ArticlePublisher preview available

The corrective social function of performative comedy demonstrated on two instances

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the author.

Abstract

I argue that performative comedy can be used to apply the corrective social function of humour to achieve a new awareness in people regarding social issues, in this case racism. Comedians are usually marginalized in some way. Some sort of Otherness characterizes their view of the world. Standing in the margins gives comics a unique view of the world, and in particular, a special view of what made them other. Performative comedy is comedians performing their marginalization. They, by being marginalized, are socially licenced to subvert and critique social norms. When they perform these subversions for us, and make us laugh, usually at ourselves, our laughter is revelatory. What is revealed is that subverted or critiqued norm. To see this social ill through the comic's eyes is revelatory. We see with new eyes. Our laughter signals the change to new thinking, the corrected thinking. Laughter is special in that we only laugh at comedy we think is true in some sense.
Vol.:(0123456789)
SN Soc Sci (2021) 1:76
https://doi.org/10.1007/s43545-021-00074-0
ORIGINAL PAPER
The corrective social function ofperformative comedy
demonstrated ontwo instances
RobinTapley1
Received: 9 August 2020 / Accepted: 31 January 2021 / Published online: 1 March 2021
© The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Nature Switzerland AG part of Springer Nature 2021
Abstract
I argue that performative comedy can be used to apply the corrective social function
of humour to achieve a new awareness in people regarding social issues, in this case
racism. Comedians are usually marginalized in some way. Some sort of Otherness
characterizes their view of the world. Standing in the margins gives comics a unique
view of the world, and in particular, a special view of what made them other. Per-
formative comedy is comedians performing their marginalization. They, by being
marginalized, are socially licenced to subvert and critique social norms. When they
perform these subversions for us, and make us laugh, usually at ourselves, our laugh-
ter is revelatory. What is revealed is that subverted or critiqued norm. To see this
social ill through the comic’s eyes is revelatory. We see with new eyes. Our laughter
signals the change to new thinking, the corrected thinking. Laughter is special in
that we only laugh at comedy we think is true in some sense.
Keywords Performative comedy· Comedy· Humour· Marginality· Corrective
humour· Social function· Function of humour
Introduction
Humour, laughing, and comedy are inherently social. We rarely laugh alone. Moreo-
ver, humour and laughing are uniquely human. While humour and comedy serve
many functions and show up in our lives in many ways, I want to focus on one
particular function of comedy: the corrective social function. It is well known in
humour studies that humour can serve to correct someone’s behaviour. A jest made
in good fun about someone’s bad acts will likely bring the person to heel and do it
more quickly and with better results than a serious remark. I am talking, however,
about using a corrective function on a social level for social problems. I am suggest-
ing that we might employ this corrective function of comedy on social ills such as
* Robin Tapley
rtapley@tru.ca
1 Thompson Rivers University, Philosophy, Kamloops, BC, Canada
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
An experiment was conducted to test the hypothesis that stereotypical television portrayals of African-Americans increase the likelihood that whites will make negative social perception judgments of an African-American (but not a white) target person. Forty white subjects were exposed to comedy skits featuring stereotypical or netural portrayals of African-American characters. Subjects then read a vignette describing an incident in which a college student was allegedly assaulted by his rommate. In half of the conditions, the alleged offender was assumed to be white; in the other half he was assumed to be African-American. Subjects rated the likelihood that the alleged offender was guilty of the assault. Guilt ratings of the white target did not differ significantly between the stereotypical and the neutral comedy skit conditions. In contrast, guilt ratings of the African-American target were higher in the stereotypical comedy skit condition than in the neutral comedy skit condition.
Article
Humor is a pervasive feature of human life which crosses racial, cultural, sexual, and class divisions. Yet its nature is elusive. This elusiveness should have piqued the philosophical imagination; its pervasiveness should have demonstrated its philosophical importance. However, it has generated relatively little theoretical interest. We find that surprising. An analysis of humor could pay handsome dividends. Practically, it could inform aspects of the current debate over political correctness. Theoretically, it could illuminate discussion of significant issues in epistemology, the philosophy of mind, and ethics.
Article
Class discussions of White privilege can increase sociocultural and international awareness in our students. Therefore, instructors should be encouraged to teach this topic in introductory college survey courses (e.g., psychology and sociology), although negative reactions by White students can affect student evaluations of an instructor’s teaching skills. We decided to further explore college students’ cognitive and emotional reactions to this topic. Principal components analysis revealed four factors. Agreement with items related to the factor Societal (e.g., American society is not a meritocracy) was associated with understanding White privilege. Agreement with items associated with the factor Personal (e.g., feeling personally attacked) was associated with failure to understand this concept. The remaining two factors, Denial and The Bad Guys (feeling like one of “the bad guys” in society), were not significantly associated with understanding White privilege. These findings are discussed in the context of successfully teaching antiracism.
Article
This article outlines the racist rhetoric employed in anti-black jokes on five internet websites. It is argued that racist jokes can act as important rhetorical devices for serious racisms, and thus work in ways that can support racism in particular readings. By offering a rhetorical discourse analysis of jokes containing embodied racism – or the discursive remains of biological racism – it is shown that internet jokes express two key logics of racism. These logics are inclusion and exclusion. It is argued that inclusion usually inferiorizes and employs race stereotypes whereas exclusion often does not. The article expands this second category by highlighting exclusionary ‘black’ and ‘nigger’ jokes. These categories of non-stereotyped race or ethnic joking have been largely ignored in humour studies because of a reliance on a problematic and celebratory definition of the ethnic joke. Thus a wider definition of racist humour is offered.
Article
Analysis of spontaneous joke-telling performances from recordings of natural conversation shows that they elicit laughter at various junctures, not just after the final punch line. Some joke texts contain multiple punch lines or funny passages in their set up, but much of the laughter during the telling of a joke is keyed to other features of the joke text or to the performance itself. In particular, joke recipients often laugh in response to the mere preface of a joke or to the initial line of exposition in the introduction. Recipients may also laugh at what they take to be the punch line of a joke. Moreover, groups of listeners tend to coordinate their laughter at specific points in the performance. These observations have consequences for the definition and subclassification of joke texts. They also reveal listening practices relevant for recipients of jokes in conversation.