ArticlePDF Available

Clarity of task difficulty moderates the impact of the explicit achievement motive on physical effort in hand grip tasks

PLOS
PLOS One
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Stable personality dispositions, like motives, are often assumed to exert a direct, stable impact on behavior. This also applies to the explicit achievement motive, which is supposed to influence the behavior that individuals select and how strongly they engage in it. Drawing on motivational intensity theory, we demonstrated in two studies that explicit achievement motive strength only predicted exerted force in a hand grip task if task difficulty was unclear. If task difficulty was clear, explicit achievement motive strength did not influence exerted force. Our findings suggest that the availability of information about the difficulty of motive satisfaction moderates the impact of the explicit achievement motive on behavior.
This content is subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Clarity of task difficulty moderates the impact
of the explicit achievement motive on physical
effort in hand grip tasks
Michael RichterID
1
*, Florence Mazeres
2
, Kerstin BrinkmannID
2
1Effort Lab, School of Psychology, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, United Kingdom, 2Geneva
Motivation Lab, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
These authors contributed equally to this work.
*m.richter@ljmu.ac.uk,m.richter2020@outlook.com
Abstract
Stable personality dispositions, like motives, are often assumed to exert a direct, stable
impact on behavior. This also applies to the explicit achievement motive, which is supposed
to influence the behavior that individuals select and how strongly they engage in it. Drawing
on motivational intensity theory, we demonstrated in two studies that explicit achievement
motive strength only predicted exerted force in a hand grip task if task difficulty was unclear.
If task difficulty was clear, explicit achievement motive strength did not influence exerted
force. Our findings suggest that the availability of information about the difficulty of motive
satisfaction moderates the impact of the explicit achievement motive on behavior.
Introduction
One of the main reasons for the interest in personality traits is the promise that they have stable
links with behavior and consequently allow the prediction of behavior [1,2]. This also applies to
motives, a complementary personality construct that refers to the disposition—originating
from childhood learning experiences—to strive for certain types of incentives[35]. All three
main classes of motives that have been examined in the McClelland tradition [4], the achieve-
ment motive, the affiliation motive, and the power motive, lead individuals to engage in motive-
relevant tasks. For instance, individuals with a strong achievement motive are considered to
experience meeting standards of excellence as rewarding and therefore to seek to attain and
exceed such standards [6]. Consequently, a strong achievement motive is postulated to drive
individuals to actively select tasks that are instrumental for motive satisfaction (i.e., tasks that
allow them to attain excellence). Moreover, it should make them willing to invest more effort
and to be more persistent in such tasks [4,7,8]. Like research on the predictive power of per-
sonality traits, the literature on motives has strongly focused on behavior initiation and perfor-
mance (i.e., how well individuals perform a task) [9,10]. Other qualities of behavior, like effort
(i.e., the investment of resources to perform a task) or disengagement, have been less frequently
examined. The aim of this article is to extend the existing literature on motives by providing a
theoretical analysis of the motive-effort relationship based on motivational intensity theory
PLOS ONE
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 1 / 19
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Richter M, Mazeres F, Brinkmann K
(2021) Clarity of task difficulty moderates the
impact of the explicit achievement motive on
physical effort in hand grip tasks. PLoS ONE 16(6):
e0252713. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.
pone.0252713
Editor: Frantisek Sudzina, Aalborg University,
DENMARK
Received: January 5, 2021
Accepted: May 20, 2021
Published: June 4, 2021
Copyright: ©2021 Richter et al. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited.
Data Availability Statement: All collected data are
permanently and openly accessible at https://doi.
org/10.24377/LJMU.d.00000077.
Funding: This work was supported by the Swiss
National Science Foundation (http://www.snf.ch)
grant 100019_162389 awarded to Kerstin
Brinkmann (KB) and Michael Richter (MR). The
funders had no role in study design, data collection
and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of
the manuscript.
[11], and to provide a test of this framework by examining the effect of the explicit achievement
motive on effort under conditions of clear and unclear task difficulty.
Motivational intensity theory suggests that two variables determine the amount of effort
that one invests in tasks that are instrumental for attaining personal goals: task difficulty and
success importance (i.e., the importance of successfully performing the instrumental task).
Two additional variables, clarity of task difficulty and type of difficulty standard (fixed vs.
unfixed) are predicted to determine how task difficulty and success importance influence
effort. If task difficulty is clear and fixed, effort should be a function of task difficulty (the
higher the difficulty, the higher the effort) up to a point where the required effort is no longer
justified by the importance of succeeding in the task. If the required effort exceeds the amount
of maximally justified effort, individuals should disengage and invest no effort. The underlying
assumption is that individuals aim to avoid wasting resources that may be important for sur-
vival [11,12]. Using task difficulty as an indicator of what is required allows individuals to
attain this goal. If the difficulty of a task is not clear (i.e., unclear or unpredictable) or not fixed
(i.e., the performer is free to perform the task at any difficulty level), task difficulty cannot be
used as an indicator of what is required in these situations. Consequently, individuals use the
next best indicator that allows them to minimize the waste of resources, and success impor-
tance should directly determine effort: the higher the success importance, the higher the effort
[1113]. Success importance will not allow individuals to invest exactly what is required, but it
will allow them to avoid investing more than justified [12]. Motivational intensity theory can
be used to derive predictions for other variables by identifying the theory’s components that
should be affected by these variables [14]. For instance, researchers demonstrated that ability
[15], gender [16], and pain [17] exert their impact on effort via influencing perceived task diffi-
culty. Need for closure [18], grit [19], and ego involvement [20] influence effort via changes in
perceived success importance. Some variables, like mood, implicit affect, and dysphoria, can
affect both task demand and success importance [2124].
Given that motives refer to how important it is for individuals to attain specific types of
incentives, motives should influence effort by changing perceived success importance. In the
case of the achievement motive, individuals with a strong motive should be more interested in
succeeding in tasks that allow them to attain and demonstrate excellence (i.e., achievement
tasks) than individuals with a weak motive. The strength of the achievement motive should
thus determine how important success is in achievement tasks and how much effort one is
willing to maximally invest in such tasks. In tasks with clear and fixed difficulty, motive
strength should consequently not exert a direct impact on effort but limit the difficulty-effort
relationship by determining the point at which individuals consider the required effort no lon-
ger to be justified and disengage. In task with unclear or unfixed task difficulty, motive
strength should directly determine how much effort individuals invest in the task. The clarity
of task difficulty and the type of difficulty standard should thus determine whether motives
exert a direct or indirect influence on effort investment. Fig 1 displays these predictions for the
impact of motive strength on effort.
It is important to note that the popular distinction between implicit and explicit motives [8,
25] does not translate into different predictions for implicit and explicit achievement motives
regarding their effects on effort. Given that both subclasses of the achievement motive refer to
the striving to attain and exceed standards of excellence, they should both exert their impact
on effort by increasing the importance of succeeding in tasks that promise motive satisfaction.
Implicit and explicit motives differ in many aspects [8,25], like accessibility, specific behav-
ioral consequences, or activating situational characteristics, but they should not differ regard-
ing the mechanism by which they exert their impact on effort. For instance, the implicit
achievement motive (need for achievement, nAch) refers to the nonconscious disposition to
PLOS ONE
Explicit achievement motive and physical effort
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 2 / 19
Competing interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist.
strive for excellence whereas the explicit achievement motive (self-attributed need for achieve-
ment, sanAch) refers to the self-attributed disposition of pursuing standards of excellence.
Correspondingly, the explicit achievement motive can be assessed via self-reports, but the
implicit motive needs to be measured using projective, implicit measures. Implicit motives are
more predictive of spontaneous and non-declarative processes whereas explicit motives are
better predictors of deliberate and declarative processes [2527]. The implicit achievement
motive is activated by tasks that offer an opportunity to improve one’s performance, whereas
explicit, external challenges to demonstrate one’s excellence and enable social comparison acti-
vate the explicit achievement motive [8]. Explicit and implicit achievement motives may thus
become activated in different types of situations, but both should exert their impact on effort
by increasing the perceived success importance of tasks that are instrumental for motive satis-
faction. The observable effects of explicit and implicit achievement motives may differ depend-
ing on the specific context, but their impact on effort should always be mediated by the same
mechanism: changes in perceived success importance.
There is already research relevant to our analysis of the impact of the achievement motive
on effort. The most direct evidence comes from a recent study by Mazeres and colleagues [28]
in which they examined the influence of the implicit achievement motive on effort under con-
ditions of clear and fixed task difficulty. In line with our theoretical analysis, the strength of the
achievement motive had an indirect impact on effort under these conditions: Effort increased
with increasing task difficulty in the strong-achievement-motive group but was low and inde-
pendent of task difficulty in the weak-achievement-motive group. Brunstein and Schmitt [7]
described a similar interaction between the implicit achievement motive and task difficulty.
Individuals with a strong implicit achievement motive invested more effort in a moderately
difficult task than in an easy or a very difficult task, whereas individuals with a weak implicit
achievement motive invested low effort independent of task difficulty. Capa and colleagues
Fig 1. Theoretical predictions for the impact of the explicit achievement motive on effort.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713.g001
PLOS ONE
Explicit achievement motive and physical effort
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 3 / 19
[29,30] found similar results examining the impact of the explicit achievement motive on
effort. Individuals with a large positive difference between the motive to achieve success and
the motive to avoid failure increased their effort investment as a function of increasing task
demand. This adaption to task demand was not observed in individuals with a large negative
difference between the two achievement motive components. In an applied context, Wegner
and Schu¨ler [31] showed that the implicit achievement motive predicted the effort invested in
playing racquet games, which can be considered tasks with an unfixed task difficulty given that
the difficulty level of each individual stroke is to some extent under the control of the
individual.
There are thus already a few studies that revealed achievement motive effects congruent
with our theoretical analysis. However, most of these studies focused on the interaction
between the achievement motive and task difficulty under conditions of clear and fixed task
difficulty. Empirical research has not yet examined the prediction that clarity of task difficulty
determines the impact that the achievement motive has on effort. The presented studies aimed
to close this gap by examining the impact of the strength of the explicit achievement motive on
physical effort invested in hand grip tasks with clear and unclear difficulty. Following our theo-
retical analysis presented in the preceding paragraphs, we expected effort (i.e., the force exerted
in the hand grip task) to be a direct function of the explicit achievement motive if participants
worked on a task with an unclear difficulty. If participants worked on a task with a clear diffi-
culty, we expected no relationship between the strength of the explicit achievement motive
and effort. These hypotheses were not explicitly pre-registered but all predictions, methods,
and analyses had been described in the Swiss National Science Foundation grant
100019_162390 before data collection started. All collected data are permanently and openly
accessible at https://doi.org/10.24377/LJMU.d.00000077.
Material and methods
Participants and design
All presented studies have been conducted with the formal approval (approval number 17/
NSP/055) of the ethic review committee (UREC) of Liverpool John Moores University (Liver-
pool, UK). All participants provided written consent. Sample size of Study 1 was determined
in an a priori power analysis using GPower [32] (alpha = .05, power = .80). Drawing on pre-
ceding work on achievement motive effects in physical tasks [31,33,34], we assumed an effect
size of ρ= .40. We used the same sample size for Study 2, which led to a low power of .34 for
the comparison of the correlation coefficients between the two difficulty conditions. All partic-
ipants were recruited on the campus of a UK University. In Study 1, 40 participants performed
a hand grip task with an unclear task difficulty and completed the achievement (sanAch), affili-
ation (sanAff), and dominance/power scales (sanPow) of the Personality Research Form (PRF)
[35]. Due to a programming error, gender and age data of these participants were not recorded
in this study. In Study 2, 40 participants (M
age
= 27.75, SD = 14.94 for the entire sample, M
age
= 26.10, SD = 15.24 in the clear-difficulty condition, M
age
= 29.40, SD = 14.83 in the unclear-
difficulty condition) performed a hand grip task with either an unclear task difficulty or a
clear, easy difficulty standard before completing the achievement, affiliation, and power PRF
scales. Allocation to the two difficulty standard conditions (unclear vs. clear) was randomized
with the constraint that there had to be 20 participants in each condition. There were three
male and 17 female participants in the clear-difficulty condition and eight male and 12 female
participants in the unclear-difficulty condition. Both studies include three main elements:
maximum force trials, the main hand grip task, and the PRF achievement, affiliation, and
power scales.
PLOS ONE
Explicit achievement motive and physical effort
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 4 / 19
Tasks and questionnaires
Maximum force trials. In the maximum force trials participants were asked to squeeze a
hand dynamometer (Vernier, Beaverton, OR) as hard as possible during a two-second mea-
surement period. During the measurement period, the force that participants were exerting
was presented on-screen. Moreover, after the measurement period, the maximum force
exerted during the preceding measurement period was presented on-screen (“The maximum
force that you exerted in this trial was XXX Newton”). During all maximum force trials, the
experimenter, who was hired and unaware of the hypotheses, was in the same room as the par-
ticipants and encouraged them to squeeze has hard as they could by asking them to exceed
their maximum force of the preceding trial. Participants performed three maximum force tri-
als before the main hand grip task and three maximum force trials after it. The main purpose
of the assessment of participants’ maximum force was to control for interindividual differences
in hand grip strength (e.g., gender-related difference [36]).
Hand grip task. The hand grip task followed a paradigm successfully employed in previ-
ous studies on motivational intensity theory [37,38]. In the hand grip task, participants were
asked to imagine that the dynamometer represented a clogged Ketchup bottle that they could
free by exerting a force equal to or higher than a force standard (i.e., a specific force in Newton)
determined by the computer. To support this cover story, a black-and-white drawing of a
hand holding a reversed Ketchup bottle was presented on-screen throughout the task and a
black-and-white drawing of a hand holding a bottle ejecting Ketchup replaced the default
image at the end of successful trials. The task consisted of 60 trials each one starting with a
four-second countdown followed by a two-second measurement period and a three-second
feedback period. The force that participants were exerting on the dynamometer during the
measurement period was measured and compared to the trial’s force standard. If the peak
(maximum) exerted force was equal to or greater than the standard, the trial counted as suc-
cess and the positive feedback drawing was displayed during the feedback period. If a partici-
pant’s peak (maximum) exerted force was less than the standard, the trial counted as failure
and the presented drawing did not change (i.e., the default drawing of a hand holding a
reversed Ketchup bottle remained on the screen). The force exerted in this isometric hand grip
task is proportional to the amount of energy (adenosine triphosphate) invested for task success
and provides thus a valid measure to test effort-related predictions in instrumental tasks [37].
To create a task version with unclear, unpredictable difficulty (Study 1 and Study 2), force
standards were varied from trial to trial. Thirty different force standards were presented twice
in random order to create the 60 task trials. To ensure that each participant would receive
roughly the same number of possible and impossible trials, the list of 30 force standards was
determined individually for each participant using participants’ maximum force assessed in
the first three maximum force trials. If a participant’s maximum force exceeded 145 Newton
(N), force standards were created by subtracting 120 N from the participant’s maximum force
and increasing the force standard in a stepwise manner by adding 5 N. This resulted in 25
force standards equal to or below a participant’s maximum force and five force standards
above the participant’s maximum force. For instance, if a participant’s pre-task maximum
force was 181 N, the used force standards were 61 N, 66 N, 71 N,. . ., up to 206 N. If a partici-
pant’s maximum force did not exceed 145 N (one participant in Study 1, six participants in
Study 2), 30 force standards ranging from 25 N to 170 N were used. To create the task version
with clear difficulty (Study 2), the same easy force standard of 60 N (corresponding to the
strength of a weak, low-effort handshake [39,40]) was used in all trials and for all participants.
To increase the likelihood that the hand grip task would activate the explicit achievement
motive, we provided participants with two sources of feedback about their performance during
PLOS ONE
Explicit achievement motive and physical effort
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 5 / 19
the task. First, exerted force was continuously displayed during the measurement period. Sec-
ond, during a three-second feedback period following the measurement, the peak force that
participants had exerted in the preceding measurement period was displayed together with the
required force standard. This setting—together with the explicit instruction that participants’
task was to free the Ketchup bottle (see section 2.3)—should have resulted in participants per-
ceiving the type of externally set challenge presented in verbal-symbolic format that promises
potential satisfaction of the explicit achievement motive and thus activates it [8,26,41].
PRF. Participants completed a computer-based version (presented using Inquisit by Milli-
second) of the achievement, affiliation, and dominance/power scales of the Personality
Research Form [35] to assess their levels of sanAch, sanAff, and sanPow. Each scale included
16 descriptive statements (for example, “I will not be satisfied until I am the best in my field of
work”, “I spend a lot of time visiting friends”, or “I try to control others rather than permit
them to control me”) and participants indicated for each statement whether it described them-
selves or not. Statements from the three scales were presented alternating. sanAch constituted
our main variable of interest. sanAff and sanPow were assessed to compare their impact on
exerted force in the hand grip task with the impact of sanAch, which may be of interest to read-
ers interested in the relative impact of sanAff and sanPow in this specific achievement motive-
arousing situation.
Procedure
After having provided informed consent, participants were seated at a table in front of a com-
puter screen and introduced to the hand dynamometer that was mounted at the table in a ver-
tical position. They learned that the device measured grip force exerted on it and were then
allowed to freely explore its functionality during the next 30 seconds. During this time, the
force that participants were exerting on the dynamometer was displayed on the screen, and the
experimenter encouraged participants to exert varying levels of force to see how the device
responded. After this familiarization period, participants performed three maximum force tri-
als (see the maximum force trails section for details) before the experimenter gave them a
sheet with instructions for the main hand grip task and left the room. For the duration of the
hand grip task, participants remained alone in the room. The instruction sheet informed par-
ticipants that they would be performing 60 trials of a hand grip task. They were asked to imag-
ine that the dynamometer represented a clogged Ketchup bottle that they could free by
exerting a force equal to or higher than a force standard set by the computer. All participants
received the general information about the hand grip task procedure presented in the hand
grip task section but did not receive any information about the specific force standards prior
to or during the measurement period. Participants in the unclear task difficulty conditions
(Study 1 and Study 2) learned that the force standards would vary randomly from one trial to
the next one, whereas participants in the clear task difficulty condition (Study 2) were
informed that the force standard would be the same in every trial. After having performed the
60 hand grip trials, participants performed again three maximum force trials in the presence of
the experimenter. Participants then completed the achievement, affiliation, and dominance/
power scales of Jacksons’ Personality Research Form. Finally, all participants were carefully
debriefed and given an Amazon voucher worth GBP 5 for their participation.
Data preparation and analysis
The PRF subscale items were added up to obtain participants’ sanAch, sanAff, and sanPow
scores. All scales had acceptable internal consistency (Study 1: ω
t
= .70 for sanAch, .86 for san-
Pow, .83 for sanAff; Study 2: ω
t
= .71 for sanAch, .86 for sanPow, .80 for sanAff). Given that
PLOS ONE
Explicit achievement motive and physical effort
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 6 / 19
the dynamometer sampled exerted force at 10 Hz during the two-second measurement peri-
ods, 20 force values were available for each trial. The highest value of the 20 force values of a
trial was used as trial peak force value and a force-time-integral (FTI) was computed by adding
up all 20 force values [42]. FTIs thus indicated the total amount of effort exerted during the
two-second measurement periods. However, peak force constituted our primary measure
given that it was instrumental for success (exerted peak force was compared with the force
standard to determine success in a trial).
The highest peak force value during the maximum force trials was used as a participant’s
maximum force score. In Study 1, this was based on all six maximum force trials but in Study
2, we only used the three post-task maximum force trials to determine maximum force scores
given that a programming error prevented our software from saving exerted force during the
pre-task maximum force trials in the clear-difficulty condition. For each participant, we com-
puted a task peak force score as arithmetic average of all 60 peak force values.
In both studies, we tested our predictions about the impact of sanAch on exerted force
under conditions of unclear task difficulty by calculating Pearson correlation coefficients
between peak force scores and sanAch. In Study 2, this analysis only considered participants in
the unclear-task difficulty condition. We also tested the relative importance of sanAch as pre-
dictor of exerted force by comparing the Pearson correlation coefficients between the three
motive scores (achievement, affiliation, and power) and peak force scores using Dunn and
Clark’s z[43] and by calculating Bayes Factors comparing regression models that tested for a
positive association between the individual motive scores and peak force scores [44,45]. In
Study 2, we additionally compared the difference between sanAch impact under conditions of
unclear and clear difficulty by comparing the Pearson correlation coefficients between sanAch
and peak force scores between both difficulty conditions using Fisher’s zand by calculating
Bayes Factors that compared the fit of a regression model that only included sanAch as predic-
tor with a model that included the interaction of sanAch with difficulty as predictor. All analy-
ses were conducted using R [46] and the psych [47], cocor [48], and BICpack [49] packages. P-
value based tests that addressed the following a-priori directional hypotheses were conducted
one-tailed: 1) the postulated positive relationship between sanAch strength and exerted force
under conditions of unclear task difficulty, 2) the prediction that sanAch would be more posi-
tively associated with exerted force than sanAff or sanPow under conditions of unclear task
difficulty, and 3) the hypothesis that the sanAch-exerted-force relationship would be more
positive under conditions of unclear task difficulty than under conditions of fixed task diffi-
culty. All other p-value based tests were conducted two-tailed. The strength of evidence pro-
vided by the observed Bayes Factors was interpreted according to Andraszewicz et al. [50]
modification of Jeffreys’ classification scheme.
Results
Study 1
Maximum force trials. Participants’ mean maximum force was 230.76 N (SD = 68.69;
pre-task M= 222.01 N, SD = 72.74, range = 85.08–467.40; post-task M= 212.60 N, SD = 69.73,
range = 98.10–476.12). There was no evidence that exerted maximum force differed before
and after the task, t(39) = 1.84, p= .07, d= 0.41, 95% CI [-0.03, 0.85]. Participants’ maximum
force was significantly correlated with task peak force, r(38) = .77, p<.001, 95% CI [.61, .87],
and task FTI, r(38) = .65, p<.001, 95% CI [.43, .80]. Moreover, maximum force was signifi-
cantly correlated with sanAff, r(38) = -.58, p<.001, 95% CI [-.76, -.33], but not with sanAch, r
(38) = .12, p= .45, 95% CI [-.20, .42], or sanPow, r(38) = -.16, p= .31, 95% CI [-.45, .15]. Given
the significant relationship between force exerted during the main task and participants’
PLOS ONE
Explicit achievement motive and physical effort
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 7 / 19
maximum force and given that it is common to use relative force values in hand grip studies
[5153], we decided to analyze relative peak force values expressed in percentages of maxi-
mum force.
Relationship between exerted force and motive scores. Means and standard deviations
of force values and motive scores as well as the correlation coefficients between these variables
can be found in Table 1. As hypothesized, participants’ sanAch was positively associated with
relative peak force (p= .005): The higher a participant’s sanAch, the higher the relative force
that she/he exerted in the hand grip task. Fig 2 illustrates this relationship and Fig 3 presents
the evolution of exerted force across the 2-second measurement period as a function of
sanAch. Participants’ sanAch was also positively correlated with absolute force (p= .01), but
there was no evidence for a positive relationship between sanAch and FTI (p= .08).
Demonstrating the strong relative impact of sanAch as predictor of exerted force in the
hand grip task, the correlation coefficient between sanAch and relative force was significantly
more positive than the correlation coefficient between sanAff and relative force, z= 1.71, p=
.04. However, even if the correlation coefficient between sanAch and relative force was more
positive in absolute terms than the correlation coefficient between sanPow and relative force,
this difference was not significant, z= 1.46, p= .07. The Bayes Factors provided strong evi-
dence in favor of the sanAch model in comparison with the sanPow (BF = 28.50) and sanAff
(BF = 58.03) models demonstrating that sanAch was more predictive of relative peak force
than sanPow or sanAff.
Study 2
Maximum force trials. Participants’ mean maximum force scores were 227.86 N
(SD = 69.66, range = 136.37–438.39) in the unclear-difficulty condition and 201.37 N
(SD = 72.37, range = 83.32–322.68) in the clear-difficulty condition. There was no evidence
that exerted maximum force differed between the two difficulty standard conditions, t(38) =
1.18, p= .25, d= 0.37, 95% CI [-0.25, 1.00]. Maximum force scores were significantly corre-
lated with task peak force scores, r(38) = .73, p<.001, 95% CI [.55, .85], and task FTI, r(38) =
.37, p= .02, 95% CI [.07, .61]. They were also significantly associated with sanPow, r(38) = .34,
p= .03, 95% CI [.03, .59], but not with sanAch, r(38) = -.07, p= .67, 95% CI [-.37, .25], or
sanAff, r(38) = -.03, p= .83, 95% CI [-.34, .28]. Given the relationship between participants’
maximum force and the force exerted during the main task as well as to be consistent with
Study 1, we decided to focus our analysis again on relative peak force scores expressed in per-
centages of maximum force.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics and Pearson correlation coefficients for force values and motive scores in Study 1.
Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Relative peak force
a
68.16 11.06
2. Absolute peak force
b
156.77 52.20 .54 [.28, .73]
3. FTI
b
1809.35 694.38 .28 [-.03, .55] .75 [.58, .86]
4. sanAch 9.82 3.19 .40 [.16, 1.00] .36 [.10, 1.00] .23 [-.03, 1.00]
5. sanPow 8.30 3.97 .14 [-.18, .43] -.04 [-.35, .28] -.01 [-.32, .30] .33 [.03, .59]
6. sanAff 10.62 3.77 .03 [-.28, .34] -.43 [-.65, -.14] -.49 [-.69, -.21] .05 [-.27, .35] .26 [-.06, .53]
To prevent type-I error inflation, p-value based tests were only conducted for the correlation coefficients central to our hypotheses. The associated p-values are
presented in the text. 95% confidence interval limits are presented in square brackets.
a
Relative peak force is expressed in percentage of maximum force.
b
Absolute peak force and FTI are in Newton.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713.t001
PLOS ONE
Explicit achievement motive and physical effort
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 8 / 19
Relationship between exerted force and motive scores. Table 2 displays means, standard
deviations, and bivariate correlations of force and motive scores. As predicted, sanAch was
positively correlated with relative peak force in the unclear-difficulty condition (p= .04) but
not in the clear-difficulty condition (p= .72). There was no evidence for positive relationships
between sanAch and absolute peak force or FTI in the unclear-difficulty (ps>.13) or clear-dif-
ficulty conditions (ps>.31). Fig 4 illustrates the relationship between sanAch and relative
peak force in the two difficulty conditions and Fig 5 shows the evolution of exerted force across
the 2-second measurement period as a function of sanAch and difficulty condition.
Fig 2. Relationship between explicit achievement motive strength and relative exerted force in Study 1. The dashed line shows the best fit regression
line.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713.g002
PLOS ONE
Explicit achievement motive and physical effort
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 9 / 19
A comparison of the correlation coefficients of sanAch with relative peak force scores
between the unclear-difficulty and the clear-difficulty conditions was significant, z= 1.68, p=
.05, supporting our hypothesis that the relationship between sanAch and exerted force would
be more positive under conditions of unclear difficulty than under conditions of clear diffi-
culty. Moreover, the Bayes Factors provided strong evidence in favor of the regression model
including the sanAch-difficulty interaction as predictor and against the model including only
sanAch as predictor (BF = 28.91).
Fig 3. Evolution of relative exerted force during a trial as a function of explicit achievementmotive strength in Study 1. The graph is based on
predicted values generated from regression equations for individuals one SD above and below the mean explicit achievement motive (sanAch) strength.
The x-axis refers to the 20 force samples collected during each task trial.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713.g003
PLOS ONE
Explicit achievement motive and physical effort
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 10 / 19
Further demonstrating the relative impact of sanAch as predictor of exerted force under
conditions of unclear task difficulty, the correlation coefficient between sanAch and relative
peak force in the unclear-difficulty condition was significantly more positive than the
Table 2. Descriptive statistics and Pearson correlation coefficients for force and motive scores in Study 2.
Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6
Unclear difficulty
1. Relative peak force
a
75.29 10.81
2. Absolute peak force
b
172.26 63.21 .48 [.05, .76]
3. FTI
b
1923.78 653.70 .27 [-.20, .63] .49 [.06, .77]
4. sanAch 10.85 2.48 .41
c
[.04, 1.00] -.22 [-.56, 1.00] .26 [-.14, 1.00]
5. sanPow 7.95 4.27 -.24 [-.61, .23] .03 [-.41, .47] .18 [-.28, .58] -.19 [-.59, .27]
6. sanAff 10.30 3.96 .25 [-.21, .62] .14 [-.31, .55] -.13 [-.54, .33] .04 [-.41, .48] .37 [-.09, .70]
Clear difficulty
1. Relative peak force
a
58.66 20.33
2. Absolute peak force
b
114.01 53.12 .58 [.18, .81]
3. FTI
b
1168.78 768.62 .77 [.50, .91] .86 [.66, .94]
4. sanAch 9.25 2.86 -.14
c
[-.49, 1.00] .02 [-.36, 1.00] .12 [-.27, 1.00]
5. sanPow 6.05 3.91 -.02 [-.46, .42] .42 [-.03, .72] .31 [-.15, .66] .32 [-.14, .67]
6. sanAff 10.45 3.09 .14 [-.33, .55] .07 [-.38, .50] .20 [-.27, .59] .06 [-.39, .49] -.10 [-.52, .36]
To prevent type-I error inflation, p-value based tests were only conducted for correlation coefficients central to our hypotheses. The associated p-values are presented in
the text. 95% confidence interval limits are presented in square brackets.
a
Relative peak force is expressed in percentage of maximum force.
b
Absolute peak force and FTI are in Newton.
c
Controlling for gender did virtually not change the correlation coefficients between sanAch and relative peak force.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713.t002
Fig 4. Relationship between explicit achievement motive strength and relative exerted force in Study 2. Dashed lines are best fit regression lines.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713.g004
PLOS ONE
Explicit achievement motive and physical effort
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 11 / 19
correlation coefficient between sanPow and relative peak force in that condition, z= 1.80, p=
.04. However, the correlation coefficients of sanAch and sanAff with relative peak force in the
unclear-difficulty condition did not differ from one another, z= 0.52, p= .30. Bayes Factors
also provided moderate to strong evidence in favor of the regression model including sanAch
as predictor of relative peak force under conditions of unclear difficulty and against the models
using sanPow (BF = 22.91) and sanAff (BF = 3.70) as predictors.
Fig 5. Evolution of relative exerted force during a trial as a function of explicit achievementmotive strength in Study 2. The graphs are based on
predicted values generated from regression equations for individuals one SD above and below the mean explicit achievement motive (sanAch) strength.
The x-axis refers to the 20 force samples collected during each task trial.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713.g005
PLOS ONE
Explicit achievement motive and physical effort
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 12 / 19
Discussion
In both studies, we found the predicted positive relationship between the strength of the
explicit achievement motive and effort under conditions of unclear, unpredictable task diffi-
culty: the higher the strength of the achievement motive, the higher the force that participants
exerted. Study 2 replicated the positive relationship found in Study 1 and revealed that this
relationship was specific to tasks with unclear difficulty. If the difficulty of the task was clear,
the strength of the explicit achievement motive did not predict exerted force. Replicating pre-
ceding work [5456], we also observed that the explicit achievement motive was more predic-
tive of effort investment than the explicit power or affiliation motives. As expected, the
employed hand grip task constituted an achievement-related context that activated the explicit
achievement motive more than the power or affiliation motives.
Our results may seem obvious from a motivational intensity theory perspective but it
should be noted that the achievement motive literature has not yet fully adopted the idea that
aroused motives do not always lead to increased effort, persistence, or performance. A large
part of the motive research is still based on the assumption that motives exert a direct impact
on behavior once that they have been activated by an opportunity for motive satisfaction [9,
57,58]. There are a few exceptions that provided more sophisticated models on how motives
interact with situational characteristics to influence different aspects of behavior [2527,59]
but many publications have an explanatory gap between the aroused motive and the behavioral
consequences. For instance, it often remains unclear how a high motive strength should lead
to increased performance. Assuming that high motive strength leads to increased effort and
persistence, which in turn increases performance, does not fully close the explanatory gap
given that effort and persistence are only two of several determinants of performance [60,61].
Even if our work does not address performance effects in the terms of success and failure, it
challenges the idea that motive activation is sufficient for motive effects on behavior. It clearly
demonstrates that an activated explicit achievement motive only has a direct impact on the
effort invested in instrumental behavior if the difficulty of the behavior is unclear or unpredict-
able. If the difficulty of the behavior is well-known and fixed to a certain difficulty standard
(i.e., there is a known and clear criterion that determines success and failure), an activated
explicit achievement motive does not directly affect effort investment.
It might seem surprising that we predicted and found effects of the explicit achievement
motive on effort given that the energetic and performance-related aspects of behavior have
often been associated with the implicit motive, not the explicit motive [10,26,62]. However, a
closer examination of the behavior that we assessed—exerting force in the Ketchup hand grip
task—reveals that it has many features that have been attributed to behavior that should be
influenced by the explicit achievement motive [25,27]. For instance, exerting force in the hand
grip task constitutes certainly not a spontaneous behavior but a deliberate decision to exert
force. Moreover, it is more respondent behavior than operant behavior given that there were
specific stimuli eliciting the response (for instance, the instruction to squeeze the dynamome-
ter to free the Ketchup bottle). Exerted force also represents a declarative measure of motiva-
tion given that exerting force in the hand grip task is a voluntary, consciously controlled
process. In sum, even if exerted force might look at first sight to be a behavioral response for
which implicit motive strength should be predictive, a closer look reveals that exerting force in
the Ketchup hand grip tasks constitutes the type of deliberate, controlled, and respondent
behavior for which explicit motive strength should be relevant.
Our work differs from a large part of the preceding work on motivational intensity theory
[14] regarding the employed effort measure. Instead of using sympathetic-driven cardiovascu-
lar measures to test our effort-related predictions, we assessed the force exerted in an isometric
PLOS ONE
Explicit achievement motive and physical effort
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 13 / 19
hand grip task. Using exerted force in the presented work had a number of advantages. First, a
lot of the current work on the achievement motive examines its impact on physical, mainly
sports-related tasks [9,33,34]. Assessing physical effort in a motor task is obviously more rele-
vant to this work than cardiovascular responses associated with mental effort. Second, it only
requires a couple of seconds to measure the force response in one trial. Consequently, many
trials can be presented in a task to collect a large number of effort measurements. Given that
each individual measurement is affected by random noise resulting from factors not related to
the main manipulations (e.g., the participant being distracted by a noise outside the lab and
missing the start of the measurement period), a larger number of effort measurements makes
it more likely that the average of the individual measurements accurately captures the effect of
the main manipulations on effort. Third, exerted force provides a good indication of the rela-
tive amount of energy used to execute the hand grip task and provides thus a valid measure to
test motivational intensity theory’s predictions related to energy investment (i.e., effort). Given
the close connection between exerted force in isometric tasks and the amount of consumed
adenosine triphosphate—the primary energy for most energy requiring actions of the human
body—exerted force enables specific tests of the determinants of the amount of energetic
resources that are invested in instrumental tasks [37]. Independent of the differences with the
cardiovascular measures employed in most of the research on motivational intensity theory,
our findings fit well with preceding work on the theory. Like other researchers who found that
variables related to success importance exert a direct impact on effort under conditions of
unclear and unfixed task difficulty but not under conditions of fixed task difficulty [14,63,64],
we observed that the impact of the explicit achievement motive was moderated by clarity of
task difficulty.
A critical reader might wonder about the implications of the confound between clarity of
difficulty and task difficulty in Study 2. Given that we presented a broad range of force stan-
dards (including five standards that exceeded participants’ maximum force) in the unclear
condition, the average difficulty in this condition was higher than in the clear condition where
we only presented low-difficulty trials with a force standard of 60 Newton. This confound
does, however, not challenge our interpretation in terms of clarity of difficulty given that an
interpretation in terms of fixed and clear task difficulty fails to explain the observed relation-
ship between achievement motive strength and exerted force. If participants had perceived the
unclear difficulty task as one with a clear and high difficulty, their exerted force should not
have increased in proportion to their achievement motive strength. According to motivational
intensity theory, task difficulty directly determines effort if participants have a clear idea about
task difficulty. Achievement motive strength, as a variable that influences success importance,
should not have a direct impact. Correspondingly, exerted force should either have been high
for all participants independent of their achievement motive strength or shown a twofold pat-
tern with some participants exerting a high force and others disengaging. Fig 4 clearly shows
that this was not the case. The only theory-based interpretation that can explain the finding
that achievement motive strength was associated with exerted force in one condition but not
in the other is thus one in terms of clarity of difficulty.
The quasi-experimental design of our studies obviously limits the conclusions that can be
drawn from the presented data. Our findings cannot provide strong evidence for the causal
link between the explicit achievement motive and effort suggested by our theoretical analysis.
It is possible that the achievement motive was not influencing effort investment but that the
effort that participants had invested in the task determined the achievement motive strength
that they reported. It is also not excluded that the observed positive relationship between
achievement motive strength and effort was due to a third variable that affected both the
explicit achievement motive and exerted force in the same manner. One way to improve the
PLOS ONE
Explicit achievement motive and physical effort
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 14 / 19
design and provide more direct evidence for the causal effect of the achievement motive on
effort might be to manipulate the situational characteristics so that the achievement motive is
either activated or not. However, it is important to note that the impact of clarity of difficulty
that we observed in Study 2 is not vulnerable to the quasi-experimental design critique. The
observed relationships between the achievement motive and effort may not provide strong evi-
dence for the causal effect of the achievement motive on effort but the manipulation of clarity
of difficulty demonstrates that variations in the clarity of task difficulty cause changes in the
relationship between achievement motive strength and effort.
In this article, we presented a theoretical analysis of achievement motive effects on effort
investment in instrumental tasks and provided empirical evidence for some of the predictions
of this analysis. We demonstrated that the impact of the explicit achievement motive on effort
depends on the clarity of task difficulty. If the difficulty of a task was known and fixed, achieve-
ment motive strength did not predict effort. However, if the difficulty standard was unknown
and unpredictable to participants, achievement motive strength predicted effort. Task charac-
teristics—in particular, the predictability of task difficulty—are thus important factors to con-
sider when using achievement motive strength to predict engagement in various achievement-
related tasks.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to Johanna Berit Slid and Lia Harrison for their help with collecting the pre-
sented data.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: Michael Richter, Florence Mazeres, Kerstin Brinkmann.
Formal analysis: Michael Richter.
Funding acquisition: Michael Richter, Kerstin Brinkmann.
Investigation: Michael Richter.
Methodology: Michael Richter, Florence Mazeres, Kerstin Brinkmann.
Project administration: Michael Richter.
Resources: Michael Richter.
Software: Michael Richter.
Supervision: Michael Richter.
Validation: Michael Richter.
Visualization: Michael Richter.
Writing original draft: Michael Richter, Florence Mazeres, Kerstin Brinkmann.
References
1. Ajzen I. Attitudes, Traits, and Actions: Dispositional Prediction of Behavior in Personality and Social
Psychology. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 1987;(20):1–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/
S0065-2601(08)60411-6 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCArticle to point out why researchers are inter-
ested in trait/personality/attitude/stable dispositions to explain behavior.
2. Shoda Y. Behavioral expressions of a personality system: Generation and perception of behavioral sig-
natures. In: Cervone D, Shoda Y, editors. The coherence of personality: Social cognitive bases of con-
sistency, variability, and organzation: Guilford Press; 1999. p. 155–81.
PLOS ONE
Explicit achievement motive and physical effort
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 15 / 19
3. Emmons RA. Exploring the Relations Between Motives and Traits: The Case of Narcissism. In: Buss
DM, Cantor N, editors. Personality psychology: Springer; 1989. p. 32–44.
4. McClelland DC. Human motivation. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 1987.
5. Winter DG, John OP, Stewart AJ, Klohnen EC, Duncan LE. Traits and motives: Toward an integration
of two traditions in personality research. Psychological Review. 1998; 105(2):230–50. Epub 1998/05/
13. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.105.2.230 PMID: 9577238; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCInter-
esting article on the relationship between motives and personality traits.
6. McClelland DC, Atkinson JW, Clark RA, Lowell EL. The achievement motive: Appleton Century Crofts;
1953. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054241 PMID: 13044863
7. Brunstein JC, Schmitt CH. Assessing individual differences in achievement motivation with the implicit
association test: Predictive validity of a chronometric measure of the self-concept “me = successful”.
In: Schultheiss OC, Brunstein JC, editors. Implicit motives. New York, NY: Oxford University Press;
2010. p. 151–85.
8. Brunstein JC. Implicit and explicit motives. In: Heckhausen J, Heckhausen H, editors. Motivation and
action: Springer; 2018. p. 369–405.
9. Mu¨ller F, Cañal-Bruland R. Motivation in the wild: A critical review of the relationship between motives
and motor performance. Motivation Science. 2020; 6(2):93–109. https://doi.org/10.1037/mot0000141
10. Spangler WD. Validity of questionnaire and TAT measures of need for achievement: Two meta-analy-
ses. Psychological Bulletin. 1992; 112(1):140–54. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.140
11. Brehm JW, Self EA. The intensity of motivation. Annual Review of Psychology. 1989; 40(1):109–31.
Epub 1989/01/01. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.40.020189.000545 PMID: 2648973.
12. Richter M. A closer look into the multi-layer structure of motivational intensity theory. Social and Person-
ality Psychology Compass. 2013; 7(1):1–12. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12007 PubMed Central
PMCID: PMCTheoretical article on the various predictions of motivational intensity theory.
13. Wright RA. Refining the prediction of effort: Brehm’s distinction between potential motivation and moti-
vation intensity. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2008; 2(2):682–701. https://doi.org/10.
1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00093.x PubMed Central PMCID: PMCTheoretical article on the distinction
between actual motivation and potential motivation in motivational intensity theory.
14. Gendolla GHE, Wright RA, Richter M. Advancing issues in motivation intensity research: Updated
insights from the cardiovascular system. In: Ryan RM, editor. The Oxford handbook of human motiva-
tion. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2019. p. 373–92.
15. Wright RA, Franklin J. Ability perception determinants of effort-related cardiovascular responses:
Mood, optimism, and performance resources. In: Wright RA, Greenberg J, editors. Motivational analy-
ses of social behavior: Building on Jack Brehm’s contributions to psychology: Lawrence Erlbaum Asso-
ciates Publishers; 2004. p. 187–204.
16. Barreto P, Wright RA, Krubinski K, Molzof H, Hur J. Effort analysis of gender differences in cardiovascu-
lar response: Further evidence involving a traditionally feminine incentive. Biological Psychology. 2015;
109:166–75. Epub 2015/06/03. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.05.006 PMID: 26032868;
PubMed Central PMCID: PMCMIT framework. Gender effects on effort.
17. Silvestrini N. On the implicit influence of pain cues on cognitive effort: Evidence from cardiovascular
reactivity. Biological Psychology. 2018; 132:45–54. Epub 2017/11/12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
biopsycho.2017.11.002 PMID: 29126963; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCMIT framework. Pain effects
on task difficulty and effort.
18. Richter M, Baeriswyl E, Roets A. Personality effects on cardiovascular reactivity: Need for closure mod-
erates the impact of task difficulty on engagement-related myocardial beta-adrenergic activity. Psycho-
physiology. 2012; 49(5):704–7. Epub 2012/03/13. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01350.x
PMID: 22404112; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCMIT framework. Need for closure effects on effort.
19. Silvia PJ, Eddington KM, Beaty RE, Nusbaum EC, Kwapil TR. Gritty people try harder: Grit and effort-
related cardiac autonomic activity during an active coping challenge. International Journal of Psycho-
physiology. 2013; 88(2):200–5. Epub 2013/04/23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.04.007 PMID:
23603450; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCMIT framework. Grit effects on effort in an unfixed task.
20. Gendolla GHE, Richter M. Ego-involvement and the difficulty law of motivation: Effects on performance-
related cardiovascular response. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2006; 32(9):1188–203.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167206288945 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCMIT framework. Ego-involv-
ment and task difficulty effects on effort. PMID: 16902239
21. Brinkmann K, Grept J, Gendolla GHE. Dysphorics can control depressive mood’s informational impact
on effort mobilization. Motivation and Emotion. 2011; 36(2):232–41. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-
011-9236-7 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCMIT framework. Dysphoria effects on task difficulty.
PLOS ONE
Explicit achievement motive and physical effort
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 16 / 19
22. Brinkmann K, Franzen J. Blunted cardiovascular reactivity during social reward anticipation in subclini-
cal depression. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2017; 119:119–26. Epub 2017/01/29.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.01.010 PMID: 28130127.
23. Richter M, Knappe K. Mood impact on effort-related cardiovascular reactivity depends on task context:
Evidence from a task with an unfixed performance standard. International Journal of Psychophysiology.
2014; 93(2):227–34. Epub 2014/05/13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.05.002 PMID:
24814934; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCMIT framework. Mood impact on task demand and success
importance.
24. Chatelain M, Silvestrini N, Gendolla GHE. Task difficulty moderates implicit fear and anger effects on
effort-related cardiac response. Biological Psychology. 2016; 115:94–100. Epub 2016/02/03. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.01.014 PMID: 26835594; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCIAPE and
MIT framework. Prime effects on task demand and effort.
25. McClelland DC, Koestner R, Weinberger J. How do self-attributed and implicit motives differ? Psycho-
logical Review. 1989; 96(4):690–702. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.690 PubMed Central
PMCID: PMCOn differences between implicit and explicit motives.
26. Schultheiss OC. Implicit motives. In: John OP, Robins RW, Pervin LA, editors. Handbook of personality:
Theory and research. 3rd ed: Guilford Press; 2008. p. 603–33.
27. Stanton SJ, Hall JL, Schultheiss OC. Properties of motive-specific incentives. In: Schultheiss O, Brun-
stein J, editors. Implicit motives. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2010. p. 245–78.
28. Mazeres F, Brinkmann K, Richter M. Implicit achievement motive limits the impact of task difficulty on
effort-related cardiovascular response. Journal of Research in Personality. 2019; 82. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.jrp.2019.06.012
29. Capa RL, Audiffren M, Ragot S. The effects of achievement motivation, task difficulty, and goal difficulty
on physiological, behavioral, and subjective effort. Psychophysiology. 2008; 45:859–68. https://doi.org/
10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00675.x PubMed Central PMCID: PMCAchievement motivation (approach
vs. avoidance) interacts with stimuls compatibility (kind of task difficulty) on MFB-HRV but not interac-
tion with task difficulty type (difficult-specific vs. easy-vague). Achievement motivation was copuated as
difference between motive to achieve success and motive to avoid failure. PMID: 18627535
30. Capa RL, Audiffren M, Ragot S. The interactive effect of achievement motivation and task difficulty on
mental effort. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2008; 70(2):144–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
ijpsycho.2008.06.007 PMID: 18627781
31. Wegner M, Schu¨ler J. The implicit achievement motive and general life stress affect time spent on com-
petitive matches in racquet sports. In: Mohiyeddini C, editor. Psychology of emotions, motivations and
actions Contemporary topics and trends in the psychology of sports: Nova Science Publishers; 2014.
p. 153–75.
32. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for
the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods. 2007; 39(2):175–91.
Epub 2007/08/19. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193146 PMID: 17695343.
33. Wegner M, Teubel T. The implicit achievement motive predicts match performance and the explicic
motive predicts choices for target distances in team sports. International Journal of Sport Psychology.
2014;(45):621–38. https://doi.org/10.7352/IJSP.2014.45.621
34. Gro
¨pel P, Wegner M, Schu¨ler J. Achievement motive and sport participation. Psychology of Sport and
Exercise. 2016; 27:93–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2016.08.007
35. Jackson DN. Personality Research Form manual. 3rd ed. Port Huron, MI: Sigma Assessment Sys-
tems; 1984.
36. Morse JL, Jung MC, Bashford GR, Hallbeck MS. Maximal dynamic grip force and wrist torque: the
effects of gender, exertion direction, angular velocity, and wrist angle. Appl Ergon. 2006; 37(6):737–42.
Epub 2006/01/31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2005.11.008 PMID: 16442072.
37. Richter M. Goal pursuit and energy conservation: energy investment increases with task demand but
does not equal it. Motivation and Emotion. 2015; 39(1):25–33. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-014-
9429-y PubMed Central PMCID: PMCHand grip tasks to compare exerted force with required force.
38. Stanek J, Richter M. Evidence against the primacy of energy conservation: Exerted force in possible
and impossible handgrip tasks. Motivation Science. 2016; 2(1):49–65. https://doi.org/10.1037/
mot0000028 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCTwo handgrip studies (one-factorial, four difficulty levels)
and a meta-analysis of MIT studies on disengagement. Evidence against complete disengagement.
39. Knoop E, Ba
¨cher M, Wall. V, Deimel R, Brock O, Beardsley P. Handshakiness: Benchmarking for
human-robot hand interactions. 2017 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and
Systems (IROS). 2017:4982–9. https://doi.org/10.1109/IROS.2017.8206381
PLOS ONE
Explicit achievement motive and physical effort
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 17 / 19
40. McGorry RW, Lin JH, Dempsey PG, Casey JS. Accuracy of the Borg CR10 scale for estimating grip
forces associated with hand tool tasks. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Hygiene. 2010; 7
(5):298–306. Epub 2010/03/24. https://doi.org/10.1080/15459621003711360 PMID: 20309773.
41. Brunstein JC, Hoyer S. Implizites versus explizites Leistungsstreben: Befunde zur Unabha
¨ngigkeit zwe-
ier Motivationssysteme. Zeitschrift fu¨r Pa
¨dagogische Psychologie. 2002; 16(1):51–62. https://doi.org/
10.1024//1010-0652.16.1.51
42. Filion RDL, Fowler SC, Notterman JM. Effort expenditure during proportionally reinforced responding.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1970; 22(3):398–405. https://doi.org/10.1080/
14640747008401913 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCforce-time-interval as measure of energy
investment.
43. Hittner JB, May K, Silver NC. A Monte Carlo evaluation of tests for comparing dependent correlations.
Journal of General Psychology. 2003; 130(2):149–68. Epub 2003/05/30. https://doi.org/10.1080/
00221300309601282 PMID: 12773018.
44. Masson ME. A tutorial on a practical Bayesian alternative to null-hypothesis significance testing. Behav-
ior Research Methods. 2011; 43(3):679–90. Epub 2011/02/09. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-010-
0049-5 PMID: 21302025.
45. Mulder J, Raftery AE. BIC Extensions for Order-constrained Model Selection. Sociological Methods &
Research. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1177/0049124119882459
46. Team RC. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation
for Statistical Computing; 2018.
47. Revelle W. psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research. Evanston,
Illinois: Northwestern University; 2020.
48. Diedenhofen B, Musch J. cocor: a comprehensive solution for the statistical comparison of correlations.
PLoS One. 2015; 10(3):e0121945. Epub 2015/04/04. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121945
PMID: 25835001; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4383486.
49. Mulder J. BICpack: What the Package Does. 2020.
50. Andraszewicz S, Scheibehenne B, Rieskamp J, Grasman R, Verhagen J, Wagenmakers E-J. An intro-
duction to Bayesian hypothesis testing for management research. Journal of Management. 2014;41
(2):521–43. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206314560412
51. Bray SR, Graham JD, Ginis KAM, Hicks AL. Cognitive task performance causes impaired maximum
force production in human hand flexor muscles. Biological Psychology. 2012; 89(1):195–200. Epub
2011/10/25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.10.008 PMID: 22020133.
52. Eksioglu M. Optimal work-rest cycles for an isometric intermittent gripping task as a function of force,
posture and grip span. Ergonomics. 2006; 49(2):180–201. Epub 2006/02/18. https://doi.org/10.1080/
00140130500465527 PMID: 16484144.
53. Sonne MW, Hodder JN, Wells R, Potvin JR. Force time-history affects fatigue accumulation during
repetitive handgrip tasks. Journal of Electromyography and Kinesiology. 2015; 25(1):130–5. Epub
2014/12/04. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jelekin.2014.10.017 PMID: 25465984.
54. Atkinson JW, Reitman WR. Performance as a function of motive strength and expectancy of goal-attain-
ment. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 1956; 53(3):361–6. https://doi.org/10.1037/
h0043477 PMID: 13376243
55. Motives Biernat M. and values to achieve: Different constructs with different effects. Journal of Person-
ality. 1989; 57(1):69–95. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1989.tb00761.x PubMed Central PMCID:
PMCon the difference between implicit and explicit achievement motives.
56. Sokolowski K, Schmalt H-D, Langens TA, Puca RM. Assessing achievement, affiliation, and power
motives all at once: The Multi-Motive Grid (MMG). Journal of Personality Assessment. 2000; 74
(1):126–45. Epub 2000/04/26. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327752JPA740109 PMID: 10779937;
PubMed Central PMCID: PMCMulti-motive-grid measure presentation and validation.
57. Bettschart M, Herrmann M, Wolf BM, Brandsta
¨tter V. Investigating the Unified Motive Scales. European
Journal of Psychological Assessment. 2020:1–5. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000571 PMID:
33058580
58. Lang JWB, Fries S. A revised 10-item version of the Achievement Motives Scale. European Journal of
Psychological Assessment. 2006; 22(3):216–24. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.22.3.216 PubMed
Central PMCID: PMCconstruction and validation of a short motive sclaes.
59. Schultheiss OC. A Memory-Systems Approach to the Classification of Personality Tests: Comment on
Meyer and Kurtz (2006). Journal of Personality Assessment. 2007; 89(2):197–201. https://doi.org/10.
1080/00223890701357431 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCexplicit and implicit motives associated with
declarative and non-declarative processes. PMID: 17764396
60. Locke EA, Latham GP. A theory of goal setting task performance: Prentice Hall; 1990.
PLOS ONE
Explicit achievement motive and physical effort
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 18 / 19
61. Norman DA, Bobrow DG. On data-limited and resource-limited processes. Cognitive Psychology. 1975;
7(1):44–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(75)90004-3 PubMed Central PMCID: PMCon data-lim-
ited and resource-limited processes. Interesting for the discussion of the relationship between effort and
performance.
62. Schultheiss OC, Brunstein J. An implicit motive perspective on competence. In: Elliot AJ, Dweck C, edi-
tors. Handbook of competence and motivation: Guilford; 2005. p. 31–51.
63. Gendolla GHE, Richter M. Ego involvement and effort: Cardiovascular, electrodermal, and performance
effects. Psychophysiology. 2005; 42(5):595–603. Epub 2005/09/24. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-
8986.2005.00314.x PMID: 16176382.
64. Silvia PJ. Mirrors, masks, and motivation: Implicit and explicit self-focused attention influence effort-
related cardiovascular reactivity. Biological Psychology. 2012; 90(3):192–201. Epub 2012/04/17.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.017 PMID: 22504295; PubMed Central PMCID:
PMC3570115.
PLOS ONE
Explicit achievement motive and physical effort
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252713 June 4, 2021 19 / 19
... Plus récemment, les travaux de Richter et ses collaborateurs (Richter, 2015 ;Richter et al., 2021 ;Stanek & Richter, 2021 ;Stanek & Richter, 2016) ont testé les prédictions de la TIM dans la mobilisation de ressources physiques (i.e., effort physique objectif). Richter (2015) a souligné une limite quant à l'utilisation des données cardiovasculaires concernant le principe de conservation d'énergie. ...
... Ces résultats supportent les prédictions de la TIM où l'investissement d'énergie dépend de la difficulté de la tâche et est modulé par la motivation potentielle, ici la récompense. Par ailleurs, Richter et al. (2021) ont montré que le besoin (i.e., motive) d'accomplissement externe influençait la mobilisation de ressources. Ce besoin d'accomplissement externe fait référence à un besoin conscient de tendre vers l'excellence. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
L'objectif principal de cette thèse est d'étudier l’effort en fonction de la difficulté, sans prendre en compte la récompense, auprès d’individus ayant un diagnostic de schizophrénie. Contrairement à la littérature qui s’est basée majoritairement sur la théorie de la décision (i.e., la valeur d'une action dépend des valeurs des bénéfices et des efforts associées), nous nous sommes basés sur la théorie de l’intensité motivationnelle, permettant de faire des prédictions distinctes entre effort et récompense. Selon cette théorie et en accord avec le principe de conservation d’énergie, l’effort est principalement déterminé par la difficulté de la tâche. Les objectifs secondaires de ce travail étaient d'explorer la relation entre effort et symptômes négatifs, mais également les relations entre effort et variables psychologiques (e.g., humeur) et physique (e.g., IMC). A travers quatre études, nous avons exploré l’effort cognitif et physique, tant sur la composante objective (i.e., mobilisation de ressources) que sur la composante subjective (i.e., perception d’effort). De plus, nous avons exploré un exemple de conservation d’énergie, l’utilisation d’outil. La première étude portant sur l’effort cognitif a mis en évidence une dissociation entre effort objectif, évalué par l’activité cardiovasculaire sympathique, et effort subjective, mesuré par une échelle de Likert, dans le groupe schizophrénie : ce groupe mobilisait peu de ressources cognitives et avait de moins bonnes performances par rapport aux individus non-cliniques mais percevait avoir mis autant d’effort que ces derniers. La faible mobilisation de ressources cognitives était associée à la fatigue, facteur rarement étudié dans la schizophrénie qui pourtant semble avoir un impact majeur. Dans une deuxième étude, la perception de l’effort était significativement plus élevée dans la schizophrénie dans une tâche de marche, et accentuée par l’IMC. Dans une troisième étude, les deux groupes avaient des comportements de mobilisation de ressources physiques similaires : les individus avec schizophrénie mobilisaient des ressources physiques dans la tâche facile mais restaient également engager dans les tâches difficiles, comme les individus non-cliniques. Dans une quatrième étude, nous avons mis en évidence que les individus avec schizophrénie anticipaient une réduction d’effort en termes de temps associée à des actions d’utilisation d’outil de manière similaire aux individus non-cliniques. Dans la plupart de nos études, les prédictions de la TIM ont été retrouvées : l’effort était proportionnel à la difficulté de la tâche. De plus, aucune de nos quatre études avec des échantillons indépendants n’ont mis en évidence de lien entre l’effort et les symptômes négatifs. L’ensemble de nos résultats mettent en évidence que 1) les individus avec schizophrénie présentent des mécanismes liés à l’effort préservés quand celui-ci est indépendant de la récompense 2) il existe une perception anormale de l’effort cognitif et physique dans cette population clinique 3) l’absence de lien avec les symptômes négatifs en l’absence de récompense apportent des arguments en faveur de l’hypothèse de la sous-estimation de la valeur de la récompense pour expliquer en partie les liens entre l’effort et les difficultés motivationnelles dans la schizophrénie.
... Therefore, under this specific condition, effort should directly depend on success importance. Supporting this idea, numerous studies have shown that certain factors-such as incentive value or motives (Mazeres et al., 2019Richter et al., 2021)-can determine success importance and thus directly influence effort under the condition that task difficulty is unclear. Only individuals suffering from depressive symptoms did not show this direct effect of success importance on effort intensity (e.g., Brinkmann et al., 2009;Brinkmann & Franzen, 2013;Franzen & Brinkmann, 2015. ...
... We expected personal choice especially to increase commitment (Brehm, 1956(Brehm, , 1962-the willingness to attain a goal (Locke et al., 1988)-which should render success important. However, as success importance only directly determines resource mobilization when difficulty is unclear (Mazeres et al., 2019Richter et al., 2021;), we expected participants in the Chosen Color condition to mobilize higher resources than those in the Assigned Color condition when task difficulty was unclear. This is what we have found for cardiac PEP reactivity-a reliable and valid measure of actual effort intensity (Kelsey, 2012;Wright, 1996). ...
Article
Full-text available
This experiment investigated how the personal choice of task characteristics influences resource mobilization assessed as effort-related cardiac response during a task of clearly low vs. unclear (but also low) difficulty. We expected that the personal choice of the color of memory task stimuli would justify higher effort during task performance than external color assignment. Applying the logic of motivational intensity theory (MIT; Brehm et al., 1983; Brehm & Self, 1989), we further predicted that the personal choice of the stimuli’s color would directly lead to higher effort intensity than external color assignment when task difficulty was unclear but not when the task difficulty was clear. When task difficulty was low and clear, we expected actual effort to be low in general, because high resources are not necessary for a clearly easy task. Results were as expected: when task difficulty was unclear, participants who had personally chosen the stimuli’s color showed significantly stronger cardiac pre-ejection period reactivity, reflecting higher effort, than those in the other three conditions. These findings provide first evidence that personal choice justifies relatively high effort and further support the principles of MIT regarding the critical role of task difficulty for resource mobilization.
... Achievement motivation is the driving force of individuals to achieve success, as well as effort and perseverance in facing various challenges that come their way (Richter et al., 2021). Achievement motivation is a drive in a person to achieve success while avoiding failure (Cadenas-Sanchez et al., 2021). ...
Article
Implementing differentiated learning is a challenge for schools that are not yet “Sekolah Penggerak”. This research aims to formulate a strategy to increase the effectiveness of differentiated learning, especially in private junior high schools that implement the "Independent Curriculum" independently. The strategy is formulated based on a structural model between the effectiveness of differentiated learning variables with academic supervision and exogenous organizational culture variables and achievement motivation as an intervening variable. This cross-sectional research was conducted on 182 private junior high school teachers in Cibinong District, Bogor, who were randomly selected proportionally. SmarPLS 3 software was used to help analyze the data. The results of this research conclude that academic supervision, organizational culture, and achievement motivation are important factors in increasing the effectiveness of differentiated learning. Achievement motivation mediates the influence of academic supervision and organizational culture on the effectiveness of differentiated learning. The effectiveness of differentiated learning can be increased by implementing academic supervision strategies and organizational culture that can increase teacher achievement motivation.
... All participants were informed about the general task structure before working on the task but participants in the clear-difficulty condition also received detailed information about the number of digits per trial and the presentation times. Following previous work that has used tasks with unclear difficulty (e.g., Brinkmann and Franzen, 2013;Franzen et al., 2019;Richter and Gendolla, 2009a;Richter et al., 2021), participants in the unclear-difficulty condition did not receive any specific information about task difficulty and were additionally informed that the number of digits and presentation times would vary randomly between trials to prevent participants from forming an impression about task difficulty. ...
Article
Work on physiological and other behavioral correlates of motives often assumes that motives exert a direct effect on behavior once activated. Motivational intensity theory, however, suggests that this does not always apply. In the context of task engagement, motive strength should exert a direct effect on myocardial beta-adrenergic activity if task difficulty is unclear, but not if task difficulty is known. The presented study tested this prediction for the impact of the explicit achievement motive on myocardial beta-adrenergic activity—assessed as pre-ejection period (PEP) reactivity during task performance. Seventy-eight participants performed one of two versions of a mental arithmetic task. After having completed the achievement motive scale of the Personality Research Form, participants were either informed about the difficulty of the task or not before working on it. Participants' PEP reactivity during task performance provided evidence for the predicted moderating impact of clarity of task difficulty: PEP reactivity increased with increasing achievement motive strength if task difficulty was unclear, but not if it was clear. These findings demonstrate that the explicit achievement motive impact on myocardial beta-adrenergic activity is moderated by clarity of task difficulty and suggest that motive strength does not always translate into direct effects on physiology and behavior.
Article
Background: The shortage of nurses is a global problem. Increasing the level of work engagement is known to effectively reduce turnover rate among nursing staff. Strategies to improve work engagement based on the job demands-resources model may effectively improve nurses' job satisfaction and reduce staff turnover. Purpose: A job demands-resources model was used in this study to test the hypothesis that work engagement is affected by both external resources (professional nursing practice environment) and internal resources (self-efficacy and achievement motivation). In addition, the mediating role of self-efficacy and achievement motivation in the relationship between professional nursing practice environment and work engagement was also verified. Methods: A cross-sectional survey employing a convenience sampling design was implemented. Five hundred fifteen registered nurses were recruited from four tertiary hospitals in Chengdu, China, from January to June 2020. The Practice Environment Scale of the Nursing Work Index, General Self-Efficacy Scale, Achievement Motive Scale, and Utrecht Work Engagement Scale were used to measure the four variables. Results: The results of the structural equation model showed that the degree of fit for each index model was good, indicating that all of the three variables, including professional nursing practice environment, self-efficacy, and achievement motivation, directly affected work engagement. In addition, the variable professional nursing practice environment was found to indirectly influence work engagement through the partial mediation of self-efficacy and achievement motivation. Conclusions: The results may be used to guide health policymakers and nurse managers attempting to enhance the work engagement of nurses by providing a supportive working environment, improving the welfare mechanism, formulating a talent training plan, and fostering appropriate empowerment to improve the working environment, self-efficacy, and motivation in nurses.
Chapter
Motives are traits of people that influence the way they perceive their environment. Motives provide the long-term energy for action, for example, in the domains of sport, exercise, and health. In this chapter we differentiate between implicit (nonconscious) and explicit (conscious) motives and describe three important motive themes—achievement, affiliation, and power—and their different effects on behavior in sports, exercise, and health settings. Standard indirect methods for measuring implicit motives are further introduced. Moreover, the motivational brain areas involved in implicit motivation processes as well as physiological associations with motive themes are briefly described. Finally, we illustrate consequences of implicit-explicit-motive (in)congruence for exercise and health behavior.KeywordsImplicitMotivePersonalitySportDispositionNonconsciousActionAchievementPowerAffiliationEffortAffectEmotionIndirectHormoneAssociative processingOperant
Article
Full-text available
What determines effort intensity in instrumental behavior? According to motivation intensity theory, effort should be proportional to experienced task difficulty as long as success is possible and justified and low when success is impossible or excessively difficult, given the available benefit. When task difficulty is unspecified or unknown, effort should be proportional to the importance of success. This chapter reports an extensive program of research that has operationalized effort intensity as cardiovascular reactivity during task performance and used multiple manipulations of variables influencing subjective task difficulty (e.g., performance standards, instrumentality, ability, fatigue, mood, depressive symptoms, implicit affect, implicit and biological aging) and the amount of justified effort (e.g., material incentive, instrumentality, needs, personal and social evaluation, mortality salience). In the second edition of this handbook, this chapter focuses on recent empirical evidence for the principles of motivation intensity theory and discusses challenges for other theoretical accounts.
Article
Full-text available
Explicit motives are well-studied in the field of personality and motivation psychology. However, the statistical overlap of different explicit motive measures is only moderate. As a consequence, the Unified Motive Scales (UMS; Schönbrodt & Gerstenberg, 2012) were developed to improve the measurement of explicit motives. The present longitudinal field study examined the predictive validity of the UMS achievement motive subscale. Applicants of a police department (n = 168, Mage = 25.11, 53 females and 115 males) completed the UMS and their performance in the selection process was assessed. As expected, UMS achievement predicted success in the selection process. The findings provide first evidence for the predictive validity of UMS achievement in an applied setting.
Article
Full-text available
The Schwarz or Bayesian information criterion (BIC) is one of the most widely used tools for model comparison in social science research. The BIC, however, is not suitable for evaluating models with order constraints on the parameters of interest. This article explores two extensions of the BIC for evaluating order-constrained models, one where a truncated unit information prior is used under the order-constrained model and the other where a truncated local unit information prior is used. The first prior is centered on the maximum likelihood estimate, and the latter prior is centered on a null value. Several analyses show that the order-constrained BIC based on the local unit information prior works better as an Occam’s razor for evaluating order-constrained models and results in lower error probabilities. The methodology based on the local unit information prior is implemented in the R package “BICpack” which allows researchers to easily apply the method for order-constrained model selection. The usefulness of the methodology is illustrated using data from the European Values Study.
Article
Full-text available
Motive research offers a comprehensive account of how differences in individuals’ motivation arise and in turn impact behavioral outcomes. In this review, we critically assess and discuss the current state of the art in research on the relation between motives and motor performance and to a lesser degree learning. Our evaluation of the literature indicates that the field is characterized by the use of heterogeneous motive measures, a strong focus on the achievement motive at the expense of other motives such as the affiliation and power motives, correlational designs, and divergent findings. Based on our critical assessment, we suggest that future research should strive to (a) carefully consider both the implicit–explicit motive distinction as well as their corresponding incentives and behavioral corre- lates, (b) broaden its scope beyond the achievement motive, and (c) aim to unravel the mechanisms that drive motives’ effect on motor performance. To inform theorizing and ultimately increase our understanding of both motivation and motor behavior and individual differences therein, we propose several changes to the overall research agenda to align efforts and research methodology and bring structure to the currently heterogeneous findings.
Article
In contrast to the motive literature, motivational intensity theory predicts that the implicit achievement motive (nAch) should only exert an indirect impact on effort by limiting the impact of task difficulty. To contrast these two views, sixty-eight participants with a low or high nAch performed an easy or difficult arithmetic task. Effort was assessed using cardiac pre-ejection period (PEP). Supporting motivational intensity theory’s view, PEP response was low in both easy-task conditions but stronger in the high-nAch group than in the low-nAch group in the difficult task. These findings suggest that nAch exerts an indirect effect on effort investment by setting the maximally justified effort instead of directly determining the amount of effort that is invested to satisfy the motive.
Chapter
McClelland, Koestner, and Weinberger (Psychological Review 96:690–702, 1989) suggested to strictly differentiate between implicit and explicit motives when analyzing motivated behavior. This chapter will first present the history of this suggestion before offering findings supporting the idea that the two types of motives are indeed independent and specific: implicit motives are primarily related to the pursuit of affective satisfaction, whereas explicit motives indicate cognitive needs to obtain information about one’s own behavior. Implicit and explicit motives may interact with each other, which can result in their cooperation or conflict due to motivational discrepancy. This chapter will also discuss how the goals individuals choose themselves can be adapted to their implicit behavioral preferences. The final part of this chapter will discuss some unanswered questions.
Article
To extend previous findings on the impact of implicit affect on behavior, two experiments investigated the influence of priming pain cues on cognitive effort. Effort was assessed as cardiovascular reactivity (PEP, SBP, DBP, and HR) during an easy or difficult cognitive task integrating briefly presented and masked pain-related words. The control condition included neutral words (Experiment 1) or anger-related words (Experiment 2). The pain primes were expected to increase the perceived difficulty of the task and to result in stronger effort during the easy task, compared to the control condition, and to lower effort during the difficult task, due to disengagement. Overall, cardiovascular reactivity of both experiments supported the predictions. Moreover, pain primes increased self-reported subjective difficulty. Finally, most participants could not report the content of the primes. Findings are discussed regarding the influence of implicit processes in pain experience and regarding the self-regulatory consequences of the influence of pain on effort mobilization.
Article
The present study extends past research about reduced reward responsiveness in depression by assessing effort-related cardiovascular responses during anticipation of a social reward. Dysphoric (i.e., subclinically depressed) and nondysphoric participants worked on a cognitive task. Half the participants in each group expected the possibility to subscribe to a social exchange internet site. Effort mobilization during task performance was assessed by participants' cardiovascular reactivity. Confirming the predictions, nondysphoric participants in the social-reward condition had higher reactivity of pre-ejection period, systolic blood pressure, and heart rate, compared to the other three cells. In contrast, dysphoric participants' cardiovascular reactivity was generally low. These findings indicate that social-reward function is indeed impaired in subclinical depression. Implications for social punishment are discussed.