ArticlePDF Available

Risk messages relating to fertility and pregnancy: a media content analysis

Authors:
  • British Pregnancy Advisory Service

Abstract and Figures

Background: The UK print and online media is an important channel by which scientific research is communicated to the public. Media risk messages relating to pregnancy or fertility contribute to the context of reproductive decision making, but their fidelity to the underlying science has been questioned. Method: We measured the volume, distribution and content of science-based risk headlines relating to pregnancy or fertility in the UK media over four months. We grouped headlines into unique stories and categorised them by exposure and outcome of interest. We selected four unique stories for closer content analysis and assessed their fidelity to the underlying science, with attention to the role of press releases. Results: We identified 171 headlines over four months (average 43 per month), comprising 56 unique stories. The unique stories most commonly concerned maternal risk factors (n=46) and child health outcomes (n=46). Maternal health outcomes were less frequently the focus (n=20). The most common risk factors in the media coverage were maternal food and drink (n=15), maternal medication and medical interventions (n=9), and maternal health factors (n=6). Media reports were largely faithful to press releases. Where substantive deviations from the underlying scientific study were identified, these could mostly be traced back to press releases or quotes from the study’s authors. Press releases often omitted caveats which were reinstated at the media reporting stage, alongside additional expert criticism. Conclusions: Frequent science-based risk messages in the UK media frame mothers as vectors of potential harm to children, who are the focus of health outcomes. Largely, the media does not introduce misinformation, but reports press releases faithfully with additional caveats and expert commentary. Press releases fulfil an interpretative role, often omitting caveats and introducing new elements and advice to women. Their role as a bridge between scientific and lay audiences is discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Risk messages relating to fertility and pregnancy: a media
content analysis [version 1; peer review: 1 approved]
Olivia Marshall 1, Rebecca Blaylock 1, Clare Murphy 1, Julia Sanders 2
1Centre for Reproductive Research and Communication, British Pregnancy Advisory Service (BPAS), London, EC4A 1JQ, UK
2School of Healthcare Sciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff, CF14 4XN, UK
First published: 14 May 2021, 6:114
https://doi.org/10.12688/wellcomeopenres.16744.1
Latest published: 14 May 2021, 6:114
https://doi.org/10.12688/wellcomeopenres.16744.1
v1
Abstract
Background: The UK print and online media is an important channel
by which scientific research is communicated to the public. Media risk
messages relating to pregnancy or fertility contribute to the context of
reproductive decision making, but their fidelity to the underlying
science has been questioned.
Method: We measured the volume, distribution and content of
science-based risk headlines relating to pregnancy or fertility in the
UK media over four months. We grouped headlines into unique
stories and categorised them by exposure and outcome of interest.
We selected four unique stories for closer content analysis and
assessed their fidelity to the underlying science, with attention to the
role of press releases.
Results: We identified 171 headlines over four months (average 43
per month), comprising 56 unique stories. The unique stories most
commonly concerned maternal risk factors (n=46) and child health
outcomes (n=46). Maternal health outcomes were less frequently the
focus (n=20). The most common risk factors in the media coverage
were maternal food and drink (n=15), maternal medication and
medical interventions (n=9), and maternal health factors (n=6). Media
reports were largely faithful to press releases. Where substantive
deviations from the underlying scientific study were identified, these
could mostly be traced back to press releases or quotes from the
study’s authors. Press releases often omitted caveats which were
reinstated at the media reporting stage, alongside additional expert
criticism.
Conclusions: Frequent science-based risk messages in the UK media
frame mothers as vectors of potential harm to children, who are the
focus of health outcomes. Largely, the media does not introduce
misinformation, but reports press releases faithfully with additional
caveats and expert commentary. Press releases fulfil an interpretative
role, often omitting caveats and introducing new elements and advice
to women. Their role as a bridge between scientific and lay audiences
is discussed.
Open Peer Review
Reviewer Status
Invited Reviewers
1
version 1
14 May 2021 report
Norah MacKendrick, Rutgers University,
New Brunswick, USA
1.
Any reports and responses or comments on the
article can be found at the end of the article.
Page 1 of 17
Wellcome Open Research 2021, 6:114 Last updated: 11 JUN 2021
Corresponding author: Olivia Marshall (olivia.marshall@bpas.org)
Author roles: Marshall O: Conceptualization, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – Original Draft
Preparation; Blaylock R: Conceptualization, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – Review & Editing; Murphy C:
Conceptualization, Writing – Review & Editing; Sanders J: Conceptualization, Writing – Review & Editing
Competing interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Grant information: This paper is part of the WRISK project, funded by the Wellcome Trust [212089/Z/18/Z].
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Copyright: © 2021 Marshall O et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution
License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
How to cite this article: Marshall O, Blaylock R, Murphy C and Sanders J. Risk messages relating to fertility and pregnancy: a media
content analysis [version 1; peer review: 1 approved] Wellcome Open Research 2021, 6:114
https://doi.org/10.12688/wellcomeopenres.16744.1
First published: 14 May 2021, 6:114 https://doi.org/10.12688/wellcomeopenres.16744.1
Keywords
Risk; fertility; pregnancy; media; science communication; health
Page 2 of 17
Wellcome Open Research 2021, 6:114 Last updated: 11 JUN 2021
Introduction
The UK media is an important channel through which new
health research is disseminated to the general public13. Health
stories are a particular staple of UK media reporting and are
frequently communicated in the form of ‘risk messages’, wherein
common exposures, habits, or lifestyle characteristics are posi-
tioned as risk factors for certain health outcomes2,3. Science-
based health messages in the media influence the public’s
understanding of health risks and their health-related decisions
and behaviours46.
Scientific studies are commonly communicated to journalists
via press releases (PRs), which bridge the academic and media
communities, enabling research institutions to generate cover-
age, and journalists to produce swift copy that conveys complex
findings to a lay readership1. PRs thus provide an important
step on the risk reporting pathway between scientific papers and
media reports (Figure 1).
Concerns have been raised that the media’s reporting of health
science is sensationalist, inaccurate, and undermines the intended
messaging of the scientific community1,3,7,8. A number of stud-
ies have examined the role of PRs in science communication,
and found they may be responsible, especially through the
removal of caveats or the framing of correlational associa-
tions as causative1,2,79. While a certain degree of interpretation is
necessary for PRs to fulfil their bridging function, such changes
in the framing of risk messages at the PR stage are likely to
later influence their public interpretation and reception, and
therefore risk a detrimental impact on public health2,4,10,11.
Pregnancy risk is an area of contemporary debate and atten-
tion. Previous research has found that pregnancy is increasingly
portrayed as a high-risk state, during which women must be
closely surveilled, not for the sake of their own health but to
protect the fetus from risks introduced by their behaviour1216.
This narrative can be traced back to the thalidomide disaster
of the 1960s17. Its reinvigoration in recent years has partly been
fueled by research into the developmental origins of health
and disease (DOHaD), which has recently focused on the impact
of chemical exposures in utero on fetal health outcomes1820.
This paper anlayses risk messages in the UK media relating to
pregnancy and fertility, to better understand the context that
informs women’s reproductive health decisions and behaviours,
and to assess the extent to which (if at all) pregnant women are
framed as vectors of potential harm (or benefit) to their fetus.
Secondly, the fidelity of media reports to the underlying science
is assessed through closer analysis of four illustrative examples.
For each example, the communication of risk messages along
the risk reporting pathway is examined, with particular attention
to the role of PRs.
Objectives
1. Understand the landscape: Describe the volume, dis-
tribution and content of science-based risk messages
relating to pregnancy or fertility, as reported in UK-
based print and online media outlets, over selected time
periods.
2. Assess reliability: For selected news stories based on
a scientific study, describe and map the risk reporting
Figure 1. The risk reporting pathway (Wrisk project, 2020)21.
Page 3 of 17
Wellcome Open Research 2021, 6:114 Last updated: 11 JUN 2021
pathway(s) “from study to story”, by tracking key
themes and informational elements across the original
study manuscript, PR and media reports.
Methods
Objective 1: Understand the landscape
We used publicly available media reports. There were no
particular ethical considerations.
Selection of relevant time period(s)
We randomly selected one month from each of the most recent
four quarters (Q4, 2018 – Q3, 2019) for analysis, to accommodate
possible seasonal variations in media reporting. The months
selected were November 2018, and February, May and August
2019 (the second month of each quarter).
Search of media database
Using the LexisNexis news database, headlines published in
UK print or online media outlets during the selected months
were searched for relevant key words (“babies” or “unborn”
or “pregnant” or “pregnancy” or “fertility” or “miscarriage”
or “stillbirth” or “stillborn”).
Exclusion process
The inclusion and exclusion criteria were agreed by two
researchers (OM and RB). The full exclusion process is
summarised in Figure 2.
Figure 2. Exclusion ow diagram.
Page 4 of 17
Wellcome Open Research 2021, 6:114 Last updated: 11 JUN 2021
Inclusion criteria: Headlines relating to a social, environ-
mental, or clinical risk to fertility, a pregnant woman or her
offspring, and based on a new scientific study (including
research conducted/published outside of the UK).
Exclusion criteria: Headlines from newswires, broadcast
media or media outlets with a predominant readership outside of
the UK; headlines with no risk element (including celebrity,
entertainment, or miscellaneous news stories); headlines about
the postnatal period or infant care; and headlines not based
on a new scientific study.
Analysis
Stories were deemed to be based on a scientific paper if
they mentioned a new scientific study or research, and the
corresponding paper was identified and noted based on its key
findings, authors and date of publication. We grouped media
reports based on the same scientific study into “unique
stories” and assigned a Unique Story ID (USID). For each
month we calculated the number of headlines and unique
stories. We calculated the number of headlines per unique
story, and the mean and median overall.
We coded each unique story according to the exposure and
outcome of interest. We categorised exposures by risk locus
(maternal; paternal; offspring; other) and by topic. We
categorised outcomes by outcome locus (maternal; paternal;
offspring; other). Two researchers (OM and RB) undertook this
process independently and established and a consensus. Some
unique stories were assigned to multiple categories as appro-
priate. For each category we calculated the number of relevant
unique stories. We used Microsoft Excel (v2103) for our
analysis.
Objective 2: Assess reliability
Selection of illustrative examples
We selected the unique story that generated the most head-
lines in each month and assessed the reliability of associated
media report. Four unique stories selected were based on
the following studies:
November 2018: McQuire, C, Mukherjee, R, et al. (2019).
Screening prevalence of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders in
a region of the United Kingdom: A population-based birth-
cohort study. Preventive Medicine, 118, 344–351. doi: 10.1016/
j.ypmed.2018.10.013
February 2019: Golding, J, Gregory, S, et al. (2019). Mater-
nal prenatal external locus of control and reduced mathematical
and science abilities in their offspring: A longitudinal birth
cohort study. Frontiers in Psychology, 10(Feb) doi: 10.3389/
fpsyg.2019.00194
May 2019: Gignac, F, Romaguera, D, et al. (2019). Maternal
nut intake in pregnancy and child neuropsychological devel-
opment up to 8 years old: a population-based cohort study in
Spain. European Journal of Epidemiology, 34(7), 661–673.
doi: 10.1007/s10654-019-00521-6
August 2019: Zhang, T, Sidorchuk, A, et al. (2019). Associa-
tion of Cesarean Delivery With Risk of Neurodevelopmental and
Psychiatric Disorders in the Offspring: A Systematic Review
and Meta-analysis. JAMA network open, 2(8), e1910236. doi:
10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.10236
Collating of materials
We collated the following documents for each illustrative
example:
· Original study manuscript
· PR as published by the university or research institution
(where applicable)
· Communications about the study published by the
Science Media Centre (SMC) (where applicable)
· Print and online media coverage across UK media
outlets
To fulfil the objective of assessing media accuracy, we consid-
ered it appropriate to analyse all the media coverage of these
four unique stories, including any articles not captured by
our original search. This was especially important since
previous research has indicated that as an individual risk
story ages, the tone of the media discourse may evolve2. To
ensure completeness, we conducted additional searches using
the LexisNexis database, with wider date parameters and more
specific keywords (for example “FASD”, etc.). Once we had
collated all media reports, we removed duplicates (defined as
an identical article published by the same news outlet on the
same day).
Coding of media reports, PRs and studies
Following methodological precedent from Lee, Sutton and
Hartley (2016) and Reisch and Spiegelhalter (2011), for each
illustrative example we identified and coded key themes and
informational elements in the original study, PRs, and media
reports. Two researchers (OM and RB) extracted this infor-
mation independently and agreed a summary by a process of
consensus.
The information extracted was grouped under four broad
headings:
1. Description of study and findings (including study design,
exposure and outcome of interest, use of statistics, description
of association, use of language, etc.)
2. Caveats, criticism or study limitations (e.g. a specific
statement that causation was not established)
3. Associated discussion topics (framing)
4. Advice, warnings, or reassurance to the public (both direct
and indirect)
Mapping and descriptive analysis
For every document, we noted the presence or absence of each
theme or informational element, and calculated the number of
Page 5 of 17
Wellcome Open Research 2021, 6:114 Last updated: 11 JUN 2021
media reports containing each theme or informational element.
This allowed us to compare the content of the original study,
PR and media reporting.
We undertook a descriptive analysis to trace the themes and
informational elements along the risk reporting pathway,
according to the four groupings outlined above.
Results
Objective 1: Understand the landscape
Volume and distribution of headlines and unique stories
171 headlines were identified that met the inclusion criteria,
which were mostly concentrated in November 2018 and May
2019. Analysis of the 171 articles found that they comprised
56 unique stories (based on 56 research studies). Again, the
unique stories were concentrated in November 2018 and May
2019 (Figure 3). Per month, the mean number of headlines was
43 and unique stories 14.
Unique stories generated varying numbers of headlines. The
median number of headlines per unique story was 2, but some
stories achieved significantly more headlines, as illustrated in
Figure 4.
Content of unique stories
Of the 56 unique stories, maternal risk factors (n=46) and
child health outcomes (n=46) were most commonly identified.
Maternal health outcomes were less frequently the focus
(n=20). The categorisation of exposures and outcomes by
subject is given in Figure 5.
The most common risk factors were maternal food and drink
(n=13), maternal medication and medical interventions
(n=9), and maternal health factors, including genetic factors or
underlying conditions (n=6). The categorisation of exposures
by topic is given in Figure 6.
Objective 2: Assess reliability
Illustrative example 1
Documents:
• Study: McQuire, C.,et al. (2019)
Press release: University of Bristol (2019). First UK
prevalence estimate FASD
Science Media Centre (2018). expert reaction to screen-
ing study on uk prevalence of fetal alcohol spectrum
disorders (FASD)
Figure 3. Unique stories and headlines per month.
Page 6 of 17
Wellcome Open Research 2021, 6:114 Last updated: 11 JUN 2021
Figure 4. Headlines per unique story.
Page 7 of 17
Wellcome Open Research 2021, 6:114 Last updated: 11 JUN 2021
Figure 5. Exposure and outcome of interest, categorised by subject.
Figure 6. Exposure of interest, categorised by topic.
Science Media Centre (2018). uk prevalence of fetal
alcohol spectrum disorders
Media reports (n=16)
Study summary:
Novel screening algorithms were applied to data from the
ALSPAC longitudinal birth cohort study to estimate the screening
prevalence of fetal alcohol spectrum disorder (FASD). Differ-
ent missing data strategies were evaluated to yield prevalence
estimates ranging from 6–17%.
Comparison of study, PR and media reports:
Key changes at each stage of the risk reporting pathway
are summarised in Figure 7.
Description of the research and its findings: The study’s
design, exposure and outcome of interest were accurately
described in the PR and in 15 of the 16 media reports. The one
remaining report included a passing reference to one of
the study’s secondary findings. The highest prevalence esti-
mate of 17% was quoted accurately in the PR and the majority
Page 8 of 17
Wellcome Open Research 2021, 6:114 Last updated: 11 JUN 2021
Figure 7. Key changes along the risk reporting pathway (illustrative example 1).
of media reports (n=13), but eight reports also approximated this
to “one in six”, and two to “one in five”, in their headlines. The
PR used appropriate language to convey uncertainty (e.g. “fea-
tures consistent with FASD”; “children could have symptoms”;
“up to 17%”) and presented the figure of 17% as an estimate
throughout, including in the headline. Three quarters of media
reports (n=12) made clear somewhere in the report that the
figure was an estimate, but seven also presented the figure as
certain at least once, in statements such as “17% of babies are
harmed by mothers’ drinking”. The PR did not state that the
prevalence estimate had varied by screening method or include
the lower estimates (which had been obtained via different
imputation methods for missing data). Only one media report
(in The Guardian) reinstated this information.
Caveats, criticism, and study limitations: The caveat that a
positive screen for FASD is not the same as a formal diagnosis
was present in the study, the PR and half of the reports (n=8).
The caveat that a causative association had not been estab-
lished (between prenatal alcohol exposure and the developmen-
tal outcomes recorded) was omitted in the PR, but reinstated in
the majority of media reports (n=10). Ten media reports included
additional expert criticism sourced from either the Science
Media Centre (n=5) or a press comment issued by the British
Pregnancy Advisory Service (BPAS) (n=10), including a
criticism that the study was “causing needless alarm”, which
was cited in eight reports. Overall, the majority of media reports
(n=10) included at least one caveat or critical statement, with
most including three or more (n=9). Of the six media reports that
included no criticism or caveats, two were very short (50
words or less) and two were passing references to the study in
articles published several months later.
Associated discussion topics: The PR developed discussion
topics from the study, including UK rates of prenatal alcohol
exposure (PAE); the prevalence of binge drinking among preg-
nant women; conceptualising FASD as an under-diagnosed or
“hidden” condition; and framing FASD as a significant public
health concern. The PR also introduced one new topic, low
public awareness of FASD, which was mentioned in half of the
media reports (n=8). The discussion topics in the media cover-
age closely mirrored those in the PR, especially the placement
of FASD as a public health concern and discussion of PAE
rates in the UK.
Advice, warnings and reassurance: The PR introduced
direct advice to women in a quote by the study’s lead author,
who referred to guidance that the safest approach is to abstain
from alcohol. Two paragraphs at the end of the PR also
summarised advice from the UK’s Chief Medical Officer (CMO),
which affirms this but also offers reassurance by stating that
those who have drunk before realising they are pregnant are
unlikely to have caused harm. The majority of media reports
(n=11) included direct or indirect advice to women, but few
included the CMO’s reassurance (n=5).
Summary: The PR described the study accurately with
appropriate language to convey uncertainty, but failed to include
alternative estimates for FASD prevalence and some key
caveats. The PR introduced both advice and reassurance to
women through quotes from the main author and others, includ-
ing the CMO. The media coverage also communicated the study’s
findings effectively, but often approximated the prevalence
estimate of 17% to “one in six” or “one in five”, and occasion-
ally presented it as certain, implying a level of confidence that
the study authors had not claimed. The majority of media
reports addressed the study’s limitations through the inclusion of
caveats and criticisms, including caveats that had been omit-
ted in the PR, and additional criticism sourced from the SMC
and BPAS.
Page 9 of 17
Wellcome Open Research 2021, 6:114 Last updated: 11 JUN 2021
Illustrative example 2
Documents:
• Study: Golding, J., et al. (2019)
Press release: University of Bristol (2019). Thinking
positive during pregnancy?
• Media reports (n=16)
Study summary:
Data from the ALSPAC longitudinal birth cohort study were
analysed to assess the association between maternal locus
of control (LOC), measured during pregnancy, and children’s
reasoning skills in maths and science. Maternal external
LOC was associated with poorer academic performance in
children. Three separate sets of factors were controlled for, to
assess the extent to which they mediated the association. Taken
together they identified at least 50% of the mechanism.
Comparison of study, PR and media reports:
Key changes at each stage of the risk reporting pathway are
summarised in Figure 8.
Description of the research and its findings: The study
design was reported accurately in the PR and all 16 media
reports. However, there were linguistic differences in the descrip-
tions of the exposure and outcome of interest. The exposure
discussed in the study was maternal LOC, which is defined as a
personality scale that “identifies individuals’ general attitude
to what happens to them as largely a matter of luck or fate or
of powerful others (externality) or whether they feel they can
influence the consequences (internality)”. However, the press
release presented this as “thinking positively during pregnancy”,
which is arguably inaccurate, since it implies optimism rather
than a sense of control. All 16 media reports mentioned both
LOC and “thinking positively”, but the latter was more promi-
nent, appearing in almost all the headlines (n=14). The PR
also stated that people with an external LOC are less “motivated
into action” or believe there is “little point in making an effort”,
which was echoed in 14 media reports. The temporal nature
of LOC is also presented misleadingly in the PR. In the study,
LOC is understood as a consistent personality characteristic,
which was measured during pregnancy as a baseline. Indeed,
its manifestation in parenting behaviours long after pregnancy
is a primary avenue of enquiry for the authors. Nonetheless,
the PR fails to explain this, and instead only discusses mater-
nal attitude during pregnancy, implying that the importance of
LOC is limited to those nine months, and that a mother’s
mindset during pregnancy can impact her children for years
to come regardless of her future behaviours.
The outcome of interest, maths and science reasoning, was
reported accurately in the PR and all 16 media reports, although
almost all reports used more generalised language such as
“bright”, “gifted” or “the next Einstein”. However, the PR intro-
duced additional outcomes which had not been included in the
study, including children’s health, confidence, independence,
sleep, emotional control, diet, school-related social difficulties,
and obesity. All of these additional outcomes were introduced
via quotes from two of the study’s authors, despite bearing no
clear relevance to the findings of this particular study. These
outcomes were mentioned in almost all (n=14) media reports.
Figure 8. Key changes along the risk reporting pathway (illustrative example 2).
Page 10 of 17
Wellcome Open Research 2021, 6:114 Last updated: 11 JUN 2021
A primary objective of the study was to assess the extent to
which the association is mediated by factors including perinatal
exposures and parenting behaviours, which were found to explain
more than half of the mechanism. Some of these parenting
behaviours were mentioned in the PR, but their role as media-
tors of the association was not discussed. This was also absent
from all 16 media reports.
Caveats, criticism and study limitations: The PR does not
mention any of the study’s limitations or caveats, including the
differentiation of association and causation. However, the
majority of media reports (n=13) did reinstate this caveat.
Associated discussion topics: The primary discussion topic
in the media reports was past research on LOC. This theme was
present in both the PR and the study itself.
Advice, warnings and reassurance: The PR introduced
direct advice to the general public. Even the phrasing of its
headline and opening sentence (“Thinking positively during
pregnancy?”; “Your attitude during pregnancy…” etc.) is clearly
intended to address women directly. There is also discussion
in the form of quotes from the study’s authors about moth-
ers changing their outlook in order to improve their children’s
outcomes. Almost all (n=14) media reports included indirect
advice through discussion of mothers altering their LOC,
mainly through the inclusion of these quotes from the PR.
Summary: The press release altered the description of the
exposure of interest to “thinking positively during pregnancy”,
arguably misrepresenting the meaning of LOC and implying
that its importance is limited to the nine months of pregnancy.
Several child outcomes that were not explored in the study were
added to the PR via the inclusion of author quotes. Additionally,
the PR did not discuss the study’s limitations or state that
causation had not been established, and there was no discussion
in the PR of parenting as a mediating factor of the association.
The press coverage echoed the PR closely with two exceptions:
the generalisation of the outcome of interest (maths and science
reasoning) to “bright”, “gifted” etc., and the reinstatement of
the caveat that causation was not established in almost all
reports (n=13).
Illustrative example 3
Documents:
• Study: Gignac, F., et al. (2019)
Press release: IS Global (2019). Maternal Nut
Consumption During Pregnancy Linked to Improvements in
Neurodevelopment in Children
• Media reports (n=17)
Study summary:
Data from the Spanish Childhood and Environment (Infancia
y Medio Ambiente, INMA) Project, a population-based
birth cohort study, were analysed to assess whether maternal
nut consumption during pregnancy is associated with child
neuropsychological outcomes. First-trimester nut consump-
tion was associated with improved child performance in four
cognitive tests. After adjusting for confounders, reduced hit
reaction time standard error in the Attention Network Test
(ANT), a measure of sustained attention, remained significant.
Third trimester nut intake showed a weaker association.
Comparison of study, PR and media reports:
Key changes at each stage of the risk reporting pathway are
summarised in Figure 9.
Figure 9. Key changes along the risk reporting pathway (illustrative example 3).
Page 11 of 17
Wellcome Open Research 2021, 6:114 Last updated: 11 JUN 2021
Description of the research and its findings: The study’s
design and the exposure of interest (maternal nut intake) were
described accurately in the PR and media reports. All 17 media
reports mentioned maternal nut intake, with the majority
specifying the first trimester of pregnancy and giving details
of serving size and/or frequency of consumption (n=13 and
n=10 respectively).
For the outcome, similarly to the previous examples, almost
all the media reports (n=16) used simpler language, express-
ing neuropsychological development as “intelligent”, “smart” or
“brainy”, although most (n=12) also mentioned the specific fac-
ets measured, i.e. attention, memory, or cognitive function.
Three media reports introduced additional outcomes, the inci-
dence of learning difficulties and super-high IQs, which had not
been mentioned in either the study or the PR. These were
included in the form of a quote from one of the study’s authors,
which was not present in the PR. The same three media reports
also stated that child performance in cognitive tests had
improved by an average of 3%, a figure that is not given in
either the PR or the study.
Caveats, criticism and study limitations: The study was clear
that while improvements were observed in all cognitive tests,
only one (hit reaction time standard error in the ART) remained
significant after fully adjusting for confounders. However, this
is not specified in the PR, which describes in its by-line “bet-
ter outcomes after birth in cognitive function, attention capac-
ity and long-term working memory”. None of the media reports
explained that only the ART results were significant after
adjusting.
The PR omitted any discussion of the study’s limitations, but
it did state that the findings should be interpreted with caution.
Only three media reports included any caveats in their report-
ing of the study. One media report included the caveat that cau-
sation had not been established since this was an observational
study, which was not stated explicitly in either the study or PR
(although both acknowledged that further research is required
to confirm the findings).
Associated discussion topics: Four media reports discussed
maternal nutrition and the benefits of eating nuts more
generally. Both these topics were present in the study and PR.
Advice, warnings, and reassurance: Just under half of the
media reports (n=8) used language that addressed advice to
women directly, for example, “eat nuts for a brainy baby” or
“mothers told” to eat nuts. This direct language was not present
in the study or PR, but the PR did contain indirect advice
through discussion of the benefits and the ideal amount of nuts
to eat. One media report sourced an expert quote from the Royal
College of Obstetricians and Gynaecologists, which discussed
the existing guidance on nut consumption during pregnancy.
Summary: The PR generally reflected the study’s findings
accurately, but omitted caveats, which remained absent in the
majority of media reports. Media reports sometimes introduced
direct advice, and almost always generalised the outcome of
interest to “brainy”, “smart”, etc., but otherwise the majority
reported the study’s findings accurately. However, three media
reports introduced a new figure (3% average improvement)
alongside two new outcome measures (incidence of learning
difficulties and super-high IQs) which had not been present
in the study or PR. All three appeared to source the lat-
ter information from one of the study’s authors. It is unclear
whether the 3% figure was obtained from the same source.
Illustrative example 4
Documents:
• Study: Zhang, T., et al. (2019)
Science Media Centre (2018). expert reaction to study
on caesareans and neurodevelopmental disorders
• Media reports (n=16)
NB no PR was published to accompany this study.
Study summary:
A systematic review and meta-analysis of 61 studies, com-
prising over 20 million births, found that caesarean birth was
significantly associated with both autistic spectrum disorder
(ASD) and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in
children.
Comparison of study and media reports:
Key differences between the study and media reports are
summarised in Figure 10.
Description of the research and its findings: All 16 media
reports described the study accurately, reporting the exposure and
outcome of interest correctly. The majority of reports specified
the relative increased risk for ASD and ADHD (n=14 and n=13
respectively). One media report also specified the increase in
absolute risk for ASD, which was not mentioned in the study
but discussed in the SMC expert reaction summary.
Caveats, criticism and study limitations:
The majority of media reports (n=10) conveyed caveats
given in the study. Of the six that did not, five were passing
references to the study in articles about caesarean births
published several weeks after the study’s publication. Ten
reports stated explicitly that causation had not been estab-
lished, with one including this caveat in the headline. Half of the
reports (n=8) included the caveat that the mechanism by which
caesarean birth is associated with ASD and ADHD is not
understood, and eight mentioned confounding factors that
could explain the link. Five reports stated that Caesarean deliv-
ery is sometimes necessary to reduce other risks. Most reports
(n=10) also added expert criticism sourced from outside the
PR (mostly from the SMC summary).
Associated discussion topics:
Half of the media reports (n=8) discussed the rate of caesar-
ean births and seven discussed previous research on caesarean
births. Both topics of discussion were present in the study.
Page 12 of 17
Wellcome Open Research 2021, 6:114 Last updated: 11 JUN 2021
Figure 10. Key changes along the risk reporting pathway (illustrative example 4).
Advice, warnings and reassurance:
No media reports advised that caesarean births should be
avoided, and most (n=14) omitted the recommendation from
the study that caesarean delivery should be used judiciously.
Nearly half of the media reports (n=7) included explicit reas-
surance from experts, sourced from the SMC, that women
should not be alarmed by the findings, with one report
including this in its headline.
Summary: All media reports described the findings accu-
rately, with the majority specifying the relative increase in risk
for both ASD and ADHD. Ten reports included caveats from the
study and also included expert commentary from other sources.
No warnings or advice were added by the media, and in fact the
study’s recommendation that caesarean delivery should be used
judiciously was mostly omitted. Instead, expert reassurance
to women was included in seven reports, including once in
the headline.
Discussion
We found that risk messages relating to pregnancy and fertility
are a common feature of UK print and online media reporting,
appearing in 43 headlines per month on average – more than
once per day. Each month there were, on average, 14 unique
stories based on a new scientific study, suggesting that roughly
every other day a novel scientific finding regarding pregnancy
or fertility risk is presented to the public. These messages
contribute to the context of pregnant women’s health-related
decisions. Since the majority of the science-based unique stories
we identified concerned lifestyle or health factors, particularly
food and drink, medication or underlying health conditions, their
cumulative effect may increase anxiety among the pregnant
population that any habit, lifestyle factor, or even emotion
they experience during pregnancy is now considered a risk
factor.
The majority of the unique stories focused on risk factors relat-
ing to the pregnant woman, which were framed in most cases
only in terms of their possible impact on fetal health, without
discussion of the health outcomes (either positive or nega-
tive) for the woman. The separation of maternal and child health
outcomes in this way positions pregnant women as vectors of
potential harm or benefit to their fetus, and frames their health
status, behaviours and mindset during pregnancy as determi-
nants of offspring wellbeing, both immediately and long into the
future. Previous authors have raised concerns about this
positioning of women as vectors, which has been used to jus-
tify increasing surveillance and scrutiny of pregnant women’s
choices without due consideration of their autonomy1216.
PRs had a strong influence on media coverage, indicating that
academic institutions have a large degree of control over the
media’s reporting of their research. Our findings support
previous research that exaggeration in PRs is echoed in media
reports1,8. To a degree, journalists fulfilled a corrective role,
reinstating caveats omitted at the PR stage and sourcing addi-
tional expert criticism. However, certain misleading elements
introduced at the PR stage (such as the framing of external
LOC in example 2 as “thinking positively during pregnancy”)
remained present in the media coverage, and some caveats or
other pertinent contextualising information (such as the lower
FASD estimates in example 1, or the role of parenting behav-
iours in example 2) were not recovered at the media stage to
the risk reporting pathway. Since media reports inform the
public’s health behaviours, understanding and decisions46, these
inaccuracies matter. The portrayal of external LOC as “thinking
Page 13 of 17
Wellcome Open Research 2021, 6:114 Last updated: 11 JUN 2021
positively during pregnancy”, for example, may conceiv-
ably cause women who have difficult pregnancies or antenatal
depression to feel that they have caused irreversible harm to their
child, and this is compounded in that example by the lack of
explanation in the PR and media reports that parental behaviour
is a mediating factor of the association. Efforts at the PR stage
to ensure that accuracy, caveats and context are preserved
may help to avoid such instances of needless anxiety.
PRs were not the only route by which information reached
journalists: several media reports included additional expert
commentary sourced from the SMC, other experts, or directly
from the study authors. In illustrative example 4, which had no
PR, we observed a noteworthy reliance on expert quotes sourced
from the SMC and elsewhere, suggesting that in the absence of a
PR journalists may choose not to add interpretation themselves,
preferring to rely on expert opinion. Conversely, in illustra-
tive example 1, the SMC materials which were highly critical
of the study were not frequently used. This may indicate
that time-pressed journalists are less compelled to consult
additional sources for interpretative expert commentary if a
comprehensive PR is available. If true, this would constitute
another way in which PRs exert influence on media report-
ing. Reduced journalist reliance on external expert commentary
is material because, in our examples, the heavy use of exter-
nal sources appeared to yield more accurate and reassuring
reporting. The media coverage in illustrative example 4 deviated
the least from the underlying study, and was particularly meas-
ured and reassuring, incorporating expert quotes that empha-
sised the study’s limitations and explicitly told women not to
worry. Notably, the lack of PR in illustrative example 4 did not
appear to hamper media coverage, and neither was coverage
hampered by the publication of critical SMC round-ups: exam-
ples 1 and 4 each generated 16 headlines, despite the cautionary
nature of the accompanying SMC commentaries.
To conclude, the PRs analysed here omitted caveats and
other pertinent information, introduced advice to women, and
occasionally included misleading elements. Greater reliance on
SMC (and other third-party) expert materials appears conducive
to reliable, measured reporting. We suggest that a new model of
science dissemination may be desirable, whereby journalists
are encouraged to consult expert opinion from a variety of
sources or make greater use of comment round-ups compiled by
independent third parties such as the SMC.
Strengths and limitations
Our study did not analyse broadcast media or social media, both
of which are important streams by which health information
is disseminated. Our four illustrative examples were selected
based on volume of coverage, which ensured a richness of
material to analyse, but also rendered them “outliers” among
our unique stories (which mostly only generated a small number
of headlines) and therefore possibly atypical. Lastly, this is an
observational analysis based on correlational relationships and
an assumption that the risk reporting pathway was followed.
It is possible that similarities between PRs and media reports
were not causal and occurred by coincidence.
Future research
The media landscape does not necessarily reflect the nature
of research undertaken in the field, since not all studies are
reported. Future research could analyse risk messages dissemi-
nated by journals or research institutions to examine whether the
fetus-centric framing observed in the media is also present ear-
lier on the risk reporting pathway. It would also be interesting
to repeat this analysis for risk headlines relating to infant care,
including breastfeeding. On the role of PRs, further research
could compare media coverage of scientific papers published
with or without a PR, to assess whether PRs alter the volume or
nature of coverage. The use of SMC commentary by journal-
ists could also be examined to see whether its uptake is reduced
by the availability of a comprehensive PR, and whether its use
generally leads to more balanced and/or reassuring reporting.
Data availability
Figshare. Risk messages relating to pregnancy and fertility:
a media content analysis (dataset). https://doi.org/10.6084/
m9.figshare.14480784.v122
Data are available under the terms of the Creative Commons
Zero “No rights reserved” data waiver (CC0 1.0 Public domain
dedication).
Acknowledgements
Special thanks are due to Dr Heather Trickey of Cardiff
University for her contributions and counsel. We also thank
members of the WRISK Oversight Committee for their sup-
port, and particularly the committee’s chair, Dame Cathy
Warwick.
References
1. SumnerP,Vivian-GrithsS,BoivinJ,et al.: The association between
exaggeration in health related science news and academic press releases:
retrospective observational study. BMJ. 2014; 349: g7015.
PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text | Free Full Text
2. RieschH,SpiegelhalterDJ:‘Careless pork costs lives’: Risk stories from
science to press release to media. Heal Risk Soc. 2011; 13(1): 47–64.
Publisher Full Text
3. TullochJC,ZinnJO:Risk, health and the media. Heal Risk Soc. 2011; 13(1): 1–16.
Publisher Full Text
4. AdamsRC,SumnerP,Vivian-GrithsS,et al.: How readers understand causal
and correlational expressions used in news headlines. J Exp Psychol Appl.
2017; 23(1): 1–14.
PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text
5. GrilliR,RamsayC,MinozziS:Mass media interventions: eects on health
Page 14 of 17
Wellcome Open Research 2021, 6:114 Last updated: 11 JUN 2021
services utilisation. Cochrane Database Syst Rev.2002;(1):CD000389.
PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text
6. ThirlawayKJ,HeggsDA:Interpreting risk messages: Women’s responses to a
health story. Heal Risk Soc. 2005; 7(2): 107–121.
Publisher Full Text
7. LeeE,SuttonRM,HartleyBL:From scientic article to press release to
media coverage: advocating alcohol abstinence and democratising risk in
a story about alcohol and pregnancy. Heal Risk Soc. 2016; 18(5–6):
247–269.
PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text | Free Full Text
8. BrattonL,AdamsRC,ChallengerA,et al.: Causal claims about correlations
reduced in press releases following academic study of health news
[version 1; peer review: 1 approved, 1 approved with reservations].
Wellcome Open Res. 2020; 5: 6.
Publisher Full Text
9. SumnerP,Vivian-GrithsS,BoivinJ,et al.: Exaggerations and Caveats in
Press Releases and Health-Related Science News. PLoS One. 2016; 11(12):
e0168217.
PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text | Free Full Text
10. AdamsRC,ChallengerA,BrattonL,et al.: Claims of causality in health news:
a randomised trial. BMC Med.2019;17(1):91.
PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text | Free Full Text
11. BottL,BrattonL,DiaconuB,et al.: Caveats in science-based news stories
communicate caution without lowering interest. J Exp Psychol Appl.2019;
25(4): 517–542.
PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text
12. Lupton D: ‘Precious cargo’: Foetal subjects, risk and reproductive
citizenship. Crit Public Health. 2012; 22(3):329–340.
Publisher Full Text
13. Ruhl L: Liberal governance and prenatal care: Risk and regulation in
pregnancy. Econ Soc.1999;28(1):95–117.
Publisher Full Text
14. Lowe P, Lee E, Yardley L: Under the Inuence? The Construction of Foetal
Alcohol Syndrome in UK Newspapers. Sociol Res Online. 2010; 15(4):75–84.
Publisher Full Text
15. LoweP,LeeE,MacvarishJ:Growing better brains? Pregnancy and
neuroscience discourses in English social and welfare policies. Health Risk
Soc. 2015; 17(1):15–29.
PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text | Free Full Text
16. Lowe P: Idealized Pregnancy. In: Reproductive Health and Maternal Sacrice.
(PalgraveMacmillanUK).2016;109–137.
Publisher Full Text
17. Timmermans S, Leiter V: The Redemption of Thalidomide: Standardizing the
Risk of Birth Defects. Soc Stud Sci. 2000; 30(1): 41–71.
PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text
18. RichardsonSS,DanielsCR,GillmanMW,et al.: Society: Don’t blame the
mothers. Nature. 2014; 512(7513): 131–132.
PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text
19. SharpGC,LawlorDA,RichardsonSS:It’s the mother!: How assumptions
about the causal primacy of maternal eects inuence research on the
developmental origins of health and disease. Soc Sci Med.2018;213: 20–27.
PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text | Free Full Text
20. HaugenAC,SchugTT,CollmanG,et al.: Evolution of DOHaD: the impact of
environmental health sciences. J Dev Orig Health Dis. 2015; 6(2): 55–64.
PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full Text | Free Full Text
21. Wriskproject:Risk reporting pathway. 2020.
Reference Source
22. MarshallO,BlaylockR,MurphyC,et al.: Risk messages relating to pregnancy
and fertility: a media content analysis (dataset). 2021.
http://www.doi.org/10.6084/m9.gshare.14480784.v1
Page 15 of 17
Wellcome Open Research 2021, 6:114 Last updated: 11 JUN 2021
Open Peer Review
Current Peer Review Status:
Version 1
Reviewer Report11 June 2021
https://doi.org/10.21956/wellcomeopenres.18465.r43901
© 2021 MacKendrick N. This is an open access peer review report distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium,
provided the original work is properly cited.
Norah MacKendrick
Department of Sociology, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ, USA
This is an important study that examines how the news media reports scientific research related
to pregnancy and fertility. The research objectives are clear and the methodologies are
appropriate. The manuscript would benefit from revisions, namely to fill in missing details across
several sections of the paper.
Introduction
The manuscript has two general objectives: assess i) risk framing in PRs and news media reports
and, ii) the accuracy of news reporting of scientific studies related to fertility and pregnancy. The
introduction could state more clearly these two general objectives. They appear only at the end of
the introductory section (p. 3).
In this section, the authors refer to the literature on the framing of risk messages in PRs and the
impact of this framing on the public’s understanding of health risks and attribution of blame.
These are important points that the authors re-visit in the discussion. The summary of this
literature is very short and could be expanded upon. On page 3, for example, the authors cite
several key articles (e.g. Reisch and Spiegelhalter; Tulloch and Zinn) but say little about the larger
findings from this literature. This is important context that helps to set the stage for the study and
the discussion section.
Methods
The methods section is well organized and the methods used are appropriate. I appreciate the use
of figures to show the reader the various stages of data collection and analysis. The authors could
say more about why certain kinds of news articles were important and others were excluded. This
section should also explain the rationale for the time period of 2018-19.
Analysis
The analysis is detailed and the use of figures is helpful. The specific approach for evaluating
accuracy remains vague. On pages 3-4, the authors refer to risk reporting pathways as a measure
of reliability. If this is the method of measuring accuracy, the authors could tell us more about it.
Page 16 of 17
Wellcome Open Research 2021, 6:114 Last updated: 11 JUN 2021
Figures 5 and 6 capture some elements of framing, as well as article content, but these figures
could be explained in more detail in the text.
Discussion
This section summarizes the main findings and their implications. The role of PRs in shaping
media reports is an important finding. The authors could say even more about what they learned
about the framing of pregnancy and fertility risks. Were risk messages in PRs the same as those in
the media reports? What do we learn about risk framing in cases where the framing between PR
and article diverged?
Finally, the authors could revise the manuscript to add more text to the paper to guide the reader
through each section of the manuscript. There are few transition and introductory sentences in
each section, which gives the paper a “choppy” flow. Adding some of these would help the reader
follow the paper from section to section.
I appreciated the opportunity to review this paper and hope to cite it in my own work.
Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?
Yes
Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?
Yes
Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?
Partly
If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?
Yes
Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?
Yes
Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?
Yes
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Reviewer Expertise: media framing, gender and risk, sociology of health, sociology of
environmental risk
I confirm that I have read this submission and believe that I have an appropriate level of
expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.
Page 17 of 17
Wellcome Open Research 2021, 6:114 Last updated: 11 JUN 2021
... reported in press statements and media headlines, which do not always fully reflect the nuances of results, which may create alarm, or additional pressure for women and the people who care for them ( Marshall et al., 2021 ). ...
... There is an implicit assumption about the "causal primacy of maternal pregnancy effects" and this sets the agenda of DOHaD research which is reinforced and reproduced, rather than challenged ( Sharp et al., 2018 ). Our research on the reporting of pregnancy related studies found that the majority of studies that were reported in the UK mainstream media frame mothers, rather than protective towards their unborn infants, as vectors of potential harm to their children, who are the focus of the health outcomes ( Marshall et al., 2021 ). A recent report found an imbalance between women's research priorities, such as perinatal mental health and research funding, suggesting a realignment of research priorities with women's needs is urgently needed ( Guthrie et al., 2020 ). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Women receive many public health messages relating to pregnancy which are intended to improve outcomes for babies and mothers. However, negotiating the risk landscape and maternity care system can feel confusing and disempowering. Relationships between women and their healthcare providers are paramount, but they can be adversely affected by issues of trust and autonomy. Methods We used a nested study design including an online survey and qualitative interviews to gain an understanding of women's experiences of risk messages during pregnancy. We purposively sampled survey participants to ensure the interview population included women whose voices are seldom heard and are disproportionately impacted by poor risk communication. Results A total of 7,009 women responded to the survey, and 34 women participated in interviews. Participants received public health and risk messages from a range of sources. Data showed that women wanted a balance between a “better safe than sorry” approach and evidence-based information and advice. Women reported a discrepancy between the topics they received a lot of information on and areas in which they felt they needed more advice. Many participants said they were given conflicting advice, and the way information was delivered sometimes challenged their autonomy. We identified that younger women (<20 years old) and women with higher BMIs experienced stigmatisation in their maternity care. Conclusions Our research shows the importance of risk communication that respects women's autonomy and trusts them to make decisions about their own pregnancy. We identified a need for a layered approach to risk communication. Whilst some women are happy to adopt precautionary behaviour without discussion, others will want a thorough examination of the evidence-base. Our findings suggest that more individualised care, continuity, and less judgement and stigmatisation from HCPs will improve experiences for women and may lead to better engagement with services.
... It may be possible that some of the women participants met these criteria given the hereditary nature of some health conditions, but this information was not captured. The WRISK study highlighted concerns that current pregnancy risk messaging prioritises fetal health over the women's health outcomes [70,71]. Therefore, this study focuses on maternal and child outcomes that are important to women with multimorbidity and information that will help women make informed decisions for their own care during pregnancy and in the postpartum period. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Heterogeneity in reported outcomes can limit the synthesis of research evidence. A core outcome set informs what outcomes are important and should be measured as a minimum in all future studies. We report the development of a core outcome set applicable to observational and interventional studies of pregnant women with multimorbidity. Methods We developed the core outcome set in four stages: (i) a systematic literature search, (ii) three focus groups with UK stakeholders, (iii) two rounds of Delphi surveys with international stakeholders and (iv) two international virtual consensus meetings. Stakeholders included women with multimorbidity and experience of pregnancy in the last 5 years, or are planning a pregnancy, their partners, health or social care professionals and researchers. Study adverts were shared through stakeholder charities and organisations. Results Twenty-six studies were included in the systematic literature search (2017 to 2021) reporting 185 outcomes. Thematic analysis of the focus groups added a further 28 outcomes. Two hundred and nine stakeholders completed the first Delphi survey. One hundred and sixteen stakeholders completed the second Delphi survey where 45 outcomes reached Consensus In (≥70% of all participants rating an outcome as Critically Important). Thirteen stakeholders reviewed 15 Borderline outcomes in the first consensus meeting and included seven additional outcomes. Seventeen stakeholders reviewed these 52 outcomes in a second consensus meeting, the threshold was ≥80% of all participants voting for inclusion. The final core outcome set included 11 outcomes. The five maternal outcomes were as follows: maternal death, severe maternal morbidity, change in existing long-term conditions (physical and mental), quality and experience of care and development of new mental health conditions. The six child outcomes were as follows: survival of baby, gestational age at birth, neurodevelopmental conditions/impairment, quality of life, birth weight and separation of baby from mother for health care needs. Conclusions Multimorbidity in pregnancy is a new and complex clinical research area. Following a rigorous process, this complexity was meaningfully reduced to a core outcome set that balances the views of a diverse stakeholder group.
... 7 There is growing concern that public health messages aimed at pregnant women, including those relating to medication use, do not always fully reflect or explain the evidence base underpinning them and the nuances and complexity of information is lost. 12 The inability to receive effective medications is not without consequence for women and babies. A survey in 2015 identified that women with severe hyperemesis gravidarum frequently had difficulty obtaining swift treatment and support for debilitating pregnancy sickness resulting in some terminating an otherwise wanted pregnancy. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives: To explore women's experiences of over-the-counter and prescription medication advice and use during pregnancy. Design: A study design consisting of an online survey and nested in-depth interviews with a subsample of participants. We analysed data from survey free-text responses and in-depth interviews using thematic analysis. Quantitative survey data is published elsewhere. Setting: The UK. Participants: Women were eligible if living in the UK, aged 16-45 years, were pregnant or had been pregnant in the last 5 years regardless of pregnancy outcome. A total of 7090 women completed the survey, and 34 women who collectively had experienced 68 pregnancies were subsequently interviewed. Results: Medication prescribing and use during pregnancy was common. The prescribing, dispensing and taking of some advised medications were restricted through women's or prescribers' fear of fetal harm. Lack of adherence to national prescribing guidance, conflicting professional opinion and poor communication resulted in maternal anxiety, avoidable morbidity and women negotiating complex and distressing pathways to obtain recommended medications. In contrast, some women felt overmedicated and that pharmacological treatments were used without exploring other options first. Conclusion: Increased translation of national guidance into practice and greater personalisation of antenatal care are needed to improve the safety, efficacy and personalisation of prescribing in pregnancy.
... 1 Similarly, a consistent theme identified in reviews of the care of women who die is inappropriate withdrawal of drug treatment before or in early pregnancy by clinicians or women themselves. 7 We must recognise the harm inherent in the pervasive risk messaging in relation to pregnancy drug treatment from the media, 8 the actions of regulatory authorities, pharmaceutical companies, and insurers. The introduction of a programme designed to prevent in utero exposure to valproate, for example, has led to increased numbers of maternal deaths from sudden unexpected death in epilepsy among women taking no or inadequate drug treatment for their epilepsy. ...
Chapter
One of the most dramatic changes to women's lives in the twentieth century was the advent of safe childbirth, reducing the maternal mortality rate from 1 in 400 births to 1 in 10,000 in just 80 years. The impetus behind this change was the Confidential Enquiries into Maternal Death (CEMD), now the world's longest running self-audit of a healthcare service. Here, leading authors in the CEMD tell the story of the pioneering clinicians behind the push for improvements, who received little recognition for their work despite its far-reaching consequences. One by one, the leading causes of maternal death were identified and resolved, from sepsis to safe abortions and more recently psychiatric illness and social and ethnic disparities in healthcare. Global maternal mortality is still too high; this valuable book shows how significant advances in maternal healthcare are possible when clinicians, politicians and the public work together.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Exaggerations in health news were previously found to strongly associate with similar exaggerations in press releases. Moreover such exaggerations did not appear to attract more news. Here we assess whether press release practice changed after these reported findings; simply drawing attention to the issue may be insufficient for practical change, given the challenges of media environments. Methods: We assessed whether rates of causal over-statement in press releases based on correlational data were lower following a seminal paper on the topic, compared to an equivalent baseline period in the preceding year. Results: We found that over-statements in press releases reduced from 28% (95% confidence interval = 16% to 45%) in 2014 to 13% (95% confidence interval = 6% to 25%) in 2015. A corresponding numerical reduction in exaggerations in news was not significant. The association between over-statements in news and press releases remained strong. Conclusions: Press release over-statements were less frequent following publication of Sumner et al. (2014), indicating that press release practice is malleable. However, this is correlational evidence and the reduction may be due to other factors.
Article
Full-text available
Science stories in the media are strongly linked to changes in health-related behavior. Science writers (including journalists, press officers, and researchers) must therefore frame their stories to communicate scientific caution without disrupting coherence and disengaging the reader. In this study we investigate whether caveats ("Further research is needed to validate the results") satisfy this dual requirement. In four experiments participants read news reports with and without caveats. In Experiments 1 to 3, participants judged how cautious or confident researchers were, and how interesting or comprehensible they found the reports. News reports with caveats were judged as more cautious that those without, but levels of reader interest and comprehensibility were unaffected. In a fourth experiment, we created a mock newsroom and recruited journalism students to make judgments about which press releases should be published. Here, neither caveats nor the introduction of qualifying expressions in headlines had an effect on judgments of newsworthiness, consistent with Experiments 1 to 3. The reasons participants gave for rejecting a press release rarely referred to the caveat. Our results therefore suggest that science writers should include caveats in news reporting and that they can do so without fear of disengaging their readers or losing news uptake. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Background Misleading news claims can be detrimental to public health. We aimed to improve the alignment between causal claims and evidence, without losing news interest (counter to assumptions that news is not interested in communicating caution). Methods We tested two interventions in press releases, which are the main sources for science and health news: (a) aligning the headlines and main causal claims with the underlying evidence (strong for experimental, cautious for correlational) and (b) inserting explicit statements/caveats about inferring causality. The ‘participants’ were press releases on health-related topics (N = 312; control = 89, claim alignment = 64, causality statement = 79, both = 80) from nine press offices (journals, universities, funders). Outcomes were news content (headlines, causal claims, caveats) in English-language international and national media (newspapers, websites, broadcast; N = 2257), news uptake (% press releases gaining news coverage) and feasibility (% press releases implementing cautious statements). Results News headlines showed better alignment to evidence when press releases were aligned (intention-to-treat analysis (ITT) 56% vs 52%, OR = 1.2 to 1.9; as-treated analysis (AT) 60% vs 32%, OR = 1.3 to 4.4). News claims also followed press releases, significant only for AT (ITT 62% vs 60%, OR = 0.7 to 1.6; AT, 67% vs 39%, OR = 1.4 to 5.7). The same was true for causality statements/caveats (ITT 15% vs 10%, OR = 0.9 to 2.6; AT 20% vs 0%, OR 16 to 156). There was no evidence of lost news uptake for press releases with aligned headlines and claims (ITT 55% vs 55%, OR = 0.7 to 1.3, AT 58% vs 60%, OR = 0.7 to 1.7), or causality statements/caveats (ITT 53% vs 56%, OR = 0.8 to 1.0, AT 66% vs 52%, OR = 1.3 to 2.7). Feasibility was demonstrated by a spontaneous increase in cautious headlines, claims and caveats in press releases compared to the pre-trial period (OR = 1.01 to 2.6, 1.3 to 3.4, 1.1 to 26, respectively). Conclusions News claims—even headlines—can become better aligned with evidence. Cautious claims and explicit caveats about correlational findings may penetrate into news without harming news interest. Findings from AT analysis are correlational and may not imply cause, although here the linking mechanism between press releases and news is known. ITT analysis was insensitive due to spontaneous adoption of interventions across conditions. Trial registration ISRCTN10492618 (20 August 2015) Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (10.1186/s12916-019-1324-7) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Article
Full-text available
Research on the developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD) has traditionally focused on how maternal exposures around the time of pregnancy might influence offspring health and risk of disease. We acknowledge that for some exposures this is likely to be correct, but argue that the focus on maternal pregnancy effects also reflects implicit and deeply-held assumptions that 1) causal early life exposures are primarily transmitted via maternal traits or exposures, 2) maternal exposures around the time of pregnancy and early infancy are particularly important, and 3) other factors, such as paternal factors and postnatal exposures in later life, have relatively little impact in comparison. These implicit assumptions about the "causal primacy" of maternal pregnancy effects set the agenda for DOHaD research and, through a looping effect, are reinforced rather than tested. We propose practical strategies to redress this imbalance through maintaining a critical perspective about these assumptions.
Article
Full-text available
Exaggerated or simplistic news is often blamed for adversely influencing public health. However, recent findings suggested many exaggerations were already present in university press releases, which scientists approve. Surprisingly, these exaggerations were not associated with more news coverage. Here we test whether these two controversial results also arise in press releases from prominent science and medical journals. We then investigate the influence of mitigating caveats in press releases, to test assumptions that caveats harm news interest or are ignored.Using quantitative content analysis, we analyzed press releases (N = 534) on biomedical and health-related science issued by leading peer-reviewed journals. We similarly analysed the associated peer-reviewed papers (N = 534) and news stories (N = 582). Main outcome measures were advice to readers and causal statements drawn from correlational research. Exaggerations in press releases predicted exaggerations in news (odds ratios 2.4 and 10.9, 95% CIs 1.3 to 4.5 and 3.9 to 30.1) but were not associated with increased news coverage, consistent with previous findings. Combining datasets from universities and journals (996 press releases, 1250 news), we found that when caveats appeared in press releases there was no reduction in journalistic uptake, but there was a clear increase in caveats in news (odds ratios 9.6 and 9.5 for caveats for advice and causal claims, CIs 4.1 to 24.3 and 6.0 to 15.2). The main study limitation is its retrospective correlational nature.For health and science news directly inspired by press releases, the main source of both exaggerations and caveats appears to be the press release itself. However we find no evidence that exaggerations increase, or caveats decrease, the likelihood of news coverage. These findings should be encouraging for press officers and scientists who wish to minimise exaggeration and include caveats in their press releases.
Article
Full-text available
Science-related news stories can have a profound impact on how the public make decisions. The current study presents 4 experiments that examine how participants understand scientific expressions used in news headlines. The expressions concerned causal and correlational relationships between variables (e.g., “being breast fed makes children behave better”). Participants rated or ranked headlines according to the extent that one variable caused the other. Our results suggest that participants differentiate between 3 distinct categories of relationship: direct cause statements (e.g., “makes,” “increases”), which were interpreted as the most causal; can cause statements (e.g., “can make,” “can increase”); and moderate cause statements (e.g., “might cause,” “linked,” “associated with”), but do not consistently distinguish within the last group despite the logical distinction between cause and association. On the basis of this evidence, we make recommendations for appropriately communicating cause and effect in news headlines.
Article
Full-text available
In this article, we follow the approach taken by Riesch and Spiegalhalter in ?Careless pork costs lives?: Risk storiesfrom science to press release to media? published in this journal, anoffer an assessment of one example of a ?risk story?. Using content and thematic qualitative analysis, we consider how the findings of an article ?Fetal Alcohol Exposure and IQ at Age 8: Evidence from a Population-Based Birth-Cohort Study? were framed in the article itself, the associated press release, and the subsequent extensive media coverage. We contextualise this consideration of a risk story by discussing a body of work that critically engages with the development and global proliferation of efforts to advocate for alcohol abstinence to pregnant (and pre-pregnant) women. This work considers the ?democratisation? of risk, a term used to draw attention to the expansion of the definition of the problem of drinking in pregnancy to include any drinking and all women. We show here how this risk story contributed a new dimension to the democratisation of risk through claims that were made about uncertainty and certainty. A central argument we make concerns the contribution of the researchers themselves (not just lobby groups or journalists) to this outcome. We conclude that the democratisation of risk was advanced in this case not simply through journalists exaggerating and misrepresenting research findings, but that communication to the press and the initial interpretation of findings played their part. We suggest that this risk story raises concerns about the accuracy of reporting of research findings, and about the communication of unwarrantedly worrying messages to pregnant women about drinking alcohol.
Article
Full-text available
In recent years, English welfare and health policy has started to include pregnancy within the foundation stage of child development. The foetus is also increasingly designated as ‘at risk’ from pregnant women. In this article, we draw on an analysis of a purposive sample of English social and welfare policies and closely related advocacy documents to trace the emergence of neuroscientific claims-making in relation to the family. In this article, we show that a specific deterministic understanding of the developing brain that only has a loose relationship with current scientific evidence is an important component in these changes. We examine the ways in which pregnancy is situated in these debates. In these debates, maternal stress is identified as a risk to the foetus; however, the selective concern with women living in disadvantage undermines biological claims. The policy claim of neurological ‘critical windows’ also seems to be influenced by social concerns. Hence, these emerging concerns over the foetus’ developing brain seem to be situated within the gendered history of policing women’s pregnant bodies rather than acting on new insights from scientific discoveries. By situating these developments within the broader framework of risk consciousness, we can link these changes to wider understandings of the ‘at risk’ child and intensified surveillance over family life.
Article
Full-text available
To identify the source (press releases or news) of distortions, exaggerations, or changes to the main conclusions drawn from research that could potentially influence a reader's health related behaviour. Retrospective quantitative content analysis. Journal articles, press releases, and related news, with accompanying simulations. Press releases (n=462) on biomedical and health related science issued by 20 leading UK universities in 2011, alongside their associated peer reviewed research papers and news stories (n=668). Advice to readers to change behaviour, causal statements drawn from correlational research, and inference to humans from animal research that went beyond those in the associated peer reviewed papers. 40% (95% confidence interval 33% to 46%) of the press releases contained exaggerated advice, 33% (26% to 40%) contained exaggerated causal claims, and 36% (28% to 46%) contained exaggerated inference to humans from animal research. When press releases contained such exaggeration, 58% (95% confidence interval 48% to 68%), 81% (70% to 93%), and 86% (77% to 95%) of news stories, respectively, contained similar exaggeration, compared with exaggeration rates of 17% (10% to 24%), 18% (9% to 27%), and 10% (0% to 19%) in news when the press releases were not exaggerated. Odds ratios for each category of analysis were 6.5 (95% confidence interval 3.5 to 12), 20 (7.6 to 51), and 56 (15 to 211). At the same time, there was little evidence that exaggeration in press releases increased the uptake of news. Exaggeration in news is strongly associated with exaggeration in press releases. Improving the accuracy of academic press releases could represent a key opportunity for reducing misleading health related news. © Sumner et al 2014.
Chapter
As I outlined earlier, the idea of maternal sacrifice structures the ways in which fertility regulation is organized for women who do not fit the image of the ‘good mother’ who is required to prevent conception until such time (if ever) that she embodies the appropriate qualities of age and behaviour. This chapter will focus on how the presumption of maternal sacrifice within good motherhood plays out in the management of pregnancy. It will examine how the focus on the foetus means that women’s status as autonomous citizens can become compromised. It will illustrate the experiences of being pregnant within a medicalized context, when every choice that a woman makes, from eating to prenatal testing, is taken as evidence of her willingness to perform idealized motherhood. In other words, whilst nominally ‘choices’ can be made, there is often only one ‘right’ option for responsible women to make. An important element within this chapter will be showing how women are expected to constrain their lives before they conceive. Moreover, given that fertility and fertility control are always uncertain, these discourses potentially constrain all heterosexually active women between puberty and menopause. Drawing on the debates around issues such as alcohol consumption, the chapter will illustrate the ways in which restrictions on women’s lives can become policy despite an acknowledged lack of evidence of harm to the foetus. This focuses negative attention on visibly pregnant women, who can be publicly castigated for failing to comply with the increasingly tight rules. Thus women can be encouraged to make specific choices through the idea of sacrifice even when there will be no significant outcomes to the welfare of the foetus.