Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
Subcortical Structures and Creative Divergent
Thinking: A Resting-State functional MRI Analysis
Zhenni Gao
East China Normal University
Xiaojin Liu
Heinrich-Heine-Universitat Dusseldorf
Delong Zhang
South China Normal University
Ming Liu
South China Normal University
Ning Hao ( nhao@psy.ecnu.edu.cn )
East China Normal University
Research Article
Keywords: creative thinking, subcortex, functional connectivity, resting-state fMRI
DOI: https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-346412/v1
License: This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Read Full License
1
Running head: Subcortical Structures and Creative Divergent Thinking
Subcortical Structures and Creative Divergent Thinking:
A Resting-state functional MRI Analysis
Zhenni Gao1,a, Xiaojin Liu3,4,5,a, Delong Zhang2, Ming Liu2, Ning Hao1*
1 Shanghai Key Laboratory of Mental Health and Psychological Crisis Intervention,
School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai,
China.
2 Center for the Study of Applied Psychology, Key Laboratory of Mental Health and
Cognitive Science of Guangdong Province, School of Psychology, South China Normal
University, Guangzhou, China.
3 Institute of Systems Neuroscience, Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf,
Germany.
4 Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine (INM-7, Brain and Behaviour), Research
Centre Jülich, Jülich, Germany.
5 Center for Educational Science and Technology, Beijing Normal University at Zhuhai
519087, China
a Zhenni Gao and Xiaojin Liu contributed equally to this paper and both should be
considered as first authors.
* Correspondence author at: Shanghai Key Laboratory of Mental Health and
2
Psychological Crisis Intervention, School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East
China Normal University, No. 3663, North Zhong Shan Road, Shanghai 200062, China.
E-mail address: nhao@psy.ecnu.edu.cn (N. Hao).
Acknowledgements
This work was sponsored by the ECNU Academic Innovation Promotion Program
for Excellent Doctoral Students (YBNLTS2019-027) to ZG.
Author Contributions
Z. G., X. L., D. Z., M. L., and N.H. conceived the experiment. Z.G., X.L., and D.
Z. performed the research. X. L., and Z. G. analyzed the data. Z. G., X. L., and N. H.
wrote the paper.
Conflict of Interest: The authors have nothing to disclose.
Ethical approval: All procedures performed in studies involving human participants
were approved by the Institutional Review Board of South China Normal University.
Informed consent: All participants provided written consent.
3
Abstract
This study aimed to testify whether spontaneous fluctuations in the subcortex contribute
to creative divergent thinking. Individuals at high- and low levels of creativity were
recruited and the resting-state fMRI data was collected. Seed-wise and dynamic
functional connectivity (FC) were used to identify differences between the two groups.
The topological properties of the subcortical network were measured, and their
relationship with performance of creative divergent thinking was calculated using
brain-behaviour correlation analyses. The results revealed higher FC between the
putamen, pallidum, and thalamus in high creativity group (HCG) compared to low
creativity group (LCG) within the subcortex. Whole-brain FC results showed stronger
connection across subcortical (i.e., the thalamus and pallidum) and cerebral regions (i.e.,
the insula, middle frontal gyrus, and middle temporal gyrus) in HCG compared to LCG.
In addition, the subcortical FC demonstrated a positive correlation with performance of
creative thinking across the pallidum, putamen, and thalamus. Our findings may
provide novel insights into the relationship between creative divergent thinking and the
activities of the subcortex. It is likely that not only fronto-striatal dopaminergic
pathways, but also “motor” pathways, are involved in creative thinking processing.
Keywords: creative thinking, subcortex, functional connectivity, resting-state fMRI
4
Introduction
Creativity includes a series of complex cognitive processes, which induce activity
within multiple cortical structures, such as the prefrontal cortex (PFC) (Beaty et al.,
2015; Wei et al., 2014), middle frontal gyrus (MFG) (Bechtereva et al., 2004; Chávez-
Eakle et al., 2007), middle temporal gyrus (MTG) (Kounios et al., 2008), and insula
(Gao et al., 2017). It has been hypothesized that dopamine-coupled brain areas
contribute to creative thought (Heilman, Nadeau, & Beversdorf, 2003). On one hand,
the idea suggests that many creative traits (such as extraversion and openness) result
from the neural activity of dopamine-coupled regions (Schuler et al., 2019). On the
other hand, some researchers have suggested that higher dopamine receptor levels cause
greater cognitive flexibility, resulting in better creative performances (Ashby, Isen &
Turken, 1999; Chermahini, & Hommel, 2010).
Neuroimaging techniques have been widely used to examine potential morphological
and functional changes within subcortical regions associated with creativity. An
increasing number of studies have investigated the important role of dopamine-related
subcortical regions in creative thinking processing (Boot et al., 2017; Chen et al., 2019).
For example, increased regional grey matter volumes in the caudate nucleus and
midbrain are significantly related to divergent thinking performances (Takeuchi et al.,
2010). The recent burgeoning of research on creative thinking is indicative of a growing
conviction that subcortical regions, such as the putamen and nucleus accumbens
(NAcc), are involved in the process of insight, which is a crucial process in problem
solving (Shen et al., 2018; Tik et al., 2018). Similar results have been found in a very
5
recent publication by Chen et al. (2019), and the results of the resting-state fMRI (r-
fMRI) study have demonstrated that verbal divergent thinking is associated with neural
activity within subcortical regions, such as the thalamus and putamen. In summary,
creativity appears to be correlated with subcortical regions (Chen et al., 2019; Shen et
al., 2018). Although the impact of dopamine levels underlying creativity has been found
not only in behavioural experiments but also in relevant neuropsychiatric disorders (i.e.
Parkinson’s disease) (Faust-Socher et al., 2014; Salvi et al., 2015), the neural basis of
these subcortical regions relating to individual creativity based on spontaneous
fluctuations of the human brain is still unclear.
Generally, there are two different dopamine loops for information transmission
across subcortical regions. One is a “motor” loop passing largely through the putamen,
which receives inputs from the sensorimotor cortex and whose influences are ultimately
returned to the premotor areas (Alexander, Crutcher & DeLong, 1986, 1990). Another
is an “association” loop passing through the caudate nucleus, which receives input from
association areas and whose influences are ultimately returned to portions of the PFC
(Alexander, Delong & Strick, 1986). Based on current available results, however,
studies investigating subcortical areas that are involved in creative thinking are mainly
focused on one aspect — the “association” loop. For example, Boot et al. (2017)
demonstrated that creative thinking is strongly based on fronto-striatal dopaminergic
pathways, which contributes to flexibility in increasing creativity. However, it does not
suggest whether other regions, such as the pallidum and thalamus, are involved in the
process of creativity. Moreover, researchers have found that intranasal administration
6
of oxytocin boosts divergent thinking, flexibility, and creative insight performance (De
Dreu and Kret, 2016). This may be caused by decreased cortisol responses and fear
signals in the amygdalar-hippocampal circuit. Thus, increased creative performance
may be due to other neurotransmitters. However, it is unclear whether other
neurotransmitters such as norepinephrine and oxytocin contribute to creative thinking.
Neurons in the brain form complex networks (Cajal, 1995), which are thought to
underlie the physiological basis of information transfer and mental representation
(Strogatz, 2001). The human brain needs to expend 20% of the body’s energy, and
oxygen consumption by the brain is no lower at rest than during task performance
(Sokoloff, Wechsler, Mangold, Balls, & Kety, 1953). Before Biswal and colleagues
(1995) published findings about spontaneous fluctuations in the human brain using
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), scientists had suggested that the
spontaneous fluctuations in the human brain are noise. However, the study published
by Biswal and colleagues indicated that blood oxygen level-dependent signals show
low frequency fluctuations in the bilateral motor cortex (Biswal, Yetkin, Haughton, &
Hyde, 1995). That is, in the absence of explicit external or internal stimuli, the human
brain maintains a specific level of fluctuations. Many studies have found that
spontaneous activity in the human brain can shape most neural activity underlying
behaviour (Dietrich & Kanso, 2010; Wei et al., 2014), indicating that spontaneous
fluctuations could provide resources for our behaviour. Therefore, the neural basis of
behaviour can be explored by examining the brain networks underlying spontaneous
fluctuations (Zou et al., 2013), it seems to be effective that the neural basis of creativity
7
can be explored using resting-state data. A recent study indicated that creative divergent
thinking trainings give rise to changes in functional connectivity patterns of human
brain networks using resting-state data (Fink et al., 2018). This provides further
evidence that there is a consistency between resting-state and task-state connectivity
patterns related to creative thinking. On the other hand, the human brain is a complex
system and never in a static state (Marusak et al., 2017). Previous fMRI researches have
revealed the variations of static functional connections during the whole scanning
(Allen et al., 2014; Calhoun et al., 2014). Generally, static functional connectivity was
regularly divided into several continuous sections that are considered as dynamic
functional connectivity. Recent evidence has shown that dynamic functional
connectivity may reveal a great deal of information in time-varying neural activity of
the human brain (Calhoun et al., 2014; Rashid et al., 2016). Therefore, it provides a
comprehensive insight that we used both static and dynamic functional connectivity to
explore the basis of creative brain network.
Although previous studies have investigated the relationship between special
dopamine loops and creativity, subcortical-wide regional connectivity with cortical
regions contributing to individual creativity ability has been neglected. Additionally,
dopamine loops within subcortical regions are more complex than between subcortico-
cortical regions. Whether the interactions of these subcortical regions are involved in
creativity should also be examined. In addition, more information could likely be
obtained to investigate the interactions between subcortical regions by using dynamic
functional connectivity. The primary purpose of this study was to address each of the
8
following issues. First, to identify the subcortical brain regions that contribute to our
creative abilities. Second, whether highly creative individuals show different functional
connectivity compared to individuals with low creativity. Third, whether the network
efficiency of the cortico-subcortical network exhibits differences between individuals
with high and low levels of creativity.
In the present study, we recruited a high creativity group (HCG, n = 22) and a low
creativity group (LCG, n = 22) based on divergent thinking test scores. We calculated
functional connectivity (FC) maps based on resting-state functional magnetic resonance
imaging (r-fMRI) data. Meanwhile, the topological properties of the whole-brain
network, which showed differences between the two groups, were measured using
graph-based analyses. We explored the relationship between subcortical regions and
creative divergent thinking by using functional connectivity and graph-based analyses.
We hypothesized that the dopamine-coupled regions may show group differences, and
high creativity individuals show increased functional connectivity and network
efficiency compared to low creativity individuals.
Methods
Participants
One hundred and eighty healthy and right-handed undergraduates (90 males and 90
females, aged 18 to 22 years old) took part in the study. In this study, we used the total
scores of the figural Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (f-TTCT) test to measure the
creative thinking ability of all subjects (the sum of originality, flexibility, fluency, and
9
elaboration scores). The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking test has shown good
predictive validity (r > 0.57) and high reliability (r > 0.90) (Torrance, 1988, 1990).
Based on the f-TTCT scores, we selected 22 subjects from the top 12% of f-TTCT
scores (11 males, 18.86 ± 1.08 years old) and 22 subjects from the bottom 12% of f-
TTCT scores (11 males, 19.13 ± 0.99 years old) as the high creativity group (HCG) and
low creativity group (LCG), respectively. Next, we collected the r-fMRI data of all 44
subjects in the two groups. During fMRI scanning, we asked participants that they
should remain awake and not be asleep. Meanwhile, we also required them to try not to
think systematically. The present study was approved by the Institutional Review Board
of South China Normal University.
Resting-state fMRI data acquisition
All participants were scanned using a 3T Siemens Trio Tim MR scanner at the Brain
Imaging Center at SCNU, Guangdong, China. The r-fMRI data were collected using a
GE-EPI sequence: 32 axial slices, repetition time (TR) = 2 s, echo time (TE) = 30 ms,
slice thickness = 3.5 mm, no gap, flip angle (FA) = 90°, matrix = 64 × 64, and field of
view (FOV) = 192 mm × 192 mm. The subjects were instructed to lie down with their
eyes closed, and to remain quiet during the scans while thinking of nothing in particular.
After scanning, a total of 240 volumes were obtained from each participant.
Resting-state fMRI data preprocessing
The r-fMRI data were preprocessed using DPARSF (Yan, et al., 2009) based on
10
SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm8). First, we discarded the first
10 volumes for signal equilibrium. Second, the time delay of the intra-volume in slices,
as well as head movements resulting in geometrical displacements, were corrected
(none of the subjects were excluded based on the criterion of displacement of > 1 mm
or angular rotation of > 1° in any direction). Third, the image data were normalized to
the standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space at 3-mm isotropic resolution
with an echo-planar imaging (EPI) template. The data were then band-pass filtered
(0.01 – 0.1 Hz) to decrease the effects of low-frequency drift and high-frequency
physiological noise. We also removed the linear trend and spatially smoothed the data
with an 8 mm FWHW Gaussian kernel. Nuisance covariates, including head motion via
the Friston 24-parameter model (Friston et al., 1996; Yan et al., 2013), white matter
(WM), and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) signals, were removed using regression.
Regions of interest (ROIs)
The regions of interest (ROIs) for subcortical regions were extracted using the
Harvard-Oxford subcortical structural probabilistic atlas from Data Processing &
Analysis for Brain Imaging (DPABI, http://rfmri.org/dpabi). We extracted all ROIs
based on the 25% probability map, including the bilateral amygdala, accumbens, brain-
stem, caudate, hippocampus, pallidum, putamen, and thalamus (16 ROIs in total). We
then resampled these selected ROIs to a voxel size of 3 mm for further analyses. Figure
1 shows the different anatomical orientations of the subcortical regions in this study.
11
Functional connectivity maps
We constructed the whole-brain functional connectivity (FC) map of each subcortical
region by using a standard seed-voxel approach. In this step, each ROI was considered
as a seed region. For each participant, we extracted the averaged time series of all voxels
within a given seed region and the time series of each voxel in the whole brain. We then
calculated FC (i.e. Pearson’s correlation coefficient r) between the time series of
selected seed regions and each voxel in the whole brain. Through the above analyses,
we obtained a whole-brain FC map of a given subcortical region for each participant.
Finally, we performed the Fisher’s r-to-z transformation to convert these FC maps to z-
value maps for further statistical analyses.
Constructing subcortical network
We constructed the subcortical network using a standard seed-wise method in this
study. For each participant, we extracted averaged time series of all voxels within each
seed region and calculated Pearson’s correlation coefficient r between any two time
series of these seed regions to generate the FC. Based on the above calculations, we
generated a 16 × 16 FC matrix for each participant. By taking all seed regions as nodes,
and all FC between any two seed regions as edges, we constructed the subcortical
network for each participant in the study.
Topological properties of the subcortical network
We estimated the topological properties of the subcortical network by using the
12
GRETNA software (Wang et al. 2015) (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/gretna/). Because
of the confounding effects of noisy correlations on network analyses, we set a threshold
of a significance level for FC to reduce the effects. Specifically, we only kept those FCs
whose corresponding p-values satisfied a statistical threshold of p < 0.05 (family-wise
error, FWE-correction) compared to all others in a given FC matrix. Because the
clustering coefficient (𝐶𝑝), characteristic path length (𝐿𝑝), global efficiency (𝐸𝑔𝑙𝑜𝑏), and
local efficiency (𝐸𝑙𝑜𝑐) describe the local and global information communication of the
networks and provide the altered information transferring within the brain networks,
we hence selected these four parameters to examine the topological properties of the
networks in this study. Based on the corrected FC matrix, we finally calculated
topological properties of the subcortical network including four global parameters: 𝐶𝑝,
𝐿𝑝, 𝐸𝑔𝑙𝑜𝑏 and 𝐸𝑙𝑜𝑐 for each subject. Graph theory have been widely used to measure
the topological properties of brain networks (Bullmore and Bassett, 2009; Wang et al.,
2010), including the creativity-related networks (Gao et al., 2017; Kenett et al., 2020).
In addition, we also estimated the nodal parameters including the nodal strength (𝑆𝑖
w)
and nodal efficiency ( 𝐸𝑖
w ) of the subcortical network. Detailed definitions and
mathematical descriptions of these global parameters are listed in Table S1.
Dynamic measures of the subcortical network
We estimated dynamic FC (dFC) and dynamic topological properties as dynamic
measures of subcortical networks in this study. The sliding-window approach (Allen et
al. 2014; Gao et al., 2020; Liu et al., 2020) was applied to dynamic measures by using
13
the DynamicBC toolbox (Liao et al. 2014). Specifically, we first segmented the whole
time series of each seed region into several sliding windows. Then, the fixed length of
the sliding window was set to 22 TRs (i.e. 44 s) and the subsequent sliding-window
began with the step of 1 TR. The entire scanning lasted for about 8 min, including 240
TRs. After removing 10 TR in the preprocessing step, only 230 TRs were conserved for
this analysis. We hence obtained 209 sliding windows based on the above calculations
for each subject. Next, we extracted the time series (22 TRs, 44 s) of each seed region
in each sliding window and calculated the Pearson’s correlation coefficient r between
any two time series of seed regions to generate the dynamic FC (dFC) of the subcortical
network. We also calculated four global parameters in each sliding window, which were
considered as dynamic topological properties of subcortical networks. Finally, we
estimated both the mean (i.e. averaged) and the variance of dFC, as well as the variance
of dynamic topological properties across all sliding windows for each subject.
Brain–behavior correlation analyses
We used Spearman’s correlation analysis to examine correlations between brain
measures (i.e. FC and global parameters) and f-TTCT scores. We only considered those
FC or global parameters that showed significant between-group differences in the
correlation analyses. We also explored a partial correlation (Pearson’s correlation)
between the subcortical nodal degree and f-TTCT scores. Age and sex were controlled,
and the statistical significance was set to p < 0.05 (false discovery rate, FDR correction).
14
Statistical analyses
We used a two-sample t-test to detect between-group differences in whole FC maps
of the subcortical region. We determined that the clusters showed statistical differences
between HCG and LCG with two criteria: 1) significant threshold p < 0.05, with a strict
multiple comparison correction strategy, Threshold-Free Cluster Enhancement
correction (TFCE-correction); 2) the number of voxels in each cluster should be more
than 50 voxels.
In addition, a nonparametric permutation t-test was used to determine the between-
group differences in each FC within the subcortical network, topological properties of
the subcortical network, as well as the mean and variance of dynamic measures. In the
calculation, for a given parameter (either FC or global parameter), we randomly paired
the parameter values between HCG and LCG to generate two new groups. Subsequently,
we calculated the mean value of each new group and estimated their differences. This
permutation was repeated 5,000 times to obtain the empirical distribution of the
difference between new paired groups. We then selected a significance level at p <0.05
to determine significant differences between HCG and LCG at 95% of the empirical
distribution in a two-tailed test. Given the small sample size of the participants in our
study (22 participants in each group), when significant between-group differences were
determined in any parameter, we also calculated the corresponding effect size (Cohen’s
d) according to Cohen (2013).
Results:
15
FC maps of subcortical regions
Figure 2 and Table S2 show significant differences in the whole-brain FC maps of
the subcortical regions between the HCG and LCG. To illustrate our results
conveniently, we have used L. to denote the left hemispheric lobules and R. to denote
the right hemispheric lobules below. We found significantly (p < 0.05, TFCE-correction)
higher FC between the left thalamus (L.THA) and left insula (L.INS) in the HCG
compared to the LCG. In addition, statistical analyses showed that the right pallidum
(R.PAL) was more strongly connected to five regions, including the bilateral
parahippocampal gyrus (L/R.PHG), left fusiform gyrus (L.FFG), left middle temporal
gyrus (L.MTG), and left middle frontal gyrus (orbital part, ORBmid), in the HCG
compared to the LCG.
FC within the subcortical network
Figure 3A and Table S3 illustrate the significant between-group differences (p < 0.05,
FDR-correction) of FC within the subcortical network. We found significantly higher
FC between the putamen (PUT), PAL, and THA in the HCG than in the LCG. In
particular, we found significantly higher FC between the bilateral PUT (p = 0.0012),
between the L.PUT and R.PAL (p = 0.0006), between the L.THA and the R.PUT (p =
0.0002) and R.PAL (p = 0.0010), as well as between the R.PUT and R.PAL (p = 0.0014)
in the HCG than in the LCG.
Topological properties of the subcortical network
16
Global parameters of the subcortical network for both HCG and LCG are shown in
Figure 4A and Table S4. Statistical analysis (p < 0.05) revealed a significantly higher
clustering coefficient (p = 0.0330) and local efficiency (p = 0.0190) in the HCG
compared to the LCG. Moreover, we found a marginally significant difference in global
efficiency (p = 0.0550) between HCG and LCG. In addition, we found that the nodal
strength was significantly lower (p = 0.0008) in the L.PUT for HCG than in the LCG.
Dynamic measures of the subcortical network
Averaged dFC. Significant (p < 0.05, FDR-correction) differences in the averaged dFC
of the subcortical network between HCG and LCG are shown in Figure 3B and Table
S5. Across all sliding windows, we found significantly higher dFC between the PUT,
PAL, and THA in the HCG than in the LCG. These results were consistent with the
findings of FC within the subcortical network. We additionally found a significantly
higher dFC between the R.THA and R.PAL (p = 0.0006), as well as between the R.THA
and R.PUT (p = 0.0010).
Variance of dFC. We found significant (p < 0.05, FDR-correction) between-group
differences in the variance of dFC across all sliding windows between the HCG and
LCG (Figure 3C, Table S6). We found that HCG showed significantly decreased
variance of dFC between the left amygdala (L.AMY) and the L.PAL (p = 0.0002),
L.THA (p = 0.0012), and R.THA (p = 0.0002) than LCG. Meanwhile, significant
between-group differences (HCG < LCG) were found in the variance of dFC between
17
the left hippocampus (L.HIPP) and the L.PAL (p = 0.0020) and L.PUT (p = 0.0008).
Moreover, significantly decreased variance of dFC between the bilateral THA (p =
0.0024), between the L.PAL and R.AMY (p = 0.0024), between the R.HIPP and R.PAL
(p = 0.0008), and between the R.THA and R.AMY (p = 0.0016) were found in the HCG
compared to the LCG.
Variance of global parameters. We found significantly decreased variance of clustering
coefficient (p = 0.0180), characteristic path length (p = 0.0196), and local efficiency (p
= 0.0052) in the HCG compared to the LCG. Figure 4B and Table S7 show detailed
results of variance of global parameters of the subcortical network for both HCG and
LCG.
Brain–behavior correlation analyses
A significant (p < 0.05, FDR-correction) positive correlation was found between the
FC values between the subcortical network and the f-TTCT scores (Figure S1). We
found that FC between the bilateral PUT (r = 0.48, p = 0.0010), the L.PUT and R.PAL
(r = 0.43, p = 0.0050), the L.THA and R.PUT (r = 0.44, p = 0.004), the L.THA and
R.PAL (r = 0.42, p = 0.006), and between the R.PUT and R.PAL (r = 0.42, p = 0.006)
were all significantly positively correlated with the f-TTCT scores. We found a
significant (p < 0.05, FDR-correction) positive correlation between the nodal strength
of the L.PUT and f-TTCT scores.
Discussion
18
The present study investigated the cortico-subcortical network involved in creativity
from a novel point of view, and examined the functional brain connectivity of subcortex
and cortico-subcortical regions underlying creative thinking in terms of brain-behavior
correlations and group differences. The main results are summarized as follows. First,
static/dynamic functional connectivity between the bilateral putamen, between the right
pallidum and bilateral putamen, and between the left thalamus and the right putamen
and right pallidum indicated group differences within the subcortex. Second,
information translation efficiency was higher in the HCG than in the LCG, along with
network stabilization in the subcortical network. Third, the voxel-wise functional
connectivity results showed group differences across the subcortical (i.e. the left
thalamus and right pallidum) and cerebral regions (i.e. insula, MFG, and MTG). Fourth,
the variance of dFC indicated significant differences between HCG and LCG across the
putamen, thalamus, pallidum amygdala, and hippocampus. Fifth, the FC between the
bilateral putamen, between the right pallidum and bilateral putamen, and between the
left thalamus and the right putamen and right pallidum demonstrated a positive
correlation with f-TTCT scores. Additionally, the f-TTCT scores and nodal degree of
the subcortical regions showed a significantly positive correlation in the left putamen.
The results of graph-based network analyses showed group differences within the
subcortical regions. We explored the subcortical topological network organization using
two different creativity groups to analyse path length and network efficiency potentially
underlying group differences. Our results demonstrated that the subcortical network of
participants with higher creativity exhibited better network efficiency. That is,
19
information translation efficiency was higher in the HCG than in the LCG. In total, the
subcortical network of participants with high creativity indicated better optimized
network organization compared with participants with low creativity. Furthermore, the
indicator of subcortical network variance was smaller in the HCG, indicating that
individuals with high creativity exhibit a more stable subcortical network. This is
consistent with our expectations.
In particular, the FC of three subcortical regions, the putamen, pallidum, and
thalamus, showed group differences. Cortical signals are transmitted through the basal
ganglia-thalamocortical circuits. The elements of each circuit include the cortex,
striatum, pallidum, and thalamus (Alexander, Delong & Strick, 1986). Each circuit
receives multiple, partially overlapping cortico-striate inputs, which are progressively
integrated during their subsequent passage through the pallidum to the thalamus and
from the thalamus back to cortex (Alexander, Delong & Strick, 1986). Our FC results
indicate that participants with higher creativity exhibited better functional connectivity
strength within the information transmission circuit.
Many previous studies have linked the dopaminergic system to creative performance
(De Manzano et al., 2010; Mayseless et al., 2013; Zabelina et al., 2016). Creativity
appears to be associated with the neural activity of the striatum, which is involved in
dopaminergic modulation (Boot et al., 2017). The release of dopamine in the striatum
is thought to be beneficial for cognitive flexibility, broadening attentional focus, and
perspective switching, which are important for creativity (Boot et al., 2017).
Furthermore, the putamen, which is a part of the striatum, is found to have a high
20
density of D2 dopaminergic receptors (Willeit et al., 2016). In addition, the putamen
contributes to behavioural inhibition (Sweiizer et al., 2018). A study by Peterson and
colleagues (2002) found that behavioural inhibition is a gating process that allows
ignoring prior related information; individuals with decreased behavioural inhibition
are more likely to develop their creative potential. However, it is worth noting that
previous studies have drawn attention to fronto-striatal pathways underlying
dopaminergic modulation of creativity (Boot et al., 2017; Schuler et al., 2019), which
is an “association” loop passing through the prefrontal cortex, association areas, and
caudate nucleus (another part of the striatum) (Alexander, Delong & Strick, 1986).
Although the putamen is found to have a high density of D2 receptors and is highly
involved in creative performance, it participates in the “motor” loop, where signals pass
through the putamen, which receives inputs from sensorimotor cortex, and whose
influences were ultimately returned to premotor areas. (Alexander, Delong & Strick,
1986). In addition, we also found group differences between the insula (sensorimotor
cortex) and thalamus. The results further suggest that modulation of the “motor”
pathway may be involved in the process of creative thinking. Previous studies (de
Manzano & Ullén, 2012; Pinho, Ullén, Castelo-Branco, Fransson & de Manzano, 2015)
found the sensorimotor cortex is related to visuospatial creative activity. Meanwhile,
the insula plays an important role in planning, motor execution, and goal-directed
behavior during visual divergent thinking (de Manzano & Ullén, 2012; Huang et al.,
2013; Pinho te al., 2015). Studies revealed a tendency for greater activities in the
thalamus during creative task compared to control task (Gilbert et al., 2010; Park et al.,
21
2015). In addition, mental imagery is thought to rely on sensorimotor experiences of
motor states, which is involved in generating images of objects in their absence, via
retrieving, modifying and recombining sensory information from memory (Kosslyn et
al. 2001). And, the ability of mental imagery plays a fundamental role in visual creative
divergent thinking (LeBoutillier & Marks, 2003). Taken together, these findings
strongly suggest an important role of sensorimotor regions in visual creative divergent
thinking. We did not find group differences in FC in the caudate. This, however, does
not necessarily imply that dopaminergic modulation of the fronto-striatal pathway is
not involved in creative thinking. Although there were no differences in the caudate,
we found group differences between the pallidum and association areas (FFG and MTG)
and the PFC (i.e. ORBmid). Creative processing requires persistence, which involves
focused, convergent, and systematic thinking and longer idea searching (Baas et al.,
2013). Effortful and focused creative thinking has been implicated in the activation of
the PFC pathway (Benedek et al., 2014). Further, activation in the PFC enhances
creative insightful behaviour and increases original idea generation (Mayseless and
Shamay-Tsoory, 2015). Nonetheless, there may be more complex mechanisms
involved in the process of creative thinking, which need to be further explored.
Our results showed group differences between the respective “motor” pathways in
the HCG and the LCG, which has not been previously demonstrated. The differences
between the “motor” loop pathways may be a result of other neuromodulators, such as
noradrenaline and oxytocin. Although previous studies have focused on dopamine
(fronto-striatal pathway) as the key neurotransmitter involved in creative thinking (Boot
22
et al., 2017; Schuler et al., 2019), cognition and behaviour are modulated by other
neurotransmitters such as noradrenaline (Aston-Jonnes and Cohen, 2005; Berridge and
Waterhouse, 2003), in addition to dopamine. Noradrenaline is thought to modulate the
balance between exploitation and exploration by promoting stabilization of salient
information in working memory and in attention shifting (Aston-Jonnes and Cohen,
2005; Berridge and Waterhouse, 2003) and may enable the persistence of creative
thinking. In addition, oxytocin facilitates flexibility and divergent thinking, which are
crucial for creative thinking (De Dreu et al., 2014). Moreover, the differences may be
caused by decreased cortisol responses and fear signals in the amygdalar-hippocampal
circuit. Our results showed that the variance of dFC indicated significant differences
between the HCG and LCG across the amygdala and hippocampus. It offers the
possibility that not only a single neurotransmitter (i.e. dopamine), but also additional
neurotransmitters (such as norepinephrine and oxytocin) contribute to creative thinking.
Our findings provide some insight into the results and possibilities. First, we used
static/dynamic functional connectivity and graph theory methods to explore group
differences in brain subcortical regions. In addition to the brain regions of the fronto-
striatal pathway, we also found that other regions of the “motor” loop exhibited group
differences. These results extend existing known findings. In addition, we also found
significant differences between the HCG and LCG in the amygdalar-hippocampal
circuit. Thus, there may be other neurotransmitters that contribute to creative behaviour
in addition to dopamine.
Although the present study has some limitations and could be improved, the results
23
of our study have some theoretical and practical implications. The study provides a new
perspective on the relationship between subcortical regions and creativity. A limitation
of the study is that the analyses were based on using resting-state data. It is not clear
how subcortical regions would perform during creative thinking tasks and what the
interaction is between the cortex and subcortex during creative thinking. This will be
explored through further research.
24
References:
Alexander, G. E., DeLong, M. R., & Strick, P. L. (1986). Parallel organization of
functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annual Review
of Neuroscience, 9, 357-381.
Alexander, G. E., Crutcher, M. D. & DeLong, M. R. (1990). Basal ganglia-
thalamocortical circuits: parallel substrates for motor, oculomotor, prefrontal and
limbic functions. Progress in Brain Research, 85, 119-146.
Allen, E. A., Damaraju, E., Plis, S. M., Erhardt, E. B., Eichele, T., & Calhoun, V. D.
(2014). Tracking whole-brain connectivity dynamics in the resting state. Cerebral
Cortex, 24, 663-676.
Aston-Jones, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2005). An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-
norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annual Review
of Neuroscience, 28, 403-450.
Baas, M., Roskes, M., Sligte, D., Nijstad, B. A., & De Dreu, C. K. (2013). Personality
and creativity: The dual pathway to creativity model and a research agenda. Social
and Personality Psychology Compass, 7, 732-748.
Berridge, C. W., & Waterhouse, B. D. (2003). The locus coeruleus–noradrenergic
system: modulation of behavioral state and state-dependent cognitive processes.
Brain Research Reviews, 42, 33-84.
Benedek, M., Beaty, R., Jauk, E., Koschutnig, K., Fink, A., Silvia, P.J., Dunst, B., &
Neubauer, A. C. (2014). Creating metaphors: The neural basis of figurative
language production. Neuroimage, 90, 99-106.
25
Biswal, B., Yetkin, F. Z., Haughton, V. M., & Hyde, J. S. (1995). Functional
connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI.
Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 34, 537-541.
Boot, N., Baas, M., van Gaal, S., Cools, R., & De Dreu, C. K. (2017). Creative cognition
and dopaminergic modulation of fronto-striatal networks: Integrative review and
research agenda. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 78, 13-23.
Bullmore, E., & Sporns, O. (2009). Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis
of structural and functional systems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 186-198.
Cajal, S. R. (1995). Histology of the nervous system of man and vertebrates (Vol. 1).
Oxford University Press, USA.
Calhoun, V. D., Miller, R., Pearlson, G., & Adalı, T. (2014). The chronnectome: time-
varying connectivity networks as the next frontier in fMRI data discovery.
Neuron, 84, 262-274.
Chen, Q., Beaty, R. E., Cui, Z., Sun, J., He, H., Zhuang, K., Ren, Z., Liu, G. & Qiu, J.
(2019). Brain hemispheric involvement in visuospatial and verbal divergent
thinking. Neuroimage, 202, 116065.
Cohen, J. (2013). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences: Routledge.
De Dreu, C.K.W., Baas, M., Roskes, M., Sligte, D.J., Ebstein, R.P., Chew, S.H., Tong,
T., Jiang, Y., Mayseless, N., & Shamay-Tsoory, S.G. (2014). Oxytonergic
circuitry sustains and enables creative cognition in humans. Social Cognitive and
Affective Neuroscience, 9, 1159-1165.
De Manzano, Ö., Cervenka, S., Karabanov, A., Farde, L., Ullen, F. (2010). Thinking
26
outside a less intact box: thalamic dopamine D2 receptor densities are negatively
related to psychometric creativity in healthy individuals. PloS one, 5, e10670.
de Manzano, Ö. & Ullén, F. (2012). Goal-independent mechanisms for free response
generation: Creative and pseudo-random performance share neural substrates.
Neuroimage, 59, 772-780.
Dietrich, A., & Kanso, R. (2010). A review of EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies of
creativity and insight. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 822.
Fink, A., Benedek, M., Koschutnig, K., Papousek, I., Weiss, E.M., Bagga, D., & Schöpf,
V., (2018). Modulation of resting-state network connectivity by verbal divergent
thinking training. Brain and Cognition, 128, 1–6.
Gao, Z., Liu, X., Zhang, D., Liu, M., & Hao, N. (2020). The indispensable role of the
cerebellum in visual divergent thinking. Scientific Reports, 10, 1-12.
Gao, Z., Zhang, D., Liang, A., Liang, B., Wang, Z., Cai, Y., Li, J., Gao, M., Jiao, B.,
Huang, R. & Liu, M. (2017). Exploring the associations between intrinsic brain
connectivity and creative ability using functional connectivity strength and
connectome analysis. Brain Connectivity, 7, 590-601.
Gilbert, S. J., Zamenopoulos, T., Alexiou, K., & Johnson, J. H. (2010). Involvement of
right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in ill‐structured design cognition: An fMRI
study. Brain Research, 1312, 79–88.
Heilman, K. M., Nadeau, S. E., & Beversdorf, D. O. (2003). Creative innovation:
possible brain mechanisms. Neurocase, 9, 369-379.
Huang, P., Qiu, L., Shen, L., Zhang, Y., Song, Z., Qi, Z. Gong, Q. & Xie,
27
P. (2013). Evidence for a left‐over‐right inhibitory mechanism during figural
creative thinking in healthy nonartists. Human Brain Mapping, 34, 2724–2732.
Kenett, Y. N., Betzel, R. F., & Beaty, R. E. (2020). Community structure of the creative
brain at rest. Neuroimage, 210, 116578.
Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 635–642.
Kounios, J., Fleck, J. I., Green, D. L., Payne, L., Stevenson, J. L., Bowden, E. M., &
Jung-Beeman, M. (2008). The origins of insight in resting-state brain activity.
Neuropsychologia, 46, 281-291.
LeBoutillier, N., & Marks, D. F. (2003). Mental imagery and creativity: A meta-
analytic review study. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 29-44.
Liao, W., Wu, G., Xu, Q., Ji, G., Zhang, Z., Zang, Y., & Liu, G. (2014). DynamicBC: a
MATLAB toolbox for dynamic brain connectome analysis. Brain Connectivity, 4,
780-790.
Liu, X. , Wu, X. , Zhong, M. , Huang, H. , & Huang, R. (2020). Dynamic properties of
human default mode network in eyes-closed and eyes-open. Brain Topography, 33,
720-732.
Marusak, H. A., Calhoun, V. D., Brown, S., Crespo, L. M., Sala‐Hamrick, K., Gotlib, I.
H., & Thomason, M. E. (2017). Dynamic functional connectivity of
neurocognitive networks in children. Human Brain Mapping, 38, 97-108.
Mayseless, N., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2015). Enhancing verbal creativity:
modulating creativity by altering the balance between right and left inferior frontal
28
gyrus with tDCS. Neuroscience, 291, 167-176.
Mayseless, N., Uzefovsky, F., Shalev, I., Ebstein, R. P., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2013).
The association between creativity and 7R polymorphism in the dopamine
receptor D4 gene (DRD4). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 502.
Park, H. R. P., Kirk, I. J., & Waldie, K. E. (2015). Neural correlates of creative thinking
and schizotypy. Neuropsychologia, 73, 94–107.
Peterson, J. B., Smith, K. W., & Carson, S. (2002). Openness and extraversion are
associated with reduced latent inhibition: Replication and commentary.
Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 1137-1147.
Pinho, A. L., Ullén, F., Castelo-Branco, M., Fransson, P., & de Manzano, Ö. (2015)
Addressing a Paradox: Dual Strategies for Creative Performance in
Introspective and Extrospective Networks. Cerebral Cortex, 26, 3052-3063.
Rashid, B., Arbabshirani, M. R., Damaraju, E., Cetin, M. S., Miller, R., Pearlson, G. D.,
& Calhoun, V. D. (2016). Classification of schizophrenia and bipolar patients
using static and dynamic resting-state fMRI brain connectivity. Neuroimage, 134,
645-657.
Salvi, C., Bricolo, E., Franconeri, S.L., Kounios, J., & Beeman, M. (2015). Sudden
insight is associated with shutting out visual inputs. Psychonomic Bulletin &
Review, 22, 1814–1819.
Schuler, A. L., Tik, M., Sladky, R., Luft, C. D. B., Hoffmann, A., Woletz, M., Zioga, i.,
Bhattacharya, J., & Windischberger, C. (2019). Modulations in resting state
networks of subcortical structures linked to creativity. NeuroImage, 195, 311-319.
29
Shen, W., Tong, Y., Li, F., Yuan, Y., Hommel, B., Liu, C., & Luo, J. (2018). Tracking
the neurodynamics of insight: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Biological
Psychology, 138, 189-198.
Slepian, M. L., & Ambady, N. (2012). Fluid movement and creativity. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 625.
Sokoloff, L., Wechsler, R. L., Mangold, R., Balls, K., & Kety, S. S. (1953). Cerebral
blood flow and oxygen consumption in hyperthyroidism before and after
treatment. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 32, 202.
Strogatz, S. H. (2001). Exploring complex networks. Nature, 410, 268-276.
Sweitzer, M. M., Kollins, S. H., Kozink, R. V., Hallyburton, M., English, J., Addicott,
M. A., Oliver, J. A., & McClernon, F. J. (2018). ADHD, smoking withdrawal, and
inhibitory control: results of a neuroimaging study with methylphenidate
challenge. Neuropsychopharmacology, 43, 851.
Thelen, E., Schöner, G., Scheier, C., & Smith, L. B. (2001). The dynamics of
embodiment: A field theory of infant perseverative reaching. Behavioral and
Brain Sciences, 24, 1-34.
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Sassa, Y., Hashizume, H., Sekiguchi, A., Fukushima, A., &
Kawashima, R., (2010). Regional gray matter volume of dopaminergic system
associate with creativity: evidence from voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage,
51, 578–585.
Wang, J., Wang, X., Xia, M., Liao, X., Evans, A., & He, Y. (2015). GRETNA: a graph
theoretical network analysis toolbox for imaging connectomics. Frontiers in
30
Human Neuroscience, 9, 386.
Wei, D., Yang, J., Li, W., Wang, K., Zhang, Q., & Qiu, J. (2014). Increased resting
functional connectivity of the medial prefrontal cortex in creativity by means of
cognitive stimulation. Cortex, 51, 92-102.
Willeit, M., Popovic, A., Bartova, L., Sauerzopf, U., Bauer, M., & Praschak-Rieder, N.
(2016). In Vivo Imaging of Dopamine Metabolism and Dopamine Transporter
Function in the Human Brain. In Neurotransmitter Transporters (pp. 203-220).
Humana Press, New York, NY.
Zabelina, D. L., Colzato, L., Beeman, M., & Hommel, B. (2016). Dopamine and the
creative mind: Individual differences in creativity are predicted by interactions
between dopamine genes DAT and COMT. PloS one, 11, e0146768.
Zou, Q., Ross, T. J., Gu, H., Geng, X., Zuo, X. N., Hong, L. E., Gao, J., Stein, E. A.,
Zang, Y. & Yang, Y. (2013). Intrinsic resting‐state activity predicts working
memory brain activation and behavioral performance. Human Brain Mapping, 34,
3204-3215.
31
Figure Captions
Figure 1 Locations of subcortical regions. Each region is labeled with different colors.
Figure 2 Cluster corresponding to the significant difference (p <0.05, Threshold-Free
Cluster Enhancement correction, TFCE-correction) in the whole-brain map of
subcortical region between high creative group (HCG) and low creative group (LCG).
Abbreviation: THA, thalamus; PAL, pallidum; L (R), left (right) hemisphere.
Figure 3 Significant differences (p <0.05, false discovery rate correction, FDR-
correction) in A) function connectivity (FC) and B) averaged dynamic FC (dFC)
between high creativity group (HCG) and low creativity group (LCG).
Figure 4 A) Global parameters of subcortical network for both high creativity group
(HCG) and low creativity group (LCG). B) The variance of global parameters of
subcortical network for both HCG and LCG. *p <0.05.
Figures
Figure 1
Locations of subcortical regions. Each region is labeled with different colors.
Figure 2
Cluster corresponding to the signicant difference (p <0.05, Threshold-Free Cluster Enhancement
correction, TFCE-correction) in the whole-brain map of subcortical region between high creative group
(HCG) and low creative group (LCG). Abbreviation: THA, thalamus; PAL, pallidum; L (R), left (right)
hemisphere.
Figure 3
Signicant differences (p <0.05, false discovery rate correction, FDR-correction) in A) function
connectivity (FC) and B) averaged dynamic FC (dFC) between high creativity group (HCG) and low
creativity group (LCG).
Figure 4
A) Global parameters of subcortical network for both high creativity group (HCG) and low creativity group
(LCG). B) The variance of global parameters of subcortical network for both HCG and LCG. *p <0.05.
Supplementary Files
Figures
Figure 1
Locations of subcortical regions. Each region is labeled with different colors.
Figure 2
Cluster corresponding to the signicant difference (p <0.05, Threshold-Free Cluster Enhancement
correction, TFCE-correction) in the whole-brain map of subcortical region between high creative group
(HCG) and low creative group (LCG). Abbreviation: THA, thalamus; PAL, pallidum; L (R), left (right)
hemisphere.
Figure 3
Signicant differences (p <0.05, false discovery rate correction, FDR-correction) in A) function
connectivity (FC) and B) averaged dynamic FC (dFC) between high creativity group (HCG) and low
creativity group (LCG).
Figure 4
A) Global parameters of subcortical network for both high creativity group (HCG) and low creativity group
(LCG). B) The variance of global parameters of subcortical network for both HCG and LCG. *p <0.05.
Supplementary Files
Figures
Figure 1
Locations of subcortical regions. Each region is labeled with different colors.
Figure 2
Cluster corresponding to the signicant difference (p <0.05, Threshold-Free Cluster Enhancement
correction, TFCE-correction) in the whole-brain map of subcortical region between high creative group
(HCG) and low creative group (LCG). Abbreviation: THA, thalamus; PAL, pallidum; L (R), left (right)
hemisphere.
Figure 3
Signicant differences (p <0.05, false discovery rate correction, FDR-correction) in A) function
connectivity (FC) and B) averaged dynamic FC (dFC) between high creativity group (HCG) and low
creativity group (LCG).
Figure 4
A) Global parameters of subcortical network for both high creativity group (HCG) and low creativity group
(LCG). B) The variance of global parameters of subcortical network for both HCG and LCG. *p <0.05.
Supplementary Files