Access to this full-text is provided by PLOS.
Content available from PLOS One
This content is subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Effect of disclosing the relation between effort
and unit reliability on system reliability: An
economic experiment
Ryoji Makino
1
, Kenju AkaiID
2
*, Jun-ichi Takeshita
1
, Takanori Kudo
3
, Keiko Aoki
4
1National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology, Onogawa, Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan,
2Shimane University, Izumo, Shimane, Japan, 3Setsunan University, Neyagawa, Osaka, Japan, 4Kyushu
University, Nishi-ku, Fukuoka, Japan
*akai@med.shimane-u.ac.jp
Abstract
The purpose is to experimentally examine the effect of disclosing the risk probability of each
unit in a production system on human behavior and the resulting system reliability. We used
an economic experiment based on the theoretical model of Hausken (2002) to evaluate the
effect of disclosing the relation between effort and unit reliability. We conducted first the
non-disclosed-risk experiment and then the disclosed-risk experiment within subjects in
both series and parallel systems. Our experimental results show that disclosing the relation
between effort and unit reliability has two positive effects. First, subjects succeeded in
improving the system reliability while cutting back on efforts to reduce the risk of their
units when the risk probability was disclosed. In each system, the disclosed-risk condition
achieves significantly higher system reliability on average than does the non-disclosed-risk
condition, although the average level of effort is significantly lower under the disclosed-risk
condition than under the non-disclosed-risk condition. Second, disclosing the risk probability
simplified the subjects’ decision-making process and reduced its cost because subjects
made their decisions on the amount of effort to exert based only on the risk probability infor-
mation without considering other factors, such as the number of accidents.
1. Introduction
The role of human behavior and decision-making in the design and operation of engineering
systems, including those in the chemical, aviation, nuclear, health care, and construction
industries, is crucial [1]. Therefore, it has been argued that human behavior should be consid-
ered in probabilistic risk analysis (PRA) to assess system reliability [2]. However, PRA has
primarily taken a non-behavioral, physical engineering approach to estimating risk and assess-
ing the reliability of a system [3], mainly because of the difficulty in understanding human
behavior.
Hausken (2002) presented a way to deal with this issue by integrating game theory into
PRA [3]. He introduced models that express production systems in which workers are
equipped with units that are connected in series (called a “series system”) and in parallel (called
PLOS ONE
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722 April 7, 2021 1 / 18
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Makino R, Akai K, Takeshita J-i, Kudo T,
Aoki K (2021) Effect of disclosing the relation
between effort and unit reliability on system
reliability: An economic experiment. PLoS ONE
16(4): e0249722. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.
pone.0249722
Editor: Camelia Delcea, Bucharest University of
Economic Studies, ROMANIA
Received: October 16, 2020
Accepted: March 23, 2021
Published: April 7, 2021
Peer Review History: PLOS recognizes the
benefits of transparency in the peer review
process; therefore, we enable the publication of
all of the content of peer review and author
responses alongside final, published articles. The
editorial history of this article is available here:
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722
Copyright: ©2021 Makino et al. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited.
Data Availability Statement: All relevant data are
within the manuscript and its Supporting
information files.
a “parallel system”), and he analyzed the situation in which (i) system reliability depends on
people’s intentional efforts to reduce risk and (ii) people influence each other in deciding the
degree of effort. In particular, he focused on the free-rider problem that emerges when expend-
ing resources to reduce risk and demonstrated that when players place different values on sys-
tem reliability, a conflict of resource allocation arises. His study aimed to merge the behavioral
theory of conflict with the physical world to produce more accurate estimates of system reli-
ability. Hausken’s (2002) work was significant because it provided the first organized linkage
between reliability analysis based on PRA and game theory in which system reliability was
treated as a public good [4].
Integrated models of PRA and game theory are important for a number of reasons. First,
such models take into account the intentional behavior of people rather than random events
or acts of nature, which can easily be assessed by ordinary PRA models. Second, they can serve
as base models for the application of mechanism design theory to design a system that reduces
risk [5]. In addition, since the work of Hausken (2002), a number of theoretical and simula-
tion-based studies of PRA and game theory have been presented in the field of anti-terrorism
policies [6–8], so these models seem to have useful real-world applications. The original idea
of Hausken who refer to Harshlierfer’s idea of home and security. The series system is for the
dike to protect an island from floods and the parallel system is for anti-missile battery that
each citizen maintains to prevent missile. Our idea is also based on such a home and security
so that we show the future issues applying for hazard map for disasters.
A number of previous studies, including Hausken (2000), have theoretically analyzed the
models with complete information and not dealt with those with incomplete information. In
that context, it is assumed that the risk probability is disclosed to the players of the game
model and that they know the relationship between their strategies and the resulting equilib-
rium risk level.
Thus, in the context of an integrated model of PRA and game theory, this study aims to
experimentally examine the effect of disclosing the risk probability of each unit in a production
system on human behavior and on the resulting reliability of the production system using
human subjects in a laboratory. An economic experiment based on the theoretical model of
Hausken (2002) was employed to achieve this goal. Following Hausken (2002), we assume that
the player has the same risk in each unit, that is, symmetric risk condition.
To evaluate the effect of disclosing the relation between effort and unit reliability, we con-
ducted one experiment in which the risk probability was disclosed to subjects (hereafter, “the
disclosed-risk condition”) and one in which the risk probability was not disclosed (hereafter,
“the non-disclosed-risk condition”). Comparing the results of these experiments, we can
understand the effect of disclosing the relation between effort and unit reliability, and we focus
on understanding how subjects’ behavior and the system’s reliability are affected by disclosing
the relation between effort and unit reliability.
This analysis is related to the further development suggested by Hausken (2002), which is a
case of games with incomplete information. Theoretical literature of incomplete information
related to this study is Bier, Oliveros, Samuelson (2007) and Hausken (2014) [9,10]. Both of
them analyze the situation in which defenders have information asymmetry for attackers in
the home and security problem. However, as our best of knowledge, there is no experiment
which shows the impact of disclosing probability of risks. Therefore, it is not clear what will
happen to player’s behavior and system reliability when information about risk is incomplete.
This is the first study that has analyzed the integrated model of PRA and game theory using an
economic experiment.
Additionally, the theoretical model is calculated in the one-shot game in which the players
only play the game once. We extend this situation into the repeated game to see the interaction
PLOS ONE
Relation between effort and unit reliability on system reliability
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722 April 7, 2021 2 / 18
Funding: This study was supported by JSPS
KAKENHI Grant Number 15K01237 (Recipient:
Ryoji Makino) and Leading Initiative for Excellent
Young Researchers, MEXT, Japan (Recipient:
Kenju Akai).
Competing interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist.
effect between players. We statistically analyze what happens after they experience the accident
or know the counterpart’s effort. We believe this experiment is worthwhile because it could
shed new light on how the disclosure of risk probability affects the level of system reliability.
The structure of the rest of this paper is as follows. The next section describes the model
developed by Hausken (2002) and makes the game theoretic prediction of subjects’ behavior
in our experiment. Section 0 presents the experimental design and procedure. We show the
experimental results and provide discussions in sections 0 and 0. Section 0 concludes the
article.
2. Theoretical model
2.1 Description and notations
The design of our experiment is based on the theoretical model presented by Hausken (2002)
to study the system reliability of a series, parallel, summation, or combined system. A system
can represent, for example, a factory consisting of some machine units connected in various
ways. In our experiment, we examine a series system and a parallel system, each consisting of
two units, which are the most fundamental classes. This subsection both explains our experi-
mental setting and also serves as a brief and simplified description of Hausken’s (2002) model.
Fig 1 depicts the structure of a series system. It is assumed that unit i(= 1, 2) is equipped
with maintenance worker i, so there are two workers in a system. Hausken (2002) provided
examples of a series system showing (i) a chain that is not stronger than its weakest link and
(ii) a case of flood protection on an island presented by Hirshleifer [11,12].
The reliability of unit iin period t, which is the probability of unit icontinuing to function
in period t= 0, 1,,, T, depends on maintenance worker i’s behavior in period t. Maintenance
worker ican increase the reliability of his/her unit in period t,P
i,t
by making efforts e
i,t
to
maintain it. We assume maintenance workers have to incur costs to make efforts. The relation-
ship between the reliability of unit iand the effort level of worker iin period tis expressed as
P
i,t
=p(e
i,t
), p0(e
i,t
)>0. In other words, unit ibreaks down with probability 1 −p(e
i,t
)when
worker i’s effort level is e
i,t
. Note that an accident occurs with probability 1 −p(e
i,t
) in each
period t.
The reliability of an entire series system depends on the reliability of each unit. Since a
series system functions when both units function, the reliability of such a system in period
t,P
series,t
, is given by P
series,t
=p(e
1,t
)p(e
2,t
).
The subjects in our experiment play the role of maintenance workers, and they decide how
much effort to exert. Depending on their effort levels, the reliability of their system varies. An
accident (one that is, needless to say, hypothetical in our experiment) happens with probability
Fig 1. Series system.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722.g001
PLOS ONE
Relation between effort and unit reliability on system reliability
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722 April 7, 2021 3 / 18
1−p(e
1,t
)p(e
2,t
). As explained later, if an accident happens during one period in the experi-
ment, subjects do not receive a reward for that period.
In our experiment, efforts to maintain the machine unit are represented by the payment of
“tokens” out of an endowment provided by the experimenter at the beginning of a period. The
actual relationship between the effort level and the unit reliability applied in the experiment is
shown in Table 1 and is the same in all sessions.
Subjects face a trade-off between the reliability of their unit and the cost of their efforts.
They can reduce the risk of not being paid due to accidents by making efforts to maintain their
units. However, in order to do that, they have to incur costs.
Furthermore, subjects face not only the above trade-off but also interdependence with their
co-workers. It might be optimal for a worker to make little effort if his/her co-worker makes
enough effort. In our experiment, subjects must decide their own effort levels before observing
their co-workers’ effort levels, which means they play simultaneous-move games.
Fig 2 depicts the structure of a parallel system. The analysis of a parallel system is the same
as that of a series system except for the system reliability. The reliability of a parallel system is
expressed as P
parallel.t
= 1 −(1 −p(e
i,t
))(1 −p(e
i,t
)). Note that this type of system breaks down
only if both units break down simultaneously, so it is, in general, more robust than a series sys-
tem is. As Hausken (2002) showed, subjects in a parallel system are expected to make less effort
than those in a series system because they have an incentive to free ride on their co-worker’s
effort. This free-rider behavior is driven by the fact that the reliability of a parallel system
remains high when at least one maintenance worker makes enough effort and the reliability of
his/her machine unit is high.
Table 1. Relation between effort and unit reliability.
Effort e
i,t
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Unit reliability p(e
i,t
) 0.20 0.20 0.60 0.60 0.60 0.60 0.90 0.90 0.90 0.90 0.90
Effort e
i,t
−11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
Unit reliability p(e
i,t
)−0.90 0.95 0.95 0.95 0.95 0.95 0.95 0.95 0.99 0.99
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722.t001
Fig 2. Parallel system.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722.g002
PLOS ONE
Relation between effort and unit reliability on system reliability
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722 April 7, 2021 4 / 18
2.2 Parameter settings
First, we describe the payoffs in the series system. In each period, each of the two subjects
(i= 1, 2) in a group receives an endowment of 20 tokens. Subjects can either keep these tokens
for themselves or pay e
i,t
tokens (0 e
i,t
20) to increase the unit reliability. Their decisions
about e
i,t
are made simultaneously. Depending on e
1,t
and e
2,t
, the reliability of the groups’
series system is computed according to the formula p(e
1,t
)p(e
2,t
). When an (hypothetical)
accident does not happen, each subject in each group receives 25 tokens. This payment repre-
sents the wage for their maintenance work and can be interpreted as a scaling factor bof Haus-
ken’s model. An accident happens with a risk probability of 1 −p(e
1,t
)p(e
2,t
), and, in that
case, the subjects in the group receive nothing. The expected payoff for each subject iin period
tis given by
pe
series;i;t¼20 ei;tþ25 Y
2
i¼1
pðei;tÞ:
The total payoff that subjects earn in the non-disclosed-risk (disclosed-risk) condition of
the series system is the sum of the period payoffs over the first (last) 30 periods of the session,
as explained in section 0.
Second, in the parallel system, only the payoff function for the subjects differs from the
series system. The expected payoff in each period for each subject iis now expressed as fol-
lows:
pe
parallel;i;t¼20 ei;tþ25f1Y2
i¼1½1pðei;tÞg:
2.3 Nash equilibrium
Each player maximizes his/her expected utility in each period given that another player is also
maximizing his/her expected utility. The equilibrium concept widely used in economic analy-
sis is the Nash equilibrium, which is defined as the strategy profile from which no player has
an incentive to deviate if the other players do not deviate [13]. In the series system, a strategy
profile e
t¼ ðe
1;t;e
2;tÞ, where the values in the brackets stand for players’ effort levels mea-
sured by the number of tokens, is a Nash equilibrium if
20 e
1;tþ25pðe
1;tÞpðe
2;tÞ 20 e1;tþ25pðe1;tÞpðe
2;tÞ
20 e
2;tþ25pðe
1;tÞpðe
2;tÞ 20 e2;tþ25pðe
1;tÞpðe2;tÞ
8ei;t2 f1;2;...;20g;ði¼1;2Þ:
In the parallel system, a Nash equilibrium ðe
1;t;e
2;tÞis expressed as follows:
20 e
1;tþ25f1 ½1pðe
1;tÞ½1pðe
2;tÞg 20 e1;tþ25f1 ½1pðe1;tÞ½1pðe
2;tÞg
20 e
2;tþ25f1 ½1pðe
1;tÞ½1pðe
2;tÞg 20 e2;tþ25f1 ½1pðe
1;tÞ½1pðe2;tÞg
8ei;t2 f1;2;...;20g;ði¼1;2Þ:
In our experimental setting, especially when the relationship between the reliability of unit i
and the effort level of worker ishown in Table 1 is disclosed to the subjects, the Nash equilibria
PLOS ONE
Relation between effort and unit reliability on system reliability
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722 April 7, 2021 5 / 18
are (0, 0) and (6, 6) for the series system and (0, 6), (6, 0), and (3, 3) for the parallel system.
The Nash equilibrium in the series system implies that subjects pay nothing or pay all of their
efforts so that the accidents will frequently occur. On the other hand, the Nash equilibrium in
the parallel system implies that one of the subjects will become a free rider but the frequency of
accidents will be decreased.
3. Experimental design and procedures
3.1 Design
Our experiment consists of four treatment conditions, as presented in Table 2. These condi-
tions include a series system with and without the disclosure of the risk probability of a unit
(the disclosed-risk condition and the non-disclosed-risk condition, respectively) and a parallel
system with and without this disclosure. There are six experimental sessions altogether. Ses-
sions 1, 2, and 3 are for the series system and sessions 4, 5, and 6 are for the parallel system.
We employ within-subject design to evaluate the difference in the behavior between the sys-
tem with and without the disclosure of the risk probability of a unit. Each session consists of
60 periods. In the first 30 periods, subjects play the game under the non-disclosed-risk condi-
tion, which means that they do not know the quantitative relationship between their effort
level and the reliability of their unit. As mentioned earlier, they know that the unit reliability is
a non-decreasing function of the effort level. In the last 30 periods, the subjects play the same
game under the disclosed-risk condition, which means that they do understand the relation-
ship between their effort level and the reliability of their unit.
The total number of subjects in a session is N= 12, and the subjects are randomly parti-
tioned into six groups of size n= 2 in each of the 60 periods. Thus, the group composition is
randomly changed from period to period. In a given session, the same Nsubjects play 30 peri-
ods under the non-disclosed-risk condition and 30 periods under the disclosed-risk condition.
The model proposed by Hausken (2002) assumes complete information, which means the
players know the specific functional form of p(e
i,t
). However, in a real work situation, reliability
(in other words, the risk level) is rarely known. Accordingly, we explore experimentally how sub-
jects’ decision-making is affected and how the resulting system reliability varies when the risk
probability is not disclosed. This experiment is expected to shed new light on the role of disclos-
ing the relation between effort and unit reliability in enhancing the reliability of various systems.
3.2 Procedures
The experiment was conducted at Niigata University. The subjects were students from various
faculties, including economics. Subjects were recruited through a flyer and voluntarily regis-
tered their names and email addresses on the website. When registering, they had to agree to a
consent form explaining the freedom to opt out, the rewards to be gained from the results of
Table 2. Treatment conditions.
Sessions 1–3: Series system
60 periods in each session
Sessions 4–6: Parallel system
60 periods in each session
Non-disclosed-
risk condition
(First 30 periods)
• 12 subjects in each session
• Six groups of size n = 2 in each period
• Random group composition in each period
• 12 subjects in each session
• Six groups of size n= 2 in each period
• Random group composition in each period
Disclosed-risk
condition
(Last 30 periods)
Conditions are the same as the non-disclosed-
risk condition other than the disclosure of the
risk probability
Conditions are the same as the non-disclosed-
risk condition other than the disclosure of the
risk probability
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722.t002
PLOS ONE
Relation between effort and unit reliability on system reliability
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722 April 7, 2021 6 / 18
the experiment, and the anonymity of the data. A paper consent form was distributed and read
to all subjects at the beginning of the experiment to reaffirm the contents of the consent form.
Subjects had to agree to the content and sign and submit the consent form in order to partici-
pate in the experiment.
The data collection complied with the Law on the Protection of Personal Information in
Japan. All institutions and universities to which the author belongs did not require ethical
approval for science research, except in instances that could be deemed life-threatening or
harmful to human subjects. Subjects were not informed of the identities of other group mem-
bers. Each subject will be allowed to participate in only one session.
Although the series and parallel systems follow the same procedure, they use different for-
mulas to compute system reliability. The experiment is conducted in a computerized labora-
tory where subjects interact anonymously with each other. To conduct the experiment, we use
the experimental software “z-Tree” developed by Fischbacher who defines it as “The z-Tree
software is implemented as a client-server application with a server application for the experi-
menter,called z-Tree,and a client application for the subjects,called z-Leaf.The applications are
programmed in C++ (Visual C++ 2015,MFC) and run on all recent released x86 32 bit and 64
bit versions of Windows,starting with Windows XP SP3.”[14]
The experimental operation process is as follows:
Step1.(Deciding subjects): More than twelve subjects including two or three extras wait in the
lobby before the session starts. Then, they are selected by the lottery to be decide who join
the actual session. The selected twelve subjects are guided to the experimental room which
has twelve laptops. The person who failed by the lottery receive 500 Japanese Yen (about
4.20 USD at that time) to leave this session. They have a chance to join the other session.
Step2.(Reading consent form): The experimenter read aloud the consent form and the subjects
hand it to the experimenter if they agree with it.
Step3.(Reading instruction): The non-disclosed-risk condition starts. The subjects listen to the
recorded instruction and read it silently by themselves at the same time. The instruction
explains that the experiment has two parts and that each part has 30 periods. Then, the
actions that subjects can take in the experiment and the method for computing the reward
that subjects receive at the end of the experiment are presented. Then, they are given 7 min-
utes to review the instruction and make a strategy.
Step4.(Decision making): At the beginning of the first period, they are randomly divided into
six groups of two subjects by the computer. Each subject receives 20 tokens as his/her
endowment. They decide how many tokens they wish to pay to maintain the machine unit
before observing the number of tokens paid by their counterpart and without knowing the
functional form of p(e
i,t
) shown in Table 1.
Step5.(Simulating accident): According to the number of tokens paid, the reliability of each
machine unit and, hence, the system reliability of the group is computed. The occurrence of
an accident is simulated based on the computed system reliability.
Step6.(Calculating payoffs): The result of accident simulation is revealed to the subjects. When
an accident happens, the payoffs in that period for the subjects in the group are determined
as π
i,t
= 20 −e
i,t
. When an accident does not happen, the payoffs are π
i,t
= 20 −e
i,t
+ 25.
Step7.(Revealing the counterpart’s effort): The subjects can see the number of tokens paid by
their counterpart, whether or not an accident happened, and his/her own payoffs in the
period. That is the final step in the first period.
PLOS ONE
Relation between effort and unit reliability on system reliability
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722 April 7, 2021 7 / 18
Step8 (Repeating the periods): After the first period completes, the second period begins.
Groups are randomly changed, each subject receives another endowment of 20 tokens,
and the process continues as described in the above seven steps. These procedures are con-
ducted in each period until the 30th period ends. Subject i’s total payoff for the first 30 peri-
ods (i.e., the non-disclosed-risk condition) is computed as P30
t¼1pi;t.
Step9.(Revealing the probability): The disclosed-risk condition starts. At the beginning of the
31st period, the experimenter provides Table 1 to the subjects and explain that the experi-
mental procedure is the same as that of the first 30 periods except that they can see the risk
of accident.
Step10.(Repeating the periods): Same six steps, Steps 3 to 8, in the non-disclosure condition
repeated from 31 to 60th periods. After the 60th period ends, subject i’s total payoff for the
last 30 periods is computed as P60
t¼31 pi;t.
Step11.(Paying rewards): The reward actually paid to a subject is a random selection of either
the total payoff from the non-disclosed-risk condition or that from the disclosed-risk con-
dition. An exchange rate of 1 token = 3 Japanese Yen is applied.
4. Experimental results
4.1 Subject
We have observations from 36 subjects for the series system and the parallel system, respec-
tively. Sessions 1 and 4 were held in November 2013, and the other sessions were held in Janu-
ary 2014. Each experimental session lasted about two hours, and subjects earned, on average,
4,228 Japanese Yen (about US $40 at the time), including a participation fee of 1,500 Japanese
Yen. Table 3 shows the gender, average rewards and extra subjects who failed by the lottery in
each session.
4.2 Series system
4.2.1 Contributions. The average number of tokens paid by the subjects (hereafter, called
“contributions”) are shown in Table 4, which summarizes our experimental results. The aver-
age contribution under the non-disclosed-risk condition, 5.43 (S.D. = 5.90), is higher than that
under the disclosed-risk condition, 4.78 (S.D. = 3.77). The difference is statistically significant
(p-value <0.01). The average and standard deviation of the contributions in a period are rep-
resented in Fig 3A–3D by a dot and its corresponding vertical line, respectively. The upper left
and upper right panels of Fig 3A–3D reflect the series system under the non-disclosed-risk
condition and the disclosed-risk condition, respectively. Although the average contribution
under the non-disclosed-risk condition gradually decreases and approaches that under the
Table 3. Demographics.
Series Parallel
Session 1 Session 2 Session 3 Session 4 Session 5 Session 6
Number of Male 8 5 5 5 5 9
Number of Female 4 7 7 7 7 3
Average payoffs (JPY) 3904 3415 4041 4616 4603 4788
Number of people dropped by lottery 3 2 4 2 4 7
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722.t003
PLOS ONE
Relation between effort and unit reliability on system reliability
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722 April 7, 2021 8 / 18
Table 4. Summary of the experimental results.
Non-disclosed-risk (1–30 periods) Disclosed-risk (31–60 periods) p-value of mean test:
Series system (a) 5.43 (5.90) (a) 4.78 (3.77) <0.01
(b) 0.31 (0.28) (b) 0.49 (0.29) <0.01
(c) 4.06 (1.33) (c) 3.10 (1.14) <0.01
(d) 680.19 (82.84) (d) 819.25 (85.18) <0.01
Parallel system (a) 3.96 (4.85) (a) 2.45 (2.86) <0.01
(b) 0.75 (0.23) (b) 0.78 (0.18) <0.01
(c) 1.41 (0.99) (c) 1.47 (1.05) 0.71
(d) 1054.89 (73.82) (d) 1093.22 (63.72) 0.02
(a) Average contribution [tokens], (b) Average system reliability [–], (c) Average number of accidents per period, (d)
Average payoff over all 30 periods, excluding the participation fee [tokens]
Values in parentheses are standard deviations.
,, and denote statistical significance at the 10%, 5%, and 1% levels, respectively.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722.t004
Fig 3. A-D. Average and standard deviation of each period’s contribution.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722.g003
PLOS ONE
Relation between effort and unit reliability on system reliability
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722 April 7, 2021 9 / 18
disclosed-risk condition as the period progresses, the average contribution remains stable
under the disclosed-risk condition. The standard deviations tend to be smaller under the dis-
closed-risk condition than under the non-disclosed-risk condition. A possible explanation for
this reduction in the standard deviations is as follows. Under the disclosed-risk condition, sub-
jects have access to information on the relationship between their contributions and the unit’s
reliability (Table 1). By referring to Table 1, the subjects could avoid the process of trial and
error. That is, they did not need to make very large or very small contributions in order to
guess and verify the relationship, as they did under the non-disclosed-risk condition. As a
result, the standard deviations of contributions under the disclosed-risk condition decreased.
We conducted a statistical test of whether the average contribution significantly differs
from the Nash equilibria of (0, 0) and (6, 6). Since the Nash equilibria presented in section 0
are those of the complete information model, they should be compared with the experimental
results under the disclosed-risk condition. The average contribution in the last period of the
disclosed-risk condition is 4.58 (S.D. = 3.71), which is not very different from the latter equilib-
rium of six. However, a t-test rejects the null hypothesis that the average contribution in the
last period is six (p-value = 0.03), and the null that the average contribution in the last period is
zero is also rejected (p-value <0.01).
We estimated a regression model to examine the cause of the variation in contributions and
determine whether the behavior of the subjects changed when the risk probability was dis-
closed. The dependent variable cont
i,t
is the number of contributions of subject i(= 1, . . ., 36)
in period t(= 2, . . ., 60). The independent variables are d
t
,cont
i,t−1
,cont
i
0
s partner,t−1
,aac
i,t−1
,
and the cross terms that are the products of d
t
and each variable. d
t
is a dummy variable that
takes a value of zero if period tis part of the non-disclosed-risk condition and a value of one
otherwise, cont
i,t−1
is the number of contributions of subject iin period t−1, cont
i
0
s partner,t−1
is
the number of contributions of subject i’s partner in period t−1, ac
i,t−1
is a dummy variable
that takes a value of one if subject ihad an accident in period t−1 and a value of zero other-
wise, and aac
i,t−1
is the accumulated number of accidents subject ihas had by period t−1. The
regression model is as follows:
conti;t¼aþb0dtþb1conti;t1þb2dtconti;t1þb3conti0s partner;t1þb4dtconti0s partner;t1
þb5aci;t1þb6dtaci;t1þb7aaci;t1þb8dtaaci;t1þei;tð1Þ
The coefficients on cont
i,t−1
,cont
i
0
s partner,t−1
,ac
i,t−1
,aac
i,t−1
are those for the non-disclosed-
risk condition, and the coefficients on the cross terms reflect the difference in coefficients
between the non-disclosed-risk condition and the disclosed-risk condition. The slopes of the
disclosed-risk condition can be measured as the sums of corresponding coefficients, like, for
example, β
1
and β
2
. The significance of those slopes can be tested using an F-test.
In our experiment, the same subjects decide how many contributions to make 30 times for
both the non-disclosed-risk condition and the disclosed-risk condition. Therefore, the data set
has a panel (longitudinal) data structure. The Hausman test rejects a random effect model, so
we present the estimation results of a fixed effect model in the left column of Table 5. For more
information concerning panel data analysis, see econometrics textbooks, such as Wooldridge
[15].
As presented in Table 5, the coefficient on aac
i,t−1
is -0.11 and is statistically significant. The
subjects had no information about the likelihood of accident under the non-disclosed-risk
condition. Therefore, it seems that they made conservatively many contributions under this
condition, especially in the first periods, and they adjusted the number of contributions by
observing their accident records in the experiment. In fact, the result of the regression analysis
shows that the subjects decreased the number of contributions as the accumulated number of
PLOS ONE
Relation between effort and unit reliability on system reliability
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722 April 7, 2021 10 / 18
accidents increased. From the subjects’ point of view, contributions are investments to earn
25 tokens. The estimation result shows that subjects who lost their investments many times
started to hesitate to invest in order to ensure a large payoff even in the case of an accident.
The coefficient on d
t
acc
i,t−1
is 0.10 and is statistically significant. This means that subjects
actually changed their behavior when the risk probability was revealed. β
7
+β
8
, which is the
slope of acc
i,t−1
for the disclosed-risk condition, is -0.01 and is not statistically significant (p-
value = 0.60). This result means that the subjects did not decrease their contributions regard-
less of the accumulated number of accidents under the disclosed-risk condition. The subjects
did not need to pay attention to the accumulated number of accidents under the disclosed-risk
condition because they made their decisions based on the disclosed risk probabilities of their
machine units, as shown in Table 1. Aside from acc
i,t−1
and d
t
acc
i,t−1
, only cont
i,t−1
is statisti-
cally significant.
4.2.2. System reliability. As shown in Table 4, the average system reliabilities of the non-
disclosed-risk condition and the disclosed-risk condition are 0.31 (S.D. = 0.28) and 0.49 (S.D.
= 0.29), respectively. The difference between them is statistically significant (p-value <0.01).
Note that the average system reliability of the disclosed-risk condition is higher than that of
the non-disclosed-risk condition despite the fact that the average contribution is higher under
the non-disclosed-risk condition than under the disclosed-risk condition. The average number
of total accidents per period in first 30 periods in all 3 sessions is 4.06 (S.D. = 1.33) under the
non-disclosed-risk condition, which is significantly higher than the average of 3.10 (S.D. =
1.14) under the disclosed-risk condition (p-value <0.01). This finding is consistent with the
result on system reliability.
In Fig 4A–4D, the average and standard deviation of the system reliabilities in a period are
represented by a dot and its corresponding vertical line, respectively. The upper left and upper
right panels of Fig 4A–4D show the non-disclosed-risk condition and the disclosed-risk condi-
tion, respectively. Clearly, the average system reliabilities under the disclosed-risk condition
are higher than those under the non-disclosed-risk condition. In order to examine whether
there is a structural change in the time series variation of the system reliability between the
Table 5. Regression analysis on contributions.
Independent variables Dependent variable: cont
i,t
Series system Parallel system
constant 4.85 (0.48) 1.89 (0.22)
d
t
-1.29 (0.65) -0.26 (0.31)
cont
i,t-1
0.31 (0.03) 0.51 (0.02)
d
t
cont
i,t-1
-0.06 (0.05) -0.15 (0.04)
cont
i’s partner,t-1
0.04 (0.02) 0.05 (0.02)
d
t
cont
i’s partner,t-1
0.03 (0.04) -0.08 (0.04)
ac
i,t-1
-0.32 (0.33) 0.87 (0.24)
d
t
ac
i,t-1
-0.02 (0.44) -0.74 (0.33)
aac
i,t-1
-0.11 (0.02) -0.16 (0.04)
d
t
aac
i,t-1
0.10 (0.03) 0.16 (0.05)
N2088 - 2088 -
F[9, 2043] 34.73 97.77
R
2
(within) 0.13 - 0.30 -
Values in parentheses are standard deviations.
,, and denote statistical significance at the 10%, 5%, and 1% levels, respectively.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722.t005
PLOS ONE
Relation between effort and unit reliability on system reliability
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722 April 7, 2021 11 / 18
non-disclosed-risk and the disclosed-risk condition, we estimated the following regression
model.
relij;t¼b0þb1tþb2dtþb3tdtþej;tð2Þ
where reli
j,t
is the system reliability of group j(= 1, . . ., 18) at period t(= 1, . . ., 60) and d
t
is a
dummy variable that takes a value of zero if period tis in the non-disclosed-risk condition and
a value of one otherwise. The result presented in the left column of Table 6 shows statistical
evidence that there is a structural change. Not only is the coefficient on t(-0.004) negatively
significant while that on td
t
(0.008) is positively significant, but also their sum (0.004) is sig-
nificantly positive (for all, p <0.01). This result means that the system reliability significantly
decreases under the non-disclosed-risk condition and increases under the disclosed-risk con-
dition as the period progresses.
Fig 4. A-D. Average and standard deviation of each period’s system reliability.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722.g004
PLOS ONE
Relation between effort and unit reliability on system reliability
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722 April 7, 2021 12 / 18
The average payoff under the disclosed-risk condition is 819.25 (S.D. = 85.18) and is higher
than that under the non-disclosed-risk condition, 680.19 (S.D. = 82.84). The difference is sta-
tistically significant (p-value <0.01). This result reflects the fact that under the disclosed-risk
condition, the subjects had fewer accidents but made lower contributions.
4.3 Parallel system
4.3.1 Contributions. Table 4 shows average contribution under the non-disclosed-risk
condition, 3.96 (S.D. = 4.85), is higher than that under the disclosed-risk condition, 2.45
(S.D. = 2.86). The difference is statistically significant (p-value <0.01). The lower left and
lower right panels of Fig 3A–3D show the average contributions under the non-disclosed-
risk condition and the disclosed-risk condition for the parallel system. As is the case with
the series system, although the average contribution of the non-disclosed-risk condition
decreases and approaches that of the disclosed-risk condition as the period progresses, the
average contribution of the disclosed-risk condition remains stable. The result for the stan-
dard deviations is similar to that of the series system. The standard deviations of contribu-
tions under the disclosed-risk condition are smaller than those under the non-disclosed-
risk condition.
The Nash equilibria of the parallel system are (0, 6), (6, 0), and (3, 3), as discussed in section
0. We form the null hypothesis that the average contribution of the last period of the disclosed-
risk condition is three, since the average contribution in the Nash equilibrium is three for each
equilibrium. The average contribution of the last period of the disclosed-risk condition is 2.33
(S.D. = 2.29). The t-test result shows that the null hypothesis is not rejected at the 5% signifi-
cance level, but it is rejected at the 10% significance level (p-value = 0.09).
The right column of Table 5 shows the results for the fixed effect model of panel regression
(1), whose dependent variable is cont
i,t
. The coefficients on the variables corresponding to the
non-disclosed-risk condition, cont
i,t−1
,cont
i
0
s partner,t−1
,ac
i,t−1
, and aac
i,t−1
, are statistically sig-
nificant. This result is more striking in the regression analysis of the parallel system than in
that of the series system. Indeed, for the series system, only the variables relevant to the accu-
mulated number of accidents are significant, with the exception of cont
i,t−1
. Since the subjects
could not see Table 1 under the non-disclosed-risk condition, they adjusted their levels of
contributions using all of the information they could obtain in the experiment as a reference.
Furthermore, all of the coefficients on the cross terms are significant, which means that the
subjects actually changed their behavior when the risk probability was revealed under the dis-
closed-risk condition. None of the coefficients on the variables corresponding to the disclosed-
Table 6. Regression analysis on system reliabilities.
Independent variable Dependent variable: reli
j,t
Series system Parallel system
constant 0.38 (0.02) 0.86 (0.01)
t-0.004 (0.001) -0.007 (0.001)
d
t
-0.08 (0.05) -0.12 (0.04)
td
t
0.008 (0.00) 0.008 (0.00)
N2160 - 2160 -
F[3, 2156] 79.74 39.41
Adj.R
2
0.099 - 0.051 -
Values in parentheses are standard deviations.
,, and denote statistical significance at the 10%, 5%, and 1% levels, respectively.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722.t006
PLOS ONE
Relation between effort and unit reliability on system reliability
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722 April 7, 2021 13 / 18
risk condition are significant except for β
1+
β
2
. This result means that, under the disclosed-risk
condition, the subjects did not take into account information on their partner’s contribution
and accident in period t−1 and the accumulated number of accidents by period t−1. Instead,
they used only Table 1 to make their contribution decisions.
4.3.2 System reliability. As shown in Table 4, the average system reliabilities of the non-
disclosed-risk condition and the disclosed-risk condition are 0.75 (S.D. = 0.23) and 0.78 (S.D.
= 0.18), respectively. This difference is statistically significant (p-value <0.01). As is the case
with the series system, the average system reliability of the disclosed-risk condition is higher
than that of the non-disclosed-risk condition despite the fact that the average contribution is
higher under the non-disclosed-risk condition than under the disclosed-risk condition.
The lower left and lower right panels of Fig 4A–4D shows the averages and standard devia-
tions of the system reliabilities under the non-disclosed-risk and the disclosed-risk conditions,
respectively. The right column of Table 6 presents the estimated coefficients of regression
model (2). As with the results for the series system, the statistical evidence shows there was a
structural change in the coefficient on twhen the risk probability was revealed under the dis-
closed-risk condition. The coefficient on t(-0.007) is negatively significant, and that on td
t
(0.008) is positively significant (for both, the p-value <0.01). However, their sum (0.001) is
not significantly different from zero (p-value = 0.26), which means that the system reliability
of the disclosed-risk condition remains stable.
The average number of accidents is 1.41 (S.D. = 0.99) under the non-disclosed-risk condi-
tion, which is not significantly different from 1.47 (S.D. = 1.05) under the disclosed-risk condi-
tion (p-value = 0.71). This result reflects the fact that the difference in the system reliability
between the non-disclosed-risk condition and the disclosed-risk condition is not very large
even though it is statistically significant.
The average payoff under the disclosed-risk condition is 1093.22 (S.D. = 63.72), which is
higher than that under the non-disclosed-risk condition, 1054.89 (S.D. = 73.82). This differ-
ence is statistically significant (p-value = 0.02). This result reflects the fact that subjects
exerted less effort under the disclosed-risk condition than under the non-disclosed-risk con-
dition, whereas the numbers of accidents per period did not differ substantially across the
two conditions.
5. Discussion
5.1 Effect of disclosing the relation between effort and unit reliability on
system reliability
First, we discuss the effect of disclosing the relation between effort and unit reliability on sys-
tem reliability based on the last 10 periods of the non-disclosed-risk condition and all periods
of the disclosed-risk condition. The subjects used a process of trial and error to learn how to
determine their contributions during the first few periods of the non-disclosed-risk condition.
The issue of learning is extensively discussed in the literature. For instance, Hausken and
Ortmann (2008), who conducted the prisoner’s dilemma, where free riding plays a role and
Hausken, Banuri, Gupta, and Abbink (2015) who conducted a type of coordination game for
terrorist attacks [16,17].
We treat the last 10 periods of the non-disclosed-risk condition as considered to have been
in a steady state. The average contribution of the non-disclosed-risk condition gradually con-
verges to that of the disclosed-risk condition for both systems, as mentioned in sections 4.2.1
and 4.3.1. For the series system, the average contribution in the last 10 periods of the non-dis-
closed-risk condition is 4.55 (S.D. = 5.33), and there is no statistically significant difference
between this value and the average contribution under the disclosed-risk condition of 4.78
PLOS ONE
Relation between effort and unit reliability on system reliability
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722 April 7, 2021 14 / 18
(S.D. = 3.77) (p-value = 0.46). For the parallel system, the same result holds, with the average
contributions being 2.59 (S.D. = 3.52) and 2.45 (S.D. = 2.86) for the last 10 periods of the non-
disclosed-risk condition and the disclosed-risk condition, respectively (p-value = 0.48). This
result indicates that subjects tried to find the optimal level of contributions by observing their
own and their counterpart’s numbers of contributions and the occurrence of accidents. How-
ever, this convergence of the averages does not mean that the subjects’ behavior in the last 10
periods of the non-disclosed-risk condition is precisely the same as that under the disclosed-
risk condition, as indicated by the standard deviations of the contributions under both condi-
tions. The standard deviation of the contributions in the last 10 periods of the non-disclosed-
risk condition is larger than that of the disclosed-risk condition for both the series and the par-
allel systems.
The average system reliability in the last 10 periods of the non-disclosed-risk condition is
0.28 for the series system, which is significantly lower than that of the disclosed-risk condition
of 0.49 (p-value <0.01). As for the parallel system, the same result holds, with a system reliabil-
ity of 0.69 in the last 10 periods of the non-disclosed-risk condition and 0.78 under the dis-
closed-risk condition (p-value <0.01). Even though the average contribution in the last 10
periods of the non-disclosed-risk condition is not different from that of the disclosed-risk con-
dition, the average system reliability is significantly higher under the disclosed-risk condition
than in the last 10 periods of the non-disclosed-risk condition for both systems.
When we incorporate the data from the first 20 periods of the non-disclosed-risk condition,
where subjects’ contributions were relatively higher, the outline of the result does not change.
As shown in Table 4, the average contribution under the non-disclosed-risk condition is
higher than that under the disclosed-risk condition for both the series and the parallel system.
As discussed in sections 4.2.2 and 4.3.2, the average system reliability of the disclosed-risk con-
dition is significantly higher than that of the non-disclosed-risk condition for both systems.
This result is interesting because it contradicts the result that the subjects actually made more
contributions to maintain the machine units under the non-disclosed-risk condition than
under the disclosed-risk condition.
This contradiction is related to the fact that the variance of the contributions under the
non-disclosed-risk condition is larger than that under the disclosed-risk condition, which
means that the contributions of some subjects are relatively low in the former condition. These
low contributions result in low system reliability. This effect is noticeable in the series system,
which can be easily affected by just one subject’s contribution, but it is also observed in the par-
allel system.
This result offers a new perspective on risk assessment and the disclosure of risk probability.
That is, the disclosure of risk probability could coordinate people’s actions to reduce risk.
Since the subjects could refer to Table 1 and share common information under the disclosed-
risk condition of our experiment, their contributions were concentrated around the Nash
equilibrium, and few subjects made extremely high or low contributions. They succeeded in
simultaneously reducing their maintenance efforts and improving the system reliability. As a
result, the average payoff that subjects received was significantly higher under the disclosed-
risk condition than under the non-disclosed-risk condition for both the series and the parallel
system (Table 4). This finding means that the economic welfare of subjects improved by dis-
closing the accident risk probability.
There are several Nash equilibria for the series system such that (0, 0) and (6, 6) imply that
both subjects spend nothing or less than half of their efforts. In the non-disclosure condition,
during the last 10 periods, the contributions are less than 5 so that system liability reduces. On
the other hand, in the disclosure condition, during the last 10 periods, their efforts spent are
PLOS ONE
Relation between effort and unit reliability on system reliability
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722 April 7, 2021 15 / 18
more than 5 so that the system liability increases. The disclosure condition makes the contri-
butions close to the Nash equilibrium (6, 6) which is good for the system liability.
For the parallel system, there are several Nash equilibria such that (6, 0), (0, 6) and (3, 3)
imply that one of them spend efforts or both of them spend a small amount of their efforts. In
the non-disclosure condition, during the last 10 periods, one of them spent around 3 but the
other do free ridings so that system liability reduces. On the other hand, in the disclosure con-
dition, both of them spent around three so that system liability induces. The disclosure condi-
tion makes the contributions close to the Nash equilibrium (3, 3) which is good for the system
liability.
In the multiple Nash equilibria, since the mixed strategy will be taken, the system liability
will not be robust. Disclosing the relation between effort and unit reliability has an effect to
their contribution or behavior to one Nash equilibria. Such a change from the other equilib-
rium to one equilibrium is affected by the subjects’ behaviors. Next section we show the differ-
ence of their behaviors to induces the different Nash equilibria.
5.2 Effect of disclosing the relation between effort and unit reliability on
subjects’ behavior
As shown in Table 5, the estimation results of regression model (1) indicate that the subjects
actually changed their behavior when the risk probability, that is, the information in Table 1,
was revealed under the disclosed-risk condition. In particular, we focus on the result that the
subjects did not take into account the information on the partner’s contribution and accident
in period t−1 and the accumulated number of accidents by period t−1 under the disclosed-
risk condition. Since, under the disclosed-risk condition, subjects could obtain information on
the risk probability from Table 1, they determined how many contributions to make based on
only that risk probability without considering other factors. In other words, the subjects’ deci-
sion-making process was simplified, or its psychological cost was reduced. Under the dis-
closed-risk condition, subjects could reduce not only their contributions but also the cost of
decision-making. This is another positive effect of the disclosure of risk probability.
This effect is analogous to that of hazard maps and durable life information in food produc-
tion. Both provide information on safety and help to avoid danger. If people have hazard
maps, they do not have to look for safe places by themselves when disasters happen. If they
know durable lives of foods, they do not have to examine the safety and hygiene of the foods
themselves when eating. This kind of information on safety can simplify the decision-making
process and reduce its cost.
6. Conclusions
We conducted an economic experiment on the basis of Hausken’s (2002) theoretical model
that merged PRA and game theory with a particular focus on the effect of disclosing the rela-
tion between effort and unit reliability on the subjects’ behavior and system reliability. Our
experimental results show that disclosing the relation between effort and unit reliability has
two positive effects. First, subjects succeeded in improving the system reliability while reducing
their efforts to reduce the risk of their units when the risk probability was disclosed. Second,
disclosing the risk probability simplified the subjects’ decision-making process and reduced its
cost. We believe that this study is the first step toward experimentally examining the integrated
model of PRA and game theory, and the results offer a new perspective on the function of the
disclosure of risk probability, namely, that it has the potential to coordinate people’s actions to
reduce risk.
PLOS ONE
Relation between effort and unit reliability on system reliability
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722 April 7, 2021 16 / 18
The following two future research agendas can be made. The first one concerns the het-
erogeneous risks of the machine units. In this study, each player faced the same failure risk,
as shown in Table 1. However, in the real industrial setting, there is a mix of equipment with
different installation times, i.e., different degrees of deterioration. The relationship between
maintenance effort and failure probability is considered to be different for newer and older
equipment. In general, the older the equipment, the more effort would be required to reduce
the failure probability. The effect of such heterogeneous risks on each player’s behavior and
on system reliability in equilibrium should be examined. Second one is related to the positive
effect of risk probability disclosure discussed in section 5.2. In this study, we provided an
example in which risk probability disclosure helped subjects improve system reliability and
avoid danger. However, there could be circumstances where this disclosure brings about
negative outcomes. For instance, people who believe the information of a hazard map and
take refuge where instructed may end up suffering damage from a disaster such as a tsunami
if the tsunami hits the place of refuge. In this case, the hazard map results in heavy damage
to the population by gathering everyone in one place. Our future experimental research will
be focused on the conditions under which risk information disclosure produces negative
outcomes.
Supporting information
S1 File.
(XLSX)
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: Ryoji Makino, Kenju Akai, Keiko Aoki.
Data curation: Ryoji Makino, Kenju Akai, Takanori Kudo, Keiko Aoki.
Formal analysis: Ryoji Makino, Kenju Akai, Jun-ichi Takeshita.
Funding acquisition: Ryoji Makino.
Investigation: Ryoji Makino, Kenju Akai, Jun-ichi Takeshita, Takanori Kudo, Keiko Aoki.
Methodology: Ryoji Makino, Kenju Akai, Jun-ichi Takeshita, Takanori Kudo.
Project administration: Ryoji Makino, Kenju Akai.
Resources: Ryoji Makino, Kenju Akai.
Supervision: Ryoji Makino, Kenju Akai.
Validation: Ryoji Makino, Kenju Akai.
Visualization: Ryoji Makino, Kenju Akai.
Writing – original draft: Ryoji Makino, Kenju Akai, Keiko Aoki.
Writing – review & editing: Ryoji Makino, Kenju Akai, Keiko Aoki.
References
1. Meel A, Seider WD, Oktem U. Analysis of management actions, human behavior, and process reliability
in chemical plants. I. Impact of management actions. Process safety progress. 2008; 27(1):7–14.
2. Bier VM. Challenges to the acceptance of probabilistic risk analysis. Risk Analysis. 1999; 19(4):703–10.
3. Hausken K. Probabilistic risk analysis and game theory. Risk Analysis. 2002; 22(1):17–27. https://doi.
org/10.1111/0272-4332.t01-1-00002 PMID: 12017358
PLOS ONE
Relation between effort and unit reliability on system reliability
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722 April 7, 2021 17 / 18
4. Guikema SD. Game theory models of intelligent actors in reliability analysis: An overview of the state of
the art. Game theoretic risk analysis of security threats: Springer; 2009. p. 13–31.
5. Bier VM, Lin SW. Should the model for risk-informed regulation be game theory rather than decision
theory? Risk Analysis: An International Journal. 2013; 33(2):281–91. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-
6924.2012.01866.x PMID: 22908942
6. Bier VM, Azaiez MN. Game theoretic risk analysis of security threats: Springer Science & Business
Media; 2008.
7. Hausken K, Levitin G. Review of systems defense and attack models. International Journal of Perform-
ability Engineering. 2012; 8(4):355–66.
8. Tambe M. Security and game theory: algorithms, deployed systems, lessonslearned: Cambridge uni-
versity press; 2011.
9. Bier V, Oliveros S, Samuelson L. Choosing what to protect: Strategic defensive allocation against an
unknown attacker, submitted to. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty. 2005.
10. Hausken K. Choosing what to protect when attacker resources and asset valuations are uncertain.
Operations Research and Decisions. 2014; 24.
11. Hirshleifer J. From weakest-link to best-shot: The voluntary provision of public goods. Public choice.
1983; 41(3):371–86.
12. Hirshleifer J. From weakest-link to best-shot: Correction. Public Choice. 1985:221–3.
13. Nash J. Non-cooperative games. Annals of mathematics. 1951:286–95.
14. Fischbacher U. z-Tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental econom-
ics. 2007; 10(2):171–8.
15. Wooldridge JM. Econometric analysis of cross section and panel data: MIT press; 2010.
16. Hausken K, Ortmann A. A first experimental test of multilevel game theory: the PD case. Applied Eco-
nomics Letters. 2008; 15(4):261–4.
17. Hausken K, Banuri S, Gupta DK, Abbink K. Al Qaeda at the bar: coordinating ideologues and mercenar-
ies in terrorist organizations. Public Choice. 2015; 164(1–2):57–73.
PLOS ONE
Relation between effort and unit reliability on system reliability
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249722 April 7, 2021 18 / 18
Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.