ArticlePDF Available

How Do People Learn About Politics When Inadvertently Exposed to News? Incidental News Paradoxical Direct and Indirect Effects on Political Knowledge

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Citizens’ political knowledge is regarded as a vital element for well-functioning democracies. Accordingly, there is a vibrant literature assessing the link between individuals’ news seeking behavior and learning about public affairs. There are, however, more limited efforts devoted to clarifying how incidental news exposure may facilitate political learning. So far, inconclusive research findings have offered positive, null or even negative effects, emphasizing an urge for scholars to further explore this relationship. Drawing upon U.S. representative survey data, this study seeks to explicate and further advance the (paradoxical) paths that connect citizens’ incidental news exposure and political knowledge, both directly and indirectly. Our analysis first shows either null or mild negative direct associations between incidental news exposure and political knowledge. However, relying on a two serial mediators’ model, when citizens thoroughly engage with and cognitively elaborate on the information they unintentionally stumble upon, incidental news exposure is associated with positive mediated effects on political learning. This study provides evidence for a better understanding over the potential direct and indirect mechanisms that both facilitate and hinder political knowledge acquisition through inadvertent news consumption.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Computers in Human Behavior 121 (2021) 106803
Available online 5 April 2021
0747-5632/Published by Elsevier Ltd.
Full length article
How do people learn about politics when inadvertently exposed to news?
Incidental news paradoxical Direct and indirect effects on
political knowledge
Homero Gil de Zú˜
niga
a
,
b
,
c
,
*
, Porismita Borah
d
,
a
, Manuel Goyanes
e
,
a
a
Democracy Research Unit, Political Science, College of Law & Public Administration, University of Salamanca, Spain
b
Media Effects Research Lab, Film Production & Media Studies Department, Donald P. Bellisario College of Communications, Pennsylvania State University, USA
c
Facultad de Comunicaci´
on y Letras, Universidad Diego Portales, Chile
d
Edward R. Murrow College of Communication, Washington State University, USA
e
Department of Communication, Universidad Carlos III, Spain
ARTICLE INFO
Keywords:
Incidental news exposure
Thorough information engagement
Cognitive elaboration
Political knowledge
Democracy
ABSTRACT
Citizenspolitical knowledge is regarded as a vital element for well-functioning democracies. Accordingly, there
is a vibrant literature assessing the link between individualsnews seeking behavior and learning about public
affairs. There are, however, more limited efforts devoted to clarifying how incidental news exposure may
facilitate political learning. So far, inconclusive research ndings have offered positive, null or even negative
effects, emphasizing an urge for scholars to further explore this relationship. Drawing upon U.S. representative
survey data, this study seeks to explicate and further advance the (paradoxical) paths that connect citizens
incidental news exposure and political knowledge, both directly and indirectly. Our analysis rst shows either
null or mild negative direct associations between incidental news exposure and political knowledge. However,
relying on a two serial mediatorsmodel, when citizens thoroughly engage with and cognitively elaborate on the
information they unintentionally stumble upon, incidental news exposure is associated with positive mediated
effects on political learning. This study provides evidence for a better understanding over the potential direct and
indirect mechanisms that both facilitate and hinder political knowledge acquisition through inadvertent news
consumption.
Over the years, scholars have studied the link between individuals
information seeking behavior and political knowledge (Chaffee &
Kanihan, 1997; Kenski & Stroud, 2006; Park & Gil de Zú˜
niga, 2020).
Research has shown that intentional consumption of mass media fosters
political learning. However, users can also be exposed to news infor-
mation incidentally online. For instance, as by product of internet or
social media use, or just utter serendipitous inadvertent exposure (e.g.,
Tewksbury, Weaver, & Maddex, 2001). This non-intentional or
non-purposive exposure to news and information is broadly termed
incidental news exposure (Kim, Chen, & Gil deZú˜
niga, 2013; Tewksbury
et al., 2001; Bates, 2002; Erdelez, 1999). Social media often makes easy
to incidentally encounter news and political information(Lane, Kim,
et al., 2017, p. 364).
Despite increasing interest in this construct, ndings from an array of
empirical studies at best have been inconclusive germane to how social
media news and incidental exposure nourishes political learning.
Scholars have demonstrated positive (e.g., Lee & Kim, 2017; Yamamoto
& Morey, 2019), negative or even null effects (Oeldorf-Hirsch, 2018; van
Erkel & Van Aelst, 2020), depending on the behavioral and cognitive
factors independently and directly examined. Moreover, most of these
studies do not examine the underlying mechanisms connecting inci-
dental news exposure and political learning, precluding us to gain a
deeper knowledge on the behavioral actions and cognitive processes
behind political learning on the online realm. This study addresses these
gaps.
Drawing upon U.S. generalizable and diverse survey data collected in
2019, our study seeks to explicate and further advance the paradoxical
paths that connect citizens incidental news exposure and political
knowledge, both directly and indirectly. The primary contribution of the
current study is to understand the underlying mechanisms that can
* Corresponding author. Political Science University of Salamanca, Campus Unamuno s/n, 37007, Salamanca, Spain.
E-mail address: hgz@usal.es (H. Gil de ˜
niga).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Computers in Human Behavior
journal homepage: http://www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2021.106803
Received 19 May 2020; Received in revised form 24 December 2020; Accepted 27 March 2021
Computers in Human Behavior 121 (2021) 106803
2
explain the path from incidental news exposure to political knowledge.
Whereas data suggests null or mild negative direct associations between
incidental news exposure and political knowledge, advancing a two
serial mediator model shows that incidental news exposure is positively
related to political knowledge through peoples news thorough infor-
mation engagement and cognitive elaboration.
1. Incidental news exposure, political knowledge, and the news
media
Although incidental news exposure is often referred to in terms of
online news gathering habits, it is not a new phenomenon (Karnowski,
Kumpel, Leonhard & Leiner (2017). In fact, Downs (1957) had explained
two different mechanisms of newsgathering: sought-for-data and
accidental data(Downs, 1957, p. 223). A few decades later Tewksbury
et al. (2001) highlighted that newsgathering can be purposive and
non-purposive. Purposive newsgathering is directional and conscious
and is prevalent in the context of traditional media since the news is
often segregated from other media content (Tewksbury et al., 2001).
However, given todays digital and social media contexts, exposure to
news may also be non-intentional, which prior literature termed as
incidental news exposure (Tewksbury et al., 2001; Bates, 2002;
Erdelez, 1999).
A large body of research has examined incidental news exposure (e.
g., Fletcher & Nielsen, 2017; Lee & Kim, 2017; Kim et al., 2013;
Valeriani & Vaccari, 2016). Most of these research demonstrates benets
of incidental news exposure, including enhanced political participation,
political knowledge, information seeking, argument repertoire recall,
and news engagement. Incidental news exposure on social media plat-
forms was associated with greater online political participation in Eu-
ropean countries (Valeriani & Vaccari, 2016) and political participation
in the U.S. (Kim et al., 2013), increased online political information
seeking (Lee & Yang, 2014; Yamamoto & Morey, 2019), use of more
diverse news sources (Fletcher & Nielsen, 2017; Strauß, Huber, & Gil
deZú˜
niga, 2020), higher news information recognition and recall (Lee &
Kim, 2017), and prompted active news engagement (Oeldorf-Hirsch,
2018).
A central concept in the political communication literature is polit-
ical knowledge (Mondak, 2001). An informed citizenry is a
much-desired democratic outcome (Amsalem & Nir, 2019; Gil de ˜
niga
& Hsuan-Ting, 2019) and as a result there is an outsized research
focusing on examining the antecedents and consequences of political
knowledge (e.g., Galston, 2001; Miller & Krosnick). Scholars have
shown that citizens who are more knowledgeable hold consistent po-
litical views (Galston, 2001), are able to process news and information
faster (Miller & Krosnick, 2000), and participate more in politics
(Althaus, 2003). Consequently, understanding the behavioral and
cognitive mechanisms that both facilitate and hinder political knowl-
edge acquisition has become a long-lasting goal in academia (Gil de
˜
niga, 2015).
Political knowledge inuences a variety of research areas including
public opinion, media effects, and voting behavior (Modak, 2001;
Amsalem & Nir, 2019). In general, political knowledge is dened as
citizens ability to answer factual questions about politics (Bourdreau &
Lupia, 2011; Delli Carpini & Keeter, 1996). Learning about politics is
usually related to news media use (e.g., Kaufhold, Valenzuela, & ˜
niga,
2010; Lecheler & Vreese, 2017). Further, individuals who consumed
unfamiliar news information become more knowledgeable (Lecheler &
Vreese, 2017) and the level of political knowledge and news media tend
to signicantly inuence each other over time (Moeller & de Vreese,
2019).
In the context of emerging media, research investigating the inter-
relatedness between information gathering and political learning
abounds (Gottfried, Hardy, Holbert, Winneg, & Jamieson, 2016; Kenski
& Stroud, 2010). Social media users exposed to political information
were signicantly more knowledgeable regarding candidate
background facts (Gottfried et al., 2016) and people with higher expo-
sure to online information about presidential campaigns had signi-
cantly higher level of political knowledge (Kenski & Stroud, 2010). In
short, a stronger and healthier democracy heavily relies on the role of
news media to generate an informed public opinion.
2. Incidental news exposure and political knowledge
Individualsincidental news exposure is also related to learning and
political knowledge acquisition (e.g., Lee & Kim, 2017; Tewksbury et al.,
2001; Zukin & Snyder, 1984). For example, Zukin and Snyder (1984)
suggested that individuals accidently exposed to more election news and
advertising demonstrated more knowledge about the candidates. Like-
wise, people gain political knowledge through incidental exposure to
public service TV (Shehata, Hopmann, Nord, & Hoijer, 2015), as well as
when incidentally exposed to news headlines on portal sites such as
Yahoo! JAPAN (Kobayashi & Inamasu, 2015). This positive association
between incidental news and public affairs learning transpires beyond
quantitative work. Qualitative research (e.g. Bergstron & Belfrage,
2018; Feezell, 2018; Yadamsuren & Erdelez, 2010) also provides evi-
dence that supports the theoretical link between incidental news expo-
sure and knowledge. For instance, individuals acknowledged its
importance in keeping them informed about current events (Bergstron &
Belfrage, 2018), providing richer exposure to diverse political news in-
formation on Facebook which facilitated learning (Feezell, 2018), and as
means for individuals to learn about new topics and acquire knowledge
(Yadamsuren & Erdelez, 2010).
Albeit important, conicting ndings have also been reported
regarding the path from incidental news exposure to political knowledge
(Feezell & Ortiz, 2019; Oeldorf-Hirsch, 2018; van Erkel & Van Aelst,
2020). In a more general analysis, a myriad of studies has underscored
the negative association between use of social media platforms and
political knowledge (Gotfried et al., 2017; Cacciatore et al., 2018; Lee &
Xenos, 2019; van Erkel & Van Aelst, 2020). Typically, these studies do
not differentiate between purposive or accidental nature of such expo-
sure. We argue that they may be crucial in accounting for citizenslevel
of political knowledge. For instance, Lee and Xenos (2019), found that
political social media use does not predict political knowledge, whereas
general social media use negatively impacted (van Erkel & Van Aelst,
2020). Specically, Facebook use is negatively associated to political
knowledge (Lee & Xenos, 2019; van Erkel & Van Aelst, 2020), a platform
growingly used for passing time (Boczkowski, Mitchelstein, & Matassi,
2018).
Similarly, Baum (2003), demonstrated that soft news consumption is
negatively related to political learning, and incidental news is typically a
crucial source of soft news encountering, especially in social media
(Boczkowski et al., 2018; Yoo & Gil DeZú˜
niga, 2019). Similarly,
Cacciatore et al. (2018) showed that increased levels of Facebook use
has a negative inuence on political knowledge. We argue that such
multitasking activities are in part, mixed with incidental news stimuli.
All told, due to the inconclusive ndings regarding the direct asso-
ciation between incidental news exposure and political knowledge
acquisition, we cannot clearly ascertain neither the direction nor the
signicance of such relation. Accordingly, the following research ques-
tion is formulated:
RQ1: What is the direct relationship between incidental news
exposure and political knowledge?
3. Incidental news exposure, thorough engagement, and
cognitive elaboration
Due to the mixed ndings related to the effects of incidental news
exposure on political knowledge acquisition, this study suggests there is
a need to examine the underlying mechanisms of inadvertent news
exposure process to determine its association to political learning. The
H. Gil de ˜
niga et al.
Computers in Human Behavior 121 (2021) 106803
3
importance of information processing variables such as deep and thor-
ough information engagement with news have received attention from
scholars over the years (e.g., Eveland, 2001, 2004; Lee & Kim, 2017).
Thorough information engagement refers to how users engage with the
news when they are exposed to this information. Reading the informa-
tion in depth, and not simply quickly skimming over, is ‘thorough in-
formation engagement. Individuals will need to pay attention to the
information upon exposure to deeply engage with the material. Atten-
tion to news is thus, a primary factor in determining the inuence of
news exposure on learning (Chaffee & Schleuder, 1986; Drew & Weaver,
1990; McLeod & McDonald, 1985). In terms of social media, attention is
often operationalized as engagement due to the difculty of measuring
social media attention (Choi, 2014; Oeldorf-Hirsch, 2018). For example,
when one is unintentionally exposed to news on social media, in-
dividuals have two options, quickly skim the headline, or conversely
read the material carefully till the end, thoroughly engaging with the
information. The latter is the construct of interest in this study.
Being incidentally exposed to news is not a happy accident
(Thorson, 2018), especially on social media. It is important to note that
in social media platforms all users are not equally exposed to news and
information (Gil de ˜
niga and Hsuan-Ting, 2019). Thus, it is vital to
pay attention to the inequalities in how people are exposed to news. This
is key to understand the breath of this phenomenon. Likewise, algo-
rithmic lters and curation can display news or information liked or
shared by users with strong interactions (Karnowski, Kümpel, Leonhard,
& Leiner, 2017). Moreover, friends and acquaintances can also share,
re-tweet or like informative contents, connecting such information with
potential users, as a form of social network curation.
Beyond intentional or accidental news seeking, a growing number of
scholars have started shed light over the factors that connect incidental
news consumption with learning and engagement (Karnowski et al.,
2017; Oeldorf-Hirsch, 2018). First, it should be noted that being
inadvertently exposed to news on SNS does not necessarily mean being
exposed to the whole piece(Karnowski et al., 2017, p. 44). This means
that users can stumble upon news, but they do not necessarily engage
with such content beyond reading a few lines or just the headline. As a
result, userscognitive elaboration processes maybe weaker than those
who have engaged in a thorough reading, consequently limiting the
potential learning (Eveland, 2001; Knoll, Matthes, & Heiss, 2018;
Stegmann, Wecker, Weinberger, & Fischer, 2012). Cognitive elaboration
is the process of reecting on the information that individuals encounter
in the news, making sense of it as it relates to their own lives. The process
of cognitive elaboration would involve critically thinking about the
material (Eveland, 2001; Jung, Kim, & Gil deZúniga, 2011) and using
additional information taken from or inferred in combination with prior
knowledge (Stegmann et al., 2012, p. 300). And this systematic
cognitive processing becomes a contributing factor for people to discern
relevance appraisal (Cho et al., 2009).
We argue that higher people get accidently exposed to news, the
higher chances they will have thoroughly engage with the information
encountered, especially when such information is interesting or the user
has previous knowledge about it (Karnowski et al., 2017). More
importantly, we theorize this type of information engagement will have
cognitive and political learning consequences. The incidental exposition
to news is pervasive (Morris & Morris, 2017), increasing the likelihood
of engaging in a thorough reading as users may feel they are missing an
important account of social reality. Therefore, news contents not sys-
tematically processed may become salient, inducing users to engage
with it. As Knoll et al. (2018) emphasize, incidental exposure describes
when people may take part in implicit processing of news content, where
even supercial or unconscious process may induce behavioral effects.
One such behavioral effect should be users deeper and thorough
engagement with the news information. Thus, we propose:
H1. Incidental news exposure is positively associated to thorough
engagement with news information.
When linking news exposure and political learning, cognitive elab-
oration is another potential variable that merits further exploration
(Shahin, Salda˜
na, & Gil deZú˜
niga, 2020). Elaboration refers to con-
necting new information to other information stored in the memory,
including prior knowledge, personal experiences, or the connection of
two new bits of information together in new waysthat are meaningful
to one-self and the surrounding life circumstances (Eveland, 2001, p.
573). In his seminal cognitive mediation model,Eveland (2001) shows
news exposure was positively associated with information processing
variables such as attention and elaboration, which in turn inu-
enced political learning. Through thorough engagement with the in-
formation and cognitive elaboration, individuals increase the memory
store and the ability to recall (Jung et al., 2001; Kim, Chadha, & Gil de
˜
niga, 2018).
A central question behind this literature is to test whether incidental
news exposure yields similar effects on elaboration as intentional news
seeking (Oeldorf-Hirsch, 2018; Kim, Chadha, & Gil deZú˜
niga, 2018). In
the cognitive mediation model (Eveland, 2001; Jung et al., 2011), the
association between new use and knowledge is mediated by information
processing variables (i.e., news cognitive elaboration). Several scholars
have furnished strong empirical research on the possible power of
incidental exposure in triggering cognitive elaboration (Oeldof-Hirsch,
2018; Wei & Lo, 2008). Many of the incidental inputs may be positively
appraised, fostering the engagement in elaborate processing (Knoll
et al., 2018; also see; Kim et al., 2013). Therefore, we propose:
H2. Incidental news exposure is positively associated to cognitive
elaboration
4. Thorough news consumption, cognitive elaboration, and
political knowledge
As indicated above, inadvertent news exposure alone does not
determine great or deeper engagement with such content and informa-
tion (Karnowski et al., 2017). Consequently, a user not interested in a
particular political topic may partially read the accidental contents
encountered, for instance skimming the headline, article or reading few
lines of the text. On the contrary, for instance, users interested in a
recent sanctioned public policy may be more prone to engage in a more
thorough reading if they accidently encounter such content while
checking their Facebook feed. Attention, time, and cognitive effort are
vital factors in processing news contents (Eveland, 2001; Jung et al.,
2011; Kim et al., 2018). Engaging in depth with the information should
spark knowledge acquisition. Prior research has shown that elaboration
of information is associated with learning (e.g., McNamara, Kintsch,
Butler-Songer, & Kintsch, 1996; Stein & Bransford, 1979). In line with
this reasoning, citizens that thoroughly engage with political informa-
tion will retain and gain political facts. Accordingly:
H3. Thorough engagement with information is positively related to
political knowledge.
Importantly, prior scholarship has also established the link between
cognitive elaboration and political knowledge. Elaboration is a mediator
impacting the relationship between motivations for active news use and
political knowledge acquisition (Eveland, 2002; Cho et al., 2009; Jung
et al., 2011). Other studies further pointed out that elaboration is related
to individualsknowledge structure density in news use, which is a more
salient form of learning (Eveland, Marton, & Seo, 2004). Accordingly,
cognitive elaboration is generally considered as a fundamental mental
process that links new information acquisition with pre-existing
knowledge (Eveland, 2001; Kim et al., 2018), as it is a causal process
after news consumption (Perse, 2001). Similarly, it theoretically plau-
sible that incidental news cognitive elaboration may lead to political
knowledge:
H4. Cognitive elaboration is positively related to political knowledge
H. Gil de ˜
niga et al.
Computers in Human Behavior 121 (2021) 106803
4
The role of news engagement and cognitive elaboration as a medi-
ator in terms of incidental news exposure and political knowledge is
relatively new (Karnowski et al., 2017; Oeldof-Hirsch, 2018; Wei & Lo,
2008). Building to this scholarship this study provides an integrative and
theoretically driven model that accounts for behavioral and cognitive
differences. Engagement may be a pre-condition of effective learning
outcomes, as may engage in systematic process of such information only
after they have payed attention to the information. By engaging with
news contents, citizensspur the cognitive process germane to incidental
news inputs, bringing the new information together with prior knowl-
edge. As result of this cognitive process, people learn from news, inci-
dental or intentional (Knoll et al., 2018). As a result, we expect that
citizens that exhibit greater levels of incidental exposure to news will
have more chances to thoroughly engage with information. And those
who do, will in turn reect on that information and cognitively elaborate
about such incidentally gathered information which ultimately, will be
related to enhanced levels of political knowledge. A more formal theo-
retical proposition:
H5. Incidental exposure to news will be positively related to political
knowledge through a double mediation mechanism via a) thorough in-
formation engagement, and b) cognitive elaboration.
5. Method
5.1. Sample
This study draws upon a survey data sample seeking US national
representativeness, elded in June of 2019. Researchers at University of
Vienna contracted IPSOS Austria to provide the subjects for the survey.
The questionnaire was elaborated and administered via Qualtrics. To
achieve US national representativeness and pursue generalizable in-
ferences on the US population, IPSOS Austria curates a massive panel of
US individuals/respondents of hundreds of thousands. They stratied a
subsample of 3000 individuals from this pool, matching key de-
mographic elements from the US census (i.e., gender, education, and
income).
The nal sample left 1338 valid cases, yielding a cooperation rate of
45.5% (AAPOR, 2018). Overall, the sample fairly resembles key US
census demographic breakdowns (see Gil de ˜
niga, Gonz´
alez
Gonz´
alez & Goyanes, 2021), and also compares fairly well to survey data
collected at a similar time by the Pew American Life Project by RDS (Pew
American Life Project, 2018).
5.2. Endogenous and exogenous measurements
5.2.1. Thorough information engagement
Individual conscientious news engagement is measured by three
items (on a 10-point scale from never =1 to all the time=10),
asking respondentstheir reading strategies, when it comes to keep up
with the news (written, video, pictures). Items included: thoroughly
follow through the entire news piece (written/video), click on the
news link and listen or read the entire news pieceand read the entire
news article until the end(M =5.64, SD =2.71,
α
=0.92).
5.2.2. Political knowledge
We used measures of participants awareness of current events and
politics in the U.S., as well as its political system and chief institutional
rules (see Delli Carpini & Keeter, 1993; Moeller & Vreese, 2015). The
rst two questions were open-ended. Participants were asked to name
the positions held by Mike Pence and Brett Kavanaugh at the moment in
which the survey was administered. The next six items were multiple
choice and inquired participants to pinpoint the interval of a term for U.
S. Senators, the budget item on which the U.S. Federal government
spent the least, the party afliation of the Senators who introduced
For the People Act bill,” “the ruling government system of the U.S,
the name of the country embassy in which WikiLeaks founder, William
Assange, was sheltered, and the name of the presidential candidate
that accused a liberal think tank to undermine Democrats chances of
taking back the White House in 2020 by using its resources to smear
contenders. Correct answers were coded as 1, while incorrect or
missing ones were coded as 0 (M =2.77, SD =2.02, Guttman λ =0.71).
5.2.3. Cognitive elaboration
Cognitive elaboration was measured with the average scores (1 =
never, 10 =all the time) to items related to elaboration of news (Eve-
land, 2001; Jung et al., 2011). The index included two items: how
frequently respondents think about what I have encountered in the
newsand try to relate the news I encountered to other things I know
(M =5.29, SD =2.70, Spearman-Brown coefcient =0.90).
5.2.4. Incidental exposure to news
This construct taps on the amount of information about current
events, political matters, or public affair issues respondents report to
inadvertently acquire accidently (Kim et al., 2013; Tewksbury et al.,
2001). The variable was measured by averaging the scores (1 =never,
10 =all the time) of ten items. The participants were asked to respond to
the question Sometimes people encounter or come across news and
information on current events, public issues, or politics when they may
have been using media for a purpose other than to get the news. How
often does that happen to you for each of these media: tv, radio,
mobile devices, search engines, blogs, SNS, WhatsApp,
Facebook, Twitterand Instagram(M =5.21, SD =2.15,
α
=0.89).
5.3. Controls
We added multiple variables to account for any confounds. As extant
research has pointed out (Cho & McLeod, 2007; McLeod & Perse, 1994),
different demographic, political and media antecedents have been
shown to impact the level our dependent variable, i.e. political knowl-
edge. A set of sociodemographic variables were shown to inuence
citizenslevels of political knowledge (Eveland, Hayes, Shah, & Kwak,
2005; Jung et al., 2011) and hence we control for gender (46.7% males),
age (Median =3 [3655]), education attainment (Median =3 [Some
college], range =1 less than high school to 8 doctoral degree), family
income (Median =4 [$50,000 to $99,999], range =1 0 to $14,999to
7 $200,000 or more), and race (75.2% white). Motivations of political
knowledge and previous political behaviors have been shown to have a
strong impact on citizens level of political knowledge. Citizens that
usually engage in political behaviors may be more prone to be more
knowledgeable about politics too. Among such motivational and
behavioral political variables, political ideology, ofine political dis-
cussion, online political discussion, political interest and political ef-
cacy where shown to strongly predict citizens levels of political
knowledge and, therefore, all of them were included as potential co-
founds (Delli Carpini & Keeter, 1996; Cho et al., 2006; Gil de ˜
niga
et al., 2017, 2019). Finally, news use and network size were also
controlled as they may inuence citizenspolitical knowledge.
5.3.1. Political ideology
This construct computes respondents general, political and eco-
nomic ideology by creating an index of three items captured on a ten-
point scale (0 =strong democrat, 5 =Independent, and 10 =Strong
Republican): generally speaking, do you usually think of yourself as a
Republican, a Democrat, an Independent, or something else?, on po-
litical issues, where would you place yourselfand on economic issues,
where would you place yourself(M =6.31, SD =2.67,
α
=0.89).
5.3.2. Ofine political discussion
This variable was computed by asking participants about the fre-
quency that they discuss politics (Borah, Edgerly, Vraga, & Shah, 2013;
Valenzuela et al., 2012; Kim et al., 2018). Specically, questions asked
H. Gil de ˜
niga et al.
Computers in Human Behavior 121 (2021) 106803
5
respondents how often they talk face-to-face with: spouse/partner,
family, relatives, friends, Neighbors, co-workers you know well,
Acquaintancesand strangers(M =4.14, SD =2.13,
α
=0.81).
5.3.3. Online political discussion
This variable was computed with the help of questions that the
participants used to discuss politics with people (Kolstad, 2011). Spe-
cically, the questions asked how often they talk online with: spouse/
partner, family, relatives, friends, Neighbors, co-workers you know
well, Acquaintancesand strangers(M =3.03, SD =2.07,
α
=0.90).
5.3.4. Political interest
This variables was measured (e.g. Kenski & Stroud, 2006) based on
questions that asked respondents about their interest in political infor-
mation and about their attention to politics (M =6.13, SD =2.72,
Spearman-Brown coefcient =0.94), on a ten-point scale (1 =not at all
to 10 =a great deal).
5.3.5. Political efcacy
Citizensperceptions on their level of internal political efcacy are
more prone to consume news about public affair and politics and be
more knowledgeable as a result (Morrell, 2003; Niemi, Craig, & Mattei,
1991). The statements used were I have a good understanding of the
important political issues facing our country and I consider myself
well qualied to participate in politics, on a ten-point scale ranging
from 1 =strongly agree to 10 =strongly disagree (M =5.91, SD =2.44,
Spearman-Brown coefcient =0.81).
5.3.6. News use
An exhaustive construct of news use consumption was created with
the help of 15 items. These were measured on a 10-point scale (1 =never
to 10 =all the time): network TV news (e.g., ABC, CBS, NBC), local
television news (cf. Local afliate stations), infotainment programs (e.
g., The Daily Show, The Late Show with Stephen Colbert), national
newspapers (e.g., New York Times, Washington Post, USA Today),
local newspapers (e.g., Oregonian, Houston Chronicle, The Miami
Herald), MSNBC cable news, CNN cable news, FOX cable news,
radio news (e.g., NPR, talk shows), citizen journalism sites (e.g.,
GroundReport, CNNs iReport), Online news sites (e.g., Politico, VOX,
BuzzFeed), local news online sites (online sites related to news in your
local community), local news on social media, national news on
social media, Podcast (online or smartphone) and International
news outlets (e.g., BBC, Al Jazeera)(M =4.16, SD =1.97,
α
=0.89).
5.3.7. Network size
This variable was computed with two items (Moy & Gastil, 2006),
that measured on a ten-point scale about the number of people the re-
spondents talked to face-to-face or over the phone and about how many
the participants have talked to online, including email,” “chat rooms,
social networks(M =4.27, SD =12.94, Spearman-Brown coefcient
=0.48).
5.4. Analysis strategy
To test our main hypothesis and research question, we conducted a
series of hierarchical ordinary least-squares regressions with three
different independent variables (H1-H3 and RQ1): thorough engage-
ment with information, cognitive elaboration and political knowledge.
After the series of OLS regressions and to test whether thorough infor-
mation engagement and cognitive elaboration mediate between inci-
dental news exposure and political knowledge, we used PROCESS macro
for SPSS (Hayes, 2013, Model 6 [2 serial mediators], 95% CI and 5.000
bootstrapped samples, H4a-b-c).
6. Results
First, to learn more about key variables in the study, a zero-order
correlation table is introduced (see Table 1). We expected incidental
exposure to news to be positively associated with both thorough infor-
mation engagement (H1a) and cognitive elaboration (H1b). Consistent
with these theoretical predictions, Table 2 shows that incidental expo-
sure to news positively affects both through information engagement (β
=0.104, p <.001) and cognitive elaboration (β =0.096, p <.001).
Therefore, the more citizens are passively exposed to news, the more are
their chances to thoroughly engage with and think about the news
contents they stumble upon.
The thorough information engagement model accounts for 36.4% of
the total variance. Next, the cognitive elaboration regression model
accounted for 37.6%, after controlling for all potential cofounders; while
the political knowledge regression model, accounted for 31.8% of the
total variance, having therefore a considerable explanatory power.
Also consistent with our expectation (H2), results from the hierar-
chical OLS regression indicate a positive relationship between through
engagement with information and political knowledge (β =.125, p <
.001). Those users who thoroughly engage in the news reading show a
positive relationship with their political knowledge as a result. Likewise,
as expected (H3), cognitive elaboration is positively associated to po-
litical knowledge (β =0.117, p <.001), and therefore, users who
frequently reected on what they encountered in the news about public
affairs and politics showed higher political knowledge too.
Results also indicate a negative and statistically signicant associa-
tion between incidental exposure to news and political knowledge (β =
0.065, p <.05). Therefore, citizens that reported higher levels of
incidental exposure to news are more likely to know less about politics.
In order to test the potential serial mechanisms that might facilitate
political knowledge, we t the model 6 for the PROCESS macro for SPSS.
Consistent with our hypothesis, citizens that reported higher levels of
incidental exposure to new are more likely to thoroughly engage with
news, which in turn positively predicts their overall levels of political
knowledge (indirect effect =0.0124, 95% MC CIs =[0033 to 0.0242];
for full model see Table 3 and Fig. 1).
H4b predicted that incidental news exposure facilitates political
knowledge through cognitive elaboration. Results of the mediation
analysis yielded a positive association, showing that our prediction is
empirically supported. Therefore, higher incidental exposure to news is
positively related to higher cognitive elaboration, which in turn is
positively associated with citizenslevel of political knowledge (indirect
effect =0.011, 95% MC CIs =[0.0025 to 0.0226]).
The nal serial mechanism by which incidental exposure to news
may facilitate political knowledge include the two mediators: thorough
information engagement and cognitive elaboration. As theorized (H4c),
incidental news exposure facilitates political knowledge through thor-
ough information engagement and cognitive elaboration (indirect effect
=0.002, 95% MC CIs =[0.0004 to 0.0041]). Therefore, citizenshigher
levels of incidental exposure to news are and higher thorough engage-
ment with news contents, were positively associated with their level of
cognitive elaboration and higher level of political knowledge.
Table 1
Zero order correlations.
Variables 1 2 3 4
1. News Use
2. Incidental News Exposure .498***
3. Thorough Information Engagement .565*** .355***
4. Cognitive Elaboration .426*** .339*** .389***
5. Political Knowledge .062* .015 .216*** .227***
Note. Sample size =1365. *p <.05; **p <.01; p <.001.
H. Gil de ˜
niga et al.
Computers in Human Behavior 121 (2021) 106803
6
7. Discussion
An extensive body of research have examined the relationship be-
tween incidental news exposure and political learning (e.g., Fletcher &
Nielsen, 2017; Kim et al., 2013; Lee & Kim, 2017; Yamamoto & Morey,
2019). Yet, the ndings are not entirely conclusive (e.g., Oeldorf-Hirsch,
2018; Yoo & Gil de ˜
niga, 2014). Our study lls that gap in several
meaningful ways. First, it offers a more nuanced model introducing
different ways through which people engage with incidental news
content. It also accounts for peoples mental elaboration and cognitive
processing. Our ndings show that the direct effects from incidental
news exposure to political knowledge seem to be either null or even
negative. However, incidental news exposure is directly related to more
deeply engaging with the content individuals get exposed to inadver-
tently, as well as to cognitive elaboration of that information. Most
importantly, our ndings reveal that there is a mediated effect from
incidental news exposure to political knowledge via these paths: a)
thorough news information use, b) cognitive elaboration and c) by
means of thorough information engagement, and cognitive elaboration.
In a nutshell, the studys results are able to offer an explanation as to
why over the years scholars have found inconsistent effects of incidental
news exposure on political knowledge such as positive (e.g., Lee & Kim,
2017; Yamamoto & Morey, 2019) or null and negative effects (Oel-
dorf-Hirsch, 2018; Gotfried et al., 2017; Cacciatore et al., 2018; Lee &
Xenos, 2019). Prior research has shown that sometimes individuals learn
from stumbling upon news and while other times they dont. Findings
from this study suggest that the answer might not necessarily depend
solely on the exposure, the amount of exposure, or even the frequency of
exposure. But rather what individuals tend to do after they get inci-
dentally exposed to news.
Deeply engaging with the information (Eveland, 2001; Jung et al.,
2011) that they were inadvertently exposed to also matters. When
people use news and information strategies that include exhaustive
reading, thorough watching/listening, and going through the ‘whole
newsexperience as intended by journalists, incidental news exposure
fuels political learning. Thus, it is important to discern how individuals
engage with news content and think about it. Individuals who thor-
oughly engage with the information yield further political knowledge
gain. The ndings show a very similar relationship with cognitive
elaboration. When individuals make better connections cognitively,
they may have a better understanding of important public affair issues,
and better connect those with their lives. Likewise, cognitive elaboration
allows citizens to connect prior knowledge more efciently with the new
information they may have been exposed to incidentally. As results
attest, the cognitive connections have important implications for
knowledge acquisition.
Nonetheless, our study comes with some important limitations. First,
relying on cross-sectional survey data to test causal mediating mecha-
nisms is problematic. However, mediation models guided by theory
have been successfully tested by prior research (e.g., Chen & Chen,
2020; Kong & Wang, 2020; Lab˘
ar & T¸epordei, 2019; Lee & Borah, 2020).
Specically, the mediating mechanism of news surveillance variables,
leading to cognitive reection and to political learning is a
well-established theoretical model in the literature (see for instance
Eveland, 2002; Jung et al., 2011; Cho et al., 2009). As such, this previous
scholarship lends a layer of condence and theoretical validity to the
directionality of our theoretical model.
Additionally, we test the effects of the incidental news exposure as
frequency self-assessment. That is, we do not content analyze the type of
incidental news exposure on social media. Future research could study
the kind of information that individuals are exposed to incidentally. The
current study is fundamental in understanding the mechanism involved
after individuals are incidentally exposed to news, leading to gain in
political knowledge. These paths are important to show that when in-
dividuals engage with the content, incidental news exposure shows
positive relationships with political learning.
Future research should attempt to understand other such processes
that could bolster potential positive effects of incidental news exposure.
Variables such as social network connections and trust could play an
important role in this relationship. For example, there might be a dif-
ference in information processing and knowledge acquisition (Levin &
Cross, 2004) when the information comes from their friends (strong-ties)
versus people they do not know well (weak-ties). Trust in the informa-
tion could also inuence this relationship (Miller & Krosnick, 2000).
These paths could also be determined by the type of content such as
videos versus text and participantsengagement with the content.
Future studies could also examine the antecedents of thorough
engagement and cognitive elaboration. We know now that thorough
engagement and cognitive elaboration are two important variables for
individuals to learn from incidental news exposure. To gain the benets
of this kind of exposure we need to think about what causes individuals
to engage with the content. Considering the high occurrence of inci-
dental news exposure on social media (Morris & Morris, 2017; Purcell,
Rainie, Mitchell, Rosenstiel, & Omstead, 2010), understanding what
makes people engage with the content is noteworthy.
Table 2
Incidental news exposure predicting thorough information engagement, cogni-
tive elaboration and political knowledge.
Thorough Information
Engagement
Cognitive
Elaboration
Political
Knowledge
Block 1: Demographic
Gender -.081* -.127*** -.229***
Age -.095** -.065* .200***
Education .100** .155*** .270***
Income .040 -.016 .147***
Race .027 -.023 .011
ΔR
2
(%) 3.0% 4.0% 25.4%
Block 2: Antecedents
Political Ideology -.060* -.100*** -.053
News Use .553*** .119*** -.006
Political Interest -.003 .032 .069*
Political Efcacy .030 .050 .054
Political Discussion
Off
.108** .426*** .117**
Political Discussion
On
-.111** .066 -.097*
Network Size .103*** .047 .128***
ΔR
2
(%) 35.6% 35.0% 29.3%
Block 3: Mediators
Thorough Info.
Engagement
.172*** .125***
Cognitive
Elaboration
.117***
ΔR
2
(%) 36.9% 31.5%
Block 4: Variable of Interest
Incidental News
Exposure
.104*** .096*** -.065*
ΔR
2
(%) 36.4% 37.6% 31.8%
Note. Sample size =1365. Cell entries are nal-entry OLS standardized Beta (β)
coefcients. *p <.05; **p <.01; p <.001.
Table 3
Direct and indirect effects of incidental news exposure on political knowledge.
В
Incidental
News
Political
Knowledge
-.059*
Incidental
News
Info.
Engagement
Political
Knowledge
.011*
Incidental
News
Cognitive
Elaboration
Political
Knowledge
.012*
Incidental
News
Info.
Engagement
Cognitive
Elaboration
Political
Knowledge
.002*
Note. Standardized path coefcients. Controlled for age, education, income,
race, political ideology, news use, political interest, political efcacy, ofine
political discussion, online political discussion and network size.
H. Gil de ˜
niga et al.
Computers in Human Behavior 121 (2021) 106803
7
The current study was conducted in the U.S. and as such are related
to the population of the U.S. The phenomenon of being incidentally
exposed to news is common around the world. Almost 50% of the global
population uses social media platforms (Clement, 2020) and they are
equally likely to be accidently exposed to news as demonstrated in our
participants from the U.S. We think that the inuence of this exposure
and the underlying mechanisms will possibly and relatively be the same
on individuals regardless of which part of the world they are from.
Although there is no research that has compared these mechanisms in
different parts of the world, ndings from research on psychological
well-being (Spencer-Rodgers, Peng, Wang, & Hou, 2004) or collective
vs. individual characteristics (Chao, Zhang, & Chiu et al., 2008)
demonstrate country-level differences. Thus, future research should test
our hypotheses in different countries and distinct cultures. A compara-
tive study would shed light into the phenomenon of incidental news
exposure and the underlying mechanisms around the world.
All in all, this study has important theoretical and practical impli-
cations. Theoretically, it takes initial steps to explain the potential un-
derlying mechanisms involved in the path from incidental news
exposure to political knowledge acquisition. A valuable contribution to
the literature because it shed light over the mixed ndings puzzle from
prior research. Our ndings also open a whole area for future research,
which can help understand these mechanisms further. Likewise, the
study encapsulates important implications for journalists. Knowing the
process by which incidental news exposure can help people gain
knowledge, journalists can pay attention to strategies that might help
individuals who are just scrolling their social media feeds to engage with
the news content. These are individuals who are not necessarily looking
for news and yet they are learning about politics from incidental news
exposure. As social media and algorithmically curated news consump-
tion mechanisms continue to ourish, incidental news exposure research
will continue to gain visibility. This study is a modest step contributing
to this literature.
Credit author statement
This document is to state that all authors fairly contributed to the
manuscript and deserve authorship. First author designed the study,
collected the data, run the analyses, and wrote the paper. Second author
contributed to rene the ideas and write the paper. Third author
contributed to rene the ideas, write the paper, and helped with the data
collection.
References
Althaus, S. L. (2003). Collective preferences in democratic politics: Opinion surveys and the
will of the people. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Amsalem, E., & Nir, L. (2019). Does interpersonal discussion increase political
knowledge? A meta-analysis. Communication Research, 123. https://doi.org/
10.1177/0093650219866357
Boczkowski, P. J., Mitchelstein, E., & Matassi, M. (2018). News comes across when Im
in a moment of leisure: Understanding the practices of incidental news consumption
on social media. New Media & Society, 20(10), 35233539. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1461444817750396
Borah, P., Edgerly, S., Vraga, E. K., & Shah, D. V. (2013). Hearing and talking to the other
side: Antecedents of cross-cutting exposure in adolescents. Mass Communication &
Society, 16(3), 391416. https://doi.org/10.1080/15205436.2012.693568
Cacciatore, M. A., Yeo, S. K., Scheufele, D. A., Xenos, M. A., Brossard, D., & Corley, E. A.
(2018). Is Facebook making us dumber? Exploring social media use as a predictor of
political knowledge. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 95(2), 404424.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1077699018770447
Chaffee, S. H., & Kanihan, S. F. (1997). Learning about politics from the mass media.
Political Communication, 14(4), 421430, 10.1080=105846097199218.
Chaffee, S. H., & Schleuder, J. (1986). Measurement and effects of attention to media
news. Human Communication Research, 13(1), 76107. https://doi.org/10.1111/
j.1468-2958.1986.tb00096.x
Chao, M., Zhang, Z. X., & Chiu, C. Y. (2008). Personal and collective culpability
judgment: A functional analysis of east asiannorth American differences. Journal of
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39(6), 730744. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0022022108323788
Chen, H., & Chen, H. (2020). Understanding the relationship between online self-image
expression and purchase intention in SNS games: A moderated mediation
investigation. Computers in Human Behavior, 112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
chb.2020.106477
Choi, S. (2014). Flow, diversity, form, and inuence of political talk in social-media-
based public forums. Human Communication Research, 40(2), 209237. https://doi.
org/10.1111/hcre.12023
Cho, J., & McLeod, D. M. (2007). Structural antecedents to knowledge and participation:
Extending the knowledge gap concept to participation. Journal of Communication, 57
(2), 205228. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2007.00340.x
Cho, J., Shah, D. V., McLeod, J. M., McLeod, D. M., Scholl, R. M., & Gotlieb, M. R. (2009).
Campaigns, reection, and deliberation: Advancing an OSROR model of
communication effects. Communication Theory, 19(1), 6688. https://doi.org/
10.1111/j.1468-2885.2008.01333.x
Clement, J. (2020). Number of global social network users 2010-2023. Retrieved from https
://www.statista.com/statistics/278414/number-of-worldwide-social-network-us
ers/.
Delli Carpini, M. X., & Keeter, S. (1993). Measuring political knowledge: Putting rst
things rst. American Journal of Political Science, 37(4), 11791206. https://doi.org/
10.2307/2111549
Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of democracy. New York: Harper & Brothers.
van Erkel, P. F., & Van Aelst, P. (2020). Why dont we learn from social media? Studying
effects of and mechanisms behind social media news use on general surveillance
political knowledge. Political Communication, 119.
Eveland, W. P. (2002). News information processing as mediator of the relationship
between motivations and political knowledge. Journalism & Mass Communication
Quarterly, 79(1), 2640. https://doi.org/10.1177/107769900207900103
Fig. 1. Direct and Indirect Relationship between Incidental News Exposure and Political Knowledge. Note. Standardized path coefcients. Controlling for age,
education, income, race, political ideology, news use, political interest, political efcacy, ofine political discussion, online political discussion and network size.
H. Gil de ˜
niga et al.
Computers in Human Behavior 121 (2021) 106803
8
Eveland, W. P., Hayes, A. F., Shah, D. V., & Kwak, N. (2005). Understanding the
relationship between communication and political knowledge: A model comparison
approach using panel data. Political Communication, 22(4), 423446. https://doi.org/
10.1080/10584600500311345
Eveland, W. P., Jr. (2001). The cognitive mediation model of learning from the news:
Evidence from nonelection, off-year election, and presidential election contexts.
Communication Research, 28(5), 571601. https://doi.org/10.1177/
009365001028005001
Eveland, W. P., Jr. (2004). The effect of political discussion in producing informed
citizens: The roles of information, motivation, and elaboration. Political
Communication, 21(2), 177193. https://doi.org/10.1080/10584600490443877
Eveland, W. P., Marton, K., & Seo, M. (2004). Moving beyond just the facts.
Communication Research, 31(1), 82108. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0093650203260203
Feezell, J. T. (2018). Agenda setting through social media: The importance of incidental
news exposure and social ltering in the digital era. Political Research Quarterly, 71
(2), 482494. https://doi.org/10.1177/1065912917744895
Feezell, J. T., & Ortiz, B. (2019). ‘I saw it on Facebook: An experimental analysis of political
learning through social media (pp. 120). Information, Communication & Society.
https://doi.org/10.1080/1369118X.2019.1697340
Fletcher, R., & Nielsen, R. K. (2017). Are people incidentally exposed to news on social
media? A comparative analysis. New Media & Society, 20(7), 24502468. https://doi.
org/10.1177/1461444817724170
Galston, W. A. (2001). Political knowledge, political engagement, and civic education.
Annual Review of Political Science, 4, 217234. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.
polisci.4.1.217
Gil de Zú˜
niga, H. (2015). Toward a European public sphere? The promise and perils of
modern democracy in the age of digital and social media. International Journal of
Communication, 9(1), 31523160.
Gil de Zú˜
niga, H., Diehl, T., & Ard´
evol-Abreu, A. (2017). Internal, external, and
government political efcacy: Effects on news use, discussion, and political
participation. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 61(3), 574596.
Gil de Zú˜
niga, H., Diehl, T., Huber, B., & Liu, J. H. (2019). The citizen communication
mediation model across countries: A multilevel mediation model of news use and
discussion on political participation. Journal of Communication, 69(2), 144167.
Gil de Zú˜
niga, H., Gonz´
alez- Gonz´
alez, P., & Goyanes, M. (2021). Pathways to political
persuasion: Linking online, social media, and fake news with political attitude
change through political discussion. American Behavioral Scientist.
Gil de Zú˜
niga, H., & Hsuan-Ting, C. (2019). Digital media and politics: Effects of the great
information and communication divides. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media,
63(3), 365373.
Gottfried, J. A., Hardy, B. W., Holbert, R. L., Winneg, K. M., & Jamieson, K. H. (2016).
The changing nature of political debate consumption: Social media, multitasking,
and knowledge acquisition. Political Communication, 34(2), 172199. https://doi.
org/10.1080/10584609.2016.1154120
Jung, N., Kim, Y., & Gil de Zúniga, H. (2011). The mediating role of knowledge and
efcacy in the effects of communication on political participation. Mass
Communication & Society, 14(4), 407430. https://doi.org/10.1080/
15205436.2010.496135
Karnowski, V., Kümpel, A. S., Leonhard, L., & Leiner, D. J. (2017). From incidental news
exposure to news engagement. How perceptions of the news post and news usage
patterns inuence engagement with news articles encountered on Facebook.
Computers in Human Behavior, 76, 4250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
chb.2017.06.041
Kaufhold, K., Valenzuela, S., & Zú˜
niga, H. G. D. (2010). Citizen journalism and
democracy: How user-generated news use relates to political knowledge and
participation. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 87(34), 515529.
https://doi.org/10.1177/107769901008700305
Kenski, K., & Stroud, N. J. (2006). Connections between Internet use and political
efcacy,knowledge, and participation. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 50,
173192, 10.1207=s15506878jobem5002_1.
Kim, J. W., Chadha, M., & Gil de Zú˜
niga, H. (2018). News media use and cognitive
elaboration. The mediating role of media efcacy. Revista Latina de Comunicaci´
on
Social, 73(2), 168183.
Kim, Y., Chen, H. T., & Gil de Zú˜
niga, H. (2013). Stumbling upon news on the Internet:
Effects of incidental news exposure and relative entertainment use on political
engagement. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(6), 26072614. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.chb.2013.06.005
Knoll, J., Matthes, J., & Heiss, R. (2018). The social media political participation model:
A goal systems theory perspective. Convergence: The International Journal of Research
Into New Media Technologies, 26(1), 135156. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1354856517750366
Kobayashi, T., & Inamasu, K. (2015). The knowledge leveling effect of portal sites.
Communication Research, 42(4), 482502. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0093650214534965
Kong, S. C., & Wang, Y. Q. (2020). Formation of computational identity through
computational thinking perspectives development in programming learning: A
mediation analysis among primary school students. Computers in Human Behavior,
106, 112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.106230
Lab˘
ar, A. V., & T¸ epordei, A. M. (2019). The interplay between time perspective, internet
use and smart phone in-class multitasking: A mediation analysis. Computers in Human
Behavior, 93, 3339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.11.050
Lecheler, S., & Vreese, C. H. D. (2017). News media, knowledge, and political interest:
Evidence of a dual role from a eld experiment. Journal of Communication, 67(4),
545564. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12314
Lee, D., & Borah, P. (2020). Self-presentation on instagram and friendship development
among young adults: A moderated mediation model of media richness, perceived
functionality, and openness. Computers in Human Behavior, 103, 5766. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.09.017
Lee, J. K., & Kim, E. (2017). Incidental exposure to news: Predictors in the social media
setting and effects on information gain online. Computers in Human Behavior, 75,
10081015. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.02.018
Lee, S., & Xenos, M. (2019). Social distraction? Social media use and political knowledge
in two U.S. Presidential elections. Computers in Human Behavior, 90, 1825. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.08.006
Lee, H., & Yang, J. (2014). Political knowledge gaps among news consumers with
different news media repertoires across multiple platforms. International Journal of
Communication, 8, 597617.
Levin, D., & Cross, R. (2004). The strength of weak ties you can trust: The mediating role
of trust in effective knowledge transfer. Management Science, 50(11), 14771490.
https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.1030.0136
McLeod, J. M., & McDonald, D. G. (1985). Beyond simple exposure: Media orientations
and their impact on political processes. Communication Research, 12(1), 333.
https://doi.org/10.1177/009365085012001001
McLeod, D. M., & Perse, E. M. (1994). Direct and indirect effects of socioeconomic status
on public affairs knowledge. Journalism Quarterly, 71, 433442. https://doi.org/
10.1177/107769909407100216
McNamara, D. S., Kintsch, E., Butler-Songer, N., & Kintsch, W. (1996). Are good texts
always better? Interaction of text coherence, background knowledge, and levels of
understanding in learning from text. Cognition and Instruction, 14(1), 143. https://
doi.org/10.1207/s1532690xci1401_1
Miller, J. M., & Krosnick, J. A. (2000). News media impact on the ingredients of
presidential evaluations: Politically knowledgeable citizens are guided by a trusted
source. American Journal of Political Science, 44(2), 301315. https://doi.org/
10.2307/2669312
Moeller, J., & Vreese, C. D. (2015). Spiral of political learning: The reciprocal
relationship of news media use and political knowledge among adolescents.
Communication Research, 46(8), 10781094. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0093650215605148
Mondak, J. J. (2001). Developing valid knowledge scales. American Journal of Political
Science, 45, 224238. https://doi.org/10.2307/2669369
Morrell, M. E. (2003). Survey and experimental evidence for a reliable and valid measure
of internal political efcacy. Public Opinion Quarterly, 67(4), 589602.
Morris, D. S., & Morris, J. S. (2017). Evolving learning: The changing effect of internet
access on political knowledge and engagement (1998-2012). Sociological Forum, 32
(2), 339358. https://doi.org/10.1111/socf.12333
Moy, P., & Gastil, J. (2006). Predicting deliberative communication: The impact of
discussion networks, media use, and political cognitions. Political Communication, 23
(4), 443460.
Niemi, R. G., Craig, S. C., & Mattei, F. (1991). Measuring internal political efcacy in the
1988 national election study. American Political Science Review, 85, 14071413.
Oeldorf-Hirsch, A. (2018). The role of engagement in learning from active and incidental
news exposure on social media. Mass Communication & Society, 21(2), 225247.
https://doi.org/10.1080/15205436.2017.1384022
Park, C. S., & Gil de ˜
niga, H. (2020). Learning about politics from mass media and
social media: Moderating roles of press freedom and public service broadcasting in
11 countries. International Journal of Public Opinion Research.
Pew American Life Project. (2018). American trends panel wave 36. Available at:
https://www.pewsocialtrends.org/dataset/american-trends-panel-wave-36/.
Purcell, K., Rainie, L., Mitchell, A., Rosenstiel, T., & Omstead, K. (2010). Understanding
the participatory news consumer. Retrieved from http://www.pewinternet.
org/2010/03/01/understanding-the-participatory-news-consumer.
Shahin, S., Salda˜
na, M., & Gil de Zú˜
niga, H. (2020). Peripheral elaboration model: The
impact of incidental news exposure on political participation. Journal of Information
Technology & Politics, 116.
Shehata, A., Hopmann, D. N., Nord, L., & H¨
oijer, J. (2015). Television channel content
proles and differential knowledge growth: A test of the inadvertent learning
hypothesis using panel data. Political Communication, 32(3), 377395. https://doi.
org/10.1080/10584609.2014.955223
Spencer-Rodgers, J., Peng, K., Wang, L., & Hou, Y. (2004). Dialectical self-esteem and
East-West differences in psychological well-being. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 30(11), 14161432. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167204264243
Stegmann, K., Wecker, C., Weinberger, A., & Fischer, F. (2012). Collaborative
argumentation and cognitive elaboration in a computer-supported collaborative
learning environment. Instructional Science, 40(2), 297323. https://doi.org/
10.1007/s11251-011-9174-5
Stein, B. S., & Bransford, J. D. (1979). Constraints on effective elaboration: Effects of
precision and subject generation. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18,
769777.
Strauß, N., Huber, B., & Gil de Zú ˜
niga, H. (2020). Yes, I saw itbut didnt read it…” A
cross-country study, exploring relationships between incidental news exposure and
news use across platforms. Digital Journalism, 8(9), 11811205.
Tewksbury, D., Weaver, A. J., & Maddex, B. D. (2001). Accidently informed: Incidental
news exposure on the world wide web. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly,
78(3), 533554. https://doi.org/10.1177/107769900107800309
Valeriani, A., & Vaccari, C. (2016). Accidental exposure to politics on social media as
online participation equalizer in Germany, Italy, and the United Kingdom. New
Media & Society, 18(9), 18571874. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444815616223
Wei, R., & Lo, V. H. (2008). News media use and knowledge about the 2006 US midterm
elections: Why exposure matters in voter learning. International Journal of Public
Opinion Research, 20(3), 347362.
H. Gil de ˜
niga et al.
Computers in Human Behavior 121 (2021) 106803
9
Yadamsuren, B., & Erdelez, S. (2010). Incidental exposure to online news. Proceedings of
the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 47(1), 18. https://doi.
org/10.1002/meet.14504701237
Yamamoto, M., & Morey, A. C. (2019). Incidental news exposure on social media: A
campaign communication mediation approach. Social Media +Society, 5(2), 112.
https://doi.org/10.1177/2056305119843619
Yoo, S. W., & Gil De Zú˜
niga, H. (2019). The role of heterogeneous political discussion and
partisanship on the effects of incidental news exposure online. Journal of Information
Technology & Politics, 16(1), 2035.
Yoo, S. W., & Gil de Zú˜
niga, H. (2014). Connecting blog, Twitter and Facebook use with
gaps in knowledge and participation. Communications Society, 27(4), 3348. https://
doi.org/10.15581/003.27.4.33-48
Zukin, C., & Snyder, R. (1984). Passive learning: When the media environment is the
message. Public Opinion Quarterly, 48(3), 629638.
Homero Gil de Zú˜
niga. Ph.D. in Politics at Universidad Europea de Madrid and Ph.D. in
Mass Communication at University of Wisconsin Madison, serves as Distinguished
Research Professor at University of Salamanca where he directs the Democracy Research
Unit (DRU), as Professor at Pennsylvania State University, and as Senior Research Fellow
at Universidad Diego Portales, Chile. His research addresses the inuence of new
technologies and digital media over peoples daily lives, as well as the effect of such use on
the overall democratic process.
Porismita Borah. Ph.D. in Mass Communication at University of Wisconsin Madison, is
part of the graduate faculty in the Murrow College and in the Prevention Science program
in WSU. She is also afliated with the Murrow Center for Media and Health Promotion. Her
research interests are emerging technology in the context of politics and health. Her
research has been published in many prestigious journals including Journal of Commu-
nication and Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication. She serves in the editorial
boards of journals including Journal of Communication and Journal of Information
Technology and Politics.
Manuel Goyanes. Ph.D. in Journalism at Universidad de Santiago de Compostela, teaches
at Carlos III University in Madrid and his main interests are in media management and
sociology of communication sciences. He has written about leadership, news overload, and
business models. His works have appeared in journals like Information, Communication &
Society, Journalism, Journalism Studies, Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly,
and so on.
H. Gil de ˜
niga et al.
... A plethora of research lent evidence to the effects of INE on a variety of societal outcomes, which include but are not limited to political participation, civic engagement (e.g., Barnidge, 2023;Kim et al., 2013;Shahin et al., 2021), and knowledge acquisition (e.g., Gil de Zúñiga et al., 2021;Takano et al., 2021). Although its role in mobilizing activities has been widely confirmed, INE's effects on learning has generated mixed, and sometimes antithetical, conclusions. ...
... In Oeldorf-Hirsch's (2018) research, INE on Facebook did not have a significant effect on knowledge acquisition; hence, the author implied that the primary influence of social media is not to facilitate knowledge acquisition but to engage users. Gil de Zúñiga et al. (2021) found that INE had null or mild direct effect on political knowledge. For non-news purpose social media users, Goyanes and Demeter (2022) also suggested an insignificant relationship between INE and public affairs knowledge. ...
... Leveraging a two-wave panel data, Shahin and associates (2021) found that elaboration had a positive relationship with political INE and played a mediating role between political INE and engagement. In their research, Gil de Zúñiga et al. (2021) suggested that although the relationship between INE and knowledge was not significant, the indirect path was rendered significant and positive when cognitive elaboration was incorporated as a mediator. Even for counter-attitudinal INE, it could encourage cognitive elaboration in the context of multi-platform social media use (Chen et al., 2022). ...
... The processing of political content depends on the speed at which (and thus the duration over which) processing occurs (Matthes et al., 2020;Stoker et al., 2015), as well as the user's goals and willingness to spend time on elaboration (Kaye & Johnson, 2004;Van Damme et al., 2019). This is especially relevant in political communication research, given the greater cognitive resources needed to affect knowledge gain or opinion change (Gil de Zúñiga et al., 2021), and the variability of cognitive resources over the course of a day. Studies of repeated exposure that do not differentiate between durations are ignoring such differences, and doing so at the expense of useful tests of theories about how political information processing works. ...
Article
Full-text available
The affordances of the smartphone are shifting individuals toward ever smaller and more fragmented units of political experience that are primarily provided by social contacts and non-traditional sources and voices. Accordingly, granular investigations of individuals' exposure to political content must motivate political communication theories away from theories based on exposure to extended, uninterrupted political narratives that are constructed by news professionals and formal information outlets. In this piece, we use a novel approach to granular assessment of political exposure on smartphones, to reveal significant 1 Daniel Muise:
... Exposure to social media, even unintentionally, can trigger cognitive clarification since the cognitive process of information processing happens after exposure . Research indicates that cognitive clarification can mediate accidental encounters and political knowledge (Gil et al., 2021). A study conducted in Jakarta demonstrated that cognitive elaboration mediates the relationship between political persuasion and social media use (Venus et al., 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
In contemporary society, social media has emerged as the predominant platform for the dissemination of political information, with a primary goal of influencing user perspectives. Despite its prevalence, scant research has scrutinized the cognitive mechanisms underlying the reception of political messages via social media and their consequent impact on attitude transformation. This study endeavors to investigate the nexus between political information consumption on social media and the efficacy of political persuasion, while also probing the roles of political interest and cognitive elaboration in this persuasive process. Employing a survey methodology distributed through social media networks, the study amassed responses from 470 participants residing in Jakarta and its environs. Findings elucidate the discernible influence of social media usage for political information consumption and cognitive elaboration on the dynamics of political persuasion, whereas political interest exhibited negligible impact. Consequently, this study underscores the potential for political information purveyors to leverage social media platforms to foster cognitive engagement among users, thereby enhancing receptivity to political messaging.
... However, few studies have explored a less motivated and intentional pattern, namely, information scanning (Borah et al., 2022;Su et al., 2024). More specifically, while some individuals actively search for medical information from online sources, a significant portion tend to acquire information incidentally, primarily through scanning behavior (Gil de Zúñiga et al., 2021;Jiang et al., 2023). This tendency becomes particularly evident in the context of algorithm-driven social media platforms, such as TikTok, which continuously recommend pertinent information to users with health-related concerns (Hu & Sun, 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
Health misinformation has become rampant in the digital age, because people often share unauthenticated health messages on online platforms. While previous studies have pinpointed different factors that influence the dissemination of such unverified health content, few have delved into the transition from exposure to online health information to its unverified sharing. Drawing upon the theory of motivated information management, this study proposes a research model that connects online health information scanning to unverified health information sharing. Using two-wave survey data in China, we found a positive association between online health information scanning and uncertainty discrepancy in Time 1. Such discrepancy subsequently led to perceived health threat and health anxiety in Time 2, which ultimately increased unverified health information sharing in Time 2. Overall, this study enriches the information management literature by theorizing the pathways from information scanning to unverified information sharing through cognitive and emotional responses. Moreover, the findings offer practical insights for social media operators and health professionals to propose informed strategies to curb the spread of unverified health information on the internet.
... In some studies, prior knowledge has played an important moderating role in incidental learning effects (e.g., Baum, 2003), whereas in other studies, beliefs and a desire to resolve cognitive dissonance seemed to be the most influential factors (Cappella et al., 2015;Zimmer et al., 2019). Some researchers have pointed to the negative effects of echo chambers (Hills, 2019) or motivated reasoning (Sinatra et al., 2014) as proximal causes of incidental and intentional learning effects, whereas others have argued users' ability to recognize misinformation, and their willingness to devote sufficient effort to scrutinize it, are stronger predictors of such effects (Gil de Zúñiga et al., 2021;Pennycook & Rand, 2019). Given the unsettled state of the literature, we argue for the need for more basic, foundational research into people's mental processing after incidental exposure, whether individual differences in knowledge and dispositions predict such processing, whether these effects vary based upon the type of information presented, and what effects, if any, individual differences and mental processing have on how people respond to incidental exposure. ...
Article
Full-text available
More and more, people are abandoning the active pursuit of news, assuming instead that important information will be pushed to them via their social media networks. This approach to news makes people susceptible to the vast amounts of misinformation online, yet research on the effects of this kind of engagement is mixed. More research is needed on technology incidental learning effects, defined as changes in knowledge, attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors as a result of being exposed to information while pursuing goals other than learning (e.g., entertainment). In this study, we examined how 51 college students responded to incidental exposure to accurate and inaccurate COVID-19 information delivered via a simulated social media environment. Participants’ verbalizations during think-aloud protocols indicated numerous mental processes including cognition, metacognition, epistemic cognition, motivation, and emotions. Positively valanced mental processing was more often expressed with accurate COVID-19 information and negatively valanced mental processing was more often verbalized with misinformation. Negatively valanced evaluations of knowledge claims and sources predicted less engagement with COVID-19 misinformation posts. However, in many cases the relations among verbalized mental processing and behavioral responses were complex or nonobvious. For example, participants’ positive metacognition and epistemic cognition verbalizations decreased their likelihood of engaging with accurate COVID-19 information, whereas positive interest was associated with an increased likelihood of engaging with misinformation. Our findings have implications for how to accurately infer people’s beliefs and intentions from their social media behaviors and how to design interventions to help people be more active and thoughtful consumers of online information.
... ,Wessely, Dhavan, & Rubin, 2020;Borah, Edgerly, Vraga, & Shah, 2013;Gil de Zúñiga, Borah, & Goyanes, 2021). Age (M = 1.55, ...
Article
More than 6 million people have died due to the COVID-19 pandemic. In order to slow down the spread of COVID-19, health authorities have created numerous guidelines. In the current study, we use survey data from the U.S. and social cognitive theory (SCT) to examine the associations among self-efficacy, outcome expectancies, and willingness to perform recommended COVID-19 related health behavior. Considering the misinformation-filled information ecology during the pandemic, we also examine the moderating role of misperceptions in these relationships. Our findings show that in general the SCT hypotheses hold for COVID-19 related behavior willingness. The interaction effects with COVID-19 misperceptions show that self-efficacy is not enough to understand people’s health behavior. Higher outcome expectancies are important and may be able to overcome even if people held high misperceptions. Our findings have direct implications for communication theory and for health organizations in the contemporary information ecology.
Article
As they approach voting age, young people aged 17 to 19 years old are at a significant turning point in their political development. In this formative stage, their perceptions and understanding of the political landscape depend, among other things, on the diversity of information sources and content to which they are exposed to on a daily basis. In such a context, political knowledge plays a key role in informed decision-making for first-time voters. However, many young people tend to overestimate their levels of political knowledge, a phenomenon explained by the Dunning-Kruger effect. This study examines factors influencing both factual and perceived political knowledge among Romanian high school students (N=519). The main findings reveal a significant discrepancy between the actual political knowledge of young people and their perceived understanding of political matters. Political efficacy (the belief in one’s ability to understand and engage in politics) proves to be a strong predictor of both factual and perceived political knowledge. Those with high levels of political efficacy are not only more informed but also believe they know more about politics than their peers. Furthermore, results show that young people who believe they know more than their peers about political affairs consume more mainstream media news, but no other correlation of political knowledge with news consumption is significant. Additionally, while male students perceive themselves as more knowledgeable than females, no significant gender differences in factual knowledge were observed. These findings highlight the need for educational strategies to improve both political knowledge and efficacy, contributing to a more informed and participatory youth electorate in Romania.
Article
Full-text available
In the context of social media becoming users' primary access to information, such as news, incidental news exposure under the influence of algorithmic recommendation and its democratic consequences are worth studying. While studies on the relationship between social media users’ incidental news exposure and online political participation have been widely explored, we know little about the current mechanisms of variable association in the context of algorithmic recommendations. This study examines the mediating role of algorithmic awareness and the moderating effect of intolerance of uncertainty on the relationship between incidental news exposure and online political participation among Chinese social media users. Findings indicate that algorithmic awareness negatively mediates this relationship, suggesting that users’ participation in online politics is heightened by their awareness of algorithmic curation. Intolerance of uncertainty positively moderates the link between incidental news exposure and online political participation, emphasizing the impact of personality traits on democratic engagement. This study underscores the importance of transparent algorithmic systems and user-centred design in fostering informed citizen participation in the digital era and offers a comprehensive perspective on the role of serendipity in Chinese democratic participation.
Article
Full-text available
There is a vast research tradition examining the antecedents that lead people to be politically persuaded. However, political opinion and attitude change in social media has received comparatively scarce attention. This study seeks to shed light on this strand of the literature by theoretically advancing and empirically testing a structural equation model linking online social media, and fake news exposure, with political discussion and political persuasion in social media. Drawing on autoregressive causal tests from two waves of US survey panel data collected in 2019 and 2020, our results indicate that online, social media fake news, and political discussion are all positive predictors of individual political attitude change. Furthermore, structural equation tests reveal that online and social media news lead individuals to be exposed to fake news, which, in turn, predict higher levels of political discussion, ultimately facilitating political persuasion in the social media realm. Limitations and further suggestions for future research are also included in the study.
Article
Full-text available
Today, people are increasingly exposed to news on various channels without actively seeking it. However, less is known about the link between the so-called incidental news exposure (INE) and actual news consumption. Using a two-wave panel data set from 18 countries around the world, we study the so-far under researched relation between INE and news consumption across various platforms over time. In doing so, we control for key micro-level variables such as news use, political interest and trust in media as well as macro-level variables, including internet connectivity, GDP, press freedom and literacy rate. The analyses yield an optimistic picture, showing that INE plays a bridging function across countries, leading to actual news consumption on traditional, online and social media platforms. However, trust in news and political interest do not seem to play key moderating roles. Instead, individual analyses per country imply that the link between INE and actual news use is more apparent for online and social media news platforms, and particularly in countries where general social media usage has been reported to be considerably high (e.g. Brazil, Philippines, Taiwan, UK and USA).
Article
Full-text available
To examine whether mass media and social media relate to political knowledge, the study draws upon an original survey of adults from 11 countries, the 2014 CESifo DICE Report on public service broadcasting, and the 2015 Press Freedom Index by Freedom House. Findings reveal that news use via television, newspapers, online news sites, and social media is positively associated with political knowledge. Furthermore, press freedom and strong public broadcasting strengthen the association between news use (via both mass and social media) and political knowledge. The findings suggest that the media system plays a crucial role in creating a political learning environment even in this social media age.
Article
Full-text available
This study places the “cognitive elaboration model” on news gathering and political behavior within the dual-processing “elaboration likelihood model” to derive hypotheses about the effects of incidental news exposure and tests them using two-wave panel data. Results indicate incidental news exposure predicts online participation but not offline participation – underlining the importance of differentiating between political behaviors in the two environments. The key finding, however, is that news elaboration mediates the positive relationship between incidental exposure and political participation, which is theorized as taking place through the peripheral route of elaboration – as opposed to intentional exposure, which engages the central route.
Article
Full-text available
Does exposure to news affect what people know about politics? This old question attracted new scholarly interest as the political information environment is changing rapidly. In particular, since citizens have new channels at their disposal, such as Twitter and Facebook, which increasingly complement or even replace traditional channels of information. This study investigates to what extent citizens have knowledge about daily politics and to what extent news on social media can provide this knowledge. It does so by means of a large online survey in Belgium (Flanders), in which we measured what people know about current political events, their so-called general surveillance knowledge. Our findings demonstrate that unlike following news via traditional media channels, citizens do not gain more political knowledge from following news on social media. We even find a negative association between following the news on Facebook and political knowledge. We further investigate why this is the case. Our data demonstrate that this lack of learning on social media is not due to a narrow, personalized news diet, as is often suggested. Rather, we find evidence that following news via social media increases a feeling of information overload, which decreases what people actually learn, especially for citizens who combine news via social media with other news sources.
Article
Full-text available
A substantial body of scholarship has long explored the ways emerging media may foster and also hamper an informed and engaged citizenry. Individually, digital media have become an integral part of citizens’ political life as a growing number of people around the world use digital media technologies for information and communication. Collectively, digital media have also constituted an important platform that people use to coordinate among themselves and mobilize each other. Nevertheless, while distributing informative and mobilizing messages, digital media also facilitate socio-political factors that raise concern over the dissemination of misinformation, information divides and political polarization. This article showcases a broad variety of studies included in a special volume encapsulating some of these important issues.
Article
This study aimed at investigating whether Computational Thinking (CT) Perspectives can foster Computational Identity (CI) among primary school students in programming learning from the perspective of situated learning theory. This study conceptualized CT perspectives as a multidimensional construct that comprises (1) ability to connect, (2) ability to question, and (3) ability to express, and CI with four subcomponents including (1) programming affiliation, (2) programming engagement, (3) programming actualization, and (4) programming goal setting. Students who received at least 6-month programming training for computational thinking development were invited to participate in this study. Results of Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) suggested that the CT perspectives instrument (n = 100) and CI instrument (n = 241) reached good fit and therefore were validated in the pilot study. In the main study, survey answers (N = 215) including CT perspectives and CI were collected. Results of Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) showed positive relationships between CT perspectives and CI formation among primary school students. Mediation results further revealed that ability to question can foster CI formation through ability to express, while ability to connect can directly foster CI formation. Implications were discussed at the end of the study.
Article
The maldistribution of political knowledge in society has important consequences for individual-level political behavior and the representativeness of governmental policies. Increased media selectivity threatens to widen the gap between the politically well-informed and the less-informed by decreasing chance encounters with incidental political information. This study asks: Does exposure to incidental political information through social media promote political learning among users? We conduct two longitudinal, controlled experiments administered through the Facebook platform, and find no statistical difference in the levels of factual political knowledge among participants exposed to political information compared to those who were not. However, those in the treatment group with low political interest may be more likely to venture an incorrect guess than those in the control group, suggesting that exposure to incidental political information through social media may lead to an increase in self-perceived knowledge among some.