ArticlePDF Available

Associations between green/blue spaces and mental health across 18 countries

Springer Nature
Scientific Reports
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Living near, recreating in, and feeling psychologically connected to, the natural world are all associated with better mental health, but many exposure-related questions remain. Using data from an 18-country survey (n = 16,307) we explored associations between multiple measures of mental health (positive well-being, mental distress, depression/anxiety medication use) and: (a) exposures (residential/recreational visits) to different natural settings (green/inland-blue/coastal-blue spaces); and (b) nature connectedness, across season and country. People who lived in greener/coastal neighbourhoods reported higher positive well-being, but this association largely disappeared when recreational visits were controlled for. Frequency of recreational visits to green, inland-blue, and coastal-blue spaces in the last 4 weeks were all positively associated with positive well-being and negatively associated with mental distress. Associations with green space visits were relatively consistent across seasons and countries but associations with blue space visits showed greater heterogeneity. Nature connectedness was also positively associated with positive well-being and negatively associated with mental distress and was, along with green space visits, associated with a lower likelihood of using medication for depression. By contrast inland-blue space visits were associated with a greater likelihood of using anxiety medication. Results highlight the benefits of multi-exposure, multi-response, multi-country studies in exploring complexity in nature-health associations.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.

Scientic Reports | (2021) 11:8903 | 
www.nature.com/scientificreports
Associations between green/blue
spaces and mental health across 18
countries
Mathew P. White1,2*, Lewis R. Elliott2, James Grellier2,3, Theo Economou4, Simon Bell5,
Gregory N. Bratman6, Marta Cirach7,8,9, Mireia Gascon7,8,9, Maria L. Lima10,
Mare Lõhmus11, Mark Nieuwenhuijsen7,8,9, Ann Ojala12, Anne Roiko13, P. Wesley Schultz14,
Matilda van den Bosch7,15,16 & Lora E. Fleming2
Living near, recreating in, and feeling psychologically connected to, the natural world are all
associated with better mental health, but many exposure-related questions remain. Using data
from an 18-country survey (n = 16,307) we explored associations between multiple measures of
mental health (positive well-being, mental distress, depression/anxiety medication use) and: (a)
exposures (residential/recreational visits) to dierent natural settings (green/inland-blue/coastal-
blue spaces); and (b) nature connectedness, across season and country. People who lived in greener/
coastal neighbourhoods reported higher positive well-being, but this association largely disappeared
when recreational visits were controlled for. Frequency of recreational visits to green, inland-blue,
and coastal-blue spaces in the last 4 weeks were all positively associated with positive well-being
and negatively associated with mental distress. Associations with green space visits were relatively
consistent across seasons and countries but associations with blue space visits showed greater
heterogeneity. Nature connectedness was also positively associated with positive well-being and
negatively associated with mental distress and was, along with green space visits, associated with
a lower likelihood of using medication for depression. By contrast inland-blue space visits were
associated with a greater likelihood of using anxiety medication. Results highlight the benets of
multi-exposure, multi-response, multi-country studies in exploring complexity in nature-health
associations.
Poor mental health is the leading cause of disease burden in high-income countries1. is may, at least in part, be
a consequence of rapid urbanisation2, 3 and a growing disconnection from the natural world4, 5. A growing body
of research suggests that living near and/or maintaining regular contact with nature is benecial for a range of
health and well-being outcomes68, but several issues remain outstanding9.
First, there is a lack of clarity about the relative importance of merely living near nature, variously referred
to as residential proximity, neighbourhood exposure or indirect contact10, compared to more direct interac-
tions including deliberate engagement through recreational visits11. Although some benets to mental health
and well-being may result from mere neighbourhood exposure, e.g. reduced noise and air pollution and lower
temperatures, others are thought to derive from voluntarily spending time in natural settings for relaxation,
meeting others, and/or undertaking physical exercise10, 12. To date, the vast majority of studies have focused on
residential proximity13 and although a positive association is sometimes reported with recreational visits14, 15,
OPEN
             
             
          
   
            
            
        
  
           
            
        Department of Forest and
 *
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2021) 11:8903 | 
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
there is also evidence that many people rarely visit local nature16, while others travel, sometimes quite far, outside
of their neighbourhood for exercise and nature-based recreation17, 18. Proximity is a far from perfect proxy for use.
Second, emerging evidence suggests that mental health may be non-linearly related to recreational exposure,
with diminishing marginal returns beyond a certain threshold19. As with many other ‘goods’, it may be that the
benets of nature-based recreation become less pronounced with each additional visit. Greater clarity about the
relative importance of residential exposure and recreational visits, as well as their potentially non-linear relation-
ships, is critical in designing public health interventions that not only improve availability but also support the
most appropriate levels of use, both locally and further aeld.
ird, most research has operationalised nature in terms of ‘green space’ (e.g. parks, woodlands, street trees,
vegetation cover) and under-explored the potential role of both inland-blue spaces (e.g. rivers, lakes)20, 21, and
coastal-blue spaces (e.g. beaches, promenades)22, for mental health. Although green and blue spaces share many
qualities (e.g. cooling eects, biodiversity), blue spaces also oer alternative recreational activities (e.g. swim-
ming) and have additional features (e.g. unique soundscapes)2325. It is only through examining both in tandem
that we will get a clearer idea of their relative potential benets for mental health.
Fourth, the eld has used a wide range of mental health metrics, including indices of both positive and nega-
tive mental health6, 8, 10, 12. Rates of poor mental health tend to be lower among populations living in greener
neighbourhoods2628, and one-o nature walks have been shown to reduce symptoms of anxiety/depression in
at-risk populations29, 30. However, there has been relatively little large-scale research exploring relationships
between voluntary, recreational time in nature and indicators of mental health11, 19, 31. is is important because
meta-analyses suggest that the benets of direct nature exposure tend to have a larger eect on promoting positive
emotions than reducing negative ones32, and thus it may be that indicators of positive mental health are more
sensitive to recreational visits than negative ones. Again, this is best explored in studies that include multiple
exposure metrics alongside multiple mental health outcomes.
Fih, research suggests that psychological connectedness to the natural world, e.g. feeling part of nature or
seeing beauty in natural things, is also positively associated with positive well-being33. Given that people high
in nature connectedness also tend to report more recreational visits34, 35, any positive association between visits
and well-being may be due to the underlying nature connectedness an individual has, rather than a product of
the environment itself. To unpack this possibility, more research is needed to explore the simultaneous relation-
ships between exposures, nature connectedness and mental health, so that their unique roles can be identied.
Finally, there may be important seasonal and societal/cultural dierences in the way nature aects mental
health9, 10, 12. For instance, most research using the Normalized Dierential Vegetation Index (NDVI) as its
measure of residential green space uses summer data, and applies it to health data for the whole year even though
relationships may be dierent when leaf cover is lower in winter months10. Similarly, blue spaces may be bet-
ter for mental health in summer/autumn when the water temperatures are higher36. Living near and spending
time in green and blue space is also likely to be quite dierent, for instance, in southern European countries
than northern European countries. Not only are temperatures and vegetation dierent, hours of daylight vary
substantially across the year potentially aecting time outdoors36, 37.
e current research used a large international survey in an attempt to begin to address these issues. We
collected data on both residential exposure, using satellite imagery of a 1000m buer around the home, and
recreational visits, using self-reported visit frequency in the last four weeks. We also explored whether individu-
als had both inland-blue and coastal-blue space within 1000m buers of their home, and how oen they had
visited each type of blue space in the last 4 weeks. We collected measures of both positive and negative mental
health. Following earlier studies in the eld38, 39 we asked participants to complete the World Health Organisa-
tion’s 5-item index of positive well-being. e aggregate 100-point WHO-5 scale has the additional benet that
low scores (i.e. < 28) are indicative of being at risk of depression/anxiety40, 41, and are thus an indicator of mental
distress. Additionally, we included two questions from the European Health Interview Survey that asked about
recent use of doctor-prescribed medication for depression and anxiety42. To explore the role of nature connected-
ness, we included the Inclusion of Nature in Self (INS) scale43, 44. Finally, our survey was conducted at four times
during a 12-month period, to explore seasonal eects, and across 18 countries/regions to explore generalisability
across locations.
We investigated four hypotheses (H). H1: Greater residential exposure to green, inland-blue and coastal-blue
spaces will be associated with (a) higher positive well-being, (b) lower probability of mental distress, and lower
probability of medication use for (c) depression and (d) anxiety. H2: More frequent recreational visits to these
three settings will show similar relations to those for residential exposure for the four outcomes. H3: e positive
association between visits and mental health in H2 will be non-linear and show diminishing marginal returns.
H4: Psychological connectedness to nature will be a signicant independent predictor of mental health outcomes
over and above residential exposure and recreational contact. Two more exploratory research questions (RQs)
focused on the consistency of any overarching relationships found between nature exposure, connectedness and
mental health across season (RQ1) and country (RQ2).
Hypotheses were tested using a series of linear mixed eects models for WHO-5 scores, and Bernoulli gener-
alised linear mixed eects models for the binary outcomes of mental distress and medication use. Main models
included: (a) residential exposure, (b) recreational visits, and (c) nature connectedness; (d) quadratic (squared)
terms for visit frequency and connectedness to test for non-linearity; and controlled for potential covariates.
Analyses were re-run using stratication on: (a) season; and (b) country, to explore RQs (see “Materials and
methods” section for more details).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2021) 11:8903 | 
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Results
Descriptive data for key predictors are presented in Table1 and data for all covariates in Supplementary TableS1.
Table2 presents core model summaries with full models including all covariates presented in Supplementary
TablesS2–S5. Due to space constraints in the text, descriptive data and covariates are only discussed in Supple-
mentary Materials, and the 95% Condence Intervals for estimates are reported in Tables and Figures. In order to
maintain model power for our more exploratory questions into seasonal and country variation we focused on the
WHO-5 positive well-being scores, rather than the dichotomous indices of mental distress and medication use.
Residential exposure (H1). ere was limited support for Hypothesis 1. e only signicant association
between residential exposure and mental health was for the WHO-5 scores for the 3rd versus 1st quartile of
greenspace (β = 1.01; p < 0.05; Table2). is was partly due to the inclusion of visit frequency in the main model.
Without visit frequency, but with socio-demographic controls (Supplementary TableS2), there were also posi-
tive associations between living in quartile 4 (vs. quartile 1) of greenspace (β = 1.78, p < 0.001) and living within
1000m of the coast (β = 1.98; p < 0.001). ere were no associations between residential exposure and mental
distress or depression/anxiety medication use in models including or excluding visit frequency (Supplementary
TablesS2, S3).
Recreational visits (H2 and H3). Supporting Hypothesis 2, the linear terms for visit frequencies were sig-
nicantly positively associated with WHO-5 scores: green space (β = 0.26; p < 0.001); inland-blue space (β = 0.12;
p < 0.001); coastal-blue space (β = 0.19; p < 0.001), and negatively associated with the likelihood of mental distress
(WHO-5 < 28; a ll three ORs = 0.97; p < 0.001). e likelihood of using depression medication was also negatively
associated with green space visit frequency (OR = 0.99, p < 0.05). In contrast, the likelihood of using anxiety
medication was positively associated with inland-blue space visits (ORs = 1.02; p < 0.05).
Partly supporting Hypothesis 3, there were also signicant quadratic terms, indicative of non-linear dimin-
ishing marginal returns, for: (a) green space and inland-blue space visits and positive well-being (WHO-5); (b)
all three visit types and mental distress (WHO-5 < 28); and (c) green space visits and depression medication use.
However, because the estimates are based on only one extra visit per 4weeks, the odds ratios are only visibly
dierent from a null result at the third decimal. To aid interpretation, Fig.1 plots the combined eects of the
linear and quadratic terms for each visit type, for each outcome (panels a–l). Taking panel (a) as an example,
the linear relationship between green space visits and WHO-5 is reected in the positive upward slope, and the
quadratic eect is reected in the rate of increase getting gradually smaller and the curve beginning to atten
out. e wider condence intervals to the right reect fewer people visiting green spaces more than 40 times in
the last four weeks and the curve ends at 56 visits due to our capping procedure at a maximum of two visits per
day (see “Methods” section). e opposite eect occurs for measures of mental distress, e.g. panel (b) shows a
decreased probability of reporting a WHO-5 score < 28 with each additional green space visit, but this decrease
gets progressively smaller as the number of visits increases. e large condence intervals for high levels of inland
visits were due to the small number of people visiting these spaces > 40 times in the last four weeks.
Table 1. e Ns, percentages (%), means (Ms), standard deviations (SDs), and correlations (r / rpb) for the
four mental health outcomes as a function of residential exposure(Q = quartile), recreational visits and nature
connectedness for the analytical sample (n = 16,302). r Pearsons correlation, rpb point bi-serial correlation (due
to binary outcome); INS= inclusion of nature in self scale. ***p < 0.001; see Supplementary TableS1 for details
of all covariates.
n % M SD
WHO-5 WHO-5 < 28 Depression Meds Anxiety Meds
M/r SD N/rpb % N/rpb % N/rpb %
Residential exposure [within 1000m]
Greenspace [Q1] 4103 25.17 1.36 1.87 58.79 21.55 381 9.29 354 8.63 366 8.92
Greenspace [Q2] 4098 25.14 19.79 9.35 59.73 21.57 352 8.59 362 8.83 389 9.49
Greenspace [Q3] 4071 24.97 62.11 14.44 61.29 21.53 333 8.18 330 8.11 374 9.19
Greenspace [Q4] 4030 24.72 96.85 4.18 60.86 22.17 352 8.73 405 10.05 416 10.32
Inland blue [no] 10,141 62.21 NA NA 60.27 22.00 897 8.85 872 8.60 962 9.49
Inland blue [yes] 6161 37.79 NA NA 59.98 21.25 521 8.46 579 9.40 583 9.46
Coastal blue [no] 14,507 88.99 NA NA 60.04 21.77 1272 8.77 1330 9.17 1410 9.72
Coastal blue [yes] 1795 11.01 NA NA 61.11 21.35 146 8.13 121 6.74 135 7.52
Recreational visits [last 4weeks]
Green NA NA 12.34 12.85 0.26*** NA − 0.12*** NA − 0.01 NA 0.03*** NA
Inland blue NA NA 6.08 8.95 0.19*** NA − 0.08*** NA 0.03* NA 0.06*** NA
Coastal blue NA NA 5.34 10.17 0.18*** NA − 0.07*** NA 0.00 NA 0.05*** NA
Nature connectedness
INS NA NA 4.14 1.65 0.24*** NA − 0.11*** NA − 0.04*** NA − 0.03*** NA
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2021) 11:8903 | 
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Nature connectedness (H4). Supporting Hypothesis 4, nature connectedness was independently: (a)
positively associated with positive well-being (β = 2.35, p < 0.001); (b) negatively associated with mental distress
(OR = 0.62; p < 0.001), with diminishing marginal returns reected in a signicant quadratic term (OR = 1.05;
p < 0.001); and (c) negatively associated with depression medication use (OR = 0.83, p < 0.05). ese relationships
are shown in panels m–p in Fig.1. Note that the larger coecients for connectedness are partly a reection of
the fact this was a seven-point scale (compared to the 0–56 scale for visits).
Seasonality (RQ1). Figure2 presents visit frequency for the last 4weeks as a function of season. Despite
the drop of approximately two visits in all three settings in autumn/winter, compared to spring/summer, visits to
inland-blue and coastal-blue both remained at an average of just above 4 (i.e. once a week). e stratied results
predicting positive well-being for each season are presented in Supplementary TableS4. Residential greenspace
was only signicantly associated with positive well-being for Q3 versus Q1 in spring (β = 1.78), and there con-
tinued to be no signicant associations with either residential inland- or coastal- blue space in any season. In
terms of visits, each additional green space visit was associated with signicantly greater WHO-5 scores across
all four seasons (spring β = 0.24, summer β = 0.22, autumn β = 0.28, winter β = 0.31, all p s < 0.001). A signicant
association with coastal-blue space visits was found in summer (β = 0.23), autumn (β = 0.21) and winter (β = 0.20;
ps < 0.01), and with inland-blue space visits only in spring β = 0.14 and winter β = 0.14 (ps < 0.05). Nature con-
nectedness was also only positively associated with WHO-5 in summer (β = 2.41), autumn (β = 2.29), and w inter
(β = 3.18; all ps < 0.05).
Country-specic results (RQ2). e stratied results predicting WHO-5 positive well-being for each
country are presented in Supplementary TableS5. Results reect the country-level heterogeneity identied
through the random eect term in the main model. In terms of residential exposure, WHO-5 scores were signi-
cantly higher in both Ireland (Q2 vs. Q1: β = 4.20; Q3 vs. Q1: β = 4.15; Q4 vs. Q1: β = 3.65, ps < 0.05) and Italy (Q3
Table 2. Mental health as a function of residential exposure, recreational visits and nature connectedness
controlling for socio-demographics, season and country. Analyses used survey weights. INS inclusion
of nature in self scale. a Variance of country-level intercepts from the random eects component of the
model; Marginal R2 includes only xed eects and Conditional R2 includes the random country eect, R2
for binary outcomes = Nakawaga Pseudo R2. Models control for sex, age, household income, employment
status, education, long-term illness/disability, marital status, number of adults and children in household,
dog and car ownership, weekly physical activity, season of data collection, and use of the alternative
depression/anxiety medication for medication models only; full models in Supplementary TablesS2 and S3.
*p < 0.05,**p < 0.01,***p < 0.001.
Predictors
WHO-5 scale (0–100) WHO-5 distress (< 28) Depression medication
use Anxiety medication use
Estimates 95% CIs Odds ratios 95% CIs Odds ratios 95% CIs Odds ratios 95% CIs
(Intercept) 48.15*** 46.06, 50.24 0.19*** 0.14, 0.27 0.06*** 0.04, 0.09 0.06*** 0.04, 0.09
Residential exposure [within 1000m]
Greenspace [Q2 vs. Q1] 0.46 − 0.38, 1.30 0.94 0.80, 1.11 0.99 0.83, 1.19 1.15 0.97, 1.36
Greenspace [Q3 vs. Q1] 1.01* 0.15, 1.87 0.93 0.78, 1.10 0.85 0.71, 1.03 1.03 0.86, 1.24
Greenspace [Q4 vs. Q1] 0.37 − 0.51, 1.25 1.02 0.86, 1.21 0.99 0.82, 1.19 1.05 0.88, 1.25
Inland blue [Yes vs. No] − 0.08 − 0.74, 0.58 0.94 0.82, 1.07 0.96 0.83, 1.10 1.00 0.87, 1.14
Coastal blue [Yes vs.
No] 0.74 − 0.31, 1.79 1.01 0.81, 1.25 0.90 0.71, 1.15 0.82 0.65, 1.03
Recreational visits [last 4weeks]
Green 0.26*** 0.22, 0.30 0.97*** 0.96, 0.98 0.99* 0.98, 1.00 1.00 0.99, 1.01
Green2 − 0.00* − 0.00, − 0.00 1.00* 1.00, 1.00 1.00* 1.00, 1.00 1.00 1.00, 1.00
Inland blue 0.12*** 0.05, 0.19 0.97*** 0.96, 0.99 1.01 0.99, 1.02 1.02* 1.00, 1.03
Inland blue2 − 0.00 − 0.00, 0.00 1.00** 1.00, 1.00 1.00 1.00, 1.00 1.00 1.00, 1.00
Coastal blue 0.19*** 0.12, 0.25 0.97*** 0.96, 0.99 0.99 0.98, 1.01 1.01 1.00, 1.02
Coastal blue2 − 0.00 − 0.00, 0.00 1.00* 1.00, 1.00 1.00 1.00, 1.00 1.00 1.00, 1.00
Nature connectedness
INS 2.35*** 1.45, 3.25 0.62*** 0.52, 0.72 0.83* 0.70, 1.00 0.96 0.81, 1.14
INS2 − 0.09 − 0.20, 0.01 1.05*** 1.03, 1.07 1.02 1.00, 1.04 1.00 0.98, 1.02
Random eects
18 country intercept
variancea6.34 0.07 0.18 0.13
Observations 16,302 16,302 16,302 16,302
Marginal R2/Condi-
tional R20.216/0.230 0.235/0.250 0.315/0.351 0.240/0.269
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2021) 11:8903 | 
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
vs. Q1: β = 4.82; Q4 vs. Q1: β = 4.54, ps < 0.05) in greener neighbourhoods. is pattern was reversed for Finland
(Q3 vs. Q1: β = − 4.20, p < 0.05), where instead, having inland water within 1000m was associated with signi-
cantly higher WHO-5 scores (β = 3.53, p < 0.01). By contrast, in Portugal, inland water was associated with sig-
nicantly lower scores (β = -3.81, p < 0.05). Ireland was the only country where living within 1000m of the coast
was associated with higher WHO-5 scores when controlling for visits and connectedness (β = 5.00, p < 0.05).
An increase of one green space visit in the last four weeks was associated with signicantly greater (at
least p < 0.05) WHO-5 scores in Australia (β = 0.41), Bulgaria (β = 0.48), California (β = 0.42), Czech Republic
(β = 0.27), Estonia (β = 0.23), Finland (β = 0.19), Greece (β = 0.54), Ireland (β = 0.39), Netherlands (β = 0.18),
Portugal (β = 0.32), and Sweden (β = 0.32). For each extra inland-blue visit, WHO-5 scores were signicantly
higher (at least p < 0.05) in Germany (β = 0.36), Hong Kong (β = 0.53) and Spain (β = 0.44), and each additional
coastal visit was associated with higher WHO-5 scores in France (β = 0.57), Portugal (β = 0.27), Spain (β = 0.24),
and Sweden (β = 0.46). Finally a one-point increase in INS scores was associated with signicantly higher (at
least p < 0.05) WHO-5 scores in Canada (β = 4.30), Czech Republic (β = 5.41), Greece (β = 4.40), Hong Kong
(β = 7.61), and UK (β = 3.59).
40
50
60
70
02040
No. of greenspace visits − last four weeks
Predicted WHO−5 score
(0−100)
a
0.0
0.2
0.4
0.6
02040
No. of greenspace visits − last four weeks
Predicted probability of
depression (WHO−5 score <28)
b
0.0
0.2
0.4
0.6
02040
No. of greenspace visits − last four weeks
Predicted probability of
taking depression medication
c
0.0
0.2
0.4
0.6
02040
No. of greenspace visits − last four weeks
Predicted probability of
taking anxiety medication
d
40
50
60
70
02040
No. of inland bluespace visits − last four weeks
Predicted WHO−5 score
(0−100)
e
0.0
0.2
0.4
0.6
02040
No. of inland bluespace visits − last four weeks
Predicted probability of
depression (WHO−5 score <28)
f
0.0
0.2
0.4
0.6
02040
No. of inland bluespace visits − last four weeks
Predicted probability of
taking depression medication
g
0.0
0.2
0.4
0.6
02040
No. of inland bluespace visits − last four weeks
Predicted probability of
taking anxiety medication
h
40
50
60
70
02040
No. of coastal bluespace visits − last four weeks
Predicted WHO−5 score
(0−100)
i
0.0
0.2
0.4
0.6
02040
No. of coastal bluespace visits − last four weeks
Predicted probability of
depression (WHO−5 score <28)
j
0.0
0.2
0.4
0.6
02040
No. of coastal bluespace visits − last four weeks
Predicted probability of
taking depression medication
k
0.0
0.2
0.4
0.6
02040
No. of coastal bluespace visits − last four weeks
Predicted probability of
taking anxiety medication
l
40
50
60
70
1234567
Nature connectedness (1−7)
Predicted WHO−5 score
(0−100)
m
0.0
0.2
0.4
0.6
1234567
Nature connectedness (1−7)
Predicted probability of
depression (WHO−5 score <28)
n
0.0
0.2
0.4
0.6
1234567
Nature connectedness (1−7)
Predicted probability of
taking depression medication
o
0.0
0.2
0.4
0.6
1234567
Nature connectedness (1−7)
Predicted probability of
taking anxiety medication
p
Figure1. Relationships between: (1) Green space visits in last 4weeks, (2) Inland-blue space visits in last
4weeks, (3) Coastal-blue space visits in last 4weeks, and (4) nature connectedness (1–7); and positive well-
being (0–100; a,e,l,m), risk of mental distress (0–1; b,f,j,n), use of depression medication (0–1; c,g,k,o), and
use of anxiety medication (0–1; d,h,l,p), averaged across 18 countries (n = 16,302). Plots are based on predicted
values from linear and logistic mixed eects regression models including linear and quadratic terms (with
95% Condence Intervals) for visit frequency and connectedness controlling for residential exposure, visit
frequencies to alternative locations, connectedness (al only), age, gender, employment status, relationship
status, household income, longstanding-illness, education level, household composition, dog ownership, car
ownership, physical activity, season (sample wave), and country (as a random eect). Depression models
also control for anxiety medication use and vice versa. Visit frequency was capped at n = 56 (i.e. two visits per
day over 4weeks). Covariates are held constant at their reference categories, or at their means for continuous
predictors.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2021) 11:8903 | 
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
To help visualise cross-country patterns we used the observed values from recreational visit frequency and
connectedness, and the predicted values of WHO-5 from our original models, averaged across all individuals
in each country (Fig.3). With lower than average visit duration and connectedness, Hong Kong, the UK, and
California, also reported the lowest positive well-being. By contrast, countries with the highest levels of posi-
tive well-being (e.g. Spain, Portugal, and Bulgaria) were among the countries with the highest nature visits and
connectedness.
Discussion
e present research provides signicant new insights into the relationships between mental health, residential
and recreational exposure to green and blue spaces, and feeling psychologically connected to the natural world.
Collecting data in four seasonal waves, across 18 dierent countries/regions allowed us to make far more nuanced
conclusions than are generally possible.
Contrary to Hypothesis 1, there was little evidence in the current sample that the amount of green, and
presence of inland- and coastal-blue space, within 1000m of the home was directly related to mental health. In
models without recreational visits, but controlling for socio-demographic confounders, residents of the greenest
and coastal areas did report higher positive well-being, but these eects disappeared when visits were added,
suggesting that visit frequency mediated these eects. In other words, the reason why residents of the greenest
and coastal neighbourhoods experienced better positive mental health might be because these neighbourhood
qualities encouraged more frequent recreational visits12, 14, 15. e only residential exposure metric that signi-
cantly predicted positive mental health controlling for visits was living in the 3rd versus 1st quartile of green
space, with the season models suggesting this was only signicant in spring.
Despite the overall picture, some residential associations did remain aer controlling for visits in the country-
specic models. Ireland showed higher WHO-5 scores for those in greener and coastal neighbourhoods, and Ital-
ians also had higher positive well-being in greener neighbourhoods even accounting for visits and connectedness.
Residents in Finland were the only sample to show signicantly lower well-being in the greenest areas, though
they did have higher well-being if they lived near rivers/lakes. Finally, those in Portugal had lower WHO-5 if
they lived near inland waters. Although tempting, we are reluctant to speculate here about possible reasons for
these cross-country dierences. Our eect sizes are small, and thus some countries may not be showing patterns
due to a lack of power. In countries where eects did emerge, we were not able to explore potential mechanisms
underlying relationships. Further cross-country research is needed with larger within-country samples and a
greater focus on potential mechanisms to address these possibilities, but the cross-country heterogeneity does
support the contention that caution is needed when trying to generalise across locations12.
Supporting Hypothesis 2, the frequency of visits to green spaces in the last 4 weeks was positively associated
with positive well-being and negatively associated with mental distress and the use of doctor-prescribed depres-
sion (though not anxiety) medication. Extending previous research, those who made more frequent visits to
both inland- and coastal- blue spaces also reported more positive well-being and lower rates of mental distress,
even controlling for the number of green space visits in the past four weeks. We recognise that despite being
signicant, these eects are, however, small in absolute terms. For instance, an extra 4 green space visits (i.e. one
0
5
10
Spring SummerAutumn Winter
Season
Average number of visits
in the last four weeks
Environment type
Greenspace
Inland bluespace
Coastal bluespace
Figure2. Average number of visits to green spaces, inland-blue spaces and coastal-blue spaces as a function of
season across the whole sample (n = 16,302). Error bars represent 95% condence intervals.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2021) 11:8903 | 
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
per week) is still only associated with a 1.04% higher WHO-5 score (i.e. β = 0.26 × 4, on the 100-point WHO-5
scale). Intriguingly, visiting inland-blue spaces was positively associated with anxiety medication use. Given that
we asked about voluntary recreational visits, it seems unlikely that visits could lead to greater anxiety sucient to
require medication (or these individuals would stop going). Rather, we suspect that it reects people with anxiety
seeking these places out for the calming eects they have, and thus using them for self-management purposes45, 46.
Although visits decreased in frequency in autumn/winter, compared to spring/summer, the drop was not
substantial and was similar for both green and blue spaces. Indeed, positive well-being remained signicantly
positively associated with visiting inland and coastal waters in winter, suggesting that potential benets to men-
tal health do not only occur in the warmer months. In terms of country-level eects, a positive association was
found between at least one type of visit and WHO-5 scores in 16/18 countries, with no associations present for
Canada or the UK. Most countries (11/18) showed a positive association with green space visit frequency, and
Spain, Hong Kong and Germany showed a positive relationship with visits to inland waters. Much of the research
on inland-blue spaces has come from Germany-based researchers20, 23, 47 potentially pointing to something more
fundamental in a country with a relatively low coastline to population ratio. ree of the four countries showing
a positive association between coastal visit frequency and mental health were in the warmer European South
(Spain, Portugal and France). e fourth country to show this relationship was Sweden, which also showed a
signicant positive association with green space visits, potentially indicating the importance of overall outdoor
nature recreation among this population for mental health37.
Partially supporting Hypothesis 3 there was also tentative evidence of non-linear relationships for visits, with
diminishing marginal returns. Nevertheless, due the cross-sectional nature of the data, and small eect sizes,
we remain cautious. Further work is needed including longitudinal work that follows people’s exposure over
time and experimental work that randomly allocates people to dierent visit frequencies within a given period.
Supporting Hypothesis 4, greater nature connectedness was positively associated with positive well-being and
negatively associated with both mental distress and depression medication use. When stratied by season and
country (for WHO-5) a more complicated picture emerged. Although the association between connectedness
and positive well-being was evident in summer, autumn and winter, it was non-signicant in spring. By contrast,
Queensland, AU
Bulgaria
California, US
Canada Czech Republic
Estonia
Finland
France
Germany
Greece
Hong Kong, CN
Ireland
Italy
Netherlands
Portugal
Spain
Sweden
United Kingdom
50
55
60
65
70
7.510.012.515.017.5
Average number of greenspace visits − last four weeks
Average predicted WHO5
wellbeing index score (0−100)
Queensland, AU
Bulgaria
California, US
Canada
Czech Republic
Estonia
Finland
France
Germany
Greece
Hong Kong, CN
Ireland
Italy
Netherlands
Portugal
Spain
Sweden
United Kingdom
50
55
60
65
70
46 18 0
Average number of inland bluespace visits − last four weeks
Average predicted WHO5
wellbeing index score (0−100)
Queensland, AU
Bulgaria
California, US
Canada
Czech Republic
Estonia
Finland
France
Germany
Greece
Hong Kong, CN
Ireland
Italy
Netherlands
Portugal
Spain
Sweden
United Kingdom
50
55
60
65
70
4812
Average number of coastal bluespace visits − last four weeks
Average predicted WHO5
wellbeing index score (0−100)
Queensland, AU
Bulgaria
California, US
Canada
Czech Republic
Estonia
Finland
France
Germany
Greece
Hong Kong, CN
Ireland
Italy
Netherlands
Portugal
Spain
Sweden
United Kingdom
50
55
60
65
70
345
Average nature connectedness (1−7)
Average predicted WHO5
wellbeing index score (0−100)
Figure3. Country level relationships between positive well-being (0–100) and: (a) Green space visits in last
4weeks; (b) Inland-blue space visits in last 4weeks; (c) Coastal-blue space visits in last 4weeks and (d) nature
connectedness (1–7). Plots are based on aggregated predicted values across countries from our original mixed
models controlling for residential exposure visit frequencies to alternative locations, connectedness (ac),
age, gender, employment status, household income, longstanding-illness, relationship status, education level,
household composition, dog ownership, car ownership, physical activity, and season (sample wave).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2021) 11:8903 | 
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
we note that residential greenness was only related to WHO-5 in spring and it may be that these are interacting in
some way but we were unable to explore this further here. Combined, the ndings suggest that researchers with
quite a broad spectrum of interests in the nature-health eld (inc. residential exposure, visits, connectedness)
might want to be more sensitive to issues of seasonality in future work.
Further, only four countries showed signicant associations between positive well-being and nature con-
nectedness in the stratied models, two of which, Canada and the UK, were the only countries to not show
signicant associations between positive well-being and at least one sort of visit. While recognising the potential
for statistical artefacts (due to shared variance), as with Germany and inland-blue space research, we note a strong
research tradition in nature connectedness in both Canada34 and the UK48. Again we wonder whether this is
merely coincidence or whether it reects a higher level of importance of nature connectedness in these countries
that has ltered through to research priorities, perhaps because of the relatively low levels of connectedness at
the population level.
Intriguingly, the other countries/regions with low levels of connectedness (and visit frequencies), Hong Kong,
California, Queensland, and Ireland, have certain commonalities in terms of language and cultural heritage with
UK/Canada. Although there are very few international studies with which to compare our ndings, Kruize etal.49
also found the lowest amount of regular time in nature in the UK city (Stoke on Trent) of their four city study
(Barcelona [Spain], Kaunas [Lithuania], and Doetinchem [Netherlands]), supporting the current visit results.
Further research is needed to explore what other commonalities these countries might have (e.g. economic
models of growth or attitudes towards the natural environment) that could explain these ndings.
Despite the robust sample and use of multiple, internationally recognised measures of well-being and mental
health, we recognise several limitations with the current work. First, we acknowledge that multiple residen-
tial buers have been used in past research, and it may be that the relatively little evidence of an association
between residential exposure and mental health here is in part a consequence of our 1000m selection based on
a 10–15min walk50. Further there may be limitations in the methods we used to establish common green/blue
space residential metrics across European and non-European countries, or the way in which we operationalised
green and blue spaces with these metrics (e.g. the landcovers we included in green space)51. Future international
studies may want to select alternative buers and/or methods of assessing residential exposure.
Second, much of the data were self-reported and we were unable to validate, for instance, peoples nature
experiences or medication use. For current purposes, we applied approximate numerical values to verbal visit
frequency response categories and it is also possible that some respondents ‘double-counted’ some visit loca-
tions (e.g. saying they had visited woodlands and a lake in the last 4weeks when in fact they only did one visit
that included both features). Similarly, although our prescription item is widely used42, it also does not account
for length of use or dosage. Although challenging to collect on a similar scale as our multi-country study, more
objective data on time in nature, e.g. using experience sampling approaches22, and mental health status should
be a goal of future research.
ird, as already noted, the data is cross-sectional and thus can only speak to associations rather than cau-
sation. is was perhaps most evident in the positive association between inland-blue space visits and anxiety
medication, which we took to suggest reverse causality. Nonetheless, many of our results are consistent with a
growing body of experimental and longitudinal research, and used the sort of sample that would not be easily
possible with these approaches.
Fourth, our results focus on averages and we recognise that individuals may vary widely in terms of the
amount of nature that may benet them personally, and that this too is likely to change over time as a function
of need45.
Fih, although our sample was collected by an international polling company and was weighted to be repre-
sentative by age, gender and region within each country, it was not fully representative of the respective countries,
in part due to limitations of online panels52. Our country-level observations therefore remain tentative at this
stage.
Finally, our sample was limited to a selection of high-income countries/regions, and further research is
needed in low-middle income nations where contact with the natural world, and consequent relationships, may
be dierent. At this stage, our ndings only speak to relatively developed settings where, typically, the natural
world presents few threats and challenges. Conclusions about whether contact with, and connectedness to, the
natural world is a universal good for human mental health and well-being will depend on the results of similar
research across a far broader range of contexts.
ese limitations notwithstanding, our ndings have a number of implications. Results suggest the associa-
tions between recreational nature contact and clinical levels of mental distress are complicated. People may be
using these environments to manage symptoms46 and perhaps we should not necessarily expect higher levels
of recreational contact to be associated with incidence of depression and/or anxiety at a population level. More
research is needed into how people with poor mental health spontaneously use nature to help with self-man-
agement, alongside more traditional research trying to support them to access these places through things such
as ‘green prescriptions’53.
Results also oer support for initiatives e.g. education programs, aimed at increasing levels of psychologi-
cal connectedness to the natural world, irrespective of direct exposure, for mental health as well as ecological
reasons54. Given how relatively disconnected from the natural world our UK sample was, alongside low levels
of well-being, it is promising that the UK government is prioritising the building of nature connectedness in
the population55. Other countries in the English speaking world with low nature connectedness and well-being
might consider a similar approach.
Finally, the results suggest that spending recreational time in both green and blue settings may be more
important than merely living near nature, at least in terms of mental health. Although social inequalities in access
and quality remain56, over 90% of people living in urban areas of Europe already have access to a public green
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2021) 11:8903 | 
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
space > 0.25 hectares within a 10-min walk of their home57. Promoting greater use of these green (and blue) spaces
may be a policy objective to go alongside structural changes in the amount of green and blue spaces in people’s
neighbourhoods. For instance, the United Nations (UN) Sustainable Development Goal [SDG] 11.7 proposes
that “by 2030, [states should] provide universal access to safe, inclusive and accessible, green and public spaces,
particularly for women and children, older persons and persons with disabilities”58. Future SDGs, or similar
programs, might consider expressing targets in terms of use of, as well as access to, green/blue spaces, analogous
to how SDG 12: ‘Ensure sustainable consumption and production patterns’, has sub-goals for both policies and
infrastructure (12.1), and citizen actions and behaviors (12.5).
Materials and methods
Sample and survey. Data came from an 18-country self-report survey conducted as part of the BlueHealth
project59, exploring recreational use of the natural environment with a particular focus on aquatic, or blue space,
environments such as rivers, lakes and seas. It was administered by an international polling company using
established online panels in four seasonal waves between June 2017 and April 2018. Stratied samples of ≈
1000 respondents were collected in 14 European countries (Bulgaria, Czech Republic, Estonia, Finland, France,
Germany, Greece, Ireland, Italy, Netherlands, Portugal, Spain, Sweden, and the United Kingdom) and four other
countries/regions (California [USA], Canada, Hong Kong [China], and Queensland [Australia]). Stratied sam-
pling by sex, age, and region of residence was undertaken to achieve broad national representativeness. e
full sample consisted of 18,838 respondents, and survey weights were provided by data collectors to adjust for
representativeness in analyses. Due to missing data (e.g. ‘don’t know’ responses on the INS scale and elsewhere)
the analytical sample was n = 16,307. Full methodological details are available on the Open Science Framework
website: https:// doi. org/ 10. 17605/ OSF. IO/ 7AZU251. Data collection was carried out in accordance with rele-
vant guidelines and regulations, and informed consent was obtained from all participants. Ethical approval was
granted by the University of Exeter Medical School’s Research Ethics Committee (Ref: Aug16/B/099).
Mental health. Following previous research in the eld38, 39, our measure of positive well-being was the
World Health Organisation 5-item wellbeing index (WHO-5). Participants responded to ve statements about
their emotional state during the past two weeks e.g. “I have felt calm and relaxed”, on scales from ‘At no time’ (0)
to ‘All of the time’ (5). Values were summed and multiplied by 4 to give a score out of 100, with higher scores
reecting higher well-being. An advantage of the WHO-5 is that scores < 28 have shown concurrent validity with
structured clinical interviews for diagnosing depression/anxiety40, 41, and thus this threshold provided our rst
indicator of poor mental health, i.e. mental distress.
Our second and third indicators of poor mental health were self-reported use of doctor-prescribed medica-
tion for: (a) depression, and (b) anxiety. Respondents were asked: “During the past two weeks, have you used
any medicines for any of the following conditions that were prescribed for you by a doctor? Please select all that
apply”, with ‘yes’/‘no’ response options. Alongside physical health conditions, e.g. high blood pressure, were the
conditions of current interest: ‘depression’ and ‘tension and anxiety’. e question was taken from the European
Health Interview Survey42. As 4.0% (n = 740) reported taking both medications, our regressions predicting either
outcome, controlled for concurrent use of the alternative medication type to identify the unique associations
with contact and connectedness with use of each medication.
Residential exposure. Participants were asked to input their home location via a Google Maps applica-
tion programming interface. For condentiality reasons, recorded coordinates were rounded to three decimal
degrees on both the longitude and latitude scale. Residential natural environment exposure indicators were
assigned to these coordinates using the Global Land Cover dataset (GlobeLand30), which is a globally-consist-
ent 30m resolution raster data set based on classication of remotely-sensed data. Full details of our processing
of this data and references to relevant earlier work can be found in the technical report51. e data feature ten
land cover classes which have demonstrated satisfactory congruence with more localised land use maps (general
accuracy level of > 80%). Land classied as “forests”, “grassland”, “shrubland” and “cultivated land” was collapsed
into a ‘green space’ measure and land classied as “water bodies” or “wetlands” into an ‘inland-blue space’ meas-
ure. Radial buers of 1000m around residential locations, representing a 10–15min walk50 were established
and the percentage of green and inland-blue spaces within these buers assigned. Residential green space was
divided into four quartiles, and due to a highly skewed distribution15, inland-blue space was categorised into
just “none” = 0% (reference) and “some” > 0% to 100%. Residential exposure to coastal-blue space within 1000m
was calculated using a Euclidean (crow-ies) distance metric. Distance from the home coordinate to the nearest
coastline was dened by the highest resolution version of the Global Self-consistent Hierarchical High-resolu-
tion Geography shoreline database from the National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration51. is dataset
provides a balance between renement in capturing a good representation of the land-sea interface, but enough
granularity that smaller rivers and other inland waterways are rarely miss-classied as coastline.
Recreational contact with green/blue spaces. Participants were presented with a list, and archetypical
pictures of, 12 types of green spaces (e.g. local park, woodlands, meadows), 9 inland-blue spaces (e.g. lake, rural
river, canal) and 8 coastal-blue spaces (e.g. esplanades, rocky shores, beaches) and asked how oen in the last 4
weeks they had visited each type of location. e last 4 weeks was chosen as an appropriate recall period due to
its use in previous leisure visit surveys51. Response options, were: “Not at all in the last 4 weeks”, “Once or twice
in the last 4 weeks”, ” Once a week” and “Several times a week”. For current purposes we estimated a numerical
equivalent of these response options to be zero, one, four and eight visits in the last 4 weeks respectively.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved

Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2021) 11:8903 | 
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Total green space visits in the last 4weeks were derived by summing the visit frequency estimates for each of
the 12 green space types. Due to a small number of people reporting very high visit frequencies, and introduc-
ing considerable skew, we capped the total number of visits to 56, which would be consistent with someone, for
instance, walking their dog twice a day over a 4-week period. Only 1.5% of respondents were capped in this way.
Four weekly inland- and coastal-blue space visit frequencies were derived in a similar way with only 0.5% and
0.6% of respondents requiring a cap for inland and coastal visits respectively.
Nature connectedness. Psychological connectedness to the natural world was measured using the Inclu-
sion of Nature in Self (INS) scale43, 44. Seven images were presented with two circles, one labelled ‘Self’ and
one labelled ‘Nature’, which increasingly overlapped with each image to indicate greater nature connectedness.
Participants were asked to select the picture “that best describes your relationship with the natural environment.
How interconnected are you with nature?” with the lowest connectedness reecting no overlap between the
circles (1), and highest connectedness reecting almost totally overlapping circles (7).
Covariates. Sociodemographic controls, comparable to related studies, included: gender (female = ref;
male); age (16–29years = ref; 30–39years; 40–49years; 50–59years; 60years); highest educational achieve-
ment (degree; below degree = ref); employment status (in paid employment, in education, retired, homemaker;
not working/unemployed = ref); disposable household income quintiles (lowest quintile = ref); longstanding ill-
ness or disability (i.e. underlying health condition, yes, no = ref); relationship status (married/cohabiting; single/
separated/divorced/widowed = ref); number of adults in the household (1 = ref; 2, ≥ 3); number of children in the
household (0 = ref; 1, 2); dog ownership (yes, no = ref); car ownership (yes, no = ref); weekly days of physical
activity ≥ 30min (0 = ref, 1–4, ≥ 5); and survey wave (spring = ref, summer, autumn, winter). Of note seasons were
approximate since ‘Spring’ data were collected in June and referred to the ‘last 4weeks’ (i.e. May–June), ‘Summer’
in September (i.e. August–September), ‘Autumn’ in December (November to December), and ‘Winter’ in March
(i.e. February–March), seasons were reversed for Australia. Again, full details are available in the technical report
online51.
Analyses. Hypotheses were tested using a series of linear mixed eects models for WHO-5 scores, and
Bernoulli generalised linear mixed eects models for the binary outcome variables of mental distress and use
of medication for depression and anxiety. Models included quadratic (squared) terms for visit frequency and
connectedness to test for non-linearity (diminishing marginal returns)19. Country of residence was included
as a random intercept term to account for national-level respondent clustering. Models were tted by maxi-
mum likelihood with Laplace approximation (to integrate the random eects), and survey weights were applied
to improve national representativeness with regards to the sampling strata within each country (sex, age, and
region of residence). Analyses controlled for covariates listed above, with models for depression medication also
controlling for anxiety medication and vice versa. Each dependent variable was analysed using three models: (a)
residential exposure and covariates only, (b) residential, covariates plus recreational contact; and (c) residential,
covariates, recreational plus connectedness. is allowed us to see how the addition of recreation and connect-
edness aected residential relationships. e largest generalized variance ination factor (VIF) of any term in
any of the fully-adjusted models was VIF = 1.81, suggesting there was no substantive multi-collinearity in any
of the models. All models are presented in Supplementary TablesS2 and S3 and only the nal models includ-
ing all exposure measures are in the main text due to space constraints (Table2). e full WHO-5 model was
subsequently stratied by season and country to explore potential variation across the year and location. We did
not perform similar stratications for mental distress or medication use due to lack of power in predicting these
binary outcomes in stratied models. Analyses were performed in R v3.6.0 (R Core Team, 2019) using the ‘lme4’
package for statistical modelling60.
Data availability
All data for the BlueHealth International Survey will be made open access in 2025 in accordance with an embargo
agreement by research partners. For queries about the specic data and analysis, including r script, used in the
present manuscript please contact the corresponding author.
Received: 17 July 2020; Accepted: 31 March 2021
References
1. World Health Organization. Depression and Other Common Mental Disorders: Global Health Estimates (No. WHO/MSD/
MER/2017.2) (World Health Organization, 2017).
2. United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Aairs, Population Division. The World’s Cities in 2018: Data Booklet (ST/
ESA/ SER.A/417) (2018).
3. Penkalla, A. M. & Kohler, S. Urbanicity and mental health in Europe: A systematic review. Eur. J. Mental Health 9, 163–177 (2014).
4. Soga, M. & Gaston, K. J. Extinction of experience: e loss of human–nature interactions. Front. Ecol. Environ. 14, 94–101 (2016).
5. Hartig, T. & Kahn, P. H. Living in cities, naturally. Science 352, 938–940 (2016).
6. Bratman, G. N. et al. Daily nature and mental health: An ecosystem service perspective. Sci. Adv. 5, 0903 (2019).
7. Gascon, M., Zijlema, W., Vert, C., White, M. P. & Nieuwenhuijsen, M. J. Outdoor blue spaces, human health and well-being: A
systematic review of quantitative studies. Int. J. Hyg. Environ. Health 220, 1207–1221 (2017).
8. World Health Organisation. Urban Greenspace Interventions and Health: A Review of Impacts and Eectiveness (WHO Regional
Oce for Europe, 2017).
9. Frumkin, H. et al. Nature contact and human health: A research agenda. Environ. Health. Persp. 125, 075001–075011 (2017).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved

Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2021) 11:8903 | 
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
10. Markevych, I. et al. Exploring pathways linking greenspace to health: eoretical and methodological guidance. Environ. Res. 158,
301–317 (2017).
11. White, M. P., Pahl, S., Ashbullby, K. J., Herbert, S. & Depledge, M. H. Feelings of restoration from recent nature visits. J. Environ.
Psychol. 35, 40–51 (2013).
12. Hartig, T., Mitchell, R., De Vries, S. & Frumkin, H. Nature and health. Annu. Rev. Publ. Health 35, 207–228 (2014).
13. Bratman, G. N., Hamilton, J. P. & Daily, G. C. e impacts of nature experience on human cognitive function and mental health.
Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1249, 118–136 (2012).
14. Grahn, P. & Stigsdotter, U. A. Landscape planning and stress. Urban For. Urban Green. 2, 1–18 (2003).
15. Elliott, L. R. et al. Dening residential blue space distance categories: Modelling distance-decay eects across eighteen countries.
Landsc. Urban Plan.198, 103800 (2020).
16. Boyd, F., White, M. P., Bell, S. L. & Burt, J. Who doesn’t visit natural environments for recreation and why: A population representa-
tive analysis of spatial, individual and temporal factors among adults in England. Landsc. Urban Plan. 175, 102–113 (2018).
17. Hillsdon, M., Coombes, E., Griew, P. & Jones, A. An assessment of the relevance of the home neighbourhood for understanding
environmental inuences on physical activity: How far from home do people roam?. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. 12, 1–8 (2015).
18. Elliott, L. R., White, M. P., Taylor, A. H. & Herbert, S. Energy expenditure on recreational visits to dierent natural environments.
Soc. Sci. Med. 139, 53–60 (2015).
19. White, M. P. et al. Spending at least 120 minutes a week in nature is associated with good health and wellbeing. Sci. Rep. 91, 7730
(2019).
20. Völker, S. & Kistemann, T. e impact of blue space on human health and well-being–Salutogenetic health eects of inland surface
waters: A review. Int. J. Hyg. Environ. Health 214, 449–460 (2011).
21. Pearson, et al. Eects of freshwater blue spaces may be benecial for mental health: A rst, ecological study in the North American
Great Lakes region. PLoS ONE 14, e0221977 (2019).
22. De Vries, S., Nieuwenhuizen, W., Farjon, H., Van Hinsberg, A. & Dirkx, J. In which natural environments are people happiest?
Large-scale experience sampling in the Netherlands. Landsc. Urban Plan. 205, 103972 (2021).
23. Völker, S. & Kistemann, T. Developing the urban blue: Comparative health responses to blue and green urban open spaces in
Germany. Health Place 35, 196–205 (2015).
24. Foley, R. et al. (eds) Blue Space, Health and Wellbeing: Hydrophilia Unbounded (Routledge, 2019).
25. White, M. P., Elliott, L. R., Gascon, M., Roberts, B. & Fleming, L. E. Blue space, health and well-being: A narrative overview and
synthesis of potential benets. Environ. Res. 191, 110169 (2020).
26. Maas, J., Verheij, R. A., Groenewegen, P. P., De Vries, S. & Spreeuwenberg, P. Green space, urbanity, and health: How strong is the
relation?. J. Epidemiol. Commun. Health 60, 587–592 (2006).
27. Helbich, M., Klein, N., Roberts, H., Hagedoorn, P. & Groenewegen, P. P. More green space is related to less antidepressant prescrip-
tion rates in the Netherlands: A Bayesian geoadditive quantile regression approach. Environ. Res. 166, 290–297 (2018).
28. Helbich, M., De Beurs, D., Kwan, M. P., O’Connor, R. C. & Groenewegen, P. P. Natural environments and suicide mortality in the
Netherlands: A cross-sectional, ecological study. Lancet. Planet. Health 2, e134–e139 (2018).
29. Roe, J. & Aspinall, P. e restorative benets of walking in urban and rural settings in adults with good and poor mental health.
Health Place 17, 103–113 (2011).
30. Berman, M. G. et al. Interacting with nature improves cognition and aect for individuals with depression. J. Aect. Disord. 140,
300–305 (2012).
31. Shanahan, D. F. et al. Health benets from nature experiences depend on dose. Sci. Rep. 6, 28551 (2016).
32. McMahan, E. A. & Estes, D. e eect of contact with natural environments on positive and negative aect: A meta-analysis. J.
Pos. Psychol. 10, 507–519 (2015).
33. Capaldi, C. A., Dopko, R. L. & Zelenski, J. M. e relationship between nature connectedness and happiness: A meta-analysis.
Front. Psychol. 5, 976 (2014).
34. Zelenski, J. M. & Nisbet, E. K. Happiness and feeling connected: e distinct role of nature relatedness. Environ. Behav. 46, 3–23
(2014).
35. Martin, L. et al. Nature contact, nature connectedness and associations with health, wellbeing and pro-environmental behaviours:
Results from a nationally representative survey in England. J. Environ. Psychol. 68, 101389 (2020).
36. Elliott, L. R. et al. e eects of meteorological conditions and daylight on nature-based recreational physical activity in England.
Urban For. Urban Green. 42, 39–50 (2019).
37. Hartig, T., Catalano, R. & Ong, M. Cold summer weather, constrained restoration, and the use of antidepressants in Sweden. J.
Environ. Psychol. 27, 107–116 (2007).
38. Mitchell, R. J., Richardson, E. A., Shortt, N. K. & Pearce, J. R. Neighborhood environments and socioeconomic inequalities in
mental well-being. Am. J. Prev. Med. 49, 80–84 (2015).
39. Garrett, J. K. et al. Urban blue space and health and wellbeing in Hong Kong: Results from a survey of older adults. Health Place
55, 100–110 (2019).
40. Topp, C. W., Østergaard, S. D., Søndergaard, S. & Bech, P. e WHO-5 well-being index: A systematic review of the literature.
Psychother. Psychosom. 84, 167–176 (2015).
41. Nicolucci, A. et al. Benchmarking network for clinical and humanistic outcomes in diabetes (BENCH-D) study: Protocol, tools,
and population. Springerplus 3, 83 (2014).
42. Eurostat. European Health Interview Survey (EHIS Wave 2) Methodological manual (Publications Oce of the European Union,
2013).
43. Schultz, P. W. Assessing the structure of environmental concern: Concern for self, other people, and the biosphere. J. Environ.
Psychol. 21, 1–13 (2001).
44. Schultz, P. W. & Tabanico, J. Self, identity, and the natural environment. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 37, 1219–1247 (2007).
45. Korpela, K. M. et al. Environmental strategies of aect regulation and their associations with subjective well-being. Front. Psychol.
9, 562 (2018).
46. Tester-Jones, M. et al. Results from an 18 country cross-sectional study examining experiences of nature for people with common
mental health disorders. Sci. Rep. 10, 19408 (2020).
47. Wüstemann, H., Kalisch, D. & Kolbe, J. Accessibility of urban blue in German major cities. Ecol. Indic. 78, 125–130 (2017).
48. Richardson, M. et al. An aective measure of nature connectedness for children and adults: Validation, performance and insights.
Sustainability 11, 3250 (2019).
49. Kruize, H. et al. Exploring mechanisms underlying the relationship between the natural outdoor environment and health and
well-being—Results from the phenotype project. Environ. Int. 134, 105173 (2019).
50. Smith, G., Gidlow, C., Davey, R. & Foster, C. What is my walking neighbourhood? A pilot study of English adults’ denitions of
their local walking neighbourhoods. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. 7, 1–8 (2010).
51. Elliott, L. R. BlueHealth International Survey Methodology and Technical Report (2020). https:// doi. org/ 10. 17605/ OSF. IO/ 7AZU2.
Accessed 8 April 2021.
52. Bethlehem, J. Selection bias in web surveys. Int. Stat. Rev. 78, 161–188 (2010).
53. Van den Berg, A. E. From green space to green prescriptions: Challenges and opportunities for research and practice. Front. Psychol.
8, 268 (2017).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved

Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2021) 11:8903 | 
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
54. Otto, S. & Pensini, P. Nature-based environmental education of children: Environmental knowledge and connectedness to nature,
together, are related to ecological behaviour. Glob. Environ. Change 47, 88–94 (2017).
55. Her Majesty’s Government. A Green Future: Our 25 Year Plan to Improve the Environment (Nobel House, 2018).
56. Dai, D. Racial/ethnic and socioeconomic disparities in urban green space accessibility: Where to intervene?. Landsc. Urban Plan.
102, 234–244 (2011).
57. Poelman, H. A walk to the park? Assessing access to green areas in Europe’s cities, update using completed Copernicus urban atlas
data, No. 01/2018, European Commission, Regional and Urban policy (2018). https:// ec. europa. eu/ regio nal_ policy/ sourc es/ docge
ner/ work/ 2018_ 01_ green_ urban_ area. pdf.Accessed 8 April 2021.
58. United Nations. Sustainable development goals: Knowledge Platform. (2020). https:// susta inabl edeve lopme nt. un. org.Accessed 8
Apirl 2021.
59. Grellier, J. et al. BlueHealth: A study programme protocol for mapping and quantifying the potential benets to public health and
well-being from Europe’s blue spaces. Brit. Med. J. Open 7, e016188 (2017).
60. Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-eects models using lme4. J. Stat. Sow. 67, 1–48. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 18637/ jss. v067. i01 (2015).
Acknowledgements
We thank Ben Butler, Gavin Ellison, and Tom Powell at YouGov for managing data collection and Joanne Garrett,
Michelle Tester-Jones, Leanne Martin, Sabine Pahl, Beth Roberts, Emma Squire, Tim Taylor, and Ben Wheeler
for comments and advice on this research.
Author contributions
M.P.W.: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, Writing—original dra, Writing—review & editing,
Project administration, Funding acquisition. L.R.E.: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, Data
curation, Writing—review & editing, Visualization. J.G.: Methodology, Formal analysis, Writing—review &
editing, Visualization, Project administration. T.E.: Formal analysis, Writing—review & editing. S.B., G.N.B.,
M.N., A.O., A.R., M.L.M. and M.v.d.B.: Writing—review & editing, Funding acquisition. M.C.: Methodology
(Residential exposure). M.G.: Conceptualization, Writing—review & editing. M.L.S., S.P. and W.S.: Writing—
review & editing. L.E.F.: Writing—review & editing, Supervision, Project administration, Funding acquisition.
Funding
is project has received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme
under grant agreement No 666773 (BlueHealth). Data collection in California was supported by the Center for
Conservation Biology, Stanford University. Data collection in Canada was supported by the Faculty of Forestry,
University of British Columbia. Data collection in Finland was supported by the Natural Resources Institute
Finland (Luke). Data collection in Australia was supported by Grith University and the University of the Sun-
shine Coast. Data collection in Portugal was supported by ISCTE—University Institute of Lisbon. Data collection
in Ireland was supported by the Environmental Protection Agency, Ireland. Data collection in Hong Kong was
supported by an internal University of Exeter—Chinese University of Hong Kong international collaboration
fund. e funders had no role in the conceptualisation, design, analysis, decision to publish or preparation of
the manuscript.
Competing interests
e authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Supplementary Information e online version contains supplementary material available at https:// doi. org/
10. 1038/ s41598- 021- 87675-0.
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to M.P.W.
Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
Open Access is article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the
Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. e images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from
the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http:// creat iveco mmons. org/ licen ses/ by/4. 0/.
© e Author(s) 2021
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
Article
This study addresses the problem of locating urban green spaces. Public health policies encompass a set of activities aimed at protecting individuals’ physical integrity and well-being, where prevention plays a critical role at both individual and collective levels. Having green spaces in urban areas is essential for providing mental relaxation, stimulating social cohesion and supporting physical activity. However, deciding where to place these green spaces is challenging, as various types of information must be considered, and the impact of such decisions needs thorough evaluation and visualization. To tackle this issue, we propose a methodology that combines mathematical programming with Geographic Information Systems (GIS). GIS tools are crucial for capturing and incorporating real-world data, which supports the mathematical model and helps visualize solutions. Our proposed multi-objective model aims to maximize coverage and equity. We demonstrate the application of this methodology with a case study focused on Valencia, Spain, where 20 potential locations were identified, and five were selected.
Article
Traditional park designs no longer meet the diverse needs of young users amid rising visitor numbers and environmental challenges. Exploring the impact of mountain city parks on youth is crucial, yet localised studies on their spatial perceptions in such unique environments are lacking. Landscape design based on spatial perception evaluation offers a promising approach for renewing mountain parks to address these complex needs. Therefore, a pilot study was conducted in Chongqing’s Pipa Mountain and Eling Parks, involving questionnaire surveys and on-site spatial data collection. Using principal component analysis to select the visual and auditory indicators most related to environmental satisfaction in the overall park and various types of gathering spaces, the results showed that the first principal component of the visual environment in the entrance platform and key nodes (r = 0.41, r = 0.45), as well as the first principal component of the auditory environment in the entrance platform, path platform, and elevated points (r = 0.67, r = 0.85, r = 0.68), all showed significant positive correlations with environmental satisfaction (p < 0.01). Moreover, naturalness and aesthetics were identified as the main factors influencing environmental satisfaction. A random forest model analysed nonlinear relationships, ranking spatial factors by importance. Simultaneously, SHAP analysis highlighted the effects of key factors like elevation changes, green view index, colour diversity, and natural elements. Elevation changes were positively correlated with satisfaction at elevated points but showed a negative correlation in the overall park environment and other gathering spaces. This study explored space-perception dynamics in mountain city parks, proposing strategies to improve environmental quality in various gathering spaces and the park. These findings support creating liveable mountainous environments and guide “human-centred health,” quality enhancement, and sustainable development in renewing mountain city parks.
Article
Urban environmental settings influence human psychological states, contributing to varying mental health outcomes. This study examines the relationships between objective environmental features and psychological states at a fine scale. Using a geo-enabled survey tool, we collected data on individuals’ perceptions of their immediate environment within their daily activity space on an urban university campus. The psychological assessment included emotional and affective states such as perceived stress, fatigue, and happiness. Objective environmental properties were derived from high-resolution imagery to analyze the association between environmental settings and psychological responses. The data were analyzed using Spearman’s correlation, moderated multiple regression, and partial correlation networks. Our findings revealed that beneficial psychological states were positively associated with the quantity of natural elements in the immediate environment such as trees, water, and grass. Conversely, negative psychological states were positively associated with barren areas, parking lots, buildings, and artificial surfaces. These relationships were not significantly moderated by gender or ethnicity in our experiment. The interconnections of psychological states show distinct patterns in three different environmental settings, which are a mostly green environment, a mixed environment with green and artificial elements, and a mostly artificial environment. A difference in such interconnections between males and females has been observed. These results highlight the complex interplay between environmental features and mental state networks.
Article
Humans and nature have always been connected. Meanwhile, with the industrial revolution, landscapes have become more artificial, reducing the human–nature relationship. Urban design should follow biophilic principles to reconnect people with nature, mitigate climate change, improve air quality, restore biodiversity loss, and solve social problems. Poor air quality affects people’s health, and vegetation plays a crucial role in purifying the air. Similarly, contact with nature benefits physical and mental health and well-being. However, there is no consensus on how urban design can be beneficial for improving air quality and human health. This review paper aims to provide a comprehensive evaluation of evidence linking nature-based solutions (NBSs), air quality, carbon neutrality, and human health and well-being. Five hundred articles published between 2000 and 2024 were analysed. A number of publications studied the benefits of green infrastructure in improving air quality, carbon sequestration, or the influence of green spaces on human health. The topic of NBSs has recently emerged related to air quality, health, and promoting physical activity, as has accessibility to green spaces and mental health, also associated with blue spaces and residential gardens. The results revealed the gaps in the literature on how to design green and blue spaces to tackle environmental and public health crises simultaneously.
Article
Full-text available
Previous studies have shown that people feel happier in more natural environments than in predominantly built-up environments; however, it is less clear whether the type of natural environment matters. In a large-scale experience sampling study in the Netherlands, we explored whether happiness differs by the type of natural environment experienced. We also investigated to what extent scenic beauty, peacefulness or fascinatingness are associated with momentary happiness. Smartphone apps were developed for both iOS and Android smartphones, and made freely available in both app stores. The app, named HappyHier, sent requests to fill in a short questionnaire, starting with how happy the participant feels. The requests were programmed to oversample experiences in natural environments. Location data were provided by the GPS of the smartphone, and the type of environment was determined based on a land-use map incorporated in the app. HappyHier was launched with a media campaign starting on 1st May 2016. In the following few months, over 4000 people participated, generating over 100,000 experience samples. Multi-level analyses were conducted, controlling for, among other things, being inside or outside, type of activity, type of company and weather conditions. The participants generally felt happier in natural environments, especially at the coast and in areas with low-lying natural vegetation, such as heathlands. Whether the environment is thought to be peaceful and fascinating appears to be more important for happiness than its scenic beauty. The representativeness of the data gathered by this relatively new method was explored from several angles: people, time and location.
Article
Full-text available
Exposure to natural environments is associated with a lower risk of common mental health disorders (CMDs), such as depression and anxiety, but we know little about nature-related motivations, practices and experiences of those already experiencing CMDs. We used data from an 18-country survey to explore these issues (n = 18,838), taking self-reported doctor-prescribed medication for depression and/or anxiety as an indicator of a CMD (n = 2698, 14%). Intrinsic motivation for visiting nature was high for all, though slightly lower for those with CMDs. Most individuals with a CMD reported visiting nature ≥ once a week. Although perceived social pressure to visit nature was associated with higher visit likelihood, it was also associated with lower intrinsic motivation, lower visit happiness and higher visit anxiety. Individuals with CMDs seem to be using nature for self-management, but ‘green prescription’ programmes need to be sensitive, and avoid undermining intrinsic motivation and nature-based experiences.
Article
Full-text available
Research into the potential health and well-being benefits from exposure to green spaces such as parks and woodlands has led to the development of several frameworks linking the different strands of evidence. The current paper builds on these to provide a model of how exposure to aquatic environments, or blue spaces such as rivers, lakes and the coast, in particular, may benefit health and well-being. Although green and blue spaces share many commonalities, there are also important differences. Given the breadth of the research, spanning multiple disciplines and research methodologies, a narrative review approach was adopted which aimed to highlight key issues and processes rather than provide a definitive balance of evidence summary. Novel aspects of our framework included the inclusion of outcomes that are only indirectly good for health through being good for the environment, the addition of nature connectedness as both a trait and state, and feedback loops where actions/interventions to increase exposure are implemented. Limitations of the review and areas for future work, including the need to integrate potential benefits with potential risks, are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Varied categorisations of residential distance to bluespace in population health studies make comparisons difficult. Using survey data from eighteen countries, we modelled relationships between residential distance to blue spaces (coasts, lakes, and rivers), and self-reported recreational visits to these environments at least weekly, with penalised regression splines. We observed exponential declines in visit probability with increasing distance to all three environments and demonstrated the utility of derived categorisations. These categories may be broadly applicable in future research where the assumed underlying mechanism between residential distance to a blue space and a health outcome is direct recreational contact.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Despite the large number of studies on beneficial effects of the natural outdoor environment (NOE) on health, the underlying mechanisms are not fully understood. Objective: This study explored the relations between amount, quality, use and experience of the NOE; and physical activity, social contacts and mental well-being. Methods: In this cross-sectional study, data on GIS-derived measures of residential surrounding greenness (NDVI), NOE within 300 m, and audit data on quality of the streetscape were combined with questionnaire data from 3947 adults in four European cities. These included time spent in NOE (use); and perceived greenness, and satisfaction with and importance given to the NOE (experience). Physical activity, social contacts and mental health were selected as key outcome indicators. Descriptive and multilevel analyses were conducted both on pooled data and for individual cities. Results: More minutes spent in the NOE were associated with more minutes of physical activity, a higher frequency of social contacts with neighbors, and better mental well-being. Perceived greenness, satisfaction with and importance of the NOE, were other strong predictors of the outcomes, while GIS measures of NOE and streetscape quality were not. We found clear differences between the four cities. Conclusions: Use and experience of the natural outdoor environment are important predictors for beneficial effects of the natural outdoor environment and health. Future research should focus more on these aspects to further increase our understanding of these mechanisms, and needs to take the local context into account.
Article
Full-text available
Research linking green space and mental health abounds. It also appears that oceanic blue spaces may be salutogenic, benefitting mental health through their expansive viewscapes, and possibly auditory and olfactory stimuli. Yet, it is unknown whether the same is true for freshwater bodies. In this ecological study, we explored associations between hospitalizations for anxiety/mood disorder in Michigan (>30,000) and proximity to the North American Great Lakes. As a sensitivity analysis, we examined associations for 15 different inland lake sizes. Results showed small, protective effects for distance to Great Lake (β = 0.06, p<0.001) and percentage of inland lakes (β = -0.04, p = 0.004). Unexpectedly, shorter distance to nearest inland lake was associated with higher anxiety/mood disorder hospitalizations. The protective effects of percentage area covered by inland lakes was observed for all lake sizes. These initial findings provide a foundation for future individual-level research with finer measurement of health outcomes and blue space exposure.
Article
Full-text available
A growing body of empirical evidence is revealing the value of nature experience for mental health. With rapid urbanization and declines in human contact with nature globally, crucial decisions must be made about how to preserve and enhance opportunities for nature experience. Here, we first provide points of consensus across the natural, social, and health sciences on the impacts of nature experience on cognitive functioning, emotional well-being, and other dimensions of mental health. We then show how ecosystem service assessments can be expanded to include mental health, and provide a heuristic, conceptual model for doing so.
Article
Full-text available
Spending time in natural environments can benefit health and well-being, but exposure-response relationships are under-researched. We examined associations between recreational nature contact in the last seven days and self-reported health and well-being. Participants (n = 19,806) were drawn from the Monitor of Engagement with the Natural Environment Survey (2014/15–2015/16); weighted to be nationally representative. Weekly contact was categorised using 60 min blocks. Analyses controlled for residential greenspace and other neighbourhood and individual factors. Compared to no nature contact last week, the likelihood of reporting good health or high well-being became significantly greater with contact ≥120 mins (e.g. 120–179 mins: ORs [95%CIs]: Health = 1.59 [1.31–1.92]; Well-being = 1.23 [1.08–1.40]). Positive associations peaked between 200–300 mins per week with no further gain. The pattern was consistent across key groups including older adults and those with long-term health issues. It did not matter how 120 mins of contact a week was achieved (e.g. one long vs. several shorter visits/week). Prospective longitudinal and intervention studies are a critical next step in developing possible weekly nature exposure guidelines comparable to those for physical activity.
Article
Full-text available
With benefits to both human well-being and pro-nature conservation behaviors, nature connectedness is emerging as an important psychological construct for a sustainable future. The growing research and applied and policy-related interests require a straightforward measure of nature connectedness that is suitable for both children and adult populations. To establish the reliability of the new Nature Connection Index (NCI) three factor analyses were conducted. One was based on a large Monitor of Engagement with the Natural Environment (MENE) dataset for adults (n = 3568) with a replication from data sets collected online (n = 553), and a third used MENE data from children (n = 351). To validate the NCI as a measure for nature connectedness an online comparison study (n = 153) included the NCI alongside other established measures. The results showed that the NCI was a reliable and valid scale that offers a short, simple alternative to other measures of nature connectedness, particularly for populations including both children and adults, measured face to face or online. The utility of the NCI is also supported, with variations associated with various pro-environmental and pro-conservation behaviors observed, and importantly the NCI also revealed changes in nature connectedness across the lifespan.
Article
Contact with, and psychological connectedness to the natural world are both associated with various health and sustainability-related outcomes. To date, though, the evidence base has been fragmented. Using a representative sample of the adult population of England (N = 4,960), we investigated the relationships between three types of nature contact, psychological connectedness, health, subjective wellbeing and pro-environmental behaviours within a single study. We found that specific types of nature contact, as well as individual differences in nature connectedness, were differentially associated with aspects of health, well-being and pro-environmental behaviours. Living in a greener neighbourhood was, unrelated to any wellbeing or sustainability outcomes. By contrast, visiting nature ≥ once a week was positively associated with general health and household pro-environmental behaviours. Moreover, people who watched/listened to nature documentaries reported higher levels of both pro-environmental behaviours. Nature connectedness was positively related to eudaimonic wellbeing and both types of pro-environmental behaviour. Moreover, connectedness moderated key relationships between nature contact, wellbeing and pro-environmental behaviours. The complexity of our findings suggests that interventions increasing both contact with, and connection to nature, are likely to be needed in order to achieve synergistic improvements to human and planetary health.