences within the groups, amplifying the tendency to stereotype group members.
Other social and cultural factors can also contribute to the consolidation of these perceptions of similarity and
difference. For example, school education, by frequently purveying ethnocentrically biased representations of the
people who belong to different national groups, can further contribute to the entrenchment of the perceived
categorical boundaries between the people who belong to different national groups (Schleicher and Kozma 1992;
Barrett 2007).
6.3
In other words, social category formation and the construction of perceived boundaries between categories are
the product of a complex interplay between, on the one hand, patterns of human similarity and difference, and, on
the other hand, linguistic, social, and cultural representations and practices. I will return to the influence of social
and cultural factors on the psychological representation of ingroups and outgroups at a later point in this paper.
On the Psychological and Behavioural Consequences of Identification
7.1
Once social categories have been constructed at the psychological level and an individual categorises himself or
herself as falling within a particular category, the potential for the formation of a personal subjective identification
with that category is present. In this section, I want to focus on some of the potential consequences of identifying
with a category.
7.2
As has already been noted, identifying with a social category serves a cognitive function of helping an individual
to define and to locate themselves within the social world. However, social identifications can also be used to fulfil
other basic human needs. As Tajfel (1978; Tajfel and Turner 1986) argued, human beings have a fundamental
need to achieve a positive sense of themselves. For this reason, when a social group membership is internalized
as part of an individual’s self-concept (i.e. the individual subjectively identifies with the group), then that individual
needs to view that group in a positive manner. To this end, the ingroup may be compared with outgroups using
dimensions of comparison which produce more favourable representations of the ingroup than of the outgroups.
The positive distinctiveness which is then ascribed to the ingroup over the outgroups on these comparative
dimensions produces positive self-esteem. Thus, the process of social comparison can result in either ingroup
favouritism or outgroup denigration (or both). Hence, negative stereotyping and the devaluation of outgroups
which are perceived to be different from the ingroup can be a direct consequence of identification with the
ingroup coupled to the need to maintain high self-esteem.
7.3
However, as Tajfel and Turner (1986) also noted, these effects are by no means universal, and occur only under
certain conditions. For example, the social context within which the evaluation of ingroups and outgroups occurs
must allow the comparison with outgroups to be mad e on dimensions which have significant evaluative meaning.
Not all dimensions have such meaning. Furthermore, outgroups must be perceived to be relevant to the definition
of the ingroup. If an outgroup is irrelevant to the way in which the ingroup is perceived, then these effects also will
not occur. Consequently, ingroup favouritism and outgroup denigration will only be exhibited if there are at least
some meaningful dimensions for intergroup comparison which permit the ingroup to be viewed as being superior
to an outgroup and if that outgroup is relevant to the way in which the ingroup is perceived by the individual.
7.4
If these conditions do not prevail, then alternative strategies are required to achieve positive self-esteem (Tajfel
and Turner 1986). One possibility is for the individual to try to leave the group, that is, to engage in individual
social mobility. This might occur, for example, when an ethnic minority individual assimilates to the national
majority culture. However, this will not always be possible if the boundary between the social groups is, for
6
example, marked by race. An alternative strategy is to redefine the meanings which are associated with the
ingroup. This occurred, for example, in the USA in the 1960s and 1970s when the Black Power and Black
Consciousness movements generated a sense of racial pride among African-Americans in order to counteract
the negative images of blackness which were prevalent within America at the time. A third possible strategy is to
change the basis of the comparison, for example by changing the comparison outgroup against which the
ingroup is evaluated. Thus, members of a low status group may look for a group with an even lower status with
which to compare themselves, in order to boost their self-esteem. A fourth strategy is to challenge the social or
political status quo in an attempt to change the prevailing societal structure and the relative status of different
groups within that structure. This strategy is most likely to be used when the existing status differentials are
perceived to be illegitimate or when the high status of an outgroup is perceived to be unstable. Hence, the
particular identity strategy which is used depends on the societal structure which is in place, the relationships
which are perceived to exist between different groups within that structure, beliefs regarding the legitimacy and
stability of the structure, and perceptions of the permeability of group boundaries. It has also been found that
choice of strategy depends on the strength of identification with the ingroup. For example, low identifiers are
more likely to adopt an individual mobility strategy while high identifiers are more likely to try to challenge the
existing social structure (Branscombe and Ellemers 1998).
7.5
Of course, many of these factors (the relative status of ethnic groups, beliefs about the legitimacy and stability of
the societal structure, and perceptions of the permeability of group boundaries) vary considerably from one
country to another. For this reason, different national contexts are likely to result in minority individuals adopting
different identity strategies depending on the specific social realities characterising the position of their own ethnic
group within the particular national context in which they are living.
On Variability in the Development of National and Ethnic Identifications and
Attitudes
8.1
Studies into the way in which national and ethnic identifications and attitudes develop through the course of
childhood and adolescence provide a further useful window on the processes that underlie the psychological
construction and representation of ingroups and outgroups (Barrett 2007; Quintana and McKown 2008). Perhaps
not surprisingly, and consistent with the preceding discussion, these studies have revealed that national and
ethnic identifications and attitudes develop in very different ways depending on the specific national context in
which individuals live.
8.2
For example, in my own research on the development of national identifications and attitudes, we have worked
with children and adolescents living in England, Scotland, Catalonia, the Basque Country, Andalusia, Italy,
Russia, Ukraine, Georgia and Azerbaijan (Barrett, Riazanova, and Volovikova 2001; Barrett 2007). This research
has revealed that national identifications are often, although not always, established in children by the age of 6
years (the youngest age that we examined). In addition, there is no standard pattern in how children’s
representations of and attitudes to other national groups develop. Sometimes children’s attitudes become more
positive with increasing age, sometimes they become more negative with age, sometimes they initially become
more negative but then become more positive again, sometimes they become more positive but then become
more negative again, and sometimes they do not change at all as a function of age. Furthermore, among any one
group of children living in a particular location, different developmental profiles are exhibited depending on the
specific national outgroup which is involved. For example, among Russian children living in Moscow, we found
that attitudes towards Ukrainian people became more positive with increasing age, while attitudes towards
Georgian people did not show any changes with age; however, attitudes to American people initially became
more positive but later on became more negative once again. We discovered similar variability in developmental
profiles according to the particular national outgroups involved in the children living in all of the other locations as
well. A similar diversity of developmental patterns has been uncovered in other independent studies into the
development of children’s attitudes to ethnic and racial groups (Quintana and McKown 2008).
8.3
7
We also found variability in patterns of development depending on the specific national languages which were
spoken in the family home. For example, in the Basque Country, there were systematic differences in the
children’s attitudes to national groups depending on whether they came from families that spoke only Spanish,
only Basque or both Spanish and Basque in the family home. The children who came from the Spanish-speaking
and bilingual families were much more positive towards French, Italian, British and German people than the
children who came from the Basque-speaking families, while the children from the Basque-speaking and bilingual
families were much more positive towards Basque people than the children from the Spanish-speaking families
(Reizábal, Valencia, and Barrett 2004). Parallel findings were obtained in Catalonia (Vila, del Valle, Perera,
Monreal, and Barrett 1998).
8.4
We also found variability in the development of the children’s national attitudes depending on their language of
education. For example, we found systematic differences in the development of Ukrainian, Georgian and Azeri
children’s attitudes to national groups depending on whether they attended Russian-language schools or schools
which provided education in the national language (Barrett et al. 2001; Barrett 2007). For example, in Georgia,
attitudes to Russian people were much more positive among the children attending Russian-language schools,
while attitudes to Georgian people were much more positive among the children attending Georgian-language
schools.
On the Sociocultural Factors Which Influence the Development of National and
Ethnic Identifications and Attitudes
9.1
This variability which occurs in the development of children’s national and ethnic identifications and attitudes
stems from a number of different sources. First and foremost, parental attitudes, discourses and practices are a
major source of influence. For example, in the Basque Country and Catalonia, it is parents who decide which
languages are spoken in the family home, while in Ukraine, Georgia and Azerbaijan, it is also parents who decide
which type of school their children will attend. These decisions are typically made on the basis of parents’
national-ideological orientations, and these orientations are systematically linked to their discourse and practices
in relationship to nations within and beyond the family home (Elejabarrieta 1994; Valencia et al. 2003). It is these
parental behaviours (which are systematically linked to and indexed by the use of language) that are the actual
drivers of the national attitudes and representations of the children (rather than language use per se ). This
interpretation is fully consistent with many other studies which have also revealed that what parents say to, and
do with, their children does indeed impact on the national, ethnic, and racial identifications and attitudes which
are exhibited by their children (Hughes et al. 2006).
9.2
A second major source of influence is the school. First, the school curriculum can affect children’s national and
ethnic identifications and attitudes (Schiffauer, Baumann, Kastoryano, and Vertovec 2004; Barrett 2007). For
example, in Ukraine, Georgia and Azerbaijan, national language schools and Russian language schools follow
very different curricula, especially in relationship to the cultural and historical heritage of the nation. These
divergences in curriculum content help to consolidate the differential patterns of national attitudes which are
being transmitted to children by their parents (indeed, this is one of the principal reasons why parents make their
decision to send their children to a particular type of school). It is precisely because the school curriculum
provides a potential means of influencing children’s attitudes towards other groups that numerous anti-prejudice
programmes have now been developed for use within schools, although it does need to be added that these are
of varying levels of effectiveness in tackling entrenched patterns of prejudice (Pfeifer, Brown, and Juvonen 2007).
9.3
The contents of school textbooks are a further source of influence on children’s attitudes. For example, the
textbooks which are used in schools in different countries can sometimes express very different kinds of
narratives about the nation, and these national narratives are often appropriated and internalised by the children
who use them (Schiffauer et al. 2004). Furthermore, the pictures which are included in textbooks can also affect
children’s attitudes: for example, it was found in one study that children who read books in which the pictures
displayed people from a variety of ethnic backgrounds developed more positive attitudes towards outgroup
8
members than children who read books in which the pictures only displayed members of their own ingroup
(Litcher and Johnson 1969).
9.4
A third type of influence emanating from the school concerns the way in which teachers themselves behave in
the classroom and the kinds of explicit and implicit messages which they transmit to the children via their own
discourse and behaviour. In particular, the practices which teachers adopt in relationship to issues of cultural
difference, discrimination, and ethnic harassment in the classroom can have a significant effect on the attitudes of
the children they teach towards other national, ethnic, and racial groups (Kinket and Verkuyten 1999).
9.5
Beyond the school itself, the mass media constitute a further major source of influence on children’s attitudes to,
and representations of, other groups (Graves 1999). For example, in their classic study on the effects of
television on children, Himmelweit, Oppenheim, and Vince (1958) discovered that children who watch factual
television programmes about the people who live in other countries become less judgemental in their attitudes
towards people from other countries than children who do not watch these programmes. They become more
objective in their attitudes, with their representations of these peoples tending to reflect the ways in which they
have been represented in the television programmes.
9.6
A further notable factor which can influence children’s attitudes to people from other national, ethnic and racial
groups is direct personal contact with people who belong to these groups. Over fifty years ago, Allport (1954)
argued that personal contact can lead to the reduction of prejudice towards other groups. Subsequent research
(Brown and Hewstone 2005; Dovidio, Glick, and Rudman 2005; Pettigrew and Tropp 2006) has confirmed his
insights and revealed that there are several specific conditions which need to prevail for such contact to be
effective. Contact works best when the individuals who meet and interact are of roughly equal status (e.g. when
the individuals who are in contact are of the same age); when the group memberships of these individuals are
made salient and are emphasised within the contact situation, rather than underplayed; when the individuals
engage in co-operative rather than competitive activity; and when, in the course of interacting with each other,
they find things out about each other (e.g. about their divergent cultural practices).
On the Psychological Factors Which Influence the Development of National
and Ethnic Identifications and Attitudes
10.1
However, it is not only external sociocultural factors which influence the development of children’s national and
ethnic identifications and attitudes. Psychological factors can also play a role. For example, children’s cognitive
abilities have been implicated here, with several distinct cognitive abilities having been linked to changes in levels
of prejudice during childhood. These include cognitive flexibility (e.g. the ability to classify a set of people in
multiple ways rather than in just a single way), the ability to perceive internal differences within groups, the ability
to perceive deeper similarities between superficially different groups, and the ability to understand that different
people holding different opinions can both be correct when considered from their own point of view (Aboud and
Amato 2001). These cognitive skills are normally acquired between 6 and 12 years of age, and when reductions
in prejudice occur across this age range, these have sometimes been found to be related to the acquisition of
these abilities. However, as we have seen earlier, children do not always show a reduction in prejudice across
these years: sometimes their prejudice towards a particular outgroup may actually increase rather than decrease.
Furthermore, a child may exhibit a reduction in prejudice in relationship to one group but an increase in prejudice
towards another group. The fact that these opposite patterns are occurring in one and the same child reveals that
developing cognitive abilities cannot be the only influence impacting on the child’s attitudes.
10.2
Another psychological factor which we have discovered in our own work is sometimes linked to children’s
attitudes is their level of psychological identification with their own ingroup. For example, among Basque, Catalan
and Spanish children in particular, the higher their level of identification with their own national ingroup, the more
likely they are to evaluate that ingroup positively over and above their evaluations of other national groups
(Barrett 2007; Barrett, Lyons, and del Valle 2004; Reizábal et al. 2004). This finding is consistent with the idea
9
that the more one identifies with a particular social group, the more highly motivated one will be to evaluate that
ingroup positively over comparison outgroups in order to achieve positive self-esteem (Tajfel and Turner 1986).
However, these Spanish children were the only children in our studies to show such a strong and clear-cut
relationship between identification and attitudes. In other words, the potential effects of not only developing
cognitive abilities but also the strength of subjective identification seem to be either attenuated or swamped out
completely by other factors in other populations.
10.3
One such factor is perceived identity threat. In another research project, a group of colleagues and I have been
investigating how national and ethnic identifications and attitudes develop in children who are growing up in
national contexts which have recently experienced, or still are experiencing, intergroup conflict, violence or
warfare, including the Basque Country, Bosnia, North and South Cyprus, Northern Ireland and Israel
(Oppenheimer and Barrett, in press). This work has revealed that in such contexts, high levels of national or
ethnic identification are often coupled to high levels of negativity towards the enemy group from a very early age,
and these attitudes often do not moderate at all as children develop. This pattern is consistent with the notion
that, in these contexts, the acute sense of identity threat elicits such strong negative feelings towards the enemy
group that all other possible influences on the children’s development are swamped out. The result is that
prejudice against the enemy group is acquired early, does not moderate with age, and sometimes even
intensifies still further during adolescence (Cf. Bar-Tal and Teichman 2005).
10.4
One final important issue which also needs to be noted here is that children are not passive recipients of
environmental influences; they are instead highly active selectors of the information which is made available to
them (Bandura 1986; Durkin 1995). Out of all the information which is potentially available, they only attend to
some of it, and this is often the information which is consistent with their own existing beliefs and representations
or which fulfils their own current needs and motivations. Information which is incompatible with their beliefs,
motivations, or needs may just be screened out and ignored. Furthermore, when children do attend to
information, they often construct their own interpretations of that information. In other words, perceptual and
attentional processes, as well as interpretative and cognitive-representational processes, invariably also impact
on the uses which children make of the information which is made available to them within their environments.
Towards an Integrated Developmental Theory of the Factors Influencing
Psychological Representations of Identity and Difference
11.1
From the preceding discussion, it is clear that there are many factors which can potentially influence
psychological representations of identity and difference. It is also clear from this discussion that substantial
variability exists in the patterns of identifications and attitudes which people exhibit at the individual and the group
level, and across contexts. Hence, in developing an integrated developmental theory of the sociocultural and
psychological factors which drive people’s national and ethnic identifications and attitudes, it is crucial to pay
attention both to the full range of factors and to the existence of this individual, group and contextual variability. A
theory which attempts to do this is shown in Figure 1.
11.2
The starting point of this theory is that the individual always develops within a particular societal niche. This niche
is defined by a specific set of historical, geographical, economic and political circumstances which define the
relationships between the individual’s own nation and ethnic group and other nations and ethnic groups, including
the relative status of the ingroups in relationship to these outgroups.
11.3
The adult members of the societies in which children develop hold their own beliefs and attitudes concerning
these macro conditions, and they often adopt ideological positions in relationship to them. Their beliefs, attitudes
and values impact on their actions and discourse, and may even lead them to engage in political or social action
to try to change those macro circumstances. The most important adults, from the perspective of the developing
child, are parents, teachers and the individuals who produce materials for school curricula, school textbooks and
the mass media.
10
11.4
Parents have both direct and indirect effects on their children. First, what they say to and do with their children
impacts directly on their children’s representations and attitudes. However, parents also determine where the
family lives, where the family goes on holiday, and those with whom the child has kinship relations. All of these
factors can affect the quantity and quality of direct personal contact which the child has with members of other
national and ethnic groups. Parents also determine (or their economic circumstances determine) which school
the child attends. The school then determines the particular curriculum, textbooks, and teachers to which the
child is exposed. The school can also influence how much personal contact the child has with people from other
national and ethnic groups. Finally, according to their economic and cultural situation, parents buy products for
use in the family home, including televisions, books, CDs, DVDs, computers connected to the Internet, etc. And,
at least in the case of younger children, parents may also control their access to some of these products.
11.5
Hence, all of the following are potentially available to children as sources of information about other nations and
ethnic groups: parents’ discourse and practices, personal contact with members of those groups, the school
curriculum, school textbooks, teachers’ discourse and practices, and representations of nations and ethnic
groups in the mass media and in other literacy and visual resources. The representations of national and ethnic
groups which appear in these various information sources encode information about the historical, geographical,
economic, and political circumstances of the child’s own nation and ethnic group, and also often implicitly (and
sometimes explicitly) encode attitudes towards those ingroups and towards comparison outgroups.
11.6
However, all of these sources of information are only potentially available to the developing child. Which
information sources are actually attended to are influenced by the child’s own psychological processes as these
are deployed within any given situation, including perceptual and attentional processes, interpretative and
cognitive-representational processes, and affective and motivational processes (including the child’s levels of
identification with the nation and ethnic group to which he or she belongs, and the child’s perceptions of threat to
those groups). Thus, depending on these psychological processes, and depending on the particular sociocultural
contexts to which the child is exposed, different clusters of environmental factors may be rendered salient for that
child. In addition, the child determines his or her own level of engagement with the school curriculum, school
textbooks and media representationssimply providing information through these means is certainly no
guarantee that this information will actually be attended to by the child. Finally, the child’s own discourse and
actions may also be responded to by parents, teachers or members of other social groups, depending on the
setting, perhaps by challenging, questioning, or reinforcing the child’s behaviour, or by providing further
information to the child.
11.7
This theory suggests that different subsets of factors can be the primary drivers of identifications and attitudes in
different individuals, and in different groups of individuals, depending on those individuals’ own psychological
processes, the particular societal niches which they occupy, and the particular sociocultural settings which they
experience in their everyday lives. This theory therefore explains why there is considerable individual variability in
the identifications and attitudes which are displayed across groups, across individuals within groups, and across
different contexts for the same individual. Hence, it accommodates not only the multiplicity of different factors
which have been found to influence identifications and attitudes, but also the individual variability and the context-
dependency which have been found to characterise both identifications and attitudes.
Conclusions
12.1
In this paper, I have explored various aspects of national and ethnic identities from a psychological perspective,
including the relationship between subjective identification with a national or ethnic group and the cognitive
construction and representation of difference. It is clear from this body of work that identification processes are
extremely complex at the psychological level, with identifications and their concomitant representations and
attitudes being driven by a wide multiplicity of factors, both sociocultural and psychological.
12.2
11
The body of research which I have described in this paper may raise the hopes of those whose aim is to develop
interventions for ameliorating negative attitudes towards particular groups of people who are perceived to be
‘different.’ This is because, if negative attitudes are influenced by a range of sociocultural factors, the implication
is that by altering these factors which are external to the individual, one may be able to reverse the effects of the
previous experiences which led to the construction of the negative attitudes that are being targeted.
12.3
However, this body of research also suggests some significant and daunting challenges for devising suitable
interventions. This is because the sheer complexity of the patterns of exogenous influence, which are mediated
by a web of endogenous psychological processes, render the task of designing effective interventions extremely
difficult. This is due to the fact that the particular factors that will have been responsible for generating the
prejudices in the first place will almost certainly have varied from one society to another, from one setting to
another within a particular society, and from one individual to another within each setting. For this reason, there
may not be any individual methods for reducing prejudice that will be equally effective across all societal settings,
all social groups and all individuals. Instead, interventions may have to be tailored specifically to the particular
constellations of influences operating within particular settings for particular subgroups of individuals.
12.4
Challenging though this may be, the task of devising such interventions should not be shirked. The
consequences of intergroup hostility and conflict can be so devastating and far-reaching in the contemporary
world that there is an urgent moral imperative on social scientists to draw upon all of the evidence which is
currently available concerning how these forms of hostility and conflict originate in order to try to devise effective
interventions. By addressing this task with a clear sense of commitment, my own hope is that social scientists will
be able to contribute to the reduction of at least some of the entrenched antagonisms and destructive behaviours
which jeopardise this globalised world which we all share.
References
Aboud, F.E., and M. Amato. 2001. “Developmental and Socialization Influences on Intergroup Bias.” Blackwell
Handbook of Social Psychology: Intergroup Processes (eds. R. Brown and S.L. Gaertner) 6585. Oxford.
Allport, G.W. 1954. The Nature of Prejudice. Cambridge, MA.
Bandura, A. 1986. Social Foundations of Thought and Action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ.
Barrett, M. 2007. Children’s Knowledge, Beliefs and Feelings about Nations and National Groups. Hove.
Barrett, M., E. Lyons, and A. del Valle. 2004. “The Development of National Identity and Social Identity
Processes: Do Social Identity Theory and Self-categorization Theory Provide Useful Heuristic Frameworks for
Developmental Research?” The Development of the Social Self (eds. M. Bennett and F. Sani) 159188. Hove.
Barrett, M., T. Riazanova, and M. Volovikova, eds. 2001. Development of National, Ethnolinguistic and Religious
Identities in Children and Adolescents. Moscow.
Bar-Tal, D., and Y. Teichman. 2005. Stereotypes and Prejudice in Conflict: Representations of Arabs in Israeli
Jewish Society. Cambridge.
Baumann, G. 1996. Contesting Culture: Discourses of Identity in Multi-Ethnic London. Cambridge.
Branscombe, N.R., and N. Ellemers. 1998. “Coping with Group-based Discrimination: Individualistic versus
Group-level Strategies.” Prejudice: The Target’s Perspective (eds. J.K. Swim and C. Stangor) 243–266. New
York.
Branscombe, N.R., N. Ellemers, R. Spears, and B. Doosje. 1999. “The Context and Content of Social Identity
Threat.” Social Identity (eds. N. Ellemers, R. Spears, and B. Doosje) 35–58. Oxford.
12
Breakwell, G.M. 2001. “Social Representational Constraints upon Identity.” Representations of the Social (eds. K.
Deaux and G. Philogène) 271–284. Oxford.
Brown, R., and M. Hewstone. 2005. “An Integrative Theory of Intergroup Contact.” Advances in Experimental
Social Psychology 37:255343.
Deaux, K. 1992. “Personalizing Identity and Socializing Self.” Social Psychology of Identity and the Self Concept
(ed. G.M. Breakwell) 933. London.
———. 1996. “Social Identification.” Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (eds. T.E. Higgins and
A.W. Kruglanski) 777798. New York.
Dovidio, J.F., P. Glick, and L.A. Rudman, eds. 2005. On the Nature of Prejudice: Fifty Years after Allport. Oxford.
Durkin, K. 1995. Developmental Social Psychology. Oxford.
Elejabarrieta, F. 1994. “Social Positioning: A Way to Link Social Identity and Social Representations.” Social
Science Information 33:241253.
Espiritu, Y.L. 1992. Asian American Panethnicity: Bridging Institutions and Identities. Philadelphia.
ETHNOS Research and Consultancy. 2006. “The Decline of Britishness: A Research Study.” Paper
commissioned in 2005 by the Commission for Racial Equality (CRE). London.
Graves, S.B. 1999. “Television and Prejudice Reduction: When Does Television as a Vicarious Experience Make
a Difference?” Journal of Social Issues 55:707725.
Himmelweit, H.T., A.N. Oppenheim, and P. Vince. 1958. Television and the Child: An Empirical Study of the
Effect of Television on the Young. Oxford.
Hughes, D., J. Rodriguez, E.P. Smith, D.J. Johnson, H.C. Stevenson, and P. Spicer. 2006. “Parents’ Ethnic-
Racial Socialization Practices: A Review of Research and Directions for Future Study.” Developmental
Psychology 42:747770.
Kinket, B., and M. Verkuyten. 1999. “Intergroup Evaluations and Social Context: A Multilevel Approach.”
European Journal of Social Psychology 29:219237.
Leach, C.W., M. van Zomeren, S. Zebel, M.L.W. Vliek, S.F. Pennekamp, B. Doosje, J.W. Ouwerkerk, and R.
Spears. 2008. “Group-level Self-definition and Self-investment: A Hierarchical (Multicomponent) Model of In-
group Identification.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95:144165.
Litcher, J.H. and D.W. Johnson. 1969. “Changes in Attitudes toward Negroes of White Elementary School
Students after Use of Multiethnic Readers.” Journal of Educational Psychology 60:148–152.
Oakes, P.J., S.A. Haslam, and J.C. Turner. 1994. Stereotyping and Social Reality. Oxford.
Oppenheimer, L. and M. Barrett, eds. Forthcoming. “National Identity and Ingroup-Outgroup Attitudes in Children:
The role of Socio-historical Settings.” Special issue of European Journal of Developmental Psychology.
Pettigrew, T.F., and L.R. Tropp. 2006. “A Meta-analytic Test of Intergroup Contact Theory.” Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology 90:751–783.
Pfeifer, J.H., C.S. Brown, and J. Juvonen. 2007. Teaching Tolerance in Schools: Lessons Learned Since Brown
v. Board of Education about the Development and Reduction of Children’s Prejudice.” Social Policy Report 21.2.
Ann Arbor.
13
Quintana, S.M., and C. McKown, eds. 2008. The Handbook of Race, Racism and the Developing Child.
Hoboken, NJ.
Reizábal, L., J. Valencia, and M. Barrett. 2004. “National Identifications and Attitudes to National Ingroups and
Outgroups amongst Children Living in the Basque Country.” Infant and Child Development 13:120.
Roccas, S., and M.B. Brewer. 2002. “Social Identity Complexity.” Personality and Social Psychology Review
6:88106.
Roccas, S., L. Sagiv, S. Schwartz, N. Halevey, and R. Eidelson. 2008. “Toward a Unifying Model of Identification
with Groups: Integrating Theoretical Perspectives.” Personality and Social Psychology Review 12:280306.
Rosch, E. 1978. “Principles of Categorization.” Cognition and Categorization (eds. E. Rosch and B. Lloyd) 2748.
Hillsdale, NJ.
Schiffauer, W., G. Baumann, R. Kastoryano, and S. Vertovec, eds. 2004. Civil Enculturation: Nation-State,
School and Ethnic Difference in The Netherlands, Britain, Germany and France. New York.
Schleicher, K., and T. Kozma, eds. 1992. Ethnocentrism in Education. Frankfurt.
Stryker, S. 1980. Symbolic Interactionism: A Social Structural Version. Menlo Park, CA.
Tajfel, H. 1978. “Social Categorization, Social Identity and Social Comparison.” Differentiation between Social
Groups: Studies in the Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations (ed. H. Tajfel) 6176. London.
Tajfel, H., and J.C. Turner. 1986. “The Social Identity Theory of Intergroup Behaviour.” Psychology of Intergroup
Relations (eds. S. Worchel and W.G. Austin) 724. Chicago.
Tajfel, H., and A.L. Wilkes. 1963. “Classification and Quantitative Judgment.” British Journal of Psychology
54:101114.
Turner, J.C., and R.S. Onorato. 1999. “Social Identity, Personality, and the Self-concept: A Self-Categorizing
Perspective.” The Psychology of the Social Self (eds. T.R. Tyler, R.M. Kramer, and O.P. John) 11–46. Mahwah,
NJ.
Tversky, A. 1977. “Features of Similarity.” Psychological Review 84:327352.
Valencia, J., F. Elejabarrieta, S. Perera, L. Reizábal, M. Barrett, I. Vila, L. Gil de Montes, G. Ortiz, and M.
Larrañaga. 2003. “Conflictual National Identities and Linguistic Strategies as Positioning Tools in Children and
Adolescents.” Les Représentations Socials: Constructions Nouvelles (eds. M. Lavallée, S. Vincent, C. Ouellet,
and C. Garnier) 1738. Montréal.
Vila, I., A. del Valle, S. Perera, P. Monreal, and M. Barrett. 1998. “Autocategorización, identidad nacional y
contexto lingüístico.” Estudios de Psicología 60:314.
14
Historical,
geographical,
economic and
political
circumstances
of the
individuals own
nation and
ethnic group,
including the
situation of that
nation and
ethnic group in
relationship to
other nations
and ethnic
groups
Beliefs,
attitudes, values
and practices of
other societal
members
(including those
of parents,
teachers and
the producers of
school curricula,
textbooks,
media texts,
internet texts
and other
literacy and
visual
resources)
Parental discourse
and practices
Parental choice
of school
Parental
choice of place of
abode, family
holidays, kinship
relations
Parental control
of access to the
mass media and
the Internet, and
purchase of home
literacy and visual
resources
Figure 1: An integrated developmental theory of the factors influencing psychological
representations of identity and difference (adapted from Barrett, 2007)
Direct personal
contact with
national and
ethnic others
and with foreign
places
School curriculum
and school
textbooks to
which the child is
exposed
Teacher
discourse and
practices to which
the child is
exposed
Representations
of nations and
ethnic groups in
the mass media,
the internet and
other literacy and
visual resources
to which the child
is exposed
The childs
perceptual
and
attentional
processes
The childs
interpretative
and cognitive-
representational
processes
The childs
affective and
motivational
processes,
including
levels of
national and
ethnic
identification
The childs
discourse and
actions
... If a woman would describe and understand herself as a mother and wife two centuries ago, today, apart from her role in the family, she describes herself as a doctor, manager, feminist, member of an ecological association, leftist, vegetarian, etc. These different identifications help us to identify, position and orientate ourselves in society in relation to other people (Barrett 2010). i Of course, not all identities have (the same) significance for defining ourselves. ...
... The basic assumption among the authors who support this dichotomy was that the strong attachment to the (ethnic) group leads to negative evaluation of those outside the group, caused by intolerance and non-existence of readiness to accept the Others (those outside the group) (Hesli and Hansen 2009;Barrett 2010). Some authors emphasize that such negative evaluation of the Others is frequently accompanied by favoring or extremely uncritical relation towards the members of one's own group (L. ...
... The main assumptions are that the groups which show a larger degree of connection to their religion will more strongly develop their feeling of belonging to their ethnic group (Hesli and Hansen, 2009). Apart from the religious, the linguistic heterogeneity is also considered to be a significant factor of cultural diversity of the group, so many authors connect the language, as a significant means of socialization, with ethno-nationalism (Rosenblatt 1964;Rahman 1997;Fournier 2002in Barrett 2010. In this sense, Hesli and Hansen (2009) in their research start from the assumption that those who use their language more are more prone to develop ethnic rather than civic identity. ...
Article
Full-text available
The research of the ethnic and national identity in a multicultural society such as the Macedonian one is relevant in reference to the question how those who identify with their ethnic group position themselves towards the broader political community. Does the strong attachment towards one's own ethnic group, especially when minority groups are concerned, means absence of attachment towards and identification with the state, as many authors who study social identities show? Taking into consideration the Macedonian social context, we have set off from the assumption that it was possible for these two identities to be mutually exclusive, but also compatible. In a survey of student population in the R. Macedonia, four categories that express the relation of ethnic versus national identity have been offered. The results proved the general hypothesis– namely, the minority effect-according to which the minority group gives greater significance to the characteristics of their own minority belonging. However, the largest majority of ethnic Albanians chose modalities that indicate compatibility of the two researched identities, even in cases when they emphasize their ethnic background. This could be an encouraging indicator in a multicultural and divided society, but still the question remains what almost half of the examined population of ethnic Albanians mean when they choose the categories that contain both ethnic and civic traits. Some of the results which refer to the attachments to different categories and the presence of the participative component, partly provide an answer to this question.
... Tema ini menjelaskan bahwa komunikasi lintas budaya membutuhkan seorang mediator sebagai pihak yang dapat berkomunikasi kepada pihak-pihak yang bertikai secara adil. Konflik antar etnis dapat (Komunikasi Lintas Budaya …) terjadi karena sudah tertanamnya secara kuat konsep diri terhadap budaya tertentu sehingga terjadi resistensi terhadap kehadiran budaya dari etnis tertentu dan bahkan terjadi perlawanan (Barrett, 2010). Keterbatasan kemampuan dari para pihak yang terlibat pada suatu konflik pada akhirnya mengharuskan mereka untuk mencari jalan tengah agar tidak terjadi kerusakan yang berkelanjutan. ...
Article
Komunikasi yang berbeda dalam keberagaman etnis menjadi salah satu pemicu terjadinya konflik sosial di Indonesia sehingga dapat mengancam perdamaian tanah air jika dibiarkan begitu saja. Komunikasi lintas budaya merupakan salah satu pendekatan yang dapat digunakan untuk membangun kedamaian dari masyarakat dengan latar belakang budaya yang beragam. Penelitian ini bertujuan untuk mengeksplorasi bentuk komunikasi lintas budaya yang telah dilakukan di Indonesia untuk meredam konflik antar etnis. Metode yang digunakan dalam penelitian ini adalah tinjauan literatur dengan pendekatan kualitatif dimana terdapat 6 literatur ilmiah yang digunakan sebagai data penelitian. Hasil penelitian menyebutkan bahwa setidaknya terdapat tiga strategi yang dapat dilakukan pada komunikasi lintas budaya untuk meredam konflik di Indonesia yakni melibatkan pimpinan tokoh adat sebagai komunikator, menggelar forum lintas budaya dan melibatkan pihak yang netral sebagai mediator. Melalui tema tersebut, terdapat tiga aspek penting yang muncul dalam komunikasi lintas budaya dalam konteks konflik etnis di Indonesia yakni kepemimpinan, media untuk bertemu serta organisasi sosial. Ketiga aspek tersebut dapat menjadi kerangka kerja komunikasi lintas budaya untuk menganalisis penyelesaian konflik antar etnis di Indonesia.
... Многие проблемы современности, как отмечают исследователи, обусловлены проблемами идентичности [3]. Человек переживает в современной культуре не просто кризис, влекущий за собой определенные трансформации и способствующий смене параметров самосознания и идентификации себя с кем-то, но кризис сущности идентичности, когда у человека отсутствует доверие к тому социальному контексту, частью которого он является. ...
... Its ideological power lies in its implicit collectivistic call for group solidarity (Collins, 1992 according to Malesevic, 2006: 10). The variations that arise between individuals as a result of the different ways in which an individual identifies with a social group (Leach et al., 2008; Roccas et al., 2008, in Barrett, 2010) are empirically reachable. One person can rank high in solidarity and readiness for joint action with other members of the group, but low in terms of sharing the values of the group and vice versa. ...
Article
Full-text available
In multicultural societies as the Macedonian one, the attachment of citizens to particular identity traits is important for the democratic stability and peace. The aim of this paper is to find out how students from different ethnic origins in the R. Macedonia relate to their identity traits, especially to their national and ethnic identities, and relative to other identity traits. The basic assumptions are based on a phenomenon called "minority effect", according to which members of minority groups tend to attach greater importance to minority affiliations that are particularly important for their group identity (language, religion, ethnicity, tradition, etc.). Aside from importance of identity traits, the emotional and behavioural components of these attachments were also examined. The research results show that regardless of the ethnic origin, students attach greatest importance to their identities connected to their immediate social environment (family, friends), but also religion. In accordance with the “minority effect” hypothesis, religion and then ethnicity, are perceived by the ethnic Albanians as a strong cohesive and mobilising factor, whereas that is not the case with the ethnic Macedonians. When it comes to nationality, the responses suggest that for ethnic Albanians it has marginal importance (through the cognitive, emotional and the action component), while ethnic Macedonians show controversial relation to their national identity.
Article
Full-text available
The study of the use of language as a system of social integration vs. differentiation in contexts of intergroup conflict has been underdeveloped in social psychology. Usually the use of language has been considered as a system of expression of "ethnic identity ". This perspective has, at least, three problems: 1) language has been taken as a direct expression of an identity, fixed and taken for granted. 2) language has been considered as a simplistic conception of the processes of identification, without taking into account the complexity of the social psychological factors that characterise "ethnic" intergroup conflicts. 3) an individualistic view of language and of the social conditions in which it circulates has been given to the speaker's position. The coexistence of two languages in socio-political contexts where nationalism plays an important role means that because of the symbolic nature of conflicts, non-univocal strategies of social differentiation are developed. The aim of the present study is to analyse such a process of integration or differentiation within the framework of the Theory of Social Representations (TSR), more specifically with the use of the three-phase model of Doise et al (1993), where SR are considered as organising principles of symbolic relationships between individuals and groups. In this sense, we first analysed the shared conceptions of self-categorisations and identities related to the in-group (Region) and the out-group (Nation). This study also aims to analyse how the use of language interacts with the origin of the parents of children in order to develop 18 strategies of identification. Those strategies of identification are understood as a tactical positioning that allow actors to express their positions towards a conflict of objectified realities, socially defined as "ethnic". Three main strategies of identification are possible: assimilation, integration and separation (Berry 1990). Summing up, these strategies of identification seem to be interesting tactical positioning tools (Elejabarrieta 1994) that allow young social actors to express their positions towards the conflict between regions with high levels of self-consciousness of "being a nation" and nations. While Assimilation strategies lead to the actors to refuse in-group members and ignore them, Separation ones drive children and adolescents towards both the recognition of the in-group and the awareness of them, whereas Integration strategies lead to the actors to recognise both in-group and out-group in evaluation and information.
Article
Full-text available
In this short introduction, the background, rationale, and hypotheses are presented for the studies that are reported in the special issue, as well as the order in which the studies are presented.
Book
Filling a critical void in the literature, Race, Racism, and the Developing Child provides an important source of information for researchers, psychologists, and students on the recent advances in the unique developmental and social features of race and racism in children's lives. Thorough and accessible, this timely reference draws on an international collection of experts and scholars representing the breadth of perspectives, theoretical traditions, and empirical approaches in this field.
Article
In this article, we introduce the concept of social identity complexity—a new theoretical construct that refers to an individual's subjective representation of the interrelationships among his or her multiple group identities. Social identity complexity reflects the degree of overlap perceived to exist between groups of which a person is simultaneously a member When the overlap of multiple ingroups is perceived to be high, the individual maintains a relatively simplified identity structure whereby memberships in different groups converge to form a single ingroup identification. When a person acknowledges, and accepts, that memberships in multiple ingroups are not fully convergent or overlapping, the associated identity structure is both more inclusive and more complex. In this article, we define the concept of social identity complexity and discuss its possible antecedents and consequences. Results from initial studies support the prediction that social identity complexity is affected by stress and is related to personal value priorities and to tolerance of outgroup members.