ArticlePDF Available

Space, time and number: common coding mechanisms and interactions between domains

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Space, time and number are key dimensions that underlie how we perceive, identify and act within the environment. They are interconnected in our behaviour and brain. In this study, we examined interdependencies between these dimensions. To this end, left- and right-handed participants performed an object collision task that required space–time processing and arithmetic tests that involved number processing. Handedness of the participants influenced collision detection with left-handers being more accurate than right-handers, which is in line with the premise that hand preference guides individual differences as a result of sensorimotor experiences and distinct interhemispheric integration patterns. The data further showed that successful collision detection was a predictor for arithmetic achievement, at least in right-handers. These findings suggest that handedness plays a mediating role in binding information processing across domains, likely due to selective connectivity properties within the sensorimotor system that is guided by hemispheric lateralisation patterns.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Vol:.(1234567890)
Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01503-8
1 3
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Space, time andnumber: common coding mechanisms
andinteractions betweendomains
DeborahJ.Serrien1 · MichielM.Spapé2
Received: 22 August 2020 / Accepted: 5 March 2021 / Published online: 23 March 2021
© The Author(s) 2021
Abstract
Space, time and number are key dimensions that underlie how we perceive, identify and act within the environment. They
are interconnected in our behaviour and brain. In this study, we examined interdependencies between these dimensions. To
this end, left- and right-handed participants performed an object collision task that required space–time processing and arith-
metic tests that involved number processing. Handedness of the participants influenced collision detection with left-handers
being more accurate than right-handers, which is in line with the premise that hand preference guides individual differences
as a result of sensorimotor experiences and distinct interhemispheric integration patterns. The data further showed that suc-
cessful collision detection was a predictor for arithmetic achievement, at least in right-handers. These findings suggest that
handedness plays a mediating role in binding information processing across domains, likely due to selective connectivity
properties within the sensorimotor system that is guided by hemispheric lateralisation patterns.
Introduction
In everyday life, we often interact with moving objects, such
as catching a ball or crossing a road. Crucial to these sen-
sorimotor activities is the ability to predict the trajectory of
the moving objects and the changes of their position over
time (Enns & Lleras, 2008; Senot, etal. 2003). To imple-
ment these predictions, the brain uses a range of quantitative
inputs, such as spatial, temporal and numerical information.
Moreover, space–time–number represent essential dimen-
sions that can be encoded through all sensory modalities
(Burr, etal. 2010). These dimensions further demonstrate
associations, such as the SNARC effect, that captures num-
ber–space interactions with faster responses occurring to
smaller/larger numbers on the left/right side of space due to
a representation of increasing numerical value from left to
right (Dehaene, etal. 1993).
To account for these interdependencies, Walsh (2003)
argued for a magnitude system that involves processing of
dimensional magnitudes and their interactions (Bonato,
etal. 2012; Burr, etal. 2010; Dehaene & Brannon, 2001;
Fabbri, etal. 2013; Hayashi, etal. 2013). Furthermore, the
proposed ATOM model (A Theory of Magnitude) underlines
that actions are instrumental in establishing the magnitude
system, with parietal circuitry providing a neural platform
to exchange information (Walsh, 2003). That is, it is through
actions that associations between magnitudes are learned for
example that larger objects tend to be heavier than smaller
ones. Thus, the magnitude system ties interactions between
dimensions such that ‘more’ in a dimension couples with
‘more’ in another dimension. The origin of these interactions
is that they reflect innate mappings or developmental pro-
cesses, although both types of mechanisms could influence
one another with innate pathways being influenced by early
experiences and learned processes by innate constraints (De
Hevia, etal. 2014; Stanescu-Cosson, etal. 2000; Walsh,
2003). Besides innate and developmental systems, atten-
tional processes also play an important role. For example,
attention can be directed towards specific task features, such
as a location in space or a moment in time, which accord-
ingly supports behavioural performance (Coull & Nobre,
1998; Dehaene, etal. 2003).
According to current viewpoints, a dimension could
emerge from another, resulting in functional similarities and
dependencies in computational and neural mechanisms. One
particular hypothesis is that space serves as a foundation for
the dimensions that are conceptually more abstract, such as
* Deborah J. Serrien
deborah.serrien@nottingham.ac.uk
1 School ofPsychology, University ofNottingham,
Nottingham, UK
2 Department ofPsychology andLogopedics, University
ofHelsinki, Helsinki, Finland
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
365Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
time and number (Bonn & Cantlon, 2012). Thus, magnitude
representations with a common code could evolve due to
the substrates that already exist for the space dimension. Of
note, however, is that the existence of dimensional interac-
tions does not imply that these are equal in strength. For
example, there is evidence that space–time interactions are
strongest due to the specialisation of the magnitude system
for sensorimotor actions, followed by space–number and
time–number interactions (Bueti & Walsh, 2009).
Taking into account that the magnitude system is estab-
lished through actions, the question emerges about the effect
of sensorimotor experiences. That is, it can be argued that
magnitude representations would differ if individuals inter-
act in different ways with the environment due to inherent
biases. Handedness is such a characteristic that captures
asymmetry of movement control and expresses how indi-
viduals use their hands during manual activities. Research
has shown that handedness not only affects the sensorimo-
tor control mechanisms (Klöppel, etal. 2007; Pool, etal.
2014; Reid & Serrien, 2014; Serrien, etal. 2012) but also
influences how individuals attend to and respond to the envi-
ronment. In other words, handedness influences a range of
abilities that involves visuospatial functions (Bareham, etal.
2015; Hécaen & Sauguet, 1971; O’Regan & Serrien, 2018;
Vogel, etal. 2003), attentional regulation in space and time
(Buckingham etal., 2009; O’Regan, etal. 2017) and visual
processing in perihand space (Le Bigot & Grosjean, 2012).
Combined, these findings illustrate that handedness has a
widespread impact on the processing requirements of space
and time for meeting behavioural goals.
The aim of the present experiment is to examine how
space–time processing naturally connects with number pro-
cessing, based on the proposed interdependencies between
the dimensions of space–time–number. In this respect, a
valuable experimental approach is to study a functional
effect at the level of the behavioural outputs. First, we use
an object collision task that requires participants to predict
whether moving objects will collide with one another or
not at a specific moment in time. To be successful, an accu-
rate estimation of the moving objects over time is required
(O’Reilly, etal. 2008; Proffitt & Gilden, 1989). It involves
information about the path of the objects in space which is
strongly linked with spatial coordinates, whereas the veloc-
ity with which the objects move implies position changes
in temporal coordinates. From a neural viewpoint, previous
work has shown that collision detection associates with the
left inferior parietal cortex (Assmuss, etal. 2003). Second,
we include arithmetic tests that require the use of numerical
information processing established by operator-dependent
rules (Friedrich & Friederici, 2009). We use tests with dif-
ferent types of arithmetic operations; additions, subtrac-
tions, and multiplications. Neurally, research has shown
that tasks that involve numbers and calculations involve
bilateral inferior parietal activity as a function of the arith-
metic operation (Arsalidou & Taylor, 2011), albeit with
a key involvement of the left hemisphere (Dehaene, etal.
1993). Third, we study left- and right-handers based on the
premise that handedness introduces distinct sensorimotor
experiences that affect the processing demands. We also
conduct an evaluation of the participants’ personality traits
as a relationship between handedness and negative affect has
been proposed due to an underlying influence of the right
hemisphere (Sutton & Davidson, 1997) with left- as com-
pared to right-handers obtaining higher self-reported levels
of behavioural inhibition (Hardie & Wright, 2014).
In the present work, we argue that the space–time calcula-
tions of the object collision task associate with arithmetic
computations due to shared neural mechanisms. We further
hypothesise that the participants’ handedness guides the
processing demands as a result of their sensorimotor expe-
riences and interactions with objects. Combined, insights
into individual differences of handedness and interdependen-
cies across space–time–number processing will be valuable
to increase our understanding of common mechanisms that
underlie our behaviour.
Methods
Participants
There were 37 participants in this study (Mage = 20.7years,
SEage = 0.6), including 19 left-handers and 18 right-hand-
ers. They reported no history of neurological or psychiatric
conditions as evaluated by a standardised questionnaire,
and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Participants
gave written consent prior to the start of the experiment in
accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was
approved by the School of Psychology Ethics Committee.
Handedness questionnaire
To characterise handedness, participants completed a
15-item handedness questionnaire that measured hand pref-
erence for manipulation tasks (i.e., write a letter, use spoon,
use toothbrush, throw ball to hit target, use a comb, hold
racquet, hold needle when sewing, draw a picture, use com-
puter mouse, open lid from can, hold knife to cut, peel an
apple, use scissors, deal cards, use eraser).
The handedness questionnaire used a 5-point Likert scale
that varied between always left and always right. The score
per item was calculated with a value of 0 (always left), 1
(usually left), 2 (both equally), 3 (usually right) or 4 (always
right). For each participant, the scores of the items were
summed, divided by the maximum score of the question-
naire, and multiplied by 100. This provided a laterality index
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
366 Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
of handedness that varied between 0 (extreme left-handed-
ness) and 100 (extreme right-handedness). The writing hand
was also included as a condition for handedness as most
people will categorise their handedness on the basis of their
writing hand (Perelle & Ehrman, 2005).
Reinforcement sensitivity theory personality
questionnaire (RST‑PQ)
To capture personality traits, participants completed subtests
of the RST-PQ (Corr & Cooper, 2016), which rely on the
premise that individual differences emerge from neurobio-
logical systems that are specialised in detecting, process-
ing, and responding to stimuli. In particular, the Behav-
ioural Inhibition System (BIS) engages risk assessment and
inhibits behaviour in response to goal conflict, resulting in
anxiety and behavioural avoidance. In contrast, the Behav-
ioural Approach System (BAS) facilitates goal-directed
activity and positive emotions, leading to optimistic mood
and achieved goals. The questionnaire covered BIS activity
with 23 items and BAS activity with 32 items; i.e., biased
attention towards reward interest (BAS-RI with 7 items),
goal drive persistence (BAS-GDR with 7 items), impulsiv-
ity (BAS-IMP with 8 items) and reward reactivity (BAS-RR
with 10 items).
The questionnaire used a 4-point Likert scale for accuracy
of statements, ranging between 1 (not at all) and 4 (highly
accurate). For each sub-test, the ratings were summed
across items to provide a total score. High scores indicated
increased sensitivity of a given neurobiological system.
Object collision task
Participants were seated at a viewing distance of 70cm from
a computer monitor. The trial presentation is illustrated in
Fig.1. Each trial started with the presentation of a fixation
cross that lasted 1000ms followed by the appearance of
a black and white object, either 3.6º on the upper, lower,
left or right side of the screen’s centre. Thereafter, the per-
pendicular presented objects with a diameter of 0.4° would
start to move in straight lines with a constant speed of 2.8
or 5.6°/s towards the screen’s centre, resulting in collision
and non-collision events. As soon as the objects started to
move, the participants were required to decide whether the
objects would collide (target hit) or not collide (target miss)
behind a mask that had a height and width of 3.6°. This point
of collision which occurred 1300ms after onset is not shown
to the participants as the mask would hide the final trajecto-
ries of the objects from 1200ms after onset. After another
700ms or until the participants made a response, a blank
screen occurred that marked the end of the trial. In 33% of
the trials, a third grey object (distractor) would move with a
similar speed alongside the black object towards the mask.
These trials were included to influence attentional selection
to the relevant objects. The performance conditions (without
distractor vs. with distractor) and type of collision (target
hit vs. target miss) were randomised. There were 32 trials
per performance condition, resulting in a total of 128 trials.
Participants were asked to respond as fast and as accurate
as possible in their decision-making using keys allocated to
the index and middle fingers of the left or right hand (coun-
terbalanced). Before the start of the experiment, a training
session with feedback was provided, and there were short
breaks throughout the experiment. The trial sequence and
data collection were implemented using e-Prime.
The measurements of the task were collision detection
time (ms) and accuracy (%). The collision detection time
comprised the time period between initiation of the moving
objects and key press responses whereas the collision detec-
tion accuracy referred to correctly confirmed collisions on
contact trials and correctly rejected collisions on no contact
trials, and represented a key measurement that captures the
ability to predict the collision event at a precise moment
in time. We also calculated the balanced integration score
to obtain a composite evaluation of both measurements.
This index integrates reaction time and accuracy with equal
weighting and is considered beneficial as compared to other
methods that assess speed-accuracy trade-offs (Liesefeld
Fig. 1 Collision task without and with distractor. Left side: after dis-
appearance of the fixation cross, the task starts with the black and
white objects moving towards the centre. Right side: after 1200ms,
these objects disappear behind a mask while their final trajectories
are hidden from view. In the collision task with distractor, the grey
object (distractor) moves along the black object on the side nearest to
the white object
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
367Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
& Janczyk, 2019; Vandierendonck, 2017). The balanced
integration score is calculated by independently standardis-
ing the reaction times and percentage correct responses to
bring them onto the same scale, and then subtracting one
standardised score from the other. Its interpretation is in
terms of performance above or below average, and there-
fore measures relative performance—for example, whether
one group of participants is more successful than another
group, or, whether one condition is more difficult than
another condition.
Arithmetic tests
Participants were asked to answer a series of arithmetic
operations, i.e., additions, subtractions, multiplications using
pen and paper. There were two lists consisting of 10 prob-
lems for each arithmetic operation, which were presented
separately. For additions and subtractions, the problems
involved double- and single-digit operands (64–7) or two
double-digit operands (69 + 15) whereas for multiplications,
the problems involved single-digit operands (7 × 4) or dou-
ble- and single-digit operands (92 × 3), excluding 0 and 1 as
one of the operands. Across the arithmetic tests, the prob-
lems required a combination of memory retrieval and com-
putation. Participants were asked to answer the problems
as fast and as accurate as possible. As a control condition,
a number copying tasks were conducted. This consisted of
copying a list of 20 numbers (four lists of five numbers)
that involved double- or triple-digit operands. Short breaks
between the tests were provided.
The measurements of the task were arithmetic perfor-
mance time (s) and accuracy (%) for each arithmetic opera-
tion in addition to the number copying time (s) per list of
five numbers.
Analysis
The object collision measurements were analysed by means
of 2 × 2 mixed-design ANOVAs (Handedness Group;left-
vs. right-handers and Distractor Presence; with vs. without
distractor). Secondary analyses assessing the start posi-
tion of the objects or their speed did not show any sig-
nificant effects, p > 0.05. The arithmetic measurements
were analysed by means of 2 × 3 mixed-design ANOVAs
(Handedness Group; left- vs. right-handers and Arithmetic
Operation; additions vs. subtractions vs. multiplications).
Frequency analyses were conducted by means of chi-square
tests. The number copying measurement and the personality
questionnaire scores were analysed by means of independ-
ent t tests on Handedness Group. A simple linear regression
analysis was conducted to assess whether space–time detec-
tion of object collision predicted mathematical achievement.
Initial checks showed that the Durbin–Watson test indicated
no concern for autocorrelation, with data homoscedasticity
and a normal probability plot of the residuals. Mean ± SE
is reported. Bonferroni correction was made for multiple
comparisons, where appropriate.
Results
Handedness questionnaire
The laterality index obtained from the handedness question-
naire was used to classify the participants, resulting into 19
left-handers (LI = 19 ± 3%, age = 23 ± 1y, 18 females) and
18 right-handers (LI = 93 ± 2%, age = 19 ± 1y, 11 females).
Personality questionnaire
The total RST-PQ scores showed no significant difference
between left-handers (M = 141.5 ± 3.7) and right-handers
(M = 138.1 ± 3.2), p > 0.05. Additional analyses for the
subtests revealed no significant differences between left-
and right-handers for BIS (M = 60.1 ± 2.6 and 57.2 ± 2.4),
BAS-RI (M = 17.4 ± 1.0 and 17.4 ± 0.8), BAS-GDR
(M = 19.5 ± 0.6 and 17.9 ± 0.7), BAS-IMP (M = 16.9 ± 1.0
and 18.6 ± 1.3), BAS-RR (M = 27.5 ± 1.3 and 26.9 ± 1.0),
all p > 0.05. In previous work, differences in negative affect
have been associated with handedness (Hardie & Wright,
2014). However, we observed no indication of a significant
shift in our sample, as shown in Fig.2 which illustrates the
participants’ BIS scores alongside their laterality index.
Fig. 2 Scatter plot of the BIS scores obtained from the personality
questionnaire (RST-PQ) as a function of the participants’ laterality
index from the handedness questionnaire. The laterality index varied
between 0 (extreme left-handedness) and 100 (extreme right-handed-
ness). The middle line exemplifies a score of 50 (ambidextrous)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
368 Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
Object collision task
For collision detection time, the ANOVA analysis demon-
strated a significant main effect of Distractor Presence, F(1,
35) = 28.1, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.44, indicating that decision time
was longer in the presence of distractors (M = 1159 ± 42ms)
than without distractors (M = 1096 ± 36ms). No other effects
were significant, p > 0.05.
For collision detection accuracy, the ANOVA analysis
pointed to a significant main effect of Handedness Group,
F(1, 35) = 13.5, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.28, showing that left-hand-
ers (M = 69 ± 2%) detected object collisions more accurately
than right-handers (M = 60 ± 2%). No other effects were sig-
nificant, p > 0.05. Figure3 provides details about the colli-
sion detection accuracy of the participants alongside their
laterality index, and shows that left-handers were more accu-
rate performers than right-handers.
To portray more in detail the collision task for both hand-
edness groups, Fig.4 presents a categorisation of frequency
counts of the participants according to three performance
intervals: superior (i.e., fast/accurate performers), interme-
diate (in-between performers), and inferior (slow/inaccurate
performers). For the collision detection time (left-sided pan-
els), the data revealed that there were no significant group
differences for the performance intervals, p > 0.05. Of note
is that distractor presence did not impact the performance
intervals (p > 0.05).
For collision detection accuracy (right-sided panels), the
data indicated that both groups performed distinctively for
the inferior and intermediate intervals, χ21,N=37 = 11.45,
p < 0.001; and χ21, N=37 = 14.58, p < 0.0001. There was no
difference for the superior interval, p > 0.05. Performing
with or without distractor did not affect any of the intervals
(p > 0.05).
The balanced integration score revealed a significant
main effect of Handedness Group, F(1, 35) = 4.2, p < 0.05,
ηp2 = 0.11 with left-handers (0.28 ± 0.16) being more suc-
cessful than right-handers (− 0.33 ± 0.28). There was
also a significant main effect of Distractor Presence. F(1,
35) = 5.8, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.14, indicating a stronger perfor-
mance without distractor (0.12 ± 0.17) than with distractor
(− 0.16 ± 0.16).
Arithmetic tests
For arithmetic performance time, the ANOVA analysis
revealed a significant main effect of Arithmetic Opera-
tion, F(2, 70) = 19.9 p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.36, demonstrating
that additions (M = 80 ± 5s) were performed fastest fol-
lowed by subtractions (M = 117 ± 9s) and multiplications
(M = 151 ± 15s). Post hoc comparisons demonstrated that
all tests differed from one another, p < 0.01.
For arithmetic performance accuracy, the ANOVA
analysis showed a significant main effect of Arithmetic
Operation, F(2, 70) = 19.3, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.35, revealing
that additions (M = 95 ± 1%) obtained highest accuracy
Fig. 3 Scatter plot of the collision detection accuracy scores as a
function of the participants’ laterality index from the handedness
questionnaire. The accuracy scores represent the combined collision
conditions. The laterality index varied between 0 (extreme left-hand-
edness) and 100 (extreme right-handedness). The middle line exem-
plifies a score of 50 (ambidextrous)
Fig. 4 Categorisation of the collision detection times (left side) and
accuracy scores (right side) for the left- and right-handers. The col-
our coding across the measurements indicates the performance level:
superior (white), intermediate (grey) and inferior (black)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
369Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
followed by subtractions (M = 90 ± 1%) and multiplications
(M = 85 ± 2%). Post hoc comparisons demonstrated that all
tests differed from one another, p < 0.01.
To illustrate the arithmetic tests in more detail, Fig.5 pre-
sents a frequency count for the arithmetic operations accord-
ing to three performance intervals: superior (i.e., fast/accu-
rate performers), intermediate (in-between performers), and
inferior (slow/inaccurate performers). Across the measure-
ments of performance time (left-sided panels) and accuracy
(right-sided panels), the data illustrate that ± 30% of the par-
ticipants performed in the intermediate range whereas ± 60%
corresponded to fast/accurate performers and ± 10% were
slow/inaccurate performers.
For the number copying task, the analysis demon-
strated no significant difference between left-handers
(M = 7.7 ± 0.3 s) and right-handers (M = 7.5 ± 0.3 s),
p > 0.05.
Object collision task andarithmetic tests: regression
analysis
Regression analysis was conducted to determine how
handedness affected behaviour at the individual level
across both handedness groups. In assessing both tasks, we
observed that collision detection accuracy and arithmetic
accuracy showed a positive association for subtractions
(top panel, Fig.6) and multiplications (lower panel, Fig.6)
as a function of handedness. The regression analyses for
right-handers revealed significant outputs for subtrac-
tions, F(1, 35) = 6.2, p < 0.03, with β = 0.53 and R2 = 0.28,
suggesting that 28% of the variance can be explained by
the model (adjusted R2 = 0.24) and for multiplications,
F(1, 35) = 5.42, p < 0.05, with β = 0.50 and R2 = 0.25,
Fig. 5 Categorisation of the arithmetic performance times (left side)
and accuracy scores (right side) for additions, subtractions and mul-
tiplications. The colour coding across the measurements indicates the
performance level: superior (white), intermediate (grey) and inferior
(black)
Fig. 6 Scatter plot of the collision detection accuracy and arithme-
tic accuracy scores, illustrating a positive association. The accuracy
scores represent the collision conditions (with and without distractor)
and the arithmetic conditions of subtractions (top panel) and multipli-
cations (lower panel)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
370 Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
suggesting that 25% of the variance can be explained by
the model (adjusted R2 = 0.21). For left-handers, no sig-
nificant outputs were observed, p > 0.05. In addition, no
effects were observed for the collision detection time and
arithmetic accuracies, p > 0.05.
Discussion
Making predictions is an innate capability of the human
brain. In particular, the brain makes sense of the environ-
ment by predicting future events and by testing whether
these are in line with incoming sensory information and
previous experiences (Clark, 2013; Schubotz, 2007). To
form these predictions, the dimensions of space, time and
number are elementary. That is, a representation can be
created by knowing where, when and how many, enabling
us to respond to and learn about environmental regulari-
ties (Burr, etal. 2010; Lourenco & Longo, 2011; Winter,
etal. 2015).
Predictive behaviour, such as estimating collisions,
is crucial to our everyday activities, such as anticipat-
ing the course of moving objects and (de)synchronising
our actions with them (Enns & Lleras, 2008; Senot, etal.
2003). We tested this real-world scenario by asking partic-
ipants to detect whether collisions between moving objects
would occur or not; decisions that are made on the basis
of the use of spatial and temporal information from the
motion trajectories, guided by attention to space and time.
In addition, the participants completed arithmetic tests of
addition, subtraction and multiplication, which are com-
mon in daily life such as required for counting and formal
mathematics. In this study, we examine interdependencies
between space–time–number by assessing both tasks in a
group of left- and right-handers due to their distinct sen-
sorimotor experiences.
Handedness group profiles: space–time andnumber
processing
Handedness is a manifestation of brain lateralisation that
provides a representational index of the hands and that
captures preference for manual activities (Corballis &
Häberling, 2017). Our results from the collision detection
task revealed that both handedness groups showed distinct
behavioural performances, with left-handers being more
accurate than right-handers; a performance advantage that
could be due to a greater range of sensorimotor experiences
and space–time integration pathways between both hemi-
spheres (Assmus, etal. 2003; Cherbuin & Brinkman, 2006;
Serrien, etal. 2012). In this context, hand use and sensori-
motor competence are bi-directionally linked, shaping the
information processing and associations between sensori-
motor and attentional systems (Buckingham, etal. 2011; Le
Bigot & Grosjean, 2012). Moreover, handedness affects the
representation of extra-personal and peripersonal space with
left-handers showing bilateral hemispheric activity whereas
right-handers demonstrate an asymmetry of both hemi-
spheres (Colman, etal. 2017; O’Regan & Serrien, 2018).
Further differences between left- and right-handers have
been proposed with distinct neglect-like patterns as a result
of alertness-related modulations. In particular, Bareham
etal. (2015) observed that left-handers experienced a left-
ward hemispheric shift in attention with drowsiness whereas
right-handers have the opposite pattern, a distinction that
could be due to differences in the attentional mechanisms
that control alertness and direct attention to external stimuli
(Liu, etal. 2009). In this study, we modified the attentional
demands of the object collision task by means of distractors.
We observed that their presence slowed the detection time
across all participants, suggesting that difficult decisions
take more time than easier ones and engage more neural
circuitry for optimising behaviour (Assmus, etal. 2005;
Smout, etal. 2019; Spapé & Serrien, 2011).
To take into account differences due to personality traits,
we included the RST-PQ (Corr & Cooper, 2016); a question-
naire that associates personality with distinct brain systems
labelled as BIS that predicts an individual’s response to
anxiety and performance avoidance as opposed to BAS that
supports motivation and desired outcomes. Previous work
has shown that left-handers have higher BIS scores than
right-handers, which has been coupled with different levels
of negative affect (Beaton, etal. 2017; Hardie & Wright,
2014). However, we observed no significant differences
between left- and right-handers for any of the subtests, sug-
gesting that there were no distinct variations in personality
traits in our sample.
We noted no clear pattern of arithmetic performance dif-
ferences between both handedness groups; a topic that has
provided mixed claims throughout the literature (Annett
& Kilshaw, 1982; Cheyne, etal. 2010; Crow, etal. 1998).
While some studies have shown that left-handers are strong
in mathematics and consistent right-handers perform least,
others have suggested that mixed-handers are more disad-
vantaged. In a more recent study, Sala, etal. (2017) con-
cluded that the relationship between handedness group pro-
files and mathematical ability is complex and depends on
several factors, such as age, gender and type of task. This
conclusion is in line with evidence that changes in math-
ematical processing occur as a function of development, and
that arithmetic achievement depends on domain-general as
well as domain-specific knowledge (Arsalidou & Taylor,
2011).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
371Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
Object collisions, arithmetic calculations
andindividual differences
Theories that account for functional overlap of
space–time–number processing suggest the use of shared
resources, common formats or a reliance on cross-dimen-
sional mappings (Fias etal., 2003; Lakoff & Johnson, 1980;
Walsh, 2003). In this respect, parietal circuits take a central
role for the processing of space, time and number (Bjoer-
tomt, etal. 2002; Coull & Nobre, 1998; Eger etal., 2009) as
well as for the interactions between space–time (Magnani,
etal. 2010; Oliveri, etal. 2009), space–number (Hubbard,
etal. 2005; Oliveri, etal. 2004), and time-number (Burr,
etal. 2010; Hayashi, etal. 2013). Of note is, however, that
each hemisphere responds to specific task characteristics.
In particular, for space and time processing, there is greater
sensitivity of right inferior parietal circuitry for orienting
in space (O’Reilly, etal. 2008) versus left inferior parietal
circuitry for cueing in time and for space–time integration
(Assmus, etal. 2003; Coull & Nobre, 2008). For num-
ber processing and numerical operations, inferior parietal
regions are usually activated across both hemispheres, with
specificity according to the type and complexity of the prob-
lem (Arsalidou & Taylor, 2011). However, there is shared
circuitry across basic numerical and arithmetic tasks that is
located within the left hemisphere, i.e., intraparietal sulcus
alongside precentral areas. Therefore, a left fronto-parietal
circuit can be considered a core network of numerical knowl-
edge in adults (Pesenti, etal. 2000; Simon, etal. 2004; Zago,
etal. 2001). The relevance of this network is that it overlaps
with sensorimotor circuitry that is recruited for predictive
control related to own actions and external perceptual events
(Coull, etal. 2008; O’Reilly, etal. 2008; Schubotz, 2007).
Moreover, this type of prediction arises when the essential
information concerns dynamic forward change with coding
of transitions in space–time, and underscores a key role of
the sensorimotor system for the prediction of future states
within the adopted reference frame, be it the body or the
environment (Schubotz, 2007).
The regression analysis revealed a positive relationship
between the detection accuracy of object collisions and the
performance of arithmetic calculations, albeit as a function
of handedness. That is, an association was observed only in
right-handers, suggesting a connection between manual later-
alisation and arithmetic. Support for such a relationship comes
from finger counting, which represents a natural routine that
supports the acquisition of basic numerical and arithmetic
principles (Butterworth, 1999). In Western cultures, counting
involves a preferred starting-hand alongside a relative order
of finger counting within a single hand. Thus, finger count-
ing strategies that are shaped by sensorimotor experience and
developed during childhood may influence and steer how
numbers are represented and processed later in life (Fischer,
2008; Pesenti, etal., 2000). In adults, these hand-starting
preferences have been observed to be different for left- and
right-handers (Zago & Badets, 2016). That is, consistent left-
handers typically started counting with their left hand whereas
the opposite pattern was noted for consistent right-handers;
a manual preference that aligned with their dominant hand
for unimanual activities. Furthermore, an fMRI study demon-
strated that left-starters showed higher activation in the right
right-sided motor and premotor cortices when they perceived
small numbers whereas right-starters showed the reverse pat-
tern (Tschentscher, etal. 2012). Thus, handedness modulates
the structural arrangement of finger counting routines and
further influences the involvement of the motor-dominant
hemisphere for number processing (Artemenko, etal. 2020).
This reliance on effector-specific circuitry in left- and right-
handers has also been observed for skilled movements, such as
grasping (Martin, etal. 2011). Together, the findings suggest
that hemispheric lateralisation of key brain regions distinc-
tively guides the covariation of functions that cross cognitive
domains.
Besides handedness, we also noted that the type of arith-
metic operation played a role in the relationship between colli-
sion detection and calculation performances. In particular, the
results showed that stronger space–time computations resulted
in increased performances for subtraction and multiplication
calculations. No effect was observed for additions, which
could be due to the fewer demands on number processing as
compared to the other tasks which likely required additional
processing steps (Fehr, etal. 2007). In using basic arithmetic
operations (addition, subtraction, multiplication), we observed
increasingly longer response times and lower accuracy rates,
confirming changes in complexity requirements. In our study,
the results did not show that the collision detection time sig-
nificantly linked with arithmetic performance, which is in line
with research that has shown that people alter where rather
than when they would hit targets, if given the choice (Brenner,
etal. 2015).
In conclusion, space, time and number are key dimen-
sions that underlie how we perceive, identify and act within
the environment. In this study, we examined interdependen-
cies between these dimensions using an object collision task
that required space–time processing and arithmetic tests that
involved number processing in left- and right-handers. Hand-
edness of the participants influenced collision detection with
left-handers being more accurate than right-handers, which
is in line with the premise that hand preference guides indi-
vidual differences as a result of sensorimotor experiences and
distinct interhemispheric integration patterns. The data further
showed that successful collision detection was a predictor for
arithmetic achievement, at least in right-handers. These find-
ings suggest that handedness plays a mediating role in binding
information processing across domains, likely due to selective
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
372 Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
connectivity properties within the sensorimotor system that are
guided by hemispheric lateralisation patterns.
Acknowledgements The work was supported by a research grant from
the BIAL foundation to DJS (no. 376/14). We thank Louise O’Regan
for assistance in the project.
Data availability The data have been stored in the Open Science Frame-
work data repository (https:// osf. io/ rq8nu/).
Declaration
Conflict of interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of
interest.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attri-
bution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adapta-
tion, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long
as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source,
provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes
were made. The images or other third party material in this article are
included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated
otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in
the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will
need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a
copy of this licence, visit http:// creat iveco mmons. org/ licen ses/ by/4. 0/.
References
Annett, M., & Kilshaw, D. (1982). Mathematical ability and lateral
asymmetry. Cortex, 18, 547–568. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/ S0010-
9452(82) 80053-1
Arsalidou, M., & Taylor, M. J. (2011). Is 2+2=4? Meta-analyses of
brain areas needed for numbers and calculations. NeuroImage,
54, 2382–2393. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro image. 2010. 10. 009
Artemenko, C., Sitnikova, M. A., Soltanlou, M., Dresler, T., & Nuerk,
H. C. (2020). Functional lateralization of arithmetic processing in
the intraparietal sulcus is associated with handedness. Scientific
Reports, 4, 1775. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1038/ s41598- 020- 58477-7
Assmus, A., Marshall, J. C., Noth, J., Zilles, K., & Fink, G. R. (2005).
Difficulty of perceptual spatiotemporal integration modulates the
neural activity of left inferior parietal cortex. Neuroscience, 132,
923–927. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro scien ce. 2005. 01. 047
Assmuss, A., Marshall, J. C., Ritzi, A., Noth, J., Zilles, K., & Fink, G.
R. (2003). Left inferior parietal cortex integrates time and space
during collision judgments. NeuroImage, 20, S82–S88. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro image. 2003. 09. 025
Bareham, C. A., Bekinschtein, T. A., Scott, S. K., & Manly, T. (2015).
Does left-handedness confer resistance to spatial bias? Scientific
Reports, 5, 9162. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1038/ srep0 9162
Beaton, A. A., Mutinelli, S., & Corr, P. J. (2017). Fractionating nega-
tive and positive affectivity in handedness: insights from the
reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality. Laterality, 22,
419–444. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 13576 50X. 2016. 12132 74
Bjoertomt, O., Cowey, A., & Walsh, V. (2002). Spatial neglect in near
and far space investigated by repetitive transcranial magnetic
stimulation. Brain, 125, 2012–2022. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/
brain/ awf211
Bonato, M., Zorzi, M., & Umilta, C. (2012). When time is space:
evidence for a mental time line. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral
Reviews, 36, 2257–2273. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neubi orev.
2012. 08. 007
Bonn, C. D., & Cantlon, J. F. (2012). The origins and structure of
quantitative concepts. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 29, 149–173.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 02643 294. 2012. 707122
Brenner, E., & Smeets, J. B. (2015). Quickly making the correct choice.
Vision Research, 113, 198–210. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. visres.
2015. 03. 028
Buckingham, G., Main, J. C., & Carey, D. P. (2011). Asymmetries in
motor attention during a cued bimanual reaching task: Left and
right handers compared. Cortex, 47, 432–440. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1016/j. cortex. 2009. 11. 003
Bueti, D., & Walsh, V. (2009). The parietal cortex and the representa-
tion of time, space, number and other magnitudes. Philosophi-
cal Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 12,
1831–1840. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1098/ rstb. 2009. 0028
Burr, D. C., Ross, J., Binda, P., & Morrone, C. (2010). Saccades com-
press space, time and number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14,
528–533. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. tics. 2010. 09. 005
Butterworth, B. (1999). The mathematical brain. Macmillan.
Cherbuin, N., & Brinkman, C. (2006). Efficiency of callosal trans-
fer and hemispheric interaction. Neuropsychology, 20, 178–184.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ 0894- 4105. 20.6. 700
Cheyne, C. P., Roberts, N., Crow, T. J., Leask, S. J., & Garcia-Finana,
M. (2010). The effect of handedness on academic ability: a mul-
tivariate linear mixed model approach. Laterality, 15, 451–464.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 13576 50090 29769 56
Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents,
and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral Brain Sciences, 36,
181–204. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1017/ S0140 525X1 20004 77
Colman, H. A., Remington, R. W., & Kritikos, A. (2017). Handedness
and graspability modify shifts of visuospatial attention to near-
hand objects. PLoS ONE, 12, e0170542. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/
journ al. pone. 01705 42
Corballis, M. C., & Häberling, I. S. (2017). The many sides of hemi-
spheric asymmetry: a selective review and outlook. Journal of the
International Neuropsychological Society, 23, 710–718. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1017/ S1355 61771 70003 76
Corr, P. J., & Cooper, A. J. (2016). The reinforcement sensitivity theory
of personality questionnaire (RST-PQ): development and valida-
tion. Psychological Assessment, 28, 1427–1440. https:// doi. org/
10. 1037/ pas00 00273
Coull, J. T., & Nobre, A. C. (1998). Where and when to pay attention:
the neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and
to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI. Journal of
Neuroscience, 18, 7426–7435. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1523/ JNEUR
OSCI. 18- 18- 07426. 1998
Coull, J. T., & Nobre, A. C. (2008). Dissociating explicit timing from
temporal expectation with fMRI. Current Opinion in Neurobi-
ology, 18, 137–144. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. conb. 2008. 07. 011
Coull, J.T., Vidal, F., Goulon, C., Nazarian, B., & Craig, C. (2008).
Using time-to-contact information to assess potential collision
modulates both visual and temporal prediction networks. Frontiers
in Human Neuroscience, 2.
Crow, T. J., Crow, L. R., Done, D. J., & Leask, S. (1998). Relative hand
skill predicts academic ability: global deficits at the point of hemi-
spheric indecision. Neuropsychologia, 36, 1275–1282. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1016/ S0028- 3932(98) 00039-6
de Hevia, M. D., Izard, V., Coubart, A., Spelke, E. S., & Streri, A.
(2014). Representations of space, time, and number in neonates.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America, 111, 4809–4813. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1073/ pnas.
13236 28111
Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representa-
tion of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
373Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
Psychology General, 122, 371–396. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ 0096-
3445. 122.3. 371
Dehaene, S., & Brannon, E. M. (2001). Space, time, and number: a
Kantian research program. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14,
517–519. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. tics. 2010. 09. 009
Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Cohen, L. (2003). Three parietal
circuits for number processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20,
487–506. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1080/ 02643 29024 40002 39
Eger, E., Michel, V., Thirion, B., Amadon, A., Dehaene, S., & Klein-
schmidt, A. (2009). Deciphering cortical number coding from
human brain activity patterns. Current Biology, 19, 1608–1615.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. cub. 2009. 08. 047
Enns, J. T., & Lleras, A. (2008). What’s next? New evidence for predic-
tion in human vision. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 327–333.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. tics. 2008. 06. 001
Fabbri, M., Cellini, N., Martoni, M., Tonetti, L., & Natale, V. (2013).
Perceptual and motor congruency effects in time-space associa-
tion. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1840–1851.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13414- 013- 0519-9
Fehr, T., Code, C., & Herrmann, M. (2007). Common brain regions
underlying different arithmetic operations as revealed by conjunct
fMRI–BOLD activation. Brain Research, 1172, 93–102. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. brain res. 2007. 07. 043
Fias, W., Lammertyn, J., Reynvoet, B., Dupont, P., & Orban, G. A.
(2003). Parietal representation of symbolic and nonsymbolic mag-
nitude. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 47–56. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1162/ 08989 29033 21107 819
Fischer, M. H. (2008). Finger counting habits modulate spatial-numer-
ical associations. Cortex, 44, 386–392. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j.
cortex. 2007. 08. 004
Friedrich, R., & Friederici, A. D. (2009). Mathematical logic in the
human brain: syntax. PLoS ONE, 4, e5599. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1371/ journ al. pone. 00055 99
Hardie, S.M., & Wright, L. (2014). Differences between left- and right-
handers in approach/avoidance motivation: influence of consist-
ency of handedness measures. Frontiers in Psychology, 134. doi:
https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fpsyg. 2014. 00134
Hayashi, M. J., Kanai, R., Hiroki, C., Tanabe, H. C., Yoshida, Y., Carl-
son, S., Walsh, V., & Sadato, N. (2013). Interaction of numerosity
and time in prefrontal and parietal cortex. Journal of Neurosci-
ence, 33, 883–893. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1523/ JNEUR OSCI. 6257- 11.
2013
Hécaen, H., & Sauguet, J. (1971). Cerebral dominance in left-handed
subjects. Cortex, 7, 19–48. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/ S0010-
9452(71) 80020-5
Hubbard, E. M., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Interac-
tions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 6, 435–448. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1038/ nrn16 84
Klöppel, S., van Eimeren, T., Glauche, V., Vongerichten, A., Münchau,
A., Frackowiak, R. S., etal. (2007). The effect of handedness
on cortical motor activation during simple bilateral movements.
NeuroImage, 34, 274–280. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro image.
2006. 08. 038
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. University
of Chicago Press.
Le Bigot, N., & Grosjean, M. (2012). Effects of handedness on visual
sensitivity in perihand space. PLoS ONE, 7, e43150. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1371/ journ al. pone. 00431 50
Liesefeld, H. R., & Janczyk, M. (2019). Combining speed and accu-
racy to control for speed-accuracy trade-offs. Behavioral Research
Methods, 51, 40–60. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 018- 1076-x
Liu, H., Stufflebeam, S. M., Sepulcre, J., Hedden, T., & Buckner, R.
L. (2009). Evidence from intrinsic activity that asymmetry of the
human brain is controlled by multiple factors. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
106, 20499–20503. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1073/ pnas. 09080 73106
Lourenco, S.F., & Longo, M.R. (2011). Origins and development of
generalized magnitude representation.In S. Dehaene & E. Bran-
non (Eds.),Space, time and number in the brain: Searching for
the foundations of mathematical thought (p. 225–244). Elsevier
Academic Press. doi: https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/ B978-0- 12- 385948-
8. 00015-3.
Magnani, B., Oliveri, M., Mangano, G. R., & Frassinetti, F. (2010).
The role of posterior parietal cortex in spatial representation of
time: a TMS study. Behavioral Neurology, 23, 213–215. https://
doi. org/ 10. 3233/ BEN- 2010- 0298
Martin, K., Jacobs, S., & Frey, S. H. (2011). Handedness-dependent
and -independent cerebral asymmetries in the anterior intrapari-
etal sulcus and ventral premotor cortex during grasp planning.
NeuroImage, 57, 502–512. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro image.
2011. 04. 036
O’Regan, L., & Serrien, D. J. (2018). Individual differences and hemi-
spheric asymmetries for language and spatial attention. Frontiers
in Human Neuroscience, 12, 80. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fnhum.
2018. 00380
Oliveri, M., Bonnì, S., Turriziani, P., Koch, G., Lo Gerfo, E., Torriero,
S., Vicario, C. M., Petrosini, L., & Caltagirone, C. (2009). Motor
and linguistic linking of space and time in the cerebellum. PLoS
ONE, 4, e7933. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/ journ al. pone. 00079 33
O’Regan, L., Spapé, M. M., & Serrien, D. J. (2017). Motor timing and
covariation with time perception: investigating the role of handed-
ness. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 147. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 3389/ fnbeh. 2017. 00147
O’Reilly, J. X., Mesulam, M. M., & Nobre, A. C. (2008). The cerebel-
lum predicts the timing of perceptual events. Journal of Neuro-
science, 28, 2252–2260. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1523/ JNEUR OSCI.
2742- 07. 2008
Park, J., Park, D. C., & Polk, T. A. (2013). Parietal functional connec-
tivity in numerical cognition. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 2127–2135.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/ cercor/ bhs193
Perelle, I. B., & Ehrman, L. (2005). On the other hand. Behavior Genet-
ics, 35, 343–350. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1007/ s10519- 005- 3226-z
Pesenti, M., Thioux, X., Seron, A., & De Volder, A. (2000). Neuro-
anatomical substrates of Arabic number processing, numerical
comparison and simple addition: a PET study. Journal of Cogni-
tive Neuroscience, 12, 461–479. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1162/ 08989
29005 62273
Pool, E. M., Rehme, A. K., Fink, G. R., Eickhoff, S. B., & Grefkes,
C. (2014). Handedness and effective connectivity of the motor
system. NeuroImage, 99, 451–460. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro
image. 2014. 05. 048
Proffitt, D. R., & Gilden, D. L. (1989). Understanding natural dynam-
ics. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 15, 384–393. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1037/ 0096- 1523. 15.2. 384
Reid, C. S., & Serrien, D. J. (2014). Primary motor cortex and ipsilat-
eral control: a TMS study. Neuroscience, 270, 20–26. https:// doi.
org/ 10. 1016/j. bbr. 2012. 02. 008
Sala, G., Signorelli, M., Barsuola, G., Bolognese, M., & Gobet, F.
(2017). The relationship between handedness and mathematics
is non-linear and is moderated by gender, age, and type of task.
Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 948. https:// doi. org/ 10. 3389/ fpsyg.
2017. 00948
Schubotz, R. I. (2007). Prediction of external events with our motor
system: towards a new framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
11, 211–218. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. tics. 2007. 02. 006
Senot, P., Prévost, P., & McIntyre, J. (2003). Estimating time to contact
and impact velocity when catching an accelerating object with the
hand. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 219–237. https://
doi. org/ 10. 1037/ 0096- 1523. 29.1. 219
Serrien, D. J., Sovijärvi-Spapé, M. M., & Farnsworth, B. (2012).
Bimanual control processes and the role of handedness. Neuropsy-
chology, 26, 802–807. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ a0030 154
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
374 Psychological Research (2022) 86:364–374
1 3
Simon, O., Kherif, F., Flandin, G., Poline, J.-B., Rivière, D., Mangin,
J.-F., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Automatized cluster-
ing and functional geometry of human parietofrontal networks
for language, space, and number. NeuroImage, 23, 1192–1202.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro image. 2004. 09. 023
Smout, C. A., Tang, M. F., Garrido, M. I., & Mattingley, J. B. (2019).
Attention promotes the neural encoding of prediction errors.
PLoS Biology, 17, e2006812. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1371/ journ al.
pbio. 20068 12
Spapé, M. M., & Serrien, D. J. (2011). Prediction of collision events:
an EEG coherence analysis. Clinical Neurophysiology, 122, 891–
896. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. clinph. 2011. 01. 047
Stanescu-Cosson, R., Pinel, P., van de Moortele, P. F., Le Bihan, D.,
Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2000). Understanding dissociations in
dyscalculia: a brain imaging study of the impact of number size
on the cerebral networks for exact and approximate calculation.
Brain, 123, 2240–2255. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1093/ br ain/ 123. 11. 2240
Sutton, S. K., & Davidson, R. J. (1997). Prefrontal brain asymmetry:
A biological substrate of the behavioral approach and inhibition
systems. Psychological Science, 8, 204–210. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1111/j. 1467- 9280. 1997. tb004 13.x
Tschentscher, N., Hauk, O., Fischer, M. H., & Pulvermüller, F. (2012).
You can count on the motor cortex: Finger counting habits modu-
late motor cortex activation evoked by numbers. NeuroImage, 59,
3139–3148. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. neuro image. 2011. 11. 037
Vandierendonck, A. (2017). A comparison of methods to combine
speed and accuracy measures of performance: A rejoinder on the
binning procedure. Behavior Research Methods, 49, 653–673.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 3758/ s13428- 016- 0721-5
Vogel, J. J., Bowers, C. A., & Vogel, D. S. (2003). Cerebral lateraliza-
tion of spatial abilities: A meta-analysis. Brain and Cognition, 52,
197–204. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/ S0278- 2626(03) 00056-3
Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of
time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 483–
488. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. tics. 2003. 09. 002
Winter, B., Marghetis, T., & Matlock, T. (2015). Of magnitudes and
metaphors: explaining cognitive interactions between space, time,
and number. Cortex, 64, 209–224. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/j. cor-
tex. 2014. 10. 015
Zago, L., & Badets, A. (2016). What is the role of manual prefer-
ence in hand-digit mapping during finger counting? A study in a
large sample of right- and left-handers. Perception, 45, 125–135.
https:// doi. org/ 10. 1177/ 03010 06615 602628
Zago, L., Pesenti, M., Mellet, E., Crivello, F., Mazoyer, B., & Tzou-
rio-Mazoyer, N. (2001). Neural correlates of simple and complex
mental calculation. NeuroImage, 13, 314–327. https:// doi. org/ 10.
1006/ nimg. 2000. 0697
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
... Furthermore, besides introducing the doubling condition and the active adjustment task, we also measured handedness, anxiety and personality traits in a sample of healthy participants. In fact, a possible influence of handedness on the processing of magnitudes has been suggested (Serrien & Spapé, 2022), including time (Hancock, 2011), and a very recent study based on the SET model revealed that, for instance, in older adults, the accuracy in time scanning would be compromised due to a slower clock (stage 1 of the SET model), accumulating less pulses, and leading to a slower scanning of time intervals (stages 2 and 3; Capizzi et al., 2022;Droit-Volet et al., 2019). Moving from these premises, we wondered whether more anxious participants have a faster clock, resulting in more pulses accumulated and thus in a faster subjective scanning of time: we expected anxiety as a predictor of the accuracy in timing paradigms, hypothesizing a wider underestimation of intervals in participants with higher anxiety scores, with faster counter resulting in anticipatory responses. ...
... The first test was administered to measure the laterality bias of the sample (Edinburgh Handedness Inventory; Salmaso & Longoni, 1983), starting from the evidence of a possible influence of handedness on the processing of magnitudes (Serrien & Spapé, 2022), including time (Hancock, 2011). The test consists of 13 items describing different motor activities and participants are asked to specify if the described activity is preferentially or absolutely carried out using the left or the right hand. ...
Article
Full-text available
Time perception is not always veridical, but it can be modulated by changes in internal and external context. The most-acknowledged theory in this regard hypothesises the existence of an internal clock allowing us to subjectively estimate time intervals. The aim of the present study is to investigate the possible effect of such an internal clock, measured as the ability to reproduce a target duration, in the mental manipulation of time: 63 healthy participants were asked to Bisect and to Double reference time intervals, besides Reproducing them. Moreover, to investigate whether time processing might be predicted by individual differences, handedness, anxiety, and personality traits were also assessed by means of standardized questionnaires. Results show that participants correctly Reproduce time intervals (internal clock), but they overestimate time intervals during Bisection and underestimate them during Doubling. We explain this unexpected pattern of results as a kind of aftereffect, due to the short-term retention (adaptation) to the subjective representation of shorter (Bisection) vs longer (Doubling) intervals, respectively. Moreover, hierarchic regression models reveal that some personality traits can predict Bisection accuracy, but they clearly show that the best predictor for both Bisection and Doubling is the accuracy in Reproducing time intervals, confirming the fundamental role of the internal clock in time estimation. We conclude that time estimation is a unique skill, mostly independent from inter-individual differences, and the new paradigms introduced here (bisection vs doubling) reveal that the correct functioning of the internal clock also explains the ability to mentally manipulate the time.
... Further on arithmetic and number processing, results have shown that when it comes to collision detection, which is a test specialised in investigating people's space-time processing and their arithmetic evaluation, The study concluded that left-handed people have higher accuracy [31]. The results of studies like this led to a convincing explanation of how different sensorimotor experiences appeared to be different between right-and left-handed people. ...
Article
Full-text available
This literature review article aimed to compile research published in recent years regarding the relationship between bodily habits, specifically handedness, and cognitive performance in memory, attention, and information processing. The article contains three chapters, discussing each aspect separately with a collection of studies and evaluations. The article is able to combine results from studies and form a model graph specialized for studies summaries. Through the synthesis of investigations, handedness seemed to have some relationship with cognition. To be more precise, ICH individuals (those people with mixed hands) have the capacity to create a connection between brain hemispheres, which enhances their performance in memory tests and allows them to better integrate divergent information. In addition, people tend to focus more on their dominant hands; this can affect a persons attention to things happening around them. Lastly, right-handed people performed better in generic cognition tests, and cross-activation of brain lateralization has been demonstrated.
... Vandierendonck has used two versions of the formula, one where S RT and S E are calculated across all conditions of a given participant (which we assume is the default and which is displayed in Eq. 2; Vandierendonck, 2017Vandierendonck, , 2021b) and one where S RT and S E are calculated separately per condition and participant (Vandierendonck, 2018; which in the following we refer to as LISAS cond as a shorthand for condition-specific LISAS). 3 Given the widespread use of within-participants designs in behavioral research and the frequent use of LISAS and BIS in within-participants comparisons, including many studies in which we have been involved (e.g., Allenmark et al., 2019;Barrientos et al., 2020;Bratzke & Ulrich, 2021;Chen et al., 2021;English et al., 2021;Liesefeld et al., 2015Liesefeld & Müller, 2021;Madrid & Hout, 2019;Mueller et al., 2020;Schuch & Pütz, 2021;Serrien & Spapé, 2021;Smith et al., 2019), it is important to note that LISAS was explicitly developed for the within-participants case (Vandierendonck, 2021b, p. 22). By contrast, BIS is by no means restricted to within-participants designs, but we and others consider many use cases even going beyond experimental psychology (e.g., Bakun Emesh et al., 2021;Draheim et al., 2019;Liesefeld & Janczyk, 2019;Liu et al., 2019;Mueller et al., 2019;Palmqvist et al., 2020;Stojan et al., 2021;Unsworth et al., 2020;White et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
Condition-specific speed–accuracy trade-offs (SATs) are a pervasive issue in experimental psychology, because they sometimes render impossible an unambiguous interpretation of experimental effects on either mean response times (mean RT) or percentage of correct responses (PC). For between-participants designs, we have recently validated a measure ( Balanced Integration Score , BIS ) that integrates standardized mean RT and standardized PC and thereby controls for cross-group variation in SAT. Another related measure ( Linear Integrated Speed–Accuracy Score, LISAS ) did not fulfill this specific purpose in our previous simulation study. Given the widespread and seemingly interchangeable use of the two measures, we here illustrate the crucial differences between LISAS and BIS related to their respective choice of standardization variance. We also disconfirm the recently articulated hypothesis that the differences in the behavior of the two combined performance measures observed in our previous simulation study were due to our choice of a between-participants design and we demonstrate why a previous attempt to validate BIS (and LISAS) for within-participants designs has failed, pointing out several consequential issues in the respective simulations and analyses. In sum, the present study clarifies the differences between LISAS and BIS, demonstrates that the choice of the variance used for standardization is crucial, provides further guidance on the calculation and use of BIS, and refutes the claim that BIS is not useful for attenuating condition-specific SATs in within-participants designs.
Article
Full-text available
Functional lateralization is established for various cognitive functions, but was hardly ever investigated for arithmetic processing. Most neurocognitive models assume a central role of the bilateral intraparietal sulcus (IPS) in arithmetic processing and there is some evidence for more pronounced left-hemispheric activation for symbolic arithmetic. However, evidence was mainly obtained by studies in right-handers. Therefore, we conducted a functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) study, in which IPS activation of left-handed adults was compared to right-handed adults in a symbolic approximate calculation task. The results showed that left-handers had a stronger functional right-lateralization in the IPS than right-handers. This finding has important consequences, as the bilateral IPS activation pattern for arithmetic processing seems to be shaped by functional lateralization and thus differs between left- and right-handers. We propose three possible accounts for the observed functional lateralization of arithmetic processing.
Article
Full-text available
The encoding of sensory information in the human brain is thought to be optimised by two principal processes: ‘prediction’ uses stored information to guide the interpretation of forthcoming sensory events, and ‘attention’ prioritizes these events according to their behavioural relevance. Despite the ubiquitous contributions of attention and prediction to various aspects of perception and cognition, it remains unknown how they interact to modulate information processing in the brain. A recent extension of predictive coding theory suggests that attention optimises the expected precision of predictions by modulating the synaptic gain of prediction error units. Because prediction errors code for the difference between predictions and sensory signals, this model would suggest that attention increases the selectivity for mismatch information in the neural response to a surprising stimulus. Alternative predictive coding models propose that attention increases the activity of prediction (or ‘representation’) neurons and would therefore suggest that attention and prediction synergistically modulate selectivity for ‘feature information’ in the brain. Here, we applied forward encoding models to neural activity recorded via electroencephalography (EEG) as human observers performed a simple visual task to test for the effect of attention on both mismatch and feature information in the neural response to surprising stimuli. Participants attended or ignored a periodic stream of gratings, the orientations of which could be either predictable, surprising, or unpredictable. We found that surprising stimuli evoked neural responses that were encoded according to the difference between predicted and observed stimulus features, and that attention facilitated the encoding of this type of information in the brain. These findings advance our understanding of how attention and prediction modulate information processing in the brain, as well as support the theory that attention optimises precision expectations during hierarchical inference by increasing the gain of prediction errors.
Article
Full-text available
Language and spatial processing are cognitive functions that are asymmetrically distributed across both cerebral hemispheres. In the present study, we compare left- and right-handers on word comprehension using a divided visual field paradigm and spatial attention using a landmark task. We investigate hemispheric asymmetries by assessing the participants’ behavioral metrics; response accuracy, reaction time and their laterality index. The data showed that right-handers benefitted more from left-hemispheric lateralization for language comprehension and right-hemispheric lateralization for spatial attention than left-handers. Furthermore, left-handers demonstrated a more variable distribution across both hemispheres, supporting a less focal profile of functional brain organization. Taken together, the results underline that handedness distinctively modulates hemispheric processing and behavioral performance during verbal and nonverbal tasks. In particular, typical lateralization is most prevalent for right-handers whereas atypical lateralization is more evident for left-handers. These insights contribute to the understanding of individual variation of brain asymmetries and the mechanisms related to changes in cerebral dominance.
Article
Full-text available
To catch a moving object with the hand requires precise coordination between visual information about the target's motion and the muscle activity necessary to prepare for the impact. A key question remains open as to if and how a human observer uses velocity and acceleration information when controlling muscles in anticipation of impact. Participants were asked to catch the moving end of a swinging counterweighted pendulum, and resulting muscle activities in the arm were measured. The authors also simulated muscle activities that would be produced according to different tuning strategies. By comparing data with simulations, the authors provide evidence that human observers use online information about velocity but not acceleration when preparing for impact.
Article
Full-text available
Time is a fundamental dimension of our behavior and enables us to guide our actions and to experience time such as predicting collisions or listening to music. In this study, we investigate the regulation and covariation of motor timing and time perception functions in left- and right-handers who are characterized by distinct brain processing mechanisms for cognitive-motor control. To this purpose, we use a combination of tasks that assess the timed responses during movements and the perception of time intervals. The results showed a positive association across left- and right-handers between movement-driven timing and perceived interval duration when adopting a preferred tempo, suggesting cross-domain coupling between both abilities when an intrinsic timescale is present. Handedness guided motor timing during externally-driven conditions that required cognitive intervention, which specifies the relevance of action expertise for the performance of timed-based motor activities. Overall, our results reveal that individual variation across domain-general and domain-specific levels of organization plays a steering role in how one predicts, perceives and experiences time, which accordingly impacts on cognition and behavior.
Article
Full-text available
The relationship between handedness and mathematical ability is still highly controversial. While some researchers have claimed that left-handers are gifted in mathematics and strong right-handers perform the worst in mathematical tasks, others have more recently proposed that mixed-handers are the most disadvantaged group. However, the studies in the field differ with regard to the ages and the gender of the participants, and the type of mathematical ability assessed. To disentangle these discrepancies, we conducted five studies in several Italian schools (total participants: N = 2,314), involving students of different ages (six to seventeen) and a range of mathematical tasks (e.g., arithmetic and reasoning). The results show that (a) linear and quadratic functions are insufficient for capturing the link between handedness and mathematical ability; (b) the percentage of variance in mathematics scores explained by handedness was larger than in previous studies (between 3 and 10% vs. 1%), and (c) the effect of handedness on mathematical ability depended on age, type of mathematical tasks, and gender. In accordance with previous research, handedness does represent a correlate of achievement in mathematics, but the shape of this relationship is more complicated than has been argued so far.
Article
Full-text available
We examined how factors related to the internal representation of the hands (handedness and grasping affordances) influence the distribution of visuospatial attention near the body. Left and right handed participants completed a covert visual cueing task, discriminating between two target shapes. In Experiment 1, participants responded with either their dominant or non-dominant hand. In Experiment 2, the non-responding hand was positioned below one of two target placeholders, aligned with the shoulder. In Experiment 3 the near-monitor hand was positioned under the placeholder in the opposite region of hemispace, crossed over the body midline. For Experiments 2 & 3, in blocked trials the palmar and back-of hand surfaces were directed towards the target placeholder such that targets appeared towards either the graspable or non-graspable space of the hand respectively. In Experiment 2, both left and right handers displayed larger accuracy cueing effects for targets near versus distant from the graspable space of the right hand. Right handers also displayed larger response time cueing effects for objects near the graspable versus non-graspable region of their dominant hand but not for their non-dominant hands. These effects were not evident for left-handers. In Experiment 3, for right handers, accuracy biases for near hand targets were still evident when the hand was crossed over the body midline, and reflected hand proximity but not functional orientation biases. These findings suggest that biased visuospatial attention enhances object identity discrimination near hands and that these effects are particularly enhanced for right-handers.
Article
In psychological experiments, participants are typically instructed to respond as fast as possible without sacrificing accuracy. How they interpret this instruction and, consequently, which speed–accuracy trade-off they choose might vary between experiments, between participants, and between conditions. Consequently, experimental effects can appear unpredictably in either RTs or error rates (i.e., accuracy). Even more problematic, spurious effects might emerge that are actually due only to differential speed–accuracy trade-offs. An often-suggested solution is the inverse efficiency score (IES; Townsend & Ashby, 1983), which combines speed and accuracy into a single score. Alternatives are the rate-correct score (RCS; Woltz & Was, 2006) and the linear-integrated speed–accuracy score (LISAS; Vandierendonck, 2017, 2018). We report analyses on simulated data generated with the standard diffusion model (Ratcliff, 1978) showing that IES, RCS, and LISAS put unequal weights on speed and accuracy, depending on the accuracy level, and that these measures are actually very sensitive to speed–accuracy trade-offs. These findings stand in contrast to a fourth alternative, the balanced integration score (BIS; Liesefeld, Fu, & Zimmer, 2015), which was devised to integrate speed and accuracy with equal weights. Although all of the measures maintain “real” effects, only BIS is relatively insensitive to speed–accuracy trade-offs.
Article
Hemispheric asymmetry is commonly viewed as a dual system, unique to humans, with the two sides of the human brain in complementary roles. To the contrary, modern research shows that cerebral and behavioral asymmetries are widespread in the animal kingdom, and that the concept of duality is an oversimplification. The brain has many networks serving different functions; these are differentially lateralized, and involve many genes. Unlike the asymmetries of the internal organs, brain asymmetry is variable, with a significant minority of the population showing reversed asymmetries or the absence of asymmetry. This variability may underlie the divisions of labor and the specializations that sustain social life. ( JINS , 2017, 23 , 710–718)
Book
The now-classic Metaphors We Live By changed our understanding of metaphor and its role in language and the mind. Metaphor, the authors explain, is a fundamental mechanism of mind, one that allows us to use what we know about our physical and social experience to provide understanding of countless other subjects. Because such metaphors structure our most basic understandings of our experience, they are "metaphors we live by"--metaphors that can shape our perceptions and actions without our ever noticing them. In this updated edition of Lakoff and Johnson's influential book, the authors supply an afterword surveying how their theory of metaphor has developed within the cognitive sciences to become central to the contemporary understanding of how we think and how we express our thoughts in language.