Conference PaperPDF Available

Effect of internal focus of attention with touching cue on the agonist muscle activity during exercise

Authors:
VOLUME 16 | Proc2 | 2021 |
S195
Supplementary Issue: Autumn Conferences of Sports Science. Costa Blanca Sports Science Events, 18-19 December 2020. Alicante, Spain.
Effect of internal focus of attention with touching
cue on the agonist muscle activity during exercise
KAZUSHIGE OSHITA
1
Department of Human Information Engineering, Okayama Prefectural University, Soja-shi, Japan
ABSTRACT
This study aimed to investigate the effect of internal focus of attention with touching cue on agonist muscular
activity during the arm curl exercise. Ten healthy males (age, 20-35 years) performed five repetitions of arm
curl under three different focus instructions: external focus condition (EXT), where participants were
instructed to concentrate on lifting the bar; internal focus condition (INT), where participants were instructed
to concentrate on contractions of biceps brachii; and, internal focus and touch condition (INT+T): participants'
biceps brachii was lightly touched by the investigator and they were instructed to concentrate on muscle
contractions at the touched point. Although the average muscular activities in INT and INT+T were
significantly higher than that of EXT (p < .01), no significant difference in muscle activity between INT and
INT+T was observed. Further, magnitudes of these effects were small (d < 0.25). These results suggest that
internal focus caused by individual's consciousness, as well as a focus attention by touching the agonist area
increased the agonist muscle activity during exercise. However, the increase in agonist muscle activity due
to focus attention is limited.
Keywords: External focus attention; Light touch effect; Resistance training; Exercise instructor.
1
Corresponding author. Department of Human Information Engineering, Okayama Prefectural University, Soja-shi, Japan.
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8049-1174
E-mail: oshita@ss.oka-pu.ac.jp
Abstract submitted to: Autumn Conferences of Sports Science. Costa Blanca Sports Science Events, 18-19 December 2020.
Alicante, Spain.
JOURNAL OF HUMAN SPORT & EXERCISE ISSN 1988-5202
© Faculty of Education. University of Alicante
doi:10.14198/jhse.2021.16.Proc2.04
Cite this article as:
Oshita, K. (2021). Effect of internal focus of attention with touching cue on the agonist muscle activity
during exercise. Journal of Human Sport and Exercise, 16(2proc), S195-S201.
doi:https://doi.org/10.14198/jhse.2021.16.Proc2.04
Proceeding
Oshita, K. / Effect of internal focus attention through touching on muscle activity JOURNAL OF HUMAN SPORT & EXERCISE
S196
INTRODUCTION
Muscle activity during exercise differs depending on an individual’s focus attention when lifting an external
resistance, even with the same load of resistance (Neumann, 2019; Schoenfeld and Contreras, 2016). In the
case of resistance training, internal focus of attention (INT) means the individual concentrates on contracting
the agonist muscle when lifting a weight, while external focus of attention (EXT), on lifting the weight. Previous
reviews (Neumann, 2019; Schoenfeld and Contreras, 2016) reported that although higher electromyographic
(EMG) activity in the agonist muscle is observed in INT as compared to EXT, movement velocity (including
angular velocity or power production) during exercise is executed faster in EXT than in INT. Therefore, these
previous studies suggest that if muscle strength gain or muscle hypertrophy gain is the desired goal, then
INT should be used during exercise. However, if individuals desire to improve movement performance, and
speed, it is argued that they should adopt EXT.
Increased muscular activity observed in INT indicates that understanding of agonist muscle group during
exercise is an important factor in enhancing the effect of resistance training. However, individuals (especially
in novice of resistance training) face difficulties in understanding the agonist muscle during resistance training
(Oshita et al., 2016a; 2016b; 2019), even in simple single-joint exercise (Oshita et al., 2018), or well-known
stretching exercise (Oshita et al., 2017). To enhance understanding of the agonist muscle, the instructor
occasionally touches the agonist muscle area, and instructs the client with cues such as concentrate on
contracting of this (touching) partduring exercise. Some researchers have reported effectiveness of touching
cue; lightly touching the body part can enhance the perception of body orientation, and this influence human
movement (Boonsinsukh et al., 2009; Oshita and Yano, 2015; 2016; 2017; 2020; Stramel et al., 2019).
Previous study reported that such an internal focus caused by a touching stimuli (INT+T) to the agonist area
increased the agonist muscle activity during exercise as compared to EXT (Oshita, in press). This result
proves that touching cue from an instructor is effective in increasing agonist muscle activity during exercise.
While INT is focus of the individual's subjective consciousness only, INT+T adds the focus of being touched
at agonist region in addition to the subjective focus. It seems that the individual can now concentrate more
on muscle contraction. If the individual achieves more concentration on muscle contraction during INT+T
than INT, agonist muscle activity will increase in INT+T as compared to INT. Therefore, this study investigated
the effect of INT with touching cue on agonist muscular activity during the arm curl exercise.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants
Data were collected from 10 healthy males (age, 20-35 years) with no current or previous medical history of
musculoskeletal system disorders. Each participant identified the preferred dominant arm they used for
writing. This was the right side for all the participants. All participants provided informed consent after the
details of this experiment were explained. This study was approved by the Human Ethics Committee of the
Graduate School of Human Development and Environment, Kobe University.
Procedures
Arm curl exercises were conducted using a preacher-curl machine on the non-preferred arm of the
participants. Muscular activity in the biceps brachii was evaluated during the exercise. Surface EMG data
were collected from the biceps brachii muscles to assess muscular activity. On the non-preferred arm, bipolar
surface electrodes with an EMG amplifier (ID2PAD; Oisaka Electronic Equipment Ltd., Japan) were placed
over the muscle belly at 2-cm inter-electrode distance. To decrease skin impedance affecting the electrical
Oshita, K. / Effect of internal focus attention through touching on muscle activity JOURNAL OF HUMAN SPORT & EXERCISE
VOLUME 16 | Proc2 | 2021 |
S197
signal, the skin was wiped by gentle abrasion with a skin preparation gel and isopropyl alcohol swabs. The
signals were amplified 500 times and acquired at sampling frequency of 1 kHz using a data logger with an
analogue-to-digital converter (LP-MS1002; Logical Product Corporation, Japan).
Experimental protocol
Maximum voluntary muscle isometric contraction (MVC) test was initially performed in the direction of elbow
flexion at 90° joint angle. The participant sat on a seat of the preacher-curl machine while his non-preferred
arm held a bar. The bar was fixed to the ground by a metal chain through a strain gauge (FG-5100, Lutron
Electronic Enterprise Co., Ltd., Taiwan). Participants were instructed to attempt to flex their arm against the
fixed bar. During the MVC tests, the visual signals depicting force were displayed on a digital indicator and
verbal encouragement was provided by investigators. Participants were asked to perform the MVC test twice
for 5 s with a 30 s pause in between the tests. The participants were allowed to reject an effort they deemed
sub-maximal.” The highest peak force was selected as the MVC force.
After the MVC tests, the participants performed an arm curl task weighted with a dumbbell. Mass of the
dumbbell was equivalent to 50% of the MVC force. Participants performed five repetitions of arm curl with full
range of motion under three different focus instructions, with 5 min of resting period between the sets. For
EXT, participants were instructed to concentrate on lifting the dumbbell, whereas for the INT, participants
were instructed to concentrate on the contractions of biceps brachii. For the INT+T, the participants’ biceps
brachii was lightly touched by the investigator and they were instructed to concentrate on muscle contractions
at the touched point (Figure 1). During these tasks, participants were asked to look straight ahead and
concentrate on the bar, agonist muscle contractions or contractions of the touched point. The movement time
of the arm curl was prescribed by using a digital metronome (MA-1, KORG Inc., Japan) set to produce a click
every 1.5 s. Participants were asked to perform their movements in synchrony with the metronome signal,
i.e., the end of each upward and downward motion should coincide with one signal. The order of focus
conditions was randomized across all the participants. The weight mass and movement tempo of this study
were based on a previous study that investigated the effects of focus of attention on muscle activity during
arm curl (Oshita et al., in press; Vance et al., 2004). Before the MVC tests, a practice session to familiarize
the participants with the arm curl task was conducted for approximately 5 min without dumbbell.
Figure 1. Schematic diagrams of the arm-curl experiment in three focus attention conditions.
Oshita, K. / Effect of internal focus attention through touching on muscle activity JOURNAL OF HUMAN SPORT & EXERCISE
S198
Data and statistical analysis
The stored EMG data were processed using the waveform analysis software SPCANA (ver. 4.92). After band-
pass filtering (1-500 Hz), the root mean square of the EMG signal (RMS-EMG) in each task was calculated.
For the MVC task, RMS-EMG was calculated at every 0.2 s, and the maximum 0.2 s interval RMS-EMG
value from the two MVCs was taken as the MVC value. In each arm curl task, a period of 0.5 seconds in the
middle portion in each upward portion was selected to calculate the RMS-EMG value. 2nd-4th repetitions in
each focus condition were selected for analysis, and the average RMS-EMG value was calculated. The
average RMS-EMGs in each arm curl task were normalized to the RMS-EMG in the MVC task (%MVC) for
muscle activity evaluation.
Muscle activities (%MVC) in each arm curl task were evaluated using one-way repeated-measures analysis
of variance (ANOVA), and differences between the three different focus were evaluated with post-hoc multiple
comparisons using Tukey’s test. Furthermore, the magnitude of the effect (Cohen’s d) of %MVC between the
two focus conditions was calculated.
Value of p < .05 was considered statistically significant. These analyses were performed using JSTAT (ver.
20.0J) software. Data were presented as mean values ± standard errors of mean (SEM), unless otherwise
stated.
RESULTS
Figure 2. Muscle activities in the biceps brachii during arm-curl exercise under three focus attention conditions
The average %MVC during arm curl tasks in the EXT, INT, and INT+T focus conditions were 62.04 ± 5.09%,
65.18 ± 4.95%, and 65.58 ± 5.23%, respectively (Figure 2). The average %MVC in the INT and INT+T focus
conditions were significantly higher than that of the EXT focus condition (p < .01). However, the effect size
compared to EXT was small for both INT (d = 0.21) and INT+T (d = 0.23). No significant difference of %MVC
Oshita, K. / Effect of internal focus attention through touching on muscle activity JOURNAL OF HUMAN SPORT & EXERCISE
VOLUME 16 | Proc2 | 2021 |
S199
between INT and INT+T focus condition was observed, and magnitude of effect was small (d = 0.03). These
results indicate that although internal focus caused by an individual's consciousness, as well as focus of
attention by touching the agonist area slightly increased the agonist muscle activity, there was no significant
difference in muscle activity among different methods of internal focus (i.e., an individual's consciousness or
a touch cueing).
DISCUSSION
The average %MVC in the INT and INT+T focus conditions were significantly higher than that in the EXT
condition. Increased agonist muscular activity is observed in the INT as compared to the EXT is consistent
with the results of previous studies (Neumann, 2019; Oshita et al., in press; Schoenfeld and Contreras, 2016;
Vance et al., 2004). Although antagonist muscle activity was not measured in this study, a previous study
reported that associated increase in the antagonist muscle activity was in line with the agonist muscle activity
during INT. This suggests an association between the antagonist and agonist muscles during human
movements (Vance et al., 2004). As a result, force output becomes difficult by this association when INT is
performed, and the agonist muscle activity might increase. An increased voluntary muscular activity not
accompanied with gross force or movement output suggests an increase in "noise" in the motor control
network (Marchant et al., 2005; Vance et al., 2004; Zachry et al., 2005). Furthermore, this study revealed that
INT caused by an individual's consciousness, as well as focus of attention by touching the agonist area,
increased the agonist muscle activity.
While INT is only an individual’s own consciousness, INT+T adds a touching stimulus to the consciousness.
Although it seems that INT+T is easier for concentration on the agonist area as compared to INT, no
significant difference in muscle activity was observed between INT and INT+T focus conditions and the
magnitude of their effect was slight. Therefore, it may be concluded that an increase in agonist muscle activity
due to focus conditions is limited. On the other hand, one participant in the present experiment told the
investigator that it was hard for him to concentrate on muscle contractions because he was distracted by the
touching. This comment makes us speculate that some individuals may not be able to utilize touch stimulation
effectively. In the actual exercise instruction, some instructor touches clients’ agonist muscle area only in
relaxation phase of the exercise (does not touch in contraction phase). Although the participants’ biceps
brachii was touched by the investigator all the time during arm curl exercise in the present study, this
instruction method suggests that touching timing might influence muscle activity. Because the effect of
muscle activity might differ depending such a touching method, further research is essential to examine the
details of touch stimuli, including timing, pressure, size of area, and so on.
Hence, the present study demonstrates that internal focus caused by individual's consciousness, as well as
a focus attention by touching the agonist area is also effective in increasing muscle activity during exercise.
Although individuals lacking resistance training face difficulties in understanding the agonist muscle during
resistance exercise (Oshita et al., 2016a; 2016b; 2018; 2019), the agonist muscle area could be touched to
effectively enhance the agonist muscle activity and direct the focus of attention to the agonist muscle.
CONCLUSION
This study investigated the effect of INT with touching cue on agonist muscular activity during the arm curl
exercise. Arm curl exercises under three different focus instructions were performed: external focus condition
(EXT), where participants were instructed to concentrate on lifting the bar; internal focus condition (INT),
where participants were instructed to concentrate on contractions of biceps brachii; and, internal focus and
Oshita, K. / Effect of internal focus attention through touching on muscle activity JOURNAL OF HUMAN SPORT & EXERCISE
S200
touch condition (INT+T): participants' biceps brachii was lightly touched by the investigator and they were
instructed to concentrate on muscle contractions at the touched point. Although the average %MVC in the
INT and INT+T focus conditions were significantly higher than that in the EXT condition (p < .01), no
significant difference in muscle activity between INT and INT+T was observed. Further, magnitudes of these
effects were small (d < 0.25). These results suggest that internal focus caused by individual's consciousness,
as well as a focus attention by touching the agonist area increased the agonist muscle activity during
exercise. However, the increase in agonist muscle activity due to focus attention is limited.
SUPPORTING AGENCIES
Research supported by KAKENHI (Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research C, Grant Number JP18K06455).
DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
The author declares no conflict of interest.
REFERENCES
Boonsinsukh, R., Panichareon, L., & Phansuwan-Pujito, P. (2009). Light touch cue through a cane
improves pelvic stability during walking in stroke. Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation,
90(6), 919-926. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2008.12.022
Marchant, D. C., Greig, M., Bullough, J., & Hitchen, D. (2011). Instructions to adopt an external focus
enhance muscular endurance. Research quarterly for exercise and sport, 82(3), 466-473.
https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2011.10599779
Neumann, D.L. (2019). A Systematic review of attentional focus strategies in weightlifting. Frontiers in
Sports and Active Living, 1, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fspor.2019.00007
Oshita, K. & Yano, S. (2015). The Effect of Lightly Gripping a Cane on the Dynamic Balance Control.
The open biomedical engineering journal, 9, 146-50. https://doi.org/10.2174/1874120701509010146
Oshita, K., & Yano, S. (2016). Effect and immediate after-effect of lightly gripping the cane on postural
sway. Journal of physiological anthropology, 35(1), 14. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40101-016-0096-4
Oshita, K., Hagiwara, G., Monma, T., Tsuno, T., Koizumi, K., Oyama, Y., Yamaguchi, K., Tashiro, T. &
Funatsu, K. (2016a). Association Between Previous Experience of Strength Training Under
Appropriate Supervision and Knowledge of Squats Among University Students who Exercise
Regularly. International Journal of Sport and Health Science, 14, 61-71.
https://doi.org/10.5432/ijshs.201606
Oshita, K., Hagiwara, G., Monma, T., Tsuno, T., Koizumi, K., Oyama, Y., Yamaguchi, K., Tashiro, T.,
Funatsu, K. & Ariyoshi, K. (2016b). Consciousness of training body part(s) by showing a picture of
an exercise to young inexperienced people: A case of squatting exercises. Japanese Journal of
Physical Fitness and Sports Medicine, 65(4), 421-429. https://doi.org/10.7600/jspfsm.65.421
Oshita, K., & Yano, S. (2017). Effect of haptic sensory input through a fluttering cloth on tandem gait
performance. Human movement science, 55, 94-99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2017.08.008
Oshita, K., Matsuzaki, T., Hikita, A., Miyazaki, R., Hagiwara, G., Tashiro, T., Tsuno, T., Higuchi, Y. &
Funatsu, K. (2017). Association between Experience of Supervised Strength Training and the
Knowledge of Stretching. Gazzetta Medica Italiana Archivio per le Scienze Mediche, 176 (11), 613-
624. https://doi.org/10.23736/S0393-3660.16.03422-7
Oshita, K. / Effect of internal focus attention through touching on muscle activity JOURNAL OF HUMAN SPORT & EXERCISE
VOLUME 16 | Proc2 | 2021 |
S201
Oshita, K., Matsuzaki, T., Hikita, A., Miyazaki, R., Hagiwara, G., Tashiro, T. & Higuchi, Y. (2018). A study
of body-part(s) training consciousness using a picture of a single-joint exercise. Japanese Journal of
Physical Fitness and Sports Medicine, 67(6), 393-401. https://doi.org/10.7600/jspfsm.67.393
Oshita, K., Matsuzaki, T., Hikita, A., Miyazaki, R., Hagiwara, G., Tashiro, T., Tsuno, T., Higuchi, Y. &
Funatsu, K. (2019). Consciousness of training body part(s) by showing a picture of various exercises
in trunk muscles. International Journal of Sport and Health Science, 17, 135-147.
https://doi.org/10.5432/ijshs.201915
Oshita, K. & Yano, S. (2020). Influence of haptic sensory input through different kinds of clothing on gait
performance. Applied Sciences, 10, 7590. https://doi.org/10.3390/app10217590
Oshita, K. (in press). Tactile cue by touching the agonist muscle increases the muscle activity during arm
curl exercise. IFMBE Proceedings, 82 (accepted).
Schoenfeld, B.J. & Contreras, B. (2016). Attentional focus for maximizing muscle development: The
mind-muscle connection. Strength and Conditioning Journal, 38(1), 27-29.
https://doi.org/10.1519/SSC.0000000000000190
Stramel, D.M., Carrera, R.M., Rahok, S.A., Stein, J. & Agrawal, S.K. (2019). Effects of a person-following
light-touch device during overground walking with visual perturbations in a virtual reality environment.
IEEE Robotics and Automation Letters, 4(4), 4139-4146. https://doi.org/10.1109/LRA.2019.2931267
Vance, J., Wulf, G., Töllner, T., McNevin, N., & Mercer, J. (2004). EMG activity as a function of the
performer's focus of attention. Journal of motor behavior, 36(4), 450-459.
https://doi.org/10.3200/JMBR.36.4.450-459
Zachry, T., Wulf, G., Mercer, J., & Bezodis, N. (2005). Increased movement accuracy and reduced EMG
activity as the result of adopting an external focus of attention. Brain research bulletin, 67(4), 304-
309. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.06.035
This work is licensed under a Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0).
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated the effects of haptic sensory input by different types of clothing worn on gait performance. Twelve healthy men performed normal and tandem gait tests with blindfolds under three different clothing conditions: (1) wearing only half tights (HT); (2) wearing a skirt-like draped outfit such as a cotton cloth wrapped around the waist and extended to the lower leg (DC); and (3) wearing a trouser-like outfit such as tracksuit bottoms (TS). Although gait speed was significantly increased in DC as compared with HT, this was not observed in TS. Missteps during tandem gait were significantly reduced with DC. In addition, DC made walking easier for individuals as compared with TS. These findings suggest that wearing a skirt-like outfit such as kilts in Scotland or the hakama in Japan may provide haptic sensory cues to enhance individuals’ perceptions of their body orientation as compared with trouser-like clothing that is in continuous contact with the legs.
Article
Full-text available
The way an athlete focuses their attention when lifting a weight has the potential to influence strength development during training and performance outcomes during competition. The effects of attentional focus strategies during weightlifting tasks was investigated through a systematic review. Major databases (SportDISCUS, PsycINFO, Scopus) were searched using key terms relevant to attentional focus and weightlifting and reference lists of identified articles were also searched. Following screening, 16 articles were retained for analysis. The review showed that researchers have recruited experienced and novice weightlifters of both genders in their studies, although male experienced weightlifters are the most commonly studied demographic. Weightlifting tasks have varied from bench press, biceps curls, squats, and leg extensions with some studies using measures of force production against a force plate. The predominant manipulations have been between internal-associative and external-associative foci. An external attentional focus has shown to be beneficial in terms of movement economy as reflected in a variety of outcome measures. The results are interpreted within the framework provided by the Constrained Action Hypothesis and more generally the advantages of an external attentional focus for motor skill learning. An external focus of attention promotes automatic control of actions, thus preventing the motor system being constrained by conscious cognitive control. Implications for informing training programs for athletes and for advising athletes to maximize performance during competition are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to investigate the consciousness among young people (n = 1016) on training body part(s), using a questionnaire-based survey showing participants a picture of a single-joint exercise. The participants were shown a picture of knee-extension exercise (KNEE-EXT), and were asked to identify the body part(s) they felt were being trained. On seeing the picture, more than 90% of the participants felt that KNEE-EXT can train the anterior thigh part. However, more than 90% of the participants who had no experience doing KNEE-EXT felt that the exercise trained not only the anterior thigh part but also other parts, including the trunk, the posterior thigh, and the upper limb. Among those participants who had performed KNEE-EXT without the experience of strength-training under professional supervision, approximately 80% felt that the exercise was appropriate to train not only the anterior thigh but also other parts. These results suggest that performing exercises by referring to only a picture may result in the individuals not properly understanding how the body part(s) should be trained during exercise, even in single-joint exercises. Appropriate supervision is of even greater importance especially for strength-training beginners. Furthermore, even among participants who had an experience of exercise under professional supervision, approximately 60% had a similar response. Therefore, to enhance the benefits of exercises, individuals (even those who have had experience in training under professional supervision) should always be instructed under appropriate supervision on the correct technique and knowledge about the exercises.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of the present study was to investigate the consciousness among young people (n = 494) of training body part(s) by showing participants a picture of an exercise, through a questionnaire-based survey. Participants were shown a picture of a Back Squat, and were asked which body part(s) they felt was being trained in the picture. Participants who had no experience of doing squatting exercises (NO-SQUAT group, n = 102) and participants who were experienced in terms of doing squatting exercises and weight training under appropriate supervision (INSTRUCTED group, n = 146) were selected for the analysis. On seeing the picture, more than half of the NO-SQUAT group felt that the Back Squat was to train not only their lower body but also another body part(s); among these participants, approximately 70% felt that a Back Squat was appropriate to train their upper torso and arms too. Further, the NO-SQUAT group had made significantly fewer attempts to consciously train their gluteal and knee flexor muscles, and had made significantly more attempts to consciously train their upper torso and arms during the squatting exercises than the INSTRUCTED group had. These results suggest that to do exercises by referring to only a picture may result in the participants not properly understanding how the body part(s) should be trained during exercise. Therefore, to enhance the benefits of training, individuals need to be instructed, under appropriate supervision, on correct technique as well as knowledge about the exercise.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of the present study was to investigate the effect of experience or inexperience with weight-training under appropriate supervision on the knowledge of squatting exercises among university students. A questionnaire-based survey was conducted among university students who exercise regularly (30 or more min per day, 2 or more days per week, and maintaining this for more than 1 year) to clarify the relationships between their experience of weight-training under appropriate supervision, knowledge of squatting exercises, and the frequency with which they perform squatting exercises (n = 309). More than half of the students who exercised regularly had never experienced weight-training under appropriate supervision. Approximately 50% of the participants performed squats periodically, even if they did not have any experience with weight-training under appropriate supervision. The participants who lacked experience with weight-training under supervision had made significantly fewer attempts to consciously train their gluteal and knee flexor muscles during squatting exercises than those who had experienced weight-training under supervision. In fact, approximately 50% of the former did not consciously train their gluteal muscles during squatting exercises at all. These results suggest that although the students performed squatting exercises periodically, those who had never experienced weight-training under supervision may not have properly understood the squatting method. Therefore, to enhance the benefits of training, people should be shown the correct method to perform training exercises and given appropriate supervision.
Article
Full-text available
Background This study investigated the effect and after-effect of lightly touching a real cane on postural sway and ankle muscle activity. Method Participants performed a single-leg stance (SLS) task with their eyes closed for 30 s under three tasks. In the first and third tasks, the participants performed a normal SLS. In the second task, the participants in light-grip group (n = 11) were asked to perform SLS while lightly gripping a cane with their hand. The participants in depend-on-cane group (n = 11) were asked to support their own body with a cane. Results Postural sway during a single-leg stance is decreased by light gripping and is accompanied by decreased co-contraction of the ankle-joint muscles. If a participant lightly gripped a cane, postural sway decreased not only during the light gripping but also immediately after the withdrawal of the cane. Although postural sway and co-contraction in the depend-on-cane group were significantly decreased during the second task compared to the first task, they were not significantly changed between the first and third tasks. Conclusion These results suggest that lightly gripped cane provides a haptic sensory cue that can be used to assist postural control mechanisms due to enhanced perception of self-motion through sensory interaction with the environment through the cane. Further, the haptic sensory cue during postural maintenance might be promoted as a practice effect of postural control.
Article
The aim of the present study was to investigate the consciousness among young people (n = 1000) of training body part(s) by showing participants a picture of trunk muscle exercises through a questionnaire-based survey. The participants were shown a picture of bent-knee sit-up (sit-up), back extension (back-EXT), and plank from elbows to toes (plank), and were asked which body part(s) they felt was being trained in each picture. As for the frequency of performing these exercises, only 3% and 8% of all the participants had never performed sit-up and plank exercises, respectively. On seeing the picture, 40–60% of the participants who had no experience doing a sit-up or back-EXT felt that these exercises were to train not only the agonist muscle but also the antagonist muscle. Approximately 40% of the participants who had no experience doing a plank exercise felt that it was not to train the epigastrium or hypogastrium area. Among the participants who experienced doing a plank exercise without a previous experience of strength training under professional supervision, more than half felt that plank exercise was appropriate to train not only the trunk but also the upper limb or lower leg. Furthermore, among the participants who had a previous experience of strength training under professional supervision, approximately 30% had such an opinion. Therefore, although the penetration rate of these trunk exercises are high, doing trunk exercises by referring to only a picture may result in the participants not properly understanding how the body part(s) should be trained during the exercise. Therefore, to enhance the benefits of trunk exercises, individuals (even those who had a previous experience of training under professional supervision) always need to be instructed through appropriate supervision on the correct technique and knowledge about the exercises.
Article
italic xmlns:mml="http://www.w3.org/1998/Math/MathML" xmlns:xlink="http://www.w3.org/1999/xlink">Background: Proprioceptive feedback represents an alternative sensory channel that may be stabilizing in both overground gait in a virtual environment (VE), as well as when using an assistive device for overground gait. A person-following robot could potentially provide more useful proprioceptive feedback as compared to conventional assistive devices, such as a cane, as the person could maintain continuous contact with the environment with a reduced cognitive load. Methods: Using a light touch support paradigm, we explore whether a person-following robot could act as a proprioceptive reference point to stabilize overground gait. Twelve healthy subjects completed ten passes across an instrumented walkway in various conditions. These conditions tested the main and interaction effects of walking in a VE, walking in a VE with visual perturbations, and walking with light touch proprioceptive feedback. Results: The light touch support caused narrower strides in all VE conditions. This decrease in the base of support could indicate increased confidence in motor planning due to light touch contact. Significance: This letter illustrates that proprioceptive signals from a person-following robot may increase overground stability when the user experiences discordant visual feedback.
Article
BACKGROUND: This study investigated the association between previous experience with strength training under proper supervision and knowledge of stretching. METHODS: A questionnaire-based survey was conducted among university students (N.=870) to clarify the relationships between their experience of strength training under appropriate supervision, and knowledge of sit-and-reach stretching (SR-stretching). RESULTS: This study revealed that more than 50% of the participants performed SR-stretching periodically, even if they did not have any experience with strength training under appropriate supervision. the participants who lacked experience with strength training under supervision had made significantly greater attempts to consciously stretch non-stretched parts (i.e. abdominals, anterior legs, arms, and so) upon being shown the SR-stretching picture than those who had experienced strength training under supervision. CONCLUSIONS: These results suggest that although the individuals performed SR-stretching periodically, those who had never experienced strength training under supervision may not have properly understood the stretching method. Therefore, to enhance the benefits of stretching, individuals should be instructed to perform the technique correctly and under appropriate supervision. Furthermore, more than 70% of those who do not have regular exercise habits perceived that non-stretched parts were being stretched. it is suggested that appropriate supervision is of greater importance for participants without regular exercise habits who begin exercise.
Article
This study investigated the effects of haptic sensory input through a fluttering cloth on balance control during locomotion. Twenty-one healthy men performed a tandem gait test for 4m with their eyes closed under two different conditions: (1) wearing only half or short tights (HT-condition), or (2) wearing a fluttering cloth that was wrapped around the waist and extended to the lower leg (CLOTH-condition). Participants performed two trials with a 3-min rest period. The first trial involved the HT-condition, whereas the second trial involved either the CLOTH-condition (n=11), or the HT-condition again (n=10). The gait time and double support phase were significantly lower in the CLOTH-condition than in the HT-condition, and the relationship between the change in these two parameters was significant (r=0.74, P<0.01). Further, the relationship between the change in gait time and subjective walking sensation (evaluated through a visual-analogue scale) was significant in the CLOTH-condition (r=0.82, P<0.01). Therefore, if the participant demonstrated improved gait performance while wearing a fluttering cloth, it was accompanied by an improvement in subjective walking sensation. These results suggest that wearing a fluttering cloth can provide a haptic sensory cue to enhance the individuals' perception of their body orientation, which contributes to better balance control during locomotion. Therefore, locomotive ability may improve depending on the shape of the garment.