ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Design research has provided numerous theories and tools on design’s emotional factor, all derived from psychology research. Today, however, research in neuroscience, together with technological developments in new instruments, promises new potential. The amount of data that cognitive neurosciences provide can describe a user’s reaction, but this information is dense and not easily accessible or directly usable for designers. The designer cannot interpret the data, not having specific training in doing so. There is a need to support the designer in interpreting the data measured by neuroscience and converting it into design input. This article showcases a tool, a set of specifically designed cards that helps designers interpret the neurophysiological data from the human–product interaction. The goal is to interpret the user’s cognitive and emotional responses, associating the neurophysiological data variations to the characteristics of the stimulus. This results in strengthening the relationship between design and neuroscience. Some limitations emerged during the application of the cards, but the results were satisfactory for being the first step, with the potential for future experiments to refine the instrument.
Content may be subject to copyright.
DES IGNPRINC IPL ESA NDPRACTICES.COM
VOLUME 15 ISSUE 1An International Journal — Annual Review
Design Principles
and Practices
__________________________________________________________________________
A Neurodesign Case StudyMeasuring the Emotional Index for RedesignALESSIO PAOLETTI AND LORENZO IMBESI
Downloaded by Alessio Paoletti on Sun May 02 2021 at 07:02:17 AM CDT
DESIGN PRINCIPLES AN D PRACTICES:
AN INTERNATIONAL J OURNALANNUAL REVIEW
http://designprinciplesandpractices.com
ISSN: 1833-1874 (Print)
ISSN: 24735736 (Online)
https://doi.org/10.18848/1833-1874/CGP (Journal)
First published by Common Ground Research Networks in 2021
University of Illinois Research Park
60 Hazelwood Drive
Champaign, IL 61820 USA
Ph: +1-217-328-0405
http://cgnetworks.org
Design Principles and Practic es: An International Journa lAnnual Review
is a peer-reviewed, scholarly journal.
COPYRIGHT
© 2021 (individual papers), the author(s)
© 2021 (selection and editorial matter),
Common Ground Research Networks
All rights reserved. Apart from fair dealing for the purposes of study,
research, criticism, or review, as permitted under the applicable
copyright legislation, no part of this work may be reproduced by any
process without written permission from the publisher. For permissions
and other inquiries, please contact cgscholar.com/cg_support.
Common Ground Research Networks, a member of Crossref
EDITORS
Lorenzo Imbesi, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Loredana Di Lucchio, University of Rome, Italy
ACTING DIRECTOR OF PUBLISHING
Jeremy Boehme, Common Ground Research Networks, USA
ADVISORY BOARD
The Design Principles and Practices Research Network recognizes
the contribution of many in the evolution of the Research Network.
The principal role of the Advisory Board has been, and is, to drive
the overall intellectual direction of the Research Network.
A full list of members can be found at
https://designprinciplesandpractices.com/about/advisory-board.
PEER REVIEW
Articles published in Design Principles and Practi ces: An International
JournalAnnual Review are peer reviewed using a two-way anonymous
peer review model. Reviewers are active participants of the Design
Principles and Practices Research Network or a thematically related
Research Network. The publisher, editors, reviewers, and authors all
agree upon the following standards of expected ethical behavior, which
are based on the Committee on Publication Ethics (COPE) Core
Practices. More information can be found at:
https://designprinciplesandpractices.com/journals/model.
ARTICLE SUBMISSION
Design Principles and Practic es: An International Journa lAnnual Review
publishes annually.
To find out more about the submission process, please visit
https://designprinciplesandpractices.com/journals/call-for-papers.
ABSTRACTING AND INDEXING
For a full list of databases in which this journal is indexed, please visit
https://designprinciplesandpractices.com/journals/collection.
RESEARCH NETWORK MEMBERSHIP
Authors in Design Principles and Practices: An International Journa l
Annual Review are members of the Design Principles and Practices
Journal Collection or a thematically related Research Network.
Members receive access to journal content. To find out more, visit
https://designprinciplesandpractices.com/about/become-a-memb er.
SUBSCRIPTIONS
Design Principles and Practic es: An International Journa lAnnual Review
is available in electronic and print formats. Subscribe to gain access to
content from the current year and the entire backlist.
Contact us at cgscholar.com/cg_support.
ORDERING
Single articles and issues are available from the journal bookstore at
https://cgscholar.com/bookstore.
HYBRID OPEN ACCESS
Design Principles and Practic es: An International JournalAnnual Revie w
is Hybrid Open Access, meaning authors can choose to make their
articles open access. This allows their work to reach an even wider
audience, broadening the dissemination of their research.
To find out more, please visit
https://designprinciplesandpractices.com/journals/hybrid-open-access.
DISCLAIMER
The authors, editors, and publish er will not accept any legal respon sibility for
any errors or omission s that may have been made in this public ation. The
publisher makes no war ranty, express or implied, with respe ct to the material
contained herein.
Downloaded by Alessio Paoletti on Sun May 02 2021 at 07:02:17 AM CDT
Design Principles and Practices: An International JournalAnnual Review
Volume 15, Issue 1, 2021, https://designprinciplesandpractices.com
© Common Ground Research Networks, Alessio Paoletti, Lorenzo Imbesi,
All Rights Reserved. Permissions: cgscholar.com/cg_support
ISSN: 1833-1874 (Print), ISSN: 2473-5736 (Online)
https://doi.org/10.18848/1833-1874/CGP/v15i01/33-44 (Article)
A Neurodesign Case Study: Measuring the
Emotional Index for Redesign
Alessio Paoletti,1 Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Lorenzo Imbesi, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Abstract: Cognitive neuroscience can measure a users neurophysiological activities during a product experience with
portable and noninvasive tools. The data collected is summarized in three indices, which express the users affective
and cognitive states during the performance of a specific action within an authentic context. This data can represent
additional helpful information for product development. However, it is first necessary to find a link between
neuroscience and design to help designers benefit from this new data. One of the indices measured by neuroscience is
the so-called emotional index, a synthesis of valence and arousal. Principles of interpretation of the emotional
index from the designer perspective are proposed to guide the designer in improving an existing product and designing
a new one from scratch. The results show how it is possible to identify the design aspects responsible for emotional
index variations. The research limitations lie in the need for further and subsequent neurotests to build a reference case
history.
Keywords: Neurodesign, Neuroscience, User-Centered Design
Introduction
esign as a discipline has always been characterized by a dynamic identity capable of
absorbing new stimuli from reality and returning design solutions that are functional,
semantic, and morphological. The act of designing has never been a gesture resulting
from a sacred fire that is the prerogative of a select few but the personal synthesis of a long and
careful research path. To manage the growing complexity in the field, research uses tools and
methods also designed by other knowledge fields. It is worth exploring the boundaries between
design and other disciplines, both when there are apparent overlapping areas and when common
areas are seen only in future perspectives, as is the case with cognitive neuroscience.
Cognitive neuroscience refers to the analysis of human responses to the external
environment, as has never been achieved before. Let alone from a future perspective, this path
can already immediately interest the designer, especially considering the persons key role in
human-centered design. Aspects such as workload memory have been known and measured for
some time; these aspects are well-known among those who evaluate a users response to a
complex stimulus such as a control panel. But neuroscience research is now also able to
describe a users emotional reaction. Ample literature attests to the relevance of the emotional
aspects of the userproduct relationship. So, we pose the question of whether it is possible to
create a bridge between neuroscience and design, with the common aim of knowing the user
who is placed at the center of the design process. In this article, we want to report the
comparison between the tools and methods of neuroscience and a review of the literature in
emotion design and highlight that these two fields of knowledge can have common ground.
Emotional Index: Linking between Neuroscience and Design
To identify a link between neuroscience and design, the first step can be neuromarketing, which
currently has more conspicuous scientific literature than that available for neurodesign. This is
1 Corresponding Author: Alessio Paoletti, PDTA Department, Faculty of Architecture, Sapienza University of Rome,
Rome, Italy. email: alessio.paoletti@uniroma1.it
D
Downloaded by Alessio Paoletti on Sun May 02 2021 at 07:02:17 AM CDT
DESIGN PRINCIPLES AND PRACTICES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNALANNUAL REVIEW
the first step to describe how neuroscience and design can perform research together around the
problem of user experience. This study’s focus is not the relationship between neuroscience and
marketing, but to point out that it is a starting point. Thanks to the portability of the tools
necessary to measure a users neurophysiological responses, it is now possible to simulate the
users actions in a real scenario. For instance, it is possible to evaluate the users shopping
experience at the point of sale by applying the tools to the users body without limiting the
actions; the devices dont significantly interfere with the quality of the experience.
BrainSign Srl (2018), a spin-off of Sapienza University of Rome, demonstrates excellence
in this field. As shown in Figure 1, BrainSign can conduct neuromarketing evaluations by
measuringwith eye-trackers and other devices described in detail laterwhat attracts the
users attention.
Figure 1: The User Operates in the Real Context of Use, While Portable Devices Detect Changes in Neurophysiological
Data. The Tools, Light and Portable, Do Not Affect the User Experience
Source: BrainSigns
BrainSign can measure neurophysiological reactions with a minimal time lag to when the
user manifests them. The neurocognitive responses are then synthesized into three indices,
namely mental effort, emotional index, and interest. Each index is the result of a series of
specific and independent measurements. With these three indices, a complete picture of the
users response is outlined. As mentioned, these tools are now portable and not invasive,
allowing the user to move freely. The user acts in the natural context, thus negating the need to
recreate a laboratory scenario. The goal of this research study is to focus on the emotional index
(EI). This index investigates the user response in terms of the emotional aspects; unlike the ME
index, EI doesnt consider brain processing but relates to peripheral parameters. When the
subject reacts to the external environment, optimizing time and results, the somatic marker
defined by Damasio (1994) brings past experiences in the decision-making process. The
somatic marker helps to understand how the cognitive and affective responses work together
rather than sequentially. The emotional response can be represented through a few fundamental
dimensions, among which the most commonly accepted are valence, arousal, and approach-
withdrawal.
The valence dimension contrasts states of pleasure (e.g., happy) with states of
displeasure (e.g., sad), and the arousal dimension contrasts states of low arousal (e.g.,
quiet) with states of high arousal (e.g., surprised). Approach motivation is
characterized by tendencies to approach stimuli (e.g., as would likely be facilitated by
34
Downloaded by Alessio Paoletti on Sun May 02 2021 at 07:02:17 AM CDT
FIRST AUTHOR LAST NAME: ARTICLE TITLE
excitement), whereas avoidance motivation is characterized by tendencies of avoidance
(e.g., as would likely be facilitated by anxiety). (Vecchiato et al. 2014, 857)
Of these four dimensions, two are of interest and constitute the emotional index: valence and
arousal. Valence and arousal variations can depict the users emotional states towards a
stimulus. Two neurophysiological variables have been used in scientific literature to measure
valence and arousal: the sweat glands activity on the hands, namely the galvanic skin response
(GSR) and the heart rate (HR). The GSR traces changes in arousal, and the HR represents the
valence (Cacioppo et al. 2000; Baumgartner, Esslen, and Jäncke 2006).
By reviewing the scientific literature on emotional design, we have identified some
contributions that allow us to create a bridge between neuroscience and design, associating the
aspects of the product that are most likely the triggers of those values. As a starting point, we
considered Normans (2004) research on the three design levels: reflective, behavioral, and
visceral. We then re-read this research in light of the construct of aesthetic pleasantness in
design defined by Hekkert (2006) and considering the research on the concerns of pleasantness
appraisal developed within the research of Desmet (2010). In light of the literature review, and
through a purely theoretical study, we hypothesize that the three constructs of mental effort
(ME), emotional index (EI), and interest (INT) may correspond to the three constructs of
Norman (2004) with an acceptable degree of approximation. Finally, we hypothesize that they
may correspond to three aspects of the product: functional, morphological, and semantic. This
article’s focus is on the emotional index; we show in Figure 2 the hypotheses of correspondence
with aspects of the product. Based on the latter, we propose the following hypothesis: the
emotional index expresses Normans behavioral level and is associated with the morphological
sphere.
Figure 2: The Hypotheses of Correspondence with Aspects of the Product
Source: Paoletti
Theories for the Interpretation of the EI
Following the literature review concerning emotional design, we therefore proposed the EI
index as representative of the level of processing that Norman has defined as behavioral. This
level is subconscious and is the place where basic emotions take place. It refers to behaviors for
which users do not need cognitive processes to structure action by action; they are linked to an
action that is well known because users have learned it in the past.
At this level, the mind has a secondary role, and it is connected only to the creation of the
desire to act but not to what action needs to be performed. According to Norman, at this level,
the interaction with the product will lead to positive emotions when the users expectations are
confirmed and vice versa, to negative emotions when the expectations are betrayed. Normans
behavioral level is the design level at which users evaluate what happened, comparing it to what
they wanted to happen. After hypothesizing a correspondence between the neurophysiological
35
Downloaded by Alessio Paoletti on Sun May 02 2021 at 07:02:17 AM CDT
DESIGN PRINCIPLES AND PRACTICES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNALANNUAL REVIEW
index EI and the Norman behavioral level, we consider the behavioral level as the most relevant
during the so-defined non-instrumental interaction phases.
The non-instrumental interaction is mainly based on the morphological aspects of the
product. For non-instrumental interaction, we refer to the research of Desmet and Hekkert
(2007). Their study describes the product experience as composed of three types of interaction:
instrumental interaction, non-instrumental interaction, and nonphysical interaction. Non-
instrumental interaction usually precedes instrumental interaction. We look at an object, we
anticipate its use, and then we use it. In these non-instrumental interactions, all the senses are
involved: the user looks at the product (sight); if the smell is present, receives it (smell);
appreciates the tactile qualities (touch), and receives sounds (hearing).
We propose some design elements that the designer should consider when designing for
non-instrumental interaction. We name these aspects, relating to the morphological sphere, M1,
M2, M3, and M4, respectively. In light of the above, the four factors can be used in the design
process focused on the morphological sphere and during the inverse process of interpreting the
neurophysiological data relating to EI. Given EIs nature, to define the four aspects relevant to
the morphological sphere (M1, M2, M3, and M4), we rely on design research that has explored
the principle of pleasantness.
The principle of pleasantness, and pleasure for the senses in research on design, has been
studied in the broader process of userproduct interaction in the niche of aesthetic interaction.
Extensive research on the construct of aesthetic pleasure in design was conducted by Hekkert
(2006). We, therefore, refer extensively to this research, adopting it as a starting point for
interpreting EI data from the designers perspective. Among other aspects of userproduct
interaction, aesthetic interaction can be considered sense gratification (Goldman 2001). Design
literature has explored why certain stimuli can gratify the senses from the perspective of
evolutionary Psychology (Ramachandran and Hirstein 1999). According to evolutionary
Psychology, aesthetic preferences go to those models, and those environmental characteristics,
that are considered beneficial to developing the senses functioning and, therefore, survival. The
research already cited by Hekkert (2006) has explored four principles that are transversal to all
senses: maximum effect for minimum means; unity in variety; most advanced, yet acceptable;
and optimal match. As shown below, we will narrow the field of research on the sense of sight,
and we will read these four principles from the Gestalt point of view.
Although the research has explored the cross-cutting principles of aesthetic responses, it does
not mean that all individuals will have the same aesthetic response; this is also influenced by the
interaction between the person and his environment (Berkowitz and Semin 2004). Therefore, the
four principles can be considered the primary references from which the respective cultural and
individual variants are derived. For this reason, different people, but influenced by similar
previous experiences or experiences, will relate to the four principles similarly and will therefore
have similar aesthetic responses: “[…]beauty is maximal when a maximum of effect is attained
with a minimum of means applied (Boselie and Leeuwenberg 1985, 1).
When users rate a stimulus as pleasant, they consider it positively for their adaptive
functions, for all senses. Thats how we consider a good smell or taste pleasantthey are signs
of their potential benefits for our survival. these senses are considered the gatekeepers of the
body, identifying what is nutritious and should be consumed and detecting what is bad and must
be rejected (Hekkert 2006, 163). The principles based on which we consider a pleasant
stimulus, in many cases, are specific to the system (the visual system, the olfactory system, etc.)
and can also be specific to the category of stimuli (the aesthetics of the landscape, the aesthetics
of music, etc.) (Hekkert 2006). The merit of Hekkerts research is to have defined the four
principles which are transversal to all senses. We have narrowed the research field in this
contribution, focusing exclusively on the sensory channel of sight; therefore, considering only
the visual stimuli for their interpretation, we refer to Gestalt. Summarizing, for the
interpretation of EIs values, our path took the first steps from Hekkerts (2006) research on the
36
Downloaded by Alessio Paoletti on Sun May 02 2021 at 07:02:17 AM CDT
FIRST AUTHOR LAST NAME: ARTICLE TITLE
construct of aesthetic pleasure in design; afterward it focused on visual stimuli, and finally re-
read Hekkerts research from the point of view of Gestalt. This choice to restrict the field of
study to the sense of sight is motivated by the commonly accepted assertion that, among all
other systems, the visual is the main sensory system. For perception, it represents the channel
that transmits most of the information about the external environment regarding obstacles,
passages, distances, actions, and identifying patterns. In this field, Gestalt psychology has
defined the principles that help convey the flow of information. According to Bregman, the
principles or the Gestalt can be seen as our best guesses to order the stream of auditory
information (Bregman 1990). Based on the literature cited, the four aspects relevant to the
morphological sphere are M1 metaphors, M2 relationships, M3 typicality and novelty, and M4
consistency of messages, as shown in Figure 3.
Figure 3: The Picture Shows the Four Aspects Relevant to the Morphological Sphere, Which We Consider Decisive during
Non-instrumental Interaction. They Can Be Used Both in Terms of Design and in Terms of Interpretation of the EI
Source: Paoletti
M1 Metaphors
Based on the assumption that the sensory system prefers a perceivable stimulus in the most
economical way possible, we try to transpose this principle on a design stimulus. Considering a
stimulus that transmits most of the information using few resources, we can imagine that this
leads to greater aesthetic pleasure. Specifically, Gestalt psychology has defined disjunctive
ambiguity as unfavorable to beauty and, therefore, aesthetic experience; conversely, conjunctive
ambiguity is favorable to beauty and aesthetic experience. An example of disjunctive ambiguity
is the rabbit-duck illusion, shown in Figure 4.
Figure 4: The Illusion of the Rabbit and the Duck, Designed in 1892 by the American Psychologist Joseph Jastrow, Is a
Clear Example of Disjunctive Ambiguity That Increases the Effort, Resources, and Capacity of the Brain Users Must
Spend on Its Interpretation. The Disjunctive Ambiguity Negatively Affects the Aesthetic Experience
Source: Paoletti
The observer has to spend more resources on perception and brain capacity to counteract
the ambiguity. On the contrary, an example of conjunctive ambiguity is the study of polygons
conducted by Boselie and Leeuwenberg (1985). They highlighted the relationship between
37
Downloaded by Alessio Paoletti on Sun May 02 2021 at 07:02:17 AM CDT
DESIGN PRINCIPLES AND PRACTICES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNALANNUAL REVIEW
beauty and complexity, showing preferences for specific proportions of simple figures over
others. It therefore appears evident that if the stimulus conveys metaphors that the users
correctly decipher, these positively affect the aesthetic experience because the metaphors are
recognized as cheap and efficient in bringing a meaning (Ramachandran and Hirstein 1999).
The power of metaphor lies in its ability to relate two distinct entities, which in turn initiates
the production of new and deeper meanings (Cila, Hekkert, and Visch 2014, 1). An example of
metaphors in design is shown in Figure 5.
Figure 5: Left Image: The Philips Senseo® Coffee Machine Uses the Embodied Metaphor of a Hunched Servant
Serving Coffee. Right Image: Metaphors Economically and Efficiently Convey a Wealth of Meaning. The Metaphor of
a Secure Memory Stick Generated with a Padlock Metaphor Gives a Deeper and More Direct Meaning
Source: Left Image Philips; Right Image Downloaded from http://www.keyables.com/2013/01/protect-pendrive-data-
with-truecrypt-password.html
According to Cila, Hekkert, and Visch (2014), the constituent elements of a product
metaphor are salience and relationship. A designer who wants to generate a product metaphor
needs to identify the sources property that conveys the desired meaning, verify that it has a
high salience, and, finally, verify that it is related to the target product. The first principle can be
considered all-encompassing for all the following principles. As seen, the first principle from
sensory stimulation was expressed by the sense of sight, but it can be adopted for all other
sensory domains (Figure 6).
Figure 6: Example of Metaphor in Product Design: a Usb Pen That Communicates Security. Designers Must Identify
the Source That Conveys the Meaning They Intend to Assign to the Product, Selecting It Based on Its Salience and
Relationship with the Final Product to Generate a Product Metaphor
Source: Cila, Hekkert, and Visch 2014
According to the metaphors principle, designers who want to interpret a negative/positive
value of EI should ask themselves:
38
Downloaded by Alessio Paoletti on Sun May 02 2021 at 07:02:17 AM CDT
FIRST AUTHOR LAST NAME: ARTICLE TITLE
a. Is the stimulus ambiguous and therefore not perceived in the cheapest way possible? The
principles of Gestalt psychology are the references with which to compare the stimulus.
b. Are there clear metaphors that economically and efficiently convey a wealth of meaning?
Participants in neurophysiological trials should be interviewed via self-reporting tools, such as
semi-structured interviews, to highlight the metaphors they have found. The survey results
should be the references for interpreting EI measures.
A research tool to collect textual data from users, also used in the design field, is the semi-
structured interview. That would allow collecting textual data that can then be compared with
the neurophysiological data measured. Galletta (2013) discussed the qualitative research method
and its critical steps in a practical manual.
M2 Relationships
Recognizing the relationships between objects has the advantage of reducing perceptual efforts.
To optimize the brains efforts, resources, and abilities, sensory systems have developed the so-
called individuation of order in chaos, or unity in variety, acquiring the tendency to group and
recognize relationships. This principle has been explored by Gestalt psychology and
neuroscientists Ramachandran and Hirstein (1999). According to these studies, the relevant
laws for identifying relationships are symmetry, good continuation, closure, peak shift,
isolation, contrast, and solving puzzles (Ramachandran and Hirstein 1999). While Gestalt
psychology has defined how individuals prefer simplicity, Ramachandran and Hirstein also
demonstrated how resource-demanding stimuli could lead to pleasurable effects. An example is
shown in Figure 7.
Figure 7: Left: Peak Shift Effect, According to Which Super-Normal Stimuli Are More Exciting for Brain Areas Than
Natural Ones, Leading to the Judgment of Pleasantness, Also According to the Principle of Solving Puzzles. Right:
Contrast Effect, in Which Luminance Affects the Perception of the Boundary between Vertical and Horizontal Lines
Source: Ramachandran and Hirstein 1999
Describing the dashed image of the Dalmatian in Figure 7, relevant for the peak shift effect,
Ramachandran and Hirstein observe:
Initially seen as a jumble of splotches, once the Dalmatian is seen, its spots are
groupeda pleasing effect, caused perhaps by activation of the limbic system by
temporal lobe cortex […] Hence a puzzle picture (or one in which meaning is implied
rather than explicit) may paradoxically be more alluring than one in which the message
is obvious. (Ramachandran and Hirstein 1999, 21)
39
Downloaded by Alessio Paoletti on Sun May 02 2021 at 07:02:17 AM CDT
DESIGN PRINCIPLES AND PRACTICES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNALANNUAL REVIEW
The contrast effect also helps the individual to find relationships between objects, which, for
example, would not have a belonging relationship but are perceived as linked by having
similarities of positioning. Most research has focused on the visual perceptual system. Similar
laws have been found in auditory information, as music is also often based on similar principles
of repetition, closure, and similarity.
Following the principle of the relationship, designers who want to interpret a
negative/positive value of EI should ask themselves:
a. Does the stimulus have an information structure that is too complex?
b. Is the structure consistent with the necessary information? The principles of Gestalt
psychology are the references with which to compare the stimulus.
c. Is the stimulus monotonous, too obvious, unsatisfactory for the peak shift effect and the need
for puzzle-solving? The principles defined in Ramachandran and Hirsteins research are the
references for interpreting the data.
M3 Typicality and Novelty
Designer Raymond Loewy defined the MAYA principle (most advanced yet acceptable)
according to which the stimulus that optimally combines typicality and novelty leads to the
maximum pleasure of the senses. That happens because, for adaptive reasons, the individual
prefers family stimuli that lead to safe choices but, at the same time, and as has been observed
in children, the human being needs novelty to facilitate learning and to absorb the effects of
boredom and saturation (Martindale 1990).
The relationships between typicality and novelty in product design are relevant to aesthetic
preference but are mutually exclusive (Hekkert, Snelders, and Van Wieringen 2003). Research
has shown that the choices are for those products with a balanced combination of typicality and
novelty. To sum up, we prefer new designs as long as the novelty character does not negate
typicality. This principle is also valid for the other perceptual systems; for example, even in
music, the role of familiarity (Gaver and Mandler 1987) and typicality (Repp 1997) has been
highlighted. An example of a combination of familiarity and novelty in music is the remix of
old songs.
Based on the typicality and novelty principle, designers who want to interpret a
negative/positive value of EI should ask themselves:
a. What is the degree of typicality and novelty in the stimulus?
b. To what extent is the stimulus recognized as a relevant example of the category in
question?
Participants in neurophysiological trials should be interviewed using self-reporting tools
such as semi-structured interviews to highlight the typicality/novelty. The surveys results
should be the references for interpreting the data.
M4 Consistency of Messages
A design product can be a complex stimulus that involves the senses in a multimodal way in the
process of perception. To increase ease of identification, essential for survival, products that
transmit the same message through different sensory channels, one consistent with the other, are
more easily identifiable (Zellner, Bartoli, and Eckard 1991). Therefore, they are perceived as
more pleasant during the aesthetic experience. Furthermore, the consistency as an aesthetic
pleasure trigger is appreciated when present between the different sensory channels and when it
is present between the message sent by the sensory channels and the given product. [...] the
famous dictum form follows function [...] could now easily be transformed to the other senses
40
Downloaded by Alessio Paoletti on Sun May 02 2021 at 07:02:17 AM CDT
FIRST AUTHOR LAST NAME: ARTICLE TITLE
in sound/touch/smell follows function (Hekkert 2006). By the term function, the researchers
understood not only utilitarian functions but also all the other functions that a product can
satisfy to enrich, inspire, strengthen the users identity, etc. A good design makes all sensory
messages harmonious and congruent with the products primary function. Therefore, it is
essential to identify the function of the product considered to be the main one. The message sent
to the sensory system should then be consistent and, as a consequence, generate a pleasant
aesthetic experience. But the primary function of the product may vary according to the
interaction phase considered. Following the consistency of messages principle, designers who
want to interpret a negative/positive value of EI should ask themselves:
a. When the EI is measured, what is the significant function that the user considers the
relevant message?
b. Which messages do the sensory systems transmit?
c. Are these messages consistent with each other?
Case StudyEI Interpretation
In the following case study, we show how to interpret EIs neurophysiological data from the
design perspective. Neurophysiological measurements have been performed in the labs of
BrainSigns, the spin-off from Sapienza University. The stimuli selected for the experiments
were packaging from the food and beverage industry. We also used flexible packaging since
this will be one of the most relevant packaging industry innovations in the coming years
(TechNavio 2016). We used as a stimulus the pouch shown in Figure 8. The stimuli were
analyzed, proposing to the users three similar pouches: the products 1A, 1B, and 1C shown in
the picture as illustrations.
Figure 8: The Stimuli Used in the Case Study Were Packaging from the Food and Beverage Industry, Namely Pouches.
The Photographic Image on the Left Shows a Generic Container Used as a Stimulus. For Reasons of Confidentiality, the
Actual Photos of the Products Used as Stimuli Are Not Shown. There Were Three Products, Similar to the One in the
Picture But Different from Each Other in the Materials Opacity-Transparency and Consistency. The Three Stimuli Are
Shown as Illustrations 1A, 1B, and 1C
Source: Paoletti
We cant show real pictures of the products for confidentiality reasons of BrainSigns and
its clients. The three pouches dont have significant differences in dimensions, weight, and
geometry. They differ in the nature of their contents in terms of density (but have a similar
smell) and have different materials that affect the packages transparency. Therefore, the
illustrations in Figure 8 show the inner content (the blue drop) with a solid line in stimulus 1A
and a dotted line for stimulus 1B to indicate whether the internal content is visible or not. In
stimulus 1C, the content was not liquid and was not visible. Therefore, the illustration depicts
drops and small cubes with dotted lines. Even if the products were very similar, they elicited
41
Downloaded by Alessio Paoletti on Sun May 02 2021 at 07:02:17 AM CDT
DESIGN PRINCIPLES AND PRACTICES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNALANNUAL REVIEW
very different EI values during the instrumental phases, precisely when they needed to tear the
label off and transfer the inner content to a given container.
The stimuli 1A and 1B elicited similar negative EI values during the first instrumental
interaction of tearing the label off, as shown in Figure 9. On the contrary, they produced
different EI values during the second instrumental interaction, where the users had to transfer
the inner content. By following the interpretation principles defined before and described in the
previous paragraph, we found principle M4consistency of messagesto be the most suitable
for data interpretation. It highlighted a lack of consistency of the messages, specifically, the
message conveyed by the sensory systems of sight and touch. Such a lack of consistency is the
most probable reason for the variation of EI values.
Figure 9: Stimuli 1A and 1B Elicited Similar Negative EI Values during the First Instrumental Interaction of Tearing
the Label Off. By Following the Interpretation Principles Defined Before and Described in the Previous Section, We
Found Principle M4Consistency of Messagesto Be the Most Suitable for Data Interpretation. It Highlighted a Lack
of Consistency of the Messages, Specifically, the Message Conveyed by the Sensory Systems of Sight and Touch
Source: Paoletti
Stimuli 1B and 1C are similar in size, weight, geometry, and package opacity. They differ
in the nature of the internal contents: liquid in stimulus 1B and semi-liquid in stimulus 1C.
Packaging 1C sends more feedback to the user about the location of the content. The content is
partially solid, and the users haptic system receives this information, which is not present in
1B. This lack of information in product 1B is detected with negative EI values, which are
negative affective states. By following the principle M4consistency of messageswe
interpreted this difference in the neurophysiological data, attributing it to a specific products
characteristic. In this way, we highlighted the packagings area of improvement from the
designers perspective.
Conclusion
In this article, we have proposed a correspondence between one of the neurophysiological
indices measured by neuroscience and one of the processing levels defined by Norman (2004)
in the context of emotional design. We proposed that through the emotional index measured on
the user, one can measure Normans behavioral level. Furthermore, that level is the most
relevant in the type of interaction defined as non-instrumental by Desmet and Hekkert (2007)
research. Combining research on emotional design, we have interpreted the data in a design key,
identifying the aspects of the product that can be improved, increasing the quality of the user
experience in terms of EI and behavioral level. Finally, we demonstrated a real case study.
In the case study, the experiments goal was to test the interpretation principles showed in
the previous paragraphs. However, this opens up future paths of research such as verifying the
specific trends. The results obtained from applying the interpretation principles should be
42
Downloaded by Alessio Paoletti on Sun May 02 2021 at 07:02:17 AM CDT
FIRST AUTHOR LAST NAME: ARTICLE TITLE
revalidated with new neurotests in the future. The product modified by following the
interpretation should be given as stimulus again to compare the product ante and the product
post. After the validation of the principles, it should also be applied in critical contexts in which
the quality of the userproduct relationship can have more significant consequences, such as in
the biomedical area. In conclusion, the interpretations principles help us think about which
aspect of the product may be considered for improvement; still, choosing where to operate and
how to redesign the stimulus remains at the designers discretion.
REFERENCES
Baumgartner, Thomas, Michaela Esslen, and Lutz Jäncke. 2006. From Emotion Perception to
Emotion Experience: Emotions Evoked by Pictures and Classical Music.
International Journal of Psychophysiology 60 (1): 3443.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.04.007.
Berkowitz, Leonard, and G. R. Semin. 2004. Socially Situated Cognition: Cognition in Its
Social Context. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 36: 53117.
https://research.vu.nl/en/publications/socially-situated-cognition-cognition-in-its-
social-context.
Boselie, F., and E. Leeuwenberg. 1985. Birkhoff Revisited: Beauty as a Function of Effect and
Means. The American Journal of Psychology 98 (1): 139.
https://doi.org/10.2307/1422765.
BrainSigns Srl. 2018. The Future Is Already Here, with BrainSigns. BrainSigns.
www.brainsigns.com.
Bregman, Albert S. 1990. Auditory Scene Analysis: The Perceptual Organization of Sound.
London: MIT Press.
Cacioppo, John T., Gary G. Berntson, Jeff T. Larsen, Kirsten M. Poehlmann, and Tiffany A.
Ito. 2000. The Psychophysiology of Emotion. In The Handbook of Emotion, edited
by Michael Lewis and Jeannette M. Haviland-Jones, 173191. New York: Guildford
Press.
Cila, Nazli, Paul Hekkert, and Valentijn Visch. 2014. Source Selection in Product Metaphor
Generation: The Effects of Salience and Relatedness. International Journal of Design
8 (1): 1528. http://www.ijdesign.org/index.php/IJDesign/article/view/1382/613.
Damasio, Antonio R. 1994. “Descartes’ Error and the Future of Human Life. Scientific
American 271 (4): 144. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican1094-144.
Desmet, Pieter. 2010. Three Levels of Product Emotion. Paper presented at the International
Conference on Kansei Engineering and Emotion Research (KEER2010), 18. Paris,
March 24, 2010. http://studiolab.ide.tudelft.nl/diopd/wp-
content/uploads/2012/02/three-levels-of-emotion.pdf.
Desmet, Pieter, and Paul Hekkert. 2007. Framework of Product Experience. International
Journal of Design 1 (1): 5766.
Galletta, Anne. 2013. Mastering the Semi-Structured Interview and Beyond: From Research
Design to Analysis and Publication. New York: New York University Press.
Gaver, William W., and George Mandler. 1987. Play It Again, Sam: On Liking Music.
Cognition and Emotion 1 (3): 25982. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699938708408051.
Goldman, A. 2001. The Aesthetic. In The Routledge Companion to Aesthetics, edited by
Berys Gaut and Dominic Mclver Lopes, 18192. London: Routledge.
Hekkert, Paul. 2006. Design Aesthetics: Principles of Pleasure in Design. Psychology Science
48 (2): 157172.
Hekkert, Paul, Dirk Snelders, and Piet C. W. Van Wieringen. 2003. ‘Most Advanced, yet
Acceptable’: Typicality and Novelty as Joint Predictors of Aesthetic Preference in
43
Downloaded by Alessio Paoletti on Sun May 02 2021 at 07:02:17 AM CDT
DESIGN PRINCIPLES AND PRACTICES: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNALANNUAL REVIEW
Industrial Design. British Journal of Psychology 94 (1): 11124.
https://doi.org/10.1348/000712603762842147.
Martindale, C. 1990. The Clockwork Muse: The Predictability of Artistic Change. New York:
Basic Books.
Norman, Donald A. 2004. Emotional DesignWhy We Love (or Hate) Everyday Things. New
York: Basic Books.
Ramachandran, Vilayanur S., and William Hirstein. 1999. The Science of Art: A Neurological
Theory of Aesthetic Experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6 (6/7): 1551.
Repp, Bruno H. 1997. The Aesthetic Quality of a Quantitatively Average Music Performance:
Two Preliminary Experiments. Music Perception 14 (4): 41944.
https://doi.org/10.2307/40285732.
TechNavio. 2016. Global Spout Pouch Market 20162020. London, UK.
Vecchiato, Giovanni, Patrizia Cherubino, Anton Giulio Maglione, Maria Trinidad Herrera
Ezquierro, Franco Marinozzi, Fabiano Bini, Arianna Trettel, and Fabio Babiloni. 2014.
How to Measure Cerebral Correlates of Emotions in Marketing Relevant Tasks.
Cognitive Computation 6 (4): 85671. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12559-014-9304-x.
Zellner, D. A., A. M. Bartoli, and R. Eckard. 1991. Influence of Color on Odor Identification
and Liking Ratings. The American Journal of Psychology 104 (4): 54761.
https://doi.org/10.2307/1422940.
ABOUT THE AUTHORS
Alessio Paoletti: Adjunct Professor, PDTA Department, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome,
Italy
Lorenzo Imbesi: Full Professor, PDTA Department, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
44
Downloaded by Alessio Paoletti on Sun May 02 2021 at 07:02:17 AM CDT
ISSN 1833-1874
Design Principles and Practices: An International Journal—Annual Review
explores the meaning and purpose of "design," as well as speaking in grounded ways about the task of design and the use of designed artifacts. The resulting conversations weave between the theoretical and the empirical, research and application, market pragmatics and social idealism.In professional and disciplinary terms, the journal traverses a broad sweep to construct a transdisciplinary dialogue which encompasses the perspectives and practices of: anthropology, architecture, art, artificial intelligence, business, cognitive science, communication studies, computer science, cultural studies, design studies, education, e-learning, engineering, ergonomics, fashion, graphic design, history, information systems, industrial design, industrial engineering, instructional design, interior design, interaction design, interface design, journalism, landscape architecture, law, linguistics and semiotics, management, media and entertainment, psychology, sociology, software engineering, technical communication, telecommunications, urban planning, and visual design.
Design Principles and Practices: An International Journal—Annual Review
, consists of articles considered to be of wide interest across the field. Five thematically focused journals also serve this Research Network:
The International Journal of Design Education• The International Journal of Design in SocietyThe International Journal of Design Management and Professional Practice• The International Journal of Designed ObjectsThe International Journal of Visual DesignDesign Principles and Practices: An International Journal—Annual Review
, is a peer-reviewed, scholarly journal.
Downloaded by Alessio Paoletti on Sun May 02 2021 at 07:02:17 AM CDT
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Nowadays, there is a growing interest in measuring emotions through the estimation of cerebral variables. Several techniques and methods are used and debated in neuroscience. In such a context, the present paper provides examples of time-varying variables related to the estimation of emotional valence, arousal and Approach-Withdrawal behavior in marketing relevant contexts. In particular, we recorded electroencephalographic (EEG), galvanic skin response (GSR) and heart rate (HR) in a group of healthy subjects while they are watching different TV commercials. Specifically, results obtained in the Experiment 1 shows a significant increase of cortical power spectral density across left frontal areas in the alpha band and an enhance of cardiac activity during the observation of TV commercials that have been judged pleasant. In the Experiment 2, frontal EEG asymmetry, GSR and HR measurements are used to draw cognitive and emotional indices in order to track the subject’s internal state frame by frame of the commercial. A specific case study shows how the variations of the defined Approach-Withdrawal and emotional indices can distinguish the reactions of younger adults from the older ones during the observation of a funny spot. This technology could be of help for marketers to overcome some of the drawbacks of the standard marketing tools (e.g., interviews, focus groups) usually adopted during the analysis of the emotional perception of advertisements.
Article
Full-text available
In this paper, we introduce a general framework for product experience that applies to all affective responses that can be experienced in human-product interaction. Three distinct components or levels of product experiences are discussed: aesthetic experience, experience of meaning, and emotional experience. All three components are distinguished in having their own lawful underlying process. The aesthetic level involves a product’s capacity to delight one or more of our sensory modalities. The meaning level involves our ability to assign personality or other expressive characteristics and to assess the personal or symbolic significance of products. The emotional level involves those experiences that are typically considered in emotion psychology and in everyday language about emotions, such as love and anger, which are elicited by the appraised relational meaning of products. The framework indicates patterns for the processes that underlie the different types of affective product experiences, which are used to explain the personal and layered nature of product experience
Article
Full-text available
We present a theory of human artistic experience and the neural mechanisms that mediate it. Any theory of art (or, indeed, any aspect of human nature) has to ideally have three components. (a) The logic of art: whether there are universal rules or principles; (b) The evolutionary rationale: why did these rules evolve and why do they have the form that they do; (c) What is the brain circuitry involved? Our paper begins with a quest for artistic universals and proposes a list of ‘Eight laws of artistic experience’ -- a set of heuristics that artists either consciously or unconsciously deploy to optimally titillate the visual areas of the brain. One of these principles is a psychological phenomenon called the peak shift effect: If a rat is rewarded for discriminating a rectangle from a square, it will respond even more vigorously to a rectangle that is longer and skinnier that the prototype. We suggest that this principle explains not only caricatures, but many other aspects of art. Example: An evocative sketch of a female nude may be one which selectively accentuates those feminine form-attributes that allow one to discriminate it from a male figure; a Boucher, a Van Gogh, or a Monet may be a caricature in ‘colour space’ rather than form space. Even abstract art may employ ‘supernormal’ stimuli to excite form areas in the brain more strongly than natural stimuli. Second, we suggest that grouping is a very basic principle. The different extrastriate visual areas may have evolved specifically to extract correlations in different domains (e.g. form, depth, colour), and discovering and linking multiple features (‘grouping’) into unitary clusters -- objects -- is facilitated and reinforced by direct connections from these areas to limbic structures. In general, when object-like entities are partially discerned at any stage in the visual hierarchy, messages are sent back to earlier stages to alert them to certain locations or features in order to look for additional evidence for the object (and these processes may be facilitated by direct limbic activation). Finally, given constraints on allocation of attentional resources, art is most appealing if it produces heightened activity in a single dimension (e.g. through the peak shift principle or through grouping) rather than redundant activation of multiple modules. This idea may help explain the effectiveness of outline drawings and sketches, the savant syndrome in autists, and the sudden emergence of artistic talent in fronto-temporal dementia. In addition to these three basic principles we propose five others, constituting a total of ‘eight laws of aesthetic experience’(analogous to the Buddha's eightfold path to wisdom).
Book
Auditory Scene Analysis addresses the problem of hearing complex auditory environments, using a series of creative analogies to describe the process required of the human auditory system as it analyzes mixtures of sounds to recover descriptions of individual sounds. In a unified and comprehensive way, Bregman establishes a theoretical framework that integrates his findings with an unusually wide range of previous research in psychoacoustics, speech perception, music theory and composition, and computer modeling. Bradford Books imprint
Article
Mastering the Semi-Structured Interview and Beyond offers an in-depth and captivating step-by-step guide to the use of semi-structured interviews in qualitative research. By tracing the life of an actual research project-an exploration of a school district's effort over 40 years to address racial equality-as a consistent example threaded across the volume, Anne Galletta shows in concrete terms how readers can approach the planning and execution of their own new research endeavor, and illuminates unexpected real-life challenges they may confront and how to address them. The volume offers a close look at the inductive nature of qualitative research, the use of researcher reflexivity, and the systematic and iterative steps involved in data collection, analysis, and interpretation. It offers guidance on how to develop an interview protocol, including the arrangement of questions and ways to evoke analytically rich data. Particularly useful for those who may be familiar with qualitative research but have not yet conducted a qualitative study, Mastering the Semi-Structured Interview and Beyond will serve both undergraduate and graduate students as well as more advanced scholars seeking to incorporate this key methodological approach into their repertoire.
Article
To generate a product metaphor, designers must select a source, discern a property (or properties) of this source, and transfer this property to the product they design. The selection of any source in particular is affected by the extent to which it represents the meaning the designer intends to convey (i.e., its salience), and the strength of its association with the product (i.e., relatedness). In this paper, we tested how different levels of salience and relatedness influenced source selection in a study conducted with design students. The results indicate that a source was chosen only when it had the intended meaning as a highly salient property, and was highly related to the target product. It was also found that being novel yet understandable, having application potential, and creating a complete, functional product were also considered as source selection criteria by designers. This study aims to relate linguistic theories on metaphors to the domain of product design, and help to clarify how designers create comprehensible and aesthetic metaphors.
Article
A statistically average or prototypical member of a category (e.g., human faces) is often perceived as more attractive than less typical category members. Two experiments explored to what extent this may hold for music performance, an artistic domain in which individuality (i.e., deviation from prototypicality) is highly valued. In Experiment 1, graduate student pianists judged 11 student performances of Schumann's "Traumerei," one of which was created by forming the mathematical average of the other 10. The average performance was rated second highest in quality, even though it was judged second lowest in individuality. In Experiment 2, pianists judged 30 performances of the beginning of Chopin's Etude in E major, synthesized so as to vary only in expressive timing and tempo. The timing patterns were derived from expert pianists' recordings and from casual student performances, and they included separate and combined averages. All three averages received high quality ratings, and the expert average was rated highest of all 30 performances. There was a negative linear correlation between rated quality and individuality. Paradoxically, therefore, the students' expressive timing patterns were preferred over the experts'. Possible explanations of this finding are discussed, such as interactions between timing and other performance parameters and conditions under which conventionality tends to be favored over individuality.
Article
This chapter proposes a new integration of social psychology and situated cognition—that is, socially situated cognition (SSC). This new approach rests directly on recent developments in psychology and cognitive science captured by the label situated cognition. It highlights four core assumptions that are common to social psychology and the situated cognition perspective: Cognition is for the adaptive regulation of action, and mental representations are action oriented; cognition is embodied, drawing on our sensorimotor abilities and environments as well as human brains; cognition and action are the emergent outcome of dynamic processes of interaction between an agent and an environment; and cognition is distributed across brains and the environment and across social agents. With regard to each of these themes, the chapter reviews and integrates relevant social psychological research and suggests ways in which the theme can be advanced by rethinking current assumptions. The overall goals are to make social psychology part of the interdisciplinary integration emerging around the concept of situated cognition, and to advance these four themes as high-level conceptual principles that can organize seemingly disparate areas of research and theory within social psychology itself.
Article
Emotional responses to music are discussed in the context of a constructivist theory of emotion, which postulates evaluative cognitions that generate the quality of the experience, and autonomic (sympathetic) arousal that influences its intensity. Arousal is frequently generated by cognitive, pcrceptual, and behavioural discrepancies. Schema theory is used to describe the listener's knowledge of the complex structure of music. Schemas serve to interpret music as well as to produce expectations about its likely form and progression. When music is discrepant from the listener's expectations (as it often is), the concatenation of arousal and evaluation produces emotional experiences. This theoretical framework guides an interpretation of emotional responses to familiar and (physically) complex music, and suggests ways of understanding structural evaluations as well as effects of music that have their source outside of musical structure.