ArticlePDF Available

Why Parents Misuse Prescription Drugs to Enhance the Cognitive Performance of Healthy Children: The Influence of Peers and Social Media

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The mechanisms affecting parents’ misuse of prescription stimulant drugs to boost healthy children’s school performance are hardly unknown. Using four web-based factorial vignette surveys (2×2 between-subjects design experiment), we investigated the willingness of U.S. parents with school-aged children to medicate a fictitious 13-year-old child whose grades had declined. We examined mechanisms of informational and normative social influence on their decision-making: others’ behavior ( N Experiment 1 = 359), others’ definitions ( N Experiment 2 = 326), social control ( N Experiment 3 = 325), and others’ experience ( N Experiment 4 = 313). In addition, we explored the moderating role of influential sources (close friends vs. social media). Parents were more willing to engage in said behavior when others reported engagement in this behavior or positive drug experiences, especially if both influences were transmitted via social media. Others’ definitions and social control had no effect. Thus, social media might be a channel for the prevention of pharmacological cognitive enhancement.
Content may be subject to copyright.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022042621994547
Journal of Drug Issues
1 –22
© The Author(s) 2021
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/0022042621994547
journals.sagepub.com/home/jod
Original Article
Why Parents Misuse Prescription
Drugs to Enhance the Cognitive
Performance of Healthy Children:
The Influence of Peers and
Social Media
Sebastian Sattler1,2 , Guido Mehlkop3, Vanessa Bahr3,
and Cornelia Betsch3
Abstract
The mechanisms affecting parents’ misuse of prescription stimulant drugs to boost healthy
children’s school performance are hardly unknown. Using four web-based factorial vignette
surveys (2×2 between-subjects design experiment), we investigated the willingness of U.S.
parents with school-aged children to medicate a fictitious 13-year-old child whose grades had
declined. We examined mechanisms of informational and normative social influence on their
decision-making: others’ behavior (NExperiment 1 = 359), others’ definitions (NExperiment 2 = 326),
social control (NExperiment 3 = 325), and others’ experience (NExperiment 4 = 313). In addition, we
explored the moderating role of influential sources (close friends vs. social media). Parents
were more willing to engage in said behavior when others reported engagement in this behavior
or positive drug experiences, especially if both influences were transmitted via social media.
Others’ definitions and social control had no effect. Thus, social media might be a channel for
the prevention of pharmacological cognitive enhancement.
Keywords
cognitive enhancement, substance misuse, descriptive norm, injunctive norm, social influence
Introduction
If other children at school were hypothetically taking prescription drugs to increase cognitive
functions such as concentration or memory without medical necessity, approximately every third
respondent in a nonrepresentative survey (conducted by the magazine Nature among their
[potential] readers) would feel pressure to give such drugs to their child (Maher, 2008). Such
nonmedical use of prescription drugs has gained growing academic interest in recent years
(d’Angelo et al., 2017; Heyes & Boardley, 2019; O’Connor & Nagel, 2017; Teter et al., 2020).
Candidate drugs for this so-called “cognitive enhancement” (CE) include methylphenidate,
1University of Cologne, Köln, Germany
2Institut de recherches cliniques de Montréal, Quebec, Canada
3University of Erfurt, Germany
Corresponding Author:
Sebastian Sattler, Institute of Sociology and Social Psychology, University of Cologne, Universitaetsstrasse 24, 50931
Köln, Germany.
Email: sattler@wiso.uni-koeln.de
994547JODXXX10.1177/0022042621994547Journal of Drug IssuesSattler et al.
research-article2021
2 Journal of Drug Issues 00(0)
modafinil, and amphetamines. These drugs are usually prescribed to treat attention-deficit hyper-
activity disorder (ADHD), sleep disorders, or narcolepsy.
A recent study among U.S. high school students found that 2.5% self-reported CE for study
purposes (Teter et al., 2020). Among the several hundred thousand adolescents (aged 12–17) in
the United States who misused prescription stimulants (Substance Abuse and Mental Health
Services Administration, 2019), cognitive performance enhancement is one key motive (Schepis
et al., 2020). The prevalence rates of CE that have been assessed in different populations (e.g.,
students or employed people) vary greatly (Maier & Schaub, 2015; Sattler, 2016; Smith & Farah,
2011). Recent research, however, found indications for increasing prevalence rates in several
countries (Maier et al., 2018). Currently, little is known about parents’ administration of drugs to
enhance the cognitive performance of their children (Sattler, 2020a). According to a survey with
parents from the United States, 1% of parents of children aged 13 to 17 (who had not been pre-
scribed stimulants to treat ADHD) assumed that their children use “study aids,” and another 4%
were not sure (C.S. Mott Children’s Hospital National Poll on Children’s Health, 2013).
Most research investigating the etiology of CE has involved college and university students,
that is, persons using the drugs for themselves (reviews: Caviola & Faber, 2015; Ragan et al.,
2013; Schelle et al., 2014). We aim to explore the decision-making process of parents to give
these drugs to their children (here defined as 18 years and below) without medical necessity
because it can be assumed that at least for younger children, parents may contribute to children’s
CE. Parents are important stakeholders in decisions regarding whether a child will engage in CE
(Sattler & Wörn, 2019) because children depend on their parents’ stewardship (Coleman, 1994).
A certain proportion of parents seem to have high expectations toward their children’s school
performance, driven by the aim of improving their children’s prospects or own status motives,
often together with competitive thinking (Doepke et al., 2019; Nadesan, 2002; Rasmussen &
Troilo, 2016). This might make some parents tempted to administer CE. Moreover, parents are
also the gatekeepers of drugs, and as they are often role models, their attitudes may impact their
children’s attitudes and potential substance use subsequently in life (Epstein et al., 2007). While
researchers forecasted a rise of CE in children (Colaneri et al., 2018), also “because parents want
the best for their child” (O’Connor & Nagel, 2017, p. 5), parents fear such a rise (Forlini &
Racine, 2009; Hiltrop & Sattler, under review).
Factors that may facilitate an increase or prevent a decrease in CE include a high availability
of drugs due to more (invalid) diagnoses, competitive admission criteria and grading, increased
performance and peer pressure in schools, parents who may convince physicians that their chil-
dren have ADHD when they are not satisfied with their child’s grades or behavior, or the spread
of information through online sources (Conrad & Bergey, 2014; ExpressScripts, 2014; Forlini &
Racine, 2009; King et al., 2014; McCabe et al., 2005; Singh et al., 2013). Given this, parents
might be susceptible to rationalizing CE as an instrument to achieve their goals (Arria & DuPont,
2010), similar to engagement in other intensive parenting practices such as private tutoring
(Wells et al., 2016).
Such developments should also consider the serious criticisms that CE in children raised due
to its individual and social consequences (Colaneri et al., 2018; Graf et al., 2013; Nagel, 2019;
Sattler & Singh, 2016). These criticisms include concerns about fairness, side effects, social dis-
advantages, and peer pressure. Thus, gaining insight into how parents decide for or against such
drugs is crucial for prevention and regulation policies and to protect children from harm.
However, limited research has investigated parents’ motivations and goals regarding CE (Ball &
Wolbring, 2014; Forlini & Racine, 2009; Sattler, 2020b; Sattler & Wörn, 2019; Smith & Farah,
2011). The initial, mainly qualitative research, however, indicates that parents would administer
CE to children under certain conditions (Ball & Wolbring, 2014; Cutler, 2014). They would
surprisingly do so, despite no clinical research on the effectiveness of CE drugs in children
(Sattler & Singh, 2016). Reviews for research in healthy adults, however, indicated moderate
Sattler et al. 3
enhancement effects (Battleday & Brem, 2015; Caviola & Faber, 2015), whereby the first clinical
research with healthy college students suggests mixed objective but strong subjective effects
(Weyandt et al., 2018). Therefore, to judge safety and effectiveness, parents must rely on their
personal experience or information from other sources. Thus, when parents lack personal experi-
ence, peers or media reports might play an important role in this process. Qualitative and survey
research shows that social influences play a role in parents’ decisions to (hypothetically) give
children CE drugs (Forlini & Racine, 2009; Maher, 2008; Sattler & Wörn, 2019), but research on
the causal role of social influences is missing.
We aim to close this gap by performing four factorial survey experiments investigating the
effect of social influence on parents’ decisions to give CE drugs to children.
Theory: The Importance of Social Influence Processes
Social influence through peer groups and social media is an important driver of individual behav-
ior. Several studies document social influences on various health-related behaviors or motiva-
tions, such as vaccination (Betsch et al., 2010), eating (Higgs & Thomas, 2016), doping in sports
(Kabiri et al., 2019), or legal and illegal substance use (Borsari & Carey, 2009; Napper et al.,
2016). Some researchers assume that peers’ behavior and norms are even more important for
actual behavior than individuals’ personal attitudes (Keuschnigg & Kratz, 2017; LaBrie, Hummer,
Neighbors, et al., 2010).
In our study, we aim to test the assumptions embedded in social learning theory (Akers &
Sellers, 2013; Ford & Ong, 2014; Peralta & Steele, 2010; Watkins, 2016) and the social norms
approach (Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004; Napper et al., 2016), which describe several mechanisms
underlying how others influence one’s behavior. These mechanisms are applicable not only to
explaining social influences from friends or other peers but also to social influences from the
internet and social media, which is important since medical information from online sources has
gained popularity (Betsch et al., 2010; Elmore et al., 2017). Based on a classical typology by
Deutsch and Gerard (1955), social influences can be grouped into informational and normative
influences. Information can be derived from interactions with others and by observing the actions
and experiences of others. This might result in the imitation (of role models) and reinforcement
of a behavior (Akers & Sellers, 2013). Relying on others might be especially important for the
onset of new behavior (Cochran et al., 2017). Thus, following others or deriving information
from their behavior and via interactions can be an adaptive strategy (Hertwig & Hoffrage, 2013)
because acquiring information privately can be more costly (e.g., time consuming or dangerous)
(Webster & Laland, 2008). Information from others can also reinforce one’s behavior because it
provides insight regarding its reward and cost structure. However, others can also be the source
of incorrect or dangerous information (Betsch et al., 2015; Epstein et al., 2007).
Normative influences can also be based on others’ actions in terms of indirectly signaling their
definitions of right and wrong, i.e., their normative expectations to conform, or directly by oth-
ers’ thoughts and speech (i.e., speaking about their definitions), and whether they reinforce
behavior by exercising social control (Akers & Sellers, 2013; Deutsch & Gerard, 1955; Napper
et al., 2016; Peralta & Steele, 2010).
These informational and normative influence processes can be direct, subtle, indirect, and
outside of awareness. Several processes of this influence have been described in the literature
related to substance use, including alcohol, opioids, and the nonmedical use of prescription drugs
(Borsari & Carey, 2009; e.g., Maahs et al., 2016; Napper et al., 2016; Watkins, 2016). For exam-
ple, research has found that the onset of the latter is driven by exposure to other users and learn-
ing about their positive and negative drug experiences (Mui et al., 2014). Minimal research on
social influence, however, exists on CE. However, since individuals often may have minimal
personal knowledge regarding CE, they may have an incentive to rely on others. Hence, social
4 Journal of Drug Issues 00(0)
influences and anecdotal evidence might play a crucial role in forming attitudes toward CE
(London-Nadeau et al., 2019).
In our factorial survey experiments, we assess the influence of others, namely, others’ behav-
ior (Experiment 1), others’ definitions, (Experiment 2), others’ control (Experiment 3), and oth-
ers’ experience (Experiment 4). In each experiment, we also examine whether social influence
differs depending on whether it occurs via face-to-face interactions with close friends or social
media. The following paragraphs present the hypotheses related to each factor.
Others’ Behavior
Individuals can directly learn via interactions with others (i.e., through differential associations
with others) (Ford & Ong, 2014; Peralta & Steele, 2010; Watkins, 2016). Individuals can also
indirectly derive information from others’ (typical) behavior since their “doing” serves as a
descriptive norm (Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004; Napper et al., 2016) that may lead to imitation
(Akers & Sellers, 2013). Moreover, exposure to others’ behavior can signal their attitudes and
normative evaluations. For example, if peers use CE, observing this can serve as behavioral con-
firmation of others’ acceptance of CE and others’ benefit-risk ratio for these drugs, thereby
revealing the preferences of peers (Borsari & Carey, 2009; Ford & Ong, 2014; Sattler, 2020a;
Sattler et al., 2014; Watkins, 2016). Knowing or believing that others engage in deviant behavior
can help neutralize or justify one’s own deviant behavior, such as by reducing personal responsi-
bility (Cutler, 2014; Heyes & Boardley, 2019). Such knowledge may also produce conformity
pressure or indirect coercion to behave similarly to avoid falling behind (Forlini & Racine, 2009;
Sattler et al., 2014; Sattler & Wörn, 2019). Previous focus group research found that parents and
health care providers are aware of peer pressure if students use CE (Forlini & Racine, 2009).
Although this knowledge rendered the parents sad and worried, the health care providers assumed
that peer pressure led to higher demands and increased the need to use CE. While another qualita-
tive study found that some parents did not react to external pressures (Ball & Wolbring, 2014), a
survey showed that classmates’ drug use would exert pressure to also medicate their children
(Maher, 2008). Similarly, survey research (Maier et al., 2016b; Molloy et al., 2019) and experi-
mental studies investigating CE in populations other than children (with hypothetical scenarios)
provide evidence of contagion effects (London-Nadeau et al., 2019; Sattler et al., 2014).
Therefore, we predict that the higher the number of others who engage their children in CE, the
greater the willingness to give CE to a child (others’ behavior hypothesis).
Others’ Definitions
By observing others’ behavior, individuals might indirectly infer the moral acceptability of this
behavior, but information regarding its moral status can also be directly communicated. Social
learning theory discusses how the moral status of a behavior affects decision-making through
definitions. These definitions are an individual’s own beliefs about or moral judgments of whether
a behavior is right or wrong (Cutler, 2014; LaBrie, Hummer, Lac, et al., 2010). Such definitions
include neutralization techniques with regard to whether the violation of norms can be justified.
The perception of the definitions of others’ has been termed injunctive norms in the social norms
approach (Napper et al., 2016). These norms can influence an individual’s definitions, since
knowledge of others’ definitions can help an individual understand a situation and deduce infor-
mation regarding which behavior is appropriate, expected, or justified, and this knowledge can
help defuse shame and guilt if the individual engages in deviant behavior. This knowledge may
also result in an individual conforming to others’ views (Napper et al., 2016) by aligning both
attitudes and behavior, which might be achieved to obtain approval from others (Cialdini &
Goldstein, 2004). However, whether parents’ CE decisions are affected by others’ moral
Sattler et al. 5
considerations remains an unanswered question. Only the study by Forlini and Racine (2009)
found that parents are receptive to the acceptance of CE among peers. We predict that the more
others find the use of CE in children morally acceptable, the greater the willingness to give CE
to a child (others’ definitions hypothesis).
Others’ Control
Knowing the degree to which others accept a behavior might be of less relevance to individuals’
behavior than whether others turn words into action in the form of social control through rein-
forcement. Others may make the effort and show (dis)approval to enforce a social norm by apply-
ing informal incentives or punishments that are either beneficial or costly to those targeted (Akers
& Sellers, 2013; Sattler et al., 2014; Watkins, 2016). Some studies have found that recommenda-
tions from friends to take CE (positive reinforcement) are rare (Maier et al., 2016b) and that only
a few individuals would use CE if employers recommended it (Franke et al., 2012). Regarding
pediatric CE, in two qualitative studies, parents said that they would lose respect for other parents
who pressure their children to take drugs for enhancement (Ball & Wolbring, 2014; Hiltrop &
Sattler, under review). Studies involving students demonstrate that hypothetical disapproval
reduces the willingness to use CE (Sattler et al., 2014), while expected approval increases the
intention to use CE (Ponnet et al., 2015). In an era of online hate speech and “shitstorms” as
extreme examples of others’ control and generally negative feedback that can be provided anony-
mously online, it is important to study whether others’ critiques voiced via social media affect
parents’ CE decisions. Here, we predict that the more strongly others show disapproval (e.g., by
criticizing) of using CE among children, the lower the willingness to give CE to a child (others’
control hypothesis).
Others’ Experiences
While observing others’ behavior is a direct influence process in the case of imitation and a more
indirect influence process when information from others’ behavior must be inferred, there is also
the possibility that individuals hear directly about others’ evaluations of and experience with a
certain behavior. Information about others’ experiences with CE can be a primary source of rein-
forcement for this behavior in addition to reinforcement via social control (Cochran et al., 2017;
London-Nadeau et al., 2019). Such vicarious experiences of a behavior include its perceived
benefits and costs; in the context of drugs, there are desired effects and unwanted side effects
(Akers & Sellers, 2013; Ford & Ong, 2014; Peralta & Steele, 2010; Sattler et al., 2014; Watkins,
2016). Schelle et al. (2014) assume that it is not others’ behavior per se that matters but rather
whether that behavior leads to positive results, thereby creating pressure to use prescription stim-
ulant drugs. This emergence of implicit pressure has also been described in qualitative research
as a reason for initiating CE use (Heyes & Boardley, 2019). Concerns regarding side effects are
mirrored in studies involving parents, who perceive CE as emotionally and physically harmful
and want to avoid harming children (Ball & Wolbring, 2014; Forlini & Racine, 2009). However,
no study has investigated the influence of personally or vicariously experienced side effects in
this context. We predict that the higher others rate the positive effects of a drug relative to the
negative effects when giving CE to children, the greater the willingness to give CE to a child
(others’ experience hypothesis).
Influence Source
The described forms of influence can occur through different sources, such as relatively proximal
or more distal reference groups (Cochran et al., 2017; Napper et al., 2016). Influences from
6 Journal of Drug Issues 00(0)
proximal others might mainly occur via direct interactions (Akers & Sellers, 2013; Cochran
et al., 2017; LaBrie, Hummer, Neighbors, et al., 2010). Influences from distal others might more-
frequently occur indirectly through media or identification with these distant others. In line with
social comparison theory (Festinger, 1954) and social impact theory (Latané, 1981), some
researchers assume that more proximal reference groups are more influential than distant refer-
ence groups (LaBrie, Hummer, Neighbors, et al., 2010; Napper et al., 2016) and; thus, the learn-
ing of certain behavior mainly occurs in intimate personal groups rather than via “interpersonal
agencies of communication, such as movies and newspapers” (Sutherland et al., 1995, p. 67).
However, influences from distal groups might exist even if these others are not directly
observed because people can learn about others’ actual or planned behavior by consulting the
internet (Hertwig & Hoffrage, 2013). Thus, media represent a source of influence from distant
reference groups (Akers & Sellers, 2013; Watkins, 2016) and searching for health or medical
information is among the most popular online activities (Fox & Fallows, 2003). Therefore, how
the media, especially the internet, including social media, can influence CE behavior remains a
highly relevant but still open question. Media outlets continuously cover the topic of CE; the
amount of related content on the internet is increasing, and the impact of media, including inter-
net forums, on CE perceptions and use has been previously expected but not tested (Conrad &
Bergey, 2014; Racine & Forlini, 2010; Vargo & Petróczi, 2016). Research in other areas indicates
that information obtained from internet sources can affect health-related behavior and perceived
social norms because users of such sources learn about others’ reported behavior, experiences,
reasoning, and (dis-)approval, such as drinking alcohol, smoking, or vaccination (Betsch et al.,
2010; Elmore et al., 2017; Moreno & Whitehill, 2014), which can have unfavorable effects
(Betsch et al., 2013; Elmore et al., 2017). Therefore, we investigate whether the four mechanisms
of social influence differ depending on the source that exerts the social influence, that is, proxi-
mate (i.e., friends) and distant (i.e., others represented in media or unknown persons involved in
discussions within an internet forum) reference groups. This question is particularly important
because information spreads easily and quickly on the internet and can influence many people
simultaneously. Therefore, investigating its influence has practical relevance by informing inter-
vention strategies (Crocco et al., 2002; Moreno & Whitehill, 2014). Given this reasoning, we
pose the research question of whether the effects of others’ behavior, others’ control, others’ defi-
nitions, and others’ experience depend on whether the sources of influence are close friends or
communication via a social media site (source question).
Methods
Experimental Vignette Design
To test the hypotheses, we presented participants with hypothetical situations (vignettes) in an
experimental design. This approach is a useful research strategy when it is difficult to directly
observe behavior or when it is ethically problematic to conduct real experiments (Graeff et al.,
2014; Rettinger & Kramer, 2009). This approach is also known to reduce socially desirable
responses (Choong et al., 2002). Conducting experiments with vignettes instead of relying only
on traditional survey research can advance research concerning social influences and substance
use, which is often correlational and thus does not provide insight into causation (Watkins, 2016).
Such correlational research also has difficulties differentiating between effects capturing how an
individual’s behavior is influenced by peers and effects resulting from selecting peers whose
behaviors are very similar to the individual’s behavior or experiences with CE (i.e., homophily)
or those exposed to similar contexts (e.g., to other parents with children at the same competitive
school). The differentiation between these effects can be better addressed with an experimental
design (Akers & Sellers, 2013). Such designs avoid problems of unobserved heterogeneity and
Sattler et al. 7
allow causal claims to be made. Experiments also have an advantage over perceptual measures
that can provoke projection biases, that is, when respondents project their behavior or attitudes
onto others’ behavior or attitudes.
Pretesting
We conducted cognitive pretests (N = 5) using the think-aloud technique and probing questions
to evaluate and improve the study procedure and comprehensibility of the instruments.
Participants
For each experiment, we recruited respondents through Clickworker, a commercial platform with
more than 2 million accounts worldwide, of which approximately 46% are located in North
America; 35% of account holders speak U.S. American English (Clickworker, 2021). Similar to
MTurk and comparable platforms (Buhrmester et al., 2011; Clifford et al., 2015), individuals can
earn additional income on Clickworker for different tasks, such as survey participation. Therefore,
such platforms have received cross-disciplinary attention for data collection (Irani, 2015).
Motivators for completing tasks are often monetary but differ by task and include fun and educa-
tion (Al-Ani & Stumpp, 2016; Dunn, 2020; Ipeirotis, 2010). Research with MTurk samples indi-
cates a sufficiently high replication rate of the direction and significance of treatment effects that
have been found in probability samples (Mullinix et al., 2015). The criteria for participation
included consent to participate, no participation in a prior experiment of this type, passing the
attention check,1 U.S. citizenship, being a parent of at least one child in elementary/primary
school, middle school, or high school (junior/senior),2 and no item nonresponse. Those who com-
pleted the study received a small monetary incentive. Participation was voluntary and anonymous.
We recruited 359 parents (25.9% women, mean age: 36.27) in Experiment 1, 326 participants
(27.3% women, mean age: 36.86) in Experiment 2, 325 participants (20.9% women, mean age:
38.11) in Experiment 3, and 319 participants (25.2% women, mean age: 36.15) in Experiment 4.3
Instruments
Experimental treatments. The four experiments used 2×2 between-subjects designs (social influ-
ence × influence source). The participants were asked to carefully read a scenario that described
parents who were worried about the declining school performance of their child who would have
a high school proficiency exam (Supplemental Appendix A for the cover story, which was identi-
cal across all experiments). Consistent with the definition of CE, the vignette said that the child
was in good psychological and physical health; thus, the use of these drugs was not necessary for
medical reasons. We randomly assigned the respondents to the following influence source condi-
tions: they either read that parents were discussing with close friends who were also parents or
reading about parents from a social media site for parents (i.e., an online discussion forum) about
the use of CE drugs to enhance their child’s cognitive performance. In both versions, we explicitly
stated that such drugs are usually prescribed to children with ADHD only but are also used off-
label for purposes for which they are not officially approved. The story randomly continued with
one of four binary social influence conditions displayed for one of two influence sources (Supple-
mental Appendix B for a detailed description of the wording). In Experiment 1, the respondents
read that either none or most others gave such drugs to their healthy children to enhance school
performance. In Experiment 2, the others stated that CE is morally acceptable or unacceptable. In
Experiment 3, the others strongly criticized parents who gave CE drugs to children or advised
parents to administer such drugs. In Experiment 4, the others reported either that the positive
consequences of such drug use outweighed the negative consequences or the opposite.
8 Journal of Drug Issues 00(0)
In all experiments, we set the age at 13 years because at this age, parents may still have con-
siderable influence over their children’s actions and whether they take medication (which might
decrease with age). Moreover, at this age, children are close to changing to a 4-year high school;
thus, their performance is important. Students also start taking the Preliminary Scholarly Aptitude
Test (PSAT) and National Merit Scholarship Qualifying Test (NMSQT) to qualify for scholar-
ships. Further studies may replicate our experiments with other ages. Due to the experimental
design, both treatments are uncorrelated (Supplemental Appendix C). Almost all correlations
between these treatments and respondent characteristics were insignificant, while two statisti-
cally significant but relatively weak correlations existed with age (Experiment 2) and sex
(Experiment 4). We therefore control our results for the respective respondent characteristics.
Manipulation check. We displayed the vignette for a minimum of 45 seconds before the respon-
dents could continue, self-paced, to the next page for the manipulation check and the dependent
variable. We used a manipulation check that referred to the content of the manipulated social influ-
ence in each experiment (items adapted from Watkins, 2016, Supplemental Appendix D).
Dependent variable. Each experiment then assessed the behavioral willingness to give a prescrip-
tion stimulant drug to a child by asking “If you were one of Charlie’s parents, would you give a
prescription stimulant drug to Charlie?” (adapted from Sattler et al., 2014). The response options
ranged from 0 “definitely not” to 100 “definitely yes.” Although we cannot measure actual
behavior, willingness is an important predictor of behavior according to the theory of planned
behavior (Ajzen & Fishbein, 2004; Epstein et al., 2007).
Prior CE. We assessed prior experience with CE in children by asking parents whether they gave
prescription stimulant drugs to their child (-ren) to enhance their children’s cognitive abilities
(e.g., for mental concentration, memory, or vigilance) without medical necessity (Sattler & Wie-
gel, 2013). Response options were “No, never,” “Yes, within the last 30 days,” “Yes, between the
last 30 days and 6 months,” “Yes, between the last 6 months and 1 year,” and “Yes, more than 1
year ago.” We dichotomized the responses into no [0] and yes [1].
Together with sex and age, we consider this variable exploratorily in the analysis to better
understand this underexplored phenomenon since prior research indicates a covariation with par-
enting and/or CE drug use (willingness) (Borra & Sevilla, 2019; Láng, 2018; Sattler & Wiegel,
2013; Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2019; Wall, 2010; Wilens
et al., 2008).
Debriefing
Given the risk of influencing parent decision-making regarding prescription drug use by pro-
viding false information, we thoroughly debriefed the participants at the end of the survey
(Supplemental Appendix E).
Ethics Approval
The ethics committee of the University of Erfurt approved all four experiments (EV-20170725).
Statistical Analysis
To test the hypotheses for each experiment, first, we ran linear regression models to regress the
willingness to give CE on the manipulated social influence factor and the influence source, and
second, we added the resulting interaction term. Simple slopes were computed to gain more
Sattler et al. 9
detailed insights into the relationship between social influence and the information source. Sex,
age, and prior CE administration were used as controls.
Results
The manipulation checks in Experiments 1 to 4 showed the expected statistically significant dif-
ferences between the social influence conditions (Table 1).
The average willingness to give prescription stimulant drugs to a child was similar across the
experiments as follows: Mean (M) = 9.95 (standard deviation, SD = 20.163) in Experiment 1,
M = 10.02 (SD = 20.412) in Experiment 2, M = 11.40 (SD = 21.763) in Experiment 3, and
M = 12.62 (SD = 22.461) in Experiment 4. The assessed prior experience with CE in children
in the form of a self-reported lifetime prevalence of giving prescription stimulant drugs to their
child (-ren) to enhance their children’s cognitive abilities was 5.57% in Experiment 1, 2.45% in
Experiment 2, 2.15% in Experiment 3, and 5.75% in Experiment 4.
In Experiment 1, the linear regression analysis showed that consistent with the others’ behav-
ior hypothesis, the willingness was 4.10 scale points higher when most other parents in the
vignette said that they gave CE drugs to their children than when no other parents did that (p =
.047, Model 1, Table 2). The willingness under the friends condition was also higher than that
under the social media condition (p = .048). Respondents who already gave CE drugs to their
children had an elevated willingness (p < .001). The respondent’s sex (p = .214) and age (p =
.110) had no effects. When entering the interaction term of both treatments, the conditional main
effects remained significant, and there was no evidence of an interaction (p = .227). An explor-
ative simple slope analysis suggested that the social influence effect in Model 1 was mainly
driven by the social media condition and probably due to the relatively low willingness when no
other parents under the social media condition engage in CE (see the difference between the
respective white bars in Figure 1, B = 6.606, t = 2.26, p = .024). Under the friends condition,
the parents were not affected by others (see respective black bars in Figure 1, B = 2.881, t =
0.58, p = .565). As indicated by the nonsignificant interaction, the two slopes did not signifi-
cantly differ.
In Experiment 2, willingness did not differ with regard to others’ definitions (inconsistent with
the others’ definitions hypothesis) or the influence of source treatment (p = .580 and p = .208,
respectively; Model 3). However, the willingness was higher for parents with prior CE engage-
ment (p = .015) as well as for mothers than it was for fathers (p = .016). Again, age was unre-
lated (p = .247). Furthermore, we did not observe an interaction effect (p = .653, Model 4).
Table 1. T-Tests of the Manipulation Checksa in Experiments 1–4.
Experiment N Mean (SD)t value
1. Others’ behavior 359 none most 55.59***
1.17 (0.858) 5.66 (0.638)
2. Others’ definitions 326 unacceptable acceptable 16.11***
1.78 (1.938) 5.65 (2.378)
3. Others’ social control 325 criticize advise 26.99***
1.17 (0.666) 5.79 (2.097)
4. Others’ experience 313 negative positive 15.98***
2.74 (2.180) 6.08 (1.339)
Note. N = number of observations.
aSee wordings in Supplemental Appendix C.
***p < .001 (two-tailed).
10
Table 2. Linear Regression Models of the Willingness to Give CE Drugs to a Healthy Child (B-Coefficients, Standard Errors in Brackets).
Experiment 1: others’ behavior Experiment 2: others’ definitions Experiment 3: others’ control Experiment 4: others’ experiences
Predictors 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
T1: Social influencea4.10* (2.06) 6.61* (2.92) −1.23 (2.22) −0.26 (3.10) −0.08 (2.38) −1.53 (3.50) 10.31*** (2.35) 15.33*** (3.35)
T2: Friends (Ref. Social
media)
4.05* (2.04) 6.35* (2.78) 2.84 (2.25) 3.79 (3.09) 2.75 (2.38) 1.43 (3.33) −2.96 (2.35) 1.60 (3.20)
Women (Ref. Men) 2.89 (2.33) 2.75 (2.33) 6.08* (2.51) 6.17* (2.52) 1.56 (2.94) 1.48 (2.95) 5.63* (2.70) 5.83* (2.68)
Age −0.18 (0.11) −0.18 (0.11) −0.15 (0.13) −0.16 (0.13) −0.27* (0.14) −0.27* (0.14) −0.09 (0.13) −0.08 (0.13)
Prior CE 23.43*** (4.43) 23.44*** (4.43) 17.66* (7.24) 17.58* (7.25) 28.60*** (8.19) 28.51*** (8.20) 30.04*** (5.00) 29.94*** (4.97)
T1 × T2 −4.95 (4.09) −2.02 (4.50) 2.69 (4.76) −9.72* (4.65)
Constant 10.17* (4.54) 9.28* (4.60) 12.66* (5.28) 12.44* (5.31) 19.42*** (5.70) 20.15*** (5.85) 9.42 (5.21) 6.63 (5.35)
Observations 359 359 326 326 325 325 313 313
Note. CE = cognitive enhancement; T = treatment.
aThe respective type of social influence is indicated in the header of the table.
*p < .05. ***p < .001 (two-tailed).
Sattler et al. 11
In Experiment 3, social control (p = .972, Model 5) and the influence source (p = .250) had
no statistically significant effects. Thus, the others’ control hypothesis was not supported. Prior
CE engagement again increased willingness (p = .001). While younger respondents also showed
a higher willingness (p = .049), sex was unrelated (p = .616). We found no interaction effect
between the treatments (p = .573; Model 6).
In Experiment 4, we found a statistically significant positive social influence effect (p < .001;
Model 7) as follows: when others reported that the positive consequences of drug administration
were higher than the negative consequences, compared to the negative consequences being
higher, the willingness was higher, which supports the others’ experience hypothesis. There was
no significant main effect of source (p = .210). Prior CE administration (p < .001) and being a
woman (p < .037) were associated with a higher willingness. Age was unrelated (p < .484).
Model 8 shows a significant interaction effect between the treatments (p = .038). A simple slope
analysis showed that the social influence effect only occurred under the social media condition
(see different heights of the respective white bars in Figure 1, B = 15.329, t = 4.58, p < .001).
No such effect existed under the friends condition (see respective black bars, B = 5.607, t =
1.73, p = .085).
Discussion
While students’ motive to use prescription stimulants nonmedically for better performance while
studying has been frequently reported in student samples of different ages (Faraone et al., 2020;
Garnier-Dykstra et al., 2012; Rabiner et al., 2009; Schepis et al., 2020; Teter et al., 2020), parents’
willingness to initiate such behavior and the etiology behind their decision-making have rarely
been studied. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first set of theory-driven research
Experiment 1:
Others’ behavior
(N=359)
Experiment 2:
Others’ definitions
(N=326)
Experiment 3:
Others’ control
(N=325)
Experiment 4:
Others’
experience
(N=313)
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
none most unacceptable acceptable criticize advise negative>
positive
positive>
negative
Predicted behavioral willingness
Figure 1. Predicted values (with standard errors) of behavioral willingness to give prescription
stimulant drugs to children depending on the experimental treatments and their interactions based on
the linear regression models shown in Table 2. The friends’ condition is indicated by black bars (),
and the social media condition is indicated by white bars ().
12 Journal of Drug Issues 00(0)
experiments investigating parents’ willingness to give CE to healthy children and the question of
the drivers that lead parents to do so. We therefore used four experiments to explore the mecha-
nisms of social influence and the potential moderating effect of the influence source. The general
findings are that a fraction of parents are willing to administer drugs for better school perfor-
mance and that social influence plays a role, but its impact depends on the type of influence and
partially on its source.
Experiment 1 tested the influence of exposure to others’ behavior. Similar to other findings
regarding the effects of others’ behavior in different contexts of (il-)legal drug use by students
(Maahs et al., 2016; Sattler et al., 2014) and the first descriptive research investigating parents’
willingness to give prescription stimulant drugs to their healthy children for CE (Maher, 2008;
Sattler & Wörn, 2019), our results show that this willingness was higher when most other parents
were also engaged in this behavior compared to when parents were not engaged in this behavior,
supporting the others’ behavior hypothesis. This effect was independent of the influence source
(source question). Others’ CE engagement can signal that this behavior is morally acceptable, has
a good benefit-risk ratio, and should be imitated; it is thus an informational and normative influ-
ence. It can also lead to conformity pressure, the justification of deviant behavior, and expected
disadvantages if one does not engage in said behavior. The results also imply that others’ nonen-
gagement in CE can serve as an informational and normative deterrent regarding CE.
Obviously, there is more than one explanation for the underlying mechanism of the effect of
social influence found in Experiment 1. Thus, in Experiment 2, we directly investigated the effect
of injunctive norms and definitions of others grasping the acceptability of CE, but we found nei-
ther the postulated effect of others’ definitions (others’ definitions hypothesis) nor a difference
between close friends and social media (source question). Other research with students found
that while descriptive norms mattered for CE, this was not always the case for injunctive norms
(Molloy et al., 2019; Ponnet et al., 2015). It has been assumed that descriptive norms are more
heuristic, whereby injunctive norms require more reasoning (Gibbons et al., 2015), and since
most parents already judge this behavior as not very acceptable, normative information might
have less influence. Others’ CE-disapproving definitions might also not imply that parents must
fear negative reactions.
As such reactions are often expensive or invasive and might have differing relevance if they
are derived from friends rather than reading social media content, it is of interest to consider the
effect of such responses. Therefore, Experiment 3 tested the effect of whether others could exert
social control and, thus, attempt to reward or punish CE behavior. While earlier work concerning
social control in the context of drug use among students is inconclusive (Bavarian et al., 2013;
Conn & Marks, 2014; Peralta & Steele, 2010; Ponnet et al., 2015; Sattler et al., 2014; Watkins,
2016), we found no effect of social control (others’ control hypothesis) and no difference between
the influence sources (source question). Receiving strong criticism for giving prescription stimu-
lant drugs to children (compared to receiving advice to perform such behavior) might not be a
sufficient sanction to cause an effect, or our respondents may not have wanted others to tell them
how to make parental decisions. This finding is consistent with earlier qualitative research
involving pediatric CE showing that some parents said that they would not react to external pres-
sure (Ball & Wolbring, 2014; Hiltrop & Sattler, under review).
Finally, Experiment 4 investigated others’ experience with CE drugs in children. Prior research
with students indicates that positive rather than negative personal experiences increase the use of
different classes of drugs (Peralta & Steele, 2010; Watkins, 2016), although whether such effects
also occur via peer reports about drug effects is unclear. In Experiment 4, we found a positive
effect of such peer reports: when the benefits outweighed the negative consequences, the willing-
ness to give prescription stimulants to children increased, corroborating the others’ experience
hypothesis. Prepared with this direct information about others’ CE experiences, parents may
engage in benefit–cost deliberation regarding whether to attempt this behavior. However, we
Sattler et al. 13
found that this effect was stronger under the social media condition (source question). Post hoc
explanations might be that information from social media is considered more reliable than infor-
mation from a more selective and biased group of friends; thus, the respondents could make a
better inference about the general population when given information under the social media
condition. The wisdom of the crowd on a social media site might also be perceived as more
knowledgeable, and the number of parents reporting might be considered larger than that under
the friends condition; thus, under the social media condition, the information may have been
more convincing. In summary, the effect of others’ experience under the social media condition
tended to be relatively strong compared to that under all other conditions (Figure 1), underlining
the persuasive power of information on the internet.
Moreover, we found a few inconsistent effects of sex and age, that is, mothers compared to
fathers and younger compared to older parents, on willingness to administer CE. The preferred
routes to success and the duty of childrearing might vary by sex (Richards et al., 1991), and younger
parents might be more familiar with CE and more open to adopt risk-taking than older parents (cf.
Turner & McClure, 2003; Wiegel et al., 2016). Further studies should elaborate on this.
We found surprisingly high levels of parents’ prior experience with administering CE drugs to
children. However, comparative data are rare, and older survey data (C.S. Mott Children’s Hospital
National Poll on Children’s Health, 2013) provide only an indirect indication of the phenomenon
because parents were asked about their awareness of their child using prescription stimulants for
studying without indication. The only data that we know are from U.S. parents with children, and
these data indicate a lifetime prevalence of 6.8% of parents giving drugs to children for CE (Sattler
et al., under review). Thus, further research is warranted. However, consistent with research on CE
users, our exploratory analysis also showed that in all four experiments, parents with experience
in giving CE to their child(-ren) had an elevated willingness to administer CE. This can be attrib-
uted to, for example, positive experience and habituation (Wiegel et al., 2016).
Strengths, Limitations, and Avenues for Future Research
Our study advances prior research in the context of prescription stimulant drug use in healthy
children and research concerning the social influences on drug misuse in general. Such research
has previously mainly employed qualitative or correlational designs. This study stands out from
prior research because the experimental design ensures high internal validity and allows for
causal claims about social influence by avoiding the risk that exposure to similar environments
or selection of peers rather than their influence is at play. This design also avoids problems
related to projection biases of perceptual measures of peer behavior/experience. Although several
analyses revealed null results, thereby revealing insight into seemingly irrelevant factors, the
manipulation check analyses provide some confidence that the manipulations worked as intended.
Further studies should use a representative sample of the U.S. population to increase the gener-
alizability of our results. Such a sample may include permanent residents, and researchers should
consider assessing whether the children of such individuals are enrolled in school in the U.S. or
abroad. In addition, samples from varying countries should be employed because the susceptibil-
ity to social influence (i.e., conformity pressure) might vary between countries with individualistic
and collectivistic orientations (Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004). Our understanding of parents’ deci-
sions could be further enriched by investigating whether personality variables (such as self-control
or parental self-efficacy) moderate the effect of social influences (Epstein et al., 2007).
The use of self-reports is a common strategy in substance research (Watkins, 2016). However,
reporting the willingness to give prescription drugs to one’s child can be considered a sensitive
topic that can result in socially desirable responses and item nonresponse. This problem seems less
profound in web-based surveys, which have been found to increase the accuracy of responses and
the likelihood of reporting sensitive information (Crutzen & Göritz, 2010; Kreuter et al., 2008). In
14 Journal of Drug Issues 00(0)
addition, our approach of using hypothetical situations could further reduce socially desirable
responses (Choong et al., 2002). We also ensured an anonymous procedure to protect the respon-
dents’ answers. When including a measure of anonymity perception concerning our survey in our
models (Patrzek et al., 2015), we found that in Experiment 1, the main effect of social influence
was only marginally significant in Model 1, while the anonymity perceptions were not significant
(Supplemental Appendix F). However, in Model 2, the conditional main effect of social influence
was statistically significant, and the anonymity perceptions were still insignificant. Moreover,
these anonymity perceptions were only significant in Experiment 3, but this effect did not change
the other effects in the model and showed an unexpected negative direction. We have no clear
interpretation of this particular effect, but in summary, our results seem to be relatively unaffected
by social desirability bias. While we cannot rule out such bias, the lack of item nonresponse in our
experiments may indicate reliable responses, although this cannot be confirmed.
Although the use of hypothetical situations with willingness measures has the abovemen-
tioned advantages, it is a general limitation of such hypothetical vignette studies that we cannot
conclude that voiced willingness will translate into actual behavior (Exum & Bouffard, 2010;
Grasmick & Bursik, 1990). For example, parents might be willing to use medications but may
not know how to access them, or they may seek other, more preferable means to enhance a child’s
performance. Nevertheless, such intentional measures showed relatively high correlations with
behavior (Beck & Ajzen, 1991; Pogarsky, 2004), and effects estimated with such designs matched
behavioral benchmarks (Hainmueller et al., 2015). This is especially important because designs
with manipulations in the real world would be ethically problematic.
Future studies may also investigate other influential sources. We only investigated whether
the effects vary between information obtained from close friends and that obtained from social
media content. Influences through real contact or other media, such as TV, radio broadcasts, or
newspapers, could differ in their attributed trustworthiness and, hence, its effects.
While the focus of our experiments was to obtain initial insights into social influences on par-
ents’ decisions regarding CE, we held the gender and age of the child in the vignette constant.
Future studies may, however, examine whether these characteristics modify parental decision-mak-
ing. For example, for parents with traditional gender roles and gendered parenting practices, the
importance of school performance and the means to achieve it may differ between sons and daugh-
ters (Hadjar et al., 2007). Moreover, parents showing hierarchical self-interest (reflecting factors
such as competitiveness, success orientation, or acceptance of inequality) may engage in parenting
styles in which girls are educated as more passive caretakers, whereas boys are trained to be domi-
nant and successful (Hagan et al., 2000). These are exemplary reasons why the willingness to
administer CE drugs might also be gendered and why parents might be more open to social influ-
ences suggesting CE for boys. However, a recent meta-analysis (Endendijk et al., 2016) dampens
such expectations, as currently, the parenting practices for boys and girls differ only minimally.
Parents’ decisions may also be influenced by the child’s age. On one hand, parents’ level of
control over whether a child takes medication should decrease with age, and parents’ influence-
ability by others should thus also decrease. However, with increasing age, school performance
might become more consequential (e.g., with regard to educational transitions, qualification for
scholarships, labor market entry). Thus, parents may want to support the child’s educational suc-
cess. They might then be particularly open to others’ suggestions regarding CE. Future research
should examine these diverging assumptions.
Conclusion
Few scholars are optimistic about CE in healthy children, for example, to compensate for disad-
vantages that emerge from understaffed and overcrowded schools (Ray, 2016). Most scholars
share the view that such behavior should be prevented, monitored, regulated, or even
Sattler et al. 15
sanctioned—also from a medical point of view (Graf et al., 2013). Our study found that a fraction
of parents are willing to engage their children in a practice, which, on the one hand, may help
them realize a benefit in terms of enhanced performance and better grades but, on the other hand,
may be dangerous for the health and brain development of children and unfair to others, and it
may infringe on the autonomous decisions of children (Sattler, 2020a). Even if this is only a frac-
tion of parents, this practice is also concerning because the onset and continuation of prescription
stimulant use among students is affected by students’ peers (Ford & Ong, 2014; London-Nadeau
et al., 2019; Mui et al., 2014; Sattler et al., 2014). Thus, if parents initiate CE in their children,
they may indirectly affect the instigation and preservation of CE by other students. This is likely
to happen via influence effects, and children could also be the source of the substances used by
their classmates because it has been shown that peers are one of the most frequent drug sources
for students (Garnier-Dykstra et al., 2012; McCabe et al., 2019; Poulin, 2001).
Our study provides evidence for contagion effects between parents. It shows the effects of
other parents’ behavior and especially of experiences with drug effects. However, contrary to the
theoretical predictions and different from several studies with students (Helmer et al., 2016;
Ponnet et al., 2015; Sattler et al., 2014), other parents’ moral evaluations and others’ exercise of
social control had no effects. Moreover, the significant effects that we found occurred only under
the social media condition. This finding indicates that social influences indeed play a role in
parental decisions and that instrumental information seems to be important, whereas normative
aspects directly transferred by others do not affect such decisions. The results imply that the
prevalence of pediatric performance enhancement could increase, as expected by several schol-
ars (Colaneri et al., 2018), if more effective drugs with fewer side effects were available or the
perception existed that such drugs have these properties despite clinical evidence because such
evidence in healthy children is lacking. Our results warrant attention because media reports
include information about prevalence rates and are sometimes enthusiastic about a forthcoming
trend of prescription drug use. Thus, media reports can impact (potential) users to start or con-
tinue such a practice (Forlini & Racine, 2009). The results also deserve attention because media
reports and comments on internet platforms represent only the perspectives of single users
(exemplars). Such exemplars can have greater effects on attitudes and behavior than verified
statistical information on health topics (Krämer & Peter, 2020). This is concerning because peo-
ple increasingly browse the internet for parenting- and health-related information and thereby
rely on nonevidence-based websites containing purely anectodical advice and false information
(Bernhardt & Felter, 2004; Herrmann-Werner et al., 2019; Stukus, 2019; Zillmann, 2002).
Public health authorities should be aware of this potential impact and may aim to search for
counterstrategies. Prevention strategies and health promotion via social media (e.g., links to
online interventions or educational messages) could be effective in influencing parents’ behavior
and reducing the potentially negative effects of prescription stimulant use by healthy children (cf.
Moreno & Whitehill, 2014). Such campaigns might be justified because the amount of user-
generated content on the internet is escalating. Given that numerous studies show that individuals
often massively overestimate the prevalence of substance misuse, presenting the real and rather
low prevalence of CE among children might lead to an adjustment of this perception (Faraone
et al., 2020). Such information needs to be carefully framed to avoid provoking curiosity among
those who are not aware of the risks of such drugs (Sattler & Wiegel, 2013). Nonmedical web-
sites might need disclaimers explaining that anecdotal evidence can be dangerous (Moreno &
Whitehill, 2014). Moreover, forum moderators need to correct exaggerated hopes or downplayed
risks and provide links to support services or online interventions. Parents might also need to
improve their media literacy skills to better classify information found online (Elmore et al.,
2017). As suggested by Singh et al. (2013), clinicians should be careful about being pushed by
parents to diagnose nonexistent diseases and help parents and their children with uncertainties
regarding the need for and dangers of medication use.
16 Journal of Drug Issues 00(0)
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Jonathan Woern for assisting with the preparation for this study. We also thank Johanne
Stuempel for conducting the cognitive pretests, Floris van Veen for programming the experiments, and
Monique Mitchell for the language edits of the vignettes.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or
publication of this article.
Funding
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publi-
cation of this article: This work was supported by a grant to the first author [Sebastian Sattler] from the John
Templeton Foundation via The Enhancing Life Project.
Ethics Approval
The questionnaire and methodology for this study were approved by the Ethics Committee of the University
of Erfurt—20170725.
Informed Consent
Informed consent was obtained from all study participants.
ORCID iD
Sebastian Sattler https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6491-0754
Supplemental Material
Supplemental material for this article is available online.
Notes
1. Despite the effort of platforms like Clickworker to validate accounts (e.g., blocking duplicate accounts),
an attention check was used to remove both bots and participants who quickly clicked through the survey
to obtain the payment (Berinsky et al., 2014; Shamon & Berning, 2019). We used a question that appeared
to ask the participants about their favorite color but with a request to select certain colors and solve a
simple addition problem involving two numbers adapted from Berinsky et al. (2014). We acknowledge
that our attention check is comparatively difficult compared to other checks. In Experiment 1, 58.3%
of the respondents failed this check, and in Experiments 2, 3% and 4, 62.4%, 60.1%, and 54.8% of the
respondents failed this check, respectively. These high rates indicate a strong obstacle but ensure that
only participants who paid sufficient attention were allowed to continue. Such ex ante filtering of careless
respondents has been suggested to increase data quality and to be a motivational influence (Shamon &
Berning, 2019). For vignettes that require the careful reading of text, this can be especially useful.
2. This was performed because parents should have an inside view when making judgments about children.
3. The demographic descriptive information shown in brackets is based on slightly lower-case numbers
due to missing values. Given the comparatively low number of women in the sample, we examined
whether women and men reacted differently to the treatments by incorporating two-way interactions
between sex and each treatment. We also used an additional round of models that added three-way
interactions between the two treatments and sex. However, we found that none of these interaction
effects were significant (results available upon request).
References
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (2004). Questions raised by a reasoned action approach: Comment on Ogden
(2003). Health Psychology, 23, 431–434.
Akers, R. L., & Sellers, C. S. (2013). Criminological theories: Introduction and evaluation. Oxford
University Press.
Sattler et al. 17
Al-Ani, A., & Stumpp, S. (2016). Rebalancing interests and power structures on crowdworking platforms.
Internet Policy Review, 5, 1–19.
Arria, A. M., & DuPont, R. L. (2010). Nonmedical prescription stimulant use among college students: Why
we need to do something and what we need to do. Journal of Addictive Diseases, 29, 417–426.
Ball, N., & Wolbring, G. (2014). Cognitive enhancement: Perceptions among parents of children with
disabilities. Neuroethics, 7, 1–20.
Battleday, R. M., & Brem, A.-K. (2015). Modafinil for cognitive neuroenhancement in healthy non-sleep-
deprived subjects: A systematic review. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 25, 1865–1881.
Bavarian, N., Flay, B., Ketcham, P., & Smit, E. (2013). Illicit use of prescription stimulants in a college
student sample: A theory-guided analysis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 132, 665–673.
Beck, L., & Ajzen, I. (1991). Predicting dishonest actions using the theory of planned behavior. Journal of
Research in Personality, 25, 285–301.
Berinsky, A. J., Margolis, M. F., & Sances, M. W. (2014). Separating the shirkers from the workers?
Making sure respondents pay attention on self-administered surveys. American Journal of Political
Science, 58, 739–753.
Bernhardt, J. M., & Felter, E. M. (2004). Online pediatric information seeking among mothers of young
children: Results from a qualitative study using focus groups. Journal of Medical Internet Research,
6, Article e7.
Betsch, C., Haase, N., Renkewitz, F., & Schmid, P. (2015). The narrative bias revisited: What drives the
biasing influence of narrative information on risk perceptions? Judgment and Decision Making, 10,
241–264.
Betsch, C., Renkewitz, F., Betsch, T., & Ulshöfer, C. (2010). The influence of vaccine-critical websites on
perceiving vaccination risks. Journal of Health Psychology, 15, 446–455.
Betsch, C., Renkewitz, F., & Haase, N. (2013). Effect of narrative reports about vaccine adverse events
and bias-awareness disclaimers on vaccine decisions: A simulation of an online patient social network.
Medical Decision Making, 33, 14–25.
Borra, C., & Sevilla, A. (2019). Competition for university places and parental time investments: Evidence
from the United Kingdom. Economic Inquiry, 57, 1460–1479.
Borsari, B., & Carey, K. B. (2009). How the quality of peer relationships influences college alcohol use.
Drug and Alcohol Review, 25, 361–370.
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inex-
pensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 3–5.
Caviola, L., & Faber, N. S. (2015). Pills or push-ups? Effectiveness and public perception of pharmaco-
logical and non-pharmacological cognitive enhancement. Frontiers in Psychology,6, Article 1852.
Choong, P., Ho, S.-J., & McDonald, R. (2002). An examination of the effects of social desirability bias on
business ethics: Research results. International Journal of Management, 19, 3–9.
Cialdini, R. B., & Goldstein, N. J. (2004). Social influence: Compliance and conformity. Annual Review of
Psychology, 55, 591–621.
Clickworker. (2021). https://www.clickworker.com/clickworker-crowd
Clifford, S., Jewell, R. M., & Waggoner, P. D. (2015). Are samples drawn from Mechanical Turk valid
for research on political ideology? Research & Politics. Advance online publication. https://journals.
sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/2053168015622072
Cochran, J. K., Maskaly, J., Jones, S., & Sellers, C. S. (2017). Using structural equations to model Akers’
social learning theory with data on intimate partner violence. Crime & Delinquency, 63, 39–60.
Colaneri, N., Sheldon, M., & Adesman, A. (2018). Pharmacological cognitive enhancement in pediatrics.
Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 30, 430–437.
Coleman, J. S. (1994). Foundations of social theory. Harvard University Press.
Conn, B. M., & Marks, A. K. (2014). Ethnic/racial differences in peer and parent influence on adolescent
prescription drug misuse. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 35, 257–265.
Conrad, P., & Bergey, M. R. (2014). The impending globalization of ADHD: Notes on the expansion and
growth of a medicalized disorder. Social Science & Medicine, 122, 31–43.
Crocco, A., Villasis-Keever, M., & Jadad, A. (2002). Analysis of cases of harm associated with use of
health information on the internet. JAMA, 287, 2869–2871.
18 Journal of Drug Issues 00(0)
Crutzen, R., & Göritz, A. (2010). Social desirability and self-reported health risk behaviors in web-based
research: Three longitudinal studies. BMC Public Health, 10, Article 720.
C.S. Mott Children’s Hospital National Poll on Children’s Health. (2013). One in ten teens using “study
drugs,” but are parents paying attention?.
Cutler, K. A. (2014). Prescription stimulants are “a okay”: Applying neutralization theory to college
students’ nonmedical prescription stimulant use. Journal of American College Health, 62, 478–486.
d’Angelo, L.-S. C., Savulich, G., & Sahakian, B. J. (2017). Lifestyle use of drugs by healthy people for
enhancing cognition, creativity, motivation and pleasure. British Journal of Pharmacology, 174,
3257–3267.
Deutsch, M., & Gerard, H. B. (1955). A study of normative and informational social influences upon
individual judgment. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 629–636.
Doepke, M., Sorrenti, G., & Zilibotti, F. (2019). The economics of parenting. Annual Review of Economics,
11, 55–84.
Dunn, M. (2020). Making gigs work: Digital platforms, job quality and worker motivations. New Technology,
Work and Employment, 35, 232–249.
Elmore, K. C., Scull, T. M., & Kupersmidt, J. B. (2017). Media as a “super peer”: How adolescents inter-
pret media messages predicts their perception of alcohol and tobacco use norms. Journal of Youth and
Adolescence, 46, 376–387.
Endendijk, J. J., Groeneveld, M. G., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Mesman, J. (2016). Gender-
differentiated parenting revisited: Meta-analysis reveals very few differences in parental control of
boys and girls. PLOS ONE, 11, Article e0159193.
Epstein, J. A., Bang, H., & Botvin, G. J. (2007). Which psychosocial factors moderate or directly affect
substance use among inner-city adolescents? Addictive Behaviors, 32, 700–713.
ExpressScripts. (2014). Turning attention to ADHD. U.S. medication trends for attention deficit hyper-
activity disorder.
Exum, M. L., & Bouffard, J. A. (2010). Testing theories of criminal decision making: Some empirical ques-
tions about hypothetical scenarios. In A. R. Piquero & D. Weisburd (Eds.), Handbook of quantitative
criminology (pp. 581–594). Springer.
Faraone, S. V., Rostain, A. L., Montano, C. B., Mason, O., Antshel, K. M., & Newcorn, J. H. (2020).
Systematic review: Nonmedical use of prescription stimulants: Risk factors, outcomes, and risk reduc-
tion strategies. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 59, 100–112.
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.
Ford, J. A., & Ong, J. (2014). Non-medical use of prescription stimulants for academic purposes among
college students: A test of social learning theory. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 144, 279–282.
Forlini, C., & Racine, E. (2009). Autonomy and coercion in academic “cognitive enhancement” using meth-
ylphenidate: Perspectives of key stakeholders. Neuroethics, 2, 163–177.
Fox, S., & Fallows, D. (2003). Internet health resources. https://www.researchgate.net/publica-
tion/256018827_Internet_Health_Resources
Franke, A., Bonertz, C., Christmann, M., Engeser, S., & Lieb, K. (2012). Attitudes toward cognitive
enhancement in users and nonusers of stimulants for cognitive enhancement: A pilot study. American
Journal of Bioethics Primary Research, 3, 48–57.
Garnier-Dykstra, L. M., Caldeira, K. M., Vincent, K. B., O’Grady, K. E., & Arria, A. M. (2012). Nonmedical
use of prescription stimulants during college: Four-year trends in exposure opportunity, use, motives,
and sources. Journal of American College Health, 60, 226–234.
Gibbons, F., Gerrard, M., Stock, M., & Finneran, S. (2015). The prototype/willingness model. In M. Conner
& P. Norman (Eds.), Predicting and changing health behaviour. Oxford University Press. https://www.
mheducation.co.uk/predicting-and-changing-health-behaviour-research-and-practice-with-social-cog-
nition-models-9780335263783-emea-group
Graeff, P., Sattler, S., Mehlkop, G., & Sauer, C. (2014). Incentives and inhibitors of abusing academic posi-
tions: Analysing university students’ decisions about bribing academic staff. European Sociological
Review, 30, 230–241.
Graf, W. D., Nagel, S. K., Epstein, L. G., Miller, G., Nass, R., & Larriviere, D. (2013). Pediatric neuroen-
hancement Ethical, legal, social, and neurodevelopmental implications. Neurology, 80, 1251–1260.
Grasmick, H., & Bursik, R. (1990). Conscience, significant others, and rational choice: Extending the deter-
rence model. Law & Society Review, 24, 837–861.
Sattler et al. 19
Hadjar, A., Baier, D., Boehnke, K., & Hagan, J. (2007). Juvenile delinquency and gender revisited: The
family and power-control theory reconceived. European Journal of Criminology, 4, 33–58.
Hagan, J., Hefler, G., Classen, G., Boehnke, K., & Merkens, H. (2000). Subterranean sources of subcultural
delinquency beyond the American dream. In S. Karstedt & K. D. Bussmann (Eds.), Social dynamics of
crime and control (pp. 27–51). Hart.
Hainmueller, J., Hangartner, D., & Yamamoto, T. (2015). Validating vignette and conjoint survey
experiments against real-world behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112,
2395–2400.
Helmer, S. M., Pischke, C. R., Van Hal, G., Vriesacker, B., Dempsey, R. C., Akvardar, Y., Guillen-Grima,
F., Salonna, F., Stock, C., & Zeeb, H. (2016). Personal and perceived peer use and attitudes towards the
use of nonmedical prescription stimulants to improve academic performance among university students
in seven European countries. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 168, 128–134.
Herrmann-Werner, A., Weber, H., Loda, T., Keifenheim, K. E., Erschens, R., Mölbert, S. C., Nikendei, C.,
Zipfel, S., & Masters, K. (2019). “But Dr Google said. . .”–Training medical students how to commu-
nicate with E-patients. Medical Teacher, 41, 1434–1440.
Hertwig, R., & Hoffrage, U. (2013). Simple heuristics in a social world. Oxford University Press.
Heyes, A. R., & Boardley, I. D. (2019). Psychosocial factors facilitating use of cognitive enhancing drugs
in education: A qualitative investigation of moral disengagement and associated processes. Drugs:
Education, Prevention and Policy, 26, 329–338.
Higgs, S., & Thomas, J. (2016). Social influences on eating. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 9,
1–6.
Hiltrop, K., & Sattler, S. (under review). Parents’ perceptions and ethical assessment of non-medical drug
use to enhance performance in healthy children and adolescents: Evidence from qualitative interviews.
Ipeirotis, P. G. (2010). Demographics of Mechanical Turk. https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract
_id=1585030
Irani, L. (2015). The cultural work of microwork. New Media & Society, 17, 720–739.
Kabiri, S., Willits, D. W., & Shadmanfaat, S. M. (2019). A multitheoretical framework for assessing perfor-
mance-enhancing drug use: Examining the utility of self-control, social learning, and control balance
theories. Journal of Drug Issues, 49, 512–530.
Keuschnigg, M., & Kratz, F. (2017). Thou shalt recycle: How social norms of environmental protection
narrow the scope of the low-cost hypothesis. Environment and Behavior, 50, 1059–1091.
King, M. D., Jennings, J., & Fletcher, J. M. (2014). Medical adaptation to academic pressure: Schooling,
stimulant use, and socioeconomic status. American Sociological Review, 79, 1039–1066.
Krämer, B., & Peter, C. (2020). Exemplification effects: A meta-analysis. Human Communication Research,
46, 192–221.
Kreuter, F., Presser, S., & Tourangeau, R. (2008). Social desirability bias in CATI, IVR, and web surveys:
The effects of mode and question sensitivity. Public Opinion Quarterly, 72, 847–865.
LaBrie, J. W., Hummer, J. F., Lac, A., & Lee, C. M. (2010). Direct and indirect effects of injunctive
norms on marijuana use: The role of reference groups. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 71,
904–908.
LaBrie, J. W., Hummer, J. F., Neighbors, C., & Larimer, M. E. (2010). Whose opinion matters? The rela-
tionship between injunctive norms and alcohol consequences in college students. Addictive Behaviors,
35, 343–349.
Láng, A. (2018). Mama Mach and Papa Mach: Parental Machiavellianism in relation to dyadic coparent-
ing and adolescents’ perception of parental behaviour. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 14, 107–124.
Latané, B. (1981). The psychology of social impact. American Psychologist, 36, 343–356.
London-Nadeau, K., Chan, P., & Wood, S. (2019). Building conceptions of cognitive enhancement:
University students’ views on the effects of pharmacological cognitive enhancers. Substance Use &
Misuse, 54, 908–920.
Maahs, J. R., Weidner, R. R., & Smith, R. (2016). Prescribing some criminological theory an examination
of the illicit use of prescription stimulants among college students. International Journal of Offender
Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 60, 146–164.
Maher, B. (2008). Poll results: Look who’s doping. Nature, 452, 674–675.
20 Journal of Drug Issues 00(0)
Maier, L. J., Ferris, J. A., & Winstock, A. R. (2018). Pharmacological cognitive enhancement among non-
ADHD individuals—A cross-sectional study in 15 countries. International Journal of Drug Policy, 58,
104–112.
Maier, L. J., Haug, S., & Schaub, M. P. (2016b). Prevalence of and motives for pharmacological neuroen-
hancement in Switzerland—Results from a national internet panel. Addiction, 111, 280–295.
Maier, L. J., & Schaub, M. P. (2015). The use of prescription drugs and drugs of abuse for neuroenhance-
ment in Europe. European Psychologist, 20, 155–166.
McCabe, S. E., Knight, J., Teter, C., & Wechsler, H. (2005). Non-medical use of prescription stimulants
among US college students: Prevalence and correlates from a national survey. Addiction, 100, 96–106.
McCabe, S. E., Veliz, P., Wilens, T. E., West, B. T., Schepis, T. S., Ford, J. A., Pomykacz, C., & Boyd, C.
J. (2019). Sources of nonmedical prescription drug misuse among US high school seniors: Differences
in motives and substance use behaviors. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry, 58, 681–691.
Molloy, B. K., Stock, M. L., Dodge, T., & Aspelund, J. G. (2019). Predicting future academic willingness,
intentions, and Nonmedical Prescription Stimulant (NPS) use with the theory of reasoned action and
prototype/willingness model. Substance Use & Misuse, 54, 2251–2263.
Moreno, M. A., & Whitehill, J. M. (2014). Influence of social media on alcohol use in adolescents and
young adults. Alcohol Research: Current Reviews, 36, 91–100.
Mui, H. Z., Sales, P., & Murphy, S. (2014). Everybody’s doing it: Initiation to prescription drug misuse.
Journal of Drug Issues, 44, 236–253.
Mullinix, K. J., Leeper, T. J., Druckman, J. N., & Freese, J. (2015). The generalizability of survey experi-
ments. Journal of Experimental Political Science, 2, 109–138.
Nadesan, M. H. (2002). Engineering the entrepreneurial infant: Brain science, infant development toys, and
governmentality. Cultural Studies, 16, 401–432.
Nagel, S. (2019). Shaping children: Ethical and social questions that arise when enhancing the young.
Springer.
Napper, L. E., Kenney, S. R., Hummer, J. F., Fiorot, S., & LaBrie, J. W. (2016). Longitudinal relationships
among perceived injunctive and descriptive norms and marijuana use. Journal of Studies on Alcohol
and Drugs, 77, 457–463.
O’Connor, C., & Nagel, S. K. (2017). Neuro-enhancement practices across the lifecourse: Exploring the
roles of relationality and individualism. Frontiers in Sociology, 2, Article 1.
Patrzek, J., Sattler, S., van Veen, F., Grunschel, C., & Fries, S. (2015). Investigating the effect of academic
procrastination on the frequency and variety of academic misconduct: A panel study. Studies in Higher
Education, 40, 1014–1029.
Peralta, R. L., & Steele, J. L. (2010). Nonmedical prescription drug use among US college students at a
Midwest university: A partial test of social learning theory. Substance Use & Misuse, 45, 865–887.
Pogarsky, G. (2004). Projected offending and contemporaneous rule-violation: Implications for heterotypic
continuity. Criminology, 42, 111–136.
Ponnet, K., Wouters, E., Walrave, M., Heirman, W., & van Hal, G. (2015). Predicting students’ intention to
use stimulants for academic performance enhancement. Substance Use & Misuse, 50, 275–282.
Poulin, C. (2001). Medical and nonmedical stimulant use among adolescents: From sanctioned to unsanc-
tioned use. CMAJ, 165, 1039–1044.
Rabiner, D., Anastopoulos, A., Costello, E., Hoyle, R. H., McCabe, S. E., & Swartzwelder, H. S. (2009).
Motives and perceived consequences of nonmedical ADHD medication use by college students.
Journal of Attention Disorders, 13, 259–270.
Racine, E., & Forlini, C. (2010). Cognitive enhancement, lifestyle choice or misuse of prescription drugs?
Neuroethics, 3, 1–4.
Ragan, C., Bard, I., & Singh, I. (2013). What should we do about student use of cognitive enhancers? An
analysis of current evidence. Neuropharmacology, 64, 588–595.
Rasmussen, K. E., & Troilo, J. (2016). “It has to be perfect!” The development of perfectionism and the
family system. Journal of Family Theory & Review, 8, 154–172.
Ray, K. (2016). Not just “study drugs” for the rich: Stimulants as moral tools for creating opportunities for
socially disadvantaged students. American Journal of Bioethics, 16, 29–38.
Sattler et al. 21
Rettinger, D. A., & Kramer, Y. (2009). Situational and personal causes of student cheating. Research in
Higher Education, 50, 293–313.
Richards, M. H., Gitelson, I. B., Petersen, A. C., & Hurtig, A. L. (1991). Adolescent personality in girls and
boys: The role of mothers and fathers. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 15, 65–81.
Sattler, S. (2016). Cognitive enhancement in Germany: Prevalence, attitudes, terms, legal status, and
the ethics debate. In F. Jotterand & V. Dubljević (Eds.), Cognitive enhancement: Ethical and policy
implications in international perspectives (pp. 159–180). Oxford University of Press.
Sattler, S. (2020a). Cognitive enhancement in children by using prescription drugs. In T. Burns &
F. Gottschalk (Eds.), Educating 21st century children (pp. 113–130). OECD.
Sattler, S. (2020b). Using social learning theories to better understand the variation of the moral acceptability
of performance enhancement drug use. AJOB Neuroscience, 11, 248–250.
Sattler, S., & Linden, P. (under review). Unhealthy parenting strategies: Situational (dis-)incentives,
Machiavellian personality, and their interaction on misuse of ADHD medication for healthy children.
Sattler, S., Mehlkop, G., Graeff, P., & Sauer, C. (2014). Evaluating the drivers of and obstacles to the will-
ingness to use cognitive enhancement drugs: The influence of drug characteristics, social environment,
and personal characteristics. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy, 9, 1–14.
Sattler, S., & Singh, I. (2016). Cognitive enhancement in healthy children will not close the achievement
gap in education. The American Journal of Bioethics, 16, 39–41.
Sattler, S., & Wiegel, C. (2013). Cognitive test anxiety and cognitive enhancement: The influence of stu-
dents’ worries on their use of performance-enhancing drugs. Substance Use & Misuse, 48, 220–232.
Sattler, S., & Wörn, J. (2019). Public perceptions of prescription drug use for cognitive enhancement drug
use in healthy children and adolescents. In S. Nagel (Ed.), Shaping children—Ethical and social ques-
tions that arise when enhancing the young (pp. 85–104). Springer.
Schelle, K. J., Faulmüller, N., Caviola, L., & Hewstone, M. (2014). Attitudes toward pharmacological
cognitive enhancement—A review. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 8, Article 53.
Schepis, T. S., Klare, D. L., Ford, J. A., & McCabe, S. E. (2020). Prescription drug misuse: Taking a lifespan
perspective. Substance Abuse: Research and Treatment, 14, 1–28.
Shamon, H., & Berning, C. (2019). Attention check items and instructions in online surveys with incentiv-
ized and non-incentivized samples: Boon or bane for data quality? Survey Research Methods, 14(1),
55–77.
Singh, I., Filipe, A. M., Bard, I., Bergey, M., & Baker, L. (2013). Globalization and cognitive enhancement:
Emerging social and ethical challenges for ADHD clinicians. Current Psychiatry Reports, 15, 1–9.
Smith, E. M., & Farah, M. J. (2011). Are prescription stimulants ‘smart pills’? The epidemiology and cogni-
tive neuroscience of prescription stimulant use by normal healthy individuals. Psychological Bulletin,
137, 717–741.
Stukus, D. R. (2019). How Dr Google is impacting parental medical decision making. Immunology and
Allergy Clinics, 39, 583–591.
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2019). Key substance use and mental health
indicators in the United States: Results from the 2018 National Survey on drug use and health.
Sutherland, E. H., Cressey, D. R., & Luckenbill, D. (1995). The theory of differential association. In J. N.
Herman (Ed.), Deviance: A symbolic interactionist approach (pp. 64–68). Rowman & Littlefield.
Teter, C. J., DiRaimo, C. G., West, B. T., Schepis, T. S., & McCabe, S. E. (2020). Nonmedical use of
prescription stimulants among US high school students to help study: Results from a national survey.
Journal of Pharmacy Practice, 33, 38–47.
Turner, C., & McClure, R. (2003). Age and gender differences in risk-taking behaviour as an explanation
for high incidence of motor vehicle crashes as a driver in young males. Injury Control and Safety
Promotion, 10, 123–130.
Vargo, E. J., & Petróczi, A. (2016). “It was me on a good day”: Exploring the smart drug use phenomenon
in England. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article 779.
Wall, G. (2010). Mothers’ experiences with intensive parenting and brain development discourse. Women’s
Studies International Forum, 33, 253–263.
Watkins, W. C. (2016). A social learning approach to prescription drug misuse among college students.
Deviant Behavior, 37, 601–614.
22 Journal of Drug Issues 00(0)
Webster, M. M., & Laland, K. N. (2008). Social learning strategies and predation risk: Minnows copy
only when using private information would be costly. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological
Sciences, 275, 2869–2876.
Wells, R., Wolniak, G., Engberg, M., & Manly, C. (2016). Socioeconomic disparities in the use of college
admission-enhancing strategies among high school seniors from the 1990s to 2000s. Teachers College
Record, 118, 1–36.
Weyandt, L. L., White, T. L., Gudmundsdottir, B. G., Nitenson, A. Z., Rathkey, E. S., De Leon, K. A., &
Bjorn, S. A. (2018). Neurocognitive, autonomic, and mood effects of Adderall: A pilot study of healthy
college students. Pharmacy, 6, Article 58.
Wiegel, C., Sattler, S., Göritz, A., & Diewald, M. (2016). Work-related stress and cognitive enhancement
among university teachers. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 29, 100–117.
Wilens, T. E., Adler, L. A., & Adams, J. (2008). Misuse and diversion of stimulants prescribed for ADHD:
A systematic review of the literature. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry, 47, 21–31.
Zillmann, D. (2002). Exemplification theory of media influence. Media Effects: Advances in Theory and
Research, 2, 19–41.
Author Biographies
Sebastian Sattler is a lecturer in sociology. His research interests include sociological theory, decision-
making, sociology of health, criminology, drug use, and quantitative methods. His work has been published
in journals, including European Sociological Review, Frontiers in Psychology, and PLOS ONE.
Guido Mehlkop is a professor of empirical research methods. His research interests include criminology,
rational choice theory, methodology, and quantitative methods. He has written a monograph regarding
the explanation of crime as a rational choice. His work has also been published in journals, including
Criminology, and the European Sociological Review.
Vanessa Bahr is a master’s psychology student focusing on cognitive psychology and cognitive neuro-
science. Her research interests include cognitive enhancement, dementia, and behavioral medicine.
Cornelia Betsch is a professor of health communication. She is a trained psychologist, and her work aims
to obtain an understanding of the principles of health behavior by applying a judgment and decision-making
and strategic-interaction perspective. Her work has been published in Nature, Nature Human Behavior, and
Health Psychology.
... Second is vicarious experience, which is gained by observing others, for example, by seeing them use substances illicitly or receiving information from them or the media about the associated dangers and benefits. Nonusers in particular, who lack personal experience, may rely largely on vicarious experience to learn about risks Stafford and Mark 1993) in part because gaining personal experience by illicitly using substances can be costly due to the legal, social, and health risks involved (Cochran et al. 2017;Hertwig and Hoffrage 2013;Sattler et al. 2021). More recently, the idea of differential updating has been proposed, namely that individuals may differ in what they learn from new information about risk (Hirtenlehner and Schulz 2021;Loughran, Paternoster, and Piquero 2018;Thomas et al. 2013). ...
... Such attitudes can be more favorable the more prevalent a behavior is, which may also provide information about the risks involved and lower the risk perceptions (Huber et al. 2023). In addition, direct communication with others about their experiences, knowledge, and expectations can be a learning source to form and adapt risk perceptions (Ford and Ong 2014;Peralta and Steele 2010;Sattler et al. 2021;Watkins 2016). In consequence, individuals may decrease the associated risk perceptions if they know or believe that many others use CE or if they receive positive information about risks, whereas information about higher risks should increase these perceptions. ...
... Similar to social influences, individuals can either passively or actively learn about risk from the media (Akers and Sellers 2013;Bandura and McClelland 1977;Sattler et al. 2021;Watkins 2016). This was shown in a literature review (Wåhlberg and Sjöberg 2000) that examined a variety of contexts and behaviors, although direct information from others and personal experience were found to be more important than media influence in changing risk perceptions. ...
... However, parental motivations to care for their disabled children can manifest in the endorsement of therapeutic genetic manipulation (Snure Beckman et al., 2019). Moreover, a recent study revealed that 2.15 % -5.75 % of participating U.S. parents have previously administered pharmacological substances without medical indication to their children to improve their cognitive capacities (Sattler et al., 2021). Additionally, general investigations into the attitudes toward genetic engineering (e.g., Marteau et al., 1995;Pew Research Center, 2016;Schönthaler et al., 2022) do not show universal rejection of this intervention. ...
Article
Full-text available
Genetic engineering of humans is a controversial practice with unknown societal effects. Gender constitutes an important evaluative background for human behavior and traits. This manifests within action-guiding normative gender stereotypes. This study investigates to which extent these stereotypes may influence the application of genetic engineering. After highlighting potential motivations to enact stereotypes biotechnologically, we propose two potential strategies. People may design future children in close accordance with contemporary gender stereotypes , e.g., to minimize their risk of being punished for non-confirmation, or may create individuals that counteract these stereotypes, e.g., to create a more gender-egalitarian future. To test these hypotheses, we analyzed a large-scale dataset (13,641 virtual children) from an interactive museum exhibit. Here, visitors could design their "perfect child." Gender-dependent differences in designed Big-5-like personality traits and intelligence , musicality, creativity, and sportiness yielded evidence for behavior predicted by both strategies and were inconclusive regarding the dominance of one strategy. Confirming contemporary stereotypes, children deliberately chosen to be male were designed with lower sensibility but higher sportiness than those deliberately chosen to be female. These effects were accompanied by a relatively higher probability of decreasing sensibility and increasing sportiness of these male children. Non-differences among traits like sociality and conscientiousness disconfirmed normative stereotypes and suggested a more egalitarian design. Effect direction, strength, and certainty depended on whether gender was picked deliberately and other factors. Although the ecological setting and methodological limitations hinder a clear interpretation, we provide initial evidence on how genetically engineered children can "essentially" embody gender normativity.
... Moral judgements of drug use are closely linked to their motivations (Copes et al., 2022;Sattler and Mehlkop et al., 2021;van Veen et al., 2021;Wilson, 2022). Most people who use drugs are motivated by a particular reason, with two common reasons being for pleasure (i.e., hedonic) or for medical intervention (i.e., utilitarian). ...
Article
Full-text available
For decades, researchers have explored how the public morally evaluates various behaviors, including those such as drug use and others traditionally considered deviant. While research suggests that attributes of people who use drugs and respondent-level characteristics influence judgments of right and wrong, research has yet to fully examine this in an experimental setting. This study uses a survey experiment to explore the ways in which the public morally evaluates drug use and how these judgments vary depending on circumstances inherent to the scenario. Using a nationwide cross-sectional MTurk sample of Americans (N = 524), I estimate a series of regression models exploring how individual attributes, situational factors, and respondent-level characteristics predict moral judgments of drug use. Findings reveal the public holds significant variation in whether they view drug use as morally acceptable or not, and that user attributes, situational factors, and respondent-level characteristics all significantly influence this relationship. I conclude by situating my findings into the larger conversation of deviancy, drug stigma, and moral evaluations of drug use, and suggest areas for future research.
... Employing pharmacological substances, primarily for improving cognitive abilities, is one of the most predominant issues in the contemporary debate (Banjo et al., 2010;Caviola & Faber, 2015;Farah et al., 2004;Mihailov & Savulescu, 2018;Napoletano et al., 2020;Racine et al., 2021;Sattler et al., 2021;Schelle et al., 2014). That also holds for the workplace, where modern pharmacological enhancement efforts can be traced back to the late nineteenth and early twentieth century. ...
Article
Full-text available
Organizations strive to ensure and maintain the reliability, safety, security, usability, and competitiveness of their processes, goods, and services. Improvement of employees’ skills and abilities contributes to these ends and is a relevant issue for the field of human factors. However, going a step further than designing ergonomics, implementing protocols, and conducting training is the attempt to enhance employee skills directly through various technological means. So-called Human Enhancement aims at direct technological interference with the employees’ skills and is a notoriously controversial yet deeply historical phenomenon. Drawing from empirical and theoretical literature on Human Enhancement, we seek to provide an initial analysis of this phenomenon in an organizational context. One motivational aspect of contemporary Human Enhancement is the need to meet internal, often self-related, or external, usually social or organizational, demands. Given the different effects and means of Human Enhancement, some forms are illicit, sanctioned, and/or condemned as morally wrong, while others are obligatory and well-established. Enhancement efforts can be based on individual initiative and, hence, without organizational knowledge. The opposite of the spectrum are enhancements applied by organizational order. We also emphasize how an organizational culture may incentivize engagement with illicit means of Human Enhancement. Potentially linked to safety and security-related aspects, its enhancement effects in relation to these two poles can inform stakeholders in their regulatory decisions.
... Directly concerned with human behavior and mind, Human Enhancement seems like an ideal phenomenon for psychology to investigate. Indeed, there is a rich body of literature on the concerns and motivations underlying Human Enhancement efforts (e.g., Hotze et al., 2011;Pew Research Center, 2016;Sample et al., 2020;Sattler et al., 2021;Sattler et al., 2014;Scheske and Schnall, 2012). But the latest literature neglects to acknowledge the intricate and multifaceted nature of the technologies employed in Human Enhancement and their transformative effects concerning various aspect of human capabilities. ...
Article
Full-text available
Technology enables humans not only to adapt their environment to their needs but also to modify themselves. Means of Human Enhancement — embodied technologies to improve the human body’s capabilities or to create a new one — are the designated means of adapting ourselves instead of the environment. The debate about these technologies is typically fought on ethical soil. However, alarmist, utopian, and science fiction scenarios distract from the fact that Human Enhancement is a historical and pervasive phenomenon incorporated into many everyday practices. In the vein of disentangling conceptual difficulties, we claim that means of Human Enhancement are either physiologically or psychologically embodied, rendering the merging with the human user their most defining aspect. To fulfill its purpose, an enhancement must pass the test-in-the-world, i.e., assisting with effective engagement with a dynamic world. Even if failing in this regard: Human Enhancement is the fundamental and semi-targeted process of changing the users relationship with the world through the physical or psychological embodiment of a hitherto external object and/or change of one’s body. This can potentially change the notion of being human. Drawing on a rich body of theoretical and empirical literature, we aim to provide a nuanced analysis of the transformative nature of this phenomenon in close proximity to human practice. Stakeholders are invited to apply the theory presented here to interrogate their perspective on technology in general and Human Enhancement in particular.
... To meet these expectations, parents increasingly engage in intense parenting practices such as private tutoring (Wells et al., 2016). Researchers also highlight the susceptibility of parents to rationalize the non-medical use of prescription stimulants as an instrument to achieve their parenting goals, a relatively new debated and potentially health-endangering practice (Arria & DuPont, 2010;Sattler et al., 2021). Such non-medical use of prescription drugs specifically aimed at enhancing "mental functioning beyond what is necessary to sustain or restore good health" has been termed cognitive enhancement (CE) (Dresler et al., 2013, p. 29). ...
Article
Full-text available
First evidence shows that some parents engage in the health-endangering practice of (mis-)using prescription drugs to boost their children's school performance. But little is known about parental perspectives on this phenomenon. This study aims to better understand parents' perspectives on the non-medical use of prescription drugs to improve healthy children's cognitive functioning. We conducted twelve semi-structured face-to-face interviews with a diverse sample of parents in Germany, and applied qualitative content analysis to explore their perspectives on instrumentalizing prescription drugs for improving the performance of healthy children, including their underlying knowledge (gaps), moral evaluations, evaluations of accompanied risks and benefits, opinions on potential motivators, and wishes regarding policy-making. The results show that parents typically believed themselves knowledgeable about such prescription drug (mis-)use, although they were not aware of anyone in their social environment taking them for enhancement. Parents generally considered such behavior to be morally reprehensible, cheating, and similar to doping in sports, and they typically claimed that no situation or occasion could motivate them to administer prescription drugs to their healthy children. Health risks (including side effects or addiction) were a typical expectation of drug use. That doctors should give such drugs to healthy young people was seen as unjustifiable. The results suggest that morality and risk-benefit evaluations of parents play a major role in their decision-making concerning this potentially risky instrumentalization of non-medical drugs. These insights are of distinct importance, especially for future research and further discussions on this topic, such as an evidence-based public dialog and ethics debates. Supplementary information: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s41465-022-00243-w.
Article
The first implantation of a brain chip into a human paralysis patient by Neuralink demonstrated much potential for treating debilitating neurological diseases and injuries. Nevertheless, brain chips can also be implanted in healthy people to provide an interface between the human brain with computers, robotic machines, and novel artificial intelligence platforms, which generates new ethical issues. The focus here is on the development of brain chip implants that can significantly improve memory, intelligence, and cognition, thereby boosting performance in national examinations for university admissions and securing civil service jobs, thus providing a “game-changer” and “shortcut” for many students and parents. Given that Islam is a major world religion, constituting a significant portion of the global population, it is crucial for the biomedical industry to comprehend Islamic perspectives on emerging medical technologies, which will enable it to more effectively cater to a substantial and growing demographic. We thus critically examine whether the application of brain chip technology to enhance academic performance in highly competitive examinations is consistent with Islamic principles. Based on the Islamic jurisprudential framework, such an application for intellectual enhancement of normal and healthy people without any mental impairment may conflict with the injunction to preserve intellect (Hifz al-Aql) and “consideration of consequences” (murāʿāt al-ma'ālāt) in Islam. It may also be viewed as tampering with Allah’s creation (Taghyir Khalq Allah). Gaining such unfair advantages in competitive examinations will likely be viewed as unethical, by transgressing the core Islamic precepts of Amanah (trustworthiness), Al-‘Adl (justice), Ikhlas (sincerity), and Mujahadah (striving).
Article
Singapore, a highly affluent island city-state located in Southeast Asia, has increasingly leveraged new assisted reproductive technologies (ART) to overcome its dismal fertility rates in recent years. A new frontier in ART is preimplantation genetic testing (PGT) for polygenic risk scores (PRS) to predict complex multifactorial traits in IVF (in vitro fertilisation) embryos, such as type 2 diabetes, cardiovascular diseases and various other characteristics like height, intelligence quotient (IQ), hair and eye colour. Unlike well-known safety risks with human genome editing, there are negligible risks with PGT-P, because there are no man-made genetic modifications that can be transmitted to future generations. Nevertheless, the current efficacy of using PGT-P to select IVF embryos for either increased or decreased probability of developing specific polygenic traits is still far from certain. Hence, the regulatory safeguards proposed here will be based on the assumption that the efficacy of this new technology platform has already been validated. These include: (1) restricting the application of PGT-P only for prevention of clinically relevant polygenic disease traits, (2) securely blocking patients’ access to the raw genomic DNA sequencing data of their IVF embryos, (3) validating diagnosis of polygenic disease traits in the prospective parents/grandparents of IVF embryos, and restricting PGT-P only for preventing specifically diagnosed polygenic disease traits and (4) mandating rigorous and comprehensive genetic counselling for IVF patients considering PGT-P. There is an urgent and dire need to prevent abuse of the PGT-P technique, as well as protect the interests and welfare of patients if its clinical application is to be permitted in the country.
Article
The aim of this research is to help children develop the language and communication skills necessary to survive in today's digital era. In the context of children's language acquisition social media has the potential to promote language continuity and support effective learning. This type of research uses a descriptive analytical approach to provide an in-depth and analytical picture of developments, views and findings in the related field of knowledge. The technique used in data collection is library research. This technique was chosen to explore a deeper and more comprehensive understanding of the research topic, by utilizing scientific literature, journals and other library sources to synthesize and analyze existing information. Social media is an application that requires an internet network as a tool designed to facilitate social interaction and is interactive because it can be used to represent oneself, interact, collaborate, share, communicate with other users and form virtual social bonds. In this day and age, technology has become a part of every individual, in fact many of us depend heavily on technology and the internet, where there are many sites including social media. Therefore, it is very important for parents to supervise when their children start using social media, especially young children, because social media has not only positive impacts but also negative impacts. For this reason, we must have a solution so that all the negative things that happen can be overcome. The solution that we can do is to supervise children when playing on social media so that they can reduce children's habits of saying words that interfere with communication, carry out socialization with parents, set time limits for children playing with gadgets, provide educational games for children, and invite children to play together.
Article
We combine empirical experimental research on biased argument processing with a computational theory of group deliberation to overcome the micro–macro problem of sociology and to clarify the role of biased processing in debates around energy. We integrate biased processing into the framework of argument communication theory in which agents exchange arguments about a certain topic and adapt opinions accordingly. Our derived mathematical model fits significantly better to the experimentally observed attitude changes than the neutral argument processing assumption made in previous models. Our approach provides new insight into the relationship between biased processing and opinion polarization. Our analysis reveals a sharp qualitative transition from attitude moderation to polarization at the individual level. At the collective level, we find that weak biased processing significantly accelerates group decision processes, whereas strong biased processing leads to a meta-stable conflictual state of bi-polarization that becomes persistent as the bias increases.
Article
Full-text available
Crowdsourcing systems do more than get information work done. This paper argues that microwork systems produce the difference between “innovative” laborers and “menial” laborers, ameliorating resulting tensions in new media production cultures in turn. This paper focuses on Amazon Mechanical Turk (AMT) as an emblematic case of microwork crowdsourcing. Ethical research on crowdsourcing has focused on questions of worker fairness and microlabor alienation. This paper focuses on the cultural work of AMT’s mediations: divisions of labor and software interfaces. This paper draws from infrastructure studies and feminist science and technology studies to examine Amazon Mechanical Turk labor practice, its methods of worker control, and the kinds of users it produces.
Article
Full-text available
In this paper, we examine rates of careless responding and reactions to detection methods (i.e., attention check items and instructions) in an experimental setting based on two different samples. First, we use a quota sample (with monetary incentive), a central data source for internet-based surveys in sociological and political research. Second, we include a voluntary opt-in panel (without monetary incentive) well suited for conducting survey experiments (e.g., factorial surveys). Respondents’ reactions to the detection items are analyzed by objective, nonreactive indicators (i.e., break-off, item nonresponse, and measurement quality), and two self-report scales. Our reaction analyses reveal that the detection methods we applied are not only well suited for identifying careless respondents, but also exert a motivational rather than a demotivating influence on respondents’ answer behavior and, hence, contribute to data quality. Furthermore, we find that break-off behavior differs across both samples suggesting that results from methodological online studies on the basis of incentivized samples do not necessarily transfer to online studies in general.
Article
Full-text available
Prescription drug misuse (PDM), or medication use without a prescription or in ways not intended by the prescriber, is a notable public health concern, especially in the United States. Accumulating research has characterized PDM prevalence and processes, but age-based or lifespan changes in PDM are understudied. Given age-based differences in the medical or developmental concerns that often underlie PDM, it is likely that PDM varies by age. This review summarizes the literature on PDM across the lifespan, examining lifespan changes in prevalence, sources, motives and correlates for opioid, stimulant, and tranquilizer/sedative (or benzodiazepine) PDM. In all, prevalence rates, sources and motives vary considerably by age group, with fewer age-based differences in correlates or risk factors. PDM prevalence rates tend to decline with aging, with greater use of physician sources and greater endorsement of self-treatment motives in older groups. Recreational motives (such as to get high) tend to peak in young adulthood, with greater use of peer sources or purchases to obtain medication for PDM in younger groups. PDM co-occurs with other substance use and psychopathology, including suicidality, across age groups. The evidence for lifespan variation in PDM is strongest for opioid PDM, with a need for more research on tranquilizer/sedative and stimulant PDM. The current literature is limited by the few studies of lifespan changes in PDM within a single sample, a lack of longitudinal research, little research addressing PDM in the context of polysubstance use, and little research on minority groups, such as sexual and gender minorities.
Article
Some parents engage in the potentially unhealthy and morally debateable parenting practice of giving prescription stimulant drugs to healthy children to boost their school and extracurricular performance. However, the parents' underlying reasoning remains unexamined. This web-based study (N Respondents = 1360) simultaneously investigates eight experimentally-varied situational (dis-)incentives (e.g., financial gains and drug properties) within a factorial vignette survey (N Vignettes = 256), Machiavellianism as a measured socially relevant personality trait, and possible interaction effects. Results show that approximately 40% of the described situations (N Evaluations = 5440) provoked some willingness to medicate healthy children. Multilevel mixed-effect models revealed that this willingness was higher, for example, with increasing financial gains and weaker side effects. Machiavellians disclosed a higher willingness. They were more responsive to financial gains and threats (e.g., probable side effects). Respondents' sex, age, ethnicity, and experience with prescription drugs also had effects. Prevention measures might emphasize the dangers and limited potential of prescription drugs for healthy children.
Book
The volume offers a unique collection of articles on pediatric neuroenhancement from an international and multidisciplinary perspective. In recent years, the topic of “neuroenhancement” has become increasingly relevant in academia and practice, as well as among the public. While autonomous adults are free to choose neuroenhancement, in children it presents its own ethical, social, legal, and developmental issues. A plethora of potential (neurotechnological) enhancement agents are on the market. While the manifold issues surrounding the topic have been extensively discussed, there is little work on the specific questions that arise in children and adolescents. This book addresses this gap in the literature: Next to conceptual and normative work on autonomy and self-control, the collection explores the implications for parenting and schooling, and provides input for a discussion of public attitudes. It is a valuable resource for the different academic communities confronted with questions of how to evaluate and approach enhancement in children and is of interest to neuroethicists, scholars in applied ethics and neurology, psychiatrists and psychologists as well as scientists developing enhancement interventions for children.
Article
Technology has driven new organisations of work and employment relationships, rendering changes that would have been unimaginable just a decade ago. The rise of digital platforms has not only enabled new forms of work activity but also transformed the way workers find new opportunities. This development, referred to as gig work, is distinct from traditional employment in that it is mediated through online platforms. While we can somewhat objectively designate traditional job characteristics as ‘good’ or ‘bad’, designating gig work itself as ‘good’ or ‘bad’ overlooks the fact that workers are inclined to evaluate the quality of their jobs according to their own individual needs, priorities, backgrounds and other circumstances—even if those jobs are objectively the same. Unlike previous scholarship on gig work, which has viewed job quality largely from a platform‐focused perspective, this article takes a worker‐centric approach and provides a typology of gig workers. The typology demarcates how gig work is used and indicates key attributes that differentiate how workers approach such jobs. Moreover, the typology reveals heterogeneity in gig workers’ motivations, characteristics and intentions. Consequently, platforms with ‘bad’ job quality characteristics can still offer work that some workers will see as ‘good’ and vice versa.
Article
The presentation of single cases as examples for larger phenomena has a long-standing tradition in journalism. However, their usage has been viewed rather critically within the scientific community, because they are employed in a highly selective manner. Consequently, over the course of the last three decades, communication scholars from different research traditions have concerned themselves with the question of how single-case information within media content affects audience judgments. Although most publications report exemplification effects of some sort, it remains unclear which types of exemplars are effective and whether they are capable of influencing both perceptual and personal judgments. Applying a multi-level meta-regression approach, we synthesize findings across different studies and investigate potential moderators. Our results suggest overall exemplification effects that seem to be most pronounced for first-level reality judgments, such as public opinion or frequency estimates, but that are limited in their robustness when controlling for interdependence of the measurements.
Article
The Internet has forever changed the manner in which we communicate with one another and gather information. Patients and the general public are using online search engines to look for information pertaining to their own health as well as the health of friends and relatives. Unfortunately, the information they encounter is often incorrect and potentially harmful. Many of the sites offering misinformation directly profit by selling nonvalidated tests or treatment options. More than ever, it is important for medical professionals to be aware of the misinformation their patients are encountering online and develop conversations to address this during individual encounters.