ArticlePDF Available

Does Negative Emotion Differentiation Influence How People Choose to Regulate Their Distress After Stressful Events? A Four-Year Daily Diary Study

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Much is known about the types of strategies people use to regulate emotions. Less is known about individual differences that influence emotion regulation strategy selection. In this study, we tested the moderating role of negative emotion differentiation (NED; i.e., the ability to label and describe subtle differences among negative emotions) on the relationship between the intensity of stressful daily events and the strategies used to regulate distress arising from these events. Prior research shows that NED is associated with low endorsement of disengagement emotion regulation (e.g., substance use), but less is known about the link to engagement regulation (e.g., problem-solving, seeking social support). Participants were college students (N = 502) completing a 30-day daily diary survey for each of four college years. We preregistered hypotheses that 1) the intensity of each day's most stressful event would be associated with greater use of disengagement and engagement regulation strategies, and 2) people higher in NED would be less likely to use disengagement and more likely to use engagement strategies when highly stressed. Results suggest that higher stress intensity is associated with greater use of all regulation strategies. Greater NED is associated with less use of disengagement regulation strategies, whereas NED was unrelated to engagement regulation strategies and did not moderate the relationship between stress and engagement strategies. The majority of hypothesized moderation effects of NED were nonsignificant, prompting a reconsideration of whether, when, and how NED plays a role in stress responding. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
Content may be subject to copyright.
Emotion
Does Negative Emotion Differentiation Influence How People Choose to
Regulate Their Distress After Stressful Events? A Four-Year Daily Diary
Study
Bradley A. Brown, Fallon R. Goodman, David J. Disabato, Todd B. Kashdan, Stephen Armeli, and Howard Tennen
Online First Publication, April 8, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000969
CITATION
Brown, B. A., Goodman, F. R., Disabato, D. J., Kashdan, T. B., Armeli, S., & Tennen, H. (2021, April 8). Does Negative
Emotion Differentiation Influence How People Choose to Regulate Their Distress After Stressful Events? A Four-Year Daily
Diary Study. Emotion. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000969
Does Negative Emotion Differentiation Influence How People Choose to
Regulate Their Distress After Stressful Events? A Four-Year Daily
Diary Study
Bradley A. Brown
1
, Fallon R. Goodman
1
, David J. Disabato
2
, Todd B. Kashdan
3
, Stephen Armeli
4
,
and Howard Tennen
5
1
Department of Psychology, University of South Florida
2
Department of Psychological Sciences, Kent State University
3
Department of Psychology, George Mason University
4
Department of Psychology, Fairleigh Dickinson University
5
Department of Public Health Sciences, University of Connecticut School of Medicine
Much is known about the types of strategies people use to regulate emotions. Less is known about indi-
vidual differences that inuence emotion regulation strategy selection. In this study, we tested the mod-
erating role of negative emotion differentiation (NED; i.e., the ability to label and describe subtle
differences among negative emotions) on the relationship between the intensity of stressful daily events
and the strategies used to regulate distress arising from these events. Prior research shows that NED is
associated with low endorsement of disengagement emotion regulation (e.g., substance use), but less is
known about the link to engagement regulation (e.g., problem-solving, seeking social support).
Participants were college students (N= 502) completing a 30-day daily diary survey for each of four
college years. We preregistered hypotheses that 1) the intensity of each days most stressful event would
be associated with greater use of disengagement and engagement regulation strategies, and 2) people
higher in NED would be less likely to use disengagement and more likely to use engagement strategies
when highly stressed. Results suggest that higher stress intensity is associated with greater use of all reg-
ulation strategies. Greater NED is associated with less use of disengagement regulation strategies,
whereas NED was unrelated to engagement regulation strategies and did not moderate the relationship
between stress and engagement strategies. The majority of hypothesized moderation effects of NED
were nonsignicant, prompting a reconsideration of whether, when, and how NED plays a role in stress
responding.
Keywords: emotion differentiation, emotion regulation, stress, coping
Supplemental materials: https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000969.supp
When stressful events occur, there is a desire to regain a sense
of equanimity. When people experience stress in their day-to-day
lives, they deploy certain emotion regulation strategies to meet the
demands of stressors and regulate emotions that may otherwise
overwhelm them (e.g., Kashdan & Rottenberg, 2010). Emotion
regulation strategies are used to manage negative emotions, most
commonly to decrease their frequency or duration (Gross, 1998).
Prior research shows that individual differences in the strategies
people use inuence psychological functioning. For example, the
use of cognitive reappraisal, which involves the reframing of emo-
tional events to alter their emotional impact (Gross, 2015), is
linked to greater well-being (Gross & John, 2003;John & Gross,
2004), whereas suppressing emotions is linked to more adverse
outcomes (e.g., psychopathology, increase in negative emotions;
Gross & John, 2003;Srivastava et al., 2009). Although different
types of emotion regulation strategies and their outcomes receive
ample scientic attention, less understood are the individual differ-
ences that inuence strategies people select to regulate distress.
Recent work suggests that certain situational factors inuence
strategy selection. For example, experimental studies show that
the intensity of emotions (e.g., Sheppes et al., 2011), the complex-
ity of a regulation strategy (e.g., Sheppes et al., 2014), and a per-
sons regulation goals (e.g., English et al., 2017;Tamir, 2009)
Fallon R. Goodman https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1115-1467
David J. Disabato https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7094-4996
Todd B. Kashdan https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6438-0485
Bradley A. Brown, Fallon R. Goodman, and David J. Disabato
contributed equally to this work. This research was supported by the
National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) Grant
5P60-AA003510. There are no conicts of interest by any of the authors.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Todd B.
Kashdan, Department of Psychology, George Mason University, David
King Hall, Room 2048, 4400 University Drive, Fairfax, VA 22030-4422,
United States. Email: tkashdan@gmu.edu
1
Emotion
©2021 American Psychological Association
ISSN: 1528-3542 https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000969
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
inuence strategy selection. The source of stress also inuences
strategy selection. One study shows that the strategies people
choose to regulate daily anger are inuenced by what or who trig-
gered the anger (Kashdan et al., 2016). Taken together, research
suggests that strategy selection to regulate distress is not a uniform
process and is inuenced by contextual (e.g., Aldao, 2013) and
individual differences (Doré et al., 2016). Research examining
how individual differences (e.g., how one processes emotional in-
formation) interact with contextual factors of distress (e.g., inten-
sity of the distress) to inuence strategy selection in daily life can
provide a nuanced view of the distress regulation process. In a
four-year daily diary study, we examined how individual differen-
ces in the ability to label and describe discrete negative felt emo-
tions moderated the relationship between the intensity of daily
events and the strategies chosen to regulate distress arising from
these events.
Disengagement and Engagement Emotion Regulation
People use numerous strategies to regulate distress. Since early
research on coping (e.g., Carver et al., 1989) and emotion regula-
tion (e.g., Gross, 1998), researchers attempted to classify and
organize a seemingly innite number of regulatory strategies. In
modern affective science, arguably the most commonly used
framework categorized strategies as either adaptiveor malad-
aptive.Classications typically match Aldao et al.s (2010) meta-
analytic ndings suggesting strong positive correlations between
psychopathological symptoms and use of strategies such as avoid-
ance and strong negative correlations between psychopathological
symptoms and use of strategies such as problem-solving. Nonethe-
less, correlations with psychopathology do not necessarily provide
information about adaptiveness; this classication creates a false
dichotomy that implies strategies are uniformly helpful or
unhelpful.
In an updated framework, Naragon-Gainey et al. (2017) con-
ducted a meta-analysis to identify how common emotion regula-
tion strategies co-occur and interact. She failed to nd support for
the adaptivemaladaptive distinction and instead found three
classes of strategies based on a persons engagement with the
emotion or source of stress: engagement (characterized by engage-
ment with the stress, primarily by problem-solving), disengage-
ment (characterized by efforts to escape the distressing emotions
and thoughts, such as through avoidance or distraction), and aver-
sive cognitive perseveration (characterized by overengagement
with negative cognitions and failed attempts to avoid negative
emotions and thoughts, such as through rumination). Of note, sup-
port for this factor structure does not imply that strategies within
each class are uniform. Naragon-Gaineyet et al. suggest that strat-
egies within each class might still be examined independently.
They write,
However, the meta-analyses of correlations among strategies revealed
only small to moderate associations. It is striking that, even in these
analyses that were based on concurrent measurement and a single
method (i.e., self-report), the emotion regulation strategies showed
reasonable discriminant validity with one another and did not suggest
empirical redundancy. (p. 410)
Accordingly, we view the categorical schemes of adaptive and
maladaptive strategies as heterogeneous. In our research program,
we examined two primary disengagement strategies (avoidance
and distraction) and the primary engagement strategy (problem-
solving).
1
In addition to these cognitive-emotion strategies, we
also examined two behavioral strategies (see Aldao & Dixon-
Gordon, 2014) that are pertinent to college students: substance use
and social support seeking (e.g., Friedlander et al., 2007;OMalley
& Johnston, 2002;Staff et al., 2010;Wilcox et al., 2005). Neither
of these strategies were included in Naragon-Gainey et al.s meta-
analysis, so we relied on existing empirical research for classica-
tion purposes. Substance use is often used to escape or avoid dis-
tress (Khantzian, 1997). For instance, substance use is common
among people who have difculties tolerating distress (i.e., dis-
tress intolerance) or who have tendencies toward impulsive behav-
iors when experiencing distress (i.e., negative urgency; e.g.,
Cyders et al., 2009;Kaiser et al., 2012); suggesting that reducing
or escaping distress is a common motive for substance use
(Cooper et al., 1995). One longitudinal study of 418 college stu-
dents transitioning to college showed that participants motiva-
tions to use substances to avoid distress accounted for a positive
relationship between their tendency toward impulsive actions
when distressed (i.e., negative urgency) and the amount of alcohol
consumed during the rst year of college (Settles et al., 2010).
Thus, we classied alcohol use as a disengagement strategy. In
contrast, social support seeking, including instrumental support
seeking (seeking social help to problem-solve) or emotional sup-
port seeking (e.g., receiving moral support), has been conceptual-
ized as actively engaging with the stressor or subsequent distress
(Carver et al., 1989). Seeking and receiving social support has
been associated with actively coping with stressors in cancer
patients (e.g., Manne et al., 1999;Schulz & Schwarzer, 2004).
Therefore, we classied social support seeking as an engagement
strategy.
Emotion Differentiation as an Individual Difference in
How People Regulate Distress
People experience and process emotions in different ways, and
these individual differences, in part, inuence how distress is regu-
lated (e.g., Gohm & Clore, 2000;Smith & Kirby, 2011;Zeidener
et al., 2006). One individual difference that appears particularly
relevant to stress responses is negative emotion differentiation
(NED), dened as the ability to label and describe subtle differen-
ces among emotions of a similar valence (negative, e.g., anxiety
vs. guilt vs. fear; Barrett et al., 2001; for a review, see Kashdan et
al., 2015). People low on NED characterize their emotional experi-
ence in more global terms (e.g., I feel bad), whereas people high
1
In the meta-analysis (Naragon-Gainey et al., 2017), Disengagement
and Aversive Cognitive Perseveration were highly correlated (r= .67) and
conceptually similar in that they both involve avoidance. Experiential
avoidance measures demonstrate high cross-loadings, and numerous
measurement concerns have been raised about experiential avoidance
measures (e.g., Doorley et al., 2020). Aversive cognitive perseveration is
primarily characterized by difculty disengaging, specically rumination
and worry, two strategies we did not measure in this study. Therefore, we
only examined disengagement strategies, but recognize that avoidance
could reasonably be considered either a disengagement or aversive
cognitive perseveration strategy.
2BROWN ET AL.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
on NED identify and label their emotions in a more granular fash-
ion (e.g., I feel guilty and a little frustrated, but I do not feel
ashamed or angry). People low in NED, for example, may have
difculty separating feelings of anxiety, anger, and fear, leading
them to rate all the discrete emotions at a similar intensity and rep-
resent their emotional state in more broad terms such as feeling
bador unpleasant––limiting emotion-specic information.
Emotion theorists suggest that discrete emotion states are more
adaptive than global affective states because they provide more in-
formation and are less vulnerable to misattribution errors (Kehner
et al., 1993;Schwarz, 2011). Distinct emotions give meaning to
our daily lives and inuence how we appraise and react to specic
situations (e.g., Barrett et al., 2007;Scherer & Moors, 2019). Peo-
ple higher in NED may be better suited to identify the sources
behind specic emotions, providing them with information about
how to actively engage with a stressor to regulate distress (e.g.,
Barrett, 2006). For example, someone labeling their affect as anger
might be assertive with others, whereas someone labeling their
affect as guilt might make amends.
Decits in the ability to recognize and identify emotions likely
hinder a person's ability to regulate their distress adaptively, possibly
by limiting contextual information needed to accurately appraise a
stressor and respond effectively. Indeed, compared to healthy coun-
terparts, research suggests that NED is dampened among individuals
with psychological maladies associated with difculties in regulating
distress and a tendency toward disengagement strategies (e.g.,
depression; Demiralp et al., 2012;Yoon & Rottenberg, 2020; bor-
derline personality disorder; Linehan, 2018;Suvak et al., 2011;
social anxiety disorder; Kashdan & Farmer, 2014;Seah et al., 2020).
For example, one study of 38 people with borderline personality dis-
order found that those lower in NED were more likely to use the
maladaptive disengagement strategy of self-harming than people
higher in NED (Zaki et al., 2013).
Considerable evidence suggests that people with high NED are
less likely to use disengagement strategies to cope with stressful
situations than people with low NED. For example, people higher
in NED were less likely than lower NED peers to drink to excess
when experiencing distress (Kashdan et al., 2010) or act aggres-
sively when angered (Pond et al., 2012). NED also buffers the
effects of stressful events on depressed mood (e.g., Nook et al.,
2020;Starr et al., 2017;2020). In a 2020 EMA study of adolescent
participants, the adverse effects of daily stressors on depressed
mood were attenuated for people with higher compared with lower
NED (Starr et al., 2020); however, it is unclear how NED inu-
enced regulatory strategies to deal with distress.
In a similar vein, ndings in college student samples have
shown that a persons ability to identify discrete emotions was
positively associated with engagement strategies and other impor-
tant facets of well-being (Barrett et al., 2001;Gohm & Clore,
2002); however, the majority of studies examining the effect of
NED on emotion regulation explored disengagement strategies
(e.g., substance use, nonsuicidal self-injury). As such, it is impor-
tant to examine NED as both a protective factor against disengage-
ment emotion regulation as well as an individual difference that
might promote the use of engagement regulation strategies.
To our knowledge, only two studies have evaluated the effect of
NED on engagement regulation strategies. Barrett and colleagues
(2001) found that NED was associated with more frequent use of
strategies; however, these ndings were limited to retrospective
reports of emotion regulation (how often participants used regula-
tion strategies over the prior two weeks). Additionally, researchers
used a sum score for regulation strategies in this study, so it is
unclear if NED was associated with specic regulation strategies
(e.g., engagement vs disengagement).
Expanding upon this work, Kalokerinos and colleagues (2019)
conducted two experience-sampling studies to examine the effects
of NED on strategy selection and strategy effectiveness (dened
as decreases in negative emotions after strategy use). They exam-
ined three engagement strategiesacceptance, reappraisal, and
social sharing. In contrast to the ndings of Barrett and colleagues
(2001), NED was negatively associated with the selection of cog-
nitive reappraisal in Study 1 and unrelated in Study 2, negatively
associated with the selection of social sharing, and unrelated to the
selection of acceptance. NED was associated with strategy effec-
tiveness, such that people with lower NED experienced increases
in negative affect after using cognitive reappraisal or social
sharing (suggesting ineffective use for those lower in NED), a
relationship also present, but attenuated, for those higher in NED.
Interestingly, acceptance was associated with reduced negative
affect (indicating strategy effectiveness) for those lower in NED in
this study. The present study aims to extend this work by examin-
ing NED and both disengagement (avoidance, substance use,
distraction) and engagement (problem-solving, seeking social sup-
port) regulation strategies in the context of stress and coping over
the course of four years.
Studying Emotion and Stress in Daily Life
Self-reports of how one generally experiences emotions (e.g.,
retrospective trait measures of emotion) and self-reports of emo-
tions in the moment likely represent two distinct psychological
processes (Robinson & Clore, 2002). This is consistent with a
prior study showing negligible relationships between trait emotion
clarity (assessing general experiences of emotion) and state NED
(in-the-moment reports of emotion; Boden et al., 2013). Meas-
uring emotions as they occur decreases the likelihood of recall
error and bias and other difculties in memory retrieval of emo-
tional experiences (Kihlstrom et al., 1999;Trull & Ebner-Priemer,
2009). Emotions, stress, and emotional reactivity to stress vary
over time within a single person (Howland et al., 2017;Sliwinski
et al., 2009). Accurate examination of these constructs requires
assessment of processes close to real time.
With recent developments in methodological tools to measure
psychological processes in real time (e.g., daily diary studies),
researchers are better able to observe how people experience emo-
tions as they occur (Barrett, 1997;Shiffman et al., 2008;Wilhelm
& Grossman, 2010). In particular, experience-sampling methodol-
ogies (e.g., daily diary, ecological momentary assessment) can
account for meaningful uctuations across consecutive days. In
this study, participants completed up to four 30-day diary periods,
each one year apart, which allowed us to account for changes in
stress patterns over the course of several years during a signicant
development period (i.e., collegiate years).
The Present Study
The present study used a four-wave daily diary design to exam-
ine how individual differences in NED moderated the relationship
NEGATIVE EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION 3
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
between stress and the use of emotion regulation strategies. We
hypothesized that the stress intensity of each day's most stressful
event would be positively associated with greater endorsement of
all regulation strategies. For disengagement strategies (i.e., avoid-
ance, substance use, distraction), we hypothesized that the positive
stress-regulation strategy relationship would be weaker for people
higher in NED compared to people lower in NED. For engagement
strategies (i.e., problem-solving and social support seeking), we
hypothesized that the positive stress-regulation strategy relation-
ship would be stronger for people higher in NED compared to peo-
ple lower in NED. We preregistered hypotheses and the analytic
plan with the Open Science Foundation (https://osf.io/2yn4z).
2
Method
Participants and Procedures
College students (N= 574) were recruited from several intro-
ductory psychology courses. Potential participants were asked to
be part of a four-year longitudinal study examining the daily
behavior of college students. The mean age of participants (86.3%
White; 52.8% women) at Wave 1 was 18.77 (SD = 1.08 years). A
majority of participants began the study in their freshman (57%)
or sophomore (34%) year at the university. Data were collected
using a daily diary design, where participants answered question-
naires examining personality traits and demographic information
in a trait assessment and then completed a 30-day daily diary, be-
ginning two to three weeks after the trait assessment. Participants
completed the trait assessment and 30-day diary each year for up
to four years. Participants began in either the fall or spring semes-
ter; 61% of participants began the study one month into the fall se-
mester, and 39% began one month into the spring semester. Those
who began in a certain semester completed their daily diary study
in that same semester each year.
Participants completed the daily diary assessment using a secure
website once per day between 2:30 p.m. and 7:00 p.m. Participants
received up to $220 in monetary compensation per year, with each
participant receiving $20 for the completion of their baseline sur-
veys and up to $100 depending on daily survey adherence. As an
additional incentive, participants who completed all 30 days were
entered into a lottery for an added prize of $100. The current study
is limited to secondary analyses of the broader study (see Armeli
et al., 2015 for additional details on original data collection).
For our nal analyses, data for each wave were only retained
for participants who completed at least 15 out of 30 days of daily
diary data per wave. Of the initial 574 participants, 502 partici-
pants in Wave 1 met the inclusion criteria for the current study for
study days. We used the same inclusionary criteria for each wave:
Wave 2 (N= 449), Wave 3 (N= 406), and Wave 4 (N= 362). Ta-
ble 1 details the demographic information of participants. The ad-
herence rate for daily diary reporting across all years in this
sample was 81.8% (24.5 days out of 30 days).
We deviated in several ways from preregistered inclusion and
exclusion criteria (responding to consultation with other research-
ers, including the senior author and peer reviewers). First, at the
suggestion of the senior author who collected the original data, we
changed our exclusion to retain only participants with at least 15
days of data (as opposed to the 10-day criteria outlined in our
preregistration). Second, per reviewer suggestions, we included
participants who had intraclass correlation (ICC) scores of zero for
the negative emotion items across the daily surveys at each wave
(as opposed to only including participants with a positive ICC
score). Analyses that match the preregistration exclusion criteria
are reported in the online supplemental materials.
Measures
Stress Intensity of Daily Event
Stress intensity was measured using a one-item daily measure.
Participants were asked to think about their most negative event of
the day. They rated how stressful the event was (How stressful
was this event?) using a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 =
(Not at all stressful)to7=(Extremely stressful).
Daily Emotion Regulation Strategies
Daily use of regulation strategies was assessed using a single-
item measure for each of the six listed emotion regulation strat-
egies. When participants were asked to think of their most nega-
tive event of the day (see above), they were then asked to rate how
much they had been engaging in each of six different emotion reg-
ulation strategies in response to the negative daily event: avoid-
ance (Avoided dealing with the situation), alcohol use (Used
alcohol to help get through it), drug use (Used drugs to help get
through it), distraction (Tried distracting myself to keep my
mind off the problem), problem-solving (Thought about what
needed to be done and did it), and seeking social support
(Sought out individuals who could help me or make me feel bet-
ter). Participants rated how much they had used each strategy to
Table 1
Participant Demographics
Characteristics N= 502 (%)
Sex
Male 237 (47.2%)
Female 265 (52.8%)
Year
Freshman 281 (56.0%)
Sophomore 168 (33.5%)
Junior 39 (7.8%)
Senior 10 (2.0%)
Fifth year or beyond 2 (0.4%)
Missing 2 (0.4%)
Race
White/Caucasian 433 (86.3%)
Black/African American 17 (3.4%)
Latino/Hispanic 15 (3.0%)
Asian/Pacific Islander 33 (6.6%)
Other 4 (0.8%)
2
Hypotheses and analyses were registered after data were collected but
before data analyses. Before preregistering this manuscript, a NED index
was created and analyses were conducted using this dataset for two
separate projects that led to a conference talk (https://osf.io/a9wm5/) and
poster (https://osf.io/s8kn4). The hypotheses for these projects differed
from the hypotheses in the present research. After these projects, we
produced research questions and then preregistered a plan with the current
study hypotheses and data analyses.
4BROWN ET AL.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
cope with the most negative event of their day on a 4-point Likert
scale from 0 = (I havent been doing this at all)to3=(Ive been
doing this a lot). The two items referring to alcohol use and drug
use were summed together and conceptualized as regulating emo-
tions through substance use, resulting in a total of ve emotion
regulation strategy items. The correlation between the two sub-
stance use items was moderate (ranging from .5 to .6 at each
wave), empirically supporting our decision to aggregate them.
Avoidance, substance use, and distraction t conceptually under
the domain of disengagement regulation strategies, whereas prob-
lem-solving and seeking social support represent two engagement
regulation strategies (Carver et al., 1989;Naragon-Gainey et al.,
2017).
Negative Emotional Intensity
Daily mood was captured using emotion items from the Positive
and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson et al., 1988) and Larsen
and Dieners circumplex model of emotion (Larsen & Diener,
1992), resulting in 15 emotion terms capturing negative and posi-
tive emotions (6 positive emotion terms and 9 negative emotion
terms). Participants rated each emotion using a 5-point Likert scale
from 1 = (Not at all)to5=(Extremely) to describe their emotions
at the time of the prompt. For the purposes of the current study,
eight of the nine negative emotion items were used (sad, dejected,
nervous, jittery, angry, hostile, guilty, ashamed). Boredwas not
included in analyses because of poor factorial validity in the mood
literature (Goetz et al., 2014). Participantstotal emotional inten-
sity scores for each day were calculated by averaging their reports
of the eight negative emotion items. We conducted multilevel reli-
ability for emotion intensity per recommendations for assessing
within-person and between-person reliability using daily diary
data (Cranford et al., 2006). Both within-person reliability (a=
.80) and between-person reliability (a= .99) were acceptable.
Negative Emotion Differentiation
NED was measured with the ICC (Shrout & Fleiss, 1979) across
the eight negative emotion items used in the momentary assess-
ments of mood for each participant. The ICC can be interpreted as
how similarly negative emotion ratings vary over time. Someone
with a high ICC would give very similar ratings for the emotions
at any given time point (e.g., rating of 2 for almost all negative
emotions), while someone with a low ICC would give very differ-
ent responses for the emotions (e.g., rating of a 1 for some emo-
tions, 2 for others, 3 for some, etc.). We deviated from our
preregistration by using the ICC(2,k), rather than the ICC(1,1), to
provide a better measure of NED. The ICC(2,k) refers to negative
emotions as a whole, rather than a single negative emotion, and
incorporates mean differences across items, rather than only their
correlations. This type of ICC is consistent with the literature on
emotion differentiation, specically when capturing NED using
experience sampling methodology (e.g., Kashdan et al., 2010;
Tugade et al., 2004). We calculated the ICC separately for each
person and wave/year of assessment. The ICCs at each wave were
estimated using the psychR package via linear mixed-effects
modeling to better allow for missing data (compared with
ANOVA based ICC estimation; Revelle, 2019). No participants
had negative ICCs, and participants with zero ICCs were retained.
NED scores at each wave were dened as 1ICC, such that higher
scores indicated higher NED, and then averaged across the waves
to obtain nal NED scores.
Data Analytic Plan
Generalized linear mixed-effects models were applied to test the
study hypotheses using the lme4package (Bates et al., 2015).
We ran ve statistical modelsone for each of the ve emotion
regulation strategy outcomes. The distribution of the disengage-
ment regulation strategy scores was very positively skewed (and
integer-only), thus informing the use of a Poisson error distribu-
tion with a natural log link function (Afifi et al., 2007). The models
had three levels with day at level 1, wave at level 2, and person at
level 3. There were no predictors at level 2, but unconditional like-
lihood ratio tests of each outcome indicated three-level random
structure t the data better than a two-level random structure that
ignored between-wave differences (ps,.05). No random slopes
were included in the models.
The only level-1 predictor was daily stress intensity, which was
person-centered (i.e., group-mean centered) to reect within-per-
son variance. Average levels of daily stress intensity across the
study for each person was a level-3 predictor reecting the
between-person variance and was grand-mean centered. The two
other level-3 predictors were NED and negative emotion intensity,
both grand-mean centered. In line with recent NED work (e.g.,
Starr et al., 2017,2020) and Bolger and Laurenceaus (2013) rec-
ommendation for daily diary research, we partitioned our level-1
predictor (i.e., stress intensity) into two independent components:
between-person and within-person components. We included both
the main effects and interactions for both within-person and
between-person components in our model (see Table 4). However,
as our hypotheses are only concerned with the within-person
effects of stress intensity, we do not describe the effects of
between-person stress intensity in the Results section. To clarify
this, we have bolded the row in the tables corresponding to the
cross-level interaction of within-person stress intensity and NED.
Sensitivity analyses revealed that the addition of the between-per-
son component of stress intensity did not change the results when
compared to analyses excluding this between-subjects component.
The four product terms in the statistical models are 1) within-
person stress intensity * NED, 2) between-person stress intensity *
NED, 3) within-person stress intensity * negative emotion inten-
sity, and 4) between-person stress intensity * negative emotion in-
tensity. While the focal product terms were those for NED, we
controlled for participants mean negative emotion intensity to
ensure that we captured the effects of NED beyond differences in
negative emotion intensity (e.g., Demiralp et al., 2012;Erbas et
al., 2018). Simple slopes were calculated to illustrate signicant
moderation effects at high NED (1 SD above the mean) and low
NED (1 SD below the mean).
We deviated from our preregistered data analytic plan following
consultation with other researchers, including peer reviewers (see
online supplemental materials for results from the original prereg-
istered analyses). First, we applied Poisson mixed-effects model-
ing rather than linear mixed-effects modeling to account for the
distribution of the disengagement regulation strategies. Second,
we combined the four waves together into a three-level model,
rather than apply separate two-level models at each wave, to
reduce the total number of hypothesis tests and study-wise type I
NEGATIVE EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION 5
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
error. Third, we included the average level of stress intensity to
test for the NED interaction at both the within-person and
between-person level.
To correct for the 10 statistical signicance tests conducted, we
used the Benjamini-Hochberg (B-H) correction (Benjamini &
Hochberg, 1995) for any statistically signicant interaction effects
that were found. The B-H correction is an approach to control for
the false discovery rate, which ensures that the likelihood of incor-
rectly rejecting the null hypothesis (i.e., type I error) for statisti-
cally signicant ndings is 5%.
Results
Descriptive Statistics and Preliminary Analyses
Descriptive statistics are summarized in Table 2. NED scores
were comparable (M= .47, SD = .22) to past research on NED
(Erbas et al., 2014;Kashdan et al., 2010). Given the nested struc-
ture of the data, correlations were decomposed to illustrate rela-
tionships for both between-person and within-person variables.
Correlations are presented in Table 3. Analyses revealed signi-
cant, positive within-person correlations between stress intensity
and each regulation strategy at the .01 level. The correlation
between our level-3 variables (NED and emotion intensity) fell
under the threshold of problematic multicollinearity (r=.46).
Primary Analyses
We examined how NED moderated the relationship between
stress intensity and each of the ve emotion regulation strategies.
A statistical summary of moderation analyses is reported in Table
4. We controlled for the moderation effect of emotion intensity.
Some of the emotion intensity moderation effects are signicant,
however, these effects were not probed as this was not the focal
point of our analyses.
Avoidance as an Outcome: Within-Person Stress Intensity
as a Predictor
Within-person stress intensity was positively associated with
the use of avoidance (b= .09, z= 18.50, p,.001, 95% CI [.08,
.10]). NED was negatively associated with the use of avoidance
(b=1.00, z=4.47, p,.001, 95% CI [1.43, .56]). Negative
emotion intensity was positively associated with the use of avoid-
ance (b= .89, z= 6.98, p,.01, 95% CI [.64, 1.15]). Moderation
analyses did not yield a signicant moderation effect of NED on
the relationship between within-person stress intensity and the use
of avoidance (p= .16). No moderation effect was found of nega-
tive emotion intensity on the relationship between within-person
stress intensity and the use of avoidance (p= .08).
Substance Use as an Outcome: Within-Person Stress
Intensity as a Predictor
Within-person stress intensity was positively associated with
substance use (b= .14, z= 11.80, p,.001, 95% CI [.11, .16]).
NED was negatively associated with substance use (b=2.04, z=
4.61, p,.001, 95% CI [2.90, 1.17]). Negative emotion inten-
sity was positively associated with substance use (b= 2.13, z=
8.92, p,.001, 95% CI [1.66, 2.60]). Moderation analyses did not
yield a signicant moderation effect of NED on the relationship
between within-person stress intensity and substance use (p= .29).
A signicant moderation effect was found of negative emotion in-
tensity on the relationship between within-person stress intensity
and substance use (b=.09, z=4.77, p,.001, 95% CI [.12,
.05]).
Distraction as an Outcome: Within-Person Stress Intensity
as a Predictor
Within-person stress intensity was positively associated with
the use of distraction (b= .11, z= 29.23, p,.001, 95% CI [.10,
.12]). NED was negatively associated with the use of distraction
(b=.73, z=3.96, p,.001, 95% CI [1.10, -.37]). Negative
emotion intensity was positively associated with distraction (p=
.47, z= 4.39, p,.001, 95% CI [.26, .68]). Moderation analyses
yielded a signicant moderation effect of NED on the relationship
between within-person stress intensity and the use of distraction
(b=.06, z=2.70, p,.01, 95% CI [.10, .02]). This modera-
tion effect remained signicant after applying the B-H correction.
Simple slopes analysis revealed a weaker positive effect at high lev-
els of NED (b=.09,z=15.36,p,.001, 95% CI [.08, .11]) com-
pared with low NED (b=.12,z=21.95,p,.001, 95% CI [.11,
.13]). This moderation effect is illustrated in Figure 1. A signicant
moderation effect was found of negative emotion intensity on the
relationship between within-person stress intensity and the use of
distraction (b=-.04,z=3.85, p,.001, 95% CI [.06, .02]).
Problem-Solving as an Outcome: Within-Person Stress
Intensity as a Predictor
Within-person stress intensity was positively associated with the use
of problem-solving (b=.12,z=44.69,p,.001, 95% CI [.11, .12]).
NED was unrelated to problem-solving (p= .12). Negative emotion in-
tensity was unrelated to the use of problem-solving (p= .28). Modera-
tion analyses did not yield a signicant moderation effect of NED on
the relationship between within-person stress intensity and the use of
problem-solving (p=.14).Asignicant moderation effect was found
of negative emotion intensity on the relationship between within-per-
son stress intensity and the use of problem-solving (b=.05, z=
5.14, p,.001, 95% CI [.06, .03]).
Social Support as an Outcome: Within-Person Stress
Intensity as a Predictor
Within-person stress intensity was positively associated with
the use of social support (b= .19, z= 52.84, p,.001, 95% CI
Table 2
Descriptive Statistics for Variables
Variable Range MSDSkewness
Stress intensity 17 4.17 1.88 .20
Avoidance 03 .47 .82 1.73
Substance use 03 .12 .50 4.49
Distraction 03 .75 .96 1.02
Problem-solving 03 1.44 1.12 .07
Social support 03 .82 1.00 .89
Emotion intensity 15 1.36 .37 2.01
NED 01 .47 .22 .22
Note. SD = standard deviation; NED = negative emotion differentiation.
6BROWN ET AL.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
[.18, .19]). NED was unrelated to the use of social support (p=
.14). Negative emotion intensity was positively associated with the
use of social support (b= .37, z= 3.51, p,.001, 95% CI [.16,
.57]). Moderation analyses yielded a marginally signicant moder-
ation effect of NED on the relationship between within-person
stress intensity and social support (b=.04, z=1.92, p= .05,
95% CI [.08, .001]), although it bordered on the threshold of sig-
nicance (p,.05). After applying the B-H correction, this effect
was no longer approaching statistical signicance. A signicant
moderation effect was found of negative emotion intensity on the
relationship between within-person stress intensity and the use of
social support (b=.09, z=7.79, p,.001, 95% CI [.11,
.06]).
Discussion
The purpose of this study was to examine how individual differ-
ences in the ability to label and describe distinct emotional experi-
ences (i.e., NED) moderate the effect of stress intensity on the
selection of emotion regulation strategies. As predicted, people
used emotion regulation strategies more often when they perceived
events as highly stressful. Consistent with our hypotheses, people
who described their emotions in a more granular fashion (i.e., high
NED) were less likely to use disengagement strategies than people
lower in NED. We found limited support for the hypothesis that
higher NED would protect against the use of disengagement
regulation strategies to cope with highly stressful situations.
Higher NED protected against the use of distraction in high-stress
situations, but not for substance use or avoidance.
NED was unrelated to engagement strategies (i.e., problem-
solving and social support). Contrary to predictions, people higher
in NED did not more frequently use engagement strategies, and
NED did not moderate the relationship between stress intensity
and engagement strategy use. A lack of empirical support for mod-
eration hypotheses prompts a reconsideration of the role that NED
plays in regulating distress and highlights potential problems with
dichotomizing regulation strategies. This reconsideration is espe-
cially important considering the methodological differences in
how NED is operationalized.
These results slightly differ from what we discovered from our
preregistered analyses (see online supplemental materials for pre-
registered Methods and Results). In our preregistered analyses, we
found a signicant moderating effect of NED on the relationship
between stress intensity and each of the ve regulation strategies
at Wave 2. These moderation effects, however, did not replicate
for any outcome at any other wave (i.e., Waves 1, 3, and 4), with
the exception of distraction, which was signicant at Wave 1 and
Wave 2. Notably, the stressdistraction relationship is the only
relationship in the present analyses to be signicantly moderated
by NED (see Table 4). These discrepancies may stem from a dif-
fering number of signicance tests conducted. Our preregistered
analyseswhich were conducted at the wave level (i.e., ve
Table 3
Correlations for Between-Person and Within-Person Variables
Variable Stress intensity Avoidance Substance use Distraction Problem-solving Social support NED Emotion intensity
Stress intensity .32*** .02 .43*** .40*** .42*** .10* .19***
Avoidance .10*** .39*** .80*** .13*** .38*** .31*** .47***
Substance use .05*** .18*** .23*** .09* .17*** .28*** .60***
Distraction .16*** .49*** .14*** .33*** .59*** .27*** .37***
Problem-solving .29*** .07*** .01* .03*** .63*** .06 .01
Social support .28*** .08*** .10*** .23*** .36*** .16*** .25***
NED ——— — .46***
Emotion intensity ——— — .02***
Note. Upper triangle represents between-subject correlations and lower triangle represents within-subject correlations. NED = negative emotion differen-
tiation.
* Correlation is significant at the .05 level (2-tailed). *** Correlation is significant at the .001 level (2-tailed).
Table 4
The Moderating Effect of NED on the Relationship Between Stress Intensity and Strategy Selection
Variable Avoidance Substance use Distraction Problem-solving Social support
bzbzbzbzbz
Intercept 1.34 31.90*** 4.04 42.19*** .70 20.10*** .18 8.38*** .61 18.09***
Within-person stress .09 18.50*** .14 11.80*** .11 29.23*** .12 44.69*** .19 52.84***
NED 1.00 4.47*** 2.04 4.61*** .73 3.96*** .17 1.57 .26 1.46
Emotion intensity .89 6.98*** 2.13 8.92*** .47 4.39*** .07 1.07 .37 3.51***
Between-person stress .40 8.18*** .01 .11 .45 11.12*** .28 11.40*** .42 10.82***
Within-Person Stress 3NED 2.04 21.42 2.05 21.05 2.06 22.70*** 2.02 21.48 2.04 21.92
Within-Person Stress 3Emotion Intensity .02 1.77 .09 4.77*** .04 3.85*** .05 5.14*** .09 7.79***
Between-Person Stress 3NED .36 1.54 .46 .98 .07 .35 .03 .22 .08 .40
Between-Person Stress 3Emotion Intensity .19 1.36 .50 1.94 .09 .78 .03 .39 .16 1.42
Note. Within-person effects are bolded. Significant NED interaction effects are bolded and italicized. NED = negative emotion differentiation.
*** p,.001.
NEGATIVE EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION 7
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
separate 2-level models at each wave)resulted in 25 signicance
tests, whereas the nal analyses, which combined all four waves
into one three-level model, resulted in 10 signicance tests for
which a B-H correction was applied. Thus, once correcting for sig-
nicance testing, the evidence for a moderating effect of NED on
the stress-regulation strategy relationship in this sample is weak.
NED as a Protective Factor Against the Use of
Disengagement Strategies
NED predicted the use of disengagement strategies. For all dis-
engagement strategies tested, people higher in NED were less
likely to use disengagement strategies to cope with daily stressors.
These ndings are consistent with prior ndings that people higher
in NED are less likely to use substances (e.g., Anand et al., 2017;
Emery et al., 2014;Kashdan et al., 2010) and avoidance (Seah et
al., 2020) to cope with stress. Moreover, our ndings that people
higher in NED less frequently use distraction are consistent with
research demonstrating that people may use distraction when they
feel overwhelmed and confused by emotional experiences (e.g.,
Sheppes et al., 2011;2014). Thus, rather than try to decipher their
emotional experiences, they may instead disengage from them.
This framework is consistent with a recent review of experiential,
behavioral, and neuroimaging research that suggests that labeling
emotions can serve as a form of implicit regulation (Torre & Lie-
berman, 2018). Thus, because people higher in NED identify and
label discrete emotions, they are less likely to use strategies aimed
at escaping negative emotions and are better able to down-regulate
their emotions. Increased emotional awareness may facilitate a
sense of control over emotions, thus reducing regulatory strategies
that often function as an escape from aversive emotions (e.g., sub-
stance use). Indeed, compared to people lower in NED, people
higher in NED report a greater sense of control over emotions
(Pond et al., 2012) and a lower proclivity to act rashly and impul-
sively in response to negative emotions (Emery et al., 2014).
Thus, identifying and labeling discrete emotional experiences may
facilitate greater coping self-efcacy, making it less likely for a
person to use disengagement strategies for a short-term reprieve.
Additionally, greater attention and awareness to a personsemo-
tional experience may prevent people from becoming entangledor
fusedwith their emotional experiences (Gillanders et al., 2014;Van
der Gucht et al., 2019). When people become more entangledin an
emotional experience, they may be more likely to experience second-
ary emotions or appraisals (i.e., emotions about emotions), which of-
ten manifest in a cascade of negative emotions linked to the inexible
use of disengagement strategies (Gratz & Roemer, 2004;Selby et al.,
2009). Indeed, recent evidence suggests that people with lower NED
report greater cognitive fusion (Plonsker et al., 2017). Future work
exploring mechanisms that might underlie NED will likely help to
inform preventative efforts against the use of problematic disengage-
ment strategies such as using substances to cope with distress.
Contrary to predictions, NED did not moderate the relationship
between stress intensity and four of the ve measured regulatory
strategies (distraction being the exception). Thus, we largely failed
to nd support that people with different levels of NED regulate
differently in response to varying levels of stress.
Figure 1
The Moderating Effect of NED on the Relationship Between Within-Person Stress and Distraction
Note. þ1SD = one standard deviation above the mean; 1SD = one standard deviation below the mean.
8BROWN ET AL.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
These ndings are inconsistent with prior research suggesting
that NED protects against the use of disengagement strategies to
regulate intense negative emotions (Kashdan et al., 2010;Seah et
al., 2020;Zaki et al., 2013). A primary benet of NED is the abil-
ity to access distinct information about certain affective states
(e.g., physiological sensations, thoughts, urges) to effectively reg-
ulate distress (Kashdan et al., 2015). In this way, NED might be
more benecial in situations where the source of negative emotion
is ambiguous or diffuse (e.g., a day consisting of low mood or
intense negative emotions) than for a more specic, unambiguous
stressful situation (e.g., I am stressed and I do not know why I
woke up feeling badvs. I am stressed because I missed my
ight). Regulatory efforts tied to a specic event may not benet
as much from NED because already knowing the source of the
negative emotion may mitigate a core mechanism of NED. Future
work examining other factors associated with a specic stressor (e.
g., perceived controllability) within the relationship of NED and
emotion regulation is indicated.
Another explanation for null ndings may be due to methodo-
logical differences. A sizable number of NED studies examined
negative emotion intensity, calculated using a wide range of nega-
tive emotion adjectives (e.g., Barrett et al., 2001;Kashdan et al.,
2010) or maladaptive cognitive styles (i.e., rumination; Seah et al.,
2020;Zaki et al., 2013) as predictors of disengagement regulation
strategies. In the current study, the event that engendered regula-
tory efforts were subject to change (different events on different
days), and it may be that in some stressful situations, low NED is
more benecial or protective to elicit a quick, adaptive response.
For example, in the event that a re alarm goes off and a quick
evacuation is needed, a granular view of specic emotions may be
unhelpful. An interesting avenue of research would be to detail if
people high in NED demonstrate a sense of exibility in how they
differentiate between emotions, perhaps becoming less likely to
identify specic emotions when quicker responses are needed.
Null Results for NED and Engagement Strategies and
the Dichotomy of Adaptive and Maladaptive Regulation
Strategies
NED was unrelated to use of engagement strategies (i.e., prob-
lem-solving and social support) and did not moderate stress-regu-
lation strategy relationships. These ndings were contrary to
prediction, and caution is warranted against overinterpreting null
effects. Nonetheless, we offer a few possible explanations for
these ndings.
In the present study, we classied strategies as involving either
engagement or disengagement with the distress or its source. Lack
of associations between NED and engagement regulation strat-
egies may be an artifact of the categorical splitting of emotion reg-
ulation strategies into adaptive and maladaptive strategies. Our
hypotheses for moderation effects were informed by research sug-
gesting that disengagement strategies tend to be maladaptive and
engagement strategies tend to be adaptive (Aldao et al., 2010) and
that NED is associated with adaptive emotion regulation (Barrett
et al., 2001;Kashdan et al., 2015). However, a growing body of
research suggests that dichotomizing strategies into putatively
adaptive and maladaptive categories makes the erroneous assump-
tion that strategies are uniformly effective (e.g., Aldao et al., 2015;
Bonanno & Burton, 2013;Ford & Troy, 2019). Affective scientists
have called for more emotion regulation research addressing con-
textual factors (Aldao, 2013), such as exibility of strategy use
(e.g., Kashdan et al., 2020), situational goals (e.g., hedonic goals
vs. instrumental goals; e.g., Tamir et al., 2008), and situational
demands (e.g., Kobyli
nska & Kusev, 2019) that inuence emotion
regulation effectiveness.
In this study, it is possible that problem-solving and social sup-
port are less relevant to NED than avoidance, substance use, and
distraction. For example, engagement strategies may not have
been indicated or effective in regulating distress for a particular
stressful event. Prior research found that the use of more complex
strategies (e.g., problem-solving) are more effective at lower stress
intensity (e.g., Ortner et al., 2016). It may be that people who iden-
tify and label instances of emotion in a granular fashion are better
suited to select strategies that t best for a particular situation
rather than using certain adaptivestrategies more often.
Another possibility for the lack of support for an association
between NED and engagement regulation strategies was that we
did not assess engagement strategies germane to NED. For exam-
ple, research found that people tend to use cognitive reappraisal in
lower intensity situations because it requires more effortful proc-
essing of stimuli than disengagement strategies like distraction
(e.g., trying to nd the silver lining of a failure vs. trying not to
think about the failure; Sheppes et al., 2014). It is possible that
NED moderates this relationship, where people higher in NED use
cognitive reappraisal in high-intensity situations because they can
appropriately sort through their emotions (and potentially even
draw causal explanations for them) and therefore be more
equipped to reinterpret the situation (cf. Kalokerinos et al., 2019).
As another example, mindfulness involves harnessing present
moment awareness (e.g., Bishop et al., 2004). Although there is
debate concerning whether mindfulness is a trait or state/skill
(e.g., Lau et al., 2006;Thompson & Waltz, 2007), people higher
in NED might be better able to direct their momentary experien-
ces, particularly emotional experiences. Thus, we might expect
people higher in NED to use mindfulness more often, even in
stressful situations. Future research on NED will benet from
including a variety of engagement regulation strategies (e.g.,
mindfulness, acceptance, cognitive reappraisal).
Limitations and Future Directions
Several study limitations warrant mention. Daily diary method-
ology allows examination of how people differentiate between
emotions and regulate their distress over time. Nonetheless, our
analyses are correlational, which prevents conclusion about cau-
sality. Our NED scores represent participantsaverage level of
NED across the study, and thus we cannot determine how partici-
pants labeled and identied emotions in the immediate aftermath
of daily stressors. Future research can assess temporal relation-
ships between stress and NED, such as by examining uctuations
of NED during stressful moments as they unfold using ecological
momentary assessment (e.g., Erbas et al., 2018) or immediately af-
ter using event-contingent recording (cf. Tennen et al., 2006).
Although our approach of quantifying NED (e.g., 1ICC) is
common, it does not separate people who truly experience mixed
negative emotions at the same time (e.g., sadness and guilt) from
low NED. This may be problematic because the most emotionally
aware individuals are likely able to accurately identify mixed
NEGATIVE EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION 9
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
emotional experiences. Open-response formats may be a better al-
ternative to capture mixed emotional experiences (Ottenstein &
Lischetzke, 2020).
We measured a xed set of emotion regulation strategies. While
they represent a diverse range of commonly used strategies, they
are not comprehensive. Future studies can allow open responses
for regulation strategies rather than relying on a xed list. We also
examined each regulatory strategy separately, which precludes
us from examining how strategies work in tandem. Emerging
research suggests that people commonly use multiple regulatory
strategies within a single emotional episode and/or in response to a
single stressor (for a review, see Ford et al., 2019). Daily reports
may be suboptimal for assessing polyregulation because they do
not allow for assessments of temporality when someone
switches strategies over time (i.e., sequential polyregulation) dur-
ing the course of an emotion episode (e.g., Kalokerinos et al.,
2017). Future experience-sampling research may use assessment
approaches with shorter time intervals (e.g., ecological momentary
assessment) to capture short-term changes in strategy use. We also
did not assess the effectiveness of each strategy. A recent study
found that NED was associated with relatively successful emotion
regulation across a range of engagement and disengagement strat-
egies (Kalokerinos et al., 2019). Similar to our ndings, this study
also found inconsistent relationships between NED and strategy
selection.
Lastly, we did not assess other individual differences pertinent
to emotion regulation. While NED provides information about the
degree to which people distinguish their experience of negative
emotion, it does not provide metaemotional information (i.e., a
persons beliefs about various aspects of their emotions). The
beliefs and values people hold about their emotions are associated
with the strategies they select to regulate emotions (e.g., Ford &
Gross, 2019). For example, people who believe emotions are rela-
tively malleable tend to use more engagement strategies (for a
review, see Kneeland et al., 2016). People also differ in their valu-
ations of emotion (i.e., how helpful/acceptable vs. unhelpful/unac-
ceptable emotions are; see Ford & Gross, 2019), especially for
negative emotions (Luong et al., 2016). Individual differences in
how people value emotions inuence how people effectively regu-
late distress. One study found that participants who placed a higher
value on emotional control (e.g., people should control their emo-
tions more) used suppression more frequently to regulate emo-
tions than those who placed a lower value on emotional control
(Goodman et al., 2020). Additionally, when people appraise their
experience of stress as adaptive or functional, it can optimize their
response to stress (e.g., Crum et al., 2020;Jamieson et al., 2018).
People who appraise negative emotions as useful or meaningful
may be more inclined to identify the nuances across negative
states (i.e., higher NED).
Conclusion
The strategies people use to regulate distress predict important
psychological and physiological outcomes. The present study
examined how individual differences in NED moderated the rela-
tionship between the stress intensity of the daysmost stressful
event and strategy use. Although people higher in NED less fre-
quently used disengagement strategies, NED was largely unrelated
to strategy selection across varying levels of stress, suggesting that
NEDs protective effect may function differently in response to
specic stressful events. NED was also unrelated to use of engage-
ment strategies, suggesting that possible benets of NED on emo-
tion regulation processes are not specic to strategy selection. In
sum, these ndings support the notion that NED is protective
against the use of certain disengagement strategies (e.g., substance
use) and raise a set of important future directions, including identi-
fying when NED is most optimal.
References
Afifi, A. A., Kotlerman, J. B., Ettner, S. L., & Cowan, M. (2007). Methods
for improving regression analysis for skewed continuous or counted
responses. Annual Review of Public Health,28,95111. https://doi.org/
10.1146/annurev.publhealth.28.082206.094100
Aldao, A. (2013). The future of emotion regulation research: Capturing
context. Perspectives on Psychological Science,8(2), 155172. https://
doi.org/10.1177/1745691612459518
Aldao, A., & Dixon-Gordon, K. L. (2014). Broadening the scope of
research on emotion regulation strategies and psychopathology. Cogni-
tive Behaviour Therapy,43(1), 2233. https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073
.2013.816769
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regula-
tion strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical
Psychology Review,30(2), 217237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009
.11.004
Aldao, A., Sheppes, G., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation flexibil-
ity. Cognitive Therapy and Research,39(3), 263278. https://doi.org/10
.1007/s10608-014-9662-4
Anand, D., Chen, Y., Lindquist, K. A., & Daughters, S. B. (2017). Emotion
differentiation predicts likelihood of initial lapse following substance
use treatment. Drug and Alcohol Dependence,180, 439444. https://doi
.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.09.007
Armeli, S., Sullivan, T. P., & Tennen, H. (2015). Drinking to cope motiva-
tion as a prospective predictor of negative affect. Journal of Studies on
Alcohol and Drugs,76(4), 578584. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2015
.76.578
Barrett, L. F. (1997). The relationships among momentary emotion experi-
ences, personality descriptions, and retrospective ratings of emotion.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,23(10), 11001110. https://
doi.org/10.1177/01461672972310010
Barrett, L. F. (2006). Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the
experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review,
10(1), 2046. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2
Barrett, L. F., Gross, J., Christensen, T. C., & Benvenuto, M. (2001).
Knowing what youre feeling and knowing what to do about it: Mapping
the relation between emotion differentiation and emotion regulation.
Cognition and Emotion,15(6), 713724. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699
930143000239
Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2007). The ex-
perience of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology,58, 373403.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085709
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear
mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software,67(1),
148. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery
rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the
Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological),57(1), 289300.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D.,
Carmody, J., Segal, Z. V., Abbey, S., Speca, M., Velting, D., & Devins,
G. (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical
10 BROWN ET AL.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Psychology: Science and Practice,11(3), 230241. https://doi.org/10
.1093/clipsy.bph077
Boden, M. T., Thompson, R. J., Dizén, M., Berenbaum, H., & Baker, J. P.
(2013). Are emotional clarity and emotion differentiation related? Cog-
nition and Emotion,27(6), 961978. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931
.2012.751899
Bolger, N., & Laurenceau, J. P. (2013). Intensive longitudinal methods: An
introduction to diary and experience sampling research. Guilford Press.
Bonanno, G. A., & Burton, C. L. (2013). Regulatory flexibility: An indi-
vidual differences perspective on coping and emotion regulation. Per-
spectives on Psychological Science,8(6), 591612. https://doi.org/10
.1177/1745691613504116
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping
strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology,56(2), 267283. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514
.56.2.267
Cooper, M. L., Frone, M. R., Russell, M., & Mudar, P. (1995). Drinking to
regulate positive and negative emotions: A motivational model of alcohol
use. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,69(5), 9901005.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.990
Cranford, J. A., Shrout, P. E., Iida, M., Rafaeli, E., Yip, T., & Bolger, N.
(2006). A procedure for evaluating sensitivity to within-person change:
Can mood measures in diary studies detect change reliably? Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin,32(7), 917929. https://doi.org/10
.1177/0146167206287721
Crum, A. J., Jamieson, J. P., & Akinola, M. (2020). Optimizing stress: An
integrated intervention for regulating stress responses. Emotion,20(1),
120125. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000670
Cyders, M. A., Flory, K., Rainer, S., & Smith, G. T. (2009). The role of
personality dispositions to risky behavior in predicting first-year college
drinking. Addiction,104(2), 193202. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360
-0443.2008.02434.x
Demiralp, E., Thompson, R. J., Mata, J., Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M.,
Barrett, L. F., Ellsworth, P. C., Demiralp, M., Hernandez-Garcia, L.,
Deldin, P. J., Gotlib, I. H., & Jonides, J. (2012). Feeling blue or tur-
quoise? Emotional differentiation in major depressive disorder. Psycholo-
gical Science,23(11), 14101416. https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976124
44903
Doorley, J. D., Goodman, F. R., Kelso, K. C., & Kashdan, T. B. (2020).
Psychological flexibility: What we know, what we do not know, and
what we think we know. Social and Personality Psychology Compass,
14(12), Article e12566. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12566
Doré, B. P., Silvers, J. A., & Ochsner, K. N. (2016). Toward a personalized
science of emotion regulation. Social and Personality Psychology Com-
pass,10(4), 171187. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12240
Emery, N. N., Simons, J. S., Clarke, C. J., & Gaher, R. M. (2014). Emotion
differentiation and alcohol-related problems: The mediating role of ur-
gency. Addictive Behaviors,39(10), 14591463. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.addbeh.2014.05.004
English, T., Lee, I. A., John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2017). Emotion regula-
tion strategy selection in daily life: The role of social context and goals.
Motivation and Emotion,41(2), 230242. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031
-016-9597-z
Erbas, Y., Ceulemans, E., Kalokerinos, E. K., Houben, M., Koval, P., Pe,
M. L., & Kuppens, P. (2018). Why I dont always know what Im feel-
ing: The role of stress in within-person fluctuations in emotion differen-
tiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,115(2), 179191.
https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000126
Erbas, Y., Ceulemans, E., Lee Pe, M., Koval, P., & Kuppens, P. (2014).
Negative emotion differentiation: Its personality and well-being correlates
and a comparison of different assessment methods. Cognition and Emotion,
28(7), 11961213. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2013.875890
Ford, B. Q., & Gross, J. J. (2019). Why beliefs about emotion matter: An
emotion-regulation perspective. Current Directions in Psychological
Science,28(1), 7481. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721418806697
Ford, B. Q., Gross, J. J., & Gruber, J. (2019). Broadening our field of
view: The role of emotion polyregulation. Emotion Review,11(3),
197208. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073919850314
Ford, B. Q., & Troy, A. S. (2019). Reappraisal reconsidered: A closer look
at the costs of an acclaimed emotion-regulation strategy. Current Direc-
tions in Psychological Science,28(2), 195203. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0963721419827526
Friedlander, L. J., Reid, G. J., Shupak, N., & Cribbie, R. (2007). Social
support, self-esteem, and stress as predictors of adjustment to university
among first-year undergraduates. Journal of College Student Develop-
ment,48(3), 259274. https://doi.org/10.1353/csd.2007.0024
Gillanders, D. T., Bolderston, H., Bond, F. W., Dempster, M., Flaxman,
P. E., Campbell, L., Kerr, S., Tansey, L., Noel, P., Ferenbach, C.,
Masley, S., Roach, L., Lloyd, J., May, L., Clarke, S., & Remington, B.
(2014). The development and initial validation of the cognitive fusion
questionnaire. Behavior Therapy,45(1), 83101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.beth.2013.09.001
Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Hall, N. C., Nett, U. E., Pekrun, R., &
Lipnevich, A. A. (2014). Types of boredom: An experience sampling
approach. Motivation and Emotion,38(3), 401419. https://doi.org/10
.1007/s11031-013-9385-y
Gohm, C. L., & Clore, G. L. (2000). Individual differences in emotional
experience: Mapping available scales to processes. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin,26(6), 679697. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0146167200268004
Gohm, C. L., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Four emotion traits and their involve-
ment in attributional style, coping and well-being. Cognition and Emo-
tion,16(4), 495518. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000374
Goodman, F. R., Kashdan, T. B., &
_
Imamo
glu, A. (2020). Valuing emo-
tional control in social anxiety disorder: A multimethod study of emotion
beliefs and emotion regulation Emotion. Advance online publication.
https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000750
Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emo-
tion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and
initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal
of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment,26(1), 4154. https://
doi.org/10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integra-
tive review. Review of General Psychology,2(3), 271299. https://doi
.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271
Gross, J. J. (2015). The extended process model of emotion regulation:
Elaborations, applications, and future directions. Psychological Inquiry,
26(1), 130137. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2015.989751
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion
regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-
being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,85(2), 348362.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348
Howland, M., Armeli, S., Feinn, R., & Tennen, H. (2017). Daily emotional
stress reactivity in emerging adulthood: Temporal stability and its pre-
dictors. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping,30(2), 121132. https://doi.org/10
.1080/10615806.2016.1228904
Jamieson, J. P., Crum, A. J., Goyer, J. P., Marotta, M. E., & Akinola, M.
(2018). Optimizing stress responses with reappraisal and mindset inter-
ventions: An integrated model. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping,31(3),
245261. https://doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2018.1442615
John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regula-
tion: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span devel-
opment. Journal of Personality,72(6), 13011334. https://doi.org/10
.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00298.x
Kaiser, A. J., Milich, R., Lynam, D. R., & Charnigo, R. J. (2012). Negative
urgency, distress tolerance, and substance abuse among college students.
NEGATIVE EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION 11
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Addictive Behaviors,37(10), 10751083. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh
.2012.04.017
Kalokerinos, E. K., Erbas, Y., Ceulemans, E., & Kuppens, P. (2019). Dif-
ferentiate to regulate: Low negative emotion differentiation is associated
with ineffective use but not selection of emotion-regulation strategies.
Psychological Science,30(6), 863879. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797
619838763
Kalokerinos, E. K., Résibois, M., Verduyn, P., & Kuppens, P. (2017). The
temporal deployment of emotion regulation strategies during negative
emotional episodes. Emotion,17(3), 450458. https://doi.org/10.1037/
emo0000248
Kashdan, T. B., Barrett, L. F., & McKnight, P. E. (2015). Unpacking emo-
tion differentiation: Transforming unpleasant experience by perceiving
distinctions in negativity. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
24(1), 1016. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414550708
Kashdan, T. B., Disabato, D. J., Goodman, F. R., Doorley, J. D., &
McKnight, P. E. (2020). Understanding psychological flexibility: A
multimethod exploration of pursuing valued goals despite the presence
of distress. Psychological Assessment,32(9), 829850. https://doi.org/
10.1037/pas0000834
Kashdan, T. B., & Farmer, A. S. (2014). Differentiating emotions across
contexts: Comparing adults with and without social anxiety disorder
using random, social interaction, and daily experience sampling. Emo-
tion,14(3), 629638. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035796
Kashdan, T. B., Ferssizidis, P., Collins, R. L., & Muraven, M. (2010).
Emotion differentiation as resilience against excessive alcohol use: An
ecological momentary assessment in underage social drinkers. Psycho-
logical Science,21(9), 13411347. https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976
10379863
Kashdan, T. B., Goodman, F. R., Mallard, T. T., & DeWall, C. N. (2016).
What triggers anger in everyday life? Links to the intensity, control, and
regulation of these emotions, and personality traits. Journal of Personal-
ity,84(6), 737749. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12214
Kashdan, T. B., & Rottenberg, J. (2010). Psychological flexibility as a fun-
damental aspect of health. Clinical Psychology Review,30(7), 865878.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.001
Kehner, D., Locke, K. D., & Aurain, P. C. (1993). The influence of attribu-
tions on the relevance of negative feelings to personal satisfaction. Per-
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin,19(1), 2129. https://doi.org/10
.1177/0146167293191003
Khantzian, E. J. (1997). The self-medication hypothesis of substance use
disorders: A reconsideration and recent applications. Harvard Review of
Psychiatry,4(5), 231244. https://doi.org/10.3109/10673229709030550
Kihlstrom, J. F., Eich, E., Sandbrand, D., & Tobias, B. A. (1999). Emotion
and memory: Implications for self-report. In A. A. Stone, J. S. Turkkan,
C. A. Bachrach, J. B. Jobe, H. S. Kurtzman, & V. S. Cain (Eds.), The sci-
ence of self-report: Implications for research and practice (pp. 81100).
Erlbaum.
Kneeland, E. T., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Dovidio, J. F., & Gruber, J. (2016).
Emotion malleability beliefs influence the spontaneous regulation of
social anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research,40(4), 496509.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-016-9765-1
Kobyli
nska, D., & Kusev, P. (2019). Flexible emotion regulation: How sit-
uational demands and individual differences influence the effectiveness
of regulatory strategies. Frontiers in Psychology,10, Article 72. https://
doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00072
Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. (1992). Promises and problems with the cir-
cumplex model of emotion. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Review of personality
and social psychology: Vol. 13.Emotion (pp. 2559). Sage.
Lau, M. A., Bishop, S. R., Segal, Z. V., Buis, T., Anderson, N. D.,
Carlson, L., Shapiro, S., Carmody, J., Abbey, S., & Devins, G. (2006).
The Toronto mindfulness scale: Development and validation. Journal of
Clinical Psychology,62(12), 14451467. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp
.20326
Linehan, M. M. (2018). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline per-
sonality disorder. Guilford Press.
Luong, G., Wrzus, C., Wagner, G. G., & Riediger, M. (2016). When bad
moods may not be so bad: Valuing negative affect is associated with
weakened affecthealth links. Emotion,16(3), 387401. https://doi.org/
10.1037/emo0000132
Manne, S. L., Pape, S. J., Taylor, K. L., & Dougherty, J. (1999). Spouse
support, coping, and mood among individuals with cancer. Annals of
Behavioral Medicine,21(2), 111121. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF0290
8291
Naragon-Gainey, K., McMahon, T. P., & Chacko, T. P. (2017). The struc-
ture of common emotion regulation strategies: A meta-analytic examina-
tion. Psychological Bulletin,143(4), 384427. https://doi.org/10.1037/
bul0000093
Nook, E., Flournoy, J., Rodman, A. M., Mair, P., & McLaughlin, K. A.
(2020). High emotion differentiation buffers against internalizing symp-
toms following exposure to stressful life events in adolescence: An in-
tensive longitudinal study. PsyArXiv.https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/
q4uy8
OMalley, P. M., & Johnston, L. D. (2002). Epidemiology of alcohol and
other drug use among American college students. Journal of Studies on
Alcohol. Supplement,63(2), 2340. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsas.2002
.s14.23
Ortner, C. N. M., Marie, M. S., & Corno, D. (2016). Cognitive costs of
reappraisal depend on both emotional stimulus intensity and individual
differences in habitual reappraisal. PLoS ONE,11(12), Article e0167253.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0167253
Ottenstein, C., & Lischetzke, T. (2020). Development of a novel method
of emotion differentiation that uses open-ended descriptions of momen-
tary affective states. Assessment,27(8), 19281945. https://doi.org/10
.1177/1073191119839138
Plonsker, R., Gavish Biran, D., Zvielli, A., & Bernstein, A. (2017). Cogni-
tive fusion and emotion differentiation: Does getting entangled with our
thoughts dysregulate the generation, experience and regulation of emo-
tion? Cognition and Emotion,31(6), 12861293. https://doi.org/10
.1080/02699931.2016.1211993
Pond, R. S., Jr. Kashdan, T. B., DeWall, C. N., Savostyanova, A.,
Lambert, N. M., & Fincham, F. D. (2012). Emotion differentiation mod-
erates aggressive tendencies in angry people: A daily diary analysis.
Emotion,12(2), 326337. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025762
Revelle, W. R. (2019). psych: Procedures for psychological, psychometric,
and personality research (R package 1.9.4) [Computer software]. https://
CRAN.r-project.org/package=psych
Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: Evidence for
an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychological Bulletin,
128(6), 934960. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.934
Scherer, K. R., & Moors, A. (2019). The emotion process: Event appraisal
and component differentiation. Annual Review of Psychology,70,
719745. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011854
Schulz, U., & Schwarzer, R. (2004). Long-term effects of spousal support
on coping with cancer after surgery. Journal of Social and Clinical Psy-
chology,23(5), 716732. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.23.5.716.50746
Schwarz, N. (2011). Feelings-as-information theory. Handbook of Theories
of Social Psychology,1, 289308. https://doi.org/10.4135/97814462
49215.n15
Seah, T. S., Aurora, P., & Coifman, K. G. (2020). Emotion differentiation
as a protective factor against the behavioral consequences of rumination:
A conceptual replication and extension in the context of social anxiety.
Behavior Therapy,51(1), 135148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2019
.05.011
Selby, E. A., Anestis, M. D., Bender, T. W., & Joiner, T. E., Jr. (2009). An
exploration of the emotional cascade model in borderline personality
disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,118(2), 375387. https://doi
.org/10.1037/a0015711
12 BROWN ET AL.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
Settles, R. F., Cyders, M., & Smith, G. T. (2010). Longitudinal validation
of the acquired preparedness model of drinking risk. Psychology of
Addictive Behaviors,24(2), 198208. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017631
Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Emotion-regula-
tion choice. Psychological Science,22(11), 13911396. https://doi.org/
10.1177/0956797611418350
Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G., Radu, P., Blechert, J., & Gross, J. J.
(2014). Emotion regulation choice: A conceptual framework and sup-
porting evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,143(1),
163181. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030831
Shiffman, S., Stone, A. A., & Hufford, M. R. (2008). Ecological momen-
tary assessment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology,4,132. https://
doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091415
Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assess-
ing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin,86(2), 420428. https://doi
.org/10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.420
Sliwinski, M. J., Almeida, D. M., Smyth, J., & Stawski, R. S. (2009). Intra-
individual change and variability in daily stress processes: Findings
from two measurement-burst diary studies. Psychology and Aging,
24(4), 828840. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017925
Smith, C. A., & Kirby, L. D. (2011). The role of appraisal and emotion in
coping and adaptation. In R. J. Contrada & A. Baum (Eds.), The hand-
book of stress science: Biology, psychology, and health (pp. 195208).
Springer.
Srivastava, S., Tamir, M., McGonigal, K. M., John, O. P., & Gross, J. J.
(2009). The social costs of emotional suppression: A prospective study
of the transition to college. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy,96(4), 883897. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014755
Staff, J., Schulenberg, J. E., Maslowsky, J., Bachman, J. G., O'Malley,
P. M., Maggs, J. L., & Johnston, L. D. (2010). Substance use changes
and social role transitions: Proximal developmental effects on ongoing
trajectories from late adolescence through early adulthood. Development
and Psychopathology,22(4), 917932. https://doi.org/10.1017/S09545
79410000544
Starr, L. R., Hershenberg, R., Li, Y. I., & Shaw, Z. A. (2017). When feel-
ings lack precision: Low positive and negative emotion differentiation
and depressive symptoms in daily life. Clinical Psychological Science,
5(4), 613631. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702617694657
Starr, L. R., Hershenberg, R., Shaw, Z. A., Li, Y. I., & Santee, A. C.
(2020). The perils of murky emotions: Emotion differentiation moder-
ates the prospective relationship between naturalistic stress exposure
and adolescent depression. Emotion,20(6), 927938. https://doi.org/10
.1037/emo0000630
Suvak, M. K., Litz, B. T., Sloan, D. M., Zanarini, M. C., Barrett, L. F., &
Hofmann, S. G. (2011). Emotional granularity and borderline personal-
ity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,120(2), 414426. https://
doi.org/10.1037/a0021808
Tamir, M. (2009). What do people want to feel and why? Pleasure and util-
ity in emotion regulation. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
18(2), 101105. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01617.x
Tamir, M., Mitchell, C., & Gross, J. J. (2008). Hedonic and instrumental
motives in anger regulation. Psychological Science,19(4), 324328.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02088.x
Tennen, H., Affleck, G., Coyne, J. C., Larsen, R. J., & DeLongis, A.
(2006). Paper and plastic in daily diary research: A reply to Green et al
Psychological Methods,11(1), 112118. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082
-989X.11.1.112
Thompson, B. L., & Waltz, J. (2007). Everyday mindfulness and mindful-
ness meditation: Overlapping constructs or not? Personality and Individ-
ual Differences,43(7), 18751885. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007
.06.017
Torre, J. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2018). Putting feelings into words:
Affect labeling as implicit emotion regulation. Emotion Review,10(2),
116124. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073917742706
Trull, T. J., & Ebner-Priemer, U. W. (2009). Using experience sampling
methods/ecological momentary assessment (ESM/EMA) in clinical
assessment and clinical research: Introduction to the special section.
Psychological Assessment,21(4), 457462. https://doi.org/10.1037/
a0017653
Tugade, M. M., Fredrickson, B. L., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2004). Psycho-
logical resilience and positive emotional granularity: Examining the bene-
fits of positive emotions on coping and health. Journal of Personality,
72(6), 11611190. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00294.x
Van der Gucht, K., Dejonckheere, E., Erbas, Y., Takano, K.,
Vandemoortele, M., Maex, E., Raes, F., & Kuppens, P. (2019). An expe-
rience sampling study examining the potential impact of a mindfulness-
based intervention on emotion differentiation. Emotion,19(1), 123131.
https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000406
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and valida-
tion of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,54(6), 10631070. https://
doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
Wilcox, P., Winn, S., & Fyvie-Gauld, M. (2005). It was nothing to do
with the university, it was just the people: The role of social support in
the first-year experience of higher education. Studies in Higher Educa-
tion,30(6), 707722. https://doi.org/10.1080/03075070500340036
Wilhelm, F. H., & Grossman, P. (2010). Emotions beyond the laboratory:
Theoretical fundaments, study design, and analytic strategies for
advanced ambulatory assessment. Biological Psychology,84(3),
552569. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.01.017
Yoon, S., & Rottenberg, J. (2020). Why do people with depression use
faulty emotion regulation strategies? Emotion Review,12(2), 118128.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073919890670
Zaki, L. F., Coifman, K. G., Rafaeli, E., Berenson, K. R., & Downey, G.
(2013). Emotion differentiation as a protective factor against nonsuicidal
self-injury in borderline personality disorder. Behavior Therapy,44(3),
529540. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2013.04.008
Zeidener, M., Matthews, G., & Roberts, R. D. (2006). Emotional intelli-
gence, coping with stress, and adaptation. In J. Ciarrochi, J. P. Forgas, &
J. D. Mayer (Eds.), Emotional intelligence in everyday life (pp. 100125).
Psychology Press.
Received July 31, 2020
Revision received January 8, 2021
Accepted January 11, 2021 n
NEGATIVE EMOTION DIFFERENTIATION 13
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... In adults, both higher emotional clarity and negative emotion differentiation were associated with fewer selfreported emotion regulation difficulties (Jones & Herr, 2018;Vine & Aldao, 2014). More specifically, higher clarity and more negative differentiation were associated with less use of repetitive negative thinking, less use of putatively maladaptive emotion regulation strategies, and more effective implementation of regulatory behavior (Brown et al., 2021;Cabello et al., 2013;Gross & John, 2003;Kalokerinos et al., 2019;Starr et al., 2017;Vine & Aldao, 2014;Vine & Marroquín, 2018). However, all of these studies focused either on the associations of emotional clarity or emotion differentiation with emotion regulation. ...
Article
Full-text available
Emotional clarity and emotion differentiation (ED) are two core aspects of the application of emotional knowledge. During adolescence, novel emotional experiences result in temporary decreases of differentiation and clarity. These temporary difficulties might profoundly impact choices of regulatory strategies. And indeed, prior research has shown that lower emotional clarity and emotion differentiation are each associated with higher use of putatively maladaptive emotion regulation strategies in youth. The two constructs, however, are rarely examined together, and it remains unclear how they are associated in daily life, particularly in children and adolescents. In addition, previous studies have focused on the regulation of negative but not positive affect. To address these gaps, the present study used an intensive longitudinal design in youth. Between June 2021 and March 2022, 172 children and adolescents (M = 12.99 years) completed a 28-day diary (>3,500 entries in total) reporting daily affect, emotional clarity, and the use of five emotion regulation strategies in response to negative and positive affect (i.e., rumination, dampening, behavioral avoidance, negative and positive suppression). As predicted, on both between- and within-person levels, higher emotional clarity was associated with decreased use of all maladaptive emotion regulation strategies after adjusting for mean affect intensity. Results for emotion differentiation were mostly nonsignificant. Only higher daily positive emotion differentiation was associated with decreased rumination. In sum, this innovative study explores multiple aspects of emotional knowledge usage and regulation during a critical developmental stage and emphasizes the role of emotional clarity in the regulation of negative and positive affect.
... There were, however, mixed results about how NED may be related to the specific strategy choice. Some studies found that individuals with higher NED generally used more cognitive reappraisal (O'Toole et al., 2014) and less disengagement strategies such as avoidance, substance use, and distraction (Brown et al., 2021) in response to daily event. Two other studies yet found no or only weak associations between NED and the selection of putatively adaptive or maladaptive strategies after controlling the mean level of negative emotions (Kalokerinos et al., 2019;O'Toole et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Emotion differentiation emphasises labelling emotional experiences in a precise and context-sensitive way. Negative emotion differentiation (NED) has been found to be associated with mental health, where emotion regulation (ER) may act as a pathway. The current study aims to explore the association between NED and flexible ER implementation in daily life. Specifically, we examined how NED was associated with two aspects of ER flexibility: contextual synchrony and temporal ER variability. 101 college students (54% female; Mage = 20.24 years) reported their momentary emotions via a 7-day experience sampling protocol, and the intraclass correlation coefficients were calculated to reflect NED. In 10-day daily diaries, they also reported information about the most negative event during the day (i.e. event type, event intensity and ER goal) and how they regulated their emotions. The results revealed that individuals with high NED showed higher levels of synchrony between change in ER use and change in event type and ER goal. In addition, NED was positively associated with both within- and between-strategy variability in ER use. The results demonstrated that the ability to differentiate between negative emotions was related to higher ER flexibility, which shed new light on understanding the role of emotion differentiation in well-being.
... This approach to assessing granularity was previously used by Lee et al. (2017), who found that granularity scores derived from this day reconstruction task predicted EEG activity during affective picture viewing. In addition to predicting how affective stimuli are processed, the validity of granularity as computed based on the intercorrelation of on-line ratings of emotional experience has been extensively demonstrated through its association with affective outcomes including well-being Nook, 2021;O'Toole et al., 2014;Ottenstein, 2020;Starr et al., 2017), emotion regulation (Barrett et al., 2001;Brown et al., 2021;Hay & Diehl, 2011;Jones & Herr, 2018;Kalokerinos et al., 2019;O'Toole et al., 2014;Ottenstein, 2020), substance use behavior (Anand et al., 2017;Kashdan et al., 2010), and disordered eating (Jones & Herr, 2018;Mikhail et al., 2020). The procedures for computing granularity based on participants' emotion ratings are described in the Data Analysis section. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Psychological constructionist theories of emotion posit that individuals differ in their representation of emotions due to differences in the emotion knowledge they use during emotion construction. As a result, individuals are thought to differ in the extent to which they experience their emotions as being differentiated and distinct, a construct referred to as emotional granularity. Recently, it has been suggested that granularity may be a form of emotional expertise in so far as highly granular individuals possess structured knowledge about emotions which guides processes such as affective prediction and action (Hoemann et al., 2021). Even so, many of the predictions that follow from this characterization have yet to be tested. In the current study, I tested one such prediction that follows from the characterization of granularity as a form of emotional expertise: that highly granular individuals would demonstrate expertise-like effects for emotional memory. In particular, because prior research suggests that experts exhibit increased true and (under certain conditions) false memory for domain-relevant information, I predicted that highly granular individuals would show increased true and false emotional memory. Participants completed experience-sampling measures of their emotional experiences to assess granularity, behavioral tasks assessing true and false emotional memory, and several self-report measures of additional constructs thought to reflect emotional expertise. Contrary to predictions, overall granularity was unrelated to both true and false emotional memory. However, expertise-like effects were observed for negative granularity, alexithymia, and emotional clarity, as participants with higher levels of emotional expertise on these measures exhibited increased memory for images rated as being of a more extreme valence. Implications of these findings regarding the characterization of granularity as an expertise construct as well as directions for future research related to emotional expertise and memory are discussed.
... Emotional intensity is typically calculated by averaging the emotions over time so that higher scores indicate greater emotional intensity. For example, a 30day daily diary study assessed the emotional intensity by averaging the daily reports of eight negative emotions (e.g., sad, guilty) (Brown et al., 2021). Although emotional intensity is relatively static, it provides an overarching picture of an individual's basic emotional experiences, which are closely related to psychological well-being . ...
Article
Full-text available
Emotion dynamics have emerged as a transdiagnostic risk factor for psychopathology and mental health problems. However, despite their transdiagnostic importance, limited research has identified how emotion dynamics are shaped and characterized by adolescents’ daily family and school experiences. The present study explored whether interparental conflict, parent-adolescent conflict, and school problems were associated with adolescent emotion dynamics across positive emotions (PE) and negative emotions (NE). Participants were 163 adolescents between the ages of 12 and 14 (55% female; Mage = 12.79, SD = 0.73) and their parents who completed 10-day, twice-daily diary assessments recruited in Taiwan. Adolescents reported school problems and emotions in the afternoon, and parent-adolescent conflict and emotions in the evening, while parents reported interparental conflict in the evening. The results showed that interparental conflict was associated with greater NE intensity and variability, and lower PE intensity and inertia. Parent-adolescent conflict was associated with greater NE intensity, variability, and inertia, and lower PE intensity and greater PE inertia. School problems were associated with greater NE intensity, variability, and inertia, and lower PE intensity and greater PE variability and inertia. Furthermore, moderation tests indicated that interparental conflict was linked to NE intensity and school problems were linked to NE inertia only among adolescents with lower support seeking. The findings highlight the importance of daily family conflict and school problems in shaping adolescent emotion dynamics and underscore the protective role of support seeking in mitigating altered emotion dynamics against family conflict and school problems.
... According to the results, 28.6% of nurses experienced moderate anxiety, while 14.3% reported extreme anxiety. These results are consistent with the findings by [28] who emphasized the impact of work-related responsibilities on the mental health of nurses. Addressing anxiety through supportive measures, such as providing resources for stress management and fostering a positive work environment, is crucial for nurses' overall well-being. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: The goal of this study was to investigate the occurrence and intensity of stress and anxiety among nurses at Tamale Teaching Hospital, along with the factors that contribute to these conditions and the coping mechanisms employed by nurses. Methodology: A cross-sectional study design was used. The researchers collected demographic information from the participants, which included variables such as age, gender, level of education, and employment experience. Furthermore, the collection of data pertaining to perceived tension levels, anxiety, and coping strategies was accomplished by employing rigorously validated scales and questionnaires. Results: The results of the study indicated a notable prevalence of stress and anxiety among nurses, exhibiting varying levels of intensity. Several factors that contribute to stress and anxiety in individuals are workload, job demands, and work-related responsibilities. Nurses utilized a variety of coping strategies, which encompassed engaging in physical exercise and activities, as well as seeking social support. Conclusion: The research emphasizes the significance of enacting supportive policies, enhancing practice interventions, and allocating resources to effectively address the mental health requirements of nurses. Additional investigation is necessary in order to acquire a more profound comprehension of the fundamental factors and enduring consequences of stress and anxiety on the overall welfare of nurses.
Article
Objective: The purpose of this study is to determine the level of happiness and spousal support in the relationships of married women in the pandemic period. Methods: The sample of the descriptive and relationship-seeking type of research consisted of 1010 married. The data were obtained using the demographic questionnaire, the Relationship Happiness Scale (RHS), and the Spousal Support Scale (SSS). Results: The average of the Relationship Happiness Scale scores of the women participating in the study is 34.16±6.47, while the average of the Spousal Support Scale is 66.61±13.16. There is a statistically significant positive and high-level linear relationship between the RHS scores and the SSS scores (r=0.741). Conclusion: As a result of the research, it was determined that women's levels of spousal support and happiness in their relationships were high. It has been determined that the support women receive from their partners increases the happiness in their relationships.
Article
This study explores former school leaders' experiences and coping mechanisms transitioning to subordinate roles within educational settings. Using a qualitative case study design, data were collected through in-depth interviews with three purposively selected participants who previously held leadership positions and are now employed in subordinate roles within schools. Participants were licensed educators who met specific inclusion criteria, including prior leadership experience. Thematic analysis of the interview data revealed that these individuals faced emotional challenges, such as identity shifts and feelings of loss, but also demonstrated growth through humility and adaptability. Key coping mechanisms included building support networks, sharing knowledge, and managing time effectively. The findings emphasize the role of flexibility and resilience in navigating role transitions, highlighting their importance in promoting a collaborative and enriching educational environment.
Article
Full-text available
p style="text-align: justify;">The article reviews of a number of modern foreign studies of emotion differentiation and its role in emotion regulation processes. An attempt is made to generalise the currently existing results on the connection of emotional differentiation with various personality and behavioural constructs, as well as about its role of in psychological well-being. The main advantages of high negative emotional differentiation and disadvantages of low negative emotional differentiation are identified. Trait and momentary emotional differentiation was distinguished, difficulties in conceptualising and measuring the momentary emotional differentiation were outlined, the lines of future studies of the relationship between both measures of emotional differentiation and emotion regulation were described. Several studies of the role of emotional differentiation in the choice and effectiveness of putatively adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies were analysed. It was found that higher levels of negative emotional differentiation were related to the effectiveness of emotion regulation strategies rather than to their choice. Similarities and differences in the methodology and obtained results of the analysed studies are identified. Issues requiring further research are formulated, prospects for future research are described. The practical significance of the research findings for the use in psychotherapy and treatment of psychological disorders is outlined.</p
Article
Full-text available
Background Disruptions in emotion processing are common across psychological disorders. Research suggests that emotion differentiation (ED; specificity in language used to characterize one’s emotional experience) and emotional self-efficacy (ESE; perceived ability to understand and manage one’s emotions) are important transdiagnostic factors associated with various psychological benefits. Whether ED and ESE can be improved in adults remains largely unclear. Methods Using a longitudinal prospective design, we tested a brief online training targeting emotion word knowledge (vs. a control condition) to see if it improved negative ED (NED) and ESE in a college sample. Moreover, we tested if changes in NED or ESE mediated the effects of the training on levels of psychological distress one week and two-months post-intervention. Results Findings provided partial support for our hypotheses. Individuals whose ESE increased post-intervention reported lower levels of distress two months later. Moreover, exploratory analyses revealed those who demonstrated greater training engagement experienced increases in NED that in turn predicted lower distress one-week post-intervention. However, there were no direct effects of intervention group on NED or ESE and distress. Conclusions These findings highlight the potential of a remotely-administered emotion-language intervention to influence key dimensions of emotion processing and suggest avenues for further refinement. Both NED and ESE may be malleable for some, and that enhancements in ESE may produce long-term psychological benefits.
Article
Full-text available
We developed a multidimensional coping inventory to assess the different ways in which people respond to stress. Five scales (of four items each) measure conceptually distinct aspects of problem-focused coping (active coping, planning, suppression of competing activities, restraint coping, seeking of instrumental social support); five scales measure aspects of what might be viewed as emotion-focused coping (seeking of emotional social support, positive reinterpretation, acceptance, denial, turning to religion); and three scales measure coping responses that arguably are less useful (focus on and venting of emotions, behavioral disengagement, mental disengagement). Study 1 reports the development of scale items. Study 2 reports correlations between the various coping scales and several theoretically relevant personality measures in an effort to provide preliminary information about the inventory's convergent and discriminant validity. Study 3 uses the inventory to assess coping responses among a group of undergraduates who were attempting to cope with a specific stressful episode. This study also allowed an initial examination of associations between dispositional and situational coping tendencies.
Article
Full-text available
Psychological flexibility is the tendency to respond to situations in ways that facilitate valued goal pursuit. Psychological flexibility is particularly useful when challenges arise during goal pursuit that produce distress. In acceptance and commitment therapy, psychological flexibility is considered the pinnacle of emotional health and well-being. A growing body of research demonstrates that psychological flexibility leads to psychological benefits and adaptive behavior change. Yet, much of what we know, or think we know, about psychological flexibility hinges on a single measurement approach using the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (AAQ and AAQ-II). Research suggests the AAQ-II is highly correlated with distress itself rather than flexible responses to distress. Existing approaches that assess psychological flexibility ignore the context in which flexibility matters most: the pursuit of valued goals. Below, we review theory and research on psychological flexibility, including its associations with healthy functioning, its measurement, and its overlap with related constructs. We discuss how gaps between theory and measurement impede our understanding and review promising evidence for a new measure of psychological flexibility. We provide new research directions in an effort to create a more generalizable foundation of knowledge. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2020;e12566. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/spc3
Article
Full-text available
Psychological flexibility (PF), defined as the ability to pursue valued life aims despite the presence of distress, is a fundamental contributor to health (Kashdan & Rottenberg, 2010). Existing measures of PF have failed to consider the valued goals that give context for why people are willing to manage distress. Using 4 independent samples and 3 follow-up samples, we examined the role of PF in well-being, emotional experience and regulation, resilience, goal pursuit, and daily functioning. We describe the development and psychometric properties of the Personalized Psychological Flexibility Index (PPFI), which captures tendencies to avoid, accept, and harness discomfort during valued goal pursuit. Correlational, laboratory, and experience-sampling methods show that the PPFI measures a trait-like individual difference dimension that is related to a variety of well-being and healthy personality constructs. Unlike existing measures of PF, the PPFI was shown to be distinct from negative emotionality. Beyond trait measures, the PPFI is associated with effective daily goals and life strivings pursuit and adaptive emotional and regulatory responses to stressful life events. By adopting our measurement index, PF may be better integrated into mainstream theory and research on adaptive human functioning.
Article
Full-text available
This study examines relationships between emotion beliefs and emotion regulation strategy use among people with social anxiety disorder (SAD) and a psychologically healthy control group. Using experience-sampling methodology, we tested group differences in 2 types of emotion beliefs (emotion control values and emotion malleability beliefs) and whether emotion beliefs predicted trait and daily use of cognitive reappraisal and emotion suppression. People with SAD endorsed higher emotion control values and lower emotion malleability beliefs than did healthy controls. Across groups, emotion control values were positively associated with suppression (but unrelated to reappraisal), and emotion malleability beliefs were negatively associated with suppression and positively associated with reappraisal. We also addressed 2 exploratory questions related to measurement. First, we examined whether trait and state measures of emotion regulation strategies were related to emotion control values in different ways and found similar associations across measures. Second, we examined whether explicit and implicit measures of emotion control values were related to daily emotion regulation strategy use in different ways-and found that an implicit measure was unrelated to strategy use. Results are discussed in the context of growing research on metaemotions and the measurement of complex features of emotion regulation. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
Preprint
Full-text available
Exposure to stressful life events is strongly associated with internalizing psychopathology, and identifying factors that reduce vulnerability to stress-related internalizing problems is critical for development of early interventions. Drawing on research from affective science, we tested whether high emotion differentiation—the ability to specifically identify one’s feelings—buffers adolescents from developing internalizing symptoms when exposed to stress. Thirty adolescents completed a laboratory measure of emotion differentiation before an intensive year-long longitudinal study in which exposure to stress and internalizing problems were assessed at both the moment-level (n=4,921 experience sampling assessments) and monthly-level (n=355 monthly assessments). High negative and positive emotion differentiation attenuated moment-level coupling between perceived stress and feelings of depression, and high negative emotion differentiation eliminated monthly-level associations between stressful life events and anxiety symptoms. These results suggest that high emotion differentiation buffers adolescents against anxiety and depression in the face of stress, perhaps by facilitating adaptive emotion regulation.
Article
Full-text available
The dominant cultural valuation of stress is that it is “bad for me.” This valuation leads to regulatory goals of reducing or avoiding stress. In this article, we propose an alternative approach—stress optimization—which integrates theory and research on stress mindset (e.g., Crum, Salovey, & Achor, 2013) and stress reappraisal (e.g., Jamieson, Mendes, Blackstock, & Schmader, 2010) interventions. We further integrate these theories with the extended process model of emotion regulation (Gross, 2015). In so doing, we explain how altering second-level valuation systems—shifting the valuation of stress from “is bad for me” to “can be good for me”—fundamentally changes the overarching goal of stress regulation from reducing stress to optimizing stress responses to achieve valued goals. With this optimization goal in mind, individuals are invited to flexibly identify, select, and engage in specific regulation tactics (e.g., situation selection, attentional control, cognitive change, and response modulation) in ways that help them achieve valued ends as opposed to merely reducing or avoiding stressful experiences. We discuss definitions and issues related to key terms including stress, stressors, stress responses, and stress regulation and outline a research agenda for testing this new integrated theory as an intervention.
Article
Full-text available
Negative emotion differentiation (NED) refers to the ability to identify and label discrete negative emotions. Low NED has been previously linked to depression and other indices of low psychological well-being. However, this construct has rarely been explored during adolescence, a time of escalating depression risk, or examined in the context of naturalistic stressors. Further, the association between NED and depression has never been tested longitudinally. We propose a diathesis-stress model wherein low NED amplifies the association between stressful life events (SLEs) and depression. A sample of 233 community-recruited midadolescents (Mage 15.90 years, 54% female) completed diagnostic interviews and reported on mood and daily stressors 4 times per day for 7 days. SLEs were assessed using a semistructured interview with diagnosis-blind team coding based on the contextual threat method. Follow-up interviews were conducted 1.5 years after baseline. Low NED was correlated with depression but did not predict prospective changes in depression as a main effect. Confirming predictions and supporting a diathesis-stress model, low NED predicted (a) within-subjects associations between daily hassles and momentary depressed mood, (b) between-subjects associations between SLE severity and depression, and (c) prospective associations between SLE severity and increases in depression at follow-up. Results were specific to negative (vs. positive) emotion differentiation. Results suggest that low NED is primarily depressogenic in the context of high stress exposure. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Rumination is thought to play a central role in affective disorders such as social anxiety disorder (SAD). Past research indicates that rumination tends to exacerbate negative emotions and increase the risk of engaging in maladaptive coping behaviors (e.g., avoiding social activities). However, little is known on how to effectively protect against the negative outcomes of rumination. Previously, Zaki, Coifman, Rafaeli, Berenson, and Downey (2013) found that negative emotion differentiation (NED) protected against rumination and nonsuicidal self-injury in borderline personality disorder. Nevertheless, it is unclear whether this protective effect would extend to other populations and behaviors. Therefore, the present investigation sought to replicate and extend Zaki et al.'s (2013) findings in the context of SAD. In two studies, we examined if NED would moderate the positive association between rumination and frequency of social avoidance. Study 1 involved 29 individuals who met criteria for SAD with or without co-occurring major depressive episode, while Study 2 involved a nonclinical sample of 190 college students. All participants completed a measure of rumination and an experience-sampling diary which provided indices of NED and social avoidance. The results from both studies were unanimous: NED significantly moderated the relationship between rumination and social avoidance such that the positive association between rumination and social avoidance was significant for low but not moderate to high NED. Overall, the findings provide a conceptual replication of Zaki et al. (2013) and further evidence for the protective effects of NED against the maladaptive behavioral consequences of rumination across populations.
Article
Why do people with psychopathology use less adaptive and more maladaptive strategies for negative emotions if such usage has self-destructive consequences? Although researchers have examined the reasons for people’s engagement in maladaptive “behaviors,” such as nonsuicidal self-injury, surprisingly little attention has been paid to the reasons why people might endorse maladaptive emotion regulation (ER) strategies. This article addresses this question, focusing on the case of depression, evaluating an array of 10 possible explanations. After considering the existing evidence, we provide a blueprint to help the field reach stronger conclusions about depression and other forms of psychopathology. Better understanding of the origins of healthy/unhealthy ER has implications for clinical science, clinical practice, as well as their integration.
Article
The field of emotion regulation has developed rapidly, and a number of emotion regulatory strategies have been identified. To date, empirical attention has focused on contrasting specific regulation strategies to determine their unique profile of consequences. However, it is becoming clear that people commonly pursue multiple regulation approaches within any given emotional episode (e.g., pursuing different regulation goals, strategies, or tactics). We refer to the concurrent or sequential use of multiple approaches to regulate emotions within a single emotion episode as polyregulation. Here, we extend existing theoretical frameworks of emotion regulation to consider polyregulation. We then pose several core questions to summarize and inspire research on polyregulation, thereby improving our understanding of emotion regulation as it unfolds in everyday life.