ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

In recent years, research on interoceptive abilities (i.e., sensibility, accuracy, and awareness) and their associations with emotional experience has flourished. Yet interoceptive abilities in alexithymia—a personality trait characterized by a difficulty in the cognitive interpretation of emotional arousal, which impacts emotional experience—remain under-investigated, thereby limiting a full understanding of subjective emotional experience processing. Research has proposed two contrasting explanations thus far: in one model, the dimensions of interoceptive sensibility and accuracy in alexithymia would increase; in the other model, they would decrease. Surprisingly, the contribution of interoceptive awareness has been minimally researched. In this study ( N = 182), the relationship between participants’ level of alexithymia and the three interoceptive dimensions was tested. Our results show that the higher the level of alexithymia is, the higher interoceptive accuracy and sensibility ( R ² = 0.29 and R ² = 0.14); conversely, the higher the level of alexithymia is, the lower interoceptive awareness ( R ² = 0.36). Moreover, an ROC analysis reveals that interoceptive awareness is the most accurate predictor of alexithymia, yielding over 92% accuracy. Collectively, these results support a coherent understanding of interoceptive abilities in alexithymia, whereby the dissociation of interoceptive accuracy and awareness may explain the underlying psycho-physiological mechanisms of alexithymia. A possible neurocognitive mechanism is discussed which suggests insurgence of psychosomatic disorders in alexithymia and related psychotherapeutic approaches.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Vol:.(1234567890)
Psychological Research (2022) 86:844–857
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01538-x
1 3
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Disentangling interoceptive abilities inalexithymia
CristinaScarpazza1 · AndreaZangrossi2,3· Yu‑ChunHuang4,5· GiuseppeSartori1· SebastianoMassaro4,6
Received: 23 September 2020 / Accepted: 25 May 2021 / Published online: 7 June 2021
© The Author(s) 2021
Abstract
In recent years, research on interoceptive abilities (i.e., sensibility, accuracy, and awareness) and their associations with emo-
tional experience has flourished. Yet interoceptive abilities in alexithymia—a personality trait characterized by a difficulty in
the cognitive interpretation of emotional arousal, which impacts emotional experience—remain under-investigated, thereby
limiting a full understanding of subjective emotional experience processing. Research has proposed two contrasting explana-
tions thus far: in one model, the dimensions of interoceptive sensibility and accuracy in alexithymia would increase; in the
other model, they would decrease. Surprisingly, the contribution of interoceptive awareness has been minimally researched.
In this study (N = 182), the relationship between participants’ level of alexithymia and the three interoceptive dimensions
was tested. Our results show that the higher the level of alexithymia is, the higher interoceptive accuracy and sensibility
(R2 = 0.29 and R2 = 0.14); conversely, the higher the level of alexithymia is, the lower interoceptive awareness (R2 = 0.36).
Moreover, an ROC analysis reveals that interoceptive awareness is the most accurate predictor of alexithymia, yielding over
92% accuracy. Collectively, these results support a coherent understanding of interoceptive abilities in alexithymia, whereby
the dissociation of interoceptive accuracy and awareness may explain the underlying psycho-physiological mechanisms of
alexithymia. A possible neurocognitive mechanism is discussed which suggests insurgence of psychosomatic disorders in
alexithymia and related psychotherapeutic approaches.
“If the sheep eats the flower, it is
for him as if, all of a sudden, all
the stars went dark”.
The Little Prince
Introduction
‘Interoception’ refers to the conscious perception and rec-
ognition of a wide range of physical internal states (Barrett
& Simmons, 2015). The capacities to interocept, or intero-
ceptive abilities, are central to the peripheral theories of
emotions, which hold that emotions are perceived as central
representations dependent on automatic bodily responses
(Fehr & Stern, 1970). It follows that one’s ability to perceive
more or less intensively their visceral responses, influences
the strengths of the emotional experience (Critchley & Gar-
finkel, 2017; Garfinkel & Critchley, 2013): a high degree
of interoception is supposed to reflect intense emotional
experience and integration of bodily signals into emotional
experience.
This understanding is particularly relevant for research
on alexithymia, a personality trait which is characterized by
a deficit in the cognitive interpretation of emotional arousal
(Lopez-Munoz & Perez-Fernandez, 2019; Taylor etal.,
2016), and therefore, impacts emotional experience. Origi-
nally defined as “absence of words for feelings” (Sifneos,
1973), this trait refers to a phenomenon characterized by
difficulty in identifying one’s own and others’ feelings—in
* Cristina Scarpazza
cristina.scarpazza@unipd.it
1 Department ofGeneral Psychology, University ofPadua, Via
Venezia 8, 35131Padua, PD, Italy
2 Department ofNeuroscience, University ofPadua, Via
Giustiniani, 5, 35128Padua, Italy
3 Padova Neuroscience Center (PCN), University ofPadua,
Via Giustiniani, 5, 35128Padua, Italy
4 The Organizational Neuroscience Laboratory, 27 Old
Gloucester Street, LondonWC1N3AX, UK
5 Department ofPsychology, University ofWarwick,
University Road, CoventryCV47AL, UK
6 Surrey Business School, University ofSurrey, Guildford, Rik
Medlik Building (MS), GuildfordGU27XH, UK
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
845Psychological Research (2022) 86:844–857
1 3
particular, negative emotions (Scarpazza etal., 2018;
Sifneos, 1973; Starita etal., 2018; Taylor etal., 1991)—,
difficulty in processing emotions (Nam etal., 2020), flat-
tened affect and emotional unawareness (Sifneos, 1973; Tay-
lor etal., 1991), low empathy (Alkan Hartwig etal., 2020;
Moriguchi etal., 2007; Valdespino etal., 2017) and difficul-
ties in social cognition (Di Tella etal., 2020; Lane etal.,
2015; Moriguchi etal., 2006; Ospina etal., 2019; Scarpazza
& Di Pellegrino, 2018). In other words, alexithymia would
have prevented the Little Prince to express his emotions so
vividly.
Research on alexithymia has blossomed in the past few
years. For instance, the neurobiological underpinnings of
alexithymia have been recently described as encompassing
a complex neural network involving the insula, anterior cin-
gulated cortex, amygdale, and prefrontal cortices, among
other brain areas (Meza-Concha etal., 2017; van der Velde
etal., 2013). Moreover, research has related alexithymia to
multi-faceted difficulties in emotional processing, ranging
from difficulties in recognizing emotions expressed by oth-
ers (Grynberg etal., 2012; Scarpazza etal., 2014, 2015,
2018; Starita etal., 2018) to deficit in regulating emotional
responses (Pollatos & Gramann, 2012; Swart etal., 2009).
Furthermore, individuals with high alexithymia levels are
defective in their ability to use emotions to guide their deci-
sion making (Scarpazza etal., 2017; Starita etal., 2019).
Thus, knowing more on the sub-clinical emotional pro-
cessing impairment of individuals with high levels of alex-
ithymia can offer a valuable opportunity to better evaluate
the contribution of interoception to the conscious experience
of emotions. Indeed, alexithymia is frequently acknowledged
as “a marker of atypical interoception” (Murphy, etal., 2018;
Murphy, etal., 2018): if individuals perceive and interpret
their bodily sensations abnormally, they will also find chal-
lenging to identify and describe their feelings, as well as to
regulate them when necessary.
While still embryonic, research has begun to focus on
the cognitive underpinnings of alexithymia, with interocep-
tion emerging as the leading candidate (Murphy etal., 2018;
Murphy etal., 2018; Nicholson etal., 2018; Scarpazza etal.,
2015; Shah etal., 2016; Shah etal., 2016; Trevisan etal.,
2019). Different theoretical models of interoception have
been proposed in the literature thus far (Garfinkel & Critch-
ley, 2013; Murphy etal., 2019), although, due to its com-
plexity, it is a construct undergoing frequent refinement in
both its conceptualization and operationalization (Trevisan
etal., 2020). According with one popular model (Garfinkel
& Critchley, 2013), interoception is a three-dimensional con-
struct, in which each dimension reflects a different level of
bodily signals elaboration. On a lower level, interoceptive
sensibility (ISb) reflects a dispositional tendency to be inter-
nally focused (i.e., attention toward inner bodily signals);
on a middle level, interoceptive accuracy (IAcc) refers to
the objective accuracy in detecting internal bodily sensa-
tions; and on a higher level, interoceptive awareness (IAw)
represents the meta-cognitive awareness of IAcc (Garfinkel
& Critchley, 2013). Here, we build on this model of intero-
ception, because the literature on alexithymia has primarily
focused on IAcc and ISb thus far (see Online Supplementary
Material) and left other interoceptive components advanced
by more recent models (Murphy etal., 2019) still virtually
unexplored in alexithymia. One of these components is for
instance the dimension of attention—defined as the objec-
tively measured attention to interoceptive signals measured
with experience sampling methods (Murphy etal., 2019).
The involvement of IAcc and ISb in alexithymia has
been explained by two competing hypotheses. One hypoth-
esis suggests that individuals with high level of alexithy-
mia—who lack of the ability to cognitively interpret bodily
changes, with clear implication on their subjective emo-
tional experience—are defective in interoceptive abilities
(Bornemann & Singer, 2017; Brewer etal., 2016; Muir etal.,
2017). According to this hypothesis, the difficulties mani-
fested by alexithymic individuals to correctly identify their
own emotions could be explained by a deficit in the accurate
detection and identification of bodily changes, namely IAcc
(Garfinkel & Critchley, 2013). This line of research also sug-
gests a possible explanation for the higher occurrence of
alexithymia in subjects presenting clinical disorders associ-
ated with poor interoception. For example, alexithymia is
highly prevalent in individuals suffering from eating dis-
orders, which are characterized by decreased interoceptive
abilities related to reduced perception of hunger and satiety
(Brewer etal., 2016). However, it is also worth to note that
this hypothesis is difficult to reconcile with observations of
increased prevalence of alexithymia in patients with psycho-
somatic disorders (Taylor, 2000; Taylor etal., 2016), whose
attention is prevalently allocated on internal bodily signals.
A second hypothesis proposes that alexithymic individu-
als are characterized by heightened interoceptive abilities
(Ernst etal., 2014; Longarzo etal., 2015; Scarpazza etal.,
2015, 2017). This argument follows the “somatosensory
amplification hypothesis” of alexithymia, which maintains
that alexithymia is characterized by a perceived amplifi-
cation of the normal visceral phenomena (Wise & Mann,
1994). This line of research appears to be prima facie incon-
sistent with previous studies reporting inter-dependence
between subjective emotional experience and IAcc. At
the same time, the precise contribution of IAcc and physi-
ological responses to the conscious experience of emotion
remains somewhat controversial (Lane & Schwartz, 1987;
Scarpazza etal., 2015). The core proposition of this second
hypothesis is that IAcc could be necessary but might not be
sufficient for the conscious experience of emotion to arise.
Indeed, emotional awareness can be graded in different lev-
els, and accuracy in detecting bodily sensation are graded
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
846 Psychological Research (2022) 86:844–857
1 3
in the lower level (Lane & Schwartz, 1987). Although
being able to detect their own visceral changes, alexithymic
subjects may fail to link these signals to higher levels of
emotional processing. Thus, this second line of research
would not contradict, rather expand the current literature
(Critchley & Garfinkel, 2017), suggesting the dissociation
between interoceptive accuracy and emotional experience
in alexithymia.
Altogether investigations of IAcc and ISb in alexithymia
have provided contrasting results (see Online Supplemen-
tary Material for an overview of the literature). With such a
mixed evidence supporting both perspectives, a recent meta-
analysis has revealed a non-significant relationship between
IAcc and alexithymia in a typically developing sample (Tre-
visan etal., 2019). Yet, to the best of our current knowledge
(see also Online Supplementary Material for an overview of
the literature), research regarding the association between
the degree of interoceptive awareness and alexithymia is
still missing (Scarpazza & Di Pellegrino, 2018). This is a
rather surprising instance given that alexithymia represents
a clear deficit in the cognitive interpretations of emotional
arousal. Seeking to address this gap is the core aim of this
study, which contributes to psychological theory by identi-
fying the distinctive role that interoceptive abilities play in
the (defective) processing of emotional experience, and to
practice by putting forward clinical implications for address-
ing psychotherapeutic approaches for alexithymia. Thus, we
hypothesize that individuals with a high level of alexithymia
will be characterized by a decreased interoceptive aware-
ness. In other words, they would lack self-confidence in their
interpretation of bodily signals. This reasoning could thus
explain why alexithymic individuals, despite being highly
focused on bodily signals (Wise & Mann, 1994), are more
prone to manifest disorders characterized by decreased inter-
oception (Brewer etal., 2016).
Materials andmethods
Participants
A priori power calculations using G*Power3 revealed that
a sample of at least 161 participants is required to detect
an association between interoception and alexithymia of
r = 0.30 (Herbert etal., 2011), with a power of 0.99 using
two-tailed tests. Participants were normal, healthy volun-
teers who replied to an online advertisement. Participants
were included if they (i) declared they had never been diag-
nosed with any neurological or psychiatric disorder; (ii) were
able to provide written informed consent; (iii) had a normal
bodyweight; (iv) were proficient in both oral and written
English.
A total of 193 participants, all with normal body-
weights (i.e., not obese), were recruited from the student
and alumni population of our Universities, both in classes
and through the SONA online recruitment system (https://
www. sona- syste ms. com/). Participants were tested indi-
vidually. Eleven participants were excluded because they
reported feeling their pulse in the fingertip during the
interoceptive accuracy task (see below). A total of 182
healthy participants (female = 95; age = 23.76; SD = 3.76)
were retained for analysis. Data regarding age, gender,
educational level and obesity (yes/no) were collected for
control.
Ethical approval was received from the Ethical Review
Board of the Warwick Business School (UK). The research
was performed according to the principles of the Declara-
tion of Helsinki.
Variables andmeasures
Alexithymia
Participants completed the validated and widely used
20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) (Parker
etal., 2003), which allowed us to evaluate their individual
levels of alexithymia. The TAS-20 is a three-dimensional
self-reported questionnaire, which measures three aspects
of the alexithymia construct: difficulty in describing feel-
ings (DDF), difficulty in identifying feelings (DIF), and
externally oriented thinking (EOT). This self-report instru-
ment has been demonstrated to have good psychometric
properties: internal consistency Cronbach alfa = 0.81;
test–retest reliability r = 0.86 (Bressi etal., 1996). In keep-
ing with the current literature (Bagby etal., 2020), TAS-20
was used as a continuous variable in the regression mod-
els (see “Results” below). Moreover, for the ROC analy-
ses (see “Results” below), following previous works that
framed alexithymia as a binary status (Taylor etal., 2003),
subjects were also classified as having high levels of alex-
ithymia (HA) if their TAS-20 scores were 61 (N = 22),
whereas they were classified as having low-level alexithy-
mia (LA) if their TAS-20 scores were 60 (N = 160).
The Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) (Beck
etal., 1961) was also administered, given that alexithy-
mia has been strongly associated with depressive symp-
toms (Allen etal., 2011; Hintikka etal., 2001; Honkalampi
etal., 2000; Li etal., 2015), and their co-occurrence might
confound results. This instrument has demonstrated good
psychometric properties (internal consistency Cronbach
alfa = 0.91; test–retest reliability r = 0.93; (Beck et al.,
1996, Beck etal., 1996).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
847Psychological Research (2022) 86:844–857
1 3
Interoceptive sensibility
In keeping with previous research (Ernst etal., 2014; Pal-
ser etal., 2018; Pearson & Pfeifer, 2020; Scarpazza etal.,
2015), the Body Perception Questionnaire (Porges, 1993)
was adopted as a measure of Interoceptive Sensibility (ISb).
This is a widely acknowledged self-reported questionnaire
(see (Critchley, 2004; Garfinkel etal., 2015; Murphy etal.,
2019) that measures one’s dispositional tendency to be
internally focused and holds “high reliability and validity
compared with other scales” (Ainley & Tsakiris, 2013). In
the BPQ, participants were asked to indicate on a five-point
Likert scale ranging from 1 (“never”) to 5 (“always”) their
cognizance of bodily sensations and autonomic nervous
system reactivity. The higher the score is, the stronger the
participant’s subjective perception of bodily sensations and
interoceptive sensibility.
Interoceptive accuracy
To allow a coherent comparison with the extant litera-
ture (Bekrater-Bodmann etal., 2020; Herbert etal., 2011;
Nicholson etal., 2018; Scarpazza etal., 2015, 2017; Shah
etal., 2016; Ueno etal., 2020), we used the heartbeat per-
ception task—HBP task—(Schandry, 1981) to estimate
Interoceptive Accuracy (IAcc) (Garfinkel & Critchley, 2013)
(see “Discussion” below for benefits and limitations of this
task).
During this task, participants were asked to count their
heartbeats silently by focusing on their heart activity, while
actual heartbeat signals were simultaneously acquired using
a wireless finger pulse oximeter (DigiO2 International Co.)
(Shah etal., 2016). Participants were not allowed to take
their pulse or attempt any other physical manipulation that
could facilitate the heartbeat count. Furthermore, partici-
pants were instructed not to guess if they could not feel their
heartbeat. This task was repeated three times to form three
trials, using time-windows of 30, 45, and 60s, presented in
randomized order. IAcc was calculated by taking the mean
score across the three heartbeat perception intervals accord-
ing to the following transformation: 1/3 ∑ (1−(|recorded
heartbeats – counted heartbeats|)/recorded heartbeats)
(Schandry, 1981; Schuette etal., 2020). The resulting score,
also called heartbeat perception index or interoceptive accu-
racy index, was calculated following previous works that
adopted this task (Herbert etal., 2011; Schandry, 1981;
Schuette etal., 2020; Shah etal., 2016); the score varies
between 0 and 1, where 1 indicates the highest accuracy
(Schandry, 1981; Schuette etal., 2020). After completion,
participants were debriefed on the eventual adoption of
exteroceptive strategies: they were explicitly asked whether
they were able to feel their pulse in the fingertip where the
pulse oximeter was clamped, or whether they used other
strategies in performing the task (e.g., counting the time).
Eleven participants disclosed that they felt their pulse in the
fingertip and were discarded from the analysis; no partici-
pant reported counting the time or using other strategies.
Interoceptive awareness
In keeping with existing research (Bekrater-Bodmann
etal., 2020; Garfinkel etal., 2015; Khalsa etal., 2020), at
the end of each heartbeat perception task trial, participants
were asked to rate how confident they were about their
performance at each IAcc trial. This rating was performed
using a paper/pencil to mark a Likert Scale ranging from 0
(“Total guess/I believe that my performance on the task was
extremely poor”) to 10 (“Complete confidence/ I believe that
my performance on the task was very good”).
Since interoceptive awareness (IAw)is defined as the way
in which confidence in the task performance reflects IAcc
(Garfinkel etal., 2015), the ratio between IAcc (reported on
a 0–10 scale to match the confidence values) and confidence,
as measured above, was computed to index an estimate of
participant-specific IAw. A resulting value of IAw = 1 indi-
cates a perfect correspondence between IAcc and confidence
(e.g., IAcc = 5; confidence = 5: IAw = 5/5 = 1); values of
IAw > 1 indicate participants with higher IAcc than con-
fidence (e.g., IAcc = 5; confidence = 3: IAw = 5/3 = 1.66),
while values of IAw < 1 indicate participants with lower
IAcc than confidence (e.g., IAcc = 3; confidence = 5;
IAw = 3/6 = 0.6). Thus, this index allows us to say that
both individuals with low IAcc and low confidence (e.g.,
IAcc = 2; confidence = 2: IAw = 1; indicating that a subject
is aware that their abilities to discriminate bodily sensation
are not good) and individuals with high IAcc and high con-
fidence (e.g., IAcc = 9; confidence = 9: IAw = 1; indicating
that a subject is aware that their abilities to discriminate
bodily sensation are not good) have high awareness of their
bodily sensations. Here, the variable IAw was used as a con-
tinuous variable (i.e., participants were not grouped depend-
ing on their IAw score).
Statistical analyses
Three linear regression models were built, using the total
TAS-20 score as a continuous dependent variable. The
normality of the TAS-20 score was assessed by means of
the Shapiro–Wilk Test (W = 0.99, p = 0.11). Each model
included one only among ISb, IAcc, and IAw as the predic-
tor of interest, while controlling for age, gender, and depres-
sive symptoms. This was made necessary by the fact that
IAw was calculated as a ratio between IAcc and confidence,
and thus IAw and IAcc are not independent. As a conse-
quence, including both IAcc and IAw within the same model
would have led to collinearity issues. For each individual
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
848 Psychological Research (2022) 86:844–857
1 3
model, we tested the assumption of normality of the residu-
als (Shapiro–Wilk test p = 0.09, 0.19, and 0.13, respectively)
and checked for collinearity across predictors by means of
variance inflation factors (VIF). VIF resulted, in every case,
in a score below 10, indicating the absence of multicollin-
earity (Bowermann & O’Connel, 1990; Myers, 1990). The
presence of influential outliers was checked by means of
the Cook’s distance (Di), which resulted, in every case, in
a score lower than 1, allowing us to rule out their presence
(Cook & Weisberg, 1982).
The regression models were compared by assessing both
their absolute and relative goodness-of-fit. The former was
measured by the amount of explained variance in the model
(R2), while the latter was evaluated by means of both the
Akaike Information Criterion (Akaike, 1987) and the Bayes-
ian Information Criterion (Schwarz, 1978). Specifically, AIC
and BIC evaluate a model’s parsimony (i.e., the balance
between the inclusion of more predictors and the related
increase in model fit). Simply put, lower values of these
measures indicate better fitting models. We also employed
the Bayes Factor (BF) as a measure of relative likelihood
(Kass & Raftery, 1995; Lavine & Schervish, 1999) to
directly compare one model against the others.
Moreover, we tested the ability of each model to pre-
dict the TAS-20 score in a Leave-One-Out Cross-Validation
(LOOCV) design. That is, one observation was left out and
each model was built using N-1 observations; the left-out
value was then predicted. This procedure was repeated
N = 182 times, until each and every observation was left out
and predicted once. The prediction accuracy was assessed
through correlation between the actual and predicted TAS-
20 values.
In addition, we compared ISb, IAcc, and IAw to iden-
tify individuals with high alexithymia levels by means of a
receiver-operating characteristic (ROC) analysis, now using
alexithymia as a binary variable. That is, this analysis was
designed to determine the strength to which a variable can
predict a binary state (i.e., high vs low levels of alexithymia
based on the TAS-20 cutoff reported above) while testing
all possible thresholds or cutoff values. Thus, the number of
correctly classified elements and errors (i.e., subjects clas-
sified as belonging to their actual class and those wrongly
classified, respectively) was computed for all predictor val-
ues as a potential detection threshold. In practice, an ROC
analysis results in a curve showing the relation between sen-
sitivity and specificity for each tested threshold, allowing the
best cutoff value to be identified. In this way, it is possible to
compare the accuracy of the three interoceptive dimensions
in the correct identification of individuals with a high level
of alexithymia. The best detection threshold, which as we
shall discuss is IAw, has the highest accuracy, maximizing
both sensitivity and specificity. All analyses were performed
in R (R Core Team, 2013).
Results
The mean TAS-20 was 46.3 ± 11.29 (DDF: 13.59 ± 4.5;
DIF: 16.36 ± 5.35; EOT: 16.37 ± 4.37). Twenty-two par-
ticipants reported high levels of alexithymia (12.08% of
the sample), which is a value fully aligned to the general
population in which alexithymia has an average prevalence
of 10% (Kokkonen etal., 2001; Muir etal., 2017; Taylor
etal., 1991). Thus, we were confident that our sample
could be considered representative of the general healthy
population. Moreover, the mean ISb was 231.8 ± 46, and
the mean IAcc index was 0.58 ± 0.18, suggesting moder-
ate accuracy in the identification of bodily sensations; the
mean IAw index was 1.11 ± 0.60, suggesting good aware-
ness of bodily sensations.
Regressions using interoceptive dimensions
aspredictors
All the three models including one interoceptive dimen-
sion as predictor and TAS-20 as dependent variable
showed a significant effect of the interoceptive dimension
considered (Table1). However, using the model compari-
son procedure described above, the model including IAw
as predictor of interest was identified as the best one to
explain the TAS-20 total score (Fig.1). That is, the IAw
model showed the highest amount of explained variance
(R2; Fig.1—left, black line), the highest likelihood (BF;
Fig.1—right), and the best prediction performance (Pear-
son’s r between actual and predicted TAS-20 values cre-
ated using the LOOCV design; Fig.1—left, grey line),
relative to the other models. Concurrently, this model min-
imized both AIC and BIC (Fig.1—center), thus providing
the optimal trade-off between model complexity and data
explanatory power.
The regression model using TAS-20 total score as a
continuous dependent variable and IAw as predictor of
interest was statistically significant [F(4, 177) = 26.77;
p < 0.001; adjusted R-squared = 0.36] and revealed a sig-
nificant effect of IAw [t(177) = 10.25; p < 0.001; 95% C.I.
(9.2, 13.6)], but no significant effects of age, gender, or
depressive symptoms, as shown in Table1. Intriguingly,
the analysis revealed that the higher the IAw index is, the
higher the alexithymia level. Since a high IAw index iden-
tifies individuals with higher IAcc than confidence, these
results indicate that the higher the alexithymia level is,
the lower the interoceptive awareness. These results are
shown in Fig.2.
The regression models using TAS-20 total score as
dependent variable and IAcc and ISb as predictors of
interest were also statistically significant (see Table1),
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
849Psychological Research (2022) 86:844–857
1 3
showing a significant effect of IAcc and ISb [t(177) = 8.73
and t(177) = 5.67 for IAcc and ISb, respectively], but no
significant effect of age, gender or depressive symptoms.
The analyses revealed that the higher the IAcc and ISb
indices are, the higher the level of alexithymia.
Interoception dimensions andalexithymia
prediction
The ROC analysis showed that the three dimensions of inter-
oception offer different levels of accuracy in the prediction
of alexithymia as a binary state, as summarized in Fig.3.
IAw is the interoceptive dimension that most accurately
predicted alexithymia in our sample. Among all the tested
thresholds, an IAw cutoff value of 1.4 shows the highest
accuracy at 93.8% [sensitivity = 100%; specificity = 87.5%;
AUC = 95.4%, 95% CI (92.5–98.2%)]. In other words, IAw
correctly classified all the 22 individuals with high alex-
ithymia level, and 140 out of 160 individuals without high
alexithymia level.
IAcc also proved to be a good predictor: Among all the
tested thresholds, an IAcc cutoff value of 0.8 showed the
highest accuracy, at 86.9% [sensitivity = 81.8%; specific-
ity = 91.9%; AUC = 92.5%, 95% C.I. (87.4–97.8%)]. That is,
IAcc correctly classified 18 out of 22 individuals with high
alexithymia level, and 147 out of 160 individuals without
high alexithymia level.
ISb proved to be a less accurate predictor of alexithy-
mia than the other interoceptive dimensions. Among all
the tested thresholds, an ISb cutoff value of 233.5 showed
Table 1 Results of the linear regression models
Models Model statistics Predictors βSE t p
F (df)pAdjusted-R2
Predictor of interest IAw
Model
26.77 (4177) < 0.001 0.36 Age − 0.28 0.18 − 1.55 0.12
Gender 0.15 1.37 0.11 0.91
BDI 0.01 0.12 0.09 0.93
IAw 11.42 1.11 10.25 < 0.001
IAcc
Model
19.51 (4177) < 0.001 0.29 Age − 0.26 0.19 − 1.37 0.17
Gender 0.40 1.44 0.28 0.78
BDI − 0.04 0.13 − 0.32 0.75
IAcc 34.38 3.94 8.73 < 0.001
ISb
Model
8.39 (4177) < 0.001 0.14 Age − 0.29 0.21 − 1.40 0.16
Gender − 0.36 1.59 − 0.23 0.82
BDI − 0.13 0.14 − 0.87 0.39
ISb 0.09 0.02 5.67 < 0.001
Fig. 1 Models’ comparison. On the left panel: the percentage of
explained variance of each model (R2) and the correlation between
actual and model-predicted TAS-20 scores (Pearson’s r) are reported.
On the central panel: the models’ parsimony is evaluated by means
of the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) and the Bayesian Informa-
tion Criterion (BIC), with lower values indicating better models. On
the right panel: comparisons among all models in terms of likelihood,
by means of the Bayes Factor (BF). Lines’ width indicate BF mag-
nitude while the colors indicate the best model for each comparison
(red = IAw model, green = IAcc model, blue = ISb model) (color fig-
ure online)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
850 Psychological Research (2022) 86:844–857
1 3
the highest accuracy, at 69% [sensitivity = 81.8%; speci-
ficity = 56.2%; AUC = 70.1%, 95% C.I. (57.6–82.7%)]. In
other words, ISb correctly classified 18 out of 22 indi-
viduals with high alexithymia level, and 90 out of 160
individuals without high alexithymia level.
Discussion
Alexithymia is generally considered to be “a marker of
atypical interoception” (Murphy etal., 2018; Murphy etal.,
2018). Yet, research on interoceptive abilities in alexithymia
is still in its infancy. Specifically, the association/dissocia-
tion of the three interoceptive dimensions in alexithymia
has received little attention thus far, with the dimension of
interoceptive awareness remaining under-investigated (see
Online Supplementary Material for an overview).
Here, we examined the link between these dimensions
and the construct of alexithymia. The results we obtained
using the heartbeat perception task reveal that, among all
interoceptive dimensions, IAw can most reliably predict
alexithymia. This is because the higher the TAS-20 total
score, the lower the IAw. Moreover, we show that intero-
ceptive abilities are dissociated in alexithymia: IAcc and
ISb increased for higher alexithymia levels, while IAw
decreased for higher levels of alexithymia. Finally, IAw and
IAcc can accurately identify alexithymic participants within
our sample.
The functional meaning of these results indicates that
individuals with a high level of alexithymia, despite being
more focused on their bodily sensations (in accordance with
the original “somatosensory amplification hypothesis”;
(Wise & Mann, 1994) and being more capable of detecting
their own bodily signals (in accordance with the second line
Fig. 2 Relationship between
interoceptive awareness and
alexithymia (TAS-20 = 20-items
Toronto Alexithymia Scale total
score)
Fig. 3 ROC curve showing sensitivity and specificity of the models
testing the relative contribution of the three interoceptive dimensions
for alexithymia prediction. For each curve, a black dot indicates the
best cutoff value and the corresponding specificity and sensitivity
(color figure online)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
851Psychological Research (2022) 86:844–857
1 3
of research) compared with individuals with low alexithy-
mia, may lack self-confidence in their bodily signals, report-
ing to not feel their judgement of their own bodily sensations
is trustworthy (). This would then result in an overall deficit
in interoception. This finding aligns with recent theoriza-
tions proposing that alexithymic individuals do not present
difficulties in perceiving or reporting internal body sensa-
tions, but rather have difficulty in interpreting their bodily
sensations (Fournier etal., 2019; Zamariola, etal., 2018).
Collectively, this study’s contribution is threefold. First,
our results indicate a possible integration of the two con-
trasting hypotheses of interoception and alexithymia. Sec-
ond, they suggest a promising neurocognitive mechanism
for higher risk of psychosomatic disorders in alexithymia.
Third, this work opens the way for promising psychological
interventions to modulate difficulties experienced by alex-
ithymic individuals.
As regards our first contribution, this work’s results
support both existing models of interoceptive abilities in
alexithymia. Since interoceptive accuracy may be a neces-
sary, yet not a sufficient feature for the conscious experience
of emotions (Critchley & Harrison, 2013), a dissociation
between interoceptive accuracy and awareness may repre-
sent the core factor underlying alexithymic deficits. Indeed,
emotional awareness can be graded in different levels, and
objective accuracy in detecting bodily sensation is graded in
the lower level (Lane & Schwartz, 1987). While individuals
with high level of alexithymia could be characterized by
heightened perception of bodily sensation, they may also
put a ‘gridlock’ on a lower level of emotional experience,
without being able to link their visceral signals to higher
levels of emotional processing. In this way, emotion-evoking
situations would be perceived only at the physical level and
remain void of any emotional implication (Scarpazza etal.,
2015). This issue would prevent the formal and symbolic
representation of emotions as a tool for effective emotional
regulation (Schuette etal., 2020). In other words, our results
provide support for the idea that individuals with high lev-
els of alexithymia believe that they are not able to feel the
internal sensations, manifesting a dissociation between their
interoceptive accuracy and awareness. This explanation
would thus offer a coherent lens to understand the currently
fragmented results present in the literature. Yet, we shall
also note that impoverished interoceptive awareness would
support the first theoretical model, which claims a deficit of
interoception in alexithymia. Congruently, functional hypo-
activation in the anterior part of insula (Bird etal., 2010;
Hogeveen etal., 2016; Lemche etal., 2013) and hyperac-
tivation of the posterior part (Goerlich-Dobre etal., 2014;
Wiebking & Northoff, 2015) have been observed in indi-
viduals with high levels of alexithymia. In addition, grey
matter volume increases in the posterior insula (Goerlich-
Dobre etal., 2014) and decreases in the anterior insula
(Borsci etal., 2009; Ihme etal., 2013) have been observed
in individuals with a high alexithymia level, as compared to
individuals with low alexithymia levels. Research has also
shown that individuals with structural acquired damage
to the anterior insula manifest acquired alexithymia—i.e.,
alexithymia emerging as a consequence of brain damage
(Hogeveen etal., 2016). Indeed, the insula, which is well
documented to be one of the brain regions most associated
with interoception (Allen, 2020; Critchley, 2004; Critchley
etal., 2004), is characterized by an anterior–posterior gradi-
ent: the posterior insula is mainly responsible for allowing
the perception of visceral/bodily signals, while the anterior
insula integrates the bodily sensations with subjective feel-
ings and awareness (Craig, 2011).
The second contribution of our work allows us to pro-
pose a potential neurocognitive mechanism to explain the
increased risk of psychosomatic disorders in alexithymia.
Due to their higher IAcc, individuals with high levels of
alexithymia would be more accurate in the perception of
emotion-related physiological reactions (Wise & Mann,
1994). However, due to their low IAw abilities, they would
be unable to correctly interpret their bodily/visceral changes
as emotions, and instead misinterpret them as a bodily symp-
toms (Scarpazza etal., 2015). This process could lead to
somatization. This argument is in line with other evidence
present in the literature. For example, it resonates with
observations that alexithymic individuals delay seeking
medical treatment (Carta etal., 2013). This behavior, which
was previously explained by a deficit of ISb or IAcc (Brewer
etal., 2016), is likely to be due to low interoceptive aware-
ness: Individuals with a high level of alexithymia are likely
to mistrust their physiological states and not to be confident
of their ability to detect their bodily states. Most of all, this
conceptualization reflects the original definition of alexithy-
mia as a deficit in the cognitive interpretation of emotional
arousal (Taylor, 2000) and with the first version of the TAS
scale, in which a factor called “difficulty in distinguishing
between feeling and bodily sensations that accompanied
emotional arousal” was originally included ().
Finally, the current study provides preliminary insights
useful to both the development and implementation of
psychotherapeutic interventions in alexithymia which is
unresponsive to classical psychotherapies (Taylor, 2000).
Intervention specifically designed to improve interoceptive
awareness and to enhance the ability to correctly interpret
emotion-related bodily changes may benefit alexithymic
individuals’ social life and mental health (Duquette, 2020;
Shalev, 2019). Since alexithymic individuals experience
strong visceral responses (Wise & Mann, 1994) without
being able to cognitively interpret them, these individuals
are thought to be “at the mercy” of their bodily sensations,
which might prevent them from effectively selecting suit-
able strategies for emotion regulation. Not surprisingly,
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
852 Psychological Research (2022) 86:844–857
1 3
the sole psychotherapies showing marginal effectiveness in
alexithymia are those emphasizing the necessity to enhance
emotional awareness (Taylor, 2000). Our work suggests
that a possible intervention can be placed on interoceptive
awareness, given its pivotal role in a correct reappraisal of
emotional responses (Duquette, 2020; Fustos etal., 2013;
Shalev, 2019). While we call for future research to repli-
cate our results using additional interoceptive measures (see
“Limitations” below), the potential future implications of
our study are widespread, given that alexithymia is not only
a critical component in some psychiatric disorders (De Pan-
filis etal., 2015), but it is also a mediating factor of mental
health problems in stressful environmental situation, such as
the recent COVID-19 pandemic (Tang etal., 2020) and it is
closely associated with aggressive behavior (Li etal., 2020).
It is thus clear that identifying a way to effectively modulate
alexithymia and the association between alexithymia and
interoceptive dimensions appears to be pivotal to prevent
both mental health problems and the overt expression of
aggressive behaviors in vulnerable individuals.
Limitations andfuture research avenues
This study should also be seen in the light of its limitations,
as well as opportunities for future research. The main limita-
tion of this study is inevitably linked to the rapid evolution of
the definition of interoception, a complex and multi-faceted
construct that is undergoing continuous refinement in con-
ceptualization and operationalization. This issue concerns,
in particular, the heartbeat perception task—HBP task—that
we applied in the current paper to measure interoceptive
accuracy by following the previous literature on alexithymia
and interoception (Bekrater-Bodmann etal., 2020; Herbert
etal., 2011; Nicholson etal., 2018; Scarpazza etal., , 2015,
2017; Shah etal., 2016; Ueno etal., 2020). Despite its exten-
sive and current use, this task has recently been questioned
in terms of its capacity to effectively capture IAcc (Desmedt
etal., 2018; Ring etal., 2015; Zamariola, etal., 2018). Some
of the reasons why the task has been criticized as a measure
of IAcc are well summarized in (Murphy, etal., 2018). First,
heartbeats may be perceived via (exteroceptive) touch recep-
tors due to the vibration of the chest wall (Brener & Ring,
2016; Desmedt etal., 2018; Khalsa etal., 2009); the extent
to which the heartbeat may be perceived exteroceptively
depends on factors such as the percentage of body fat and
systolic body pressure. Thus, future research might consider
adding further control measures possibly impacting task per-
formance, such as body mass index, systolic blood pressure,
heart rate variability (Castaldo etal., 2017; Massaro & Pec-
chia, 2019) among others. In this work, we did not collect
such variables; however, at the completion of the task, we
probed participants on whether or not they felt the pulse, as
a way to control for possible exteroceptive strategies, and
we excluded participants who did. Given that the inclusion
of these variables in previous research showed modest influ-
ence on the effect size of the relationship between alexithy-
mia and HBP task performance (Murphy, etal., 2018), we
can reasonably suppose that our results were not greatly
affected by the absence of such control variables.
Second, the knowledge of one’s own, or the average per-
son’s heart rate may impact the results obtained using the
HBP task (Ring etal., 2015). A growing body of research
demonstrates that manipulating participants’ beliefs about
one’s own resting heart rate may alter heartbeat counting
estimates in the HBP (Ring etal., 2015). Similarly, accurate
knowledge of average heart rate correlates with improved
performance on the HBP task (Murphy et al., 2018).
Although, in this paper, we did not probe the participants
about their knowledge of their heart rate, the results were
corrected for depressive symptoms and gender, with the
latter variable having a stronger impact on the task perfor-
mance than mere knowledge of resting heart rate (Murphy
etal., 2018).
Third, the HBP task results may be affected depending on
whether participants are encouraged to guess if they cannot
feel their heartbeat (Murphy, etal., 2018). If participants
are instructed to guess (or if they do so regardless of the
instruction not to), then a sensible strategy is to estimate the
duration of the interval over which one is required to count
one’s heartbeats and/or to count seconds instead of heartbeat
to arrive at an estimate of the number of heartbeats. Here,
after task completion, we asked participants whether they
counted seconds instead of heart beats and no participant
reported counting the time or using other strategies.
Additional issues associated with the HBT that might
have affected our study’s results are described in (Zamariola,
etal., 2018) They can be summarized as follows: (i) HBT
does not distinguish between over and underestimation of
heartbeats; (ii) the correlation between actual and reported
heart rate is low; (iii) the IAcc scores vary across the time
intervals used in the task. We did not consider these issues
in the current study, which might thus limit the robustness of
our findings. However, it is worth noting that, Murphy etal.,
(2018) recently showed consistency among results obtained
using multidimensional interoceptive tasks, including the
heartbeat perception task, thus giving us confidence in its
inferential capabilities for the purposes of this study.
It is also worth noting that there is increasing evidence
providing support for the notion that heartbeat perception
might be a good index for interoception research. Several
functional neuro-imaging studies show that HBT activates a
network of brain regions including the insula, primary soma-
tosensory cortex, and the anterior cingulate cortex, which
are regions considered fundamental for both the representa-
tion of one’s internal state and for the conscious experience
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
853Psychological Research (2022) 86:844–857
1 3
of emotions (Craig, 2002; Critchley etal., 2004; Pollatos
etal., 2007, Pollatos etal., 2007).
As regards possible other limitations of our study, we
also note that recently, a preprint has appeared among the
scientific community, questioning the body perception
index as a specific measure of interoceptive sensibility
(Gabriele etal., 2020). In this research, which refers to
Murphy’s model of interoception (Murphy etal., 2019),
the BPQ is shown to be potentially prone to misinterpre-
tation as it seems to confound interoceptive accuracy and
interoceptive attention. While we find these insights ben-
eficial for future research, the present study was designed
before our awareness of the new model of interoception
by (Murphy etal., 2019), and thus we cannot interpret
the results of the current study in light of this framework.
Finally, we note that our sample includes, on average,
young participants with a rather homogeneous background
and that this study is largely based on self-report instru-
ments. While such results may vary on a more general or
pathological population, there are multiple occurrences
in the literature of alexithymia studies with similar sam-
ples (Herbert etal., 2011; Longarzo etal., 2015; Maier
etal., 2016; Muir etal., 2017; Scarpazza etal., 2015).
Regarding the use of self-report, these instruments imply
that individuals with a high level of alexithymia are aware
of their problems, which is not always true. Despite this
limitation, the TAS-20 is currently the gold standard for
the non-clinical assessment of alexithymia.
Overall, future research is thus needed to replicate our
findings, using the most recent and innovative ways to meas-
ure interoceptive components and heart signals (Murphy,
etal., 2018; Owens etal., 2018). Nonetheless, the value
of our current results lies in their theoretical and practical
implications and in their potential to guide future research.
Conclusions
In conclusion, we believe that this work provides opening,
convincing evidence on the dissociation of three interocep-
tive components in alexithymia as a candidate mechanism
to explain the impaired processing of emotional experi-
ence. In this sample and with the current task, individuals
with high alexithymic level manifest higher ISb and IAcc,
but lower IAw; they also tend to underestimate their inter-
oceptive abilities, while showing no actual interoceptive
deficit. Finally, given the significant implications that our
findings put forward both for theoretical alexithymia mod-
els and practical implications, we call for future research
to replicate these results with more recent ways to assess
interoceptive components.
Supplementary Information The online version contains supplemen-
tary material available at https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s00426- 021- 01538-x.
Acknowledgements The authors are grateful to Domenico Chirullo for
help with data collection.SM dedicates this work to Diana andRichard
Ernest Massaro.
Author contributions CS and SM designed the experiment; YH, CS
and SM collected the data; AZ analyzed the data; CS wrote the draft;
AZ, SM and CS revised the manuscript and provided critical content;
all the authors approved the final version of the manuscript.
Funding Open access funding provided by Università degli Studi di
Padova within the CRUI-CARE Agreement. CS was supported by a
grant from the University of Padua (Supporting TAlent in ReSearch
at University of Padua—STARS Grants 2017). The present study was
carried out within the scope of the research program “Dipartimenti di
Eccellenza” (art.1, commi 314–337 legge 232/2016), which was sup-
ported by a grant from MIUR to the Department of General Psychol-
ogy, University of Padua.
Availability of data and material Data are available upon request.
Code availability All analyses were performed in R (R Core Team,
2013).
Declarations
Conflict of interest SM is the director of Organizational Neuroscience
Ltd. The authors declare no other real or perceived conflicts of interest.
Ethical approval All the procedures performed in studies involving
human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of
the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964
Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical
standards.
Informed consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual
participants included in the study.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attri-
bution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adapta-
tion, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long
as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source,
provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes
were made. The images or other third party material in this article are
included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated
otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in
the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will
need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a
copy of this licence, visit http:// creat iveco mmons. org/ licen ses/ by/4. 0/.
References
Ainley, V., & Tsakiris, M. (2013). Body conscious? Interoceptive
awareness, measured by heartbeat perception, is negatively corre-
lated with self-objectification. PLoS ONE, 8(2), e55568. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1371/ journ al. pone. 00555 68
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
854 Psychological Research (2022) 86:844–857
1 3
Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52,
317–332.
Alkan Hartwig, E., Aust, S., Heekeren, H. R., & Heuser, I. (2020). No
words for feelings? Not only for my own: Diminished emotional
empathic ability in alexithymia. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuro-
science, 14, 112. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fnbeh. 2020. 00112
Allen, L. B., Qian, L., Tsao, J. C., Hayes, L. P., & Zeltzer, L. K. (2011).
Depression partially mediates the relationship between alexithy-
mia and somatization in a sample of healthy children. Journal of
Health Psychology, 16(8), 1177–1186. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1177/
13591 05311 402407
Allen, M. (2020). Unravelling the Neurobiology of Interoceptive Infer-
ence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(4), 265–266. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1016/j. tics. 2020. 02. 002
Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D., & Taylor, G. J. (2020). Twenty-five years
with the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale. Journal of Psycho-
somatic Research, 131, 109940.
Barrett, L. F., & Simmons, W. K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in
the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(7), 419–429. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1038/ nrn39 50
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., Ball, R., & Ranieri, W. (1996). Compari-
son of Beck Depression Inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric
outpatients. Journal of Personality Assessment, 67(3), 588–597.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1207/ s1532 7752j pa6703_ 13
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck
Depression Inventory-II. Psychological Corporation.
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J.
(1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of Gen-
eral Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.
Bekrater-Bodmann, R., Azevedo, R. T., Ainley, V., & Tsakiris, M.
(2020). Interoceptive awareness is negatively related to the exter-
oceptive manipulation of bodily self-location. Frontiers in Psy-
chology, 11, 562016. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fpsyg. 2020. 562016
Bird, G., Silani, G., Brindley, R., White, S., Frith, U., & Singer, T.
(2010). Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by lev-
els of alexithymia but not autism. Brain, 133(Pt 5), 1515–1525.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/ brain/ awq060
Bornemann, B., & Singer, T. (2017). Taking time to feel our body:
Steady increases in heartbeat perception accuracy and decreases
in alexithymia over 9 months of contemplative mental training.
Psychophysiology, 54(3), 469–482. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1111/ psyp.
12790
Borsci, G., Boccardi, M., Rossi, R., Rossi, G., Perez, J., Bonetti, M.,
etal. (2009). Alexithymia in healthy women: A brain morphol-
ogy study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 114(1–3), 208–215.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jad. 2008. 07. 013
Bowermann, B. L., & O’Connel, R. T. (1990). Linear statistical mod-
els: An applied approach (2nd ed.). Duxbury.
Brener, J., & Ring, C. (2016). Towards a psychophysics of interocep-
tive processes: the measurement of heartbeat detection. Philo-
sophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 371, 1708. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1098/ rstb. 2016. 0015
Bressi, C., Taylor, G., Parker, J., Bressi, S., Brambilla, V., Aguglia,
E., etal. (1996). Cross validation of the factor structure of the
20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale: An Italian multicenter study.
Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 41(6), 551–559. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1016/ s0022- 3999(96) 00228-0
Brewer, R., Cook, R., & Bird, G. (2016). Alexithymia: A general deficit
of interoception. R Soc Open Sci, 3(10), 150664. https:// doi. org/
10. 1098/ rsos. 150664
Carta, M. G., Sancassiani, F., Pippia, V., Bhat, K. M., Sardu, C., &
Meloni, L. (2013). Alexithymia is associated with delayed treat-
ment seeking in acute myocardial infarction. Psychotherapy and
Psychosomatics, 82(3), 190–192. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1159/ 00034
1181
Castaldo, R., Montesinos, L., Wan, T. S., Serban, A., Massaro, S., &
Pecchia, L. (2018). Heart rate variability analysis and perfor-
mance during a repeated mental workload task. In: Eskola H.,
Väisänen O., Viik J., Hyttinen J. (Eds.) EMBEC & NBC 2017.
EMBEC 2017, NBC 2017. IFMBE Proceedings, vol 65. Springer,
Singapore. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ 978- 981- 10- 5122-7_ 18
Cook, T. D., & Weisberg, S. (1982). Residuals and influence in regres-
sion. Chapman and Hall.
Craig, A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: The sense of the
physiological condition of the body. Nature Reviews Neurosci-
ence, 3(8), 655–666. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1038/ nrn894
Craig, A. D. (2011). Significance of the insula for the evolution of
human awareness of feelings from the body. Annals of the New
York Academy of Sciences, 1225, 72–82. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1111/j. 1749- 6632. 2011. 05990.x
Critchley, H. D. (2004). The human cortex responds to an interoceptive
challenge. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences in
USA, 101(17), 6333–6334. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1073/ pnas. 04015
10101
Critchley, H. D., & Garfinkel, S. N. (2017). Interoception and Emo-
tion. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17, 7–14. https:// doi. org/
10. 1016/j. copsyc. 2017. 04. 020
Critchley, H. D., & Harrison, N. A. (2013). Visceral influences on brain
and behavior. Neuron, 77(4), 624–638. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j.
neuron. 2013. 02. 008
Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Ohman, A., & Dolan, R.
J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness.
Nature Neuroscience, 7(2), 189–195. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1038/
nn1176
De Panfilis, C., Ossola, P., Tonna, M., Catania, L., & Marchesi, C.
(2015). Finding words for feelings: The relationship between per-
sonality disorders and alexithymia. Personality and Individual
Differences, 74, 285–291.
Desmedt, O., Luminet, O., & Corneille, O. (2018). The heartbeat
counting task largely involves non-interoceptive processes:
Evidence from both the original and an adapted counting task.
Biological Psychology, 138, 185–188. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j.
biops ycho. 2018. 09. 004
Di Tella, M., Adenzato, M., Catmur, C., Miti, F., Castelli, L., & Ardito,
R. B. (2020). The role of alexithymia in social cognition: Evi-
dence from a non-clinical population. Journal of Affective Dis-
orders, 273, 482–492. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. jad. 2020. 05. 012
Duquette, P. (2020). More than words can say: A multi-disciplinary
consideration of the psychotherapeutic evaluation and treatment
of alexithymia. Front Psychiatry, 11, 433. https:// doi. org/ 10.
3389/ fpsyt. 2020. 00433
Ernst, J., Boker, H., Hattenschwiler, J., Schupbach, D., Northoff, G.,
Seifritz, E., etal. (2014). The association of interoceptive aware-
ness and alexithymia with neurotransmitter concentrations in
insula and anterior cingulate. Social Cognitive and Affective Neu-
roscience, 9(6), 857–863. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/ scan/ nst058
Fehr, F. S., & Stern, J. A. (1970). Peripheral physiological variables
and emotion: The James-Lange theory revisited. Psychological
Bulletin, 74(6), 411–424. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ h0032 958
Fournier, A., Luminet, O., Dambrun, M., Dutheil, F., Pellissier, S., &
Mondillon, L. (2019). Importance of considering interoceptive
abilities in alexithymia assessment. PeerJ, 7, e7615. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 7717/ peerj. 7615
Fustos, J., Gramann, K., Herbert, B. M., & Pollatos, O. (2013). On
the embodiment of emotion regulation: Interoceptive awareness
facilitates reappraisal. Social Cognitive and Affective Neurosci-
ence, 8(8), 911–917. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/ scan/ nss089
Gabriele, E., Spooner, R., Brewer, R., & Murphy, J. (2020). Dissocia-
tions between interoceptive accuracy and attention: Evidence
from the interoceptive attention scale. PsyArXiv Preprints.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 31234/ osf. io/ vjgh6
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
855Psychological Research (2022) 86:844–857
1 3
Garfinkel, S. N., & Critchley, H. D. (2013). Interoception, emotion
and brain: new insights link internal physiology to social behav-
ior. Commentary on:: “Anterior insular cortex mediates bodily
sensibility and social anxiety” by Terasawa etal. (2012). Soc
Cogn Affect Neurosci, 8(3), 231–234. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/
scan/ nss140
Garfinkel, S. N., Seth, A. K., Barrett, A. B., Suzuki, K., & Critchley, H.
D. (2015). Knowing your own heart: Distinguishing interoceptive
accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biological Psychology,
104, 65–74. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. biops ycho. 2014. 11. 004
Goerlich-Dobre, K. S., Bruce, L., Martens, S., Aleman, A., & Hooker,
C. I. (2014). Distinct associations of insula and cingulate vol-
ume with the cognitive and affective dimensions of alexithymia.
Neuropsychologia, 53, 284–292. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro
psych ologia. 2013. 12. 006
Grynberg, D., Chang, B., Corneille, O., Maurage, P., Vermeulen, N.,
Berthoz, S., etal. (2012). Alexithymia and the processing of
emotional facial expressions (EFEs): Systematic review, unan-
swered questions and further perspectives. PLoS ONE, 7(8),
e42429. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/ journ al. pone. 00424 29
Herbert, B. M., Herbert, C., & Pollatos, O. (2011). On the relationship
between interoceptive awareness and alexithymia: Is interocep-
tive awareness related to emotional awareness? Journal of Per-
sonality, 79(5), 1149–1175. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1111/j. 1467- 6494.
2011. 00717.x
Hintikka, J., Honkalampi, K., Lehtonen, J., & Viinamaki, H. (2001).
Are alexithymia and depression distinct or overlapping con-
structs?: A study in a general population. Comprehensive Psychi-
atry, 42(3), 234–239. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1053/ comp. 2001. 23147
Hogeveen, J., Bird, G., Chau, A., Krueger, F., & Grafman, J. (2016).
Acquired alexithymia following damage to the anterior insula.
Neuropsychologia, 82, 142–148. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro
psych ologia. 2016. 01. 021
Honkalampi, K., Hintikka, J., Tanskanen, A., Lehtonen, J., & Vii-
namaki, H. (2000). Depression is strongly associated with alex-
ithymia in the general population. Journal of Psychosomatic
Research, 48(1), 99–104.
Ihme, K., Dannlowski, U., Lichev, V., Stuhrmann, A., Grotegerd, D.,
Rosenberg, N., etal. (2013). Alexithymia is related to differences
in gray matter volume: A voxel-based morphometry study. Brain
Research, 1491, 60–67. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. brain res. 2012.
10. 044
Kass, R. E., & Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayes factors. Journal of the
American Statistical Association, 90(430), 773–795.
Khalsa, S. S., Rudrauf, D., Hassanpour, M. S., Davidson, R. J., &
Tranel, D. (2020). The practice of meditation is not associated
with improved interoceptive awareness of the heartbeat. Psycho-
physiology, 57(2), e13479. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1111/ psyp. 13479
Khalsa, S. S., Rudrauf, D., Sandesara, C., Olshansky, B., & Tranel, D.
(2009). Bolus isoproterenol infusions provide a reliable method
for assessing interoceptive awareness. International Journal of
Psychophysiology, 72(1), 34–45. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. ijpsy
cho. 2008. 08. 010
Kokkonen, P., Karvonen, J. T., Veijola, J., Laksy, K., Jokelainen, J.,
Jarvelin, M. R., etal. (2001). Prevalence and sociodemographic
correlates of alexithymia in a population sample of young adults.
Comprehensive Psychiatry, 42(6), 471–476. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1053/ comp. 2001. 27892
Lane, R. D., Hsu, C. H., Locke, D. E., Ritenbaugh, C., & Stonnington,
C. M. (2015). Role of theory of mind in emotional awareness
and alexithymia: Implications for conceptualization and meas-
urement. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 398–405. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1016/j. concog. 2015. 02. 004
Lane, R. D., & Schwartz, G. E. (1987). Levels of emotional aware-
ness: A cognitive-developmental theory and its application
to psychopathology. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144(2),
133–143. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1176/ ajp. 144.2. 133
Lavine, M., & Schervish, M. J. (1999). Bayes factors: What they
are and what they are not. The American Statistician, 52(2),
119–122.
Lemche, E., Brammer, M. J., David, A. S., Surguladze, S. A., Phillips,
M. L., Sierra, M., etal. (2013). Interoceptive-reflective regions
differentiate alexithymia traits in depersonalization disorder. Psy-
chiatry Research, 214(1), 66–72. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. pscyc
hresns. 2013. 05. 006
Li, S., Zhang, B., Guo, Y., & Zhang, J. (2015). The association between
alexithymia as assessed by the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia
Scale and depression: A meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research,
227(1), 1–9. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. psych res. 2015. 02. 006
Li, X., Li, B., Lu, J., Jin, L., Xue, J., & Che, X. (2020). The relationship
between alexithymia, hostile attribution bias, and aggression.
In Press.
Longarzo, M., D’Olimpio, F., Chiavazzo, A., Santangelo, G., Trojano,
L., & Grossi, D. (2015). The relationships between interocep-
tion and alexithymic trait. The Self-Awareness Questionnaire in
healthy subjects. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1149. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 3389/ fpsyg. 2015. 01149
Lopez-Munoz, F., & Perez-Fernandez, F. (2019). A history of the alex-
ithymia concept and its explanatory models: An epistemological
perspective. Front Psychiatry, 10, 1026. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/
fpsyt. 2019. 01026
Maier, M. E., Scarpazza, C., Starita, F., Filogamo, R., & Ladavas, E.
(2016). Error monitoring is related to processing internal affec-
tive states. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience,
16(6), 1050–1062. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13415- 016- 0452-1
Massaro, S., & Pecchia, L. (2019). Heart rate variability (HRV) analy-
sis: A methodology for organizational neuroscience. Organiza-
tional Research Methods, 22(1), 354–393.
Meza-Concha, N., Arancibia, M., Salas, F., Behar, R., Salas, G., Silva,
H., etal. (2017). Towards a neurobiological understanding of
alexithymia. Medwave, 17(4), e6960. https:// doi. org/ 10. 5867/
medwa ve. 2017. 04. 6960
Moriguchi, Y., Decety, J., Ohnishi, T., Maeda, M., Mori, T., Nemoto,
K., etal. (2007). Empathy and judging other’s pain: An fMRI
study of alexithymia. Cerebral Cortex, 17(9), 2223–2234.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/ cercor/ bhl130
Moriguchi, Y., Ohnishi, T., Lane, R. D., Maeda, M., Mori, T.,
Nemoto, K., etal. (2006). Impaired self-awareness and theory
of mind: An fMRI study of mentalizing in alexithymia. Neu-
roImage, 32(3), 1472–1482. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro
image. 2006. 04. 186
Muir, K., Madill, A., & Brown, C. (2017). Individual differences in
emotional processing and autobiographical memory: Interocep-
tive awareness and alexithymia in the fading affect bias. Cogni-
tion and Emotion, 31(7), 1392–1404. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/
02699 931. 2016. 12250 05
Murphy, J., Brewer, R., Hobson, H., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2018).
Is alexithymia characterised by impaired interoception? Further
evidence, the importance of control variables, and the problems
with the Heartbeat Counting Task. Biological Psychology, 136,
189–197. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. biops ycho. 2018. 05. 010
Murphy, J., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2018). Alexithymia is associated
with a multidomain, multidimensional failure of interoception:
Evidence from novel tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 147(3), 398–408. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ xge00 00366
Murphy, J., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2019). Classifying individual dif-
ferences in interoception: Implications for the measurement of
interoceptive awareness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(5),
1467–1471. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13423- 019- 01632-7
Murphy, J., Geary, H., Millgate, E., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2018).
Direct and indirect effects of age on interoceptive accuracy
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
856 Psychological Research (2022) 86:844–857
1 3
and awareness across the adult lifespan. Psychonomic Bul-
letin & Review, 25(3), 1193–1202. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/
s13423- 017- 1339-z
Myers, R. (1990). Classical and modern regression with applications
(2nd ed.). Duxbury.
Nam, G., Lee, H., Lee, J. H., & Hur, J. W. (2020). Disguised emotion
in alexithymia: subjective difficulties in emotion processing and
increased empathic distress. Front Psychiatry, 11, 698. https://
doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fpsyt. 2020. 00698
Nicholson, T. M., Williams, D. M., Grainger, C., Christensen, J. F.,
Calvo-Merino, B., & Gaigg, S. B. (2018). Interoceptive impair-
ments do not lie at the heart of autism or alexithymia. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 127(6), 612–622. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1037/ abn00 00370
Ospina, L. H., Shanahan, M., Perez-Rodriguez, M. M., Chan, C. C.,
Clari, R., & Burdick, K. E. (2019). Alexithymia predicts poorer
social and everyday functioning in schizophrenia and bipolar
disorder. Psychiatry Research, 273, 218–226. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1016/j. psych res. 2019. 01. 033
Owens, A. P., Allen, M., Ondobaka, S., & Friston, K. J. (2018). Intero-
ceptive inference: From computational neuroscience to clinic.
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 90, 174–183. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neubi orev. 2018. 04. 017
Palser, E. R., Palmer, C. E., Galvez-Pol, A., Hannah, R., Fotopoulou,
A., & Kilner, J. M. (2018). Alexithymia mediates the relation-
ship between interoceptive sensibility and anxiety. PLoS ONE,
13(9), e0203212. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/ journ al. pone. 02032 12
Parker, J. D., Taylor, G. J., & Bagby, R. M. (2003). The 20-item toronto
alexithymia. Scale. III. Reliability and factorial validity in a com-
munity population. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 55(3),
269–275.
Pearson, A., & Pfeifer, G. (2020). Two measures of interoceptive sen-
sibility and the relationship with introversion and neuroticism in
an adult population. Psychological Reports. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1177/ 00332 94120 965461
Pollatos, O., & Gramann, K. (2012). Attenuated modulation of brain
activity accompanies emotion regulation deficits in alexithymia.
Psychophysiology, 49(5), 651–658. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1111/j. 1469-
8986. 2011. 01348.x
Pollatos, O., Gramann, K., & Schandry, R. (2007). Neural systems con-
necting interoceptive awareness and feelings. Human Brain Map-
ping, 28(1), 9–18. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1002/ hbm. 20258
Pollatos, O., Schandry, R., Auer, D. P., & Kaufmann, C. (2007). Brain
structures mediating cardiovascular arousal and interoceptive
awareness. Brain Research, 1141, 178–187. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1016/j. brain res. 2007. 01. 026
Porges, S. W. (1993). Body perception questionnaire. University of
Maryland.
Ring, C., Brener, J., Knapp, K., & Mailloux, J. (2015). Effects of heart-
beat feedback on beliefs about heart rate and heartbeat counting:
A cautionary tale about interoceptive awareness. Biological Psy-
chology, 104, 193–198. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. biops ycho. 2014.
12. 010
Scarpazza, C., & Di Pellegrino, G. (2018). Alexithymia, embodiment
of emotions and interoceptive abilities. Current developments in
alexithymia: A cognitive and affective deficit (pp. 35–53). Nova
Science Publishers.
Scarpazza, C., di Pellegrino, G., & Ladavas, E. (2014). Emotional modu-
lation of touch in alexithymia. Emotion, 14(3), 602–610. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1037/ a0035 888
Scarpazza, C., Ladavas, E., & Cattaneo, L. (2018). Invisible side of emo-
tions: Somato-motor responses to affective facial displays in alex-
ithymia. Experimental Brain Research, 236(1), 195–206. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s00221- 017- 5118-x
Scarpazza, C., Ladavas, E., & di Pellegrino, G. (2015). Dissociation
between emotional remapping of fear and disgust in alexithymia.
PLoS ONE, 10(10), e0140229. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/ journ al.
pone. 01402 29
Scarpazza, C., Sellitto, M., & di Pellegrino, G. (2017). Now or not-now?
The influence of alexithymia on intertemporal decision-making.
Brain and Cognition, 114, 20–28. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. bandc.
2017. 03. 001
Schandry, R. (1981). Heart beat perception and emotional experience.
Psychophysiology, 18(4), 483–488.
Schuette, S. A., Zucker, N. L., & Smoski, M. J. (2020). Do interoceptive
accuracy and interoceptive sensibility predict emotion regulation?
Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung. https:// doi. org/
10. 1007/ s00426- 020- 01369-2
Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. The Annals of
Statistics, 6(2), 461–464.
Shah, P., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2016a). Emotional decision-making in
autism spectrum disorder: The roles of interoception and alexithy-
mia. Mol Autism, 7, 43. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1186/ s13229- 016- 0104-x
Shah, P., Hall, R., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2016). Alexithymia, not
autism, is associated with impaired interoception. Cortex, 81,
215–220. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. cortex. 2016. 03. 021
Shalev, I. (2019). Motivated cue integration in alexithymia: improving
interoception and emotion information processing by awareness-
of-sensation techniques. Front Psychiatry, 10, 329. https:// doi. org/
10. 3389/ fpsyt. 2019. 00329
Sifneos, P. E. (1973). The prevalence of “alexithymic” characteristics in
psychosomatic patients. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 22(2),
255–262. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1159/ 00028 6529
Starita, F., Borhani, K., Bertini, C., & Scarpazza, C. (2018). Alexithymia
is related to the need for more emotional intensity to identify static
fearful facial expressions. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 929. https://
doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fpsyg. 2018. 00929
Starita, F., Pietrelli, M., Bertini, C., & Pellegrino, G. D. (2019). Aber-
rant reward prediction error during Pavlovian appetitive learning in
alexithymia. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1093/ scan/ nsz089
Swart, M., Kortekaas, R., & Aleman, A. (2009). Dealing with feelings:
Characterization of trait alexithymia on emotion regulation strate-
gies and cognitive-emotional processing. PLoS ONE, 4(6), e5751.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/ journ al. pone. 00057 51
Tang, W., Hu, T., Yang, L., & Xu, J. (2020). The role of alexithymia in the
mental health problems of home-quarantined university students
during the COVID-19 pandemic in China. IN Press.
Taylor, G. J. (2000). Recent developments in alexithymia theory and
research. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 45(2), 134–142. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1177/ 07067 43700 04500 203
Taylor, G. J., Bagby, R. M., & Parker, J. D. (1991). The alexithymia con-
struct. A potential paradigm for psychosomatic medicine. Psycho-
somatics, 32(2), 153–164. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/ s0033- 3182(91)
72086-0
Taylor, G. J., Bagby, R. M., & Parker, J. D. (2003). Disorders of affect
regulation: Alexithymia in medical and psychiatric illness. Cam-
bridge University Press.
Taylor, G. J., Bagby, R. M., & Parker, J. D. A. (2016). What’s in the name
“alexithymia”? A commentary on ‘Affective agnosia: Expansion of
the alexithymia construct and a new opportunity to integrate and
extend Freud’s legacy.Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews,
68, 1006–1020. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neubi orev. 2016. 05. 025
Trevisan, D. A., Altschuler, M. R., Bagdasarov, A., Carlos, C., Duan,
S., Hamo, E., etal. (2019). A meta-analysis on the relationship
between interoceptive awareness and alexithymia: Distinguish-
ing interoceptive accuracy and sensibility. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 128(8), 765–776. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ abn00 00454
Trevisan, D. A., Mehling, W. E., & McPartland, J. C. (2020). Adap-
tive and maladaptive bodily awareness: distinguishing interocep-
tive sensibility and interoceptive attention from anxiety-induced
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
857Psychological Research (2022) 86:844–857
1 3
somatization in autism and alexithymia. Autism Research. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1002/ aur. 2458
Ueno, D., Matsuoka, T., Kato, Y., Ayani, N., Maeda, S., Takeda, M.,
etal. (2020). Individual differences in interoceptive accuracy are
correlated with salience network connectivity in older adults. Front
Aging Neurosci, 12, 592002. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fnagi. 2020.
592002
Valdespino, A., Antezana, L., Ghane, M., & Richey, J. A. (2017). Alex-
ithymia as a Transdiagnostic precursor to empathy abnormalities:
The functional role of the insula. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2234.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fpsyg. 2017. 02234
van der Velde, J., Servaas, M. N., Goerlich, K. S., Bruggeman, R., Horton,
P., Costafreda, S. G., etal. (2013). Neural correlates of alexithy-
mia: A meta-analysis of emotion processing studies. Neuroscience
and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(8), 1774–1785. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1016/j. neubi orev. 2013. 07. 008
Wiebking, C., & Northoff, G. (2015). Neural activity during interoceptive
awareness and its associations with alexithymia-An fMRI study in
major depressive disorder and non-psychiatric controls. Frontiers
in Psychology, 6, 589. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fpsyg. 2015. 00589
Wise, T. N., & Mann, L. S. (1994). The relationship between somatosen-
sory amplification, alexithymia, and neuroticism. Journal of Psy-
chosomatic Research, 38(6), 515–521.
Zamariola, G., Maurage, P., Luminet, O., & Corneille, O. (2018). Intero-
ceptive accuracy scores from the heartbeat counting task are prob-
lematic: Evidence from simple bivariate correlations. Biological
Psychology, 137, 12–17. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. biops ycho. 2018.
06. 006
Zamariola, G., Vlemincx, E., Cornellle, O., & Luminet, O. (2018). Rela-
tionship between interoceptive accuracy, interoceptive sensibil-
ity, and alexithymia. Personality and Individual Differences, 125,
14–20. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. paid. 2017. 12. 024
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
... 26 This diminished interoceptive awareness of internal bodily states carries significant implications for an individual's mental health and overall well-being, and may also be a mechanism impacting appetite loss 27 and alexithymia. 28,29 Therefore our hypothesis is that HST-IIP will improve interoceptive awareness for depressed patients, as measured by changes in Heartbeat-Evoked Potential (HEP) 30,31 from electroencephalography (EEG) and electrocardiogram (ECG), which will contribute to depression symptom improvement. We will test this hypothesis in this mechanistic pilot study with EEG, ECG and other physiological measures collected before and after the HST-IIP program compared to a waitlist control arm, combined with clinical and questionnaire assessments of symptom improvements. ...
... This pilot study focuses on depressed US adults as the target population out of several considerations: (1) Depressed patients have been known to have compromised interoceptive abilities 21,22 such as appetite changes 27 and alexithymia 28,29 with well-documented neural 25 and physiological representations, 24 therefore is an ideal condition for studying the feasibility of HST as an interoception based intervention, as well as for studying its neurophysiological mechanisms. (2) Because the HST intervention was originally developed in Korea, it is an important step to test its feasibility in an US based patient population to shed light on its cultural and linguistic adaptability as part of the feasibility research. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Heart-Smile Training (HST) is an interoceptive compassion-based behavioral intervention that in case reports has been beneficial for depression. Interoception refers to the awareness and regulation of physiological signals from inside the body. Depressed patients often have diminished interoceptive awareness and often experience disconnection from bodily needs and sensations. In addition to interoceptive dysfunction, depression often involves negative self-evaluation and self-critical rumination. HST is a compassion-based meditation training program that explicitly cultivates interoceptive awareness of the heart area. This study aims to investigate the possible neurocardiac mechanisms engaged through HST for depression patients. Methods We plan to enroll 50 subjects to be randomized into a 4-week HST intervention group and a waitlist group. A battery of psychological questionnaires will be administered at baseline and post-intervention timepoints, and electroencephalography (EEG) will be collected during compassion meditation guided by pre-recorded audio. The primary clinical outcome measures are on the feasibility of the intervention and research procedures, the primary mechanistic outcome measure is the post-intervention change in Heartbeat Evoked Potential (HEP) amplitude. Secondary outcome measures include changes in depression severity and EEG gamma spectral activity. Exploratory outcome measures include effects of HST on skin conductance response, heart rate variability, EEG spectral properties in other frequency bands, as well as a list of psychological questionnaires that measure depression and anxiety symptoms, emotion regulation, mindfulness, interoceptive awareness, self-compassion, gratitude, sleep quality, quality of life and social connectedness. Results Results not yet available. Conclusion This is the first study on the feasibility and interoceptive neurocardiac mechanism of HST. Our findings will provide frontier knowledge on the physiological working mechanism of behavioral interventions with an interoception-based meditative approach. https://clinicaltrials.gov/study/NCT05564533 .
... The under-responding facet of alexithymia seems to be related to somatization, where a somatic symptom emerges in the absence of a detectable pathology or illness (Ebert and Martus, 1994;Simon et al., 1999). If these patients are, additionally, in an altered mood state such as depression, they might lose their ability to identify these feelings and interpret them only as physical symptoms (Mattila et al., 2008;Scarpazza et al., 2014Scarpazza et al., , 2022. The over-responding facet of alexithymia corresponds to the central position of "crying" in the joint network of depression and alexithymia (Fig. 4). ...
... The results dispel the notion that alexithymia is confounded by depression but can explain different symptoms of depression. We could also depict the pivotal role of the embodiment of emotions and feelings in this relationship (Scarpazza et al., 2022). Furthermore, the results could help to identify how different treatment approaches (such as online vs. face-to-face psychotherapy) influence the symptom network and the burden of depression. ...
... Body perception questionnaire (BPQ). The BPQ [29,33] was employed by seven studies to measure 'interoception' [59], 'interoceptive awareness' [68], 'interoceptive sensibility' [81][82][83], 'self-reported interoceptive attention' [40,65,66,77], and 'subjective interoception' [79]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although research indicates that self-reported interoception is associated with deficits in identifying and describing emotional experience, and externally oriented thinking styles (alexithymia), this relationship appears moderated by how interoception is measured. A systematic review and meta-analyses examined the association between self-reported interoception and alexithymia, investigating how different interoceptive questionnaires relate to alexithymia at global and facet levels. PsychINFO, PubMed, Scopus, and Web of Science databases were searched with predefined terms related to self-reported interoception and alexithymia. Three reviewers independently assessed articles, extracted data, and undertook risk of bias assessment. Thirty-two cross-sectional studies published between 1996 and 2023 were included. Random-effects meta-analyses and narrative synthesis indicated that global alexithymia was positively associated with measures of interoceptive confusion, autonomic nervous system reactivity, and heightened interoceptive attention, and inversely associated with interoceptive accuracy and adaptive interoception, indexed by composite Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness scores, but particularly interoceptive trusting, self-regulation, and attention regulation. These patterns were observed for alexithymic facets and stronger in magnitude for difficulty identifying feelings and difficulty describing feelings, relative to externally oriented thinking. Overall, results suggested that the association between self-reported interoception and alexithymia differs as a function of the interoceptive self-report. The review highlighted issues with construct definition and operationalisation and determined that existing interoceptive self-reports broadly capture maladaptive and adaptive sensing, attention, interpretation, and memory. The findings underscore the importance of specifying interoceptive constructs and using appropriate assessments to improve convergence between constructs and measurements, further suggesting potential clinical utility in using existing self-reports to measure interoception and alexithymia, facilitating interventions targeting mind-body connections.
... Research has demonstrated that interoception and interoceptive awareness problems exists in these clinical populations, including autism spectrum disorder (Williams et al., 2023), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Kutscheidt et al., 2019), alexithymia (Scarpazza et al., 2022), various eating disorders (Khalsa et al., 2015;Willem et al., 2019) and chronic pain conditions. Further, as interoception plays a key role in homeostasis where the connection between body and mind is essential for everyday function (Bonaz et al., 2021;Craig, 2002), it is important to consider how interoception is related to somatic-visceral disorders such as chronic pain (Di Lernia et al., 2016) and musculoskeletal disorders (Bonaz et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Interoception is relatively new within pediatric occupational therapy practice. Interoception allows us to feel and interpret internal body sensations and is related to our ability to regulate emotions and behavior. It is associated with other functions including sensory processing, self-regulation and executive functioning, making it important for everyday participation in life activities. Pediatric occupational therapists are considering interoception as part of intervention but there is limited research to understand how and why occupational therapists use interoception in clinical practice. Objectives The aim of this protocol is to describe the methodology and analyses to be used to undertake a scoping review. Method This paper presents the protocol for a scoping review. A search of literature from ten electronic databases (Ovid Medline, Ovid Embase, Ovid PsycINFO, ERIC, Scopus, CINAHL, ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global, AMED, OTDBASE and Google Scholar), grey literature and article reference searching was conducted. Title and abstract screening was completed before full-text review. Selected articles met stringent inclusion criteria. The PRISMA-ScR Checklist was used alongside the Crowe Critical Appraisal Tool to ensure robust data extraction and synthesis from selected studies. Results A total of 2449 articles were retrieved. Eighteen articles were included in the review. A narrative synthesis approach will be used to synthesise data. Conclusion Findings from this scoping review will inform future research related to interoception and pediatric occupational therapy practice to support clinicians to consider evidence to underpin practice.
... The posterior insula is mainly responsible for processing the perception of visceral/bodily signals while the anterior insula may integrate bodily sensations with subjective feelings and awareness. 97,98 The anterior insula cortex and the ACC have Von Economo neurons (VENs) which are unique cells found only in these areas. It is hypothesized that their function is to relay information from the insula and ACC to frontal and temporal regions and are related to the ability to make adaptive responses to changing environmental conditions. ...
Article
Full-text available
While exteroceptive sensory processing is a hallmark of autism spectrum disorder, how interoceptive processing may impact and contribute to symptomatology remains unclear. In this comprehensive narrative review on interoception in autism, we discuss: 1) difficulties with assessing interoception; 2) potential interoceptive differences; 3) interactions between neural systems for interoception, attention, sensorimotor processing, and cognition; and 4) potential differences in neural circuits involved in interoception. In general, there are mixed findings on potential interoception differences in autism. Nevertheless, some data indicate differences in integration of interoceptive and exteroceptive information may contribute to autism symptomatology. Neurologically, interoceptive processing in autism may be impacted by potential differences in the development, morphometry, and connectivity of key interoceptive hubs (vagal processing, brainstem, thalamus, insula), though much work is needed on this topic.
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Alcohol craving, characterized by a strong desire or compulsion to consume alcohol, is a prominent symptom of substance dependence syndrome. Research indicates that alcohol craving is a significant factor leading to the termination of abstinence. The mechanisms underlying the activation of alcohol craving remain not fully understood. The urge to reach for alcohol may be stimulated by emotions, memories, thoughts, or bodily sensations, as well as external factors. It has been postulated that individuals with high levels of interoceptive sensibility tend to exhibit a high degree of alexithymia and emotion dysregulation in the context of alcohol craving. Deficits in identifying and verbalizing emotions, along with an operational thinking style, facilitate alcohol consumption by impeding accurate insight into one’s mental state, thereby hindering the comprehension of bodily states, emotions, and the regulation of self. Method This study involved 160 inmates incarcerated in a prison in Poland, awaiting participation in therapy for individuals with substance dependence following psychiatric diagnosis. Four questionnaires were used in the study: multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Sensibility (MAIA) for interoceptive sensibility, Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) for alexithymia, Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) for emotional dysregulation, and the Penn Alcohol Craving Scale (PACS) for alcohol craving assessment. Results The results of the study are as follows: the study findings indicated that alexithymia and emotional dysregulation significantly mediates the relationship between interoceptive sensibility and alcohol craving. The indirect effect for both factors was found to be significant, similar to the indirect effect observed for alexithymia as an mediator. However, in the case of emotional dysregulation, no significant indirect effect was observed. Conclusion Our study provides insights into the potential contribution of interoceptive sensibility to the heightened risk of alcohol dependence. Specifically, impaired interoceptive sensibility may be associated with the development of alexithymia and emotional dysregulation, potentially rendering individuals more susceptible to alcohol craving. Interoceptive sensibility could serve as a prerequisite for the cultivation of positive emotional processing skills.
Article
The aim of the current study was to replicate findings from prior work among adults showing that individuals with better interoceptive skills have more emotional awareness, and show better emotion regulation abilities, in a sample of adolescents and by relying on instructions that reduce the contamination of known confound variables. A total of 102 Belgian adolescents ( M age = 14.10 years, SD age = .63; 50 males) completed self-report questionnaires of emotional processes (FEEL-KJ and DERS) and the modified heartbeat counting task. From this task, interoceptive accuracy, interoceptive sensibility (confidence ratings), and interoceptive awareness scores (within-person correlations) were derived per participant. Results revealed no associations between the three dimensions of interoception and adolescents’ levels of emotional awareness, adaptive, and maladaptive emotion regulation. The lack of associations which contrast some prior work with adults may be due to developmental differences. However, these might also support the low validity of the heartbeat counting task, or could be attributed to the measurement of the emotion measures (i.e., self-report). Future studies should, nevertheless, also test whether these non-significant results can be explained by the developmental differences in adolescents. Longitudinal research is needed to capture interoceptive changes during adolescence, as well as to replicate the current findings using rigorous multimethod approaches that increase the validity of interoception measurement.
Article
Full-text available
Background Emotion dysregulation (ED) is a core symptom of borderline personality disorder (BPD), whose aetiology has been attributed to biosocial factors. In autism spectrum condition (ASC), although ED is prevalent and is associated with decreased well-being (e.g. self-harm, suicidality), it has been understudied, especially in adults. It is therefore crucial to further understand ED in autistic adults to improve its treatment. Our study investigates ED, its behavioural correlates (e.g. self-harm, suicidality) and biosocial predictors in autistic adults relative to BPD and nonclinical controls (NC). Methods A total of 724 participants (ASC = 154; BPD = 111; NC = 459) completed 11 self-reported questionnaires assessing ED, ASC and BPD traits, co-occurring disorders, alexithymia, emotional vulnerability and invalidating experiences (e.g. bullying, autistic camouflaging). The occurrence of ED behavioural correlates (i.e. self-harm, history of suicide attempts, and psychiatric hospitalizations) was collected. In addition, between-groups analyses, linear regressions and machine learning (ML) models were used to identify ED predictors in each group. Results ED and its behavioural correlates were higher in ASC compared to NC, but milder than in BPD. While gender did not predict ED scores, autistic women had increased risk factors to ED, including sexual abuse and camouflaging. Interestingly, BPD traits, emotional vulnerability and alexithymia strongly predicted ED scores across the groups. Using ML models, sensory sensitivity and autistic camouflaging were associated with ED in ASC, and ADHD symptoms with ED in BPD. Limitations ASC and BPD diagnoses were self-reported, which did not allow us to check their accuracy. Additionally, we did not explore the transactional and the moderating/mediating relationships between the different variables. Moreover, our research is cross-sectional and cannot draw conclusions regarding the direction and causality of relationships between ED and other clinical dimensions. Conclusions ED and its behavioural correlates are heightened in BPD compared to ASC and nonclinical controls. In the ASC group, there were no gender differences in ED, despite the heightened exposure of autistic women to ED risk factors. BPD traits, emotional vulnerability, and alexithymia are core to ED regardless of diagnosis. Although less central, sensory sensitivity and autistic camouflaging seem to be specific predictors of ED in autistic adults.
Article
Full-text available
Recent research has drawn attention to the prevalence of self-reported autism within online communities of involuntary celibates (incels). These studies suggest that some individuals with autism may be particularly vulnerable to the impact of incel forums and the hopelessness they generate. However, a more precise description of the experiential connection between inceldom, self-reported autism, and hopelessness has remained unarticulated. Therefore, this article combines empirical studies on the incel community with phenomenological and embodiment approaches to autism, hopelessness, and online affectivity. We analyze three interrelated aspects of online interactions in incel communities – worldview, bodily self-relation, and mutual dismissals – and examine how these elements contribute to the consolidation of the loss of significant life possibilities. By investigating the potential negative influence of specific online environments on affective dispositions, our approach contributes to the debate on current challenges to “situate” phenomenological psychopathology.
Article
Full-text available
Interoceptive accuracy refers to the ability to consciously perceive the physical condition of the inner body, including one’s heartbeat. In younger adults, interoceptive accuracy is correlated with insular and orbitofrontal cortical connectivity within the salience network (SN). As interoceptive accuracy and insular cortex volume are known to decrease with aging, we aimed to evaluate the correlation between SN connectivity and interoceptive accuracy in older adults. 27 older adults (mean age, 77.29 years, SD = 6.24; 19 female) underwent resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging, followed by a heartbeat counting task and neuropsychological test. We evaluated the correlation between interoceptive accuracy and SN connectivity with age, sex, cognitive function, and total gray matter volume as covariates. Region of interest-to-region of interest analyses showed that interoceptive accuracy was positively correlated with the functional connectivity (FC) of the left rostral prefrontal cortex with the right insular, right orbitofrontal, and anterior cingulate cortices [F(6,16) = 4.52, false discovery rate (FDR)-corrected p < 0.05]. Moreover, interoceptive accuracy was negatively correlated to the FC of the left anterior insular cortex with right intra-calcarine and visual medial cortices (F(6,16) = 2.04, FDR-corrected p < 0.10). These findings suggest that coordination between systems, with a positive correlation between left rostral prefrontal cortex and the SN and a negative correlation between left insular cortex and vision-related exteroceptive brain regions, is important for maintaining interoceptive accuracy in older adults.
Article
Full-text available
The perception of being located within one's body (i.e., bodily self-location) is an essential feature of everyday self-experience. However, by manipulating exteroceptive input, healthy participants can easily be induced to perceive themselves as being spatially dislocated from their physical bodies. It has previously been suggested that interoception, i.e., the processing of inner physiological signals, contributes to the stability of body representations; however, this relationship has not previously been tested for different dimensions of interoception and bodily self-location. In the present study, using an advanced automatized setup, we systematically manipulated participants' perspective of their own body (first-vs third-person perspective) as well as the synchrony of visuotactile stimulation (synchronous vs asynchronous). The malleability of bodily self-location was assessed using a questionnaire targeting in-body and out-of-body experiences. Participants also performed a heartbeat discrimination task to assess their interoceptive accuracy (behavioral performance), interoceptive sensibility (confidence in their interoceptive abilities), and interoceptive awareness (meta-cognitive representation of interoceptive signals). Bodily self-location was significantly influenced by perspective, with third-person perspective being associated with stronger out-of-body experiences compared to first-person perspective. Furthermore, there was a significant perspective × stimulation interaction, with subsequent analyses showing that participants reported out-of-body experiences particularly under third-person perspective combined with synchronous visuotactile stimulation. Correlation and regression analyses revealed that meta-cognitive interoceptive awareness was specifically and negatively related to the exteroceptively mediated malleability of body experiences. These results indicate that the perception of the self being located within one's body relies on the interaction of exteroceptive input and higher-order interoceptive abilities. This has implications for theoretical considerations about the bodily self in health as well as for the understanding of disturbed bodily self-processing in clinical contexts.
Preprint
Full-text available
Previous evidence suggests a dissociation between self-reported interoceptive accuracy and self-reported interoceptive attention. However, it remains unclear whether such dissociations are driven by differences in the interoceptive signals rated across these questionnaires, or a genuine dissociation between different facets of interoception (accuracy and attention). Across three studies, we examined the relationship between existing measures of self-reported interoceptive accuracy and self-reported interoceptive attention by developing a novel measure of self-reported interoceptive attention – the Interoceptive Attention Scale (IATS) – designed to match the interoceptive sensations included in an existing questionnaire measure of interoceptive accuracy. In addition, we examined whether the interpretation of questionnaire measures of interoception altered associations across measures. Results support the proposed distinction between self-reported interoceptive attention and self-reported interoceptive accuracy and highlight the importance of considering the interpretation of questionnaire measures of interoception.
Preprint
Full-text available
Interoception, the ability to detect internal bodily signals, is vital for an individual's well-being and is increasingly connected to mental health disorders. Research investigating relationships between individual differences in interoception and personality types is limited, and mixed results are reported across a variety of interoceptive tasks, measures, and conceptualisations. Guided by biological theories, this study contributed further to the understanding of the relationship between interoception and personality by utilising two interoceptive measures. A sample of adults (N ¼ 114) completed three questionnaires online questionnaire, two assessing interoceptive sensibility (Body Perception Questionnaire, BPQ; and the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness, MAIA) and one that assessed personality (Eysenck Personality Inventory, EPI). Multiple regression and correlational analyses showed no significant relationship between interoceptive sensibility and introversion, whereas a predictive relationship was demonstrated between interoceptive sensibility and neuroticism. Furthermore, the BPQ and subscales of the MAIA predicted neuroticism in different directions suggesting the two measures assess different constructs and thus strengthened support for a multidimensional consideration of interoception. The results have clinical implications, including the targeting of contemplative training
Article
Full-text available
The present study has been designed to disentangle cognitive and emotional dimensions of empathy in a group of mentally healthy and highly alexithymic individuals (ALEX, n = 24) and well-matched controls (n = 26) through questionnaire Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) and Multifaceted Empathy Task (MET) used during the fMRI and after the fMRI. Simultaneously, Skin Conductance Response (SCR) has been acquired as an implicit measure of emotional reaction. Results show an impaired emotional empathic ability in alexithymic individuals, with lower levels of SCR and higher activation in prefrontal brain regions such as the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). Cognitive empathy was not impaired in the alexithymic group and the results were accompanied by a higher activation left IFG. The study leads to the conclusion that alexithymia does not only involve a diminished ability to identify and describe one’s own emotions. Furthermore, it is related to a deeper disability of emotion regulation, which becomes visible through impaired emotional concern for others and higher levels of personal distress.
Article
Full-text available
Despite decades of speculation, many causal aspects that contribute to the heterogeneity of alexithymia still must be clarified. This study examined the extent of the alexithymia phenotype and its contribution to social function in the general population. In total, 200 participants (females = 111) completed the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 (TAS-20), multiple self-reporting questionnaires measuring emotion intelligence, empathy, hostility and impulsivity, and the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET). In the multivariate analysis, highly alexithymic individuals appeared to report subjective deficits in emotion recognition and regulation as well as increased impulsivity; however, their empathy skills were intact, and even the proneness to experiencing empathic distress with others’ suffering was increased among alexithymic individuals. We also compared the clinical and behavioral manifestations of highly alexithymic male and female subjects to those of each gender control group. As a result, in contrast to their subjective self-reports of emotion processing impairment, the RMET performance appeared to be preserved in alexithymic females; however, highly alexithymic males showed actual deficits in the emotion identification task. Future research needs to further refine the constructs of alexithymia to incorporate the phenotypic changes in affected individuals in relation to measuring instruments, the extent of empathic distress, and gender.
Article
Full-text available
IntroductionInteroception refers to awareness, interpretation, and integration of sensations in the body. While interoceptive accuracy has long been regarded as a core component of emotional experience, less is known about the relationship of interoceptive accuracy and related facets of interoception to emotion regulation deficits. This study explores how interoceptive accuracy and interoceptive sensibility relate to emotion regulation in a non-clinical sample.Methods Undergraduate participants completed a heartbeat perception task and the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (Noticing and Body Listening sub-scales), and rated their confidence in performance on the heartbeat perception task. Participants also completed self-report measures of emotional awareness and regulation (Profile of Emotional Competence, intrapersonal emotion identification and emotion regulation sub-scales), and rated their use of different coping strategies (Brief COPE).ResultsNoticing predicted emotion identification, emotion regulation, and the use of adaptive but not maladaptive coping strategies. Heartbeat perception accuracy did not significantly contribute to the prediction of any outcome variables.DiscussionFuture work is needed to extend these findings to clinical populations. The results from this study support the use of interoceptive training interventions to promote emotional wellbeing.
Article
Models of interoception highlight the importance of considering participants’ beliefs regarding their interoceptive ability. Research focusing on such beliefs suggests a dissociation between self-reported interoceptive accuracy and attention. However, it remains unclear whether such dissociations are driven by differences in the sensations rated across these questionnaires, or a genuine dissociation between different facets of self-reported interoception. Here we examined the relationship between self-report measures of interoceptive accuracy and attention using a novel measure– the Interoceptive Attention Scale– designed to match the interoceptive sensations included in an existing questionnaire measure of interoceptive accuracy. In addition, we examined whether the interpretation of questionnaire measures of interoception altered associations across measures. Results support the proposed distinction between self-reported interoceptive attention and accuracy and highlight the importance of considering the interpretation of questionnaire measures of interoception. The implications of these findings for theory, future research, and interpretation of existing research are discussed.
Article
There is growing interest in “interoception” (i.e., detection and interpretation of internal body signals) as a relevant mechanism contributing to certain symptoms and features of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and alexithymia. However, inconsistent measurement and operationalization of interoception has led to confusion and inconsistent findings in the literature. In this commentary, we present alternative interpretations of findings from existing studies to demonstrate that ASD and alexithymia are conditions associated with reduced adaptive forms of interoceptive attention (i.e., attention toward bodily signals) but heightened maladaptive forms of interoceptive attention related to anxiety‐induced somatization. Differentiating adaptive and maladaptive forms of interoceptive attention reveals a clearer pattern of findings in the research literature for further investigation of interoceptive processes that are involved in the neurobiology of ASD. However, interoception is a complex and multi‐faceted construct that requires continual refinement in conceptualization and operationalization. Interoception research may benefit from self‐report measures that clearly differentiate adaptive and maladaptive forms of bodily awareness. Lay Summary Some research suggests that autistic people have difficulty understanding bodily feelings such as hunger, illness, or emotions, whereas some studies have reported the opposite pattern of findings. We argue that this latter subset of studies reached false conclusions from using measures of bodily awareness that largely measure physical symptoms of anxiety. While attention to unpleasant bodily signals is an important ability necessary for maintaining healthy bodily functioning, excessive attention, and worry toward bodily signals can increase anxiety and be harmful.
Article
Objective While it is well known that mental health problems are common consequences of deadly pandemics, the association with alexithymia is less clear. This study examined this association in an evaluation of home-quarantined university students during the 2019/2020 COVID-19 pandemic in China. Methods In total, 2501 home-quarantined students from six southwest Chinese universities completed the following questionnaires: the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20), the Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist-Civilian Version (PCLC), and the Patients Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9), after which structural equation modeling (SEM) and mediation analyses were employed to extract and evaluate the possible associations. Results It was found that participants with probable depression or PTSD also reported more severe alexithymia features, such as difficulties in identifying feelings (DIF) or describing feelings (DDF). Alexithymia was also found to partially mediate the effect of number of exposures on mental health problems. Conclusion These results suggested that implementing strategies to assist young people identify and deal with their own emotions and those of others could prevent or mitigate the mental health problems associated with deadly pandemic events. However, future longitudinal studies are needed to examine the specific involvement of DIF or DDF in people with mental health problems.