Content uploaded by Michael Poulin
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Michael Poulin on Apr 19, 2021
Content may be subject to copyright.
Minding Your Own Business? Mindfulness Decreases Prosocial Behavior for those with
Independent Self-Construals
Michael J. Poulin1
Lauren M. Ministero2
Shira Gabriel1
C. Dale Morrison1
Esha Naidu1
1Department of Psychology, University at Buffalo
2MITRE Corporation†
Corresponding author:
Michael J. Poulin
mjpoulin@buffalo.edu
Department of Psychology
College of Arts and Sciences
University at Buffalo
204 Park Hall, North Campus
Buffalo, NY 14260-4110
†The author's affiliation with The MITRE Corporation is provided for identification purposes only, and is not intended to convey
or imply MITRE's concurrence with, or support for, the positions, opinions, or viewpoints expressed by the author.
©2021 The MITRE Corporation. ALL RIGHTS RESERVED.
Approved for Public Release; Distribution Unlimited. Public Release Case Number 21-0869.
Abstract
Mindfulness appears to promote individual well-being, but its interpersonal effects are
less clear. Two studies in adult populations tested whether the effects of mindfulness on
prosocial behavior differ by self-construals. In Study 1 (N = 366), a brief mindfulness induction,
compared to a meditation control, led to decreased prosocial behavior among people with
relatively independent self-construals, but had the opposite effect among those with relatively
interdependent self-construals. In Study 2 (N = 325), a mindfulness induction led to decreased
prosocial behavior among those primed with independence, but had the opposite effect among
those primed with interdependence. The effects of mindfulness on prosocial behavior appear to
depend on individuals' broader social goals. This may have implications for the increasing
popularity of mindfulness training around the world.
Statement of Relevance
Mindfulness is big business. The U.S. meditation market, which includes classes, studios,
and apps, is predicted to grow to over $2 billion by 2022. Employers, health care settings,
schools, and even prisons are increasingly urging mindfulness practice, with over one in five
employers currently offering mindfulness training. By some accounts, being mindful increases
prosocial behavior. In samples of college students, we tested whether this was inevitably the
case. We found that for people who tend to view themselves as more interdependent,
mindfulness increased prosocial actions. However, for people who tend to view themselves as
more independent, mindfulness actually decreased prosocial behavior. Importantly, simple
manipulations impacted this trend and made prosocial behavior more likely, even for people who
view themselves as more independent of others. By providing a better understanding of how and
when mindfulness affects prosocial behavior, this research allows us to make more informed
decisions about the conditions under which mindfulness practices promote positive outcomes for
individuals and for society.
Minding Your Own Business? Mindfulness Decreases Prosocial Behavior for those with
Independent Self-Construals
Research on mindfulness—nonjudgmental monitoring of moment-by-moment cognition,
emotion, perception, and sensation without fixation on thoughts of past and future (Kabat-Zinn
1990; Lutz et al. 2008) suggests that it improves individual well-being. Whether manipulated by
meditation or assessed as a trait, mindfulness predicts positive outcomes including stress
reduction (Chiesa and Serretti 2009), reduced negative affect and increased hope (Sears & Kraus,
2009), and increased subjective well-being (Keng et al., 2011). But what about the prosocial
effects of mindfulness? Does mindfulness make people more generous and cooperative, or is it
possible that it can actually make people more selfish? We propose that the prosocial effects of
mindfulness depend greatly on whether one sees the self as independent from or as
interdependent with other individuals—and that accordingly, mindfulness may actually reduce
prosocial behavior among independent-minded individuals.
Mindfulness and Prosociality
Mindfulness is a trait: some individuals tend to be mindful more thoroughly and more
often than others. It is also a state: that is, at any given time, it is possible for an individual to be
more or less engaged in nonjudgmental awareness of their present thoughts and experiences.
Accordingly, mindfulness can be affected by experiences, including laboratory inductions, and
by meditation practices known collectively as mindfulness meditation (Kabat-Zinn 1990; Lutz et
al., 2008).
There are cultural ties between mindfulness and prosocial behavior. As a traditional
component of Buddhist religious practice it is thought to play a role in promoting moral behavior
(Norenzayan, 2013). Modern practitioners share this view. For example, the Dalai Lama has
been quoted as saying, "If every eight year old in the world is taught meditation, we will
eliminate violence from the world within one generation." (quoted in Kreplin, Farias, & Brazil,
2018). Some empirical research supports the notion that mindfulness promotes prosociality,
including increased empathy and prosocial behavior (e.g., Hafenbrack et al., 2020; Lim, Condon,
& DeSteno, 2015; Lutz et al., 2008) and decreased ostracism (e.g., Jones, Wirth, Ramsey, &
Wynsma, 2019). However, there is also evidence that mindfulness may not straightforwardly
promote prosocial behavior. At least one rigorous study has found that mindfulness can decrease
motivation, including motivation to perform a task on behalf of others (Hafenbrack & Vohs,
2018). In addition, a recent meta-analysis found that the effects of various kinds of meditation on
prosocial behavior were limited to methodologically-weak studies (Kreplin et al., 2018), and a
subsequent meta-analysis of more methodologically-rigorous studies found that mindfulness
affected only certain types of prosocial behavior (Berry et al., 2020). In other words, there is still
much to learn about when—or whether—mindfulness makes people more likely to help others.
The Role of Self-Construal
We propose that understanding the complex effects of mindfulness on prosocial behavior
requires a recognition that mindfulness originated in societies that were highly interdependent in
self-construal, but mindfulness in the West, including the U.S., is practiced in a context of
predominantly independent self-construals (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). An independent self-
construal is a conception of the self as separate from others. The self is seen as singular and
ideas about who one is come from comparing the self to others (e.g. I am smart, outgoing, and
funny). Conversely, an interdependent self-construal is a conception of self that situates the self
in an interpersonal context (e.g., "I am a woman, African American, a daughter, a sister, a
college freshman, a feminist”). These contexts can include close relationship partners (a
relational interdependent self-construal) and/or collectives such as groups or nationalities (a
collective interdependent self-construal; Brewer & Gardner, 1996; Gardner, Gabriel, & Lee,
1999). Self-construal is one of the most robust and reliable predictors of social goals and
behaviors (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Individuals with independent self-construals tend to act
in ways that are consistent with goals of autonomy, separateness, and self-maximization;
whereas those with interdependent self-construals tend to value the well-being of other group
members, relationships, and interpersonal harmony (Gardner et al., 1999; Holland et al., 2004).
The emphasis of Western cultures on individualism and Eastern cultures on collectivism
has led people from Western cultures to have more salient, active, and easily accessible
independent self-construals as compared to people from Eastern cultures (Gardner et al., 1999).
However, all people have both independent and interdependent aspects of self that can be
activated by situation, including by experimental primes (Brewer & Gardner, 1996; Gardner et
al., 1999). Thus self-construal is both a trait and a state. Those accessible goals then influence
decisions and behaviors (Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Verplanken & Holland, 2002).
Mindfulness and the Self. Mindfulness could amplify the effects of self-construals on
prosocial behavior by enhancing self-awareness. Although mindfulness is often discussed as
decreasing ego-involvement (e.g., Brown, Berry, & Quaglia, 2016; Farb et al., 2007) and self-
referential processing (Shi & He, 2019), some research and theorizing links mindfulness to
increased attention to, and awareness of, the self. Specifically, cultivating awareness of the self
is a central goal of mindfulness (Hanh, 1999), and research suggests a positive relationship
between mindfulness and self-referential processing (e.g., Berkovich-Ohana et al., 2012) as well
as self-enhancement (Gebauer et al., 2018; CITE in press). There is also some evidence that
mindfulness is associated with greater self-concept clarity (Hanley & Garland, 2017). Thus,
although it is premature to definitively state the exact relationship between mindfulness and self,
much research suggests that mindfulness increases self-awareness, suggesting that self-relevant
goals should be particularly salient when people are mindful.
The Present Research
We hypothesized that the effects of mindfulness would differ depending on self-
construal, such that mindfulness would predict increased prosocial behavior in the context of an
interdependent self-construal, but decrease it in the presence of an independent self-construal. To
test this, we examined existing data from one study and conducted one new study. Both of these
studies examined a previously-validated manipulation of mindfulness, and between the two
studies we examined both trait self-construal (Study 1) as well as primed self-construal (Study
2).
STUDY 1
Method
Participants
Participants (N = 366) were a sample of convenience drawn from the undergraduate
research pool at the University at Buffalo and received course credit for participating in this
study. Study 1 was originally designed to examine the effects of mindfulness meditation on
prosocial behavior, but all participants in this study had also completed measures of self-
construals during beginning-of-semester mass testing. This allowed us to test the hypothesis that
the effects of mindfulness would be moderated by self-construal on a post-hoc basis.
The mean age in the sample was 19.00 (SD = 1.85), with 53% identifying as female, 46%
as male, and 1% as transgender or other. A majority (53%) of participants were White, while
10% were Black, 27% were Asian or Asian American, while 10% were mixed race or other.
Across racial groups, 7% of participants identified as Hispanic. We had targeted an N of 400 in
order to have the power to detect a small interaction effect (full details about power are in
Supplemental Materials; all study data and supplemental materials are available online at
https://osf.io/3ahkf). A total of 416 individuals participated in the study, of whom 366 had
completed the pre-study measure of self-construals, resulting in the final N = 366. Procedures for
this and subsequent studies were approved by the IRB of the University at Buffalo.
Procedure
Participants came to the lab one at a time for a study on meditation and the self.
Participants first completed measures of personality, trust, and prosocial tendencies that were not
the focus of this investigation and then were randomly assigned to a meditation condition: either
mindfulness meditation or a meditation control (mind wandering).
Meditation Manipulation.
Mindfulness meditation. Participants in the mindfulness meditation condition listened to
instructions to induce mindfulness via mindful breathing (Kiken & Shook, 2011). The
mindfulness meditation procedures draw from existing conceptualizations of mindfulness,
involving a focus and regulation of attention and awareness toward experiences in the present
moment (Brown & Ryan, 2003). These procedures focus on developing mindful breathing to
induce state mindfulness (originally adapted from Arch & Craske, 2006). This condition
included prompts such as “Start by bringing your attention to your belly and chest – wherever
you feel your breath moving in your torso – feel this area rise or expand gently as you breathe in,
and then feel it fall or draw back as you breathe out. Then continue to observe the feelings of
each breath in and out, without trying to control your breathing if you can.”
Meditation control. Previous research suggests that inactive control groups (such as
assigning participants to receive mindfulness training later, a “waitlist” condition) may conflate
any effects of mindfulness with attentional instructions as well as preconceived thoughts about
the benefits of meditation (Berry et al., 2020; Kreplin, Farias, & Brazil, 2018; Zeidan, Johnson,
Gordon, & Goolkasian, 2010). Therefore, we utilized a meditation control procedure that still is
presented as "meditation" but does not increase state mindfulness, unlike the mindful breathing
instructions (Kiken & Shook, 2011).
Participants in the meditation control condition listened to instructions on unfocused
attention, or mind wandering (also originally adapted from Arch & Craske, 2006). As in the
mindfulness condition, the instructions were labeled as "meditation" instructions and encouraged
participants to sit quietly, with their eyes closed if they chose. However, the instructions directed
participants toward a neutral state that did not involve a focus on the present moment or the
present experience of the breath. This condition included prompts such as, “Use the time to let
your mind wander and think freely without needing to focus hard on anything in particular.”
Both the mindful breathing and meditation control instructions were presented over the course of
a 15-minute meditation period.
Charity procedure. Following the meditation manipulation, participants read an article
from a local newspaper, ostensibly randomly chosen to assess how meditation affects
information processing. All participants actually read about a regional charity that offers
assistance to rural poor and homeless people. Following this, participants completed a measure
of compassion and were then presented with a letter, supposedly from the lab director, noting
that some participants had seen this article and expressed an interest in helping. Participants
were told that the university had arranged to send letters to alumni requesting their financial
support for this charity, but that the university could use the help of students to stuff envelopes.
Participants were offered the chance to do so, while being assured that their decision would have
no bearing on their credit for the study. Participants who offered to stuff envelopes were given
materials to do so, and were left alone to complete that task for as long as they wished. When
they notified an assistant that they were ready to leave, they were debriefed and thanked for their
participation. During the debriefing procedure, 13 participants (4%) expressed some degree of
suspicion about the purpose of the envelope stuffing task. Excluding these participants did not
change the reported results. Their data are included.
Measures
Self-Construal. Self-construal was assessed as both relational interdependent self-
construal using the Relational Interdependent Self Construal scale (Cross, Bacon, & Morris,
2000) and collective interdependent self-construal using the Collective Interdependent Self
Construal scale (Gabriel & Gardner, 1999). Example items for relational self-construal included,
"My close relationships are an important reflection of who I am," and "When I think of myself, I
often think of my close friends or family also." Example items for collective self-construal
included, "The groups I belong to are an important reflection of who I am," and "When I think of
myself, I often think of groups I belong to as well." An independent self-construal was thus
operationalized as low levels of both types of interdependent self-construal (Brewer & Gardner,
1996). Both scales exhibited very good internal consistency (relational: = .86; collective: =
.88).
Compassion. Cameron and Payne’s compassion scale (2011) includes nine items rated
on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 7 (extremely) measuring feelings of sympathy, warmth, and
compassion, as well as questions about perceived importance and appropriateness of helping.
Example items included "How compassionate do you feel toward the people in the article you
read?" and "How much do you value the welfare of the people in the article you read?" This
scale had excellent internal consistency ( = .93).
Prosocial Behavior. The number of envelopes participants stuffed, which has been used
often in research on prosocial outcomes (cf. Batson, 2011), was used as a measure of prosocial
behavior.
Results
Most of the 366 participants (84%) decided to stuff envelopes, with the number of
envelopes actually stuffed ranging from 1 to 158. However, given that a substantial number of
participants (16%) declined to stuff envelopes (i.e., stuffing 0 envelopes) this variable was
potentially appropriate for zero-inflated Poisson regression (Cameron & Trivedi, 2009).
Screening the data using this procedure revealed that no predictors were significant in zero-
inflated portion of the model, so analyses of this count variable proceeded using just Poisson
regression. Descriptive statistics and correlations for assessed variables are in Table 1.
Table 1
Study 1: Descriptive Statistics and Correlations for Assessed Variables (N = 366)
r
Variable
M (SD)
Mindfulness
manipulation
Relational
self-construal
Collective
self-construal
Compassion
Envelopes stuffed
36.77 (33.04)
-.01
.07
.06
.20***
Mindfulness manipulation
0.50 (0.50)
--
.03
-.04
.002
Relational self-construal
5.02 (0.61)
--
--
.55***
.23***
Collective self-construal
4.70 (0.68)
--
--
--
.16**
Compassion
5.18 (1.09)
--
--
--
--
** p < .01, *** p < .001
A Poisson regression analysis predicting envelopes stuffed (the DV) from meditation
condition, relational self-construal, and collective self-construal as well as their two- and three-
way interactions indicated the presence of a three-way interaction among these variables (b =
0.05, 95% CI [0.02, 0.07], z = 3.91, p < .001, = .28). In order to interpret this, we centered
both self-construal variables at low (M - 1 SD) values, which allowed us to examine the simple
effect of mindfulness among individuals with a relatively independent self-construal. This
analysis indicated that mindfulness (n = 178) (vs. meditation control; n = 188) led to fewer
envelopes being stuffed among individuals with relatively independent self-construals (b = -0.15,
95% CI [-0.20, -0.10], z = -5.64, p < .001, = .25, IRR = 0.85). By contrast, centering both self-
construal variables at high (M + 1 SD) values allowed us to examine the effect of mindfulness
among individuals with a relatively interdependent self-construal, and indicated that mindfulness
increased the number of envelopes stuffed among those with relatively interdependent self-
construals (b = 0.17, 95% CI [0.12, 0.22], z = 6.91, p < .001, = .22, IRR = 1.17). The incidence
rate ratios (IRRs) for these effects indicated that mindfulness led to a 15% decrease in the rate of
stuffing envelopes for those with independent self construals, but to a 17% increase in the rate of
stuffing envelopes among those with interdependent self-construals. These effects are illustrated
in Figure 1. Further analyses also examined the effect of mindfulness on envelopes stuffed when
individuals were high in relational but low in collective self-construal or vice versa. Mindfulness
decreased the number of envelopes stuffed for those high in relational but low in collective self-
construal (b = -0.14, z = -3.15, p = .002), but had no significant effect for those low in relational
but high in collective self-construal (b = -0.03, z = -0.64, p = .522). There was no significant
main or interaction effect of mindfulness predicting compassion (ps > .60).
Consistent with our predictions, mindfulness led to decreased prosocial behavior among
those with relatively independent self-construals. It also led to increased prosocial behavior
among individuals with both interdependent relational and collective self-construals.
Figure 1. Effects of mindfulness meditation versus meditation control on prosocial behavior in
the form of stuffing envelopes among those with relatively independent versus interdependent
self-construals (both ps < .001). Error bars represent standard error of the estimate of model-
estimated values.
STUDY 2
Method
Participants
30
32
34
36
38
40
42
44
46
Meditation Control Mindfulness
Meditation
Envelopes
Independent
30
32
34
36
38
40
42
44
46
Meditation Control Mindfulness
Meditation
Envelopes
Interdependent
Study 2 was designed to manipulate both mindfulness and salience of independent versus
interdependent self-construals. Although this study was designed to be run in the lab during
Spring 2020, the COVID-19 crisis prevented that. Instead, it was modified to run as an online
study, and participants were recruited separately from students in the undergraduate research
pools at the University at Buffalo during the Spring and Summer academic terms. No
participants had previously participated in Study 1. Mean age in this combined sample (N = 325)
was 19.16 (SD = 1.38), with 30% identifying as female, 70% as male, and <1% as transgender or
other. A majority (55%) of participants were White, while 11% were Black, 24% were Asian or
Asian American, while 10% were mixed race or other. Across racial groups, 11% of participants
identified as Hispanic. As in Study 1, we targeted an N of 400 but were only able to recruit an N
of 333 due to Covid-19 altering data collection. Also consistent with the challenges of
conducting this study online, eight participants reported not being able to comply with the
instruction to be in a quiet, distraction-free location, which we believed would undermine the
validity of the meditation manipulation. Omitting these participants resulted in the final N = 325.
Procedure
The Study 2 procedure was exactly the same as in Study 1 with the primary exception
that, before the mindfulness manipulation, participants were randomly assigned to a pronoun
selection task that primed either independence or interdependence. There were two additional
differences from Study 1 due to the online design. First, participants were instructed to make
sure they wore headphones and to find a quiet place where they could be alone to participate in
the meditation manipulation. Second, rather than being asked to stuff envelopes to contact
donors, participants were told that they could sign up for time slots to chat online with potential
alumni donors to request their financial support for the charity. Participants were asked to give
their contact information, so that an organizer could give them further details about volunteering.
Thus, similarly to the in-person measure of Study 1, this online measure of prosocial behavior
required the participant to make a non-private and specific time commitment in which to
volunteer to raise money for a charity. Previous research has used willingness to contact alumni
donors as an indicator of motivation to engage in prosocial behavior (Grant et al., 2007). The
planned dependent variable was the number of hours signed up for.
Self-Construal Manipulation. Independent versus interdependent self-construals were
primed via a pronoun selection task. Specifically, participants completed the “pronoun circling
task” (Brewer & Gardner, 1996) modified for online administration. Participants were provided
with a short first-person paragraph about a trip to the city. The paragraph was either written in
the singular (e.g. "I went to the city") or the plural (e.g. "we went to the city"). Participants were
asked to click on all of the pronouns as they read through the paragraph. Previous research
suggests that this prime activates self-construal and related goals (Gabriel et al., 1999), and a
subsequent meta-analysis has found that it leads to differences in self-construal and related
differences in values, relationality, and cognition consistent in direction and size with cross-
national effects (Oyserman & Lee, 2008).
Compassion. As in Study 1, Cameron and Payne’s compassion scale (2011) assessed
compassion ( = .93).
Prosocial Behavior. Participants were given the opportunity to sign up for multiple hour-
long blocks over the course of a week to volunteer. The total number of hours was originally
designed to be an indicator of prosocial behavior, but given that most students actually did not
volunteer at all (see below), whether participants signed up to volunteer (for any hours) or not
was ultimately used to assess prosocial behavior.
Self-construal manipulation check. To ensure that the self-construal manipulation
functioned as intended, at the end of the survey participants were asked to complete the Twenty
Statements Task (TST). The TST was developed to provide a standardized method for assessing
the self-concept. In the task, participants are pretested with 20 word stems that begin “I am” and
they are directed to complete those word stems to describe themselves. These statements were
used to assess differences in interdependent versus independent self-construal by coding whether
the sentences reflected the independent self (e.g. I am smart) or the interdependent self (e.g. I am
a member of my family). The TST has since become one of the most commonly used methods
of assessing differences in self-construal (for a review, see Oyserman & Lee, 2008).
Results
Consistent with past research (Gabriel et al., 1999; Oyserman & Lee, 2008), the self-
construal manipulation affected participants' levels of independent cognition, with those in the
independent condition using significantly more independent statements (M = 13.52, SD = 0.41)
than those in the interdependent condition (M = 12.14, SD = 0.44, t[307] = 2.30, p =
.022). Unlike in Study 1, the large majority (70%) of our 325 participants declined to volunteer
(range of hours: 0 – 34, M = 2.02, SD = 4.55), owing perhaps to the increased time commitment
required outside of a lab, or to the online and therefore more anonymous format. We nonetheless
screened the data using zero-inflated Poisson regression. This procedure revealed no significant
predictors within the Poisson component of the model, so analyses proceeded using volunteering
as a dichotomous (yes/no) variable. A logistic regression with robust standard errors to account
for clustering between different terms (spring and summer) was used to predict volunteering
from dummy codes corresponding to meditation condition, self-construal priming condition, and
their interaction. This analysis revealed a significant interaction (b = -0.60, 95% CI [-0.66, -
0.55], z = -21.02, p < .001, OR = 0.54, 95% CI [0.52, 0.58]). Recentering showed that
mindfulness led to reduced likelihood of volunteering among those primed with independence (b
= -0.27, 95% CI [-0.50, -0.03], z = -2.19, p = .028, OR = 0.77, 95% CI[0.60, 0.97]) but increased
volunteering among those primed with interdependence (b = 0.34, 95% CI [0.16, 0.52], z = 3.64,
p < .001, OR = 1.40, 95% CI [1.17, 1.68]). The odds ratios (ORs) for these effects indicate that
mindfulness led to a 33% decrease in the odds of volunteering among those primed with
independence, but a 40% increase in the odds of volunteering among those primed with
interdependence. This pattern is illustrated in Figure 2. There were no main or interactive effects
of mindfulness on compassion (ps > .23).
Figure 2. Percentage of participants within each condition who volunteered. Conditions were
mindfulness meditation versus meditation control crossed with an independence prime versus an
interdependence prime. Mindfulness (n = 88) versus control (n = 76) decreased volunteering
among those primed with independence (p = .03,) but mindfulness (n = 79) increased
volunteering versus control (n = 82) among those primed with interdependence (p < .001).
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
45
50
Independent Interdependent
% Volunteering
Meditation Control Mindfulness
Independence (versus interdependence) decreased volunteering in the mindfulness condition (p <
.001), but only marginally in the control condition (p = .09). Error bars represent standard error
of the estimate of model-estimated values.
The results of this online study are consistent with our predictions: mindfulness decreased
prosocial behavior when participants were primed with independence. By contrast, mindfulness
increased prosocial behavior when participants were primed with interdependence.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
In two studies, we found support for the hypothesis that the effects of mindfulness on
prosocial behavior would be moderated by self-construal. Specifically, mindfulness led to
decreased prosocial behavior for individuals with independent self-construals, but increased
prosocial behavior for those with interdependent self-construals. These findings were present for
both trait and manipulated self-construals. We believe these findings shed light on the
association between mindfulness and prosocial behavior, and have practical implications for
mindfulness training, especially in individualistic societies.
The present research provides a stringent test of the effects of mindfulness on prosocial
behavior in that both studies employed the use of a mindfulness manipulation with an active,
rather than an inactive, control. Research suggests that the use of an active control is vital for
controlling for placebo effects of meditation—that is, existing beliefs that meditation makes
people “better,” more calm, and more compassionate (Berry et al., 2020; Zeidan et al., 2010).
Thus, these studies provide a strong test of the effects of mindfulness meditation versus another
task labeled as meditation, reflecting a transition toward more carefully matched mindfulness
intervention conditions.
What Mindfulness Is, and Isn't
Our findings suggest that mindfulness on its own is not prosocial—or antisocial. To
reiterate a typical definition of mindfulness, it consists of non-judgmental attention to thoughts
and sensations in the present moment (Kabat-Zinn 1990; Lutz et al. 2008). This conception of
mindfulness is value-free, with no prosocial or even social content at all. Despite the lack of a
clear theoretical link between mindfulness and prosociality, practitioners and researchers have
speculated that there is something inherently prosocial about mindfulness—for example, that it
bolsters empathy (Berry et al., 2020; Lim et al., 2015). However, we did not find associations
between mindfulness and the related construct of compassion. Moreover, our finding that the
prosocial effects of mindfulness are wholly moderated by self-construal, such that mindfulness
can actually decrease prosocial behavior, calls this conceptualization into question. Our results
are much more consistent with the possibility that mindfulness bolsters self-awareness, or
another general mechanism that leads people to act on whatever social goals are salient. For
some, those goals may be consistent with prosocial behavior, but for others they are not.
Buddhist monk and humanitarian Matthieu Ricard endorsed this view, noting that a successful
sniper embodies a certain type of mindfulness (Ricard, 2009), and writing that, "[b]are attention,
as consummate as it might be, is no more than a tool [that] can also be used to cause immense
suffering" (for another example of military uses of mindfulness, see also Jha et al., 2015).
Mindfulness in Context
If mindfulness is merely a tool, then understanding its effects requires consideration of
the context in which it is practiced or experienced. Mindfulness practices originated in Buddhist
religious traditions with accompanying ethical precepts, worldviews, and social practices
(Purser, 2019). Notably, in light of the current findings, mindfulness also originated in societies
that were and are highly interdependent (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). In such societies,
mindfulness could foster prosocial behavior, as mindfulness serves to activate and promote
representations of the self as connected to close others and to larger social groups. By contrast,
people in the U.S. and other Western nations experience mindfulness in societies that greatly
value independence. Moreover, modern mindfulness training is secular by design and frequently
focused on individual goals and well-being (Kucinskas, 2018; Purser, 2019), thus lacking
(pro)social content. Our findings suggest that mindfulness, practiced outside of the context of
interdependence, may not provide prosocial benefits, and may in fact have the opposite effect. In
fact, data from one of our studies (Study 1) suggests that positive effects of mindfulness on
prosocial behavior may be limited to individuals who view themselves as high in both relational
and collective interdependence. However, it is crucial to note that even those with relatively
independent self-construals in independent societies can adopt interdependent goals: our Study 2
results demonstrate that priming interdependence can reverse antisocial effects of mindfulness.
Although our research focused on prosocial behavior, and not on general effects of
mindfulness, our findings may be relevant to the debate about mindfulness and self-focus (cf.
Brown et al., 2016; Gebauer et al., 2018). Specifically, accounting for self-construals may help
resolve the apparent contradiction between mindfulness as boosting self-awareness versus
enabling self-transcendence. That is, increased attention to an independent self may mean
something quite different from increased attention to a, more expansive, interdependent self.
Limitations and Future Directions
The present work draws from literature suggesting that mindfulness increases self-
awareness, but this link was not assessed in the present research. Additional research should test
the role of this possible mechanism. Given that past research indicates that mindfulness may
specifically motivate prosocial behavior directly in response to a person’s suffering (Berry et al.,
2020), another limitation of our research is that we focused specifically on donations to and
soliciting donations on behalf of strangers. On the other hand, these behaviors were assessed
following stories linking donations to suffering others. Moreover, the fact that one of our studies
(Study 1) assessed actual physical helping behaviors (stuffing envelopes) is a methodological
strength that suggests generalizability to real world contexts. Another limitation related to our
assessments of prosocial behavior is that we were not able to assess suspicion for the online
Study 2, so we cannot address how much the observed results reflect demand characteristics.
However, Study 2 was very similar in content to Study 1, for which suspicion did not affect the
observed results.
Future research should further test the generalizability of the present research. For
example, research can and should examine non-Western samples and different age groups.
Results may also differ with sustained rather than very brief mindfulness training.
Implications
The present research suggests that understanding the social effects of mindfulness
requires attention to the role of self-construals, which differ across cultures and among
individuals. By better understanding how and when mindfulness affects prosocial behavior,
stakeholders can make more informed decisions about whether—and under what conditions—
mindfulness practices are appropriate. Additionally, our results point towards ways to modify
mindfulness interventions, perhaps by incorporating a focus on interdependence, to promote the
best outcomes for individuals and for society.
References
Arch, J. J., & Craske, M. G. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness: Emotion regulation following a
focused breathing induction. Behavior Research and Therapy, 44, 1849-1858.
Batson, C. D. (2011). Altruism in humans. Oxford University Press, USA.
Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., & Goldstein, A. (2012). Mindfulness-induced changes in
gamma band activity–implications for the default mode network, self-reference and
attention. Clinical Neurophysiology, 123, 700-710.
Berry, D. R., Hoerr, J. P., Cesko, S., Alayoubi, A., Carpio, K., Zirzow, H., Walters, W., Scram,
G., Rodriguez, K., & Beaver, V. (2020). Does Mindfulness Training Without Explicit
Ethics-Based Instruction Promote Prosocial Behaviors? A Meta-Analysis. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 0146167219900418.
Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this "We"? Levels of collective identity and self
representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 83–93.
Brown, K. W., Berry, D. R., & Quaglia, J. T. (2016). The hypo-egoic expression of
mindfulness. In: (K. W. Brown & M. R. Leary, Eds.) The Oxford Handbook of Hypo-
Egoic Phenomena (pp. 147-159). New York: Oxford University Press.
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in
psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822-848.
Cameron, A. C., & Trivedi, P. K. (2009). Microeconometrics Using Stata (Vol. 5, p. 706).
College Station, TX: Stata Press.
Chiesa, A., & Serretti, A. (2009). Mindfulness-based stress reduction for stress management in
healthy people: a review and meta-analysis. The Journal of Alternative and
Complementary Medicine, 15, 593-600.
Cross, S. E., Bacon, P. L., & Morris, M. L. (2000). The relational-interdependent self-construal
and relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 791-808.
Farb, N. A., Segal, Z. V., Mayberg, H., Bean, J., McKeon, D., Fatima, Z., & Anderson, A. K.
(2007). Attending to the present: mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes of
self-reference. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 313-322.
Gabriel, S., & Gardner, W. L. (1999). Are there "his" and "hers" types of interdependence? The
implications of gender differences in collective versus relational interdependence for
affect, behavior, and cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 642–
655.
Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., & Lee, A. Y. (1999). "I" value freedom, but "we" value
relationships: Self-construal priming mirrors cultural differences in
judgment. Psychological Science, 10, 321–326.
Gebauer, J. E., Nehrlich, A.D., Stahlberg, D., Sedikides, C., Hackenschmidt, D., Schick,
D., Stegmaie, C.A., Windfelder, C.C., Bruk, A. & Mander, J.V. (2018). Mind-body
practices and the self: yoga and meditation do not quiet the ego, but instead boost self-
enhancement. Psychological Science, 29, 1299-1308.
Grant, A. M., Campbell, E. M., Chen, G., Cottone, K., Lapedis, D., & Lee, K. (2007). Impact
and the art of motivation maintenance: The effects of contact with beneficiaries on
persistence behavior. Organizational behavior and human decision processes, 103(1),
53-67.
Hafenbrack, A. C., Cameron, L. D., Spreitzer, G. M., Zhang, C., Noval, L. J., & Shaffakat, S.
(2020). Helping people by being in the present: Mindfulness increases prosocial
behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 159, 21-38.
Hafenbrack, A. C., & Vohs, K. D. (2018). Mindfulness meditation impairs task motivation but
not performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 147, 1-15.
Hanley, A. W., & Garland, E. L. (2017). Clarity of mind: Structural equation modeling of
associations between dispositional mindfulness, self-concept clarity and psychological
well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 106, 334-
339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.10.028
Holland, R. W., Roeder, U.-R., van Baaren, R. B., Brandt, A. C., & Hannover, B. (2004). Don't
stand so close to me: The effects of self-construal on interpersonal
closeness. Psychological Science, 15, 237–242.
Jha, A. P., Morrison, A. B., Dainer-Best, J., Parker, S., Rostrup, N., & Stanley, E. A. (2015).
Minds “at attention”: Mindfulness training curbs attentional lapses in military cohorts.
PloS one, 10(2), e0116889.
Jones, E. E., Wirth, J. H., Ramsey, A. T., & Wynsma, R. L. (2019). Who is less likely to
ostracize? Higher trait mindfulness predicts more inclusionary behavior. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 45, 105-119.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your mind to face stress,
pain and illness. New York, NY: Dell.
Keng, S. L., Smoski, M. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011). Effects of mindfulness on psychological
health: A review of empirical studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 1041-1056.
Kiken, L. G., & Shook, N. J. (2011). Looking up: Mindfulness increases positive judgments and
reduces negativity bias. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2, 425–431.
Kreplin, U., Farias, M., & Brazil, I. A. (2018). The limited prosocial effects of meditation: A
systematic review and meta-analysis. Scientific Reports, 8, 1-10.
Kucinskas, J. (2018). The mindful elite: mobilizing from the inside out. Oxford University Press,
USA.
Lim, D., Condon, P., & DeSteno, D. (2015). Mindfulness and compassion: An examination of
mechanism and scalability. PLoS ONE, 10(2): e0118221.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118221
Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and
monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 163-169.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion,
and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.
Norenzayan, A. (2013). Big Gods: How Religion Transformed Cooperation and Conflict.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. S. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects
of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 311–
342. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.311.supp (Supplemental)
Purser, R. (2019). McMindfulness: How Mindfulness Became the New Capitalist Spirituality.
London: Watkins Media Limited.
Ricard, M. (2009, April 28). A Sniper's "Mindfulness". matthieuricard.org.
https://www.matthieuricard.org/en/blog/posts/a-sniper-s-mindfulness
Sears, S., & Kraus, S. (2009). I think therefore I om: Cognitive distortions and coping style as
mediators for the effects of mindfulness meditation on anxiety, positive and negative
affect, and hope. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65, 561-573.
Verplanken, B., & Holland, R. W. (2002). Motivated decision making: Effects of activation and
self-centrality of values on choices and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 82, 434–447.
Zeidan, F., Johnson, S.K., Gordon, N.S., & Goolkasian, P. (2010). Effects of brief sham and
mindfulness meditation on mood and cardiovascular variables. The Journal of Alternative
and Complementary Medicine, 16, 867-873.