ArticlePDF Available

Associative processes in addiction relapse models: A review of their Pavlovian and instrumental mechanisms, history, and terminology

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Animal models of relapse to drug-seeking have borrowed heavily from associative learning approaches. In studies of relapse-like behaviour, animals learn to self-administer drugs then receive a period of extinction during which they learn to inhibit the operant response. Several triggers can produce a recovery of responding which form the basis of a variety of models. These include the passage of time (spontaneous recovery), drug availability (rapid reacquisition), extinction of an alternative response (resurgence), context change (renewal), drug priming, stress, and cues (reinstatement). In most cases, the behavioural processes driving extinction and recovery in operant drug self-administration studies are similar to those in the Pavlovian and behavioural literature, such as context effects. However, reinstatement in addiction studies have several differences with Pavlovian reinstatement, which have emerged over several decades, in experimental procedures, associative mechanisms, and terminology. Interestingly, in cue-induced reinstatement, drug-paired cues that are present during acquisition are omitted during lever extinction. The unextinguished drug-paired cue may limit the model’s translational relevance to cue exposure therapy and renders its underlying associative mechanisms ambiguous. We review major behavioural theories that explain recovery phenomena, with a particular focus on cue-induced reinstatement because it is a widely used model in addiction. We argue that cue-induced reinstatement may be explained by a combination of behavioural processes, including reacquisition of conditioned reinforcement and Pavlovian to Instrumental Transfer. While there are important differences between addiction studies and the behavioural literature in terminology and procedures, it is clear that understanding associative learning processes is essential for studying relapse.
Content may be subject to copyright.
H
ε
Behaviour
Neuroanatomy
and
Neuroanatomy and Behaviour, 2021, 3(1), e18.
ISSN: 2652-1768
doi: 10.35430/nab.2021.e18
REVIEW
Associative processes in addiction relapse models: A
review of their Pavlovian and instrumental
mechanisms, history, and terminology
Belinda Po Pyn Lay1,*and Shaun Yon-Seng Khoo2,†
1Center for Studies in Behavioral Neurobiology/Groupe de Recherche en Neurobiologie Comportementale,
Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montreal, Canada
2Department of Pharmacology and Physiology, Faculty of Medicine, Université de Montréal, Montreal, Quebec,
Canada
*belinda.lay@concordia.ca
shaun.khoo@umontreal.ca
Abstract
Animal models of relapse to drug-seeking have borrowed heavily from associative learning approaches. In studies of
relapse-like behaviour, animals learn to self-administer drugs then receive a period of extinction during which they learn to
inhibit the operant response. Several triggers can produce a recovery of responding which form the basis of a variety of models.
These include the passage of time (spontaneous recovery), drug availability (rapid reacquisition), extinction of an alternative
response (resurgence), context change (renewal), drug priming, stress, and cues (reinstatement). In most cases, the
behavioural processes driving extinction and recovery in operant drug self-administration studies are similar to those in the
Pavlovian and behavioural literature, such as context effects. However, reinstatement in addiction studies have several
differences with Pavlovian reinstatement, which have emerged over several decades, in experimental procedures, associative
mechanisms, and terminology. Interestingly, in cue-induced reinstatement, drug-paired cues that are present during
acquisition are omitted during lever extinction. The unextinguished drug-paired cue may limit the model’s translational
relevance to cue exposure therapy and renders its underlying associative mechanisms ambiguous. We review major behavioural
theories that explain recovery phenomena, with a particular focus on cue-induced reinstatement because it is a widely used
model in addiction. We argue that cue-induced reinstatement may be explained by a combination of behavioural processes,
including reacquisition of conditioned reinforcement and Pavlovian to Instrumental Transfer. While there are important
differences between addiction studies and the behavioural literature in terminology and procedures, it is clear that
understanding associative learning processes is essential for studying relapse.
Key words: Addiction; Reinstatement; Cues; Extinction; Conditioned reinforcement; Pavlovian to Instrumental Transfer
Behavioural paradigms in addiction neuroscience often rely
on classic Pavlovian and operant conditioning processes. While
theoretical associative learning mechanisms are based on both
appetitive and aversive conditioning, in addiction neuroscience
they are applied to model appetitive motivation for drugs. In
Pavlovian conditioning, animals readily learn to expect the deliv-
ery of an appetitive or aversive outcome upon the presentation
of a cue that has consistently been paired with outcome deliv-
ery. For example, a light (conditioned stimulus, CS) that reli-
ably predicts the occurrence of a food-reward or foot-shock (un-
conditioned stimulus, US) can evoke conditioned responding such
as magazine approach or freezing, respectively, in the absence
of that outcome. In instrumental or operant conditioning, ani-
mals learn to perform a response such as a lever press or nose-
poke to obtain a desired outcome, which is often paired with
the presentation of a visual and/or auditory cue. Addiction neu-
roscience takes advantage of both Pavlovian and operant condi-
tioning through procedures such as conditioned place preference,
which studies the development of Pavlovian associations between
experimenter-administered drugs and specic contexts [1], and
drug self-administration studies, where animals perform an op-
erant response to obtain drug rewards [2]. These conditioning
1
2|Neuroanatomy and Behaviour, 2021
paradigms enable the study of how animals learn about rewards
and their associated cues and are often complemented by studiesof
extinction and reinstatement or recovery-from-extinction which
are designed to study how changes in the relationship between
the cue and the outcome can alter the behavioural response to
that cue. Each type of recovery-from-extinction phenomenon has
its own behavioural processes and theoretical explanations, with
some of the most complex associative processes occurring during
cue-induced reinstatement. As recently reviewed by Konova and
Goldstein, there are a number of parallels between extinction in
experimental animal models and in humans [3]. Therefore, un-
derstanding the behavioural and psychological processes mediat-
ing these forms of learning can provide critical insight to the treat-
ment and relapse of problem behaviours observed in patients suf-
fering from psychological disorders that affect appetitive motiva-
tion, particularly substance use disorders.
Extinction and its Signature Characteristics
Extinction is the most basic and reliable method for reducing
unwanted learned behaviours. In essence, extinction of the be-
havioural response occurs when the association between the cue
and the outcome is weakened through the continual presentation
of the conditioned cue alone without the delivery of the expected
outcome [4]. Extinction is a fundamental process underlying psy-
chotherapy. Extinction-based addiction therapies include cue ex-
posure therapy which is effective for alcohol use disorder [5] and
virtual exposure therapies which are employed for substance use
disorders and behavioural addictions [6]. However, cue exposure
therapies have also frequently been found to be ineffective in clin-
ical populations [7–9] and cue exposure therapy is not often im-
plemented [5]. These shortcomings have led many researchers to
examine why cue exposure therapy is often ineffective and to pro-
pose various modications to clinical approaches that might im-
prove its efcacy [3, 9, 10]. A major clinical limitation of the ef-
cacy of extinction-based treatments is that extinction is transient
and the extinguished response can return under a variety of con-
ditions such as spontaneous recovery, rapid reacquisition, resur-
gence, renewal, and reinstatement [11–14]. This return of the orig-
inal behaviour is taken as evidence that extinction does not com-
pletely erase the original association between the cue and the out-
come. Rather, it produces a new learning that competes with the
original memory for behavioural control and is highly dependent
on environmental cues for its retrieval.
Behavioural studies have identied several factors which can
trigger a recovery of responding after extinction. For instance, a
previously extinguished Pavlovian conditioned response can re-
emerge simply with the passage of time, known as spontaneous
recovery [4, 15, 16]. In rapid reacquisition, re-establishing the
cue-outcome association results in a faster rate ofacquisition com-
pared to initial conditioning [4, see also 17]. Resurgence occurs
when an operant response is extinguished while an alternative be-
haviour is reinforced; if the alternative behaviour then undergoes
extinction the former operant response can recover or resurge[18].
In the case of renewal, recovery of the extinguished response can
be triggered by changing contexts after extinction, where a novel
context is sufcient to renew responding to that extinguished cue
and a return to the acquisition context produces the most robust
recovery effects [19–21]. Finally, in reinstatement paradigms,
unsignalled presentations of the outcome are sufcient to cause
a return of responding to an extinguished cue [22].
Similar recovery-from-extinction phenomena have been doc-
umented in drug conditioning and self-administration studies
[23–26]. Many relapse models in addictionneuroscience are based
on these classic Pavlovian and operant models, with similar recov-
ery mechanisms. However, there are some procedural differences
that distinguish recovery phenomena between the two elds. We
briey review the associative basis of spontaneous recovery, reac-
quisition, and resurgence [for more detail, see 14, 27, 28] in Pavlo-
vian and instrumental paradigms and argue that the associative
mechanisms between them are largely comparable. However,
most addiction neuroscience research is centred on the renewal
and reinstatement paradigms and so these phenomena are the fo-
cus of the current paper. While renewal and the drug-primed and
stress-induced variants of reinstatement are also readily compa-
rable with Pavlovian recovery phenomena, a comparison between
experimental approaches reveals that cue-induced reinstatement
is driven by ambiguous associative processes. We therefore decon-
struct cue-induced reinstatement at a behavioural level to further
understand the associative mechanisms driving recovery and ar-
gue that it may be driven by a combination of (reacquisition of)
conditioned reinforcement and Pavlovian to Instrumental Trans-
fer.
Spontaneous Recovery
Spontaneous recovery is one of the most basic recovery-from-
extinction phenomena that provides evidence that the original
learning survives the extinction procedure. In Pavlov’s original
studies [4], an extinguished conditioned response spontaneously
recovered to a level above the minimum achieved at the end of ex-
tinction after a period of time had elapsed [4, 29–31]. This level
of recovery in behaviour can vary depending on the length of time
that intervenes between the extinction and test session such that
the longer the period between extinction and test, the higher lev-
els of spontaneous recovery [32]. The spontaneous recovery ef-
fect has also been observed in instrumental studies by Skinner and
others as early as the 1930s [29–31]. Spontaneous recovery has
been shown occasionally in the addiction literature and can occur
in drug self-administration studies [33–39] and in Pavlovian con-
ditioning studies with drug reinforcers [26, 40].
The current, dominant view of the spontaneous recovery effect
in both Pavlovian and operant studies is that a shift in the tempo-
ral context triggers a recovery of responding [41]. Skinner has ar-
gued that unavoidable cues close to the beginning of the test ses-
sion such as transport and handling help to promote spontaneous
recovery [42]. Bouton has since incorporated Skinner’s argument
as evidence of contextual effects in spontaneous recovery [41]. In
addition to the temporal context view, Rescorla has also reviewed
several alternative explanations to describe why spontaneous re-
covery is observed following extinction ofthe response [32]. These
include local performance effects, such as emotional states that
build up during extinction [43] but which may have dissipated by
the time of the test, and the effects of response fatigue which re-
duce responding during the extinction session but dissipate over
time allowing for the restoration of responding in a subsequent
session [32]. However, Rescorla notes that for several of these al-
ternative explanations, empirical support has been mixed or lack-
ing [32].
An alternative view considers spontaneous recovery as a re-
sult of differential rates of decay between the original associa-
tion and the inhibitory extinction memory [27]. During acqui-
sition, excitatory associations between the cue and the outcome
are learned, while during extinction a separate inhibitory associa-
tion is learned. Over time, the original association decays slowly,
while the inhibitory association decays more quickly. A given pe-
riod of time between the end of extinction and test will therefore
involve much greater loss of the extinction memory’s inhibitory
association than decay in the acquisition memory’s excitatory as-
sociation. Differential decay explains observations that longer pe-
riods of time between extinction training and test tend to result
in greater spontaneous recovery, while spontaneous recovery is
reduced if a delay also occurs between the end of acquisition and
beginning of extinction training [44]. In the case of an extended
Lay & Khoo |3
interval between extinction and test, there has been more time for
the extinction memory to decay quickly, but the acquisition mem-
ory is relatively intact. In the case of delayed extinction and a de-
layed test, although the extinction memory decays prior to test,
the extended period of time since acquisition gives the original
memory time to decay as well [27].
Another possible explanation for how the passage oftime inu-
ences recovery is due to changes in sensitization and habituation
that occur over time [45]. Sensitization describes a response that
is increased (becomes more sensitive) to a repeatedly presented
stimulus, while habituation describes a response decrement that
occurs to a stimulus across repetitions. Sensitization and habit-
uation are separate processes that can occur at the same time in
response to repeated stimulus presentations. For example, Mc-
Sweeney and colleagues have argued that sensitization and habit-
uation both occur within operant sessions, as responding is sen-
sitized and increases early in the session but then declines later
in the session as habituation becomes dominant [45]. Moreover,
aversive Pavlovian conditioning studies have shown that there is
substantial overlap in the behaviour and neuropharmacology of
habituation and extinction [46, 47], providing further support for
the idea that habituation is an inhibitory process distinct from sen-
sitization. According to this view, spontaneous recovery occurs
because the habituation decays over time, even when animals re-
main in the conditioning chamber between sessions, leavinga sen-
sitized response [48].
The idea that sensitization may drive increased responding
with the passage of time nds empirical support in addiction mod-
els, such as incubation of craving and psychostimulant sensitiza-
tion studies. The incubation of craving model was based on clin-
ical addiction studies where participants reported the subjective
feeling of craving returning in phases or episodes [49, 50]. In the
laboratory, animals learn to self-administer drugs and are then
forced to have a period of abstinence. At test, elevated respond-
ing is observed based on the length of the abstinence period. Thus,
despite the fact that incubation of craving does not involve instru-
mental extinction prior to testing [49, 51–53], this model draws
parallels with spontaneous recovery by demonstrating that the
passage of time can increase operant responding. Moreover, like
spontaneous recovery, incubation of craving has been observed
with a variety of drug and non-drug reinforcers, includingcocaine,
methamphetamine, and palatable food [49, 51–53]. It is thought
that sensitization may develop over the course of abstinence to
drive an increase in responding [49, 54], suggesting that sensiti-
zation lasts longer than habituation.
Psychostimulant sensitization studies also support the idea
of long-lasting sensitization effects that become more apparent
with the passage of time. In psychostimulant sensitization stud-
ies, animals receive experimenter-administered drugs (e.g. am-
phetamine) for several days. At test, they receive a challenge dose
and sensitization is seen when animals have an elevated response
to the drug compared to controls. A period of time between the
end of the experimenter-administered drug exposure phase and
test is essential to observe sensitization in these experiments [55].
It is notable that while spontaneous recovery can be observed in
minutes with a food reinforcer [48], incubation ofcraving and psy-
chostimulant sensitization studies require days to weeks to elapse
before effects can be observed [49, 55]. However, this timeline is
consistent with the procedures used to test for spontaneous recov-
ery after extinction in animals trained to self-administer cocaine
[33, 34], alcohol [35–37], and nicotine [38, 39]. Taken together,
evidence from incubation of craving and psychostimulant sensiti-
zation studies show that the increased responding observed after
a period of time may be driven by sensitization that persists longer
than habituation.
Rapid Reacquisition
Rapid reacquisition in a Pavlovian design involves the restoration
of the cue-outcome association after extinction. Pavlov was the
rst to observe that the fresh application of the US restored con-
ditioned responding after extinction [4]. Similarly, Skinner stud-
ied both operant conditioning and reconditioning animals after
extinction [31]. In both cases, it is understood that the availabil-
ity of the outcome promotes the recovery effect and demonstrates
that extinction has not completely erased the initial training mem-
ory [56]. Classical associative learning models can account for
rapid reacquisition if the extinguished cue retains some latent as-
sociative strength that facilitatesreacquisition, though some mod-
els such as Rescorla-Wagner have difculty explaining the related
phenomena of slow reacquisition [27]. For Bouton, the conditions
of rapid reacquisition return the animal to the original training
context, facilitating retrieval of the original acquisition memory
[14]. Consistent with the context retrieval account, Skinner ob-
served that conducting multiple rounds of extinction and recon-
ditioning produced successively more rapid extinction curves [31].
Animals may therefore become more adept at distinguishing be-
tween the acquisition or self-administration context and the ex-
tinction context. In either case, drug self-administration studies
have shown that reacquisition after extinction is more rapid than
the initial acquisition of the response [57].
However, if reacquisition is driven by a context effect, then it
should be inuenced by context manipulations and the evidence
for this inuence is mixed. Bouton and Swartzentruber found that
context had an effect on reacquisition of conditioned suppression,
with slower reacquisition in a distinct extinction context [58]. In
contrast, Willcocks and McNally found overall performance dur-
ing reacquisition of alcohol self-administration sessions was not
signicantly different when tested in the same context as acquisi-
tion and extinction, a novel context, or a distinct extinction con-
text [59]. The only context effect that they observed was altered
latency to rst response in the extinction context during reacqui-
sition testing [59]. Moreover, Willcocks and McNally have also
shown distinct neurobiological substrates mediate these effects
[60]. They found that inactivation of the prelimbic cortex reduced
contextual renewal but increased responding during reacquisition
[60]. These neurobiological differences were recently extended to
the pattern of activation in the mesolimbicdopamine system, such
as the medial ventral tegmental area, which was required for reac-
quisition but not renewal [61]. While rapid reacquisition is gener-
ally an underutilised paradigm in drug self-administration stud-
ies [62], these neurobiological ndings indicate that contextual
renewal and rapid reacquisition are dissociable, suggesting that
mechanisms other than renewal contribute to rapid reacquisition.
Resurgence
Resurgence is a recovery phenomenon dened by the reappear-
ance of an extinguished response during the extinction of an alter-
native response, and is unique to operant paradigms [14, 63, 64].
For example, during Phase 1, rats are trained to press a lever for
food reward. In Phase 2, pressing on the original lever is no longer
reinforced and pressing on an alternative lever is reinforced. In
Phase 3, the alternative lever is extinguished which causes re-
sponding on the original lever to increase. According to Epstein,
the earliest reports of resurgence effects were by Hull in 1934 [65].
Hull trained rats to run down a 20-foot runway, performed ex-
tinction (then described as “frustration”), then retrained them to
run down a 40-foot runway [66]. During extinction with the 40-
foot runway, rats appeared to run faster in the leadup to the 20-
foot marker, reminiscent of the original response [66]. Another
early report of resurgence was a 1951 conference presentation by
Carey, where the effect was described in rats trained to lever press
4|Neuroanatomy and Behaviour, 2021
for food pellets and described as “reinstatement” or “regression”
[67]. Resurgence was then rediscovered in 1970 by Leitenberg,
Rawson, and Bath, who found rats would resume operant respond-
ing during the extinction of an alternative behaviour [68]. How-
ever, it would not be until Epstein and Skinner began studying the
effect in the 1980s that it would take on the name of resurgence
[18, 65].
Multiple theories have been proposed to explain resurgence.
For example, Leitenberg and colleagues originally approached
resurgence from the perspective of response competition [68], a
theory that Bouton and colleagues argue is contradicted by evi-
dence that resurgence is unaffected by the schedule of reinforce-
ment used for the alternative behaviour [14]. A second explana-
tion is based on behavioural momentum theory, where reinforce-
ment of the alternative response increases the disruption to the
original response but simultaneously strengthens the original re-
sponse by providing additional reinforcement in the same context
[69]. However, Shahan, who had previously argued for the appli-
cation of behavioural momentum theory to resurgence, has more
recently argued that this theory has encountered difculty ex-
plaining results such as the original response becoming more per-
sistent during extinction even though alternative reinforcement
was available [70]. Shahan and Craig therefore have proposed the
“resurgence as choice” model, which argues that animals allocate
responses based on the values of those responses over time [70].
As the alternative response is extinguished, the value of the orig-
inal response becomes relatively higher and therefore receives an
increased behavioural allocation [70].
In contrast with Shahan, Bouton views resurgence as a context
effect [14]. Bouton and colleagues have previously suggested that
the alternative response and its reinforcement represent a distinct
extinction context for the original response. When the alternative
response is extinguished, this creates a novel context which re-
sults in renewal [14]. In response to Shahan and Craig’s adoption
of the resurgence as choice model, Bouton and colleagues noted
that contextual elements are now incorporated into resurgence as
choice [28], suggesting that there is growing theoretical conver-
gence in the behavioural understanding of resurgence.
Interest in resurgence as an addiction model arises from its
potential relevance to contingency management and related ap-
proaches which incentivise abstinence from substance use by pro-
viding non-pharmacological rewards, in other words, by reinforc-
ing alternative behaviours [71, 72]. The resurgence effect suggests
that there is potential for relapse when therapy concludes and in-
centives are withdrawn [14] which is known to occur [73]. Ani-
mal studies have shown that resurgence occurs when drugs such
as alcohol or cocaine are used as reinforcers [74–76]. One poten-
tial point of contention is that Bouton and colleagues have concep-
tualised resurgence as renewal due to a novel context (‘C’) that is
distinct from both the acquisition context (‘A’) and the extinction
context (‘B’), also known as ABC renewal [14]. However, attempts
to show ABC renewal in alcohol have not been successful in either
an operant design [77] or Pavlovian design [78]. It is therefore
possible that resurgence in drug self-administration studies may
be driven by factors other than contexts, underscoring the impor-
tance of understanding the behavioural mechanisms which drive
resurgence as an avenue for further research.
Renewal
Contextual theories of recovery phenomena have been extraordi-
narily inuential. In 1978, Welker and McAuley rst demonstrated
that contextual stimuli could produce a recovery of operant re-
sponding for food [79]. In 1979, Bouton and Bolles published two
papers on contextual effects on the recovery of extinguished con-
ditioned suppression [21, 80]. In one paper, they showed that rats
that received conditioning in one context and extinction in a dis-
tinct context, recovered or renewed conditioned suppressionwhen
they were returned to the original training context [21]. In another
study, they showed that when the US (foot-shocks) was delivered
in the conditioning chambers the day after extinction but before
test, this only produced reinstatement when the foot-shocks were
delivered in the same context as testing [80]. These early papers
showed the importance of contexts in extinction learning and re-
covery phenomena and would form the basis of a decades-long re-
search programme that has since extended the renewal model to
include both Pavlovian and operant conditioning, multiple contex-
tual manipulations, and an increasingly broad denition of con-
texts [14, 17, 19, 21, 28, 41].
The denition of context has now expanded beyond environ-
ments that can be distinguished by various stimuli. As alluded
to in sections on spontaneous recovery, reacquisition, and resur-
gence, Bouton also views the presence or absence of manipulanda
and reinforcement as contextual factors, as well as the passage
of time [14]. In addition to the spatial contexts dened by en-
vironmental stimuli, there are also temporal, interoceptive (e.g.
hormonal or physiological states), cognitive, and social or cul-
tural contexts [19, 28]. Contextual renewal processes are there-
fore thought to be common to virtually all forms of relapse-like
behaviour [14, 28, 41].
In classical renewal models, contexts are dened by environ-
mental visual, olfactory, and tactile cues. These cues are used to
present animals with distinct contexts during acquisition, extinc-
tion, and renewal sessions. In the classic ABA design, a Pavlovian
or operant response is acquired in one context (‘A’), followed by ex-
tinction in a second context (‘B’), and then animals are returned
to the original training context (‘A’) for renewal [21]. It has since
been shown that renewal can also be triggered simply by removing
animals from the extinction context, whether extinction occurs in
the same context as acquisition (AAB renewal) or whether extinc-
tion occurs in its own distinct context and testing for renewal oc-
curs in a novel context (ABC renewal) [14]. Experiments by Bouton
and King showed that contexts tend not to accrue inhibitory as-
sociative value during extinction [81], in contrast with the predic-
tions of the Rescorla-Wagner model [82]. Bouton and colleagues
have therefore maintained, since the 1980s, that contexts have
an occasion setting mechanism which retrieves specic associa-
tions [14, 28, 83]. In other words, acquisition and extinction cre-
ate separate associations with a specic Pavlovian cue or operant
response and context determines which of these associations will
be retrieved, driving renewal in various contexts [14, 28, 83].
In addiction models, interest centres primarily on the ABA re-
newal paradigm [28, 62]. ABA renewal has been observed in self-
administration studies with a variety of drugs, such as alcohol
[59, 60, 77, 84], cocaine [85, 86], cocaine-heroin mixtures [87],
heroin [88], and nicotine [89]. However, other forms of renewal
such as AAB and ABC renewal are not so readily observed in studies
using drug reinforcers. For example, Zironi and colleagues tested
rats trained to self-administer alcohol in a novel context (ABC re-
newal) and did not observe signicant recovery while rats trained
to self-administer sucrose did show ABC renewal [77]. Similar re-
sults have been seen in an appetitive Pavlovian design, where ABA
renewal was observed with both alcohol and sucrose reinforcers
[78]. However, neither AAB nor ABC renewal was observed with
either reinforcer [78]. Similarly, Crombag and colleagues did not
observe AAB renewal following self-administration of a cocaine-
heroin mixture [87] and Fuchs and colleagues observed ABA but
not AAB renewal of cocaine self-administration [90]. Bossert
and colleagues also did not observe AAB renewal of heroin self-
administration [88], nor was AAB renewal for nicotineobser vedby
Diergaarde and colleagues [89]. Prior studies that have shown AAB
or ABC renewal in operant designs have all used food pellets or su-
crose as reinforcers [78, 91–93]. In instrumental studies with food
reinforcers, manipulations such as conducting acquisition train-
ing in multiple contexts have been shown to enhance ABC renewal
Lay & Khoo |5
[94], indicating the potential for improved generalisation of the
acquisition memory to promote ABC renewal in addiction studies.
However, this possibility has not yet been tested, so it remains un-
clear why these other forms of renewal are not easily observed us-
ing drug reinforcers. Nonetheless, the contextual renewal effect
appears to be sensitive to the identity of the reinforcer and this
may be important for its applications in addiction neuroscience
and clinical practice.
The addiction literature also refers to renewal designs as
“context-induced reinstatement”. While Bouton and colleagues
have consistently used the term renewal to refer to recovery due
to context change since 1979 [14, 21, 28, 41, 81], there is some
historical precedent for this alternative nomenclature. In 1978,
Welker and McAuley reported that when contextual and transport
cues were “reinstated” after extinction, that operant responding
for food returned [79]. Although their work is cited as an ex-
ample of renewal [41], they were actually interested in extinc-
tion and spontaneous recovery and do not use the term renewal
[79]. In fact, Welker and McAuley refer to their contextual ma-
nipulations as “reinstating responding during the nal session of
extinction” [79]. By the early 2000s the term renewal had be-
come well-dened and widespread in both the Pavlovian and op-
erant literature [14, 41, 95, 96], including studies involving drug
self-administration [25, 85, 87, 89]. However, addiction neuro-
scientists also began to refer to renewal designs as reinstatement
around the same time [88, 97–99], with some authors transition-
ing from using renewal to reinstatement [87, 100, 101] and oth-
ers applying both terms interchangeably [84, 86, 89, 102, 103]. As
will be discussed further below, reinstatement in the addiction lit-
erature has become an umbrella term that covers multiple recov-
ery phenomena beyond the classical design of Pavlovian reinstate-
ment studies. These differing recovery phenomena have diverse
associative mechanisms due to differences between Pavlovian and
instrumental learning, as well as the various processes used to pre-
cipitate recovery-from-extinction.
Reinstatement
In Pavlovian experiments, reinstatement of responding to an ex-
tinguished cue is typically observed following re-exposure to the
aversive or appetitive event, often unsignalled and usually prior to
testing [22]. For instance, studies of extinction following Pavlo-
vian fear conditioning show that presentations of the foot-shock
can reinstate fear responding [17, 21, 22, 41, 56, 104, 105] as well
as other aversive triggers that induce the state of fear such as ex-
posure to stressors (e.g. a milder foot-shock than that used in con-
ditioning; [105]) or to a conditioned context [106, 107]. This phe-
nomenon has also been demonstrated in animal studies of reward
learning [108], as well as in human studies [109–112]. The term
reinstatement is also used to describe recovery that occurs when
the CS is tested with other stimuli that have been separately con-
ditioned with the US [41], although denitions of reinstatement
do not always recognise this usage [28]. For example, Halladay
and colleagues found that presentations of an equally aversive, un-
extinguished CS after extinction training reinstated freezing to a
different, extinguished CS on test [113]. Moreover, this reinstate-
ment of responding induced in the absence of the US is evident in
human research [114].
Reinstatement is thought to depend on the restoration or re-
trieval of the association between the CS and US. A typical Pavlo-
vian design is shown in Figure 1a-c. During acquisition, an asso-
ciation between the CS and US is acquired (Figure 1a). This asso-
ciation is weakened throughout extinction (Figure 1b). The pre-
sentation of the US, which usually occurs the day prior to the re-
instatement test [41], then produces a restoration of the CS-US
association (Figure 1c). Since Pavlov, it has been thought that ex-
tinction involves an inhibitory response that suppresses the condi-
tioned response [4, 41]. Even researchers who have argued that ex-
tinction may involve (partial) erasure do notargue against the sur-
vival of the original association and new learning of an inhibitory
response [56]. According to one view, the presentation of the US
is thought to reactivate the original association with the CS and
thus lead to a restoration of conditioned responding [41]. Alterna-
tively, Bouton and colleagues argue that reinstatement depends on
the context being associated with the US [28], because if it is pre-
sented in a different context, then reinstatement does not occur
[80]. Presentations of the US can also strengthen the CS-US asso-
ciation, despite the absence of the CS, via mediated conditioning
[104]. The prior presentations of the US may therefore restore its
association with the context, which then enables retrieval of the
CS-US association during test.
Historical Use of the Term “Reinstatement” in
Addiction Studies
In operant drug self-administration studies, what is described as
reinstatement differs from the stricterdenition used in the Pavlo-
vian and behavioural literature [28, 41]. The use of the term “rein-
statement” in a manner distinct from but related to the denition
used in Pavlovian conditioning began to emerge in the addiction
neuroscience literature in the 1970s and early 1980s [115]. These
early studies all used the term reinstatement to describe the return
of responding that was observed as a result of re-exposureto drugs
or drug-associated cues. As early as 1971, Stretch and colleagues
reported that instrumental responding for amphetamine could be
reinstated by injections of amphetaminewhich they theorised was
caused by “reinstatement of the drug state” [116]. In this 1971 pa-
per, and in two subsequent reports, they also use reinstatement to
refer to the restoration of response rates that occurred due to drug
injections [116–118]. In 1976, Davis and Smith described training
a neutral stimulus as a conditioned reinforcer by pairing it with
intravenously self-administered morphine [119]. After extinction
of the instrumental response, the conditioned reinforcer was de-
scribed as causing “reinstatement” or “restoration” of the instru-
mental response [119]. The experimental approach of Davis and
Smith is now the basis of the cue-induced reinstatement model in
widespread contemporary use.
In the early 1980s, de Wit and Stewart reported “reinstate-
ment” of operant responding for cocaine and heroin following
injection of various drugs or presentation of a cue that had pre-
viously been paired with drug delivery [120–123]. These semi-
nal papers are credited with establishing the reinstatement model
which has been used, in various forms, by addiction neuroscien-
tists ever since [115]. Numerous other studies have now shown
that presentations of a food or drug prime can also reinstate oper-
ant responding following extinction of the instrumental response
[23, 24, 116, 119, 124–129]. Moreover, in addition to the drug-
primed and cue-induced reinstatement models already developed,
subsequent studies also showed that reinstatement could be pre-
cipitated in animals by stressors such as foot-shocks [130, 131] or
by combining multiple precipitatingfactors, for example, by using
both cue presentation and drug priming [132]. As discussed above,
renewal is now also described as context-induced reinstatement
in the addiction neuroscience literature [100].
The term “reinstatement” has also been used to describe mod-
els of relapse after abstinence. Abstinence models may involve
forced abstinence, where animals are not given access to drugs
such as in the incubation of craving model, or abstinence that
is technically self-imposed due to punishment or the availability
of a more desirable alternative [133–135]. For example, Panlilio
and colleagues suppressed operant responding for opioids by pun-
ishment with foot-shocks and then “reinstated” responding us-
ing drug-priming [136]. Other studies using punishment-induced
abstinence have also referred to relapse-like processes as “re-
6|Neuroanatomy and Behaviour, 2021
Out ome
- Identit (Oi)
- V lue (Ov)
Outc me
- Identit (Oi)
- V lue (O )
Pavlovian Conditioning
Operant Reinstatement (Drug-primed or Stress-induced)
CS US
R
S
CS US
R
S
R
S
CS US
US
(Prior to test)
(a) Acquisition (b) Extinction (c) Reinstatement
(d) Acquisition (e) Extinction (f) Reinstatement
Outcome
- Identity (Oi)
- Value (Ov)
Drug-prime or stressor
(At start of test)
Figure 1. Associative learning in Pavlovian and operant drug-primed and stress-induced reinstatement paradigms. (a) In Pavlovian conditioning, acquisition occurs by
repeated pairings of a previously neutral conditioned stimulus (CS) with an unconditioned stimulus (US). (b) During extinction, the CS is presented in the absence of the
US. (c) Prior to reinstatement, animals are exposed to the US. During test, the performance of the conditioned response is increased. (d) During operant conditioning, a
response (R), such as a lever press, is paired with discrete stimuli (S) and a reinforcer which has both a perceptual identity (Oi) and incentive value (Ov). (e) In extinction,
only the drug (Oi/Ov) is withheld, resulting in extinctionof the R-Oi/Ovand S-Oi/Ovassociations. (f ) Immediately priorto reinstatement testing, rats are administered drug
to prime reinstatement or are exposed to a stressor. In either case, reinstatement must rely on the reactivation of the R-Oi/Ovassociations because the S-Oi/Ovassociation
was previously extinguished. Note that in some drug-primed or stress-induced reinstatement paradigms, the cue (S) is omitted, though this does not alter the importance
of the R-Oi/Ovassociations.
instatement” [137]. However, drug-seeking after abstinence is
not a recovery-from-extinction phenomenon. Rather, other au-
thors have tended to refer to such relapse-like processes as in-
cubation of craving [138] or simply as relapse [133, 139, 140].
While an in-depth discussion of the associative and behavioural
processes underlying relapse-after-abstinence models is beyond
the scope of the present paper it seems possible that punish-
ment and relapse models involve reacquisition, as suggested by
Panlilio and colleagues [136], or contextual or occasion-setting ef-
fects that reactivate the acquisition memory. Marchant and col-
leagues have also argued that punishment effects are dominated
by response-outcome associations that, like extinction, produce
context-dependent suppression of responding [141]. Where ab-
stinence produced by the availability of a desirable alternative,
response competition is the most obvious possibility and there
have even been studies that show, at least for cocaine, that drugs
can suppress the response for the appetitive non-drug alternative
[142].
Behavioural Processes in Drug-Primed Rein-
statement
Drug-primed reinstatement is consistent with the classical Pavlo-
vian denition of reinstatement, due to its precipitation by pre-
sentation of the drug outcome (Figure 1d-f). Associative learning
models posit that during acquisition the operant response (R) be-
comes associated with both the drug-paired cues (S) and with the
drug outcome (O), which has both a perceptual identity (Oi) and
incentive value (Ov; Figure 1d) [143]. The result is associations be-
tween R, S and Oi/Ov. In drug-primed reinstatement protocols, an-
imals may be trained without discrete drug-paired cues [123] or ex-
tinction can involve presentation of the cue, omitting only drug de-
livery [130]. For example, extinction procedures for drug-primed
reinstatement may be designed to merely withhold drug delivery
by substituting drug for saline or disconnecting the syringe pump
[123, 130], leaving any cues paired with infusions in place. Since
extinction is otherwise identical to self-administration training, it
is clear in these paradigms that the association between the oper-
ant response and drug delivery is extinguished (R-Oi/Ov; Figure
1e). Moreover, if cues are present, then their association with the
drug outcome (S-Oi/Ov) is also be extinguished. Drug-primed re-
instatement must therefore rely on the response-drug outcome
(R-Oi/Ov) association, which follows the classical denition of re-
instatement as occurring in response to the US [17, 22, 41, 56]. As
with Pavlovian reinstatement, theoretical accounts largely differ
with respect to whether the response-drug outcome association
is reactivated by contextual associations or whether its associative
value is restored.
Bouton and colleagues argue that reinstatement in Pavlovian
and fear conditioning designs is a context effect [14, 28] and their
reasoning clearly applies to drug-primed reinstatement. Since re-
instatement is context-dependent in Pavlovian fear conditioning
[80], Bouton and colleagues argue that reinstatement relies on
the animal expecting the US in that context [14, 28]. In Pavlo-
vian designs, this expectation is restored by presenting the US
prior to test. In addiction studies, the drug-priming injection
serves the same purpose. The subjective effects of the drug pro-
duce an interoceptive context and these can inuence extinction.
For example, alcohol has previously been shown to result in state-
dependent learning [144]. Citing these and other ndings showing
that drug-induced interoceptive states can inuence behaviour,
Bouton and colleagues argue that drugs produce interoceptive con-
texts [28]. Drug-primed reinstatement is therefore a function of
an interoceptive version of ABA renewal, where the priming injec-
tion returns the animal to the acquisition context, retrieving the
Lay & Khoo |7
response-drug outcome association which produces recovery of
responding.
Another explanation for drug-primed reinstatement can be
drawn from the Rescorla-Wagner model, according to Delamater
and Westbrook [56]. Delamater and Westbrook argue that the
Rescorla-Wagner model [82] predicts reinstatement because the
US presentations restore the associative strength of the previously
extinguished stimulus. Since the design of drug-primed reinstate-
ment is analogous to Pavlovian extinction and reinstatement, it
could also be argued that drug-priming restores the associative
strength between the response and the drug outcome (R-Oi/Ov),
driving a recovery of responding. In other words, both context
theory and the Rescorla-Wagner model rely on the response-drug
outcome association, but differ with respect to whether this asso-
ciation is reactivated by a drug-induced interoceptive context or
restored by drug priming.
Behavioural Processes in Stress-Induced Rein-
statement
Similar associative mechanisms may be involved in stress-
induced reinstatement. Stress-induced reinstatement experi-
ments are conducted in a manner that is essentially identical
to drug-primed reinstatement, except reinstatement is triggered
by presentation of a stressor (Figure 1d-f). Stress-induced re-
instatement paradigms can use a wide variety of stressors, such
as acute food deprivation, foot-shock, and pharmacological stres-
sors such as the anxiogenic drug yohimbine [131]. Stress-induced
reinstatement has also been observed for a variety of drugs, in-
cluding heroin, cocaine, methamphetamine, nicotine, and alcohol
[131]. While most studies of food-seeking have found that stres-
sors did not induce reinstatement of food-seeking [131], it has
been shown to be possible under certain conditions, such as when
rats receive daily exposure to the calorie-dense cafeteria diet [145].
Just as for drug-primed reinstatement, extinction procedures for
stress-induced reinstatement can merely withhold drug delivery
by substituting drug for saline or disconnecting the syringe pump
[130, 146]. Some stress-induced reinstatement designs present
the cue during reinstatement [147], while others have also omit-
ted drug-paired cues during reinstatement testing, despite their
presence during acquisition and extinction [146]. Stress-induced
reinstatement can also be paired with early life stress, such as post-
weaning social isolation, though this did not alter reinstatement
[148]. This procedural exibility demonstrates that cue-drug out-
come associations (S-Oi/Ov) are not necessary for stress-induced
reinstatement. Therefore, as with drug-primed reinstatement,
the stressor must act to restore or reactivate the response-drug
outcome association (R-Oi/Ov) to cause recovery of responding.
Bouton and colleagues have applied context theory specically
to stressors and argue that stressors are most likely to promote re-
covery of responding if they have also been paired with acquisition
[28, 149, 150]. Schepers and Bouton have conducted a series of ex-
periments using hunger and a chronic variable stress protocol to
produce interoceptive contexts [149, 150]. When hunger or stress
was associated with acquisition, but not extinction, then renewal
was observed when these conditions were restored prior to test
[149, 150]. Bouton and colleagues argue that these ndings show
that stress produces an interoceptive context [28]. Therefore, as
with drug-primed reinstatement, stress-induced reinstatement
removes animals from the extinction context which enables re-
trieval of the response-drug outcome association and recovery of
responding.
However, there are some procedural differences between
Schepers and Bouton’s studies and the stress-induced reinstate-
ment paradigm that suggest alternative explanations. In Schepers
and Bouton’s studies, hunger and stress were present during both
acquisition and test [149, 150]. In contrast, typical stress-induced
reinstatement designs do not introduce the stressor until after ex-
tinction [131]. For example, the rst stress-induced reinstatement
papers used 10 min of intermittent foot-shock in the test context
prior to the start of the session [146, 147]. Therefore, the design
of stress-induced reinstatement studies does not follow the ABA
renewal design used by Schepers and Bouton, but more closely re-
sembles an interoceptive ABC renewal design because acquisition,
extinction, and reinstatement are each associated with their own
interoceptive states produced, respectively, by the drug’s subjec-
tive effects, the absence of drug, and stress. Moreover, Schepers
and Bouton used food as a reinforcer [149, 150], while most stud-
ies of stress-induced reinstatementhave shown that food-seeking
is not reinstated by stress [131]. These procedural differences raise
the possibility that stress-induced reinstatement is driven by fac-
tors others than interoceptive contexts.
One possibility is that stress-induced reinstatement functions
through largely non-associative affective mechanisms as animals
attempt to relieve their negative affective state via drug-seeking.
Drug addiction has previously been theorised to involve processes
of negative reinforcement, where drug use alleviates aversive
states [151–153]. If this were true, then it would imply that the
recovery of responding observed during stress-induced reinstate-
ment is goal-directed. As Trask and colleagues have argued, Pavlo-
vian and operant extinction and recovery phenomena share many
features and common processes, but goal-directed vs habitual ac-
tions are unique to operant behaviours [154]. If stress-induced
reinstatement is directed towards alleviating aversive states pro-
duced by the stressor, then the recovery of responding relies on the
association between the operant response and theaffective value of
the drug outcome. Therefore, stress-induced reinstatement may
be sensitive to outcome devaluation manipulations and this possi-
bility invites empirical verication.
Behavioural Processes in Cue-Induced Reinstatement
Cue-induced reinstatement is driven by ambiguous behavioural
and associative processes. Cue-induced reinstatement is a com-
mon relapse model that is driven by the presentation of an un-
extinguished drug-paired cue [98, 155, 156]. During acquisition,
animals rst learn an operant response (e.g. lever press) for food-
or drug-reward paired with a light or tone cue (Figure 2a). The
operant response is then extinguished such that lever presses no
longer results in outcome delivery or presentations of the cue (Fig-
ure 2b) [103, 157–159]. This extinguishes both the response-cue
(R-S) and response-drug (R-O) associations, but leaves the cue-
drug (S-O) associations intact (Figure 2b). Unlike other Pavlovian
reinstatement paradigms, reinstatement of responding in cue-
induced reinstatement is assessed using response-contingentpre-
sentations of the food- or drug-paired cue (Figure 2c). In other
words, the animal makes a response for the non-extinguished
reward-associated cue. Now, given that reward-paired cues can
elicit conditioned responding in and of themselves (see [160]),
dissociating the mechanism mediating the return in responding
when the cue is presented as the outcome for the extinguished re-
sponse becomes challenging. It has also been noted that the us-
age of the term reinstatement in cue-induced reinstatement is not
consistent with the denition used in Pavlovian conditioning and
most of the behavioural literature [28].
The associative processes underlying cue-induced reinstate-
ment are important from both theoretical and practical or transla-
tional perspectives. Procedural differences between cue-induced
reinstatement and the therapeutic approaches it is supposed to
model, reduce its translational potential. Specically, the lack
of extinction of the S-O association in the cue-induced reinstate-
ment model does not properly model the cue-exposure therapy
paradigms in humans [161]. In a clinical context, cue-exposure
therapy involves repeated presentation of drug-associated cues,
8|Neuroanatomy and Behaviour, 2021
Out ome
- Identit (Oi)
- V lue (Ov)
Outc me
- Identit (Oi)
- V lue (O )
Cue-induced Reinstatement
R
S
RR
S
?
?
(a) Acquisition (b) Extinction (c) Reinstatement
Outcome
- Identity (Oi)
- Value (Ov)
Figure 2. Associative processes in cue-induced reinstatement are ambiguous. (a) During operant conditioning, a response (R), such as a lever press, is paired with discrete
stimuli (S) and a reinforcer which has both a perceptual identity (Oi) and incentive value (Ov). (b) In extinction, both the cues and drug (Oi/Ov) are withheld, resulting in
extinction of the R-S and R-Oi/Ovassociations. (c) During cue-induced reinstatement, the cue is available again (in some procedures, it is presented non-contingently at
the beginning of the session), but it is unclear whether reinstatement occurs via reactivation of the R-S, R-Oi, or R-Ovassociations. In the absence of drug, reinstatement
also serves as extinction for S-Oiand S-Ov.
which would be more appropriately modelled by Pavlovian extinc-
tion of drug-paired cues rather than instrumental extinction. In
the cue-induced reinstatement paradigm, it is only the instrumen-
tal response that is extinguished, so cue-induced reinstatement is
not an ideal model for cue-exposure therapy and relapse.
The ambiguity regarding the associative processes which drive
cue-induced reinstatement also limit its contribution to theory.
If the instrumental response is extinguished alone, it is unclear
what is causing the restoration of responding observed during re-
instatement. As discussed above, in Pavlovian conditioning it is
the restoration or reactivation of the CS-US association that drives
reinstatement. However, in operant cue-induced reinstatement
models, the analogous S-O association was never extinguished.
With respect to associative learning, this leaves three possibilities
– the R-S, S-Oi, and S-Ovassociations. Experimental evidence
suggests that the R-S association alone is not the driver of cue-
induced reinstatement, for reasons that will be discussed below,
but there remains some ambiguity regarding the role of S-Oior S-
Ovassociations.
Cue-Induced Reinstatement Relies on Cue-Outcome Associations
A small number of studies have demonstrated that cue-induced
reinstatement relies on cue-drug outcome (S-O) associations
because separate Pavlovian conditioning or extinction of drug-
paired cues can alter later cue-induced reinstatement. One ex-
ample is the Pavlovian cue-conditioned reinstatement approach,
which demonstrates that a Pavlovian conditioned cue can promote
reinstatement [162, 163]. As shown in Figure 3, rats are trained to
self-administer cocaine without cues and givena single Pavlovian
conditioning session in the middle of self-administration training.
These Pavlovian conditioned cues can later precipitate reinstate-
ment after instrumental extinction when they are presented con-
tingently [162, 163]. Since the operant response and drug-paired
cue were never combined, this design demonstrates that reinstate-
ment relies on the Pavlovian associations between the cue and the
drug outcome.
Studies that combined Pavlovian non-contingent extinction
with instrumental extinction have further shown the importance
of the S-O associations and further demonstrated context effects.
In these designs, rats are trained to self-administer in the pres-
ence of cues (Figure 4a), before receiving two separate kinds of ex-
tinction (Figure 4b). Instrumental extinction follows standard pro-
cedures, omitting both cue and drug. However, additional Pavlo-
vian extinction sessions present the cue alone in a non-contingent
manner, extinguishing the S-O associations. At test, reinstate-
ment is diminished because all of the associations have been ex-
tinguished (Figure 4c), but these demonstrate that the Pavlovian
S-O association is important because the reinstatement test was
not simply identical to the instrumental extinction sessions as it
was in previous studies [164]. Buffalari and colleagues also con-
ducted extinction with the cues present, which leaves R-S intact,
or Pavlovian extinction of the S-O association alone, leaving R-O
intact [164]. They found, unsurprisingly, that rats extinguished
with cues present showed the least reinstatement while rats that
received Pavlovian extinction of the cuealone had the highest level
of reinstatement [164]. These effects may also be context depen-
dent. Torregrossa and colleagues gave rats instrumental extinc-
tion and then a phase of non-contingent cue extinction in either
the training context (A) or a distinct extinction context (B). They
found that when non-contingent extinction was given in context
A, this produced the lowest levels of cue-induced reinstatement
[165]. Non-contingent cue extinction in context B was not effec-
tive unless combined with d-cycloserine treatment [165].
Separate studies using the same approach in a single-context
paradigm have replicated these results. In a study by Perry and
colleagues, rats were trained to self-administer cocaine followed
by standard instrumental extinction [166]. Rats that subsequently
received Pavlovian non-contingentcue extinction showed reduced
cue-induced reinstatement relative to controls [166]. Follow-up
studies from the same group have replicated these ndings in
adult, but not adolescent rats undergoing cue-induced reinstate-
ment [167], and shown that non-contingent cue extinction can
effectively abolish incubation of craving [168]. Together, these
ndings seem to indicate that it is learned associations between
the drug-paired cue and the drug outcome (S-O) that drive cue-
induced reinstatement. However, while these studies clearly
demonstrate that the S-O associations are important, they don’t
provide evidence about whether it is S-Oior general affective S-Ov
associations that drive reinstatement.
Unfortunately, there is no simple solution to this ambiguity be-
cause cue-induced reinstatement designs require there to be dis-
crete drug-paired cues during self-administration training and for
those cues to be omitted during extinction. Unlike other recov-
ery procedures, such as contextual renewal studies where drug-
paired cues can be present [102, 169] or absent [103] during ex-
tinction, cue-induced reinstatement has no alternative trigger for
recovery. If drug-paired cues are retained during extinction, then
the response rate will decline and the cue-induced reinstatement
test will simply be identical to another extinction session. Con-
temporary reinstatement designs do not reinforce responses dur-
ing test [103, 157–159], nor is this a possible solution because oth-
erwise they would be rapid reacquisition experiments [41, 169].
Lay & Khoo |9
Outcome
- Identity (Oi)
- V lue (Ov)
Pavlovian cue-conditioned reinstatement
R
(a) Instrumental Training
S
With one Pavlovian session
(b) Extinction (c) Reinstatement
R
Outcome
- Identity (Oi)
- Value (Ov)
Outcome
- Identity (Oi)
- Value (Ov)
Out om
- Identit (Oi)
- V lue (O )
R
S
Figure 3. A Pavlovian cue, conditioned separately to the self-administration, can trigger reinstatement. (a) During instrumental training, animals learn to associate the
operant response with the outcome. They also receive a single, separate, Pavlovian conditioning sessionwhich pairs a cue with the same outcome. (b) Animals then receive
standard extinction for the operant response. (c) The Pavlovian conditioned cue is able to promote reinstatement without ever having been associated with the operant
response.
In order to produce a return of responding, there must be some
kind of precipitating factor. In Pavlovian designs, this is achieved
with the US, but for cue-induced reinstatement, this has to be the
never-extinguished drug-paired cues. Current views centre on
the idea that it is a form of conditioned reinforcement [28], how-
ever,we argue that cue-induced reinstatement is more ambiguous
and complex than this. There are procedural and empirical reasons
to believe that cue-induced reinstatement may be better concep-
tualised as reacquisition of conditioned reinforcement or, alterna-
tively, that it may involve Pavlovian to Instrumental Transfer [28].
Reinstatement as Reacquisition of Conditioned Reinforcement
Conditioned reinforcers are previously neutral stimuli that have
become reinforcers through repeated pairingswith a primar y rein-
forcer [170–172]. In some cases, the denition is operationalised
by the requirement that they can support and maintain new oper-
ant responses [173, 174]. The absence of extinction for the drug-
paired cues’ S-O association suggests that the reinstatement ef-
fect observed during cue-induced reinstatement might be better
classied as conditioned reinforcement. This is analogous with
the idea proposed by Davis and Smith in 1976 that cues can pro-
mote reinstatement [119]. Moreover, some researchers have gone
as far as referring to cue-induced reinstatementas an alias for con-
ditioned reinforcement [175]. Bouton and colleagues have also re-
cently considered cue-induced reinstatement and argue that it is
driven by conditioned reinforcement, rendering it distinct from
drug-primed or stress-induced reinstatement [28]. Conditioned
reinforcement involves animals responding for the cue and the
classical Pavlovian view of conditioned reinforcement is that the
cue itself acquires conditioned value [170, 171]. Conditioned rein-
forcement is also one of the key phenomena cited in support of the
incentive sensitization theory of addiction [176]. Consistent with
the classical Pavlovian view, incentive sensitization theory posits
that repeated pairings between the cue and the drug results in
some of the drug’s incentive motivational properties being trans-
ferred to the cue. This incentive motivational transfer is thought
to be observable in sign-tracking behaviour, where animals ap-
proach and attempt to interact with appetitive cues [176].
Evidence for Conditioned Reinforcement in Cue-Induced Reinstate-
ment
Several studies have shown that discrete cues paired with drug
delivery acquire conditioned reinforcing properties during self-
administration as animals will respond for the presentation of
these cues alone in later tests [174, 177–180]. These effects may be
particularly pronounced for nicotine because nicotine-paired cues
alone have been shown to maintain responding for several days af-
ter a prolonged 40-dayself-administration phase [181]. Even if cue
omission during extinction results in the extinction of the R-S as-
sociation, reinstatement is explained by the conditioned reinforc-
ing properties of the drug-paired cues, which have acquired their
own incentive value. It would therefore not be the R-O association
that is reactivated during reinstatement, but an R-S-O association
that drives responding.
However, if cue-induced reinstatement really is driven by con-
ditioned reinforcement, as both Kawa and colleagues and Bouton
and colleagues have suggested [28, 175], then it may actually be a
form of reacquisition. In their study, Kawa and colleagues trained
rats to nosepoke for cocaine. Each cocaine delivery was simultane-
ously paired with presentation of a cue light. Following standard
protocols, nosepokes made during extinction had no programmed
consequences, but during their reinstatement test nosepokes re-
sulted in cue presentation but not drug delivery [175]. However,
the R-S association should have been extinguished by cue omis-
sion during the extinction phase. When the CS and US are paired
again after extinction in Pavlovian designs, this is referred to as
reacquisition [41]. If the cue is a conditioned reinforcer and is
paired with the response again after extinction, then this design
more closely matches reacquisition of conditioned reinforcement
than simply conditioned reinforcement.
Even if cue-induced reinstatement is driven by conditioned re-
inforcement or its reacquisition, this does not necessarily clarify
the mechanism by which the cue elicits responding. For example,
is the elevation in responding during cue-induced reinstatement
because the cue is reinforcing in itself or does cue presentationpro-
duce an excitatory signal that stimulates further responding? Sha-
han has argued that conditioned reinforcement occurs because the
cue acts as a sign-post towards the physiologically-relevant rein-
forcer [182]. According to this view, animals respond for predictive
stimuli because of their temporal relationship with the reinforcer
[182–184]. Along with the classical Pavlovian account of condi-
tioned reinforcement as acquiring conditioned value [170, 171],
this would imply that conditioned reinforcement is driven by the
S-O association. Parkinson and colleagues found that a sucrose-
paired Pavlovian conditioned reinforceris not sensitive to outcome
devaluation, which they suggest may be because conditioned rein-
forcers activate a central appetitive motivational state or can be-
come a goal in their own right [185]. Further studies are required
to assess whether these ndings are relevant to cue-induced rein-
statement for drugs, for example by conducting devaluation of the
cue or outcome prior to reinstatement testing.
Conditioned Reinforcement Does Not Fully Explain Cue-Induced Re-
instatement
Conditioned reinforcement provides a compelling explanation for
cue-induced reinstatement, but it does not fully explain all aspects
10 |Neuroanatomy and Behaviour, 2021
Out ome
- Identit (O )
- V lue (Ov
Separate instrumental and Pavlovian extinction
R
S
R
S
R
S
(a) Acquisition (b) Extinction (c) Reinstatement diminished
S
Separate Pavlovian extinction
Outcome
- Identity (Oi)
- Value (Ov)
Outcome
- Identit (Oi)
- V lue (Ov)
Outcome
- Identit (Oi)
- V lue (O )
Figure 4. Reinstatement relies on the Pavlovian association. (a) In rats trained under standard conditions, where their operant response is paired with both a drug outcome
and a cue, (b) if instrumental extinction is complemented with separatePavlovian extinction sessions, (c) their reinstatement response is diminished.
of the phenomenon. For example, non-contingent cue presen-
tations at the start of the session have been used to precipitate
cue-induced reinstatement in mice trained to nosepoke for nico-
tine [186]. Non-contingent presentation of the cue prior to ex-
tension of the lever can also promote reinstatement of cocaine-
seeking and sucrose-seeking in rats [187, 188]. Moreover, non-
contingent cue presentationis not a redundant reinstatement trig-
ger because studies of cue-induced reinstatement of alcohol and
cocaine-seeking have used non-contingent cue presentations in
cases when animals did not earn their own cue-presentations by
operant responding early in the session [157, 167, 189]. The time
course of responding during cue-induced reinstatement also sug-
gests that there are contributing factors other than conditioned re-
inforcement. Tunstall and Kearns have reported, for example, that
approximately half of lever presses during cue-induced reinstate-
ment for cocaine occurred during the 10 s cue presentation [190].
Indeed, if responses during cue presentation are excluded, it ap-
pears as if rats are barely increasing their respondingabove extinc-
tion levels (approximately 20 responses under extinction vs. 30
responses during reinstatement) [190]. Similar results have been
found with cue-induced reinstatement of sucrose-seeking, where
approximately half of responses were during cue presentation or
time-out [191]. Assuming cue-induced reinstatement is driven
by conditioned reinforcement, this pattern of responding suggests
that rats are responding not only to obtain the cue, but because of
the cue.
Furthermore, contingent cocaine-paired cues appear to have
no effect on established instrumental responding, suggesting
they provide little conditioned reinforcement in many self-
administration studies. If animals are trained to self-administer
cocaine in the presence of cues, the removal of these cues does not
alter responding [192]. Moreover, if extinction is initiated without
cocaine delivery but with the presentation of cues, rats will rapidly
extinguish their responding [192] with no signicant difference
when compared with rats that receive extinction of the lever alone
[164]. If the cocaine-paired cues were indeed acting as conditioned
reinforcers, as they have previously been shown to support the ac-
quisition of a new response in sessions across multiple days [180],
then an operant response that is still paired with the cue should be
more resistant to extinction than for the lever alone. These results
indicate that, at least for cocaine, the presence or absence of the
cue during extinction is not sufcient to maintain responding.
In the case of nicotine, conditioned reinforcement may make a
larger contribution to cue-induced reinstatement. Nicotine facili-
tates the acquisition of conditioned reinforcement [193] and cues
are important for the acquisition of nicotine self-administration
[194]. Once self-administration has become established, nicotine-
paired cues can then maintain responding on their own (i.e. in the
absence of nicotine), for months [195] demonstrating a powerful
and persistent conditioned reinforcement effect. Similarly, cue-
induced reinstatement for nicotine persists across multiple tests,
although lower doses of nicotine may only support a single rein-
statement test [196]. These results suggest although conditioned
reinforcement might not fully explain cue-induced reinstatement
for several drugs of abuse, its relative contribution varies between
drugs and may be greater for nicotine.
Cue-Induced Reinstatement as Pavlovian to Instrumental Transfer
Cue-induced reinstatement protocols also strongly resemble
Pavlovian to Instrumental Transfer (PIT) and there is empirical
evidence that supports a role for PIT rather than conditioned re-
inforcement alone. Indeed, conditioned reinforcement itself has
been shown to be mediated by PIT mechanisms in some circum-
stances [173] and procedures which produce PIT can also produce
conditioned reinforcement [197]. For example, acquisition of an
operant response for a conditioned reinforcer can be insensitive to
outcome devaluation [173, 185], suggesting a general affective or
excitatory effect analogous to general transfer in PIT (also called
non-selective PIT). While there may be overlapin the processes in-
volved in both conditioned reinforcement and PIT, they can be dis-
tinguished behaviourally – such as in circumstances where a pro-
cedure produces one but not the other – and neuropharmacologi-
cal manipulations may also be specic for conditioned reinforce-
ment or PIT [197]. In PIT paradigms, animals receive separate
instrumental and Pavlovian conditioning for the same reinforcer
(Figure 5a-b). At test, animals continue to perform instrumental
responses, but presentations of the cue modulatethe rate of instru-
mental responding [197–199]. PIT mechanisms would therefore
explain the pattern of responding observed during cue-inducedre-
instatement, where a very high percentage of responses occur dur-
ing cue presentation [190, 191]. PIT is most commonly conducted
using non-drug reinforcers such as food pellets [200], but studies
using drug reinforcers have also been conducted [201–203]. In hu-
mans, a nicotine-paired cue potentiated instrumental responding
more than a food-paired cue [204], demonstrating that PIT may
vary depending on the reinforcer. As shown in Figure 5c, specic
PIT involves responding driven by the predictive value of the cue
via an S-Oi-R association while general PIT (Figure 5d) involves
responding driven by retrieval of the affective value via an [S-Ov]-
R association [143]. As discussed above, reinstatement is driven by
S-O associations [162, 164]. However, there remains some ambi-
guity in whether reinstatement is driven by S-Oior S-Ovassocia-
tions. PIT studies may therefore help to clarify this ambiguity.
The design of cue-induced reinstatement studies is more con-
sistent with a general or non-selective PIT effect driven by an [S-
Ov]-R association due to its use of a single outcome. There are
Lay & Khoo |11
Pavlovian to Instrumental Transfer
R
(a) Instrumental Training
(c) Specific Transfer
R
SOi
(d) General Transfer
R
SOv
S
(b) Pavlovian Conditioning
Outcome
- Identity (Oi)
- Value (Ov)
Outcome
- Identity (Oi)
- Value (Ov)
Outcome
Identity
Outcome
Value
Figure 5. Pavlovian to Instrumental Transfer (PIT) effects may explain cue-induced reinstatement. During PIT animals receive separate (a) instrumental training and (b)
Pavlovian conditioning for the same reinforcer. At test, presentation of the conditioned stimulus (S) increases instrumental responding (R) via (c) specic transfer of the
predictive associationswith the perceptual identity of the drug outcome (Oi) or (d) general transfer of the affective value(Ov) of the drug outcome that has become associated
with the cue.
different PIT procedures that can preferentially evoke general and
specic transfer, with the main variants called non-selective PIT
and outcome-specic PIT [197, 198]. In non-selective paradigms,
the Pavlovian conditioning phase involves two stimuli of which
only one is reinforced and the instrumental phase involves a sin-
gle lever paired with a single outcome. In outcome-specic trans-
fer paradigms, the Pavlovian conditioning phase provides a dif-
ferent reinforcer for each stimulus and the instrumental phase
similarly trains two levers each paired with their own outcome
[197, 198, 205, 206]. As reviewed by Cartoni and colleagues, non-
selective PIT usually produces general transferrather than specic
transfer [197], which is thought to be mediated by the general ex-
citatory or motivational function of the cue [198, 207, 208]. In
cue-induced reinstatement, the design of the study is most sim-
ilar to non-selective PIT because, although there are usually two
levers and only one cue, there is only one outcome. Following Hol-
land [209], Cartoni and colleagues suggest that general transfer
tends to be associated with non-specic PIT paradigms because
of their less detailed representations about the outcome. There-
fore, it would be expected that cue-induced reinstatement would
be mediated by a general PIT effect because the outcome represen-
tations in these designs are singular. Holland also showed that ex-
tended instrumental training (20 sessions) was more likely to re-
sult in general transfer than minimal instrumental training (5 ses-
sions) [209], which would also imply a role for general PIT in cue-
induced reinstatement since self-administration studies typically
involve 10 or more days of self-administration [157, 168, 189, 210–
214]. Bouton and colleagues have recently suggested a role forgen-
eral PIT in cue-induced reinstatement, notingmany common neu-
robiological substrates between them [28].
There is also some experimental evidence that drug-paired
cues can have outcome-specic effects. Rubio and colleagues
trained rats to press one lever for cocaine and, on alternate days, to
press a second lever for heroin [215]. Each drug delivery was paired
with activation of a cue light above its respective lever. Rats were
then subjected to standard lever extinction, where levers were in-
serted but had no programmed consequences. At test, rats re-
ceived initial non-contingent presentations of either the cocaine
cue or heroin cue immediately prior to extension of all levers, with
the lever corresponding to the drug presented at the start of the
session triggering cue presentations [215]. They found that cues
specically reinstated responding on their lever, but that they did
not trigger reinstatement on the alternative drug lever [215]. Their
ndings are consistent with previous studies of drug-primed rein-
statement of polydrug use, where animals weretrained on both co-
caine and heroin, but a priming injection only reinstated respond-
ing on the lever that matched the drug prime [216]. These ndings
do not rule out a role for general PIT because they involve much
more complex outcome representations, but they do suggest that
reinstatement may be goal-directed.
The procedural parallels between cue-induced reinstatement
and PIT, combined with evidence that suggests a potential goal-
directed component to reinstatement, suggest that PIT may con-
tribute to cue-induced reinstatement. However, further studies
that more precisely examine whether the specic S-Oior general
affective S-Ovassociations drive cue-induced reinstatement are
required. One approach is suggested by Clemens and colleagues
who combined outcome devaluation with extinction and drug-
primed reinstatement [217]. In their study, rats received nicotine
self-administration training followed by outcome devaluation by
pairing nicotine with lithium injections. Nicotine-primed rein-
statement was impaired in animals that had received 10, but not
47 days of self-administration training [217]. If this kind of design
could be replicated for cue-induced reinstatement, it might pro-
vide evidence about whether cue-induced reinstatement is driven
primarily by general PIT or whether it is a goal-directed behaviour.
Alternative Mechanisms in Cue-Induced Reinstatement
Alternative explanations for cue-induced reinstatement may arise
from other associative and non-associative mechanisms. Recent
work has shown that performance in a PIT paradigm does not dif-
fer signicantly from rats characterised as having an addiction-
like phenotype, based on motivation in a progressive ratio, persis-
tent responding during intermittent periods of drug unavailabil-
ity, and punishment-resistant responding [218]. Although per-
formance in the PIT paradigm did correlate with performance dur-
ing cocaine self-administration, it is not clear whether this would
translate to cue-induced reinstatement [218]. These studies did
not address whether PIT mediated reinstatement directly, but be-
cause PIT did not correlate with other addiction-like behaviours,
it suggests that these behaviours may not be completely driven by
the associative mechanisms discussed above.
One possible alternative mechanism is habit learning. Habit
learning is thought to involve cue-elicited drug-seeking without
retrieval of drug outcome (Oior Ov) memories [143]. While some
have disputed the importance of habits in drug addiction [219],
habit formation is commonly thought to support drug addiction
[217, 220, 221]. However, if habitual responding does not rely on
retrieval of the drug outcome, then this raises whether it is reacti-
vating R-O associations like Pavlovian reinstatement approaches
12 |Neuroanatomy and Behaviour, 2021
are thought to. It also might not be expected that Pavlovian non-
contingent cue extinction would be effective in reducing habitual
responding if no drug outcome memories are required.
Another mechanism that could play a role in cue-induced rein-
statement is incubation of craving. As discussed above, incubation
of craving refers to the time-dependent increase in drug-seeking
after cessation. Evidence from both human [222] and animal [133]
studies have shown an increase in the degree of cue-induced crav-
ing or reinstatement after longer periods of abstinence [223]. For
instance, humans experiencing incubation of craving report crav-
ing the drug more when exposed to drug-related cues after 35
days than after 7 days [222]. This increase in craving initially ap-
pears to be a non-associative mechanism that runs contrary to
the classical associative learningview that associative strength can
decay over time [27]. However, there are also plausible associa-
tive accounts of incubation of craving, such as a loss of reactive
inhibition [224], weakening of an opponent process that was in-
hibiting craving [153], or the Kamin effect – a U-shaped mem-
ory retention curve [225–229]. Further, incubation of craving ap-
pears to modulate drug memory retrieval because it can be inhib-
ited with further extinction training. This appears to be effective
whether animals are given instrumental extinction [230] or Pavlo-
vian non-contingent cue extinction [168], indicating that retrieval
of both the R-O and S-O associations may be important in rein-
statement. Therefore, cue-induced reinstatement may, at least in
part, rely on alternative mechanisms that modulate retrievalof the
previously-extinguished R-O associationand unextinguished S-O
association.
Reinstatement Nomenclature
As noted above, the terminology of reinstatement differs between
the addiction neuroscience literature and the behavioural litera-
ture [28]. This is not unusual in historical terms, as the term re-
instatement has been variously used to refer to resurgence [67],
and with respect to cues in a study now considered an antecedent
of contextual renewal [79]. Reinstatement has also been used
since the earliest operant relapse models in the addiction neuro-
science literature emerged in the 1970s and 1980s [119, 120, 123].
However, the differing usage of the term reinstatement between
drug self-administration studies and the generally Pavlovian be-
havioural literature does need to be recognised [28]. Despite both
being described as reinstatement, Pavlovian reinstatement and
cue-induced reinstatement in drug self-administration studies
are clearly driven by diverse associative mechanisms. Indeed, the
term reinstatement in the addiction literature has become more
of an umbrella term, encompassing relapse-like models driven by
drug priming, stress, cues, and context change.
The literature contains other examples of such pragmatic
resolutions of differences in nomenclature. For example, the
orexin/hypocretin system was simultaneously discovered by two
research groups via different approaches and given two names –
orexin and hypocretin [231, 232]. Both terms have neuroanatomic
or behavioural merit and are widely used resulting in a compro-
mise on nomenclature – hypocretin is the ofcial gene name and
pharmacologists use orexins to describe the ligands and recep-
tors [233]. Corticotropin releasing factor or corticotropin releas-
ing hormone (CRF/CRH) also has a disputed nomenclature based
on considerations of molecular structure and hormonal or extra-
hormonal functions [234, 235]. Like the orexin/hypocretins, one
term (CRH) became the ofcial nomenclature for geneticists while
the other (CRF) is used by pharmacologists to describe the protein
products [234]. In each case it is now a practical necessity to recog-
nise both terms because of their widespread usage. It seems that
a similar compromise is emerging for reinstatement, as it is now
acknowledged that the term’s usage is different between the addic-
tion and behavioural literature [28].
Conclusions
Several recovery-from-extinction approaches are currently used
in addiction neuroscience to model relapse. These include sponta-
neous recovery, rapid reacquisition, resurgence, renewal, and re-
instatement. In each case, there are multiple associative learning
approaches that can elucidate or provide insight into how the op-
erant response recovers after extinction, with context theory be-
ing one of the most inuential. In most cases, the associative pro-
cesses in Pavlovian designs and operant drug self-administration
studies are similar with the exception of cue-induced reinstate-
ment, where recovery of responding is driven by an ambigu-
ous process associated with the unextinguished drug-paired cue.
Since the instrumental response is extinguished with respect to
the drug outcome, the reinstatement effect is described by some
as a conditioned reinforcement effect, even though the cue is also
omitted during extinction. However, examination of the exper-
imental design suggests it is more akin to reacquisition of con-
ditioned reinforcement. The pattern of responding during cue-
induced reinstatement also implies that animals are responding
because of the cue, in addition to responding for the cue, suggest-
ing a potential role for Pavlovian to Instrumental Transfer. There
are also alternative mechanisms, such as incubation of craving,
that may modulate the retrieval of operant associations, including
the response-drug outcome (R-O). While the associative processes
that contribute to cue-induced reinstatement remain ambiguous,
this ambiguity suggests several additional hypotheses related to
conditioned reinforcement and PIT in cue-induced reinstatement
that invite empirical validation. Although reinstatement terminol-
ogy and experimental procedures differ between associative learn-
ing and addiction neuroscience, it is clear that associative learning
mechanisms are highly relevant and informative to understand-
ing the processes mediating relapse-like behaviours. As scientists
turn to associative learning models to develop improvements in
extinction-based therapies for addiction [3, 9, 10], a better under-
standing of the associative learningthat underpins relapse is likely
to be essential for improving future clinical outcomes.
Declarations
Acknowledgements
The authors gratefully acknowledge Mihaela Iordanova and Gavan
McNally for comments on the draft manuscript.
Funding
BPPL and SYK are supported by Fonds de Recherche du Québec -
Santé (Award IDs: #276908 & #270051).
Conict of Interest Declaration
BPPL is an editor for Neuroanatomy and Behaviour and Treasurer
of Episteme Health. SYK is Editor-in-Chief for Neuroanatomy and
Behaviour and President of Episteme Health.
Editorial Notes
History
Received: 2020-12-19
Revisions Requested: 2021-02-06
Revised: 2021-02-15
Accepted: 2021-02-19
Published: 2021-02-23
Lay & Khoo |13
Editorial Checks
Plagiarism: Plagiarism detection software found no evidence
of plagiarism.
References: Zotero did not identify any references in the Re-
tractionWatch database.
Peer Review
The review process for this paper was conducted double-blind be-
cause at least one of the authors is a member of the committee of
management of the publisher, Episteme Health Inc. During re-
view, neither the authors nor the reviewers were aware of each
other’s identities.
For the benet of readers, reviewers are asked to write a public
summary of their review to highlight the key strengths and weak-
nesses of the paper. Signing of reviews is optional.
Reviewer 1 (Sarah Baracz, UNSW Sydney, Australia.)
This review article provides a comprehensive overview of associa-
tive learning theories and demonstrates how suchtheories provide
insight into patterns of behaviour evident when modelling aspects
of addiction, particularly reinstatement. The authors provide a
compelling argument that cue-induced reinstatement is likely ex-
plained by two theories, these being reacquisition of conditioned
reinforcement and Pavlovian to Instrumental Transfer.
Reviewer 2 (Anonymous)
This review provides a comprehensiveand scholarly analysis of the
studies that use particularly the extinction-reinstatement model
to study relapse in substance use disorder. It covers both histor-
ical and seminal ndings as well as recent ndings, and is both
engaging and informative. It deals almost exclusively with oper-
ant self-administration, not taking into account models such as
conditioned place preference (mentioned but not really reviewed).
Another notable omission are more recent variants of extinction-
reinstatement model (e.g. voluntary abstinence following punish-
ment/social interaction), which may perhaps be seen as beyond
the scope of the review. Nevertheless, the conclusion drawn -
that an understanding of associative processes is important to un-
derstanding substance use and relapse - is worthwhile and well-
justied by the arguments presented.
Reviewer 3 - References Review (Anonymous)
I have checked the paper’s references and have found that the in-
formation on each reference is correct and complete, papers have
been cited appropriately and the referencelist contains only papers
in legitimate peer-reviewed sources with no applicable editorial
notices.
References
1. Hoffman DC. The use of place conditioning in studying
the neuropharmacology of drug reinforcement. Brain Re-
search Bulletin. 1989;23(4–5):373–387. doi: 10.1016/0361-
9230(89)90224-4.
2. Weeks JR. Experimental morphine addiction: Method for au-
tomatic intravenous injections in unrestrained rats. Science.
1962;138(3537):143–144. doi: 10.1126/science.138.3537.143.
3. Konova AB, Goldstein RZ. The emerging neuroscience of ap-
petitive and drug cue extinction in humans. Psychophar-
macology. 2019;236(1):407–414. doi: 10.1007/s00213-018-
5098-y.
4. Pavlov IP. Conditional reexes: An investigation of the phys-
iological activity of the cerebral cortex. London: Oxford Uni-
versity Press; 1927. OCLC: 930518342.
5. Byrne SP, Haber P, Baillie A, Giannopolous V, Morley K.
Cue exposure therapy for alcohol use disorders: What can
be learned from exposure therapy for anxiety disorders?
Substance Use & Misuse. 2019;54(12):2053–2063. doi:
10.1080/10826084.2019.1618328.
6. Segawa T, Baudry T, Bourla A, Blanc JV, Peretti CS, Mouch-
abac S, et al. Virtual reality (VR) in asse ssmentand treatment
of addictive disorders: A systematic review. Frontiers in Neu-
roscience. 2020;13(1409). doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.01409.
7. Conklin CA, Tiffany ST. Applying extinction research
and theory to cue-exposure addiction treatments. Ad-
diction. 2002;97(2):155–167. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-
0443.2002.00014.x.
8. Marissen MAE, Franken IHA, Blanken P, van den Brink W,
Hendriks VM. Cue exposure therapy for the treatment of
opiate addiction: Results of a randomized controlled clini-
cal trial. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 2007;76(2):97–
105. doi: 10.1159/000097968.
9. Torregrossa MM, Taylor JR. Learning to forget: manipu-
lating extinction and reconsolidation processes to treat ad-
diction. Psychopharmacology. 2013;226(4):659–672. doi:
10.1007/s00213-012-2750-9.
10. Havermans RC, Jansen ATM. Increasing the efcacy of
cue exposure treatment in preventing relapse of addictive
behavior. Addictive Behaviors. 2003;28(5):989–994. doi:
10.1016/S0306-4603(01)00289-1.
11. Maren S. Fear of the unexpected: Hippocampus mediates
novelty-induced return of extinguished fear in rats. Neu-
robiology of Learning and Memory. 2014;108:88–95. doi:
10.1016/j.nlm.2013.06.004.
12. Kehoe EJ, Macrae M. Savings in animal learning: Im-
plications for relapse and maintenance after therapy. Be-
havior Therapy. 1997;28(1):141–155. doi: 10.1016/S0005-
7894(97)80039-1.
13. Rescorla RA. Retraining of extinguished Pavlovian stimuli.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Pro-
cesses. 2001;27(2):115–124. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.27.2.115.
14. Bouton ME, Winterbauer NE, Todd TP. Relapse pro-
cesses after the extinction of instrumental learning: Re-
newal, resurgence, and reacquisition. Behavioural Processes.
2012;90(1):130–141. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2012.03.004.
15. Rescorla RA. Spontaneous recovery after Pavlovian condi-
tioning with multiple outcomes. Animal Learning & Behav-
ior. 1997;25(1):99–107. doi: 10.3758/BF03199028.
16. Leung HT, Westbrook FR. Spontaneous recovery of extin-
guished fear responses deepens their extinction: A role for
error-correction mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psy-
chology: Animal Behavior Processes. 2008;34(4):461–474.
doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.34.4.461.
17. Bouton ME. Context, ambiguity, and unlearning: sources
of relapse after behavioral extinction. Biological Psychiatry.
2002;52(10):976–986. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01546-
9.
18. Epstein R, Skinner BF. Resurgence of responding after the
cessation of response-independent reinforcement. Proceed-
ings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1980;77(10):6251–
6253. doi: 10.1073/pnas.77.10.6251.
19. Maren S, Phan KL, Liberzon I. The contextual brain: impli-
cations for fear conditioning, extinction and psychopathol-
ogy. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2013;14(6):417–428. doi:
10.1038/nrn3492.
20. G oode TD, Maren S. Animal models of fear relapse. ILAR Jour-
nal. 2014;55(2):246–258. doi: 10.1093/ilar/ilu008.
14 |Neuroanatomy and Behaviour, 2021
21. Bouton ME, Bolles RC. Contextual control of the extinction of
conditioned fear. Learning and Motivation. 1979;10(4):445–
466. doi: 10.1016/0023-9690(79)90057-2.
22. Rescorla RA, Heth CD. Reinstatement of fear to an extin-
guished conditioned stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psy-
chology: Animal Behavior Processes. 1975;1(1):88–96. doi:
10.1037/0097-7403.1.1.88.
23. Kalivas PW, McFarland K. Brain circuitry and the reinstate-
ment of cocaine-seeking behavior. Psychopharmacology.
2003;168(1):44–56. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1393-2.
24. McFarland K, Kalivas PW. The circuitry mediating cocaine-
induced reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior. The
Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21(21):8655–8663. doi:
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-21-08655.2001.
25. Crombag HS, Grimm JW, Shaham Y. Effect of dopamine re-
ceptor antagonists on renewal of cocaine seeking by reexpo-
sure to drug-associated contextual cues. Neuropsychophar-
macology. 2002;27(6):1006–1015. doi: 10.1016/S0893-
133X(02)00356-1.
26. Peters J, De Vries TJ. Pavlovian conditioned approach, extinc-
tion, and spontaneous recovery to an audiovisual cue paired
with an intravenous heroin infusion. Psychopharmacology.
2014;231(2):447–453. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3258-7.
27. McConnell BL, Miller RR. Associative accounts of recovery-
from-extinction effects. Learning and Motivation.
2014;46:1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2014.01.003.
28. Bouton ME, Maren S, McNally GP. Behavioral and neurobi-
ological mechanisms of Pavlovian and instrumental extinc-
tion learning. Physiological Reviews. 2020;doi: 10.1152/phys-
rev.00016.2020.
29. Ellson DG. Quantitative studies of the interaction of sim-
ple habits. I. Recovery from specic and generalized ef-
fects of extinction. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
1938;23(4):339–358. doi: 10.1037/h0056285.
30. Youtz REP. Reinforcement, extinction, and spontaneous re-
covery in a non-Pavlovian reaction. Journal of Experimental
Psychology. 1938;22(4):305–318. doi: 10.1037/h0056358.
31. Skinner BF. The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-
Century-Crofts; 1938. OCLC: 1130955019.
32. Rescorla RA. Spontaneous recovery. Learning & Memory.
2004;11(5):501–509. doi: 10.1101/lm.77504.
33. Di Ciano P, Everitt BJ. Reinstatement and spontaneous re-
covery of cocaine-seeking following extinction and differ-
ent durations of withdrawal. Behavioural Pharmacology.
2002;13(5). doi: 10.1097/00008877-200209000-00013.
34. Peters J, Vallone J, Laurendi K, Kalivas PW. Opposing
roles for the ventral prefrontal cortex and the basolateral
amygdala on the spontaneous recovery of cocaine-seeking
in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2008;197(2):319–326. doi:
10.1007/s00213-007-1034-2.
35. Rodd-Henricks ZA, Bell RL, Kuc KA, Murphy JM, McBride WJ,
Lumeng L, et al. Effects of ethanol exposure on subsequent
acquisition and extinction of ethanol self-administration
and expression of alcohol-seeking behavior in adult alcohol-
preferring (P) rats: II. Adult exposure. Alcoholism: Clini-
cal and Experimental Research. 2002;26(11):1642–1652. doi:
10.1111/j.1530-0277.2002.tb02466.x.
36. Rodd-Henricks ZA, Bell RL, Kuc KA, Murphy JM, McBride WJ,
Lumeng L, et al. Effects of ethanol exposure on subsequent
acquisition and extinction of ethanol self-administration
and expression of alcohol-seeking behavior in adult alcohol-
preferring (P) rats: I. Periadolescent exposure. Alcoholism:
Clinical and Experimental Research. 2002;26(11):1632–1641.
doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2002.tb02465.x.
37. Hauser SR, Wilden JA, Deehan Jr GA, McBride WJ, Rodd
ZA. Cocaine inuences alcohol-seeking behavior and relapse
drinking in alcohol-preferring (P) rats. Alcoholism: Clini-
cal and Experimental Research. 2014;38(10):2678–2686. doi:
10.1111/acer.12540.
38. Shaham Y, Adamson LK, Grocki S, Corrigall WA. Rein-
statement and spontaneous recovery of nicotine seeking
in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1997;130(4):396–403. doi:
10.1007/s002130050256.
39. Struik RF, De Vries TJ, Peters J. Detrimental effects of
a retrieval-extinction procedure on nicotine seeking, but
not cocaine seeking. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience.
2019;13(243). doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00243.
40. LeCocq MR, Lahlou S, Chahine M, Padillo LN, Chaudhri
N. Modeling relapse to pavlovian alcohol-seeking in
rats using reinstatement and spontaneous recovery
paradigms. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Re-
search. 2018;42(9):1795–1806. doi: 10.1111/acer.13825.
41. Bouton ME, Swartzentruber D. Sources of relapse after ex-
tinction in Pavlovian and instrumental learning. Clinical
Psychology Review. 1991;11(2):123–140. doi: 10.1016/0272-
7358(91)90091-8.
42. Skinner BF. Are theories of learning necessary? Psychologi-
cal Review. 1950;57(4):193–216. doi: 10.1037/h0054367.
43. Amsel A. The role of frustrative nonreward in noncontinuous
reward situations. Psychological Bulletin. 1958;55(2):102–
119. doi: 10.1037/h0043125.
44. Johnson JS, Escobar M, Kimble WL. Long-term mainte-
nance of immediate or delayed extinction is determined
by the extinction-test interval. Learning & Memory.
2010;17(12):639–644. doi: 10.1101/lm.1932310.
45. McSweeney FK, Hinson JM, Cannon CB. Sensitization-
habituation may occur during operant conditioning. Psy-
chological Bulletin. 1996;120(2):256–271. doi: 10.1037/0033-
2909.120.2.256.
46. Storsve AB, McNally GP, Richardson R. US habituation, like
CS extinction, produces a decrement in conditioned fear re-
sponding that is NMDA dependent and subject to renewal
and reinstatement. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.
2010;93(4):463–471. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.12.011.
47. Storsve AB, McNally GP, Richardson R. Renewal and re-
instatement of the conditioned but not the unconditioned
response following habituation of the unconditioned stim-
ulus. Behavioural Processes. 2012;90(1):58–65. doi:
10.1016/j.beproc.2012.03.007.
48. McSweeney FK, Johnson KS. The effect of time between
sessions on within-session patterns of responding. Be-
havioural Processes. 1994;31(2):207–217. doi: 10.1016/0376-
6357(94)90007-8.
49. Grimm JW, Hope BT, Wise RA, Shaham Y. Incubation of co-
caine craving after withdrawal. Nature. 2001;412(6843):141–
142. doi: 10.1038/35084134.
50. Gawin FH, Kleber HD. Abstinence symptomatology and
psychiatric diagnosis in cocaine abusers: Clinical observa-
tions. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1986;43(2):107–113.
doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1986.01800020013003.
51. Hollander JA, Carelli RM. Cocaine-associated stimuli
increase cocaine seeking and activate accumbens core
neurons after abstinence. The Journal of Neuroscience.
2007;27(13):3535–3539. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3667-
06.2007.
52. Nicolas C, Russell TI, Pierce AF, Maldera S, Holley A, You
ZB, et al. Incubation of cocaine craving after intermittent-
access self-administration: Sex differences and estrous
cycle. Biological Psychiatry. 2019;85:915–924. doi:
10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.01.015.
53. Krasnova IN, Marchant NJ, Ladenheim B, McCoy MT,
Panlilio LV, Bossert JM, et al. Incubation of metham-
phetamine and palatable food craving after punishment-
induced abstinence. Neuropsychopharmacology.
2014;39(8):2008–2016. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.50.
54. Pickens CL, Airavaara M, Theberge F, Fanous S, Hope BT,
Lay & Khoo |15
Shaham Y. Neurobiology of the incubation of drug crav-
ing. Trends in Neurosciences. 2011;34(8):411–420. doi:
10.1016/j.tins.2011.06.001.
55. Paulson PE, Robinson TE. Amphetamine-induced time-
dependent sensitization of dopamine neurotransmission in
the dorsal and ventral striatum: A microdialysis study
in behaving rats. Synapse. 1995;19(1):56–65. doi:
10.1002/syn.890190108.
56. Delamater AR, Westbrook RF. Psychological and neural
mechanisms of experimental extinction: A selective review.
Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2014;108:38–51. doi:
10.1016/j.nlm.2013.09.016.
57. Perry CJ, McNally GP. Naloxone prevents the rapid reacqui-
sition but not acquisition of alcohol seeking. Behavioral Neu-
roscience. 2012;126(4):599–604. doi: 10.1037/a0029079.
58. Bouton ME, Swartzentruber D. Slow reacquisition following
extinction: Context, encoding, and retrieval mechanisms.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Pro-
cesses. 1989;15(1):43–53. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.15.1.43.
59. Willcocks AL, McNally GP. The role of context in re-
acquisition of extinguished alcoholic beer-seeking.
Behavioral Neuroscience. 2011;125(4):541–550. doi:
10.1037/a0024100.
60. Willcocks AL, McNally GP. The role of medial prefrontal
cortex in extinction and reinstatement of alcohol-seeking in
rats. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;37(2):259–268.
doi: 10.1111/ejn.12031.
61. Liu Y, Jean-Richard-dit Bressel P, Yau JOY, Willing A,
Prasad AA, Power JM, et al. The mesolimbic dopamine
activity signatures of relapse to alcohol-seeking. The
Journal of Neuroscience. 2020;40(33):6409–6427. doi:
10.1523/jneurosci.0724-20.2020.
62. Marchant NJ, Li X, Shaham Y. Recent developments in ani-
mal models of drug relapse. Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
2013;23(4):675–683. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.003.
63. Lattal KA, Cançado CRX, Cook JE, Kincaid SL, Nighbor TD,
Oliver AC. On dening resurgence. Behavioural Processes.
2017;141:85–91. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.04.018.
64. Trask S. Cues associated with alternative reinforcement dur-
ing extinction can attenuate resurgence of an extinguished
instrumental response. Learning & Behavior. 2019;47(1):66–
79. doi: 10.3758/s13420-018-0339-9.
65. Epstein R. On the rediscovery of the principle of resurgence.
Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis. 2015;41(2):19–43. doi:
10.5514/rmac.v41.i2.63722.
66. Hull CL. The rat’s speed-of-locomotion gradient in the
approach to food. Journal of Comparative Psychology.
1934;17(3):393–422. doi: 10.1037/h0071299.
67. Carey JP. Reinstatement of previously learned responses un-
der conditions of extinction: a study of ”regression.”. Amer-
ican Psychologist. 1951;6(7):284. doi: 10.1037/h0051240.
68. Leitenberg H, Rawson RA, Bath K. Reinforcement
of competing behavior during extinction. Science.
1970;169(3942):301–303. doi: 10.1126/science.169.3942.301.
69. Podlesnik CA, Shahan TA. Extinction, relapse, and behav-
ioral momentum. Behavioural Processes. 2010;84(1):400–
411. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.02.001.
70. Shahan TA, Craig AR. Resurgence as choice. Behavioural Pro-
cesses. 2017;141:100–127. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2016.10.006.
71. Stanger C, Budney AJ. Contingency management: Us-
ing incentives to improve outcomes for adolescent sub-
stance use disorders. Pediatric Clinics of North America.
2019;66(6):1183–1192. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2019.08.007.
72. Higgins ST, Heil SH, Lussier JP. Clinical implications of
reinforcement as a determinant of substance use disorders.
Annual Review of Psychology. 2004;55(1):431–461. doi:
10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142033.
73. Preston KL, Umbricht A, Epstein DH. Abstinence rein-
forcement maintenance contingency and one-year follow-
up. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2002;67(2):125–137. doi:
10.1016/S0376-8716(02)00023-6.
74. Quick SL, Pyszczynski AD, Colston KA, Shahan TA. Loss
of alternative non-drug reinforcement induces relapse of
cocaine-seeking in rats: Role of dopamine D1 receptors.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36(5):1015–1020. doi:
10.1038/npp.2010.239.
75. Frye CCJ, Rung JM, Nall RW, Galizio A, Haynes JM, Odum AL.
Continuous nicotine exposure does not affect resurgence of
alcohol seeking in rats. PLoS ONE. 2018;13(8):e0202230. doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0202230.
76. Podlesnik CA, Jimenez-Gomez C, Shahan TA. Resur-
gence of alcohol seeking produced by discontinuing
non-drug reinforcement as an animal model of drug
relapse. Behavioural Pharmacology. 2006;17(4). doi:
10.1097/01.fbp.0000224385.09486.ba.
77. Zironi I, Burattini C, Aicardi G, Janak PH. Context is a
trigger for relapse to alcohol. Behavioural Brain Research.
2006;167(1):150–155. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.09.007.
78. Khoo SYS, Sciascia JM, Brown A, Chaudhri N. Compar-
ing ABA, AAB, and ABC renewal of appetitive Pavlovian con-
ditioned responding in alcohol- and sucrose-trained male
rats. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2020;14(5). doi:
10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00005.
79. Welker RL, McAuley K. Reductions in resistance to extinction
and spontaneous recovery as a function of changes in trans-
portational and contextual stimuli. Animal Learning & Be-
havior. 1978;6(4):451–457. doi: 10.3758/BF03209643.
80. Bouton ME , Bolles RC. Role of conditioned contextual stimuli
in reinstatement of extinguished fear. Journal of Experimen-
tal Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 1979;5(4):368–
378. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.5.4.368.
81. Bouton ME, King DA. Contextual control of the extinction
of conditioned fear: Tests for the associative value of the
context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Be-
havior Processes. 1983;9(3):248–265. doi: 10.1037/0097-
7403.9.3.248.
82. Rescorla RA, Wagner AR. A theory of Pavlovian condition-
ing: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcementand non-
reinforcement. In: Black AH, Prokasy WF, editors. Classi-
cal Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory. New York:
Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1972. p. 64–99. OCLC: 174453572.
83. Bouton ME. Context and ambiguity in the extinction of
emotional learning: Implications for exposure therapy. Be-
haviour Research and Therapy. 1988;26(2):137–149. doi:
10.1016/0005-7967(88)90113-1.
84. Hamlin AS, Newby J, McNally GP. The neural correlates and
role of D1 dopamine receptors in renewal of extinguished
alcohol-seeking. Neuroscience. 2007;146(2):525–536. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.01.063.
85. Kearns DN, Weiss SJ. Contextual renewal of cocaine seek-
ing in rats and its attenuation by the conditioned effects
of an alternative reinforcer. Drug and Alcohol Dependence.
2007;90(2):193–202. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.03.006.
86. Hamlin AS, Clemens KJ, McNally GP. Renewal of extin-
guished cocaine-seeking. Neuroscience. 2008;151(3):659–
670. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.11.018.
87. Crombag HS, Shaham Y. Renewal of drug seeking by con-
textual cues after prolonged extinction in rats. Behav-
ioral Neuroscience. 2002;116(1):169–173. doi: 10.1037/0735-
7044.116.1.169.
88. Bossert JM, Liu SY, Lu L, Shaham Y. A role of ven-
tral tegmental area glutamate in contextual cue-induced
relapse to heroin seeking. The Journal of Neuroscience.
2004;24(47):10726–10730. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3207-
04.2004.
89. Diergaarde L, de Vries W, Raasø H, Schoffelmeer ANM,
16 |Neuroanatomy and Behaviour, 2021
De Vries TJ. Contextual renewal of nicotine seek-
ing in rats and its suppression by the cannabinoid-
1 receptor antagonist Rimonabant (SR141716A).
Neuropharmacology. 2008;55(5):712–716. doi:
10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.06.003.
90. Fuchs RA, Evans KA, Ledford CC, Parker MP, Case JM, Mehta
RH, et al. The role of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, baso-
lateral amygdala, and dorsal hippocampus in contextual re-
instatement of cocaine seeking in rats. Neuropsychopharma-
cology. 2005;30(2):296–309. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300579.
91. Todd TP. Mechanisms of renewal after the extinction of
instrumental behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
ogy: Animal Behavior Processes. 2013;39(3):193–207. doi:
10.1037/a0032236.
92. Trask S, Shipman ML, Green JT, Bouton ME. Inactivation of
the prelimbic cortex attenuates context-dependent operant
responding. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;37(9):2317
2324. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3361-16.2017.
93. Bouton ME, Schepers ST. Renewal after the punishment
of free operant behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
ogy: Animal Learning and Cognition. 2015;41(1):81–90. doi:
10.1037/xan0000051.
94. Todd TP, Winterbauer NE, Bouton ME. Effects of the amount
of acquisition and contextual generalization on the renewal
of instrumental behavior after extinction. Learning & Behav-
ior. 2012;40(2):145–157. doi: 10.3758/s13420-011-0051-5.
95. Bouton ME, Ricker ST. Renewal of extinguished respond-
ing in a second context. Animal Learning & Behavior.
1994;22(3):317–324. doi: 10.3758/BF03209840.
96. Nakajima S, Tanaka S, Urushihara K, Imada H. Renewal of
extinguished lever-press responses upon return to the train-
ing context. Learning and Motivation. 2000;31(4):416–431.
doi: 10.1006/lmot.2000.1064.
97. Bossert JM, Gray SM, Lu L, Shaham Y. Activation of group II
metabotropic glutamate receptors in the nucleus accumbens
shell attenuates context-induced relapse to heroin seeking.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31(10):2197–2209. doi:
10.1038/sj.npp.1300977.
98. Bossert JM, Poles GC, Shefer-Collins SI, Ghitza UE. The
mGluR2/3 agonist LY379268 attenuates context- and dis-
crete cue-induced reinstatement of sucrose seeking but not
sucrose self-administration in rats. Behavioural Brain Re-
search. 2006;173(1):148–152. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.06.008.
99. Bossert JM, Poles GC, Wihbey KA, Koya E, Shaham Y. Dif-
ferential effects of blockade of dopamine D1-family recep-
tors in nucleus accumbens core or shell on reinstatement
of heroin seeking induced by contextual and discrete cues.
The Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(46):12655–12663. doi:
10.1523/jneurosci.3926-07.2007.
100. Crombag HS, Bossert JM, Koya E, Shaham Y. Context-
induced relapse to drug seeking: a review. Philosophi-
cal Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
2008;363(1507):3233–3243. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0090.
101. Marchant NJ, Kaganovsky K, Shaham Y, Bossert JM. Role
of corticostriatal circuits in context-induced reinstatement
of drug seeking. Brain Research. 2015;1628, Part A:219–232.
doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.09.004.
102. Hamlin AS, Clemens KJ, Choi EA, McNally GP. Paraventric-
ular thalamus mediates context-induced reinstatement (re-
newal) of extinguished reward seeking. European Journal
of Neuroscience. 2009;29(4):802–812. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-
9568.2009.06623.x.
103. Hamlin AS, Blatchford KE, McNally GP. Renewal of
an extinguished instrumental response: Neural correlates
and the role of D1 dopamine receptors. Neuroscience.
2006;143(1):25–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.07.035.
104. Westbrook RF, Iordanova M, McNally G, Richardson R, Har-
ris JA. Reinstatement of fear to an extinguished conditioned
stimulus: two roles for context. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 2002;28(1):97. doi:
10.1037/0097-7403.28.1.97.
105. Kim JH, Richardson R. A developmental dissociation in re-
instatement of an extinguished fear response in rats. Neu-
robiology of Learning and Memory. 2007;88(1):48–57. doi:
10.1016/j.nlm.2007.03.004.
106. Morris RW, Westbrook RF, Killcross AS. Reinstatement of ex-
tinguished fear by �-adrenergic arousal elicited by a condi-
tioned context. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2005;119(6):1662.
doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.6.1662.
107. Morris RW, Furlong TM, Westbrook RF. Recent exposure to
a dangerous context impairs extinction and reinstates lost
fear reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Ani-
mal Behavior Processes. 2005;31(1):40. doi: 10.1037/0097-
7403.31.1.40.
108. Rhodes SEV, Killcross S. Lesions of rat infralimbic cortex
enhance recovery and reinstatement of an appetitive Pavlo-
vian response. Learning & Memory. 2004;11(5):611–616. doi:
10.1101/lm.79704.
109. Dirikx T, Hermans D, Vansteenwegen D, Baeyens F, Ee-
len P. Reinstatement of conditioned responses in human
differential fear conditioning. Journal of Behavior Ther-
apy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2007;38(3):237–251. doi:
10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.04.001.
110. Hermans D, Dirikx T, Vansteenwegenin D, Baeyens F,
Van den Bergh O, Eelen P. Reinstatement of fear responses in
human aversive conditioning. Behaviour Research and Ther-
apy. 2005;43(4):533–551. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.03.013.
111. Ebrahimi C, Koch SP, Pietrock C, Fydrich T, Heinz A, Schla-
genhauf F. Opposing roles for amygdala and vmPFC in the
return of appetitive conditionedresponses in humans. Trans-
lational Psychiatry. 2019;9(1):148. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-
0482-x.
112. Torres SJ, Nowson CA. Relationship between stress, eating
behavior, and obesity. Nutrition. 2007;23(11):887–894. doi:
10.1016/j.nut.2007.08.008.
113. Halladay L, Zelikowsky M, Blair H, Fanselow M. Reinstate-
ment of extinguished fear by an unextinguished conditional
stimulus. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2012;6(18).
doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00018.
114. Krisch KA, Bandarian-Balooch S, Neumann DL, Zhong J. Elic-
iting and attenuating reinstatement of fear: Effects of an un-
extinguished CS. Learning and Motivation. 2020;71:101650.
doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2020.101650.
115. Shaham Y, Shalev U, Lu L, de Wit H, Stewart J. The reinstate-
ment model of drug relapse: history, methodology and ma-
jor ndings. Psychopharmacology. 2003;168(1-2):3–20. doi:
10.1007/s00213-002-1224-x.
116. Stretch R, Gerber GJ, Wood SM. Factors affecting behavior
maintained by response-contingent intravenous infusions
of amphetamine in squirrel monkeys. Canadian Journal
of Physiology and Pharmacology. 1971;49(6):581–589. doi:
10.1139/y71-075.
117. Gerber GJ, Stretch R. Drug-induced reinstatement of extin-
guished self-administration behavior in monkeys. Pharma-
cology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1975;3(6):1055–1061. doi:
10.1016/0091-3057(75)90016-7.
118. Stretch R, Gerber GJ. Drug-induced reinstatement of
amphetamine self-administration behaviour in monkeys.
Canadian Journal of Psychology. 1973;27(2):168–177. doi:
10.1037/h0082466.
119. Davis WM, Smith SG. Role of conditioned reinforcers in
the initiation, maintenance and extinction of drug-seeking
behavior. The Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science.
1976;11(4):222–236. PMID: 1033507.
120. de Wit H, Stewart J. Drug reinstatement of heroin-reinforced
responding in the rat. Psychopharmacology. 1983;79(1):29–
Lay & Khoo |17
31. doi: 10.1007/BF00433012.
121. Stewart J. Reinstatement of heroin and cocaine self-
administration behavior in the rat by intracerebral applica-
tion of morphine in the ventral tegmental area. Pharma-
cology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1984;20(6):917–923. doi:
10.1016/0091-3057(84)90017-0.
122. Stewart J, de Wit H, Eikelboom R. Role of unconditioned and
conditioned drug effects in the self-administration of opiates
and stimulants. Psychological Review. 1984;91(2):251–268.
doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.91.2.251.
123. de Wit H, Stewart J. Reinstatement of cocaine-reinforced re-
sponding in the rat. Psychopharmacology. 1981;75(2):134–
143. doi: 10.1007/BF00432175.
124. Calu DJ, Kawa AB, Marchant NJ, Navarre BM, Henderson
MJ, Chen B, et al. Optogenetic inhibition of dorsal medial
prefrontal cortex attenuates stress-induced reinstatement of
palatable food seeking in female rats. The Journal of Neu-
roscience. 2013;33(1):214–226. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2016-
12.2013.
125. Bossert JM, Stern AL, Theberge FRM, Cifani C, Koya E, Hope
BT, et al. Ventral medial prefrontal cortex neuronal ensem-
bles mediate context-induced relapse to heroin. Nature Neu-
roscience. 2011;14(4):420–422. doi: 10.1038/nn.2758.
126. McLaughlin RJ, Floresco SB. The role of different sub-
regions of the basolateral amygdala in cue-induced
reinstatement and extinction of food-seeking be-
havior. Neuroscience. 2007;146(4):1484–1494. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.03.025.
127. Ishikawa A, Ambroggi F, Nicola SM, Fields HL. Contribu-
tions of the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex to incen-
tive cue responding. Neuroscience. 2008;155(3):573–584. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.06.037.
128. Chen YW, Fiscella KA, Bacharach SZ, Tanda G, Shaham Y,
Calu DJ. Effect of yohimbine on reinstatement of operant
responding in rats is dependent on cue contingency but not
food reward history. Addiction Biology. 2015;20(4):690–700.
doi: 10.1111/adb.12164.
129. Skinner BF, Heron WT. Effects of caffeine and benzedrine
upon conditioning and extinction. Psychological Record.
1937;1:340–346.
130. Shaham Y, Stewart J. Effects of opioid and dopamine receptor
antagonists on relapse induced by stress and re-exposure to
heroin in rats. Psychopharmacology. 1996;125(4):385–391.
doi: 10.1007/bf02246022.
131. Mantsch JR, Baker DA, Funk D, Le AD, Shaham Y. Stress-
induced reinstatement of drug seeking: 20 years of progress.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41(1):335–356. doi:
10.1038/npp.2015.142.
132. Buffalari DM, See RE. Footshock stress potentiates
cue-induced cocaine-seeking in an animal model of re-
lapse. Physiology & Behavior. 2009;98(5):614–617. doi:
10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.09.013.
133. Reiner DJ, Fredriksson I, Lofaro OM, Bossert JM, Shaham
Y. Relapse to opioid seeking in rat models: behavior,
pharmacology and circuits. Neuropsychopharmacology.
2019;44(3):465–477. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0234-2.
134. Venniro M, Caprioli D, Shaham Y. Chapter 2 - Animal
models of drug relapse and craving: From drug priming-
induced reinstatement to incubation of craving after volun-
tary abstinence. In: Ekhtiari H, Paulus MP, editors. Neuro-
science for Addiction Medicine: From Prevention to Rehabil-
itation - Methods and Interventions. vol. 224 of Progress in
Brain Research. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2016. p. 25–52. doi:
10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.08.004.
135. Vanderschuren LJMJ, Ahmed SH. Animal models of the be-
havioral symptoms of substance use disorders. Cold Spring
Harbor Perspectives in Medicine. 2020;Advance online publi-
cation. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a040287.
136. Panlilio LV, Thorndike EB, Schindler CW. Reinstatement of
punishment-suppressed opioid self-administration in rats:
an alternative model of relapse to drug abuse. Psychophar-
macology. 2003;168(1):229–235. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-
1193-0.
137. Farrell MR, Ruiz CM, Castillo E, Faget L, Khanbijian C, Liu
S, et al. Ventral pallidum is essential for cocaine relapse af-
ter voluntary abstinence in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology.
2019;44(13):2174–2185. doi: 10.1038/s41386-019-0507-4.
138. Venniro M, Russell TI, Zhang M, Shaham Y. Operant social
reward decreases incubation of heroin craving in male and
female rats. Biological Psychiatry. 2019;86(11):848–856. doi:
10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.05.018.
139. Marchant NJ, Khuc TN, Pickens CL, Bonci A, Shaham Y.
Context-induced relapse to alcohol seeking after punish-
ment in a rat model. Biological Psychiatry. 2013;73(3):256–
262. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.07.007.
140. Reiner DJ, Lofaro OM, Applebey SV, Korah H, Venniro M,
Cifani C, et al. Role of projections between piriform cor-
tex and orbitofrontal cortex in relapse to fentanyl seeking
after palatable food choice-induced voluntary abstinence.
The Journal of Neuroscience. 2020;40(12):2485–2497. doi:
10.1523/jneurosci.2693-19.2020.
141. Marchant NJ, Campbell EJ, Pelloux Y, Bossert JM, Shaham
Y. Context-induced relapse after extinction versus punish-
ment: similarities and differences. Psychopharmacology.
2019;236(1):439–448. doi: 10.1007/s00213-018-4929-1.
142. Freese L, Durand A, Guillem K, Ahmed SH. Pre-trial co-
caine biases choice toward cocaine through suppression of
the nondrug option. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behav-
ior. 2018;173:65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2018.07.010.
143. Hogarth L, Balleine BW, Corbit LH, Killcross S. Associa-
tive learning mechanisms underpinning the transition from
recreational drug use to addiction. Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences. 2013;1282(1):12–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-
6632.2012.06768.x.
144. Cunningham CL. Alcohol as a cue for extinction: State depen-
dency produced by conditioned inhibition. Animal Learning
& Behavior. 1979;7(1):45–52. doi: 10.3758/BF03209656.
145. Chen YW, Fiscella KA, Bacharach SZ, Calu DJ. Effect of cafete-
ria diet history on cue-, pellet-priming-, and stress-induced
reinstatement of food seeking in female rats. PLoS ONE.
2014;9(7):e102213. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0102213.
146. Shaham Y, Stewart J. Stress reinstates heroin-seeking in
drug-free animals: An effect mimicking heroin, not with-
drawal. Psychopharmacology. 1995;119(3):334–341. doi:
10.1007/BF02246300.
147. Shaham Y, Rajabi H, Stewart J. Relapse to heroin-seeking
in rats under opioid maintenance: the effects of stress,
heroin priming, and withdrawal. The Journal of Neuro-
science. 1996;16(5):1957–1963. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.16-05-
01957.1996.
148. Baarendse PJJ, Limpens JHW, Vanderschuren LJMJ. Dis-
rupted social development enhances the motivation for co-
caine in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2014;231(8):1695–1704.
doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3362-8.
149. Schepers ST, Bouton ME. Hunger as a context: Food seeking
that is inhibited during hunger can renew in the context of
satiety. Psychological Science. 2017;28(11):1640–1648. doi:
10.1177/0956797617719084.
150. Schepers ST, Bouton ME. Stress as a context: Stress causes
relapse of inhibited food seeking if it has been associated
with prior food seeking. Appetite. 2019;132:131–138. doi:
10.1016/j.appet.2018.10.016.
151. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug addiction, dysregulation ofreward,
and allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;24(2):97–
129. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00195-0.
152. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Addiction and the brain antireward sys-
18 |Neuroanatomy and Behaviour, 2021
tem. Annual Review of Psychology. 2008;59(1):29–53. doi:
10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093548.
153. Solomon RL, Corbit JD. An opponent-process theory of moti-
vation: I. Temporal dynamics of affect. Psychological Review.
1974;81(2):119–145. doi: 10.1037/h0036128.
154. Trask S, Thrailkill EA, Bouton ME. Occasion setting, inhi-
bition, and the contextual control of extinction in Pavlovian
and instrumental (operant) learning. Behavioural Processes.
2017;137:64–72. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2016.10.003.
155. Floresco SB, McLaughlin RJ, Haluk DM. Oppos-
ing roles for the nucleus accumbens core and shell
in cue-induced reinstatement of food-seeking be-
havior. Neuroscience. 2008;154(3):877–884. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.04.004.
156. Yager LM, Robinson TE. Cue-induced reinstatement of food
seeking in rats that differ in their propensity to attribute in-
centive salience to food cues. Behavioural Brain Research.
2010;214(1):30–34. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.04.021.
157. Brown RM, Kim AK, Khoo SYS, Kim JH, Jupp B, Lawrence
AJ. Orexin-1 receptor signalling in the prelimbic cortex
and ventral tegmental area regulates cue-induced reinstate-
ment of ethanol-seeking in iP rats. Addiction Biology.
2016;21(3):603–612. doi: 10.1111/adb.12251.
158. Moorman DE, James MH, Kilroy EA, Aston-Jones G.
Orexin/hypocretin neuron activation is correlated with
alcohol seeking and preference in a topographically specic
manner. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;43(5):710–
720. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13170.
159. Smith RJ, See RE, Aston-Jones G. Orexin/hypocretin signal-
ing at the orexin 1 receptor regulates cue-elicited cocaine-
seeking. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;30(3):493–
503. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06844.x.
160. Namba MD, Tomek SE, Olive MF, Beckmann JS, Gipson CD.
The winding road to relapse: Forging a new understanding
of cue-induced reinstatement models and their associated
neural mechanisms. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience.
2018;12(17). doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00017.
161. Perry CJ, Zbukvic I, Kim JH, Lawrence AJ. Role of cues and
contexts on drug-seeking behaviour. British Journal of Phar-
macology. 2014;171(20):4636–4672. doi: 10.1111/bph.12735.
162. See RE. Neural substrates of cocaine-cue associations
that trigger relapse. European Journal of Pharmacology.
2005;526(1):140–146. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2005.09.034.
163. Kruzich PJ, See RE. Differential contributions of the ba-
solateral and central amygdala in the acquisition and ex-
pression of conditioned relapse to cocaine-seeking behavior.
The Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21(14):RC155–RC155. doi:
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-14-j0002.2001.
164. Buffalari DM, Feltenstein MW, See RE. The effects of var-
ied extinction procedures on contingent cue-induced rein-
statement in Sprague-Dawley rats. Psychopharmacology.
2013;230(2):319–327. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3156-z.
165. Torregrossa MM, Sanchez H, Taylor JR. D-Cycloserine
reduces the context specicity of pavlovian extinction of
cocaine cues through actions in the nucleus accumbens.
The Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30(31):10526–10533. doi:
10.1523/jneurosci.2523-10.2010.
166. Perry CJ, Reed F, Zbukvic IC, Kim JH, Lawrence AJ. The
metabotropic glutamate 5 receptor is necessary for extinc-
tion of cocaine-associated cues. British Journal of Pharma-
cology. 2016;173(6):1085–1094. doi: 10.1111/bph.13437.
167. Zbukvic IC, Ganella DE, Perry CJ, Madsen HB, Bye CR,
Lawrence AJ, et al. Role of dopamine 2 receptor in im-
paired drug-cue extinction in adolescent rats. Cerebral Cor-
tex. 2016;26(6):2895–2904. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw051.
168. Madsen HB, Zbukvic IC, Luikinga SJ, Lawrence AJ, Kim
JH. Extinction of conditioned cues attenuates incubation
of cocaine craving in adolescent and adult rats. Neuro-
biology of Learning and Memory. 2017;143:88–93. doi:
10.1016/j.nlm.2016.09.002.
169. Prasad AA, McNally GP. Ventral pallidum output path-
ways in context-induced reinstatement of alcohol seeking.
The Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36(46):11716–11726. doi:
10.1523/jneurosci.2580-16.2016.
170. Williams BA. Conditioned reinforcement: Experimental and
theoretical issues. The Behavior Analyst. 1994;17(2):261–285.
doi: 10.1007/BF03392675.
171. Williams BA. Conditioned reinforcement: Neglected or out-
moded explanatory construct? Psychonomic Bulletin & Re-
view. 1994;1(4):457–475. doi: 10.3758/BF03210950.
172. Shahan TA. Conditioned reinforcement and response
strength. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior.
2010;93(2):269–289. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2010.93-269.
173. Burke K, Franz T, Miller D, Schoenbaum G. Conditioned re-
inforcement can be mediated by either outcome-specic or
general affective representations. Frontiers in Integrative
Neuroscience. 2007;1(2). doi: 10.3389/neuro.07.002.2007.
174. Samaha AN, Minogianis EA, Nachar W. Cues paired with ei-
ther rapid or slower self-administered cocaine injections ac-
quire similar conditioned rewarding properties. PLoS ONE.
2011;6(10). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026481.
175. Kawa AB, Valenta AC, Kennedy RT, Robinson TE. Incentive
and dopamine sensitization produced by intermittentbut not
long access cocaine self-administration. European Journal of
Neuroscience. 2019;50(4):2663–2682. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14418.
176. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The incentive sensitization
theory of addiction: some current issues. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
2008;363(1507):3137–3146. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0093.
177. Arroyo M, Markou A, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Acquisition,
maintenance and reinstatement of intravenous cocaine self-
administration under a second-order schedule of reinforce-
ment in rats: effects of conditioned cues and continuous ac-
cess to cocaine. Psychopharmacology. 1998;140(3):331–344.
doi: 10.1007/s002130050774.
178. Smith SG, Werner TE, Davis WM. Alcohol-associated
conditioned reinforcement. Psychopharmacology.
1977;53(3):223–226. doi: 10.1007/BF00492355.
179. Di Ciano P. Facilitated acquisition but not persistence of re-
sponding for a cocaine-paired conditioned reinforcer follow-
ing sensitization with cocaine. Neuropsychopharmacology.
2008;33(6):1426–1431. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301542.
180. Di Ciano P, Everitt BJ. Conditioned reinforcing proper-
ties of stimuli paired with self-administered cocaine, heroin
or sucrose: implications for the persistence of addictive
behaviour. Neuropharmacology. 2004;47:202–213. doi:
10.1016/j.neuropharm.2004.06.005.
181. Clemens KJ, Lay BPP, Holmes NM. Extended nicotine self-
administration increases sensitivity to nicotine, motivation
to seek nicotine and the reinforcing properties of nicotine-
paired cues. Addiction Biology. 2017;22(2):400–410. doi:
10.1111/adb.12336.
182. Shahan TA. Moving beyond reinforcement and response
strength. The Behavior Analyst. 2017;40(1):107–121. doi:
10.1007/s40614-017-0092-y.
183. Thrailkill EA, Shahan TA. Temporal integration and instru-
mental conditioned reinforcement. Learning & Behavior.
2014;42(3):201–208. doi: 10.3758/s13420-014-0138-x.
184. Ward RD, Gallistel CR, Jensen G, Richards VL, Fairhurst S,
Balsam PD. Conditioned stimulus informativeness governs
conditioned stimulus−unconditioned stimulus associability.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Pro-
cesses. 2012;38(3):217–232. doi: 10.1037/a0027621.
185. Parkinson JA, Roberts AC, Everitt BJ, Di Ciano P. Acqui-
sition of instrumental conditioned reinforcement is resis-
tant to the devaluation of the unconditioned stimulus. The
Lay & Khoo |19
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B.
2005;58(1b):19–30. doi: 10.1080/02724990444000023.
186. Plaza-Zabala A, Flores A, Martin-Garcia E, Saravia
R, Maldonado R, Berrendero F. A role for hypocre-
tin/orexin receptor-1 in cue-induced reinstatement of
nicotine-seeking behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology.
2013;38(9):1724–1736. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.72.
187. Kumaresan V, Yuan M, Yee J, Famous KR, Anderson
SM, Schmidt HD, et al. Metabotropic glutamate recep-
tor 5 (mGluR5) antagonists attenuate cocaine priming-
and cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking. Be-
havioural Brain Research. 2009;202(2):238–244. doi:
10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.039.
188. Guercio LA, Schmidt HD, Pierce RC. Deep brain stim-
ulation of the nucleus accumbens shell attenuates cue-
induced reinstatement of both cocaine and sucrose seeking
in rats. Behavioural Brain Research. 2015;281:125–130. doi:
10.1016/j.bbr.2014.12.025.
189. Brown RM, Khoo SYS, Lawrence AJ. Central orexin
(hypocretin) 2 receptor antagonism reduces ethanol self-
administration, but not cue-conditioned ethanol-seeking,
in ethanol-preferring rats. The International Journal of
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;16(9):2067–2079. doi:
10.1017/S1461145713000333.
190. Tunstall BJ, Kearns DN. Cocaine can generate a stronger
conditioned reinforcer than food despite being a weaker pri-
mary reinforcer. Addiction Biology. 2016;21(2):282–293. doi:
10.1111/adb.12195.
191. Caballero JP, Scarpa GB, Remage-Healey L, Moorman
DE. Differential effects of dorsal and ventral medial pre-
frontal cortex inactivation during natural reward seek-
ing, extinction, and cue-induced reinstatement. eNeuro.
2019;6(5):ENEURO.0296–19.2019. doi: 10.1523/eneuro.0296-
19.2019.
192. Deroche-Gamonet V, Piat F, Le Moal M, Piazza PV. Inu-
ence of cue-conditioning on acquisition, maintenance and
relapse of cocaine intravenous self-administration. Euro-
pean Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;15(8):1363–1370. doi:
10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.01974.x.
193. Caggiula AR, Donny EC, White AR, Chaudhri N, Booth
S, Gharib MA, et al. Cue dependency of nicotine self-
administration and smoking. Pharmacology Biochemistry
and Behavior. 2001;70(4):515–530. doi: 10.1016/S0091-
3057(01)00676-1.
194. Caggiula AR, Donny EC, White AR, Chaudhri N, Booth S,
Gharib MA, et al. Environmental stimuli promote the acqui-
sition of nicotine self-administration in rats. Psychophar-
macology. 2002;163(2):230–237. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-
1156-5.
195. Cohen C, Perrault G, Griebel G, Soubrié P. Nicotine-
associated cues maintain nicotine-seeking behavior in
rats several weeks after nicotine withdrawal: Reversal
by the cannabinoid (CB1) receptor antagonist, rimonabant
(SR141716). Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30(1):145–155.
doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300541.
196. Liu X, Caggiula AR, Palmatier MI, Donny EC, Sved AF. Cue-
induced reinstatement of nicotine-seeking behavior in rats:
effect of bupropion, persistence over repeated tests, and
its dependence on training dose. Psychopharmacology.
2008;196(3):365–375. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0967-9.
197. Cartoni E, Balleine B, Baldassarre G. Appetitive
Pavlovian-instrumental Transfer: A review. Neuro-
science & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2016;71:829–848. doi:
10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.020.
198. Holmes NM, Marchand AR, Coutureau E. Pavlovian to in-
strumental transfer: A neurobehavioural perspective. Neuro-
science & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2010;34(8):1277–1295. doi:
10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.03.007.
199. Trick L, Hogarth L, Duka T. Prediction and uncertainty in
human Pavlovian to instrumental transfer. Journal of Ex-
perimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition.
2011;37(3):757–765. doi: 10.1037/a0022310.
200. Derman RC, Ferrario CR. Enhanced incentive motiva-
tion in obesity-prone rats is mediated by NAc core CP-
AMPARs. Neuropharmacology. 2018;131:326–336. doi:
10.1016/j.neuropharm.2017.12.039.
201. Alarcón DE, Delamater AR. Outcome-specic Pavlovian-
to-instrumental transfer (PIT) with alcohol cues
and its extinction. Alcohol. 2019;76:131–146. doi:
10.1016/j.alcohol.2018.09.003.
202. Krank MD. Pavlovian conditioning with ethanol: sign-
tracking (autoshaping), conditioned incentive, and
ethanol self-administration. Alcoholism: Clinical
and Experimental Research. 2003;27(10):1592–8. doi:
10.1097/01.Alc.0000092060.09228.De.
203. Lamb RJ, Schindler CW, Pinkston JW. Conditioned
stimuli’s role in relapse: preclinical research on
Pavlovian-Instrumental-Transfer. Psychopharmacology.
2016;233(10):1933–1944. doi: 10.1007/s00213-016-4216-y.
204. Manglani HR, Lewis AH, Wilson SJ, Delgado MR. Pavlovian-
to-instrumental transfer of nicotine and food cues in de-
prived cigarette smokers. Nicotine & Tobacco Research.
2017;19(6):670–676. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntx007.
205. Pielock SM, Lex B, Hauber W. The role of dopamine in
the dorsomedial striatum in general and outcome-selective
Pavlovian-instrumental transfer. European Journal of
Neuroscience. 2011;33(4):717–725. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-
9568.2010.07561.x.
206. Hall J, Parkinson JA, Connor TM, Dickinson A, Everitt
BJ. Involvement of the central nucleus of the amygdala
and nucleus accumbens core in mediating Pavlovian inu-
ences on instrumental behaviour. European Journal of
Neuroscience. 2001;13(10):1984–1992. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-
816x.2001.01577.x.
207. Rescorla RA, Solomon RL. Two-process learning theory: Re-
lationships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumen-
tal learning. Psychological Review. 1967;74(3):151–182. doi:
10.1037/h0024475.
208. Estes WK. Discriminative conditioning. II. Effects of a
Pavlovian conditioned stimulus upon a subsequently estab-
lished operant response. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
1948;38(2):173–177. doi: 10.1037/h0057525.
209. Holland PC. Relations between Pavlovian-instrumental
transfer and reinforcer devaluation. Journal of Experimen-
tal Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 2004;30(2):104–
117. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.30.2.104.
210. Augur IF, Wyckoff AR, Aston-Jones G, Kalivas PW, Peters
J. Chemogenetic activation of an extinction neural cir-
cuit reduces cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking.
The Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36(39):10174–10180. doi:
10.1523/jneurosci.0773-16.2016.
211. Mahler SV, Brodnik ZD, Cox BM, Buchta WC, Bentzley BS,
Quintanilla J, et al. Chemogenetic manipulations of ven-
tral tegmental area dopamine neurons reveal multifaceted
roles in cocaine abuse. The Journal of Neuroscience.
2019;39(3):503–518. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0537-18.2018.
212. Smith RJ, Aston-Jones G. Orexin / hypocretin 1 recep-
tor antagonist reduces heroin self-administration and cue-
induced heroin seeking. European Journal of Neuroscience.
2012;35(5):798–804. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08013.x.
213. Khoo SYS, McNally GP, Clemens KJ. The dual orexin receptor
antagonist TCS1102 does not affect reinstatement ofnicotine-
seeking. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(3):e0173967. doi: 10.1371/jour-
nal.pone.0173967.
214. Castino MR, Cornish JL, Clemens KJ. Inhibition of histone
deacetylases facilitates extinction and attenuates reinstate-
20 |Neuroanatomy and Behaviour, 2021
ment of nicotine self-administration in rats. PLoS ONE.
2015;10(4):e0124796. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124796.
215. Rubio FJ, Quintana-Feliciano R, Warren BL, Li X, Witonsky
KFR, Valle FSd, et al. Prelimbic cortex is a common brain area
activated during cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine and
heroin seeking in a polydrug self-administration rat model.
European Journal of Neuroscience. 2019;49(2):165–178. doi:
10.1111/ejn.14203.
216. Leri F, Stewart J. Drug-induced reinstatement to heroin
and cocaine seeking: A rodent model of relapse in poly-
drug use. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology.
2001;9(3):297–306. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.9.3.297.
217. Clemens KJ, Castino MR, Cornish JL, Goodchild AK, Holmes
NM. Behavioral and neural substrates of habit formation in
rats intravenously self-administering nicotine. Neuropsy-
chopharmacology. 2014;doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.111.
218. Takahashi TT, Vengeliene V, Enkel T, Reithofer S, Spanagel R.
Pavlovian to instrumental transfer responses do not correlate
with addiction-like behavior in rats. Frontiers in Behavioral
Neuroscience. 2019;13(129). doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00129.
219. Hogarth L. Addiction is driven by excessive goal-directed
drug choice under negative affect: translational critique of
habit and compulsion theory. Neuropsychopharmacology.
2020;45(5):720–735. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-0600-8.
220. Belin D, Belin-Rauscent A, Murray JE, Everitt BJ. Addic-
tion: failure of control over maladaptive incentive habits.
Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2013;23(4):564–572. doi:
10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.025.
221. Kalivas PW. Addiction as a pathology in prefrontal cortical
regulation of corticostriatal habit circuitry. Neurotoxicity Re-
search. 2008;14(2-3):185–189. doi: 10.1007/BF03033809.
222. Bedi G, Preston KL, Epstein DH, Heishman SJ, Marrone GF,
Shaham Y, et al. Incubation of cue-induced cigarette craving
during abstinence in human smokers. Biological Psychiatry.
2011;69(7):708–711. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.014.
223. Cannella N, Oliveira AMM, Hemstedt T, Lissek T, Buech-
ler E, Bading H, et al. Dnmt3a2 in the nucleus accum-
bens shell is required for reinstatement of cocaine seeking.
The Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38(34):7516–7528. doi:
10.1523/jneurosci.0600-18.2018.
224. Hull CL. Principles of behavior: An introduction to behav-
ior theory. Century Psychology Series. New York: Appleton-
Century-Crofts Inc.; 1943. OCLC: 123755287.
225. Kamin LJ. The retention of an incompletely learned avoid-
ance response. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psy-
chology. 1957;50(5):457–460. doi: 10.1037/h0044226.
226. Kamin LJ. Retention of an incompletely learned avoid-
ance response: Some further analyses. Journal of Compar-
ative and Physiological Psychology. 1963;56(4):713–718. doi:
10.1037/h0043941.
227. Tarpy RM. Incubation of anxiety as measured by response
suppression. Psychonomic Science. 1966;4(1):189–190. doi:
10.3758/BF03342243.
228. Pinel JP, Mucha RF. Incubation and Kamin effects in the rat:
Changes in activity and reactivity after footshock. Journal of
Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1973;84(3):661–
668. doi: 10.1037/h0034890.
229. Anderson DC, Johnson L, Schwendiman G, Dunford G.
Retention of an incompletely learned avoidance response:
Some problems with replication. Psychonomic Science.
1966;6(1):23–24. doi: 10.3758/BF03327938.
230. Markou A, Li J, Tse K, Li X. Cue-induced nicotine-seeking
behavior after withdrawal with or without extinction in rats.
Addiction Biology. 2018;23(1):111–119. doi: 10.1111/adb.12480.
231. Sakurai T, Amemiya A, Ishii M, Matsuzaki I, Chemelli RM,
Tanaka H, et al. Orexins and orexin receptors: A family of hy-
pothalamic neuropeptides and G protein-coupled receptors
that regulate feeding behavior. Cell. 1998;92(4):573–585. doi:
10.1016/s0092-8674(00)80949-6.
232. de Lecea L, Kilduff TS, Peyron C, Gao XB, Foye PE, Daniel-
son PE, et al. The hypocretins: Hypothalamus-specic
peptides with neuroexcitatory activity. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences. 1998;95(1):322–327. doi:
10.1073/pnas.95.1.322.
233. Gotter AL, Webber AL, Coleman PJ, Renger JJ, Winrow CJ.
International Union of Basic and Clinical Pharmacology.
LXXXVI. Orexin receptor function, nomenclature and phar-
macology. Pharmacological Reviews. 2012;64(3):389–420.
doi: 10.1124/pr.111.005546.
234. Hauger RL, Grigoriadis DE, Dallman MF, Plotsky PM, Vale
WW, Dautzenberg FM. International Union of Pharmacology.
XXXVI. Current status of the nomenclature for receptors for
corticotropin-releasing factor and their ligands. Pharmaco-
logical Reviews. 2003;55(1):21–26. doi: 10.1124/pr.55.1.3.
235. Roger G. Hypothalamic hormones a.k.a. hypothalamic releas-
ing factors. Journal of Endocrinology. 2005;184(1):11–28. doi:
10.1677/joe.1.05883.
Copyright and License
Copyright © 2021. Belinda Po Pyn Lay, Shaun Yon-Seng Khoo. Ex-
cept where otherwise noted, the content of this article is licensed
under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
You are free to reuse or adapt this article for any purpose, provided
appropriate acknowledgement is provided. For additional permis-
sions, please contact the corresponding author.
... McLemon and Chesworth discussed the potential for targeting the cannabinoid system in opioid addiction, considering both the preclinical effects on withdrawal and self-administration against some of the undesirable or off-target effects of cannabinoids [2]. Lay and Khoo reviewed the associative processes in addiction relapse models, arguing that cue-induced reinstatement is driven by a combination of conditioned reinforcement and Pavlovian-to-Instrumental transfer [3]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Neuroanatomy and Behaviour was founded to be a journal for rigorous and open science. In 2021, all of the empirical papers published engaged in at least one open science practice, such as open data or open protocols. The papers published have been carefully reviewed by two experts, but may also be sent to additional specialist reviewers for specific tasks, such as checking references or statistical approaches. In 2021, Neuroanatomy and Behaviour reached a key milestone and was accepted into the Directory of Open Access Journals, the world’s leading database of trustworthy open access journals. As we look towards 2022, we will continue improving our publication processes and working to share quality neuroscience without financial barriers for authors or readers.
Article
Full-text available
Adolescence is pivotal for neural and behavioral development across species. During this period, maturation occurs in several biological systems, the most well-recognized being activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal axis marking pubertal onset. Increasing comparative studies of sex differences have enriched our understanding of systems integration during neurodevelopment. In recent years, immune signaling has emerged as a key node of interaction between a variety of biological signaling processes. Herein, we review the age- and sex-specific changes that occur in neural, hypothalamic-pituitary, and microbiome systems during adolescence. We then describe how immune signaling interacts with these systems, and review recent preclinical evidence indicating that immune signaling may play a central role in integrating changes in their typical and atypical development during adolescence. Finally, we discuss the translational relevance of these preclinical studies to human health and wellness.
Article
Full-text available
Contextual stimuli associated with drug exposure can modulate various effects of drugs, but little is known about their role in relapse to drug seeking. Using a renewal procedure, the authors report that drug-associated contextual stimuli play a critical role in relapse to drug-seeking previously maintained by a heroin–cocaine mixture (speedball). Rats were trained to self-administer speedball, after which drug-reinforced behavior was extinguished over 20 days in the self-administration context or in a different context. On the test day, rats exposed to the drug-associated context, after extinction in a different context, reliably renewed drug seeking. The authors suggest that the renewal procedure can be used to study mechanisms underlying relapse to drug seeking elicited by drug-associated contextual stimuli.
Article
Full-text available
Results of 3 experiments with 80 female Wistar rats show that shocks presented to Ss following the extinction of conditioned suppression reliably reinstated suppression to the CS, but only when they were presented in the context in which testing was later to occur. Reinstatement was also reversed by extinguishing fear to the context through nonreinforced exposure to the context between shock presentation and testing. Reinstatement was obtained in spite of procedures that have been used in the past to minimize the influence of context conditioning. Moreover, fear of the context was never detected directly by depressed barpress rates in the absence of the CS. Results do not support the hypothesis that reinstatement results from an increment in the strength of a memory of the UCS that has been weakened during extinction. (29 ref)
Article
Full-text available
This article reviews the behavioral neuroscience of extinction, the phenomenon in which a behavior that has been acquired through Pavlovian or instrumental (operant) learning decreases in strength when the outcome that reinforced it is removed. Behavioral research indicates that neither Pavlovian nor operant extinction depends substantially on erasure of the original learning, but instead depends on new inhibitory learning that is primarily expressed in the context in which it is learned, as exemplified by the renewal effect. Although the nature of the inhibition may differ in Pavlovian and operant extinction, in either case the decline in responding may depend on both generalization decrement and the correction of prediction error. At the neural level, Pavlovian extinction requires a tripartite neural circuit involving the amygdala, prefrontal cortex, and hippocampus. Synaptic plasticity in the amygdala is essential for extinction learning, and prefrontal cortical inhibition of amygdala neurons encoding fear memories is involved in fear retrieval. Hippocampal-prefrontal circuits mediate fear relapse phenomena, including renewal. Instrumental extinction involves distinct ensembles in corticostriatal, striatopallidal, and striatohypothalamic circuits as well as their thalamic returns for inhibitory (extinction) and excitatory (renewal and other relapse phenomena) control over operant responding. The field has made significant progress in recent decades, although a fully integrated biobehavioral understanding still awaits.
Article
Full-text available
Conditioned responding can be renewed by re-exposure to the conditioning context following extinction in a different context (ABA renewal) or by removal from the extinction context (AAB or ABC renewal). ABA renewal is robust in Pavlovian and operant conditioning paradigms. However, fewer studies have investigated AAB and ABC renewal of appetitive conditioning, and those that did predominantly used operant conditioning tasks. Renewal has theoretical relevance for extinction and for exposure-based treatments for substance use disorders that aim to extinguish reactivity to drug-predictive cues. We therefore investigated ABA, AAB, and ABC renewal of Pavlovian conditioned responding to cues that predicted either alcohol or sucrose. Male, Long-Evans rats (Charles River) were exposed to either 15% ethanol (Study 1: “alcohol”) or 10% sucrose (Study 2: “sucrose”) in their home cages. Next, they were trained to discriminate between two auditory stimuli (white noise and clicker; 10 s) in conditioning chambers equipped with distinct olfactory, visual, and tactile contextual stimuli (context A). One conditioned stimulus (CS+) was paired with fluid delivery (0.2 ml/CS+; 3.2 ml/session; alcohol or sucrose in separate experiments), and the second CS (CS−) was not. In all sessions (conditioning, extinction, and test), each CS was presented 16 times/session on a variable-time 67-s schedule, and entries into the fluid port were recorded. CS+ port entries were then extinguished by withholding fluid delivery either in context A or in a second, different context (context B). Next, we assessed ABA, AAB, and ABC renewal in the absence of fluid delivery. During extinction, CS+ port entries were initially elevated in context A relative to context B. ABA renewal of CS+ port entries occurred in both alcohol- and sucrose-trained rats. ABC renewal approached statistical significance when data from both experiments were combined. No AAB renewal was observed, and, in fact, alcohol-trained rats showed AAB suppression. These results corroborate the reliability of ABA renewal and suggest that ABC renewal is a modest effect that may require greater statistical power to detect. From a treatment perspective, the lack of AAB renewal suggests that exposure-based treatments for substance use disorders might benefit from implementation in real-world, drug-use contexts.
Article
Full-text available
Werecently developed a rat model of relapse to drug seeking after food choice-induced voluntary abstinence. Here, we used this model to study the role of the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and its afferent projections in relapse to fentanyl seeking. Wetrained male and female rats to self-administer palatable food pellets for 6 d (6h/d) and intravenous fentanyl (2.5μg/kg/infusion) for 12 d (6 h/d). Weassessed relapse to fentanyl seeking after 13-14 voluntary abstinence days, achieved through a discrete choice procedure between fentanyl infusions and palatable food (20 trials/d). In both sexes, relapse after food choice-induced abstinence was associated with increased expression of the activity marker Fos in the OFC. Pharmacological inactivation of the OFC with muscimol plus baclofen (50+50 ng/side) decreased relapse to fentanyl seeking. We then determined projection-specific activation of OFC afferents during the relapse test by using Fos plus the retrograde tracer cholera toxin B (injected into the OFC). Relapse to fentanyl seeking was associated with increased Fos expression in the piriform cortex (Pir) neurons projecting to the OFC, but not in projections from the basolateral amygdala and thalamus. Pharmacological inactivation of the Pir with muscimol plus baclofen decreased relapse to fentanyl seeking after voluntary abstinence. Next, we used an anatomical disconnection procedure to determine whether projections between the Pir and OFC are critical for relapse to fentanyl seeking. Unilateral muscimol plus baclofen injections into the Pir in one hemisphere plus unilateral muscimol plus baclofen injections into the OFC in the contralateral, but not ipsilateral, hemisphere decreased relapse. Our results identify Pir-OFC projections as a new motivation-related pathway critical to relapse to opioid seeking after voluntary abstinence.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Substance Use Disorder (SUD) and behavioral addictions are common and require a multidisciplinary approach. New technologies like Virtual Reality could have the potential to improve assessment and treatment of these disorders. Objective: In the present paper, we therefore present an overview of Virtual Reality (Head Mounted Devices) in the field of addiction medicine for craving assessment and treatment. Method: We conducted a systematic review by querying PubMed database for the titles of articles published up to March 2019 with the terms [virtual] AND [addictive] OR [addiction] OR [substance] OR [alcohol] OR [cocaine] OR [cannabis] OR [opioid] OR [tobacco] OR [nicotine] OR [methamphetamine] OR [gaming] OR [gambling]. Results: We screened 319 abstracts and analyzed 37 articles, dividing them into two categories, the first for assessment of cue reactivity (craving, psychophysiological response and attention to cue) and the second for intervention, each drug (nicotine, cocaine, alcohol, cannabis, gambling) being detailed within each category. Conclusions: This overview suggest that VR provide benefits in the assessment and treatment of substance use disorders and behavior addictions and achieve high levels of ecological validity. While, craving provocation in VR is effective across addiction disorders, treatments based exclusively on virtual exposure to drug related cues as shown heterogenous results.
Article
Full-text available
Drug addiction may be a goal-directed choice driven by excessive drug value in negative affective states, a habit driven by strong stimulus−response associations, or a compulsion driven by insensitivity to costs imposed on drug seeking. Laboratory animal and human evidence for these three theories is evaluated. Excessive goal theory is supported by dependence severity being associated with greater drug choice/economic demand. Drug choice is demonstrably goal-directed (driven by the expected value of the drug) and can be augmented by stress/negative mood induction and withdrawal—effects amplified in those with psychiatric symptoms and drug use coping motives. Furthermore, psychiatric symptoms confer risk of dependence, and coping motives mediate this risk. Habit theory of addiction has weaker support. Habitual behaviour seen in drug-exposed animals often does not occur in complex decision scenarios, or where responding is rewarded, so habit is unlikely to explain most human addictive behaviour where these conditions apply. Furthermore, most human studies have not found greater propensity to habitual behaviour in drug users or as a function of dependence severity, and the minority that have can be explained by task disengagement producing impaired explicit contingency knowledge. Compulsion theory of addiction also has weak support. The persistence of punished drug seeking in animals is better explained by greater drug value (evinced by the association with economic demand) than by insensitivity to costs. Furthermore, human studies have provided weak evidence that propensity discount cost imposed on drug seeking is associated with dependence severity. These data suggest that human addiction is primarily driven by excessive goal-directed drug choice under negative affect, and less by habit or compulsion. Addiction is pathological because negative states powerfully increase expected drug value acutely outweighing abstinence goals.
Article
The mesolimbic dopamine system comprises distinct compartments supporting different functions in learning and motivation. Less well understood is how complex addiction-related behaviors emerge from activity patterns across these compartments. Here we show how different forms of relapse to alcohol-seeking in male rats are assembled from activity across the VTA and the nucleus accumbens. First, we used chemogenetic approaches to show a causal role for VTA TH neurons in two forms of relapse to alcohol-seeking: renewal (context-induced reinstatement) and reacquisition. Then, using gCaMP fiber photometry of VTA TH neurons, we identified medial and lateral VTA TH neuron activity profiles during self-administration, renewal, and reacquisition. Next, we used optogenetic inhibition of VTA TH neurons to show distinct causal roles for VTA subregions in distinct forms of relapse. We then used dLight fiber photometry to measure dopamine binding across the ventral striatum (medial accumbens shell, accumbens core, lateral accumbens shell) and showed complex and heterogeneous profiles of dopamine binding during self-administration and relapse. Finally, we used representational similarity analysis to identify mesolimbic dopamine signatures of self-administration, extinction, and relapse. Our results show that signatures of relapse can be identified from heterogeneous activity profiles across the mesolimbic dopamine system and that these signatures are unique for different forms of relapse.
Article
Reinstatement of fear is a proposed mechanism for return of fear following exposure therapy. A standard reinstatement procedure in the laboratory involves a conditional stimulus (CS) paired with an unconditional stimulus (US) during acquisition (i.e., CS+), and a CS paired without an US (CS-) during acquisition. In extinction the CS + and CS- are presented alone, then the US is presented without the CS in reinstatement, and followed by test trials of the CS. The current study examined whether reinstatement of fear can be triggered by a CS that was previously paired with the US (i.e., unextinguished CS) and reduced by extinction to the CSextinguished and CSunextinguished but not the CS- in a sample of first year psychology students (expectancy data N = 93; heart rate data N = 73). A differential aversive conditioning procedure presented within a virtual reality environment was used to examine reinstatement of the US expectancy and conditioned heart rate responses. As predicted, presentation of an unextinguished CS reinstated fear of a second previously extinguished CS. Moreover, conducting extinction with multiple stimuli attenuated this reinstatement of fear as indexed by self-report and heart rate measures. The present results suggest that therapy should include exposure to multiple stimuli to reduce the likelihood of reinstatement of fear.
Article
Multiple interventions for treating adolescents with substance use disorders have demonstrated efficacy, but most teens do not show an enduring positive response to these treatments. Contingency management (CM)-based strategies provide a promising alternative, and clinical research focused on the development and testing of innovative CM models continues to grow. This article provides information on the principles that underlie CM interventions, key metrics that define their development and implementation, a brief review of studies that have tested these approaches, and some clinical CM tools. As with other interventions to help youth with substance use problems, there is much to learn about CM approaches.