ArticlePDF Available

Achievement goals model validation: Is the 2X2 better than the Trichotomous?

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

span>Achievement motivation evolved fast in the educational field. In this development, the trichotomous and the 2X2 models received myriad attention from the educational specialist. However, there is a debate about which is better between the two models. This study aimed to intercede this debate and argue that the study's duration should be accounted for in the validation. Approach goals should dominate new students' achievement goals, and old students' achievement goals will show the balance of approach and avoidance goals. For these reasons, this study gathers the data from 350 new students and 203 old students. Confirmatory factor analysis reveals that the trichotomous is the best model for new student segments. While for the old student segment, the 2X2 model shows its efficacy. Therefore, for the new students' segment, achievement goals consist of mastery-approach, performance-approach, and performance-avoidance goals. For the old students, besides those three-goal orientations, mastery-avoidance goals are also included. As expected, the independent sample t-test shows that new students have higher mastery-approach and performance-approach goals than old students have. Self-efficacy is more influential in the new than old student segments, as shown by simple linear regression. This study is still stuck to a single cross-sectional design. Further research can utilize longitudinal research with segmental-based analysis and pay attention to gender, major, social class, or other potential moderation variables.</span
Content may be subject to copyright.
International Journal of Evaluation and Research in Education (IJERE)
Vol. 10, No. 1, March 2021, pp. 142~149
ISSN: 2252-8822, DOI: 10.11591/ijere.v10i1.20869 142
Journal homepage: http://ijere.iaescore.com
Achievement goals model validation: Is the 2X2 better than the
Trichotomous?
Bilson Simamora1, Elisabeth Vita Mutiarawati2
1Department of Management, Kwik Kian Gie School of Business and Information Technology, Indonesia
2Department of Business Administration, Kwik Kian Gie School of Business and Information Technology, Indonesia
Article Info
ABSTRACT
Article history:
Received Jul 16, 2020
Revised Dec 15, 2020
Accepted Jan 29, 2021
Achievement motivation evolved fast in the educational field. In this
development, the trichotomous and the 2X2 models received myriad
attention from the educational specialist. However, there is a debate about
which is better between the two models. This study aimed to intercede this
debate and argue that the study's duration should be accounted for in the
validation. Approach goals should dominate new students' achievement
goals, and old students' achievement goals will show the balance of approach
and avoidance goals. For these reasons, this study gathers the data from 350
new students and 203 old students. Confirmatory factor analysis reveals that
the trichotomous is the best model for new student segments. While for the
old student segment, the 2X2 model shows its efficacy. Therefore, for the
new students' segment, achievement goals consist of mastery-approach,
performance-approach, and performance-avoidance goals. For the old
students, besides those three-goal orientations, mastery-avoidance goals are
also included. As expected, the independent sample t-test shows that new
students have higher mastery-approach and performance-approach goals than
old students have. Self-efficacy is more influential in the new than old
student segments, as shown by simple linear regression. This study is still
stuck to a single cross-sectional design. Further research can utilize
longitudinal research with segmental-based analysis and pay attention to
gender, major, social class, or other potential moderation variables.
Keywords:
Achievement goals
Achievement motivation
Mastery-approach goals
Mastery-avoidance goals
Performance-approach goals
Performance-avoidance goals
This is an open access article under the CC BY-SA license.
Corresponding Author:
Bilson Simamora
Department of Management
Kwik Kian Gie School of Business and Information Technology
Jl. Yos Sudarso Kavling 87 Sunter Podomoro, Jakarta 14350, Indonesia
Email: bilson.simamora@kwikkiangie.ac.id
1. INTRODUCTION
There are many theories of motivation. Initially, the researchers tried to develop a general theory of
motivation [1]. However, in their development, motivation theories did not move to a more unified but more
diverse perspective. The differences in researchers' academic background, in which the motivation theories
developed, are the main factors that caused this problem emerged [1, 2].
Motivation theories are divided into two big categories. They are, first, the main theories such as
Maslow's Hierarchy of Need Theory, Hull's Drive Theory, and Rotter's Social Learning Theory. Second, the
temporary theories such as self-efficacy, intrinsic and extrinsic motivations, need for achievement, and
achievement motivation [1]. Achievement motivation is derived from the expectancy-value belief theory [1].
This basic theory underlines that motivation is generated by expectancy that by performing a specific
behavior, one can generate expected outcomes or avoid unexpected outcomes [3, 4]. Individuals' choice,
Int J Eval & Res Educ ISSN: 2252-8822
Achievement goals model validation: Is the 2X2 better than the Trichotomous? (Bilson Simamora)
143
persistence, and motivation to perform the task are influenced by the belief about how well they will do the
job and how important the outcomes are [4-6].
The expectancy-value belief theory gave birth to Atkinson's achievement theory [1], which defines
achievement in terms of comparing one's performances and others’ performances on certain standard
activities. More specifically, the intention to pursue achievement-related goals (Ts) is determined by the need
for achievement or motive for success (Ms), success probability achieves goals (Ps), and success incentive value
(Is). Mathematically, the relationship between Ts and its determinants is expressed in (1):
𝑇𝑠 =𝑀𝑠 𝑋 𝑃𝑠 𝑋 𝐼𝑠 (1)
The Ms represents a personality character (i.e., striving for success trait) that is relatively stable and
enduring. Ps is the subjective judgment of an individual about the probability of success in achieving goals.
As a probability, the value of Ps ranges from 0 (no probability at all) to 1 (a definite possibility). The
perceived difficulty of the task can approximate this variable. Incentive (Is) as the affect labeled as "pride of
accomplishment" of the task. 'Is' is the opposite of the Ps. It means that the more difficult it is to achieve
success (the lower the Ps), the higher the incentive (the Is) of achieving success [3].
With future-oriented thinking, people can predict the desired and undesired outcomes of their
behavior [7, 8]. When an individual wants to get or avoid them, the desired or undesired outcomes become
goals [7]. In approach motivation, people direct their motivational factors (emotions, cognitions, and actions)
to achieve desired outcomes. In avoidance motivation, those motivational factors are directed away from
aversive situations or undesired outcomes [9]. When the outcomes are uncertain, people are involved in
behavioral trying and driving force that impels them to such behavior, called motivation of trying [10].
A behavioral outcome can be produced by skill-related or chance-related factors [11]. In skill-
related factors, the results are determined by one’s ability. The higher the ability, the higher the expectancy
is. Prior success or failure will influence the ability perception. In chance-related situations, such as the flip
of a coin, expectancy remains the same regardless of whether the subject is successful or failed in prior experience.
Initially, achievement motivation deals with the behaviors in which the skill-related factors produce
excellent performance. In other words, achievement motivation is viewed as relevant only for high-ability or
self-efficacy [3, 12]. Self-efficacy is one’s belief that he or she can perform a task [5]. It determines the
feeling, thinking, and motivation [5, 6]. People with strong self-efficacy are more confident to accept and
perform tasks. They also tend to set up higher goals and have higher motivation. They are more receptive to
difficult tasks because they perceive it as something to be mastered instead of avoiding it as a threat [5, 6].
Nichol [13] accordingly said that individuals aim to demonstrate high achievement to show off their
ability. In the same way, Ames baptized the purposes of achievement behavior as an achievement goal. It
consists of a mastery goal that represents the development of an ability and performance goals that reflect a
willingness to demonstrate ability. Both have the approach sense [14] and are covered by the so-called
dichotomous model [9]. The concept of achievement goals has become the icon of achievement motivation [15].
However, although correlated, both have a different understanding. Achievement motivation is a driving
force by which emotions, competences, cognition, and behavior are energized and directed to achieve
achievement goals [9, 13, 16]. Achievement goal is a competence-based purpose that guides achievement
behavior [13, 16]. Achievement goal orientation is a relatively stable tendency toward which an individual is
more attracted [16-18].
In its operationalization, scientists reuse the approach and avoidance valences of motivation. With
this new approach, in addition to the Ames’s mastery and performance goals [19], Elliot and Harackiewicz [20]
introduced the third goal called performance-avoidance goals, and build a trichotomous model that consists
of three goal orientations. First, mastery goals are purposed to develop competence or skill used to master the
task. Second, performance-approach goals, activating by the willingness to show off one's performance or
ability. Third, performance avoidance-goals, driven by the willingness to avoid the status of being looser or
viewed as incompetent. High self-efficacy people should own the first two goals, and the third goal generally
related to low efficacy people [20]. There are vast numbers of studies that confirm this model [17].
In 2001, Elliot and McGregor [18] added the fourth dimension called mastery avoidance, a goal
through which an individual avoids failure to master a skill or competence. The new model has two focuses
(mastery and performance) and two valences (approach and avoidance). It is called as the 2x2 model. In
detail, this model consists of a mastery-approach, mastery-avoidance, performance-approach, and
performance-avoidance goals. Many studies confirmed the validity of this model [15].
The question is, which is the better one, the trichotomous model or the 2X2 model? There is no
unified answer to this question. Some researchers proved that the 2X2 model is valid and reliable [15, 21].
However, it may not be easy to conceptualize the mastery-avoidance goal [22]. This category is less adaptive
and tends to be detrimental [23]. Moreover, generating hypotheses about the relationship between mastery-
ISSN: 2252-8822
Int J Eval & Res Educ, Vol. 10, No. 1, March 2021: 142 - 149
144
avoidance goals and performance is challenging [24]. Therefore, the trichotomous model is the primary
choice in many studies [25].
This study aims to solve this scientific dispute and emphasizes that students’ expectancy and self -
efficacy should be accounted for in the validation. As stated before, mastery-approach and performance-
approach goals owned by high self-efficacy people [13, 19, 20], and mastery-avoidance and performance-
avoidance goals correlate with low self-efficacy [26]. Therefore, the 2X2 model should be evident in a lower
self-efficacy segment, and the trichotomous model should be relevant in the higher self-efficacy people.
The previous studies uncovered that new students have higher expectations than old students do [17, 27],
and many of them express unrealistic expectations [28]. For the same student, a longitudinal research design
reveals that the students' expectations in the first week are higher than their perceived experience at the end
of year one [27]. The expectation is determined by self-efficacy [3, 5]. Therefore, new students’ higher
expectation should be caused by their higher self-efficacy and expectations. Logically, for the new student
segment, the dichotomous model is more relevant. However, a previous study uncovered that self-efficacy
can also correlate positively with performance-avoidance goals, especially if the sense of loss to others is
vulnerable to self-esteem [29]. Consequently, performance-avoidance goals should be involved, and the
trichotomous model is more relevant for the new students.
With their experience, the old students set up more reasonable achievement goals. They potentially
set up mastery and performance goals with approach and avoidance directions, as found in the previous study
that involved their kind [15]. Therefore, we can expect that the 2X2 model of achievement goals has a higher
chance of being valid in the old students segment. In sum, the objectives of this study are, first, to study,
which is the most suitable model of achievement goals for new and old students. Second, to compare the
level of goal orientations found as valid in the new and old student segments. Third, to investigate the
influence of self-efficacy on achievement goals element simultaneously found as valid in the two segments to
check the models' structural validity. There is a rare discussion about these issues; therefore, the findings
related to them are the original contributions of this study.
2. RESEARCH METHOD
2.1. Sample of new students
This study was conducted in a business college located in North Jakarta, the capital of Indonesia.
There were two considerations for the choice of this business school as the research site. First, new students
face a relatively soft selection process to get into college. There were many alternatives available to the new
students for the same category of educational services. Therefore, they should have made deep considerations
before choosing it. Second, as a brand, this college's name gave no halo effect on new students' perceptions.
Consequently, the choice should be based on rational considerations of its educational service attributes,
features, and anticipated outcomes of their choice.
The authors invited the students that participated in the Study Orientation and Campus Introduction
in the final week of August 2019. As many 350 respondents, out of 521 new students (response rate is
67.18%), were involved voluntarily in the study. They consisted of 198 males (56.6%) and 152 females
(43.4%). The age average was 18.29 years and the median was 18 years.
2.2. Sample of old students
The old students are defined as those who have gone through more than four semesters of standard
eight-semester tuition. In line with this definition, with a convenience sampling method, the authors choose
students enrolled in the Intermediate Management Accounting class, a subject taught in the 5-th semester, in
the aforementioned business school. The study was conducted in the first week of November 2019 or two
weeks before the mid-test. That was the proper time to avoid the intervening effect of the mid-test result on
the respondents’ responses. As many of 203 students were involved in the study voluntarily. They consist of
91 males (44. 8%) and 112 females (55.2%) with an average age of 19.43.
2.3. Measurement
The Indonesian version of Eliot and Murayam’s AGQ-R (achievement goal questionnaire-revised) [30]
is used to measure dependent variables. This tool consists of 12 questions grouped into four achievement
goal elements. The tool to measure self-efficacy is taken from Pintrich, et al. [31]. The original questions of
the measures were translated into Bahasa Indonesia to fit them with local research contexts, and then re-
translated to English. The authors evaluated the similarity of the original and re-translated version of
measurements. The Indonesian version is finally used after an English teacher ensured that original and re-
translated versions of measurements were the same. The responses are recorded using seven levels of Likert-
type scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).
Int J Eval & Res Educ ISSN: 2252-8822
Achievement goals model validation: Is the 2X2 better than the Trichotomous? (Bilson Simamora)
145
2.4. Data collection method
The data were collected using an online questionnaire linked with an internet link and shared to
targeted respondents via Whattsapp. The respondents could fill out questionnaires anytime during the data
collection period. To reduce position bias, the order of the questions was randomized. In the introduction part
of the questionnaire, the author is informed that their participation was voluntary. There was no reward for
their participation. However, they were also guaranteed that their participation should not affect their fate
during their campus study. The respondents were listed unanimously to make them feel free to fill the
questionnaires.
2.5. Data analysis method
We utilize confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) using LISREL 8.8 as the analysis tool to validate the
achievement goals models. With the maximum likelihood approach, this method extracts only the common
variance shared among a construct's predetermined variables. To be categorized as valid, each variable
should at least have factor loading (FL) of 0.5, the average variance extracted is at least 0.6, and the
composite reliability should be 0.6 or higher. Besides, the instrument should have a Cronbach alpha
reliability of 0.7 or above [32].
To compare the achievement goals, we use an independent sample t-test because the two segments
involved in the comparison are independent. The achievement goal orientations and self-efficacy found as
valid in the two segments are the test objects. The point of comparison is that the two groups mean
differences. Since the new student segment is expected to have higher achievement goals and self-efficacy
than the old student segment, the decision about mean difference is made under one-tailed significance with
error rate or alpha being 0.05 or lower. The influence of self-efficacy on achievement goals is investigated
using simple linear regression. To compare the influence under investigation between the two groups, we use
a standardized coefficient, t-value, and adjusted determinant coefficient as the values to be compared [33].
3. RESULTS
3.1. Achievement goals validity
3.1.1. New students
The confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) with LISREL 8.8 is utilized to validate the 2X2 model. The
first run of the program reveals that the data fail to confirm the validation of mastery-avoidance goals
because of the low factor loading (MAV1=0.35, MAV2=0.07, MAV=0.67). We expect that the FL should be
at least 0.5 as long as the average variance extracted >0.50 and the construct reliability >0.60 [32]. In other
words, the data give no evidence to confirm the existence of mastery-avoidance goals. Three other
achievement goals elements are valid and reliable, as shown in Table 1. Therefore, the study reveals that, in
new student segments, the trichotomous model is relevant as expected.
Measurement model of LISREL used for this analysis has a fair-fit according to root mean square
error of approximation (RMSEA)=0.083. However, most of other criteria indicate that the model is good-fit,
as shown by goodness of fit index (GFI)=0.95, normed fit index (NFI)=0.98, Parsimony Normed Fit Index
(PNFI)=0.65, non-normed fit index (NNFI)=0.97, comparative fit index (CFI)=0.98, root mean square
residual (RMR)=0.022, Standardized RMR=0.035, incremental fit index (IFI), and Adjusted goodness of fit
index (AGFI)=0.91.
Table 1. Achievement goals validity in new students’ segment
Instruments
FL
AVE
CR
CA
Mastery approach
My aim is to completely master the material presented in this class
0.78
0.51
0.61
0.74
I am striving to understand the content of this course as thoroughly as possible
0.80
“My goal is to learn as much as possible
0.52
Performance approach
I am striving to do well compared to other students
0.81
0.69
0.82
0.87
My aim is to perform relatively well relatively to other students
0.82
My goal is to perform better than the other students
0.86
Performance avoidance
My goal is to avoid performing poorly compared to other students
0.76
0.56
0.69
0.79
I am striving to avoid performing worse than other students”
0.83
My aim is to avoid doing worse than other students
0.65
Notes. FL=factor loading, AVE=average variance extracted, CR=construct reliability, CA=Cronbach alpha
ISSN: 2252-8822
Int J Eval & Res Educ, Vol. 10, No. 1, March 2021: 142 - 149
146
3.1.2. Old students
The validation of Eliot and Murayam’s [30] 2X2 AGQ-R measurement used the data from 203
students. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) with LISREL reveals that all achievement goals dimensions are
valid, as shown by all validity indicators that exceed the minimal threshold (FL>0.50, AVE>0.60, CR>0.70),
specified by Hair, et al. [32]. Cronbach alpha also indicates excellent reliability (r11>0.70) of the instrument.
Therefore, in the old student segments, the model of 2X2 is confirmed. The measurement model used for the
CFA is good-fit as shown by RMSEA=0.078, incremental fit index (IFI)=0.98, non-normed fit index
(NNFI)=0.96, comparative fit index (CFI)=0.98, relative fit index (RFI)=0.93, and normed fit index
(NFI)=0.96.
3.2. Comparing the level of common achievement goals orientation
In this section, the author compares the level of achievement goals found as simultaneously valid in
the new and old student segments. As we can see in Table 2, the new students has higher mastery-approach
(mean difference=0.20, t=2.37, α=0.009) and performance-approach goals (mean difference=0.37, t=4.25,
α=0.000), but the difference between both is not significant for performance-avoidance goals (mean
difference=0.08, t=1.03, α=0.153).
The results confirm previous studies [17, 27] that the new students are more optimistic than old
students. The additional analysis supports this result. As shown in Table 3, the new students have higher self-
efficacy than the old students (mean differences=0.33, t=4.30, α=0.000). This result confirms that high self-
efficacy people tend to set up higher goals [4, 5].
Table 2. Validity and reliability of achievement goals in old students segment
Instruments
FL
AVE
CR
CA
Mastery approach
My aim is to completely master the material presented in this class
0.76
0.79
0.92
0.82
I am striving to understand the content of this course as thoroughly as possible
0.99
My goal is to learn as much as possible
0.90
Mastery avoidance
My aim is to avoid learning less than I possibly could
0.74
0.58
0.81
0.75
My goal is to avoid learning less than it is possible to learn
0.74
I am striving to avoid an incomplete understanding of the course material
0.81
Performance approach
I am striving to do well compared to other students
0.84
0.78
0.88
0.87
My aim is to perform relatively well relative to other students
0.90
“My goal is to perform better than the other students
0.90
Performance avoidance
My goal is to avoid performing poorly compared to other students
0.92
0.77
0.91
0.78
I am striving to avoid performing worse than other students
0.86
My aim is to avoid doing worse than other students
0.85
Notes. FL=factor loading, AVE=average variance extracted, CR=construct reliability, CA=Cronbach alpha
Table 3. The comparison of the level of achievement goals between new versus old student
Component
Group
Size
Grand
mean
S.Dev
Mean
difference
t-test for mean difference
t
α (1-tailed)
Mastery-approach
New students
350
5.53
0.96
0.20
2.37
0.009
Old students
203
5.33
0.98
Performance-approach
New students
350
5.49
1.06
0.37
4.25
0.000
Old students
203
5.12
0.94
Performance-avoidance
New students
350
5.36
1.02
0.08
1.03
0.153
Old students
203
5.27
0.88
Self-efficacy
New students
350
5.30
0.86
0.33
4.30
0.000
Old students
203
4.96
0.88
3.3. The influence of self-efficacy on achievement goals
The simple regression is utilized to investigate the influence of self-efficacy on achievement goals.
In both segments, the influence of self-efficacy on each achievement goal element is valid (p-value<0.000).
This result confirms the trichotomous model's structural validity in new students and the 2X2 model in the
old student segments. The criteria to measure an independent variable's determinant power to a dependent
variable are t-value, correlation, and determinant coefficient, as suggested by Hair, et al. [32]. In terms of
mastery-approach goals, the new student segment is higher than the old student segment according to the
standardize coefficient (0.761 vs. 0.384), t-value (21.873 vs. 5.898), and adjusted determinant coefficient
Int J Eval & Res Educ ISSN: 2252-8822
Achievement goals model validation: Is the 2X2 better than the Trichotomous? (Bilson Simamora)
147
(0.578 vs. 0.243). The same result is also found for performance-approach (β: 0.742 vs 0.409; t-value: 20.638
vs 6.347; adj. R2: 0.578 vs 0.243), and performance-avoidance (β: 0.742 vs 0.409; t-value: 20.638 vs 6.347;
adj. R2: 0.390 vs 0.108) goals. Therefore, the influence of self-efficacy on mastery-approach, performance-
approach, and performance-avoidance goals in the new student segment is stronger than in the old students'
segment (Table 4).
Table 4. The influence of self-efficacy on achievement goals in new and old students segments
Independent
variable
Dependent variable
New students (N=350)
Old students (N=203)
β
t-value
α#
Adj. R2
β
t-value
α#
Adj. R2
Self-efficacy
Mastery-approach
0.761
21.873
0.000
0.578
0.384
5.898
0.000
0.243
Performance-approach
0.742
20.638
0.000
0.550
0.409
6.347
0.000
0.163
Performance-avoidance
0.626
14.996
0.000
0.390
0.336
5.050
0.000
0.108
Notes. β=Standardized coefficient, #=1-tailed, *α<0.05.
4. DISCUSSION
This study reveals that the trichotomous model is valid for the new student segment, while the 2X2
model is valid for the old student segment. The absence of mastery-avoidance goals elements indicates that
new students common high achievement motivation and generally have no consideration about the risk of
failure in carrying the task. They demonstrated common care in mastering the task and satisfying ego goals
by showing off their performance [34]. Three related findings support this notion. First, mastery-approach
and performance-approach goals of the new students are higher than those of the old segments. These results
are underlined by the new student segment's higher self-efficacy, as found in previous studies [17, 27].
Second, new students have higher self-efficacy than the old one's segment. Third, the influence of self-
efficacy on achievement goals is more robust in the new students' segment than the old students' segment.
Goals orientation is a dynamic concept [35]. The success and failure in previous task
accomplishment [10, 35, 36], as well as the experienced situation in the classrooms [37, 38], are used to
redefine future goal orientations and intention. The old students have generally experienced the success and
failure in their study. The experiences are related to two points. First, concerning achievement goals setting,
the role of real efficacy increased, and the role of self-efficacy decreased as shown by its lower influence on
achievement goals elements. Second, the experiences enable the old students to refine their achievement
goals and think about the possibility of failure to master the teaching materials. This cognition gave birth to
the mastery-avoidance goals and confirms the validity of the 2X2 model. On the other hand, with their high
expectation, the new students tend to avoid thinking about the failure of mastering a task [28]. That is why
the trichotomous is more suitable for them, as found in this study.
The high expression of performance-approach goals among new students is vulnerable to negative
emotions. In a performance comparison term, success is a limited commodity. Being a loser has a higher
possibility than being a winner. Only a few students can achieve the position as the winners, and most are
end up as losers [34]. This negative emotion explains why so many students left their university or college,
especially in the small and private ones, in the early years of their study in Indonesia [39].
On the other hand, mastery-approach goals can hinder students' burn-out. When facing difficult
situations, students who pursue task goals view a problematic situation as a challenge, hold more optimistic
orientation, maintain positive affect, and implement problem-solving strategies [34]. Mastery approach goals
were positively associated with well-being [22], and well-being positively influences loyalty [22]. Therefore,
by continuously maintaining mastery-approach goals, an educational institution can maintain students’ loyalty.
5. CONCLUSION
The Trichotomous model is valid for the new student segment, and the 2X2 model is valid for the
old student segment. For the new students, achievement goals consist of mastery-approach, performance-
approach, and performance-avoidance goals. For the old student segment, besides those mentioned three
goals, the fourth goal, i.e., mastery-avoidance goals, is obvious. The new student segment has a higher
mastery-approach, and performance-approach goals than the old student segment has. Both segments have
the same level of performance-avoidance goals. The new student segment has higher self-efficacy than the
old student segment has. The influence of self-efficacy on mastery-approach, mastery-avoidance, and
performance-avoidance goals is higher in the new student segment than in the old student segment.
This research employed different sample size for the new and old student segments. With this
approach, we can conclude the difference of self-efficacy influence on achievement goals between the two
segments only judgmentally. To make a statistical decision about the difference in self-efficacy influence on
ISSN: 2252-8822
Int J Eval & Res Educ, Vol. 10, No. 1, March 2021: 142 - 149
148
achievement goals, the sample size between the two compared groups must be the same. Further research
canto considers this requirement.
This study used a single cross-sectional design in which the data are collected only once at a
particular point of time from two different populations. The potential differences between the two
populations’ characteristics reduce the confidence level of the conclusion. Further research can utilize a
longitudinal research design to enable data collection of the same populations at two or more different time
points. This design enables a more accurate comparison of the variables under investigation in a before and
after context. We use aggregate analysis in this study, in which we compared the two groups on an aggregate
base and made no segmental comparison that may generate more detailed results. Further research can consider
this issue and the involvement of gender, major, social class, or other potential moderating variables.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This study is supported financially and technically by Kwik Kian Gie School of Business and
Information Technology Research Center and Society Development.
REFERENCES
[1] S. Graham and B. Weiner, “Theories and Principles of Motivation,” in Handbook of Educational Psychology,
D.C. Berliner and R.C. Calfee Eds. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster McMillan, pp. 6384, 1996.
[2] P. Steel and C. J. Koeniq, “Integrating Theories of Motivation,” The Academy of Management Review, vol. 31,
no. 4, pp. 889-913, 2006.
[3] J. W. Atkinson, “Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior,” Psychological Review, vol. 64, no. 6,
pp. 359-372, 1957.
[4] J. Eccless, “Expectancies, values, and academic behaviors,” in J. T. Spence (Ed.) Achievement and achievement
motives: Psychological and sociological approaches. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman, pp. 75146, 1983.
[5] A. Bandura, “Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change,” Advances in Behavior Research and
Therapy, vol. 1, no. 4, pp. 139-161, 1978.
[6] D. H. Schunk, “Self-efficacy and academic motivation,” Educational Psychologist, vol. 26, no. 3-4, pp. 207-231,
1991, doi: 10.1080/00461520.1991.9653133.
[7] I. Ajzen, I., Madden, T. J., “Prediction of goal-directed behavior: Attitudes, intentions, and perceived behavioral
control,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 22, no. 5, pp. 453-474, 2019.
[8] M. Eskritt, J. Doucette, and L. Robitaille, “Does future-oriented thinking predict adolescent decision making?” The
Journal of Genetic Psychology, vol. 175, no. 2, pp. 163-179, 2014.
[9] A. J. Elliot, “Approach and avoidance motivation and achievement goals,” Educational Psychologist, vol. 34, no. 3,
pp. 169-189, 1999.
[10] S. Gould, F. Houston, and J. Mundt, “Failing to try to consume: A reversal of the usual consumer research
perspective,” Advances in Consumer Research, vol. 24, pp. 211-216, 1997.
[11] H. Evanschitzky, G. R. Iyer, H. Plassmann, J. Niessing, and H. Meffert, “The relative strength of affective commitment
in securing loyalty in service relationships,” Journal of Business Research, vol. 59, no. 12, pp. 1207-1213, 2006.
[12] M. D. Johnson, A. Herrmann, and F. Huber, “The evolution of loyalty intentions,” Journal of Marketing,
vol. 70, no. 2, pp. 122-132.
[13] J. G. Nicholls, “Achievement motivation: Conceptions of ability, subjective experience, task choice, and
performance,” Psychological Review, vol. 91, no. 3, pp. 328-346, 1984.
[14] S. Wimmer, H. K. Lackner, I. Papousek, and M. Paechter, “Goal orientation and activation of approach and avoidance
motivation while awating an achievement situation in the laboratory,Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 9, no. 1552, 2018,
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01552.
[15] J. S. Rosas, “Validation of the achievement goal questionnaire-Revised in Argentinean university students (A-
AGQ-R),” International Journal of Psychological Research, vol. 8, no. 1, pp. 10-23, 2015.
[16] A. Gegenfurtner and G. Hagenauer, “Achievement goals and achievement goal orientations ineducation,”
International Journal of Educational Research, vol. 61, no. 1, pp. 1-4, 2013.
[17] R. Brinkworth, B. McCann, C. Mattheuews, and K. Nordstrom, “First year expectations and experiences: student
and teacher perspectives,” Educational Psychologist, vol. 58, pp. 157-173, 2009, doi: 10.1007/s10734-008-9188-3.
[18] A. J. Eliot, and H. A. McGregor, “A 2×2 achievement goal framework,” Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, vol. 80, no. 3, pp. 501-519, 2001.
[19] C. Ames, “Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation,” Journal of Educational Psychology, vol. 84,
no. 3, pp. 261-271, 1992.
[20] A. J. Elliot and Harackiewicz, “Approach and avoidance achievement goals and intrinsic motivation: a mediational
analysis,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 70, no. 3, pp. 461-475, 1996.
[21] R. M. Korn and A. J. Elliot, “The 2 × 2 Standpoints Model of Achievement Goals,” Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 7,
no. 742, pp. 1-12, 2016.
[22] H. Tuominen-Soini, K. Salmela-Aro, and M. Niemivirta, “Achievement goal orientations and subjective well-
being: A person-centered analysis,” Learning and Instruction, vol. 18, no. 3, pp. 251-266, 2008.
Int J Eval & Res Educ ISSN: 2252-8822
Achievement goals model validation: Is the 2X2 better than the Trichotomous? (Bilson Simamora)
149
[23] C. Senco and A. M. Freud, “Are mastery-avoidance achievement goals always detrimental? An adult development
perspective,” Motivation and Emotion, vol. 39, no. 4, pp. 477-488, 2015.
[24] L. Tian, T. Yu, and E.S. Huebner, “Achievement goal orientations and adolescents’ subjective well-being in school:
The mediating roles of academic social comparison directions,” Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 8, no. 37, 2017, doi:
10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00037.
[25] A. Hofverberg and M. Winberg, “Challenging the Universality of Achievement Goal Models: A Comparison of Two
Culturally Distinct Countries,” Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, vol. 64, no. 3, pp. 333-354, 2020.
[26] C. Huang, “Achievement goals and self-efficacy: A meta-analysis,” Educational Research Review, vol. 19,
pp. 119-137, 2016, doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2016.07.002.
[27] S. Pather and N. Dorasamy, “The mismatch between first-year students’ expectations and experience alongside
university access and success: A South African University case study,” Journal of Student Affairs in Africa, vol. 6,
no. 1, pp. 49-64, 2018.
[28] S. Hassel and N. Ridout, “An Investigation of First-Year Students’ and Lecturers’ Expectations of University
Education,” Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 8, no. 2218, pp. 1-13, 2018.
[29] P. -H. Hsieh, J. R. Sullivan, and N. S. Guerra, “A closer look at college students: Self-efficacy and goal
Orientation,” Journal of Advanced Academics, vol. 18, no. 3, pp. 454-476, 2007.
[30] A.J. Elliot and K. Murayam, “On the measurement of achievement goals: Critique, illustration, and application,”
Journal of Educational Psychology, vol. 100, no. 3, pp. 613-628, 2008.
[31] P. R. Pintrich, D. A. F. Smith, T. Garcia, and W. J. Mc Keachie, “A manual for the use of the motivated strategies
for learning questionnaire (MSLQ),National Center for Research to Improve Postsecondary Teaching and
Learning, 1991. [Online]. Available: https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED338122.pdf
[32] J. Hair, W. Black, B. Babin, and R. Anderson, Multivariate data analysis. Upper Saddle River, NJ, 2014.
[33] N. K. Malhotra, Marketing Research an Applied Approach. Harlow, U.K: Pearson Education Limited, 2020.
[34] A. Kaplan, and M.L. Maehr, “Achievement goals and student well-being,” Contemporary Educational Psychology,
vol. 24, no. 4, pp. 330-358, 1999.
[35] R. P. Bagozzi and U. Dholakia “Goal setting and goal striving in consumer behavior,” Journal of Marketing,
vol. 63, pp. 19-32, 1999, doi: https://doi.org/10.2307/1252098.
[36] B.M. Kwan, A.D. Bryan, and P. Sheeran, “The dynamics of success and failure: how post-behaviour evaluations
relate to subsequent exercise intentions and behaviour,” Psychology & Health, vol. 33, no. 7, 2017, doi:
10.1080/08870446.2018.1429612.
[37] M.P. Hazley, D.F. Shell, L.-K. Soh, L.D. Miller, M. Chiriacescu, and E. Ingraham, “Changes in student goal
orientation across the semester in undergraduate computer science courses,” 2014 IEEE Frontiers in Education
Conference (FIE) Proceedings, Madrid, 2014, pp. 1-7.
[38] M. Fokkens-Bruinsma, E. C. M. Van Rooij, and E. T. Canrinus, “Perceived classroom goal structures as predictors
of students’ personal goals,” Teachers and Teaching, vol. 26, no. 1, pp. 88-102, 2020.
[39] B. A. Tejo, “Unemployed scholars and Indonesian education revolution (in Bahasa),” Detik.com, 2019. [Online].
Availabe: https://news.detik.com/kolom/d-4727746/sarjana-menganggur-dan-revolusi-pendidikan-tinggi.
... Baudoin and Galand (2020) documented the simultaneous consideration of the relevance of personal goals to classroom goal structures using multilevel models. Simamora and Mutiarawati (2021), in a single cross-sectional study, also confirmed that the 2x2 achievement goal model is better and more accurate for measuring goal orientations of old-timer students and that the trichotomous model is better for newer students. Other studies also confirmed that the validity and reliability of the 2x2 model (Awofala et al., 2013;Korn & Elliot, 2016;Ratsameemonthon, 2015;Sanchez, 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study attempted to discern the factor structure of the achievement goal orientation and goal structure constructs across the domain-specific task of essay writing in an Iranian EFL context. A convenience sample of 116 public university learners participated in a single-session, in-class study of an essay writing sampling and an immediate post-task inventory completion. Cronbach’s alpha reliability estimates for all the goal subscales ranged from .86 to .92, and the factor analysis results divulged that 33 items from the goal orientation and goal structure scale produced valid and reliable scores, with the seven-factor solution found to be well-fitted for the observed data. Principal component analysis revealed a two-factor model separating the goal subcomponent variables into a binary orientation and dichotomy of mastery and performance. Findings provided cultural and contextual support for the stability and applicability of the four-factor (2x2) achievement goal model; namely, mastery-approach, masteryavoidance, performance-approach, and performance-avoidance in specific domain of writing. The findings provided evidence that the three-factor goal structure is also invariant in the Iranian EFL context. All told, the findings imply the need for bridging the research gap on the interplay of person- and structure-related goals.
... Shi (2018) stated that students with a mastery approach have a lower tendency to procrastinate because they have the motivation to learn and study the lessons in schools. On the other hand, Mutiarawati and Simamora (2021) added that students with a mastery approach tend to be more optimistic when facing difficulties and view them as a challenge. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to analyze the relationship between self-efficacy and academic procrastination among tourism and hospitality programs in the new normal setting. Survey-questionnaires served as the primary instrument for data collection. It utilized frequency, percentage, and weighted mean to analyze the collected data. The findings revealed that the level of self-efficacy is very high and the level of academic procrastination is very low. Moreover, the result shows that there is a weak negative correlation between two variables. Hence, as the level of self-efficacy increases, the level of academic procrastination decreases.
Article
Full-text available
In a competency-based curriculum (CBC), the development of logical and critical thinking skills in mathematics is essential. As a cornerstone of Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics (STEM), mathematics is pivotal for addressing real-world challenges. However, overall pass rates remain unsatisfactory in Kenyan secondary schools, particularly in Kisii County. This study investigated the mediating role of academic disidentification in the relationship between achievement goals, perceived teacher support, and mathematics achievement. Grounded in the revised 3x2 Achievement Goal Model, Self-Determination Theory, and Expectancy-Value Theory, a correlational design was employed with a random sample of 418 Form Three students from thirty seven schools in Kisii County; Kenya in 2023. Multiple linear regression analysis revealed that academic disidentification partially mediates the relationship among achievement goals, perceived teacher support, and mathematics achievement. School categories differed significantly in avoidance motivation, with co-educational schools exhibiting the highest levels. Based on these findings, the study recommends that mathematics teachers prioritize fostering approach motivation, especially in co-educational schools, to enhance student achievement in mathematics.
Article
Full-text available
This study focused on the question of how to enhance a mastery-goal orientation in the classroom. We started from the perspective of the achievement goal theory, which assumes that the goals that students have (personal goals) relate to the goals that are set in the classroom (classroom goal structures). In classrooms where teachers focus on learning and effort -instead of normative standards and social comparison- mastery goals are enhanced. This type of classroom can be endorsed by focusing on the following classroom structures: Task design, Autonomy, Recognition, Grouping, Evaluation, and Time (TARGET). The present study investigated how classroom goal structures as perceived by students are related to students’ personal goals. Based on survey data from 501 students from three secondary schools in the Netherlands, we found that the Task design dimension, concerning designing challenging and varied tasks, and the Time dimension, concerning e.g., pacing, predicted both mastery-approach and avoidance goals. Our findings provide insight in how perceived classroom goal structures are related to students’ personal goals that focus on understanding/competence, thus informing teachers and educational developers where to start when it comes to improving student motivation.
Article
Full-text available
Achievement goal theory is one of the most widespread motivation models within education research. Strong empirical support exists for the trichotomous model, comprising mastery-approach, performance-approach, and performance-avoidance goals. However, research also indicates problems with model transferability between contexts. In this study, based on questionnaire data from 4201 students, we use confirmatory factor analysis to compare the factor structures of students’ achievement goals in two culturally distinct countries. Factor structures for Grades 5–11 within the two countries were also compared. Results show that the separation between performance-approach and performance-avoidance goals differs between the two countries, and that this difference is consistent over the grades. Hence, results indicate that the model is not freely transferable between countries. The results are discussed in relation to differences in national culture and other proposed explanations such as age, perceived competence, and questionnaire characteristics.
Article
Full-text available
While some students try to give their best in an achievement situation, others show disengagement and just want to get the situation over and done with. The present study investigates the role of students’ tendencies for approach or avoidance motivation while anticipating tasks and the corresponding activation of the approach/avoidance motivational system as indicated by transient changes of EEG alpha asymmetry. Overall, 62 students (50 female; age: M = 23.8, SD = 3.5) completed a goal orientation questionnaire (learning goals, performance-approach, performance-avoidance, and work avoidance). They joined a laboratory experiment where EEG was recorded during resting condition as well as when students were anticipating tasks. Standard multiple regression analysis showed that higher values on performance-avoidance were related to a higher activation of the approach system whereas higher values on work avoidance were related to a higher activation of the avoidance system. Results question present assumptions about avoidance related goal orientations.
Article
Full-text available
Abstract The widening of access into higher education institutions in South Africa has rapidly transformed the student population to become more diverse. Students vary in age, race, culture, backgrounds, educational experiences, academic potential and university expectations. Widening university access with the commensurate need for success requires intervention mechanisms to ensure university management addresses student challenges, especially at first-year undergraduate level. Access and success cannot be achieved without understanding students’ university expectations and experiences, as these are critical factors that are integrated with retention and success. This paper examines the gap between students’ expectation and experience and argues that the intensity of such a gap can negatively impact the goal of achieving access and success amongst students from diverse backgrounds. The study utilised a pre- and post-survey to collect quantitative data from 95 first-year teacher education students at a university of technology in South Africa. The results indicate that there is a significant gap between students’ expectations and their actual university experience with regard to the following indicators: social engagement, academic engagement and seeking academic support. It is posited that such a mismatch between students’ university expectations and experience can result in students feeling disconnected to the institution, which could lead to academic failure and high drop-out rates. This study recommends that an intentionally planned first-year experience programme is required to entrench a more inclusive and sustainable first-year experience for ‘all students’ which could close the gap between students’ expectation and experience and access and success.
Article
Full-text available
Transition from school to university can cause concern for many students. One issue is the gap between students' prior expectations and the realities of university life, which can cause significant distress, poor academic performance and increased drop-out rates if not managed effectively. Research has shown several similarities in the expectations of staff and students in regards to which factors determine academic success, but there is also evidence of dissonance. For example, staff consider independent study and critical evaluation as key factors, whereas students view feedback on drafts of work and support from staff as being most important. The aim of the current study was to determine what expectations students hold when starting university education, and what expectations university lecturers have of students entering university. Lecturers (n = 20) and first year students (n = 77) completed a series of questionnaires concerning their expectations of learning in HE (staff and students) and their approach to teaching (staff). Results revealed that students have largely realistic expectations of university. For example, the majority expected to be in charge of their own study. Some unrealistic expectations were also evident, e.g., most expected that teaching would be the same at university as it had been at school. The expectation that lecturers would provide detailed notes varied as a function of student age. Lecturers reported modifying their expectations of students and adapting their teaching approach according to year of study. Information-transmission/teacher-focused style was more common when teaching 1st year students; a more concept-changing/student-focused approach tended to be used when teaching 2nd year students (and above). Lecturer's expectations of student engagement did not differ according to year. Less experienced lecturers reported more negative expectations of student engagement than did experienced lecturers. In line with previous work, we observed overlap in expectations of staff and students, but some clear differences too.
Article
Full-text available
Presents an integrative theoretical framework to explain and to predict psychological changes achieved by different modes of treatment. This theory states that psychological procedures, whatever their form, alter the level and strength of self-efficacy. It is hypothesized that expectations of personal efficacy determine whether coping behavior will be initiated, how much effort will be expended, and how long it will be sustained in the face of obstacles and aversive experiences. Persistence in activities that are subjectively threatening but in fact relatively safe produces, through experiences of mastery, further enhancement of self-efficacy and corresponding reductions in defensive behavior. In the proposed model, expectations of personal efficacy are derived from 4 principal sources of information: performance accomplishments, vicarious experience, verbal persuasion, and physiological states. Factors influencing the cognitive processing of efficacy information arise from enactive, vicarious, exhortative, and emotive sources. The differential power of diverse therapeutic procedures is analyzed in terms of the postulated cognitive mechanism of operation. Findings are reported from microanalyses of enactive, vicarious, and emotive modes of treatment that support the hypothesized relationship between perceived self-efficacy and behavioral changes. (21/2 p ref)
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study was to examine the multiple mediational roles of academic social comparison directions (upward academic social comparison and downward academic social comparison) on the relationships between achievement goal orientations (i.e., mastery goals, performance-approach goals, and performance-avoidance goals) and subjective well-being (SWB) in school (school satisfaction, school affect) in adolescent students in China. A total of 883 Chinese adolescent students (430 males; Mean age = 12.99) completed a multi-measure questionnaire. Structural equation modeling was used to examine the hypotheses. Results indicated that (1) mastery goal orientations and performance-approach goal orientations both showed a statistically significant, positive correlation with SWB in school whereas performance-avoidance goal orientations showed a statistically significant, negative correlation with SWB in school among adolescents; (2) upward academic social comparisons mediated the relation between the three types of achievement goal orientations (i.e., mastery goals, performance-approach goals, and performance-avoidance goals) and SWB in school; (3) downward academic social comparisons mediated the relation between mastery goal orientations and SWB in school as well as the relation between performance-avoidance goal orientations and SWB in school. The findings suggest possible important cultural differences in the antecedents of SWB in school in adolescent students in China compared to adolescent students in Western nations.
Article
Full-text available
In the present research, we proposed and tested a 2 × 2 standpoints model of achievement goals grounded in the development-demonstration and approach-avoidance distinctions. Three empirical studies are presented. Study 1 provided evidence supporting the structure and psychometric properties of a newly developed measure of the goals of the 2 × 2 standpoints model. Study 2 documented the predictive utility of these goal constructs for intrinsic motivation: development-approach and development-avoidance goals were positive predictors, and demonstration-avoidance goals were a negative predictor of intrinsic motivation. Study 3 documented the predictive utility of these goal constructs for performance attainment: Demonstration-approach goals were a positive predictor and demonstration-avoidance goals were a negative predictor of exam performance. The conceptual and empirical contributions of the present research were discussed within the broader context of existing achievement goal theory and research.
Article
Objective: Exercise behaviour change involves multiple experiences with success and failure. The Model of Action Phases (MAP) offers a dynamic account of how success and failure influence both immediate evaluations and future decisions and actions. However, predictions from the MAP have not been formally tested. Design: A longitudinal daily diary study was used to examine how post-behaviour evaluations of exercise success and failure influence subsequent exercise intentions and behaviour. Participants (N = 104) set exercise goals, and then kept a daily online exercise diary for four weeks. Main outcome measures: Participants self-reported exercise behaviour, affective response to exercise, self-evaluations after success or failure at following through on intentions to exercise, and intentions to exercise in the next week. Results: Multilevel modelling revealed significant within- and between-participant relationships among post-behaviour evaluations, intentions and subsequent behaviour. Findings supported MAP-derived predictions about how success and failure at exercise are associated with feelings about exercise and the self, and inform subsequent exercise intentions and behaviour. Conclusion: Positive post-behaviour evaluations of success or failure may stabilise positive intentions and aid maintenance of exercise behaviour. Implications of these MAP-based findings for intervention design are discussed. Access full text here: https://doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2018.1429612
Article
This meta-analysis examined the relations between achievement goals and self-efficacy. One hundred and twenty-five studies consisting of 148 samples (N = 61,456) reporting the relations between academic achievement goals and academic self-efficacy were included. The correlations of mastery and mastery approach goals with self-efficacy were generally moderate to strong, while those of performance avoidance and mastery avoidance goals with self-efficacy were low. Goal valence was meaningfully related to self-efficacy, whereas the support for the goal definition was inconsistent. Publication status, proportion of males, mean age, and achievement goal measure did not exert significant moderating effects, whereas those for country where the research was conducted, the proportion of Caucasians, the self-efficacy measure, the domains of achievement goals and self-efficacy, and matching between achievement goal and self-efficacy domains varied with the achievement goal factor. The four-factor model was based on a relatively small number of samples, and so future research is needed to determine whether there are differences in correlations of mastery avoidance and performance avoidance goals with antecedents and consequences.