Access to this full-text is provided by Springer Nature.
Content available from Psychological Research
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Vol:.(1234567890)
Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01503-8
1 3
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Space, time andnumber: common coding mechanisms
andinteractions betweendomains
DeborahJ.Serrien1 · MichielM.Spapé2
Received: 22 August 2020 / Accepted: 5 March 2021 / Published online: 23 March 2021
© The Author(s) 2021
Abstract
Space, time and number are key dimensions that underlie how we perceive, identify and act within the environment. They
are interconnected in our behaviour and brain. In this study, we examined interdependencies between these dimensions. To
this end, left- and right-handed participants performed an object collision task that required space–time processing and arith-
metic tests that involved number processing. Handedness of the participants influenced collision detection with left-handers
being more accurate than right-handers, which is in line with the premise that hand preference guides individual differences
as a result of sensorimotor experiences and distinct interhemispheric integration patterns. The data further showed that suc-
cessful collision detection was a predictor for arithmetic achievement, at least in right-handers. These findings suggest that
handedness plays a mediating role in binding information processing across domains, likely due to selective connectivity
properties within the sensorimotor system that is guided by hemispheric lateralisation patterns.
Introduction
In everyday life, we often interact with moving objects, such
as catching a ball or crossing a road. Crucial to these sen-
sorimotor activities is the ability to predict the trajectory of
the moving objects and the changes of their position over
time (Enns & Lleras, 2008; Senot, etal. 2003). To imple-
ment these predictions, the brain uses a range of quantitative
inputs, such as spatial, temporal and numerical information.
Moreover, space–time–number represent essential dimen-
sions that can be encoded through all sensory modalities
(Burr, etal. 2010). These dimensions further demonstrate
associations, such as the SNARC effect, that captures num-
ber–space interactions with faster responses occurring to
smaller/larger numbers on the left/right side of space due to
a representation of increasing numerical value from left to
right (Dehaene, etal. 1993).
To account for these interdependencies, Walsh (2003)
argued for a magnitude system that involves processing of
dimensional magnitudes and their interactions (Bonato,
etal. 2012; Burr, etal. 2010; Dehaene & Brannon, 2001;
Fabbri, etal. 2013; Hayashi, etal. 2013). Furthermore, the
proposed ATOM model (A Theory of Magnitude) underlines
that actions are instrumental in establishing the magnitude
system, with parietal circuitry providing a neural platform
to exchange information (Walsh, 2003). That is, it is through
actions that associations between magnitudes are learned for
example that larger objects tend to be heavier than smaller
ones. Thus, the magnitude system ties interactions between
dimensions such that ‘more’ in a dimension couples with
‘more’ in another dimension. The origin of these interactions
is that they reflect innate mappings or developmental pro-
cesses, although both types of mechanisms could influence
one another with innate pathways being influenced by early
experiences and learned processes by innate constraints (De
Hevia, etal. 2014; Stanescu-Cosson, etal. 2000; Walsh,
2003). Besides innate and developmental systems, atten-
tional processes also play an important role. For example,
attention can be directed towards specific task features, such
as a location in space or a moment in time, which accord-
ingly supports behavioural performance (Coull & Nobre,
1998; Dehaene, etal. 2003).
According to current viewpoints, a dimension could
emerge from another, resulting in functional similarities and
dependencies in computational and neural mechanisms. One
particular hypothesis is that space serves as a foundation for
the dimensions that are conceptually more abstract, such as
* Deborah J. Serrien
deborah.serrien@nottingham.ac.uk
1 School ofPsychology, University ofNottingham,
Nottingham, UK
2 Department ofPsychology andLogopedics, University
ofHelsinki, Helsinki, Finland
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
365Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
time and number (Bonn & Cantlon, 2012). Thus, magnitude
representations with a common code could evolve due to
the substrates that already exist for the space dimension. Of
note, however, is that the existence of dimensional interac-
tions does not imply that these are equal in strength. For
example, there is evidence that space–time interactions are
strongest due to the specialisation of the magnitude system
for sensorimotor actions, followed by space–number and
time–number interactions (Bueti & Walsh, 2009).
Taking into account that the magnitude system is estab-
lished through actions, the question emerges about the effect
of sensorimotor experiences. That is, it can be argued that
magnitude representations would differ if individuals inter-
act in different ways with the environment due to inherent
biases. Handedness is such a characteristic that captures
asymmetry of movement control and expresses how indi-
viduals use their hands during manual activities. Research
has shown that handedness not only affects the sensorimo-
tor control mechanisms (Klöppel, etal. 2007; Pool, etal.
2014; Reid & Serrien, 2014; Serrien, etal. 2012) but also
influences how individuals attend to and respond to the envi-
ronment. In other words, handedness influences a range of
abilities that involves visuospatial functions (Bareham, etal.
2015; Hécaen & Sauguet, 1971; O’Regan & Serrien, 2018;
Vogel, etal. 2003), attentional regulation in space and time
(Buckingham etal., 2009; O’Regan, etal. 2017) and visual
processing in perihand space (Le Bigot & Grosjean, 2012).
Combined, these findings illustrate that handedness has a
widespread impact on the processing requirements of space
and time for meeting behavioural goals.
The aim of the present experiment is to examine how
space–time processing naturally connects with number pro-
cessing, based on the proposed interdependencies between
the dimensions of space–time–number. In this respect, a
valuable experimental approach is to study a functional
effect at the level of the behavioural outputs. First, we use
an object collision task that requires participants to predict
whether moving objects will collide with one another or
not at a specific moment in time. To be successful, an accu-
rate estimation of the moving objects over time is required
(O’Reilly, etal. 2008; Proffitt & Gilden, 1989). It involves
information about the path of the objects in space which is
strongly linked with spatial coordinates, whereas the veloc-
ity with which the objects move implies position changes
in temporal coordinates. From a neural viewpoint, previous
work has shown that collision detection associates with the
left inferior parietal cortex (Assmuss, etal. 2003). Second,
we include arithmetic tests that require the use of numerical
information processing established by operator-dependent
rules (Friedrich & Friederici, 2009). We use tests with dif-
ferent types of arithmetic operations; additions, subtrac-
tions, and multiplications. Neurally, research has shown
that tasks that involve numbers and calculations involve
bilateral inferior parietal activity as a function of the arith-
metic operation (Arsalidou & Taylor, 2011), albeit with
a key involvement of the left hemisphere (Dehaene, etal.
1993). Third, we study left- and right-handers based on the
premise that handedness introduces distinct sensorimotor
experiences that affect the processing demands. We also
conduct an evaluation of the participants’ personality traits
as a relationship between handedness and negative affect has
been proposed due to an underlying influence of the right
hemisphere (Sutton & Davidson, 1997) with left- as com-
pared to right-handers obtaining higher self-reported levels
of behavioural inhibition (Hardie & Wright, 2014).
In the present work, we argue that the space–time calcula-
tions of the object collision task associate with arithmetic
computations due to shared neural mechanisms. We further
hypothesise that the participants’ handedness guides the
processing demands as a result of their sensorimotor expe-
riences and interactions with objects. Combined, insights
into individual differences of handedness and interdependen-
cies across space–time–number processing will be valuable
to increase our understanding of common mechanisms that
underlie our behaviour.
Methods
Participants
There were 37 participants in this study (Mage = 20.7years,
SEage = 0.6), including 19 left-handers and 18 right-hand-
ers. They reported no history of neurological or psychiatric
conditions as evaluated by a standardised questionnaire,
and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Participants
gave written consent prior to the start of the experiment in
accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was
approved by the School of Psychology Ethics Committee.
Handedness questionnaire
To characterise handedness, participants completed a
15-item handedness questionnaire that measured hand pref-
erence for manipulation tasks (i.e., write a letter, use spoon,
use toothbrush, throw ball to hit target, use a comb, hold
racquet, hold needle when sewing, draw a picture, use com-
puter mouse, open lid from can, hold knife to cut, peel an
apple, use scissors, deal cards, use eraser).
The handedness questionnaire used a 5-point Likert scale
that varied between always left and always right. The score
per item was calculated with a value of 0 (always left), 1
(usually left), 2 (both equally), 3 (usually right) or 4 (always
right). For each participant, the scores of the items were
summed, divided by the maximum score of the question-
naire, and multiplied by 100. This provided a laterality index
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
366 Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
of handedness that varied between 0 (extreme left-handed-
ness) and 100 (extreme right-handedness). The writing hand
was also included as a condition for handedness as most
people will categorise their handedness on the basis of their
writing hand (Perelle & Ehrman, 2005).
Reinforcement sensitivity theory personality
questionnaire (RST‑PQ)
To capture personality traits, participants completed subtests
of the RST-PQ (Corr & Cooper, 2016), which rely on the
premise that individual differences emerge from neurobio-
logical systems that are specialised in detecting, process-
ing, and responding to stimuli. In particular, the Behav-
ioural Inhibition System (BIS) engages risk assessment and
inhibits behaviour in response to goal conflict, resulting in
anxiety and behavioural avoidance. In contrast, the Behav-
ioural Approach System (BAS) facilitates goal-directed
activity and positive emotions, leading to optimistic mood
and achieved goals. The questionnaire covered BIS activity
with 23 items and BAS activity with 32 items; i.e., biased
attention towards reward interest (BAS-RI with 7 items),
goal drive persistence (BAS-GDR with 7 items), impulsiv-
ity (BAS-IMP with 8 items) and reward reactivity (BAS-RR
with 10 items).
The questionnaire used a 4-point Likert scale for accuracy
of statements, ranging between 1 (not at all) and 4 (highly
accurate). For each sub-test, the ratings were summed
across items to provide a total score. High scores indicated
increased sensitivity of a given neurobiological system.
Object collision task
Participants were seated at a viewing distance of 70cm from
a computer monitor. The trial presentation is illustrated in
Fig.1. Each trial started with the presentation of a fixation
cross that lasted 1000ms followed by the appearance of
a black and white object, either 3.6º on the upper, lower,
left or right side of the screen’s centre. Thereafter, the per-
pendicular presented objects with a diameter of 0.4° would
start to move in straight lines with a constant speed of 2.8
or 5.6°/s towards the screen’s centre, resulting in collision
and non-collision events. As soon as the objects started to
move, the participants were required to decide whether the
objects would collide (target hit) or not collide (target miss)
behind a mask that had a height and width of 3.6°. This point
of collision which occurred 1300ms after onset is not shown
to the participants as the mask would hide the final trajecto-
ries of the objects from 1200ms after onset. After another
700ms or until the participants made a response, a blank
screen occurred that marked the end of the trial. In 33% of
the trials, a third grey object (distractor) would move with a
similar speed alongside the black object towards the mask.
These trials were included to influence attentional selection
to the relevant objects. The performance conditions (without
distractor vs. with distractor) and type of collision (target
hit vs. target miss) were randomised. There were 32 trials
per performance condition, resulting in a total of 128 trials.
Participants were asked to respond as fast and as accurate
as possible in their decision-making using keys allocated to
the index and middle fingers of the left or right hand (coun-
terbalanced). Before the start of the experiment, a training
session with feedback was provided, and there were short
breaks throughout the experiment. The trial sequence and
data collection were implemented using e-Prime.
The measurements of the task were collision detection
time (ms) and accuracy (%). The collision detection time
comprised the time period between initiation of the moving
objects and key press responses whereas the collision detec-
tion accuracy referred to correctly confirmed collisions on
contact trials and correctly rejected collisions on no contact
trials, and represented a key measurement that captures the
ability to predict the collision event at a precise moment
in time. We also calculated the balanced integration score
to obtain a composite evaluation of both measurements.
This index integrates reaction time and accuracy with equal
weighting and is considered beneficial as compared to other
methods that assess speed-accuracy trade-offs (Liesefeld
Fig. 1 Collision task without and with distractor. Left side: after dis-
appearance of the fixation cross, the task starts with the black and
white objects moving towards the centre. Right side: after 1200ms,
these objects disappear behind a mask while their final trajectories
are hidden from view. In the collision task with distractor, the grey
object (distractor) moves along the black object on the side nearest to
the white object
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
367Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
& Janczyk, 2019; Vandierendonck, 2017). The balanced
integration score is calculated by independently standardis-
ing the reaction times and percentage correct responses to
bring them onto the same scale, and then subtracting one
standardised score from the other. Its interpretation is in
terms of performance above or below average, and there-
fore measures relative performance—for example, whether
one group of participants is more successful than another
group, or, whether one condition is more difficult than
another condition.
Arithmetic tests
Participants were asked to answer a series of arithmetic
operations, i.e., additions, subtractions, multiplications using
pen and paper. There were two lists consisting of 10 prob-
lems for each arithmetic operation, which were presented
separately. For additions and subtractions, the problems
involved double- and single-digit operands (64–7) or two
double-digit operands (69 + 15) whereas for multiplications,
the problems involved single-digit operands (7 × 4) or dou-
ble- and single-digit operands (92 × 3), excluding 0 and 1 as
one of the operands. Across the arithmetic tests, the prob-
lems required a combination of memory retrieval and com-
putation. Participants were asked to answer the problems
as fast and as accurate as possible. As a control condition,
a number copying tasks were conducted. This consisted of
copying a list of 20 numbers (four lists of five numbers)
that involved double- or triple-digit operands. Short breaks
between the tests were provided.
The measurements of the task were arithmetic perfor-
mance time (s) and accuracy (%) for each arithmetic opera-
tion in addition to the number copying time (s) per list of
five numbers.
Analysis
The object collision measurements were analysed by means
of 2 × 2 mixed-design ANOVAs (Handedness Group;left-
vs. right-handers and Distractor Presence; with vs. without
distractor). Secondary analyses assessing the start posi-
tion of the objects or their speed did not show any sig-
nificant effects, p > 0.05. The arithmetic measurements
were analysed by means of 2 × 3 mixed-design ANOVAs
(Handedness Group; left- vs. right-handers and Arithmetic
Operation; additions vs. subtractions vs. multiplications).
Frequency analyses were conducted by means of chi-square
tests. The number copying measurement and the personality
questionnaire scores were analysed by means of independ-
ent t tests on Handedness Group. A simple linear regression
analysis was conducted to assess whether space–time detec-
tion of object collision predicted mathematical achievement.
Initial checks showed that the Durbin–Watson test indicated
no concern for autocorrelation, with data homoscedasticity
and a normal probability plot of the residuals. Mean ± SE
is reported. Bonferroni correction was made for multiple
comparisons, where appropriate.
Results
Handedness questionnaire
The laterality index obtained from the handedness question-
naire was used to classify the participants, resulting into 19
left-handers (LI = 19 ± 3%, age = 23 ± 1y, 18 females) and
18 right-handers (LI = 93 ± 2%, age = 19 ± 1y, 11 females).
Personality questionnaire
The total RST-PQ scores showed no significant difference
between left-handers (M = 141.5 ± 3.7) and right-handers
(M = 138.1 ± 3.2), p > 0.05. Additional analyses for the
subtests revealed no significant differences between left-
and right-handers for BIS (M = 60.1 ± 2.6 and 57.2 ± 2.4),
BAS-RI (M = 17.4 ± 1.0 and 17.4 ± 0.8), BAS-GDR
(M = 19.5 ± 0.6 and 17.9 ± 0.7), BAS-IMP (M = 16.9 ± 1.0
and 18.6 ± 1.3), BAS-RR (M = 27.5 ± 1.3 and 26.9 ± 1.0),
all p > 0.05. In previous work, differences in negative affect
have been associated with handedness (Hardie & Wright,
2014). However, we observed no indication of a significant
shift in our sample, as shown in Fig.2 which illustrates the
participants’ BIS scores alongside their laterality index.
Fig. 2 Scatter plot of the BIS scores obtained from the personality
questionnaire (RST-PQ) as a function of the participants’ laterality
index from the handedness questionnaire. The laterality index varied
between 0 (extreme left-handedness) and 100 (extreme right-handed-
ness). The middle line exemplifies a score of 50 (ambidextrous)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
368 Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
Object collision task
For collision detection time, the ANOVA analysis demon-
strated a significant main effect of Distractor Presence, F(1,
35) = 28.1, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.44, indicating that decision time
was longer in the presence of distractors (M = 1159 ± 42ms)
than without distractors (M = 1096 ± 36ms). No other effects
were significant, p > 0.05.
For collision detection accuracy, the ANOVA analysis
pointed to a significant main effect of Handedness Group,
F(1, 35) = 13.5, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.28, showing that left-hand-
ers (M = 69 ± 2%) detected object collisions more accurately
than right-handers (M = 60 ± 2%). No other effects were sig-
nificant, p > 0.05. Figure3 provides details about the colli-
sion detection accuracy of the participants alongside their
laterality index, and shows that left-handers were more accu-
rate performers than right-handers.
To portray more in detail the collision task for both hand-
edness groups, Fig.4 presents a categorisation of frequency
counts of the participants according to three performance
intervals: superior (i.e., fast/accurate performers), interme-
diate (in-between performers), and inferior (slow/inaccurate
performers). For the collision detection time (left-sided pan-
els), the data revealed that there were no significant group
differences for the performance intervals, p > 0.05. Of note
is that distractor presence did not impact the performance
intervals (p > 0.05).
For collision detection accuracy (right-sided panels), the
data indicated that both groups performed distinctively for
the inferior and intermediate intervals, χ21,N=37 = 11.45,
p < 0.001; and χ21, N=37 = 14.58, p < 0.0001. There was no
difference for the superior interval, p > 0.05. Performing
with or without distractor did not affect any of the intervals
(p > 0.05).
The balanced integration score revealed a significant
main effect of Handedness Group, F(1, 35) = 4.2, p < 0.05,
ηp2 = 0.11 with left-handers (0.28 ± 0.16) being more suc-
cessful than right-handers (− 0.33 ± 0.28). There was
also a significant main effect of Distractor Presence. F(1,
35) = 5.8, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.14, indicating a stronger perfor-
mance without distractor (0.12 ± 0.17) than with distractor
(− 0.16 ± 0.16).
Arithmetic tests
For arithmetic performance time, the ANOVA analysis
revealed a significant main effect of Arithmetic Opera-
tion, F(2, 70) = 19.9 p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.36, demonstrating
that additions (M = 80 ± 5s) were performed fastest fol-
lowed by subtractions (M = 117 ± 9s) and multiplications
(M = 151 ± 15s). Post hoc comparisons demonstrated that
all tests differed from one another, p < 0.01.
For arithmetic performance accuracy, the ANOVA
analysis showed a significant main effect of Arithmetic
Operation, F(2, 70) = 19.3, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.35, revealing
that additions (M = 95 ± 1%) obtained highest accuracy
Fig. 3 Scatter plot of the collision detection accuracy scores as a
function of the participants’ laterality index from the handedness
questionnaire. The accuracy scores represent the combined collision
conditions. The laterality index varied between 0 (extreme left-hand-
edness) and 100 (extreme right-handedness). The middle line exem-
plifies a score of 50 (ambidextrous)
Fig. 4 Categorisation of the collision detection times (left side) and
accuracy scores (right side) for the left- and right-handers. The col-
our coding across the measurements indicates the performance level:
superior (white), intermediate (grey) and inferior (black)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
369Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
followed by subtractions (M = 90 ± 1%) and multiplications
(M = 85 ± 2%). Post hoc comparisons demonstrated that all
tests differed from one another, p < 0.01.
To illustrate the arithmetic tests in more detail, Fig.5 pre-
sents a frequency count for the arithmetic operations accord-
ing to three performance intervals: superior (i.e., fast/accu-
rate performers), intermediate (in-between performers), and
inferior (slow/inaccurate performers). Across the measure-
ments of performance time (left-sided panels) and accuracy
(right-sided panels), the data illustrate that ± 30% of the par-
ticipants performed in the intermediate range whereas ± 60%
corresponded to fast/accurate performers and ± 10% were
slow/inaccurate performers.
For the number copying task, the analysis demon-
strated no significant difference between left-handers
(M = 7.7 ± 0.3 s) and right-handers (M = 7.5 ± 0.3 s),
p > 0.05.
Object collision task andarithmetic tests: regression
analysis
Regression analysis was conducted to determine how
handedness affected behaviour at the individual level
across both handedness groups. In assessing both tasks, we
observed that collision detection accuracy and arithmetic
accuracy showed a positive association for subtractions
(top panel, Fig.6) and multiplications (lower panel, Fig.6)
as a function of handedness. The regression analyses for
right-handers revealed significant outputs for subtrac-
tions, F(1, 35) = 6.2, p < 0.03, with β = 0.53 and R2 = 0.28,
suggesting that 28% of the variance can be explained by
the model (adjusted R2 = 0.24) and for multiplications,
F(1, 35) = 5.42, p < 0.05, with β = 0.50 and R2 = 0.25,
Fig. 5 Categorisation of the arithmetic performance times (left side)
and accuracy scores (right side) for additions, subtractions and mul-
tiplications. The colour coding across the measurements indicates the
performance level: superior (white), intermediate (grey) and inferior
(black)
Fig. 6 Scatter plot of the collision detection accuracy and arithme-
tic accuracy scores, illustrating a positive association. The accuracy
scores represent the collision conditions (with and without distractor)
and the arithmetic conditions of subtractions (top panel) and multipli-
cations (lower panel)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
370 Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
suggesting that 25% of the variance can be explained by
the model (adjusted R2 = 0.21). For left-handers, no sig-
nificant outputs were observed, p > 0.05. In addition, no
effects were observed for the collision detection time and
arithmetic accuracies, p > 0.05.
Discussion
Making predictions is an innate capability of the human
brain. In particular, the brain makes sense of the environ-
ment by predicting future events and by testing whether
these are in line with incoming sensory information and
previous experiences (Clark, 2013; Schubotz, 2007). To
form these predictions, the dimensions of space, time and
number are elementary. That is, a representation can be
created by knowing where, when and how many, enabling
us to respond to and learn about environmental regulari-
ties (Burr, etal. 2010; Lourenco & Longo, 2011; Winter,
etal. 2015).
Predictive behaviour, such as estimating collisions,
is crucial to our everyday activities, such as anticipat-
ing the course of moving objects and (de)synchronising
our actions with them (Enns & Lleras, 2008; Senot, etal.
2003). We tested this real-world scenario by asking partic-
ipants to detect whether collisions between moving objects
would occur or not; decisions that are made on the basis
of the use of spatial and temporal information from the
motion trajectories, guided by attention to space and time.
In addition, the participants completed arithmetic tests of
addition, subtraction and multiplication, which are com-
mon in daily life such as required for counting and formal
mathematics. In this study, we examine interdependencies
between space–time–number by assessing both tasks in a
group of left- and right-handers due to their distinct sen-
sorimotor experiences.
Handedness group profiles: space–time andnumber
processing
Handedness is a manifestation of brain lateralisation that
provides a representational index of the hands and that
captures preference for manual activities (Corballis &
Häberling, 2017). Our results from the collision detection
task revealed that both handedness groups showed distinct
behavioural performances, with left-handers being more
accurate than right-handers; a performance advantage that
could be due to a greater range of sensorimotor experiences
and space–time integration pathways between both hemi-
spheres (Assmus, etal. 2003; Cherbuin & Brinkman, 2006;
Serrien, etal. 2012). In this context, hand use and sensori-
motor competence are bi-directionally linked, shaping the
information processing and associations between sensori-
motor and attentional systems (Buckingham, etal. 2011; Le
Bigot & Grosjean, 2012). Moreover, handedness affects the
representation of extra-personal and peripersonal space with
left-handers showing bilateral hemispheric activity whereas
right-handers demonstrate an asymmetry of both hemi-
spheres (Colman, etal. 2017; O’Regan & Serrien, 2018).
Further differences between left- and right-handers have
been proposed with distinct neglect-like patterns as a result
of alertness-related modulations. In particular, Bareham
etal. (2015) observed that left-handers experienced a left-
ward hemispheric shift in attention with drowsiness whereas
right-handers have the opposite pattern, a distinction that
could be due to differences in the attentional mechanisms
that control alertness and direct attention to external stimuli
(Liu, etal. 2009). In this study, we modified the attentional
demands of the object collision task by means of distractors.
We observed that their presence slowed the detection time
across all participants, suggesting that difficult decisions
take more time than easier ones and engage more neural
circuitry for optimising behaviour (Assmus, etal. 2005;
Smout, etal. 2019; Spapé & Serrien, 2011).
To take into account differences due to personality traits,
we included the RST-PQ (Corr & Cooper, 2016); a question-
naire that associates personality with distinct brain systems
labelled as BIS that predicts an individual’s response to
anxiety and performance avoidance as opposed to BAS that
supports motivation and desired outcomes. Previous work
has shown that left-handers have higher BIS scores than
right-handers, which has been coupled with different levels
of negative affect (Beaton, etal. 2017; Hardie & Wright,
2014). However, we observed no significant differences
between left- and right-handers for any of the subtests, sug-
gesting that there were no distinct variations in personality
traits in our sample.
We noted no clear pattern of arithmetic performance dif-
ferences between both handedness groups; a topic that has
provided mixed claims throughout the literature (Annett
& Kilshaw, 1982; Cheyne, etal. 2010; Crow, etal. 1998).
While some studies have shown that left-handers are strong
in mathematics and consistent right-handers perform least,
others have suggested that mixed-handers are more disad-
vantaged. In a more recent study, Sala, etal. (2017) con-
cluded that the relationship between handedness group pro-
files and mathematical ability is complex and depends on
several factors, such as age, gender and type of task. This
conclusion is in line with evidence that changes in math-
ematical processing occur as a function of development, and
that arithmetic achievement depends on domain-general as
well as domain-specific knowledge (Arsalidou & Taylor,
2011).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
371Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
Object collisions, arithmetic calculations
andindividual differences
Theories that account for functional overlap of
space–time–number processing suggest the use of shared
resources, common formats or a reliance on cross-dimen-
sional mappings (Fias etal., 2003; Lakoff & Johnson, 1980;
Walsh, 2003). In this respect, parietal circuits take a central
role for the processing of space, time and number (Bjoer-
tomt, etal. 2002; Coull & Nobre, 1998; Eger etal., 2009) as
well as for the interactions between space–time (Magnani,
etal. 2010; Oliveri, etal. 2009), space–number (Hubbard,
etal. 2005; Oliveri, etal. 2004), and time-number (Burr,
etal. 2010; Hayashi, etal. 2013). Of note is, however, that
each hemisphere responds to specific task characteristics.
In particular, for space and time processing, there is greater
sensitivity of right inferior parietal circuitry for orienting
in space (O’Reilly, etal. 2008) versus left inferior parietal
circuitry for cueing in time and for space–time integration
(Assmus, etal. 2003; Coull & Nobre, 2008). For num-
ber processing and numerical operations, inferior parietal
regions are usually activated across both hemispheres, with
specificity according to the type and complexity of the prob-
lem (Arsalidou & Taylor, 2011). However, there is shared
circuitry across basic numerical and arithmetic tasks that is
located within the left hemisphere, i.e., intraparietal sulcus
alongside precentral areas. Therefore, a left fronto-parietal
circuit can be considered a core network of numerical knowl-
edge in adults (Pesenti, etal. 2000; Simon, etal. 2004; Zago,
etal. 2001). The relevance of this network is that it overlaps
with sensorimotor circuitry that is recruited for predictive
control related to own actions and external perceptual events
(Coull, etal. 2008; O’Reilly, etal. 2008; Schubotz, 2007).
Moreover, this type of prediction arises when the essential
information concerns dynamic forward change with coding
of transitions in space–time, and underscores a key role of
the sensorimotor system for the prediction of future states
within the adopted reference frame, be it the body or the
environment (Schubotz, 2007).
The regression analysis revealed a positive relationship
between the detection accuracy of object collisions and the
performance of arithmetic calculations, albeit as a function
of handedness. That is, an association was observed only in
right-handers, suggesting a connection between manual later-
alisation and arithmetic. Support for such a relationship comes
from finger counting, which represents a natural routine that
supports the acquisition of basic numerical and arithmetic
principles (Butterworth, 1999). In Western cultures, counting
involves a preferred starting-hand alongside a relative order
of finger counting within a single hand. Thus, finger count-
ing strategies that are shaped by sensorimotor experience and
developed during childhood may influence and steer how
numbers are represented and processed later in life (Fischer,
2008; Pesenti, etal., 2000). In adults, these hand-starting
preferences have been observed to be different for left- and
right-handers (Zago & Badets, 2016). That is, consistent left-
handers typically started counting with their left hand whereas
the opposite pattern was noted for consistent right-handers;
a manual preference that aligned with their dominant hand
for unimanual activities. Furthermore, an fMRI study demon-
strated that left-starters showed higher activation in the right
right-sided motor and premotor cortices when they perceived
small numbers whereas right-starters showed the reverse pat-
tern (Tschentscher, etal. 2012). Thus, handedness modulates
the structural arrangement of finger counting routines and
further influences the involvement of the motor-dominant
hemisphere for number processing (Artemenko, etal. 2020).
This reliance on effector-specific circuitry in left- and right-
handers has also been observed for skilled movements, such as
grasping (Martin, etal. 2011). Together, the findings suggest
that hemispheric lateralisation of key brain regions distinc-
tively guides the covariation of functions that cross cognitive
domains.
Besides handedness, we also noted that the type of arith-
metic operation played a role in the relationship between colli-
sion detection and calculation performances. In particular, the
results showed that stronger space–time computations resulted
in increased performances for subtraction and multiplication
calculations. No effect was observed for additions, which
could be due to the fewer demands on number processing as
compared to the other tasks which likely required additional
processing steps (Fehr, etal. 2007). In using basic arithmetic
operations (addition, subtraction, multiplication), we observed
increasingly longer response times and lower accuracy rates,
confirming changes in complexity requirements. In our study,
the results did not show that the collision detection time sig-
nificantly linked with arithmetic performance, which is in line
with research that has shown that people alter where rather
than when they would hit targets, if given the choice (Brenner,
etal. 2015).
In conclusion, space, time and number are key dimen-
sions that underlie how we perceive, identify and act within
the environment. In this study, we examined interdependen-
cies between these dimensions using an object collision task
that required space–time processing and arithmetic tests that
involved number processing in left- and right-handers. Hand-
edness of the participants influenced collision detection with
left-handers being more accurate than right-handers, which
is in line with the premise that hand preference guides indi-
vidual differences as a result of sensorimotor experiences and
distinct interhemispheric integration patterns. The data further
showed that successful collision detection was a predictor for
arithmetic achievement, at least in right-handers. These find-
ings suggest that handedness plays a mediating role in binding
information processing across domains, likely due to selective
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
372 Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
connectivity properties within the sensorimotor system that are
guided by hemispheric lateralisation patterns.
Acknowledgements The work was supported by a research grant from
the BIAL foundation to DJS (no. 376/14). We thank Louise O’Regan
for assistance in the project.
Data availability The data have been stored in the Open Science Frame-
work data repository (https:// osf. io/ rq8nu/).
Declaration
Conflict of interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of
interest.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attri-
bution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adapta-
tion, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long
as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source,
provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes
were made. The images or other third party material in this article are
included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated
otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in
the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will
need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a
copy of this licence, visit http:// creat iveco mmons. org/ licen ses/ by/4. 0/.
References
Annett, M., & Kilshaw, D. (1982). Mathematical ability and lateral
asymmetry. Cortex, 18, 547–568. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/ S0010-
9452(82) 80053-1
Arsalidou, M., & Taylor, M. J. (2011). Is 2+2=4? Meta-analyses of
brain areas needed for numbers and calculations. NeuroImage,
54, 2382–2393. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro image. 2010. 10. 009
Artemenko, C., Sitnikova, M. A., Soltanlou, M., Dresler, T., & Nuerk,
H. C. (2020). Functional lateralization of arithmetic processing in
the intraparietal sulcus is associated with handedness. Scientific
Reports, 4, 1775. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1038/ s41598- 020- 58477-7
Assmus, A., Marshall, J. C., Noth, J., Zilles, K., & Fink, G. R. (2005).
Difficulty of perceptual spatiotemporal integration modulates the
neural activity of left inferior parietal cortex. Neuroscience, 132,
923–927. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro scien ce. 2005. 01. 047
Assmuss, A., Marshall, J. C., Ritzi, A., Noth, J., Zilles, K., & Fink, G.
R. (2003). Left inferior parietal cortex integrates time and space
during collision judgments. NeuroImage, 20, S82–S88. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro image. 2003. 09. 025
Bareham, C. A., Bekinschtein, T. A., Scott, S. K., & Manly, T. (2015).
Does left-handedness confer resistance to spatial bias? Scientific
Reports, 5, 9162. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1038/ srep0 9162
Beaton, A. A., Mutinelli, S., & Corr, P. J. (2017). Fractionating nega-
tive and positive affectivity in handedness: insights from the
reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality. Laterality, 22,
419–444. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 13576 50X. 2016. 12132 74
Bjoertomt, O., Cowey, A., & Walsh, V. (2002). Spatial neglect in near
and far space investigated by repetitive transcranial magnetic
stimulation. Brain, 125, 2012–2022. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/
brain/ awf211
Bonato, M., Zorzi, M., & Umilta, C. (2012). When time is space:
evidence for a mental time line. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral
Reviews, 36, 2257–2273. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neubi orev.
2012. 08. 007
Bonn, C. D., & Cantlon, J. F. (2012). The origins and structure of
quantitative concepts. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 29, 149–173.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 02643 294. 2012. 707122
Brenner, E., & Smeets, J. B. (2015). Quickly making the correct choice.
Vision Research, 113, 198–210. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. visres.
2015. 03. 028
Buckingham, G., Main, J. C., & Carey, D. P. (2011). Asymmetries in
motor attention during a cued bimanual reaching task: Left and
right handers compared. Cortex, 47, 432–440. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1016/j. cortex. 2009. 11. 003
Bueti, D., & Walsh, V. (2009). The parietal cortex and the representa-
tion of time, space, number and other magnitudes. Philosophi-
cal Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 12,
1831–1840. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1098/ rstb. 2009. 0028
Burr, D. C., Ross, J., Binda, P., & Morrone, C. (2010). Saccades com-
press space, time and number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14,
528–533. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. tics. 2010. 09. 005
Butterworth, B. (1999). The mathematical brain. Macmillan.
Cherbuin, N., & Brinkman, C. (2006). Efficiency of callosal trans-
fer and hemispheric interaction. Neuropsychology, 20, 178–184.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ 0894- 4105. 20.6. 700
Cheyne, C. P., Roberts, N., Crow, T. J., Leask, S. J., & Garcia-Finana,
M. (2010). The effect of handedness on academic ability: a mul-
tivariate linear mixed model approach. Laterality, 15, 451–464.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 13576 50090 29769 56
Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents,
and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral Brain Sciences, 36,
181–204. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1017/ S0140 525X1 20004 77
Colman, H. A., Remington, R. W., & Kritikos, A. (2017). Handedness
and graspability modify shifts of visuospatial attention to near-
hand objects. PLoS ONE, 12, e0170542. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/
journ al. pone. 01705 42
Corballis, M. C., & Häberling, I. S. (2017). The many sides of hemi-
spheric asymmetry: a selective review and outlook. Journal of the
International Neuropsychological Society, 23, 710–718. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1017/ S1355 61771 70003 76
Corr, P. J., & Cooper, A. J. (2016). The reinforcement sensitivity theory
of personality questionnaire (RST-PQ): development and valida-
tion. Psychological Assessment, 28, 1427–1440. https:// doi. org/
10. 1037/ pas00 00273
Coull, J. T., & Nobre, A. C. (1998). Where and when to pay attention:
the neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and
to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI. Journal of
Neuroscience, 18, 7426–7435. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1523/ JNEUR
OSCI. 18- 18- 07426. 1998
Coull, J. T., & Nobre, A. C. (2008). Dissociating explicit timing from
temporal expectation with fMRI. Current Opinion in Neurobi-
ology, 18, 137–144. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. conb. 2008. 07. 011
Coull, J.T., Vidal, F., Goulon, C., Nazarian, B., & Craig, C. (2008).
Using time-to-contact information to assess potential collision
modulates both visual and temporal prediction networks. Frontiers
in Human Neuroscience, 2.
Crow, T. J., Crow, L. R., Done, D. J., & Leask, S. (1998). Relative hand
skill predicts academic ability: global deficits at the point of hemi-
spheric indecision. Neuropsychologia, 36, 1275–1282. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1016/ S0028- 3932(98) 00039-6
de Hevia, M. D., Izard, V., Coubart, A., Spelke, E. S., & Streri, A.
(2014). Representations of space, time, and number in neonates.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America, 111, 4809–4813. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1073/ pnas.
13236 28111
Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representa-
tion of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
373Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
Psychology General, 122, 371–396. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ 0096-
3445. 122.3. 371
Dehaene, S., & Brannon, E. M. (2001). Space, time, and number: a
Kantian research program. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14,
517–519. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. tics. 2010. 09. 009
Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Cohen, L. (2003). Three parietal
circuits for number processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20,
487–506. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 02643 29024 40002 39
Eger, E., Michel, V., Thirion, B., Amadon, A., Dehaene, S., & Klein-
schmidt, A. (2009). Deciphering cortical number coding from
human brain activity patterns. Current Biology, 19, 1608–1615.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. cub. 2009. 08. 047
Enns, J. T., & Lleras, A. (2008). What’s next? New evidence for predic-
tion in human vision. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 327–333.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. tics. 2008. 06. 001
Fabbri, M., Cellini, N., Martoni, M., Tonetti, L., & Natale, V. (2013).
Perceptual and motor congruency effects in time-space associa-
tion. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1840–1851.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13414- 013- 0519-9
Fehr, T., Code, C., & Herrmann, M. (2007). Common brain regions
underlying different arithmetic operations as revealed by conjunct
fMRI–BOLD activation. Brain Research, 1172, 93–102. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. brain res. 2007. 07. 043
Fias, W., Lammertyn, J., Reynvoet, B., Dupont, P., & Orban, G. A.
(2003). Parietal representation of symbolic and nonsymbolic mag-
nitude. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 47–56. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1162/ 08989 29033 21107 819
Fischer, M. H. (2008). Finger counting habits modulate spatial-numer-
ical associations. Cortex, 44, 386–392. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j.
cortex. 2007. 08. 004
Friedrich, R., & Friederici, A. D. (2009). Mathematical logic in the
human brain: syntax. PLoS ONE, 4, e5599. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1371/ journ al. pone. 00055 99
Hardie, S.M., & Wright, L. (2014). Differences between left- and right-
handers in approach/avoidance motivation: influence of consist-
ency of handedness measures. Frontiers in Psychology, 134. doi:
https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fpsyg. 2014. 00134
Hayashi, M. J., Kanai, R., Hiroki, C., Tanabe, H. C., Yoshida, Y., Carl-
son, S., Walsh, V., & Sadato, N. (2013). Interaction of numerosity
and time in prefrontal and parietal cortex. Journal of Neurosci-
ence, 33, 883–893. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1523/ JNEUR OSCI. 6257- 11.
2013
Hécaen, H., & Sauguet, J. (1971). Cerebral dominance in left-handed
subjects. Cortex, 7, 19–48. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/ S0010-
9452(71) 80020-5
Hubbard, E. M., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Interac-
tions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 6, 435–448. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1038/ nrn16 84
Klöppel, S., van Eimeren, T., Glauche, V., Vongerichten, A., Münchau,
A., Frackowiak, R. S., etal. (2007). The effect of handedness
on cortical motor activation during simple bilateral movements.
NeuroImage, 34, 274–280. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro image.
2006. 08. 038
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. University
of Chicago Press.
Le Bigot, N., & Grosjean, M. (2012). Effects of handedness on visual
sensitivity in perihand space. PLoS ONE, 7, e43150. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1371/ journ al. pone. 00431 50
Liesefeld, H. R., & Janczyk, M. (2019). Combining speed and accu-
racy to control for speed-accuracy trade-offs. Behavioral Research
Methods, 51, 40–60. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 018- 1076-x
Liu, H., Stufflebeam, S. M., Sepulcre, J., Hedden, T., & Buckner, R.
L. (2009). Evidence from intrinsic activity that asymmetry of the
human brain is controlled by multiple factors. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
106, 20499–20503. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1073/ pnas. 09080 73106
Lourenco, S.F., & Longo, M.R. (2011). Origins and development of
generalized magnitude representation.In S. Dehaene & E. Bran-
non (Eds.),Space, time and number in the brain: Searching for
the foundations of mathematical thought (p. 225–244). Elsevier
Academic Press. doi: https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/ B978-0- 12- 385948-
8. 00015-3.
Magnani, B., Oliveri, M., Mangano, G. R., & Frassinetti, F. (2010).
The role of posterior parietal cortex in spatial representation of
time: a TMS study. Behavioral Neurology, 23, 213–215. https://
doi. org/ 10. 3233/ BEN- 2010- 0298
Martin, K., Jacobs, S., & Frey, S. H. (2011). Handedness-dependent
and -independent cerebral asymmetries in the anterior intrapari-
etal sulcus and ventral premotor cortex during grasp planning.
NeuroImage, 57, 502–512. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro image.
2011. 04. 036
O’Regan, L., & Serrien, D. J. (2018). Individual differences and hemi-
spheric asymmetries for language and spatial attention. Frontiers
in Human Neuroscience, 12, 80. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fnhum.
2018. 00380
Oliveri, M., Bonnì, S., Turriziani, P., Koch, G., Lo Gerfo, E., Torriero,
S., Vicario, C. M., Petrosini, L., & Caltagirone, C. (2009). Motor
and linguistic linking of space and time in the cerebellum. PLoS
ONE, 4, e7933. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/ journ al. pone. 00079 33
O’Regan, L., Spapé, M. M., & Serrien, D. J. (2017). Motor timing and
covariation with time perception: investigating the role of handed-
ness. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 147. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 3389/ fnbeh. 2017. 00147
O’Reilly, J. X., Mesulam, M. M., & Nobre, A. C. (2008). The cerebel-
lum predicts the timing of perceptual events. Journal of Neuro-
science, 28, 2252–2260. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1523/ JNEUR OSCI.
2742- 07. 2008
Park, J., Park, D. C., & Polk, T. A. (2013). Parietal functional connec-
tivity in numerical cognition. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 2127–2135.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/ cercor/ bhs193
Perelle, I. B., & Ehrman, L. (2005). On the other hand. Behavior Genet-
ics, 35, 343–350. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s10519- 005- 3226-z
Pesenti, M., Thioux, X., Seron, A., & De Volder, A. (2000). Neuro-
anatomical substrates of Arabic number processing, numerical
comparison and simple addition: a PET study. Journal of Cogni-
tive Neuroscience, 12, 461–479. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1162/ 08989
29005 62273
Pool, E. M., Rehme, A. K., Fink, G. R., Eickhoff, S. B., & Grefkes,
C. (2014). Handedness and effective connectivity of the motor
system. NeuroImage, 99, 451–460. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro
image. 2014. 05. 048
Proffitt, D. R., & Gilden, D. L. (1989). Understanding natural dynam-
ics. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 15, 384–393. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1037/ 0096- 1523. 15.2. 384
Reid, C. S., & Serrien, D. J. (2014). Primary motor cortex and ipsilat-
eral control: a TMS study. Neuroscience, 270, 20–26. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1016/j. bbr. 2012. 02. 008
Sala, G., Signorelli, M., Barsuola, G., Bolognese, M., & Gobet, F.
(2017). The relationship between handedness and mathematics
is non-linear and is moderated by gender, age, and type of task.
Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 948. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fpsyg.
2017. 00948
Schubotz, R. I. (2007). Prediction of external events with our motor
system: towards a new framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
11, 211–218. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. tics. 2007. 02. 006
Senot, P., Prévost, P., & McIntyre, J. (2003). Estimating time to contact
and impact velocity when catching an accelerating object with the
hand. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 219–237. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1037/ 0096- 1523. 29.1. 219
Serrien, D. J., Sovijärvi-Spapé, M. M., & Farnsworth, B. (2012).
Bimanual control processes and the role of handedness. Neuropsy-
chology, 26, 802–807. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ a0030 154
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
374 Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
Simon, O., Kherif, F., Flandin, G., Poline, J.-B., Rivière, D., Mangin,
J.-F., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Automatized cluster-
ing and functional geometry of human parietofrontal networks
for language, space, and number. NeuroImage, 23, 1192–1202.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro image. 2004. 09. 023
Smout, C. A., Tang, M. F., Garrido, M. I., & Mattingley, J. B. (2019).
Attention promotes the neural encoding of prediction errors.
PLoS Biology, 17, e2006812. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/ journ al.
pbio. 20068 12
Spapé, M. M., & Serrien, D. J. (2011). Prediction of collision events:
an EEG coherence analysis. Clinical Neurophysiology, 122, 891–
896. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. clinph. 2011. 01. 047
Stanescu-Cosson, R., Pinel, P., van de Moortele, P. F., Le Bihan, D.,
Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2000). Understanding dissociations in
dyscalculia: a brain imaging study of the impact of number size
on the cerebral networks for exact and approximate calculation.
Brain, 123, 2240–2255. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/ br ain/ 123. 11. 2240
Sutton, S. K., & Davidson, R. J. (1997). Prefrontal brain asymmetry:
A biological substrate of the behavioral approach and inhibition
systems. Psychological Science, 8, 204–210. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1111/j. 1467- 9280. 1997. tb004 13.x
Tschentscher, N., Hauk, O., Fischer, M. H., & Pulvermüller, F. (2012).
You can count on the motor cortex: Finger counting habits modu-
late motor cortex activation evoked by numbers. NeuroImage, 59,
3139–3148. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro image. 2011. 11. 037
Vandierendonck, A. (2017). A comparison of methods to combine
speed and accuracy measures of performance: A rejoinder on the
binning procedure. Behavior Research Methods, 49, 653–673.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 016- 0721-5
Vogel, J. J., Bowers, C. A., & Vogel, D. S. (2003). Cerebral lateraliza-
tion of spatial abilities: A meta-analysis. Brain and Cognition, 52,
197–204. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/ S0278- 2626(03) 00056-3
Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of
time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 483–
488. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. tics. 2003. 09. 002
Winter, B., Marghetis, T., & Matlock, T. (2015). Of magnitudes and
metaphors: explaining cognitive interactions between space, time,
and number. Cortex, 64, 209–224. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. cor-
tex. 2014. 10. 015
Zago, L., & Badets, A. (2016). What is the role of manual prefer-
ence in hand-digit mapping during finger counting? A study in a
large sample of right- and left-handers. Perception, 45, 125–135.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1177/ 03010 06615 602628
Zago, L., Pesenti, M., Mellet, E., Crivello, F., Mazoyer, B., & Tzou-
rio-Mazoyer, N. (2001). Neural correlates of simple and complex
mental calculation. NeuroImage, 13, 314–327. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1006/ nimg. 2000. 0697
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com