ArticlePDF Available

Neural responses to morally laden interactions in female inmates with psychopathy

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Much of social cognition requires making inferences about the mental and emotional states of others. Moreover, understanding the emotions of others is an important foundation for moral decision-making. Psychopathy is associated with both aberrant emotional understanding and atypical hemodynamic responses when viewing and evaluating morally laden social interactions. In the present functional MRI study, female inmates (N=107) were asked to evaluate the likely emotional state of either the recipient or the initiator of harmful or helpful interactions. Psychopathy was assessed with the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R). PCL-R scores were not associated with differences in confidence or accuracy ratings. However, psychopathy scores were significantly related to increased hemodynamic response in right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex when viewing harmful interactions and decreased functional connectivity from right amygdala to inferior parietal and insula, and from temporal parietal junction to dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. Overall, this work indicates that in females, psychopathy is associated with normal behavioral accuracy and confidence but alterations in neural network activity during moral decision-making.
Content may be subject to copyright.
NeuroImage: Clinical 30 (2021) 102645
Available online 27 March 2021
2213-1582/© 2021 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Neural responses to morally laden interactions in female inmates
with psychopathy
Keith J. Yoder
a
,
*
, Carla Harenski
b
,
c
, Kent A. Kiehl
b
,
c
, Jean Decety
a
,
d
a
Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
b
The Mind Research Network and Lovelace Biomedical, Albuquerque, NM, USA
c
Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA
d
Department of Psychiatry, and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
ARTICLE INFO
Keywords:
Emotion understanding
Decision-making
Empathy
Functional connectivity
Functional MRI
Forensic neuroscience - Moral evaluation
Female Psychopathy
ABSTRACT
Much of social cognition requires making inferences about the mental and emotional states of others. Moreover,
understanding the emotions of others is an important foundation for moral decision-making. Psychopathy is
associated with both aberrant emotional understanding and atypical hemodynamic responses when viewing and
evaluating morally laden social interactions. In the present functional MRI study, female inmates (N =107) were
asked to evaluate the likely emotional state of either the recipient or the initiator of harmful or helpful in-
teractions. Psychopathy was assessed with the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R). PCL-R scores were not
associated with differences in condence or accuracy ratings. However, psychopathy scores were signicantly
related to increased hemodynamic response in right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex when viewing harmful in-
teractions and decreased functional connectivity from right amygdala to inferior parietal cortex and insula, and
from temporal parietal junction to dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. Overall, this work indicates that in females,
psychopathy is associated with normal behavioral accuracy and condence but alterations in neural network
activity during moral decision-making.
1. Introduction
Humans, as social creatures, readily attribute emotional and cogni-
tive states to others when engaging in or observing third-party social
interactions. Emotion is an adaptive orienting system that evolved to
guide behavior. Emotion is also an interpersonal communication system
that elicits response from others. Thus, emotions can be viewed both as
intrapersonal and interpersonal states (Decety and Skelly, 2014).
Cognitive (e.g., attentional processes) and emotional processes are not
entirely separate entities. Rather, signals interact and are integrated at
both perceptual and executive levels (Pessoa, 2014).
Psychopathy is a personality disorder which includes a constellation
of traits, such as dishonesty, supercial charm, lack of empathy or guilt,
and impulsive behavior (Cleckly, 1941). Importantly, dysfunctional
socioemotional processing is a core feature of psychopathy (Hare, 2016;
Kiehl, 2015), and atypical emotional processing, such as decits in
empathy, is a risk factor for violent or criminal behavior (Anderson and
Kiehl, 2012; Blair, 2008; Olver and Wong, 2015; Seara-Cardoso and
Viding, 2014). Conicting behavioral and neuroscience investigations
have led to ongoing debates about the specicity of atypical empathic
processing, particularly whether decits are limited to distinct emotions
(e.g. fear and/or sadness) or are more general (Book et al., 2007; Dawel
et al., 2012; Deming et al., 2020; Glass and Newman, 2006; Hastings
et al., 2008; Marsh and Blair, 2008; Seara-Cardoso et al., 2012). While
both cognitive and emotional decits have been documented in in-
dividuals with high levels of psychopathic traits (Anderson et al., 2017),
most neuroscience studies report atypical neural responses during
perception and recognition of the emotions of others (Decety et al.,
2015, 2013b; Deming et al., 2020; Marsh et al., 2013; Sato et al., 2011).
Further, two recent meta-analyses indicate that across a variety of tasks,
psychopathy is marked by atypical neural responses in prefrontal, par-
alimbic, and insular regions (Deming and Koenigs, 2020; Poeppl et al.,
2019).
Socioemotional processing plays a critical role in moral reasoning
(Haidt and Graham, 2007; Van Bavel et al., 2015). Notably, harm
aversion is a crucial component of moral cognition (Decety and Cowell,
2018; Gray et al., 2012; Miller and Cushman, 2013). Studies reporting
moral insensitivity in individuals with psychopathy often interpret these
* Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology 5848 S. University Ave., Chicago, IL, 60637, USA.
E-mail address: kjyoder@uchicago.edu (K.J. Yoder).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
NeuroImage: Clinical
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynicl
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102645
Received 18 December 2020; Received in revised form 24 February 2021; Accepted 22 March 2021
NeuroImage: Clinical 30 (2021) 102645
2
effects as stemming from lack of empathy and callous disregard for
others (Blair, 2007; Cheng et al., 2012; Decety and Cowell, 2014a;
Harenski and Kiehl, 2011; Lockwood, 2016). Using the Moral Founda-
tions framework, which articulates distinct moral domains (Graham
et al., 2011), psychopathy was originally linked to specic reductions in
concern for the domains of harm and fairness (Aharoni et al., 2011;
Glenn et al., 2009a), though more recent work has identied reduced
concern across all domains (Jonason et al., 2015). Conversely, research
utilizing sacricial moral dilemmas has produced conicting results,
with some studies nding associations between psychopathic traits and
greater endorsement of utilitarian judgments among both un-
dergraduates and incarcerated populations (Bartels and Pizarro, 2011;
Koenigs et al., 2012), while others nd no differences in behavioral
responses (Cima et al., 2010; Glenn et al., 2009a; Tassy et al., 2013). A
recent meta-analysis concluded that psychopathy is weakly associated
with abnormal moral decision-making, rather than being characterized
by pronounced or overarching moral decits (Marshall et al., 2018). A
complementary motivational account suggests that psychopathy is
associated with relatively intact moral understanding but a lack of
motivation to apply this moral knowledge (Cima et al., 2010; Glenn
et al., 2009a; Tassy et al., 2013). Moreover, a growing body of neuro-
science evidence suggests that when individuals with psychopathy
provide moral evaluations that are indistinguishable from controls, as is
often the case, they do so by recruiting different brain circuits (Aharoni
et al., 2012; Yoder et al., 2015a).
However, the vast majority of studies examining neural functioning
in psychopathy have included only male participants. Psychopathy is a
well-documented risk factor for violent behavior and criminality in
males (Blais et al., 2014; Guy et al., 2010), and predicts violent recidi-
vism and future violence in prison (Camp et al., 2013; Olver and Wong,
2015). Emerging evidence from adolescents with conduct disorder
suggests that there are important sex differences in the impact of psy-
chopathic traits on brain structure and function (Michalska et al., 2015;
Smaragdi et al., 2017). The rates of female incarceration have risen over
the last decade (Carson and Anderson, 2016), indicating the pressing
importance of investigating psychopathic traits in female offenders and
determining whether the established links between psychopathic traits
and abnormal neural functioning observed in men also manifest in
women.
There is some debate about the specicity of empathic decits in
psychopathy. Empathy is a multifaceted construct which includes af-
fective, cognitive and motivational facets (Decety and Jackson, 2004;
Lockwood, 2016; Shamay-Tsoory, 2009), and each facet seems uniquely
related to moral cognition (Decety and Cowell, 2014b). Affective
empathy includes both the tendency to experience emotional distress in
response to the distress of others, as well as a motivation to respond
appropriately to another persons emotional state. Cognitive empathy
refers to an individuals propensity to adopt the perspective of another
person and imagine what another person is thinking or feeling (Shamay-
Tsoory, 2009). Cognitive empathy is closely related to theory of mind,
the ability to infer the beliefs and intentions of others as separate from
oneself (Decety and Jackson, 2004). Though some have argued that sex
differences in empathy are fundamental to psychological differences
(Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright, 2004), decades of research indicates
that sex differences in empathy are large when measured using self-
reports, but quickly diminish, or even become nonexistent, when
using behavioral measures and functional neuroimaging methods, such
as responding to otherspain (Baez et al., 2017; Eisenberg and Lennon,
1983; Michalska et al., 2013). Some evidence suggests that psychopathy
is associated with reduced accuracy when inferring the emotional states
of others (Brook et al., 2013; Brook and Kosson, 2013), though the
impact of psychopathic traits on emotion processing does not always
replicate in women (e.g., Vitale et al., 2011). Other work indicates high
levels of psychopathic traits are associated with decits in inferring af-
fective states (e.g. emotions) alongside an intact ability to infer cognitive
states (e.g. beliefs) of others (Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2010). Converging
evidence from functional neuroimaging and lesion studies indicate that
the temporoparietal junction (TPJ), which is situated at the posterior
superior temporal sulcus (pSTS), and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC)
are essential for detecting and representing mental states of others, and
play an important role in cognitive empathy and moral cognition
(Decety and Lamm, 2007; Gallagher and Frith, 2003; Lamm et al., 2007;
Moll et al., 2007; Saxe et al., 2004; Silani et al., 2013; Yoder and Decety,
2014a). In healthy participants, viewing others in pain reliably elicits
response in dorsal anterior cingulate (dACC) and anterior insula (aINS;
Decety et al., 2013a; Fallon et al., 2020; Lamm et al., 2011), core nodes
of the salience network which integrate multiple stimuli to coordinate
cortical and subcortical resources to respond to motivationally relevant
stimuli (Decety, 2011; Harsay et al., 2012; Shackman et al., 2011; Yoder
and Decety, 2018).
Moreover, these regions play critical roles in supporting moral
decision-making (Decety and Yoder, 2017; Krueger and Hoffman, 2016).
Previous work demonstrates that psychopathy, even when not associ-
ated with differences in socio-emotional judgments, is related to atypical
neural responses within these regions, especially pSTS/TPJ, amygdala,
and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), as well as the functional
connectivity with dACC and aINS (Harenski et al., 2010; Yoder et al.,
2015a, 2015b). Moreover, individuals with high levels of psychopathic
traits appear to not encode the pain of others as personally relevant,
though they can make use of this information if it becomes relevant to
the task at hand (Yoder et al., 2015a) or if they are imaging themselves
suffering (Decety et al., 2013a, 2013b). In fact, recent work suggests that
psychopaths do not spontaneously adopt the spatial perspective of
others, and that the magnitude of this dysfunction in altercentric
interference is correlated with real-world callous behaviors (Drayton
et al., 2018).
Given the complexity of the decits in socioemotional processing and
moral cognition in psychopathy, understanding the links between psy-
chopathic traits and moral cognition requires experimental manipula-
tions which focus on specic aspects of socioemotional processing in
ecologically meaningful contexts. In particular, there is good evidence
that just as individuals with high psychopathic traits knowmoral rules
but dont careabout them (Cima et al., 2010), such individuals do not
attend to socioemotional information in the same way as individuals
without psychopathy. Psychopathy is associated with reductions in
neural activity and functional connectivity within the salience network
when viewing visual depictions of morally laden scenarios, especially
when the moral content of the scenarios is relevant to the task (Yoder
et al., 2015a). Psychopathy has also been linked to reduced anatomical
connectivity of the uncinate fasciculus (Motzkin et al., 2011; Wolf et al.,
2015), which links the anterior temporal lobe, including amygdala and
aINS, to inferior frontal cortex, including vmPFC.
The rst, and to our knowledge, the only fMRI study of psychopathy
in female inmates found negative associations between PCL-R scores and
hemodynamic responses in right amygdala and rostral ACC during
emotional processing, and in the right TPJ specically during the pro-
cessing of moral scenarios (Harenski et al., 2014). Recent neuroimaging
work in non-incarcerated women identied associations between PCL-R
scores and connectivity, both anatomical white matter integrity (Lind-
ner et al., 2017) and functional connectome dened from resting-state
data (Lindner et al., 2018). These effects were more particularly pro-
nounced for Factor 2 scores, which reect the affective/interpersonal
dimension of psychopathy, suggesting that this dimension of psychop-
athy may be most important for understanding the impact of psycho-
pathic traits on neural functioning in female inmates.
The current study was designed to examine socioemotional pro-
cessing in response to third-party morally laden interactions in female
offenders. The emotional expressions of the protagonists were situated
in the context of dyadic interactions that were either intentionally
harmful or intentionally helpful. Harm and help represent prototypical
morally bad and morally good actions and provide a useful platform for
examining low-level sociomoral cognition (Yoder et al., 2015a). Based
K.J. Yoder et al.
NeuroImage: Clinical 30 (2021) 102645
3
on previous work, it was expected that inmates with higher levels of
psychopathy traits would be less likely to nd harmful outcomes as
salient, leading to reduced hemodynamic response in core nodes of the
salience network, particularly dACC, aINS, and amygdala. Moreover,
psychopathy was expected to be associated with little to no difference in
behavioral responses coincident with a shift towards reliance on pre-
frontal executive control systems, leading to increased response in
dlPFC. This shift was also expected to manifest as reductions in func-
tional connectivity seeded in amygdala and TPJ to other nodes of the
salience network and social cognition networks, such as insula, TPJ, and
dmPFC.
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Participants
115 women in a medium-maximum security state prison completed
all aspects of the study protocol. Eight participants showed excessive
movement in the MRI data (rotation >3 degrees or translation >3 mm)
and were excluded from analysis. Thus, the nal sample consisted of 107
women (M
age
=35.0, SD =8.2, range =20 53). Inclusion criteria were
age 1859 years, female sex (not transitioning), no uncorrectable
auditory or visual decits, ability to speak and understand English,
reading level of at least 5th grade, not currently pregnant, no central
nervous system disease, no current major medical conditions, no hy-
pertension with complications, no lifetime history of psychotic disorder,
no self-reported psychotic disorder (with psychiatric hospitalization) in
a rst degree relative, no traumatic brain injury with loss of con-
sciousness >10 min, no drug use in last three months (self-report or
institution records), and no MRI contraindications such as metal in the
body. One participant was ambidextrous, while the remaining 106 were
right-handed. 65 (61%) of the women had been convicted of at least one
violent crime. The women were compensated for study participation at a
rate proportional to the institutional wages for work assignments at their
correctional facility, and provided written informed consent. All pro-
cedures and materials were approved by the Institutional Review Boards
at the University of Chicago and Ethical and Independent Review
Services.
Psychopathy was assessed using the Hare Psychopathy Checklist-
Revised (Hare, 2003) which was administered by trained research as-
sistants. The PCL-R includes four correlated facets, which can be
grouped into two higher order factors (Hare, 2016). Factor 1 captures
interpersonal and affective dimensions, while Factor 2 captures devel-
opmental, lifestyle, and antisocial aspects of psychopathy. Intelligence
Quotient (IQ) was assessed using the vocabulary and matrix reasoning
subtests from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd Edition or the
Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence 2nd Edition. PCL-R scores
showed small but signicant negative relationships with age (Spear-
mans rho =-0.19, p =0.049) and IQ (Spearmans rho =-0.20, p =
0.042). PCL-R scores were not signicantly related to conviction for at
least one violent crime (Odds Ratio =1.17, 95% CI [0.79, 1.73], p =
0.435).
2.2. Task stimuli
Participants completed a task previously used in a population of
incarcerated males (Decety et al., 2015). In each trial, participants were
shown a dyadic interactions depicting either intentional interpersonal
harm or intentional assistance. Depictions consisted of three static im-
ages presented to create apparent motion (image durations of 1.0, 0.2,
and 1.0 s). Following each scenario, participants were shown a cutout of
either the recipient of the behavior or the individual who initiated the
behavior. After a jittered interval (M =3 s, SD =1.2 s), a 2s video clip
appeared next to the cut-out and showed a person making one of six
expressions: happy, sad, frightened, angry, disgust, or in pain. Impor-
tantly, the interaction and cutout did not show a face, so it was possible
to counter-balance actors and expressions with recipients or agents (see
Fig. 1). After the video ended, the nal frame remained on the screen
next to the cutout, and participants were asked Do you think the person
felt this way?Participants indicated their response by pressing a key to
stop a red bar which began on the left (No, not at all) and moved to the
right (Yes, denitely). Trials were separated by a jittered interval (M
=3, SD =1.1 s). Stimuli were presented using the E-Prime 2.0 stimuli
presentation suite (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA, USA).
2.3. MRI acquisitions and analysis
Function images were acquired using the Mind Research Network
1.5 Tesla Siemens Magnetom Avanto Mobile unit (Washington, DC,
USA) which was equipped with a 32-element head coil. Echo-planar
images were acquired using a multiband sequence (posterior-to-ante-
rior phase encoding, multiband factor =12, repetition time/echo time
=350 ms / 39 ms, ip angle =37 degrees, eld of view =248 ×248
mm, matrix =70 ×70, voxel size =3.5 ×3.5 ×3.5 mm
3
). These images
were then realigned and motion-corrected using INRIAign (Freire et al.,
2002). Rather than performing slice-timing correction at this step, the
temporal derivative of each event was included (see below). EPI images
were normalized to the EPI MNI template (Calhoun et al., 2017) before
smoothing with an 8 mm Gaussian kernel. Images were preprocessed
and analyzed using SPM12 (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuro-
science, London, UK) in MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA).
A general linear modeling (GLM) framework was used, where a ca-
nonical hemodynamic response function was convolved with a boxcar
function representing the onsets and durations of the events of interest.
Specically, the onsets of each scenario through the end of the third
picture, and the onset of each decision phase, beginning at the onset of
the actor cutout through the response. This created six trial regressors:
HarmScene, HelpScene, IdentifyAgentHarm, IdentifyRecipientHarm,
IdentifyAgentHelp, IdentifyRecipientHelp. Temporal derivatives were
also modeled for each event. The beta image pairs for each modeled
event amplitude and temporal derivative were combined into a single
magnitude image which was then passed to the second-level analysis
(Calhoun et al., 2004). Six movement parameters were entered as
nuisance regressors.
Second-level contrasts were derived by combining rst-level contrast
estimates. Psychopathy scores were modeled using either total PCL-R, or
Factor 1 and Factor 2. For each, mean-centered age in months and IQ
were entered as covariates of no interest. For group-based analysis,
participants with PCL-R scores of 30 or above (n =24) were categorized
as high psychopathy, consistent with the diagnosticcutoff proposed by
Hare (2003), and scores of 20 or below (n =45) were categorized as low.
Functional connectivity was assessed by modeling a psychophysiologi-
cal interaction between the task contrasts and mean signal extracted
from an anatomically dened right amygdala mask and a 10 mm radius
sphere placed in rTPJ (MNI × = 52, y =-54, z =16) based on previous
work investigating rTPJ connectivity during socioemotional processing
(Yoder et al., 2015a; Yoder and Decety, 2014a). Images were thresh-
olded to achieve family-wise error corrected p <0.05, determined using
the rst-level residual images to estimate smoothness for 3dClustSim
(Cox, 1996).
2.4. Behavioral data analysis
Two separate measures were extracted from the behavioral re-
sponses (Decety et al., 2015). First, the midpoint was subtracted from
the responses and the absolute value was taken to create a condence
measure, ranging from 0, the midpoint, to 3, the extreme end of the
scale. Accuracy was evaluated by limiting analyses to those trials where
there was a natural congruence between an individuals emotion and the
situation: happy expressions for either individuals following a helpful
interaction, angry expressions for the perpetrators of harmful in-
teractions, and pain or sad expressions for victims of harmful
K.J. Yoder et al.
NeuroImage: Clinical 30 (2021) 102645
4
interactions.
Behavioral data were analyzed using complementary approaches in
R (version 4.0.2, R Core Team, 2015). First, repeated-measures analysis
of variance (ANOVA) modeled condence and accuracy in a 2 (Morality:
Harm|Help) ×2 (Actor: Agent|Recipient) basic model using the ‘afex
package (Singmann et al., 2020) with pairwise comparisons interro-
gated with the ‘emmeans package (Lenth, 2020). Once t to the full
model, another model specically examined high and low psychopathy
groups. A complementary multilevel linear modeling (MLM) approach,
as implemented in the ‘lme4package (Bates et al., 2015), regressed
behavioral responses on Morality and Actor, with participant modeled
using a random intercept. Psychopathy was modeled continuously using
PCL-R scores.
3. Results
The ANOVA for condence identied a marginal main effect of Actor
(F(1,106, F =3.92,
η
2G
=0.003, p =0.050) and a signicant Moral *
Actor interaction (F(1,106), F =15.89,
η
2G
=0.013, p <0.001; Fig. 1B).
Tukeys comparisons revealed higher condence for recipients than
agents in harmful interactions (p =0.002) and higher condence for
helpful agents than harmful agents (p <0.001). These effects remained
the same when limiting the analysis to only the high and low psychop-
athy groups. No Group effects were signicant (all p >0.3). Modeling
psychopathy scores continuously also did not identify any signicant
effects (all p >0.2), and including psychopathy scores did not signi-
cantly improve model t over a model with just Moral and Actor terms
(Х
2
(4) =6.17, p =0.187).
Accuracy for the subset of trials with clear emotional mapping
revealed a signicant main effect of Moral (F(1, 106) =190.53,
η
2G
=
0.278, p <0.001) and a Moral * Actor interaction (F(1, 106) =9.75,
η
2G
=0.023, p =0.002; Fig. 1C). All pairwise comparisons were signicant
(largest p =0.035 for recipients of harm compared to agents of harm).
Accuracy was highest for recipients of help, then agents of help, agents
and harm, and recipients of harm had the lowest accuracy. As with
condence, restricting the analysis to only high and low psychopathy
individuals produced similar results, though the pairwise difference
between agents and recipients of harm became non-signicant (p =
0.192). No Group effects were signicant (all p >0.3). Modeling psy-
chopathy continuously in an MLM framework produced similar effects,
with psychopathy scores producing no signicant behavioral effects (all
p >0.6) and psychopathy score not improving explanatory power of the
model (Х
2
(4) =0.69, p =0.953).
Viewing harmful social interactions compared to helpful social in-
teractions elicited increased hemodynamic response throughout visual
cortex, much of the social decision-making network, including pSTS/
TPJ, and the core nodes of the salience network i.e. dACC, aINS
(Fig. 2A, Table S1). In contrast, helpful actions were associated with
greater signal in bilateral caudate, vmPFC, and dlPFC. PCL-R scores
were associated with greater responses in right dlPFC when viewing
harmful compared to helpful interactions. Factor 1 and Factor 2 scores
were not uniquely associated with any signicant clusters.
During the emotion identication phase, decisions about harmful
interactions, compared to helpful interactions, elicited greater response
in lateral occipital cortex, fusiform gyrus, and bilateral inferior frontal
gyrus (IFG) extending into aINS and left inferior parietal (Fig. 2B;
Table S1). Decisions about helpful interactions were associated with
greater response in primary visual cortex, left precentral gyrus, right
aINS, SMA, and right caudate body. Combined with the viewing phase,
these main effects replicate previous whole-brain results using the same
Fig. 1. Task schematic and behavioral responses. A) Sample trial of harmful interaction with pain expression for recipient (top) and angry expression for agent
(bottom). Below are shown plots for the Actor * Moral interaction for condence across all responses (B) and accuracy for trials with clear emotion mappings (C).
K.J. Yoder et al.
NeuroImage: Clinical 30 (2021) 102645
5
task in a sample of incarcerated males (Decety et al., 2015), with the
exception of rTPJ, in which activation was not observed during the
evaluation phase in this study. PCL-R scores were associated with
reduced response in right STS when identifying the emotions of in-
dividuals involved in harmful compared to helpful interactions. Factor 1
scores were specically associated with reduced signal in right STS. No
clusters showed any signicant associations with Factor 2 scores.
When evaluating the emotions of protagonists who initiated harmful
compared to helpful interactions, greater neuro-hemodynamic response
was observed in bilateral IFG and left aINS, dlPFC, and TPJ (Fig. 3A;
Table S2). Identifying the emotional state of actors in helpful scenarios
elicited greater signal in cuneus, SMA, precentral gyrus and caudate.
When identifying the emotions of recipients of helpful compared to
harmful interactions, increased signal was observed in cuneus, SMA,
caudate body, and bilateral dlPFC (Fig. 3B). No regions showed greater
response when identifying recipients of harm compared to help. PCL-R
scores showed no signicant associations when identifying the emo-
tions of agents, but were associated with reduced signal in bilateral pSTS
when identifying the emotional state of the recipients of harm compared
to help (Fig. 3B). Factor 1 scores were not signicantly associated with
either contrast, but Factor 2 scores were signicantly associated with
reduced response in right pSTS, caudate, and ACC (Table S2).
When viewing harmful compared to helpful interactions, right
amygdala demonstrated increased functional connectivity with left aINS
(Fig. 4; Table S3). The rTPJ seed showed increased neuronal coupling
with left medial temporal areas, caudate and right medial and superior
frontal cortex (Fig. 4; Table S4). Right TPJ showed increased connec-
tivity with an overlapping cluster in left parietal, as well as increased
connectivity with precuneus and right parietal. When viewing harmful
compared to helpful interactions, PCL-R scores were associated with
reduced functional connectivity from amygdala to left parietal and right
temporal cortex, and with reduced connectivity from TPJ to left parietal
and parahippocampal gyrus (Fig. 4A).
While identifying emotions for harmful compared to helpful in-
teractions (Fig. 4B), right amygdala demonstrated increased connec-
tivity with a cluster in left parietal cortex extending to TPJ and into
anterior insula. Right TPJ also showed decreased connectivity with right
fusiform gyrus and dACC. PCL-R scores were associated with decreased
amygdala connectivity with precuneus and bilateral pSTS, extending
into left insula. Connectivity between rTPJ and dACC/SMA was also
negatively related to PCL-R scores.
Identifying the emotions of agents who initiated actions (Fig. 5A;
Table S6) elicited reduced connectivity from rTPJ to vmPFC and
increased connectivity with caudate and bilateral postcentral gyri.
Specically focusing on identifying the emotional state of the agent of
the interaction, PCL-R scores were negatively related to connectivity
between right amygdala and left aINS, TPJ, IFG, precuneus (Table S5).
PCL-R scores were not related to functional connectivity seeded in rTPJ
during identication of agent emotions. Factor 2 was specically asso-
ciated with decreased functional connectivity between amygdala and
left pSTS and bilateral aINS.
For recipients (Fig. 5B), TPJ showed greater connectivity with
dACC/SMA. No regions showed signicant functional connectivity in-
creases or decreases with right amygdala when examining harmful
compared to helpful interaction specically for the actor or specically
for the recipient. PCL-R scores were signicantly related to reduced
connectivity between right amygdala and left temporal regions,
including pSTS, and reduced connectivity between right TPJ and SMA.
Factor 2 scores were associated with decreased connectivity between
both amygdala and TPJ with SMA, and between amygdala and right
aINS, inferior frontal gyrus, and inferior parietal cortex.
Fig. 2. Whole-brain results for morally laden content during task phases. Regions more sensitive to harmful (red) or helpful (blue) interactions during the viewing
phase (A) or emotion identication phase (B). Also shown are regions identied by the Harm-Help contrast as having a positive association (green) or negative
association (violet) with PCL-R scores. All regions signicant at FWEp <0.05 (height =0.005, extent =100). (For interpretation of the references to colour in this
gure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)
K.J. Yoder et al.
NeuroImage: Clinical 30 (2021) 102645
6
Fig. 3. Whole-brain results for identifying emotional stats of different individuals. Regions more sensitive to harmful (red) or helpful (blue) interactions when
evaluating the emotional state of agents who initiated actions (A) or recipients of actions (B). Also shown are regions identied by the Harm-Help contrast as having a
positive association (green) or negative association (violet) with PCL-R scores. All regions signicant at FWEp <0.05 (height =0.005, extent =100). (For inter-
pretation of the references to colour in this gure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)
Fig. 4. Whole-brain functional connectivity re-
sults for task phases. Regions show increased
(red) or decreased (blue) connectivity with right
amygdala (left) or TPJ (right) during the view
phase (A) or emotion identication phase (B).
Regions where connectivity was negatively asso-
ciated with PCL-R scores are shown in green. All
regions signicant at FWEp <0.05 (height =
0.005, extent =100). (For interpretation of the
references to colour in this gure legend, the
reader is referred to the web version of this
article.)
K.J. Yoder et al.
NeuroImage: Clinical 30 (2021) 102645
7
4. Discussion
The goal of this study was to examine the impact of psychopathic
traits on behavioral and neuro-hemodynamic measures of socioemo-
tional understanding of morally laden interactions in adult female of-
fenders. Overall, and as predicted, psychopathy, whether viewed as a
dichotomous variable or a continuous trait, was not related to differ-
ences in behavioral responses. Condence ratings and behavioral ac-
curacy were not signicantly related to PCL-R scores, and the high and
low psychopathy groups did not signicantly differ on either measure.
Contrary to predictions, psychopathy was not signicantly related to
reduced responses in amygdala or dACC during either third-party
evaluation or identication phases. While women overall did demon-
strate greater dACC response when viewing harmful compared to
helpful interactions, no amygdala response was detected in either phase.
However, psychopathy scores were signicantly related to increased
hemodynamic response in rdlPFC when viewing harmful scenarios and
widespread decreases in functional connectivity seeded in right amyg-
dala and right TPJ. Overall, these results suggest that the nodes of the
salience and social cognition networks respond similarly in female
psychopaths, but the networks are largely disconnected in comparison
to inmates with low levels of psychopathic traits.
The whole-brain results for viewing harmful compared to helpful
interactions is consistent with a large body of work in moral neurosci-
ence and third-party evaluations (Eres et al., 2018; Krueger and Hoff-
man, 2016), with regions important for theory of mind and saliency,
especially bilateral TPJ, insula, and dACC/SMA showing greater
response to intentional harm (Decety and Yoder, 2017; Yoder and
Decety, 2014a). In contrast, core regions of the reward circuitry, namely
vmPFC and caudate, as well as dlPFC demonstrated greater response
when observing helpful interactions (Decety and Porges, 2011).
Right dlPFC response directly replicates previous moral judgment
work using similar stimuli in undergraduates and inmates (Yoder et al.,
2015a; Yoder and Decety, 2014a, 2014b). The dlPFC was the only region
showing signicant associations with PCL-R total score (Fig. 2A). This
result ts with the notion that individuals with high levels of psycho-
pathic traits rely on prefrontal recruitment in order to maintain similar
behavior responses (Glenn et al., 2009b; Yoder et al., 2015a).
When identifying emotions of individuals involved in harmful
compared to helpful interactions, psychopathic traits were associated
with reduced response in a region of right STS extending into deep
pSTS/TPJ (Fig. 2B). Response in pSTS is reliably implicated when
inferring mental states of others in pain (Lamm et al., 2011), but pSTS
also plays important integrative roles for incorporating mental state
information into social decision-making contexts (Carter and Huettel,
2013; Yoder and Decety, 2018). Reduced response in this region was
specically associated with Factor 1, but not Factor 2. Thus, reduced TPJ
response here suggests that women with higher levels of psychopathic
traits, particularly the interpersonal-affective dimension of callousness,
may rely less on others mental states when attempting to label
emotional expressions of others. Future work could clarify this rela-
tionship by examining emotional accuracy while varying the amount of
mental state information that is available and testing whether Factor
scores are associated with specic decreases in performance.
When identifying the emotion of the recipient of an interaction
(Harm Help), PCL-R scores were associated with reduced response in
bilateral pSTS (Fig. 3B). Interestingly, whereas pSTS response when
identifying emotions generally was associated with Factor 1 scores,
Factor 2 scores were associated with reduced pSTS response specically
for identifying the emotions of recipients (Table S2). This dissociation
between Factor 1 and 2 suggests that the Interpersonal/Affective and
Developmental/Lifestyle/Antisocial dimensions of psychopathy may
differentially impact use of mental state information when evaluating
the emotional states of others in general or specically of victims. The
lack of an association between psychopathic traits and amygdala or
dACC response is surprisingly. However, this result could be a
Fig. 5. Whole-brain functional connectivity results for different actors. Regions show increased (red) or decreased (blue) connectivity with right amygdala (left) or
TPJ (right) when identifying agents who initiated actions (A) or recipients (B). Regions where connectivity was negatively associated with PCL-R scores are shown in
green. At right are shown regions where Factor 2 scores were negatively associated with functional connectivity seeded in amygdala (violet) or TPJ (yellow). All
regions signicant at FWEp <0.05 (height =0.005, extent =100). (For interpretation of the references to colour in this gure legend, the reader is referred to the
web version of this article.)
K.J. Yoder et al.
NeuroImage: Clinical 30 (2021) 102645
8
consequence of directly comparing harmful and helpful interactions,
rather than including a neutral condition. Further work is required to
directly test this possibility.
The impact of psychopathic traits was more robust when examining
functional connectivity. When viewing morally laden scenarios (Harm
Help), PCL-R scores were associated with reduced functional connec-
tivity to left inferior parietal cortex for both the amygdala and TPJ seeds
(Fig. 4). Moreover, the amygdala seed revealed psychopathy-linked
reduced connectivity between right amygdala and right TPJ. These al-
terations in functional connectivity are consistent with recent reports of
disrupted functional networks association with higher psychopathic
traits in incarcerated males (Espinoza et al., 2018; Tillem et al., 2019).
Thus, psychopathy appears to alter connectivity within the social
cognition network when inmates viewed morally laden images.
During the emotion identication phase, PCL-R scores predicted
reduced connectivity between right amygdala and left TPJ. This effect
remained regardless of whether the trial was focused on the agent or the
recipient, suggesting that for individuals with high levels of psycho-
pathic traits the usually aversive salience of interpersonal harm is
disconnected from mental state representations (Buckholtz and Marois,
2012). For agents, PCL-R was also related to reduced connectivity to
aINS. This ts with previous work demonstrating reduced aINS response
during face processing among adolescent females with conduct disorder
(Fairchild et al., 2014). This is particularly important given the role of
the insula in salience processing and signaling motivationally relevant
information (Harsay et al., 2012; Krueger and Hoffman, 2016). The
negative relationship between PCL-R and amygdala-insula coupling
suggests that individuals with high levels of psychopathic traits dont
encode violent others as personally salient, potentially because they
dont view others behaving antisocially as unexpected. This would be
consistent with studies linking increased psychopathic traits in the
general population to reduced amygdala connectivity (e.g., Dotterer
et al., 2020; Waller et al., 2019; Yoder et al., 2015b). However, future
studies could directly test this effect using violation of expectation
paradigms.
Interestingly, while Factor 1 scores were not signicantly associated
with any changes in functional connectivity during the recipient iden-
tication phase, higher Factor 2 scores were associated with reductions
in connectivity, specically to dACC/SMA. Some previous work with
undergraduate and incarcerated males found that callous-unemotional
traits were specically linked to reduced connectivity with dACC and
right amygdala (Yoder et al., 2015b, 2015a). Moreover, large-scale in-
vestigations of functional connectivity in incarcerated males has linked
Factor 1, rather than Factor 2, to altered network connectivity, partic-
ularly with the salience network (Espinoza et al., 2018; Thijssen and
Kiehl, 2017). Thus, the somewhat surprising link with Factor 2 in the
current study suggests that the dimensions of psychopathy may impact
different neural systems in women and men.
5. Conclusion
Overall, the results of our study replicate previous work demon-
strating links between higher levels of psychopathic traits and wide-
spread decreases in functional connectivity seeded in amygdala and TPJ
during socioemotional processing and decision-making. Importantly,
the current study extends these ndings to incarcerated females with
psychopathic traits, a population that is severely understudied. Much
previous work has highlighted specic links between psychopathy and
reduced hemodynamic response and connectivity within neural net-
works anchored by amygdala, dACC, and aINS. However, these results
provide important preliminary evidence that the antisocial dimension of
psychopathy is more important than the affective/interpersonal
dimension for explaining this effect in women.
CRediT authorship contribution statement
Keith J. Yoder: Methodology, Software, Writing - original draft,
Writing - review & editing, Visualization. Carla Harenski: Data cura-
tion, Validation, Writing - original draft. Kent A. Kiehl: Data curation,
Project administration, Writing - original draft. Jean Decety: Concep-
tualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Writing - review &
editing, Funding acquisition.
Declaration of Competing Interest
The authors declare that they have no known competing nancial
interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to inuence
the work reported in this paper.
Acknowledgments
The work was supported by the National Institutes of Health [grant
number R01MH109329].
Appendix A. Supplementary data
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102645.
References
Aharoni, E., Antonenko, O., Kiehl, K.A., 2011. Disparities in the moral intuitions of
criminal offenders: The role of psychopathy. J. Res. Pers. 45, 322327. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.jrp.2011.02.005.
Aharoni, E., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., Kiehl, K.A., 2012. Can psychopathic offenders
discern moral wrongs? A new look at the moral/conventional distinction. J. Abnorm.
Psychol. 121, 484497. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024796.
Anderson, N.E., Kiehl, K.A., 2012. The psychopath magnetized: Insights from brain
imaging. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 5260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.008.
Anderson, N.E., Steele, V.R., Maurer, J.M., Rao, V., Koenigs, M.R., Decety, J., Kosson, D.,
Calhoun, V., Kiehl, K.A., 2017. Differentiating emotional processing and attention in
psychopathy with functional neuroimaging. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 17,
491515. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-016-0493-5.Differentiating.
Baez, S., Flichtentrei, D., Prats, M., Mastandueno, R., García, A.M., Cetkovich, M.,
Ib´
a˜
nez, A., 2017. Men, women..who cares? A population-based study on sex
differences and gender roles in empathy and moral cognition. PLoS One 12, 121.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0179336.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., 2004. The Empathy Quotient: An investigation of
adults with Asperger Syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex
differences. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 163175.
Bartels, D.M., Pizarro, D.A., 2011. The mismeasure of morals: antisocial personality traits
predict utilitarian responses to moral dilemmas. Cognition 121, 154161. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.05.010.
Bates, D., M¨
achler, M., Bolker, B., Walker, S., 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models
using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 51. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01.
Blair, R.J.R., 2008. The amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex: functional
contributions and dysfunction in psychopathy. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol.
Sci. 363, 25572565. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0027.
Blair, R.J.R., 2007. The amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex in morality and
psychopathy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 387392. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
tics.2007.07.003.
Blais, J., Solodukhin, E., Forth, A.E., 2014. A meta-analysis exploring the relationship
between psychopathy and instrumental versus reactive violence. Crim. Justice
Behav. 41, 797821. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854813519629.
Book, A.S., Quinsey, V.L., Langford, D.J., 2007. Psychopathy and the perception of affect
and vulnerability. Crim. Justice Behav. 34, 531544. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0093854806293554.
Brook, M., Brieman, C.L., Kosson, D.S. 2013. Emotion processing in Psychopathy
Checklist - assessed psychopathy: A review of the literature. Clin. Psychol. Rev.
10.1016/j.cpr.2013.07.008.
Brook, M., Kosson, D.S., 2013. Impaired cognitive empathy in criminal psychopathy:
Evidence from a laboratory measure of empathic accuracy. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 122,
156166. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030261.
Buckholtz, J.W., Marois, R., 2012. The roots of modern justice: cognitive and neural
foundations of social norms and their enforcement. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 655661.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3087.
Calhoun, V.D., Stevens, M.C., Pearlson, G.D., Kiehl, K.A., 2004. fMRI analysis with the
general linear model: Removal of latency-induced amplitude bias by incorporation
of hemodynamic derivative terms. Neuroimage 22, 252257. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.029.
Calhoun, V.D., Wager, T.D., Krishnan, A., Rosch, K.S., Seymour, K.E., Nebel, M.B.,
Mostofsky, S.H., Nyalakanai, P., Kiehl, K., 2017. The impact of T1 versus EPI spatial
K.J. Yoder et al.
NeuroImage: Clinical 30 (2021) 102645
9
normalization templates for fMRI data analyses. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 53315342.
https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23737.
Camp, J.P., Skeem, J.L., Barchard, K., Lilienfeld, S.O., Poythress, N.G., 2013.
Psychopathic predators? Getting specic about the relation between psychopathy
and violence. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 81, 467480. https://doi.org/10.1037/
a0031349.
Carson, E.A., Anderson, E. 2016. Prisoners in 2015 (NCJ 250229). Washington, DC.
Carter, R.M., Huettel, S.A., 2013. A nexus model of the temporal-parietal junction.
Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 328336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.05.007.
Cheng, Y., Hung, A.-Y., Decety, J., 2012. Dissociation between affective sharing and
emotion understanding in juvenile psychopaths. Dev. Psychopathol. 24, 623636.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941200020X.
Cima, M., Tonnaer, F., Hauser, M.D., 2010. Psychopaths know right from wrong but
dont care. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 5, 5967. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/
nsp051.
Cleckly, H., 1941. The Mask of Sanity: An Attempt to Reinterpret the So-Called
Psychopathic Personality. Mosby Company, St. Louis.
Cox, R.W., 1996. AFNI: Software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic
resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 29, 162173. https://doi.org/
10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014.
Dawel, A., OKearney, R., McKone, E., Palermo, R., 2012. Not just fear and sadness:
Meta-analytic evidence of pervasive emotion recognition decits for facial and vocal
expressions in psychopathy. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 22882304. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.006.
Decety, J., 2011. Dissecting the neural mechanisms mediating empathy. Emot. Rev. 3,
92108. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073910374662.
Decety, J., Chen, C., Harenski, C., Kiehl, K.A., 2013a. An fMRI study of affective
perspective taking in individuals with psychopathy: Imagining another in pain does
not evoke empathy. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 489. https://doi.org/10.3389/
fnhum.2013.00489.
Decety, J., Chen, C., Harenski, C.L., Kiehl, K.A., 2015. Socioemotional processing of
morally-laden behavior and their consequences on others in forensic psychopaths.
Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 20152026. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22752.
Decety, J., Cowell, J.M., 2018. Interpersonal harm aversion as a necessary foundation for
morality: A developmental neuroscience perspective. Dev. Psychopathol. 30,
153164. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579417000530.
Decety, J., Cowell, J.M., 2014a. The complex relation between morality and empathy.
Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 337339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.008.
Decety, J., Cowell, J.M., 2014b. Friends or foes : Is empathy necessary for moral
behaviour? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 9, 525537. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1745691614545130.
Decety, J., Jackson, P.L., 2004. The functional architecture of human empathy. Behav.
Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 3, 71100. https://doi.org/10.1177/1534582304267187.
Decety, J., Lamm, C., 2007. The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social
interaction: How low-level computational processes contribute to meta-cognition.
Neurosci. 13, 580593. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858407304654.
Decety, J., Porges, E.C., 2011. Imagining being the agent of actions that carry different
moral consequences: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 49, 29943001. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.06.024.
Decety, J., Skelly, L.R., 2014. The neural underpinnings of the experience of empathy:
Lessons for psychopathy, in: Ochsner, K.N., Kosslyn, S.M. (Eds.), The Oxford
Handbook of Cognitive Neuroscience - Volume 2. Oxford University Press, New
York, NY, pp. 228243.
Decety, J., Skelly, L.R., Kiehl, K.A., 2013b. Brain response to empathy-eliciting scenarios
involving pain in incarcerated individuals with psychopathy. JAMA Psychiatry 70,
638645. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.27.
Decety, J., Yoder, K.J., 2017. The emerging social neuroscience of justice motivation.
Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 614. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.10.008.
Deming, P., Dargis, M., Haas, B.W., Brook, M., Decety, J., Harenski, C., Kiehl, K.A.,
Koenigs, M., Kosson, D.S., 2020. Psychopathy is associated with fear-specic
reductions in neural activity during affective perspective-taking. Neuroimage 223.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117342.
Deming, P., Koenigs, M., 2020. Functional neural correlates of psychopathy: A meta-
analysis of MRI data. Transl. Psychiatry 10. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-
0816-8.
Dotterer, H.L., Waller, R., Hein, T.C., Pardon, A., Mitchell, C., Lopez-Duran, N., Monk, C.
S., Hyde, L.W., 2020. Clarifying the link between amygdala functioning during
emotion processing and antisocial behaviors versus callous-unemotional traits
within a population-based community sample. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 8, 918935.
https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702620922829.
Drayton, L.A., Santos, L.R., Baskin-Sommers, A., 2018. Psychopaths fail to automatically
take the perspective of others. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 115, 33023307.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1721903115.
Eisenberg, N., Lennon, R., 1983. Sex differences in empathy and related capacities.
Psychol. Bull. 94, 100131. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.94.1.100.
Eres, R., Louis, W.R., Molenberghs, P., 2018. Common and distinct neural networks
involved in fMRI studies investigating morality: An ALE meta-analysis. Soc.
Neurosci. 13, 384398. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2017.1357657.
Espinoza, F.A., Vergara, V.M., Reyes, D., Anderson, N.E., Harenski, C.L., Decety, J.,
Rachakonda, S., Damaraju, E., Rashid, B., Miller, R.L., Koenigs, M., Kosson, D.S.,
Harenski, K., Kiehl, K.A., Calhoun, V.D., 2018. Aberrant functional network
connectivity in psychopathy from a large (N =985) forensic sample. Hum. Brain
Mapp. 39, 26242634. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24028.
Fairchild, G., Hagan, C.C., Passamonti, L., Walsh, N.D., Goodyer, I.M., Calder, A.J., 2014.
Atypical neural responses during face processing in female adolescents with conduct
disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 53, 677687.e5. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.jaac.2014.02.009.
Fallon, N., Roberts, C., Stancak, A., 2020. Shared and distinct functional networks for
empathy and pain processing: A systematic review and meta-analysis of fMRI
studies. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 15, 709723. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/
nsaa090.
Freire, L., Roche, A., Mangin, J.F., 2002. What is the best similarity measure for motion
correction in fMRI time series? IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 21, 470484. https://doi.
org/10.1109/TMI.2002.1009383.
Gallagher, H.L., Frith, C.D., 2003. Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind. Trends Cogn.
Sci. 7, 7783. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00025-6.
Glass, S.J., Newman, J.P., 2006. Recognition of facial affect in psychopathic offenders.
J. Abnorm. Psychol. 115, 815820. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.115.4.815.
Glenn, A.L., Iyer, R., Graham, J., Koleva, S., Haidt, J., 2009a. Are all types of morality
compromised in psychopathy? J. Pers. Disord. 23, 384398. https://doi.org/
10.1521/pedi.2009.23.4.384.
Glenn, A.L., Raine, A., Schug, R.A., Young, L., Hauser, M., 2009b. Increased DLPFC
activity during moral decision-making in psychopathy. Mol. Psychiatry 14, 909911.
https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2009.76.
Graham, J., Nosek, B.A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., Ditto, P.H., 2011. Mapping the
moral domain. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 366385. https://doi.org/10.1037/
a0021847.
Gray, K., Young, L., Waytz, A., 2012. Mind perception is the essence of morality. Psychol.
Inq. 23, 101124. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2012.651387.
Guy, L.S., Douglas, K.S., Hendry, M.C., 2010. The role of psychopathic personality
disorder in violence risk assessments using the HCR-20. J. Pers. Disord. 24, 551580.
https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2010.24.5.551.
Haidt, J., Graham, J., 2007. When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral
intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Soc. Justice Res. 20, 98116. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s11211-007-0034-z.
Hare, R.D., 2016. Psychopathy, the PCL-R, and criminal justice: Some new ndings and
current issues. Can. Psychol. 57, 2134. https://doi.org/10.1037/cap0000041.
Hare, R.D., 2003. The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised, 2nd ed. Multi-Health
Systems, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
Harenski, C.L., Edwards, B.G., Harenski, K.A., Kiehl, K.A., 2014. Neural correlates of
moral and non-moral emotion in female psychopathy. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8,
110. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00741.
Harenski, C.L., Harenski, K.A., Shane, M.S., Kiehl, K.A., 2010. Aberrant neural processing
of moral violations in criminal psychopaths. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 119, 863874.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020979.
Harenski, C.L., Kiehl, K.A., 2011. Emotion and morality in psychopathy and paraphilias.
Emot. Rev. 3, 299301. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911402378.
Harsay, H.A., Spaan, M., Wijnen, J.G., Ridderinkhof, K.R., 2012. Error awareness and
salience processing in the oddball task: Shared neural mechanisms. Front. Hum.
Neurosci. 6, 246. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00246.
Hastings, M.E., Tangney, J.P., Stuewig, J., 2008. Psychopathy and identication of facial
expressions of emotion. Pers. Individ. Dif. 44, 14741483. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.paid.2008.01.004.
Jonason, P.K., Strosser, G.L., Kroll, C.H., Duineveld, J.J., Baruf, S.A., 2015. Valuing
myself over others: The Dark Triad traits and moral and social values. Pers. Individ.
Dif. 81, 102106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.10.045.
Kiehl, K.A., 2015. The Psychopath Whisperer: Inside the Minds of Those Without a
Conscience. Crown Publishers, New York, NY.
Koenigs, M., Kruepke, M., Zeier, J., Newman, J.P., 2012. Utilitarian moral judgment in
psychopathy. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 708714. https://doi.org/10.1093/
scan/nsr048.
Krueger, F., Hoffman, M., 2016. The emerging neuroscience of third-party punishment.
Trends Neurosci. 39, 499501. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2016.06.004.
Lamm, C., Batson, C.D., Decety, J., 2007. The neural substrate of human empathy: Effects
of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 4258. https://
doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.42.
Lamm, C., Decety, J., Singer, T., 2011. Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct
neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain.
Neuroimage 54, 24922502. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.014.
Lenth, R.V. 2020. emmeans: Estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means.
Lindner, P., Budhiraja, M., Westerman, J., Savic, I., Jokinen, J., Tiihonen, J., Hodgins, S.,
2017. White matter correlates of psychopathic traits in a female community sample.
Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 15001510. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx070.
Lindner, P., Flodin, P., Budhiraja, M., Savic, I., Jokinen, J., Tiihonen, J., Hodgins, S.,
2018. Associations of psychopathic traits with local and global brain network
topology in young adult women. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimag. 3,
10031012. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.04.010.
Lockwood, P.L., 2016. The anatomy of empathy: Vicarious experience and disorders of
social cognition. Behav. Brain Res. 311, 255266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
bbr.2016.05.048.
Marsh, A.A., Blair, R.J.R., 2008. Decits in facial affect recognition among antisocial
populations: A meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 454465. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.08.003.
Marsh, A.A., Finger, E.C., Fowler, K.A., Adalio, C.J., Jurkowitz, I.T.N., Schechter, J.C.,
Pine, D.S., Decety, J., Blair, R.J.R., 2013. Empathic responsiveness in amygdala and
anterior cingulate cortex in youths with psychopathic traits. J. Child Psychol.
Psychiatry 54, 900910. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12063.
Marshall, J., Watts, A.L., Lilienfeld, S.O., 2018. Do psychopathic individuals possess a
misaligned moral compass? A meta-analytic examination of psychopathys relations
with moral judgment. Personal. Disord. Theory Res. Treat. 9, 4050. https://doi.org/
10.1037/per0000226.
K.J. Yoder et al.
NeuroImage: Clinical 30 (2021) 102645
10
Michalska, K.J., Kinzler, K.D., Decety, J., 2013. Age-related sex differences in explicit
measures of empathy do not predict brain responses across childhood and
adolescence. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 3, 2232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
dcn.2012.08.001.
Michalska, K.J., Zefro, T.A., Decety, J., 2015. Brain response to viewing others being
harmed in children with conduct disorder symptoms. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry.
https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12474.
Miller, R., Cushman, F., 2013. Aversive for me, wrong for you: First-person behavioral
aversions underlie the moral condemnation of harm. Soc. Personal. Psychol.
Compass 7, 707718. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12066.
Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Garrido, G.J., Bramati, I.E., Caparelli-Daquer, E.M.A.,
Paiva, M.L.M.F., Zahn, R., Grafman, J., 2007. The self as a moral agent: Linking the
neural bases of social agency and moral sensitivity. Soc. Neurosci. 2, 336352.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910701392024.
Motzkin, J.C., Newman, J.P., Kiehl, K.A., Koenigs, M., 2011. Reduced prefrontal
connectivity in psychopathy. J. Neurosci. 31, 1734817357. https://doi.org/
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4215-11.2011.
Olver, M.E., Wong, S.C.P., 2015. Short- and long-term recidivism prediction of the PCL-R
and the effects of age: A 24-year follow-up. Personal. Disord. Theory Res. Treat. 6,
97105. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000095.
Pessoa, L. 2014. The impact of emotion on cognition, in: Ochsner, K.N., Kosslyn, S.M.
(Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Neuroscience - Volume 22. Oxford
University Press, New York, NY, pp. 7993.
Poeppl, T.B., Donges, M.R., Mokros, A., Rupprecht, R., Fox, P.T., Laird, A.R., Bzdok, D.,
Langguth, B., Eickhoff, S.B., 2019. A view behind the mask of sanity: Meta-analysis
of aberrant brain activity in psychopaths. Mol. Psychiatry 24, 463470. https://doi.
org/10.1038/s41380-018-0122-5.
R Core Team, 2015. R: A language and environment for statistical computing.
Sato, J.R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Thomaz, C.E., Basílio, R., Bramati, I.E., Amaro, E.,
Tovar-Moll, F., Hare, R.D., Moll, J., 2011. Identication of psychopathic individuals
using pattern classication of MRI images. Soc. Neurosci. 6, 627639. https://doi.
org/10.1080/17470919.2011.562687.
Saxe, R., Xiao, D.-K., Kovacs, G., Perrett, D.I., Kanwisher, N., 2004. A region of right
posterior superior temporal sulcus responds to observed intentional actions.
Neuropsychologia 42, 14351446. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
neuropsychologia.2004.04.015.
Seara-Cardoso, A., Neumann, C., Roiser, J., McCrory, E., Viding, E., 2012. Investigating
associations between empathy, morality and psychopathic personality traits in the
general population. Pers. Individ. Dif. 52, 6771. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
paid.2011.08.029.
Seara-Cardoso, A., Viding, E., 2014. Functional neuroscience of psychopathic personality
in adults. J. Pers. 83, 723737. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12113.
Shackman, A.J., Salomons, T.V., Slagter, H.A., Fox, A.S., Winter, J.J., Davidson, R.J.,
2011. The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate
cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 154167. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2994.
Shamay-Tsoory, S.G., 2009. Empathic processing: Its cognitive and affective dimensions
and neuroanatomical basis. In: Decety, J., Ickes, W. (Eds.), The Social Neuroscience
of Empathy. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 215232.
Shamay-Tsoory, S.G., Harari, H., Aharon-Peretz, J., Levkovitz, Y., 2010. The role of the
orbitofrontal cortex in affective theory of mind decits in criminal offenders with
psychopathic tendencies. Cortex 46, 668677. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
cortex.2009.04.008.
Silani, G., Lamm, C., Ruff, C.C., Singer, T., 2013. Right supramarginal gyrus is crucial to
overcome emotional egocentricity bias in social judgments. J. Neurosci. 33,
1546615476. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1488-13.2013.
Singmann, H., Bolker, B.M., Westfall, J., Aust, F., Ben-Shachar, M.S. 2020. afex: Analysis
of factorial experiments.
Smaragdi, A., Cornwell, H., Toschi, N., Riccelli, R., Gonzalez-Madruga, K., Wells, A.,
Clanton, R., Baker, R., Rogers, J., Martin-Key, N., Puzzo, I., Batchelor, M.,
Sidlauskaite, J., Bernhard, A., Martinelli, A., Kohls, G., Konrad, K., Baumann, S.,
Raschle, N., Stadler, C., Freitag, C., Sonuga-Barke, E.J.S., De Brito, S., Fairchild, G.,
2017. Sex differences in the relationship between conduct disorder and cortical
structure in adolescents. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 56, 703712.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.05.015.
Tassy, S., Deruelle, C., Mancini, J., Leistedt, S., Wicker, B., 2013. High levels of
psychopathic traits alters moral choice but not moral judgment. Front. Hum.
Neurosci. 7, 16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00229.
Thijssen, S., Kiehl, K.A., 2017. Functional connectivity in incarcerated male adolescents
with psychopathic traits. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 265, 3544. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.pscychresns.2017.05.005.
Tillem, S., Harenski, K., Harenski, C., Decety, J., Kosson, D., Kiehl, K.A., Baskin-
Sommers, A., 2019. Psychopathy is associated with shifts in the organization of
neural networks in a large incarcerated male sample. NeuroImage Clin. 24, 102083
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2019.102083.
Van Bavel, J.J., FeldmanHall, O., Mende-Siedlecki, P., 2015. The neuroscience of moral
cognition: From dual processes to dynamic systems. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 6, 167172.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.08.009.
Vitale, J.E., Maccoon, D.G., Newman, J.P., 2011. Emotion facilitation and passive
avoidance learning in psychopathic female offenders. Crim. Justice Behav. 38,
641658. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854811403590.
Waller, R., Gard, A.M., Shaw, D.S., Forbes, E.E., Neumann, C.S., Hyde, L.W., 2019.
Weakened functional connectivity between the amygdala and the ventromedial
prefrontal cortex is longitudinally related to psychopathic triats in low-income males
during early adulthood. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 7, 628635. https://doi.org/10.1177/
2167702618810231.
Wolf, R.C., Pujara, M.S., Motzkin, J.C., Newman, J.P., Kiehl, K.A., Decety, J., Kosson, D.
S., Koenigs, M., 2015. Interpersonal traits of psychopathy linked to reduced integrity
of the uncinate fasciculus. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 42024209. https://doi.org/
10.1002/hbm.22911.
Yoder, K.J., Decety, J., 2018. The neuroscience of morality and social decision-making.
Psychol. Crime Law 24, 279295. https://doi.org/10.1080/
1068316X.2017.1414817.
Yoder, K.J., Decety, J., 2014a. The good, the bad, and the just: Justice sensitivity predicts
neural response during moral evaluation of actions performed by others. J. Neurosci.
34, 41614166. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4648-13.2014.
Yoder, K.J., Decety, J., 2014b. Spatiotemporal neural dynamics of moral judgment: A
high-density ERP study. Neuropsychologia 60, 3945. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
neuropsychologia.2014.05.022.
Yoder, K.J., Harenski, C., Kiehl, K.A., Decety, J., 2015a. Neural networks underlying
implicit and explicit moral evaluations in psychopathy. Transl. Psychiatry 5, e625.
https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2015.117.
Yoder, K.J., Porges, E.C., Decety, J., 2015b. Amygdala subnuclei connectivity in response
to violence reveals unique inuences of individual differences in psychopathic traits
in a nonforensic sample. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 14171428. https://doi.org/
10.1002/hbm.22712.
K.J. Yoder et al.
... Within the selected articles, the identified research designs comprised: quantitative non-randomized studies (n = 9) and descriptive (n = 4) (see Table 1). As illustrated in Table 2, none of the studies met the criteria of excellent, while eight studies demonstrated good criteria, that is, met four of the five criteria (Burley et al., 2017;Eisenbarth et al., 2008;Eisenbarth et al., 2013;Harenski et al., 2014;Justus and Finn, 2007;Verona et al., 2013;Vitale et al., 2011;Yoder et al., 2021). Three studies fulfilled three out of the five criteria (Edwards et al., 2019;Lamoureux and Glenn, 2021;Louth et al., 1998), and one study met only two of the quality criteria (Pinheiro et al., 2019). ...
... In the included studies, the assessment of psychopathy involved the utilization of several instruments, including Hare's Psychopathy Checklist -Revised (PCL-R; n = 9; Edwards et al., 2019;Eisenbarth et al., 2008;Eisenbarth et al., 2013;Harenski et al., 2014;Louth et al., 1998;Pinheiro et al., 2019;Verona et al., 2013;Vitale et al., 2011;Yoder et al., 2021), the Psychopathy Checklist -Screening Version (PCL:SV; n = 1; Lamoureux and Glenn, 2021), the Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI; n = 1; Justus and Finn, 2007) and its short form (Rilling et al., 2007), as well as the Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale (LSRPS; n = 2; Pinheiro et al., 2019;Rilling et al., 2007). Studies employing PCL measures were primarily conducted in prison and forensic hospital settings. ...
... The Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS) was utilized in two studies (Lamoureux and Glenn, 2021;Louth et al., 1998). Seven studies employed visual stimuli to assess emotional processing, including dyadic interactions depicting intentional interpersonal harm or assistance (Yoder et al., 2021), a video clip featuring various facial expressions (happy, sad, scared, angry, disgusted, or in pain; Yoder et al., 2021), images from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; Burley et al., 2017;Eisenbarth et al., 2013;Harenski et al., 2014;Justus and Finn, 2007;Verona et al., 2013), static facial expressions from the Radboud Faces Database and dynamic facial expressions from the Amsterdam Dynamic Facial Expression Set (Burley et al., 2017), Prisoner's Dilemma Game (Rilling et al., 2007), and photos from the Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces (KDEF; Eisenbarth et al., 2008), measuring heart rate and skin conductance in response to visual stimuli (Lamoureux and Glenn, 2021). Additionally, auditory stimuli were employed in two studies: Acoustic startle probes (Verona et al., 2013) and affective sound clips from the International Affective Digitized Sounds (Burley et al., 2017). ...
... Of these, only one set of studies used a minimally reproducible method (direct replication attempt involving adequately powered studies in independent samples relevant to defendants). Methodologically, therefore, these two studies met the criteria for admissibility Yoder et al., 2021). However, Yoder et al. (2021) failed to replicate any psychopathy-related findings from Decety et al. ...
... Methodologically, therefore, these two studies met the criteria for admissibility Yoder et al., 2021). However, Yoder et al. (2021) failed to replicate any psychopathy-related findings from Decety et al. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. ...
Article
Full-text available
The amount of neuroimaging evidence introduced in courts continues to increase. Meanwhile, neuroimaging research is in the midst of a reproducibility crisis, as many published findings appear to be false positives. The problem is mostly due to small sample sizes, lack of direct replications, and questionable research practices. There are concerns that a significant proportion of neuroimaging evidence introduced in court may therefore be unreliable. Guidelines governing the admissibility of scientific evidence—Frye and Daubert—are not designed to weed out such data. We propose supplementing Frye and Daubert with minimal reproducibility criteria that allow judges to make informed admissibility decisions about neuroimaging research. To demonstrate how this could work, we subjected functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) findings on psychopathy—evidence that has been admitted in court—to a minimal reproducibility test. A systematic PRISMA search found 64 relevant studies but no sufficiently powered, directly replicated evidence of a psychopathy-related neurobiological profile. This illustrates two things: (a) the probability of false positives in this data set is likely to be unacceptably high and (b) the reproducibility of similar neuroimaging evidence can be evaluated in a straightforward way. Our findings suggest an urgent need to modify admissibility guidelines to exclude low-quality neuroimaging data.
... Likewise, the tendency to score higher on the interpersonal and affective components has been documented in previous work (Verona, Sprague, and Sadeh 2012). These results seem to be in line with previous investigations that found deficits in the level of processing and categorization of emotional content in women with psychopathic traits (Eisenbarth et al. 2008), mainly due to changes in the level of brain connectivity associated with emotional processing (Eisenbarth et al. 2013;Harenski et al. 2014;Yoder et al. 2021). Some research also shows differences in terms of deficits in emotional regulation between men and women, with women with psychopathic characteristics having difficulties in regulating positive emotions, while men have difficulties in regulating negative emotions (Hicks et al. 2004). ...
... We found that the secondary variant was characterized by altered connectivity between the left amygdala and pSTS/TPJ compared to the primary variant. Indeed, some researchers have found that altered amygdala-pSTS/TPJ connectivity was associated with the severity of callousness during resting-state (Espinoza et al., 2019) as well as levels of psychopathic traits when identifying emotions during harmful interactions (Yoder et al., 2021). Furthermore, Sethi and colleagues (Sethi et al., 2018) found that deficient activity of the pSTS/TPJ was significantly more pronounced in secondary than in the primary variant during fearful face processing. ...
Article
Over the past years, research has shown that primary (high callousness and low anxiety) and secondary (high callousness and anxiety) variants of CU traits may be associated with opposite amygdala activity (hypo- and hyper-reactivity, respectively). However, their differences in amygdala functional connectivity remains largely unexplored. We conducted a Latent Profile Analysis on a large sample of adolescents (n = 1416) to identify homogeneous subgroups with different levels of callousness and anxiety. We then performed a seed-to-voxel connectivity analysis on resting-state fMRI data to compare subgroups on connectivity patterns of the amygdala. We examined the results in relation to conduct problems to identify potential neural risk factors. The Latent Profile Analysis revealed four subgroups, including primary and secondary variants of psychopathy, anxious adolescents, and typically developing adolescents. The seed-to-voxel analyses showed that the primary variant was mainly characterized by increased connectivity between the left amygdala, and left thalamus. The secondary variant exhibited deficient connectivity between the amygdala and the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, temporo-parietal junction, premotor, and postcentral gyrus. Both variants showed increased connectivity with the right thalamus but exhibited opposite functional connectivity between the left amygdala and the parahippocampal gyrus. Dimensional analyses indicated that conduct problems may play a mediating role between callousness and amygdala-dmPFC functional connectivity across youths with already high levels of callousness. Our study highlights that both variants largely differ in the functional connectivity of the amygdala. The results support the importance of disentangling the heterogeneity of adolescents at risk for conduct problems.
... In addition, higher magnetic field strength might not always provide superior fMRI results in social cognition studies since susceptibility artifacts are greater and can have adverse effects in www.nature.com/scientificreports/ the detection of activity in critical regions such as the amygdala and potentially others, including the anterior hippocampus, the anterior temporal pole, and the inferior orbitofrontal cortex 116 . Finally, not only classical studies on the neural bases of moral cognition have been carried out using 1.5 T scanners [119][120][121] , but also recent ones 122,123 . In sum, while we cannot rule out potential false negative results, our statistical approach to fMRI analysis (corrections for multiple comparisons and bootstrapping) controlled for false positive findings. ...
... In addition, higher magnetic field strength might not always provide superior fMRI results in social cognition studies since susceptibility artifacts are greater and can have adverse effects in www.nature.com/scientificreports/ the detection of activity in critical regions such as the amygdala and potentially others, including the anterior hippocampus, the anterior temporal pole, and the inferior orbitofrontal cortex 116 . Finally, not only classical studies on the neural bases of moral cognition have been carried out using 1.5 T scanners [119][120][121] , but also recent ones 122,123 . In sum, while we cannot rule out potential false negative results, our statistical approach to fMRI analysis (corrections for multiple comparisons and bootstrapping) controlled for false positive findings. ...
Article
Full-text available
Individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) present difficulties in integrating mental state information in complex moral tasks. Yet, ASD research has not examined whether this process is influenced by emotions, let alone while capturing its neural bases. We investigated how language-induced emotions modulate intent-based moral judgment in ASD. In a fMRI task, 30 adults with ASD and 27 neurotypical controls read vignettes whose protagonists commit harm either accidentally or intentionally, and then decided how much punishment the protagonist deserved. Emotional content was manipulated across scenarios through the use of graphic language (designed to trigger arousing negative responses) vs. plain (just-the-facts, emotionless) language. Off-line functional connectivity correlates of task performance were also analyzed. In ASD, emotional (graphic) descriptions amplified punishment ratings of accidental harms, associated with increased activity in fronto-temporo-limbic, precentral, and postcentral/supramarginal regions (critical for emotional and empathic processes), and reduced connectivity among the orbitofrontal cortex and the angular gyrus (involved in mentalizing). Language manipulation did not influence intentional harm processing in ASD. In conclusion, in arousing and ambiguous social situations that lack intentionality clues (i.e. graphic accidental harm scenarios), individuals with ASD would misuse their emotional responses as the main source of information to guide their moral decisions. Conversely, in face of explicit harmful intentions, they would be able to compensate their socioemotional alterations and assign punishment through non-emotional pathways. Despite limitations, such as the small sample size and low ecological validity of the task, results of the present study proved reliable and have relevant theoretical and translational implications.
... Duygusal empatiyi gösterebilme yeteneği, antisosyallerde hasarlıdır. Diğer bir değişle, eşduyum kuramazlar (Yoder vd, 2021;Domes vd, 2013;Cardoso vd, 2012;March, 2019). Aslında "psikopatların sergiledikleri özelliklerin pek çoğu, özellikle benmerkezcilik, pişmanlık duymama, sığ duygular ve aldatmaya yatkınlık, empatiden (eşduyum) derinlemesine yoksun olmalarıyla yakından ilişkilidir." ...
Thesis
Full-text available
It seems like the representation of antisocials on the series is more popular than ever. Then, one can ask: How much do these series represent antisocial characters like they really are? In this study, it has been firstly employed the history of antisocial personality disorder, and then, in the second chapter, the disease has been defined in detail, and in the third chapter, it has been correlated with media. In the fourth and the last chapter of the study, which is the research part, it has been analysed the representation of antisocial characters on different categories of Netflix series through qualitative content analysis, and has been given analyses in detail about the representation of characters as much as possible. In this context, two serial killer/murder series, two adolescent series and two prison series, in total, six Netflix series and seven characters on these series has been analysed. The research contains the following series: Dexter, How to Get Away with Murder, Élite, 13 Reasons Why, Locked Up and Orange Is the New Black. The research shows that antisocials are represented as criminals who comits felonies like murder and rape, and with positive qualifications, which someone with the disorder can’t have. Why are these representations important? Because, in consequence of these representations, people might tend to think that only the felons are antisocials, and thus they may not recognize antisocials who doesn’t commit a crime and may think antisocials have deep emotions and are capable of feeling strong positive emotions for others. All of these wrong beliefs can cause losses and damages in people’s lives.
Article
Full-text available
Psychopathy assessments are widely used in the legal system to inform decisions about sentencing, rehabilitation, etc. Recently, these assessments have become controversial as long-held beliefs about psychopathy are contested. One common claim that has yet to be scrutinized is the assumption that psychopathic persons lack empathy. This hypothesis has been studied extensively in forensic samples, but there are few relevant systematic reviews to inform forensic practices. We conducted a systematic review of empirical studies testing empathy in persons assessed with the Hare Psychopathy Checklist (PCL) scales. Included studies (n = 66) measured general, cognitive, and affective empathy across two different paradigms using (a) empathy assessment tools and (b) emotion recognition tests. These studies had great heterogeneity in their reporting of effects, precluding a meaningful meta-analysis. A structured overview of results showed a total of 1,672 effects were reported across 66 included studies (N = 5,711) of which 182 effects (10.89%) were statistically significant and 1,490 effects (89.11%) were nulls. Exploratory analyses revealed that high-powered studies had a significantly larger proportion of nulls (89.29%) compared to low-powered studies (81.45%), suggesting a potential problem with false-positive discoveries in the research literature. In conclusion, we found little evidence to suggest that PCL psychopathic persons have different empathic capacities, thus challenging the common assumption that psychopathy is associated with a lack of empathy. We discuss the implication of these results for the current and future use of PCL assessment in forensic settings.
Article
Full-text available
Theories have posited that psychopathy is caused by dysfunction in the medial frontal cortex, including ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC). Recent reviews have questioned the reproducibility of neuroimaging findings within this field. We conducted a systematic review to describe the consistency of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) findings according to anatomical subregion (vmPFC, ACC, dmPFC), experimental task, psychopathy assessment, study power, and peak coordinates of significant effects. Searches of PsycInfo and MEDLINE databases produced 77 functional and 24 structural MRI studies that analyzed the medial frontal cortex in relation to psychopathy in adult samples. Findings were predominantly null (85.4 % of 1573 tests across the three medial frontal regions). Studies with higher power observed null effects at marginally lower rates. Finally, peak coordinates of significant effects were widely dispersed. The evidence failed to support theories positing the medial frontal cortex as a consistent neural correlate of psychopathy. Theory and methods in the field should be revised to account for predominantly null neuroimaging findings.
Article
The amygdala is a key component in predominant neural circuitry models of psychopathy. Yet, after two decades of neuroimaging research on psychopathy, the reproducibility of amygdala findings is questionable. We systematically reviewed MRI studies (81 of adults, 53 of juveniles) to determine the consistency of amygdala findings across studies, as well as within specific types of experimental tasks, community versus forensic populations, and the lowest- versus highest-powered studies. Three primary findings emerged. First, the majority of studies found null relationships between psychopathy and amygdala structure and function, even in the context of theoretically relevant tasks. Second, findings of reduced amygdala activity were more common in studies with low compared to high statistical power. Third, the majority of peak coordinates of reduced amygdala activity did not fall primarily within the anatomical bounds of the amygdala. Collectively, these findings demonstrate significant gaps in the empirical support for the theorized role of the amygdala in psychopathy and indicate the need for novel research perspectives and approaches in this field.
Article
Full-text available
Psychopathic individuals are notorious for their callous disregard for others' emotions. Prior research has linked psychopathy to deficits in affective mechanisms underlying empathy (e.g., affective sharing), yet research relating psychopathy to cognitive mechanisms underlying empathy (e.g., affective perspective-taking and Theory of Mind) requires further clarification. To elucidate the neurobiology of cognitive mechanisms of empathy in psychopathy, we administered an fMRI task and tested for global as well as emotion-specific deficits in affective perspective-taking. Adult male incarcerated offenders (N=94) viewed images of two people interacting, with one individual's face obscured by a shape. Participants were cued to either identify the emotion of the obscured individual or identify the shape from one of two emotion or shape choices presented on each trial. Target emotions included anger, fear, happiness, sadness, and neutral. Contrary to predictions, psychopathy was unrelated to neural activity in the Affective Perspective-taking > Shape contrast. In line with predictions, psychopathy was negatively related to task accuracy during affective perspective-taking for fear, happiness, and sadness. Psychopathy was related to reduced hemodynamic activity exclusively during fear perspective-taking in several areas: left anterior insula extending into posterior orbitofrontal cortex, right precuneus, left superior parietal lobule, and left superior occipital cortex. Although much prior research has emphasized psychopathy-related abnormalities in affective mechanisms mediating empathy, current results add to growing evidence of psychopathy-related abnormalities in a cognitive mechanism related to empathy. These findings highlight brain regions that are hypoactive in psychopathy when explicitly processing another's fear.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Empathy for pain is a complex phenomenon incorporating sensory, cognitive and affective processes. Functional neuroimaging studies indicate a rich network of brain activations for empathic processing. However, previous research focused on core activations in bilateral anterior insula (AI) and anterior/anterior mid-cingulate cortex (ACC/aMCC) which are also typically present during nociceptive (pain) processing. Theoretical understanding of empathy would benefit from empirical investigation of shared and contrasting brain activations for empathic and nociceptive processing. Method: Thirty-nine empathy for observed pain studies (1112 participants; 527 foci) were selected by systematic review. Coordinate based meta-analysis (activation likelihood estimation) was performed and novel contrast analyses compared neurobiological processing of empathy with a comprehensive meta-analysis of 180 studies of nociceptive processing (Tanasescu et al., 2016). Results: Conjunction analysis indicated overlapping activations for empathy and nociception in AI, aMCC, somatosensory and inferior frontal regions. Contrast analysis revealed increased likelihood of activation for empathy, relative to nociception, in bilateral supramarginal, inferior frontal and occipitotemporal regions. Nociception preferentially activated bilateral posterior insula, somatosensory cortex and aMCC. Conclusion: Our findings support the likelihood of shared and distinct neural networks for empathic, relative to nociceptive, processing. This offers succinct empirical support for recent tiered or modular theoretical accounts of empathy.
Article
Full-text available
Neuroimaging studies over the last two decades have begun to specify the neurobiological correlates of psychopathy, a personality disorder that is strongly related to criminal offending and recidivism. Despite the accumulation of neuroimaging studies of psychopathy, a clear and comprehensive picture of the disorder’s neural correlates has yet to emerge. The current study is a meta-analysis of functional MRI studies of psychopathy. Multilevel kernel density analysis was used to identify consistent findings across 25 studies (460 foci) of task-related brain activity. Psychopathy was associated with increased task-related activity predominantly in midline cortical regions overlapping with the default mode network (dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate, and precuneus) as well as medial temporal lobe (including amygdala). Psychopathy was related to decreased task-related activity in a region of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex overlapping with the salience network. These findings challenge predominant theories of amygdala hypoactivity and highlight the potential role of hyperactivity in medial default mode network regions and hypoactivity in a key node of the salience network during task performance in psychopathy.
Article
Full-text available
Psychopathy is a personality disorder defined by antisocial behavior paired with callousness, low empathy, and low interpersonal emotions. Psychopathic individuals reliably display complex atypicalities in emotion and attention processing that are evident when examining task performance, activation within specific neural regions, and connections between regions. Recent advances in neuroimaging methods, namely graph analyses, attempt to unpack this type of processing complexity by evaluating the overall organization of neural networks. Graph analyses have been used to better understand neural functioning in several clinical disorders but have not yet been used in the study of psychopathy. The present study applies a minimum spanning tree graph analysis to resting-state fMRI data collected from male inmates assessed for psychopathy with the Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (n = 847). Minimum spanning tree analysis provides several metrics of neural organization optimality (i.e., the effectiveness, efficiency, and robustness of neural network organization). Results show that inmates higher in psychopathy exhibit a more efficiently organized dorsal attention network (β = 0.101, pcorrected = .018). Additionally, subcortical structures (e.g., amygdala, caudate, and hippocampus) act as less of a central hub in the global flow of information in inmates higher in psychopathy (β = -0.104, pcorrected =.048). There were no significant effects of psychopathy on neural network organization in the default or salience networks. Together, these shifts in neural organization suggest that the brains of inmates higher in psychopathy are organized in a fundamentally different way than other individuals.
Article
Full-text available
Psychopathy is a disorder of high public concern because it predicts violence and offense recidivism. Recent brain imaging studies suggest abnormal brain activity underlying psychopathic behavior. No reliable pattern of altered neural activity has been disclosed so far. This study sought to identify consistent changes of brain activity in psychopaths and to investigate whether these could explain known psychopathology. First, we used activation likelihood estimation (p < 0.05, corrected) to meta-analyze brain activation changes associated with psychopathy across 28 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies reporting 753 foci from 155 experiments. Second, we characterized the ensuing regions functionally by employing meta-data of a large-scale neuroimaging database (p < 0.05, corrected). Psychopathy was consistently associated with decreased brain activity in the right laterobasal amygdala, the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, and bilaterally in the lateral prefrontal cortex. A robust increase of activity was observed in the fronto-insular cortex on both hemispheres. Data-driven functional characterization revealed associations with semantic language processing (left lateral prefrontal and fronto-insular cortex), action execution and pain processing (right lateral prefrontal and left fronto-insular), social cognition (dorsomedial prefrontal cortex), and emotional as well as cognitive reward processing (right amygdala and fronto-insular cortex). Aberrant brain activity related to psychopathy is located in prefrontal, insular, and limbic regions. Physiological mental functions fulfilled by these brain regions correspond to disturbed behavioral patterns pathognomonic for psychopathy. Hence, aberrant brain activity may not just be an epiphenomenon of psychopathy but directly related to the psychopathology of this disorder.
Article
Prominent theories suggest that disruptions in amygdala reactivity and connectivity when processing emotional cues are key to the etiology of youth antisocial behavior (AB) and that these associations may be dependent on co-occurring levels of callous-unemotional (CU) traits. We examined the associations among AB, CU traits, and amygdala reactivity and functional connectivity while viewing emotional faces (fearful, angry, sad, happy) in 165 adolescents (46% male; 73.3% African American) from a representative, predominantly low-income community sample. AB was associated with increased amygdala activation in response to all emotions and was associated with greater amygdala reactivity to emotion only at low levels of CU traits. AB and CU traits were also associated with distinct patterns of amygdala connectivity. These findings demonstrate that AB-related deficits in amygdala functioning may extend across all emotions and highlight the need for further research on amygdala connectivity during emotion processing in relation to AB and CU traits within community populations.
Article
Psychopathy is a complex disorder consisting of harmful personality traits and impulsive-lifestyle and antisocial behaviors. Weakened functional connectivity between limbic and prefrontal brain regions is thought to underlie impaired sensitivity to others’ emotions that contribute to the interpersonal and affective personality traits associated with psychopathy. We tested whether weakened functional connectivity between the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) during the processing of fearful, angry, and neutral facial expressions was prospectively related to psychopathic traits in early adulthood. The sample included 167 low-income, racially diverse, urban males who completed a functional MRI scan at age 20 and questionnaire measures at ages 20 and 22. Weakened amygdala-vmPFC functional connectivity to fearful but not neutral or angry faces at age 20 was related to higher psychopathic traits at age 22.
Article
Background: Psychopathic traits vary dimensionally in the population and are associated with multiple negative outcomes. The impaired integration theory (IIT) proposes that psychopathic traits are associated with abnormal neural network topology, such that disturbed integration of neural networks results in a self-perpetuating impairment in rapid integration and learning from multiple components of information. The IIT is based on findings from male offenders presenting high scores on all psychopathic traits. The present study investigated whether IIT predictions of topology abnormalities were associated with psychopathic traits, measured dimensionally, in young adult women with subsyndromal scores. Methods: Seventy-three women, with an average age of 25 years, were assessed using the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised and completed resting-state magnetic resonance imaging. Preprocessed time series from 90 anatomical regions were extracted to form connectivity matrices and used to calculate network topology based on graph theory. Correlations between total psychopathy and factor scores with both the raw connectivity matrix and global and local graph theory measures were computed. Results: Total psychopathy scores and behavioral factor scores were related to connectivity between several pairs of regions, primarily limbic/paralimbic. Psychopathic traits were not associated with global topology measures. Topology abnormalities, robust across network formation thresholds, were found in nodes of the default mode network and in hubs connecting several resting-state networks. Conclusions: IIT predictions of abnormal topology of hubs and default mode network nodes with dimensionally measured psychopathic traits were confirmed in a sample of young women. Regional abnormalities, accompanied by preserved global topology, may underlie context-specific abnormal information processing and integration.
Article
Psychopathic individuals display a chronic and flagrant disregard for the welfare of others through their callous and manipulative behavior. Historically, this behavior is thought to result from deficits in social-affective processing. However, we show that at least some psychopathic behaviors may be rooted in a cognitive deficit, specifically an inability to automatically take another person’s perspective. Unlike prior studies that rely solely on controlled theory of mind (ToM) tasks, we employ a task that taps into automatic ToM processing. Controlled ToM processes are engaged when an individual intentionally considers the perspective of another person, whereas automatic ToM processes are engaged when an individual unintentionally represents the perspective of another person. In a sample of incarcerated offenders, we find that psychopathic individuals are equally likely to show response interference under conditions of controlled ToM, but lack a common signature of automatic ToM known as altercentric interference. We also demonstrate that the magnitude of this dysfunction in altercentric interference is correlated with real-world callous behaviors (i.e., number of assault charges). These findings suggest that psychopathic individuals have a diminished propensity to automatically think from another’s perspective, which may be the cognitive root of their deficits in social functioning and moral behavior.
Article
Psychopathy is a personality disorder characterized by antisocial behavior, lack of remorse and empathy, and impaired decision making. The disproportionate amount of crime committed by psychopaths has severe emotional and economic impacts on society. Here we examine the neural correlates associated with psychopathy to improve early assessment and perhaps inform treatments for this condition. Previous resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies in psychopathy have primarily focused on regions of interest. This study examines whole-brain functional connectivity and its association to psychopathic traits. Psychopathy was hypothesized to be characterized by aberrant functional network connectivity (FNC) in several limbic/paralimbic networks. Group-independent component and regression analyses were applied to a data set of resting-state fMRI from 985 incarcerated adult males. We identified resting-state networks (RSNs), estimated FNC between RSNs, and tested their association to psychopathy factors and total summary scores (Factor 1, interpersonal/affective; Factor 2, lifestyle/antisocial). Factor 1 scores showed both increased and reduced functional connectivity between RSNs from seven brain domains (sensorimotor, cerebellar, visual, salience, default mode, executive control, and attentional). Consistent with hypotheses, RSNs from the paralimbic system—insula, anterior and posterior cingulate cortex, amygdala, orbital frontal cortex, and superior temporal gyrus—were related to Factor 1 scores. No significant FNC associations were found with Factor 2 and total PCL-R scores. In summary, results suggest that the affective and interpersonal symptoms of psychopathy (Factor 1) are associated with aberrant connectivity in multiple brain networks, including paralimbic regions.