- Access to this full-text is provided by Springer Nature.
- Learn more
Download available
Content available from Reading and Writing
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Vol.:(0123456789)
Reading and Writing (2021) 34:1825–1850
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-021-10135-8
1 3
A sentence‑combining intervention forstruggling writers:
response tointervention
KirstyWalter1,2 · JulieDockrell1· VinceConnelly2
Accepted: 1 February 2021 / Published online: 20 February 2021
© The Author(s) 2021
Abstract
Children who struggle with writing are a heterogeneous group and may experience
difficulties in a range of domains, including spelling, reading, and oral language.
These difficulties are reflected in their writing and may influence their responsive-
ness to writing interventions. The effectiveness of a targeted sentence-combin-
ing intervention to improve the writing skills of 71 struggling writers, aged 7 to
10years, was compared with a spelling intervention and a business as usual (waiting
list) control condition. Some struggling writers also performed poorly on measures
of reading and oral language. Children’s performance on a range of writing meas-
ures were assessed at baseline (t1), immediate post-test (t2) and delayed post-test
(t3). Children receiving the sentence-combining intervention showed significant
improvements in the sentence combining measure at t2 and t3 compared to both
the spelling intervention and waiting list controls. Exploratory regression analyses
found that children in the sentence-combining intervention, with a low t1 sentence
combining score, low reading skills or better t1 spelling skills, were more likely to
show improvements at t2. Findings indicate that when devising interventions for
struggling writers, specific profiles of skills should be considered. Specifically, sen-
tence combining may be more appropriate for SWs whose primary area of difficulty
is reading, rather than poor spelling or oral language.
Keywords Intervention· Writing· Sentence combining· Spelling· Writing
difficulties
* Kirsty Walter
kwalter@brookes.ac.uk
1 Institute ofEducation, University College London, London, UK
2 Department ofPsychology, Health andProfessional Development, Oxford Brookes University,
Gipsy Lane, OxfordOX30BP, UK
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1826
K.Walter et al.
1 3
Introduction
Writing development
Becoming a skilled writer is necessary for academic success and participation in
the global economy; yet, it is well-established that around 20% of students strug-
gle with the writing process (DfE, 2019; Graham & Harris, 2009; NCES, 2012).
The written products of struggling writers (SWs) are exemplified by shorter texts,
increased errors in spelling, punctuation and grammar, reduced lexical diversity,
poorly constructed, short or incomplete sentences, and poor compositional qual-
ity (APA, 2013; Dockrell & Connelly, 2015; Dockrell, Connelly, & Arfe, 2019;
Saddler, Behforooz, & Asaro, 2008a, b; Sumner, Connelly, & Barnett, 2014).
Understanding the skills that underpin writing development supports the develop-
ment of interventions for SWs.
Models are designed to capture both the skills that children need to produce
a written text, the more distal factors which underpin these skills and the wider
task environment (Graham etal., 2018). The initial models of writing develop-
ment identified key components in the writing process (transcription and idea
generation) and other factors, such as working memory, which supports written
text production (Berninger & Winn, 2006; McCutchen, 1996; Olive, 2014). More
recently, researchers have moved towards considering both direct and indirect
factors influencing writing development (Dockrell etal., 2019; Kim & Schatsche-
nider, 2017). Proximal factors include those skills which directly impact on the
production of written text such as spelling. By contrast, distal factors are those
which indirectly impact on the writing process, such as oral language and read-
ing. SWs often experience associated difficulties in these areas (see O’Rourke,
Connelly, & Barnett, 2018 for a review).
The role of reading and oral language in writing interventions remains under-
explored. Poor readers produce texts with more spelling errors, less lexical diver-
sity and reduced compositional quality than typically-developing peers (Cara-
volas, Hulme, & Snowling, 2001; Sumner, 2013; Sumner etal., 2014). Reading
abilities are most closely associated with spelling skills (Abbott & Berninger,
1993; Berninger etal., 2008) and so reading may influence writing both directly
and indirectly through spelling. By corollary oral language at word, sentence
and text level supports text generation (Abbott & Berninger, 1993; Babayiğit &
Stainthorp, 2010; Kim, Al Otaiba, Folsom, Greulich, & Puranik, 2014; Oling-
house & Leaird, 2009; Savage, Kozakewich, Genesee, Erdos, & Haigh, 2017;
Sénéchal, Hill, & Malette, 2018). The importance of oral language in supporting
written text production is evident by the significant difficulties in writing experi-
enced by children with language problems (for review see Graham, Hebert, Fish-
man, Ray, & Rouse, 2020).
There is evidence which indicates that children with poor oral language are
less responsive to effective reading interventions (Al Otaiba & Fuchs, 2002) or
may need more intensive interventions as such poor oral language skills may also
affect writing interventions. By extension, reading difficulties may reduce the
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1827
1 3
A sentence‑combining intervention forstruggling writers:…
efficacy of writing interventions given that reading interventions support writing
performance (Graham et al., 2018). Thus, understanding the indirect influence
of oral language and reading to SWs response to intervention provides the basis
for developing effective and targeted writing interventions. The current study
explores the role of reading (both directly and indirectly through spelling) and
oral language (listening comprehension and oral expression) in SWs responsive-
ness to writing interventions.
Effective interventions forstruggling writers
Identifying where to intervene for SWs in upper primary (ages 7 to 11) is challeng-
ing as although many children will have automaticity in the word-level transcrip-
tion skills (e.g. spelling) the classroom teaching has typically moved to higher-
level skills, such as morphology and sentence-level skills (Applebee & Langer,
2011; Dockrell, Marshall, & Wyse, 2016). The process of identifying and adapt-
ing effective interventions can be considered within the Response to Intervention
(RTI) framework (Jimerson, Burns, & VanDerHeyden, 2007). Tier 1 interventions
focus on whole-class teaching practices when children who do not respond to regu-
lar effective classroom teaching, progressing them to interventions at Tier 2 should
be considered. Tier 2 interventions are designed to supplement classroom-based
instruction and typically occur in small groups. Finally, those still falling behind
need to progress to more intensive and specialised Tier 3 interventions, delivered
individually. The current study implements a writing intervention designed to sup-
plement classroom teaching by providing small-group interventions to support the
writing skills in those students who are most at risk of falling behind their peers and
are implemented as a Tier 2 intervention (Jimerson etal., 2007).
Sentence combining interventions
Complex, syntactically-correct sentences characterise competent writing. Construct-
ing well-formed sentences can be problematic for SW’s. Berninger etal., (2011) found
the ability to combine syntactically-correct written sentences develops around seven
to eight years of age in typically-developing children. However, this can be delayed
for SWs, for whom sentence-level difficulties are a significant weakness up to age 11
and often beyond (Dockrell etal., 2019). Research has established sentence-combining
teaching practice as an effective way to develop sentence-level competence for children
aged five to 18years, with moderate to large effect sizes (Andrews etal., 2004; Gra-
ham etal., 2012; Graham, Harris, & Hebert, 2011; Graham & Perin, 2007; Saddler,
Ellis-Robinson, & Asara-Saddler, 2018; Santangelo & Olinghouse, 2009). Sentence-
combining instruction teaches students to combine two or more simple sentences to
make one grammatically correct, sentence. Studies have found this leads to significant
improvements in sentence-combining ability and the syntactic maturity of written sen-
tences and also improves the compositional quality of children’s stories, with moderate
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1828
K.Walter et al.
1 3
to large effect sizes (Andrews etal., 2004; Datchuk & Kubina, 2013; Graham & Perin,
2007; Saddler, Asaro, & Behforooz, 2008a, b).
Morphological spelling interventions
Children who experience significant difficulties with spelling may find sentence level
interventions too challenging (Berninger & Amtmann, 2003). Consequently, a word-
level spelling intervention complementing existing classroom-based teaching may be
more effective. Morphological spelling interventions reflect the shift in focus of class-
room-based instruction in upper primary from phonology to morphology. Research has
shown morphological spelling interventions can improve children’s spelling, sentence-
combining, and text-level writing (Bryant & Nunes, 2000; McCutchen & Stull, 2015;
McCutchen, Stull, Herrera, Lotas, & Evans, 2014; Nunes, Bryant, & Olsson, 2003). A
series of studies by Nunes and Bryant (2006), with children eight years of age, dem-
onstrated that making children explicitly aware of morphemic spelling principles,
such as the use of derivational suffixes, improved children’s spelling ability. Similarly,
McCutchen etal., (2014) found significant improvements in 10–11-year-olds’ use of
morphologically complex words in a sentence-combining task and an extended writing
measure following a 12-week morphological spelling intervention.
Assessing writing
Measures to identify SWs need to be reliable while capturing the key components of
written text production. Measures must also discriminate between typically developing
writers and SWs at different points in development (Bew, 2011; Dockrell, Connelly,
Walter, & Critten, 2017). Standardised measures often focus on the compositional qual-
ity of the text, using holistic or analytical scoring. These can be quick to mark; how-
ever, they can be unreliable and often lack the sensitivity to change which is required
for evaluating intervention effectiveness and often cannot be administered repeatedly
across short time intervals (Dockrell etal., 2017; Dunsmuir etal., 2015).
Curriculum-based Measures of Writing (CBM-W), have a dual focus on the writ-
ers’ productivity and accuracy; they are quick to administer, reliable, valid, sensitive to
change, and able to discriminate between SWs and typically-developing children aged
7–12 at the word-, sentence- and text-level (Dockrell etal., 2017; Gansle, Noell, Van-
DerHeyden, Naquin, & Slider, 2002; Weissenburger & Espin, 2005). CBM-Ws involve
pupils writing, for a short (three to seven minutes) time, in response to a prompt, texts
are then scored on a range of measures (Dockrell etal., 2017; Gansle etal., 2002).
These measures can be repeated over time and thus offer a useful tool for evaluating
interventions.
Current study
Significant numbers of students struggle to learn to write; therefore, practitioners
need access to effective resources that can be utilized as Tier 2 interventions. To
date, research suggests that sentence combining interventions have the potential
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1829
1 3
A sentence‑combining intervention forstruggling writers:…
to improve children’s writing. Yet little is known about the moderating effect of
reading or oral language in response to the sentence combining interventions nor
whether word-level interventions would be more effective for these children.
To address these limitations, the current study used a RTI framework to eval-
uate the effectiveness of a Tier 2 sentence-combining (SC), intervention on writ-
ten composition skills in comparison to a word-level morphological spelling (MS)
intervention and a waiting list control (WLC) group receiving standard classroom
teaching. The SC intervention was adapted from the work of Saddler and colleagues
(e.g. Graham etal., 2008; Saddler, 2012; Saddler & Graham, 2005) to be a Tier 2,
small group, intervention to support SWs. Furthermore, since children’s writing is
influenced indirectly by oral language and reading, the current study explored the,
currently neglected, role of these skills in children’s response to intervention. A
CBM-W measure was used to assess writing; capturing both productivity (e.g. total
words written, words spelled correctly, and number of sentences) and accuracy (e.g.
proportion of correct word sequences) at the word-, sentence- and text level.
It was hypothesized that the sentence-combining intervention would improve
sentence combining ability, compositional quality and measures of productivity and
accuracy captured by the CBM-W. It was also expected that children with poorer
language and spelling skills would be more resistant to change. The MS intervention
was predicted to improve spelling accuracy within the text. It was anticipated that
this intervention would be beneficial for weaker spellers.
Method
Participants
Participants were drawn from three primary schools in the UK which were also par-
ticipating in a parallel longitudinal study on children’s writing development. Screen-
ing for the intervention, conducted at the start of the academic year, was the longi-
tudinal study’s first-time point. 532 children were screened in years 4 (aged 8–9)
and 5 (aged 9–10) and were identified as SWs if, on a standardised writing measure
(Progress in English 9, PiE, Kirkup, Reardon, & Sainsbury, 2006), they were in the
bottom 20% of their year group from each school.
These 123 identified SWs had significantly lower writing scores on the PiE
screening measure than their typically-developing peers, t (326) = 11.72, p < 0.001,
d = -1.73. Parental and child consent was provided. Two further exclusionary cri-
teria removed children who were not monolingual English language speakers and
children already receiving another writing intervention. Thus, a total of 108 SWs
were invited to participate in the intervention, and 76 (70.4%) received parental con-
sent. Attrition rate was 6.6% as five participants failed to complete the intervention
through withdrawal (n = 2) or disruptive behaviour (n = 3).
A total of 71 SWs, aged between 7years 10 months to 10years and 2months
(mean age = 9.08, SD = 7.85) completed the intervention period and were present for
t1 and at least one post-test session (t2 and, or t3). There were 22 girls (10 from
school 1, 7 from school 2, 5 from school 3) and 49 boys (24 from school 1, 13 from
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1830
K.Walter et al.
1 3
school 2, 12 from school 3) and no significant school-based differences in t1 perfor-
mance were found in any measures in the study.
Assessment battery
The assessment battery was administered in class for the PiE, 1:1 for the oral lan-
guage and reading measures, or in small groups for CBM-W.
Screening andmatching
Progress inEnglish (screening) Children completed the long form for the PiE 9
(Kirkup etal., 2006) in two blocks on consecutive days. This included narrative and
non-narrative reading comprehension tasks, a story writing task, a letter-writing task,
a ten-word spelling test and a grammar test. SWs were identified using the writing
subtests; correlation with teacher assessment levels for writing = 0.65; reliability of
the whole PiE assessment battery = 0.93.
Oral expression (t1) Wechsler Individual Achievement Test 2nd Edition UK (WIAT-
II) Oral Expression (Wechsler, 2005) subtests were administered to assess children’s
ability to communicate using oral language. There are three subtests for this age
range, word fluency, visual passage retell and giving directions. The visual passage
retell task requires children to look at tell a story based on a pictorial storyboard.
Stories are marked, 0, 1 or 2, with the inclusion of specific story elements and elabo-
ration being rewarded. Test–retest reliability = 0.86, internal reliability = 0.83–0.89.
The standardised score for this scale was used as a measure of children’s oral expres-
sion.
Listening comprehension (t1) WIAT-II Listening Comprehension (Wechsler, 2005)
subtests (receptive vocabulary, sentence comprehension and expressive vocabulary)
were administered to assess children’s ability to understand what they are hearing;
reliability = 0.80. The standardised score for this scale was used as a measure of chil-
dren’s listening comprehension.
Single word reading (t1) The British Ability Scales 2nd Edition (BAS-II, Elliott,
Smith, & McCulloc, 1997) word reading subtest was administered to assess chil-
dren’s oral reading of single words, with a focus on their word decoding skills; reli-
ability = 0.93. The measure’s ability score was used to assess children’s single word
reading skills.
Target measures
Sentence combining (t1, t2, t3) Children completed all five items, including those
designed for older children, from the WIAT-II Sentences subtest (Wechsler, 2005).
They combined a series of five sentences, of gradually increasing complexity in writ-
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1831
1 3
A sentence‑combining intervention forstruggling writers:…
ing. Each sentence received a score of 0, 1 or 2. Therefore, the maximum score was
10. Inter-rater reliability κ = 0.86.
Single word spelling (t1, t2, t3) The BAS-II (Elliott etal., 1997) single word spelling
subtest was administered to assess children’s spelling ability. Children were asked to
write the given words which were read out alone and in the context of a sentence.
Words gradually increased in difficulty, ceiling and basal rules were applied, and
raw scores were converted to ability scores. The test was discontinued when children
passed two or fewer words in a block. Reliability = 0.91.
Writing product: curriculum‑based measures ofwriting (CBM‑W)
The outcome measure of intervention effectiveness was the CBM-W narrative writ-
ing task (Dockrell, Connelly, Walter, & Critten, 2015) undertaken by the children
at all three assessment time points. Children were given five minutes to write in
response to a prompt, e.g. One day I had the best weekend ever. This measure was
used to establish the extent to which the interventions were effective in generalizing
to writing by assessing writing productivity and accuracy.
Compositional quality (t1, t2, t3) The text-level outcome was the compositional qual-
ity of the text; this was scored using an adaptation of the WIAT-II Holistic Scoring
criteria for written expression (Wechsler, 2005). The stories were scored on a scale
from 0 to 6. A low score indicates a limited attempt to respond without additional
details. A high score means the text is well organised, clear, uses effective transitions
and vivid vocabulary. Inter-rater reliability, κ = 0.82.
CBM‑W accuracy measures (t1, t2, t3) Accuracy measures for the CBM-W were the
proportion of correct word sequences (CWS) and the proportion of words spelled
correctly (WSC). A CWS is defined as a pair of consecutive words that are grammati-
cally and syntactically correct within the context of the phrase. Interrater reliability
(Cohen’s Kappa) for the proportion of CWS and WSC were 0.80 and 0.90, respec-
tively. All scoring followed the criteria set out by Dockrell etal., (2015).
CBM‑W productivity measures (t1, t2, t3) Productivity measures for the CBM-W task
were the total words written (TWW), the number of complete sentences (CS) and the
number of words in complete sentences (WiCS). A sentence was counted as com-
plete if it started with a capital letter, appropriate ending punctuation, had a recognis-
able subject and ending punctuation. Inter-rater reliability (Cohen’s Kappa) for these
measures were 1.00, 0.85 and 0.86, respectively. These were scored according to the
criteria developed by Dockrell etal., (2015).
CBM‑W lexical diversity (t1, t2, t3) In addition to the established scoring criteria for
CBM-W, the narrative scripts’ lexical diversity was analysed using the online soft-
ware, Text Inspector (textinspector.com). Due to the brevity of the texts and to enable
normalised gains to be calculated, Type Token Ratio (TTR) was selected as the meas-
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1832
K.Walter et al.
1 3
ure of lexical diversity. TTR is calculated by dividing the number of different words
(types) divided by the total number of words produced (tokens).
General procedure
SWs were matched in triads across intervention groups according to their reading
and oral language profiles. Children within each triad were randomly assigned to
one of the three intervention groups. There were no differences between the groups
(SC, MS and WLC) on the oral language and reading measures used for matching
(Table1). The descriptive statistics for these measures suggest many of the SWs also
had difficulties with oral language and reading. Furthermore, comparisons between
the intervention groups (SC, MS and WLC) at t1 showed they were equivalent in
the key measures of WIAT 2 sentence combining (F (2, 70) = 0.63, p = 0.535),
BAS spelling (F (2, 69) = 0.63, p = 0.535) and CBM compositional quality (F (2,
62) = 0.41, p = 0.665) (see Table2).
SWs in the intervention groups were given an intervention targeting either sen-
tence-combining or morphological spelling. Those in the WLC group continued
with regular teaching for the duration of the study. The interventions ran twice a
week for eight weeks, in small group sessions (4–6 children per group). Sessions
lasted 25–30 min. All sessions followed a standard manualized procedure, with a
script for each activity, within which it was possible to provide minor adaptations to
meet the needs of the individual children. The progress of SWs in the interventions,
during the intervention period, at both immediate post-test (t2) and 3-month delayed
follow up (t3) was compared to a WLC group. Both interventions were administered
by the first author.
Children’s writing (spelling, sentence combining and text level), reading,
and oral language skills were assessed at baseline (t1, mean age = 9.08 years,
SD = 7.93 months). Following the completion of the intervention writing skills
at the word-, sentence- and text-level (spelling, sentence combining and text pro-
duction measures from the CBM-W) were re-assessed (t2, mean age = 9.12 years,
SD = 7.85months). The post-test assessment battery was repeated at a 3-month fol-
low-up (t3, mean age = 9.05 years, SD = 7.84 months). Testing sessions were con-
ducted over two days. At the end of the study, those in the WLC group received the
SC intervention.
Intervention procedures
The SC programme was adapted from Saddler (2012). The alternative, MS interven-
tion programme was adapted from the work of Nunes and Bryant (Nunes & Bry-
ant, 2006; Nunes etal., 2003; Nunes, Bryant, & Olsson, 2009). Adaptations focused
on developing Tier 2, small group interventions for those at risk of falling behind,
which complemented classroom teaching. A brief outline of the interventions is pro-
vided below. Contact the first author for further details.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1833
1 3
A sentence‑combining intervention forstruggling writers:…
Table 1 Descriptive Statistics for matching variables by intervention group
SC, Sentence−combining; MS, Morphological Spelling; WLC, Waiting List Control
SC MS WLC One-way ANOVA
N Mean SD N Mean SD N Mean SD
Age (months) 23 107.52 7.83 26 108.65 8.40 22 110.45 7.39 F(2,70) = 0.78, p = .461
Oral language measures
WIAT-II Listening Comprehension Standard Score 23 87.00 13.53 26 89.34 10.96 22 87.45 12.91 F(2,70) = 0.25, p = .781
WIAT-II Oral Expression Standard Score 23 77.09 10.37 26 77.00 10.22 22 77.17 10.08 F(2,70) = 0.01, p = .987
Reading measures
BAS Single Word Reading Test Standard Score 23 90.43 11.00 26 84.31 15.28 22 87.27 12.55 F(2,70) = 0.33, p = .274
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1834
K.Walter et al.
1 3
Target intervention: sentence combining
The main body of each session provided strategies, or techniques, teaching the chil-
dren how to combine sentences, gradually increasing in complexity using guided
practice. For the first session, the researcher explained that they were going to learn
some ways to make their sentences better, children then practised sentence combin-
ing and discussed how they found the activity, they were then introduced to the first
strategy of identifying important words. Each subsequent session followed a stand-
ard format of a revision of the previous session (approximately 3min), then two or
three activities focused on practising verbal and written sentence combining (lasting
between 5 to 15min). The first activity typically began with explicit modelling by
the instructor, followed by guided practice. The final activity asked the children to
practice the skills independently. All sessions encouraged students to provide peer
feedback. Sessions finished with a summary (approximately 2min). The final two
sessions taught children to break long sentences into simple sentences and then
improve them. Children were encouraged to discuss answers, provide formative
feedback, and write down responses using either whiteboards or on paper.
Table3 shows the topics for each intervention session. An example script for a
whole session is presented in Fig.1.
Alternative intervention: morphological spelling
The main body of each session used activities or games to teach children mor-
phemes and their spellings. Children were given opportunities to receive feedback
Table 2 Raw score means and SD for WIAT-II sentence combining, BAS spelling and CBM-W compo-
sitional quality scores at each time point
SC = Sentence−combining intervention, MS = Morphological Spelling Intervention, WLC = Wait-
ing List Control. Maximum scores: WIAT−II Sentence Combining = 10, BAS Spelling Ability = 200,
CBM−W Compositional Quality = 5
SC mean (SD) MS mean (SD) WLC mean (SD)
WIAT-II sentence combining
t1 2.43 (1.50) 2.92 (1.90) 3.05 (2.03)
t2 4.04 (1.92) 2.96 (1.84) 2.95 (2.01)
t3 4.30 (2.22) 3.48 (1.78) 3.11 (1.91)
BAS spelling ability score
t1 92.00 (14.33) 86.88 (15.46) 89.38 (17.95)
t2 97.30 (12.65) 93.46 (17.33) 93.18 (14.72)
t3 98.43 (15.97) 95.26 (15.51) 96.53 (14.04)
CBM-W compositional quality
t1 1.76 (0.83) 1.75 (1.19) 2.00 (0.77)
t2 2.48 (0.85) 2.31 (1.12) 2.38 (1.02)
t3 2.39 (1.20) 2.04 (1.12) 2.18 (0.80)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1835
1 3
A sentence‑combining intervention forstruggling writers:…
and correct their answers. The target morphemic principles, and adapted instruc-
tional materials, were taken from Nunes and Bryant (2006), who designed a series
of inflectional and derivational morphological spelling interventions for 8-year-
olds. In session 1, children were introduced to the structure of the intervention and
told they were going to be taught some spelling rules; they discussed how they felt
about spelling and discussed the different ways words can be broken down using a
short spelling test. Subsequent sessions started with a revision of the previous ses-
sion and included two or three activities designed to support the learning of the ses-
sion’s principle. An example activity, called the ’analogy task’, taken from Nunes
and Bryant (2006) is presented in Fig.2, for this activity, children were asked to find
the missing word. Other activities included: grouping words into word classes, iden-
tifying morphemes, finding the missing word and discussion and identification of
affixes. See Table4 for an overview of the topics for each session. To control for dif-
ferences in the amount of time spent writing between the two interventions, children
wrote sentences using some of the target words at the end of each session.
Intervention fidelity
One researcher administered the interventions. To ensure intervention fidelity,
the researcher completed checklists designed to ensure each child participated
and received feedback and timed the sessions. Checklists did not differ between
Table 3 Overview of sentence combining intervention
Session Overview
1 Introduce sentence combining
Strategy 1: Identify important words within kernel sentences
2 Practice Strategy 1 when combining more than two sentences
3 Strategy 2: Using connectives
Practice on two sentences, with a connective provided
4 Practising rearranging the sentences using the two strategies learnt so far on two sentences
5 Practice skills so far on three sentences
6 The role of adjectives
7 Strategy 3: Alter some words by adding affixes
8 Practice Strategy 3 on three or more sentences
9 What makes a good sentence? Practice with less support combining two sentences
10 Practice Strategy 1 solo and combine 2–3 sentences
11 Develop understanding of connectives. Make own list of connectives and use these when
combining sentences
12 Group connectives into similar categories. Practice Strategy 2 with their connectives
13 Practice Strategy 1 and 2 solo, using connectives list, on two sentences
14 As above, with three or more sentences
15 Introduce de-combining sentences. Practice re-combining new simple sentences
16 Practice de-combining skills from session 15
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1836
K.Walter et al.
1 3
intervention sessions, and there was no difference in the duration of the interven-
tion sessions, t (10) = 0.24, p = 0.817, with the SC and MS interventions lasting
an average of 24.95 and 25.07min respectively. With the exception of one session
from the sentence combining intervention, where one activity was cut from the
session due to time constraints, all activities from each session were successfully
administered.
Fig. 1 Example session script for Sentence Combining Intervention, taken from Session
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1837
1 3
A sentence‑combining intervention forstruggling writers:…
Data analysis
Two approaches were taken to control for t1 performance. First, for those measures
with a maximum score (sentence combining, BAS spelling raw score, compositional
quality, CBM-W accuracy measures and CBM-W Lexical Diversity), normalized
Fig. 2 Examples of materials used for analogy game, used to teach suffixes. Adapted from Nunes and
Bryant (2006, pp. 71–74)
Table 4 Overview of morphological spelling intervention
Session Overview
1 Focus on breaking down simple words, e.g. letters, sounds or syllables
2 Explore receptive and expressive knowledge of word classes
3 Grouping words into word classes. Introduce the concept of morphemes
4 Count morphemes. Introduce and use affixes
5 Focus on prefixes that tell us numbers or opposites. Recognise similar
spelling patterns
6 Introduction to suffixes
7 Magic ’e’ when adding suffixes
8 Affixes for person words from verbs and nouns
9 Practice person word affixes
10 Difference between –ion and –ian: same sound, different spelling
11 Continue from previous session. Correct others mis-spellings
12 Past tense endings
13 Use the –less and –ful suffixes
14 Use the –en suffix
15 Recap magic ’e’ and person word suffixes
16 Recap prefixes for number, past tense suffix and –less and –full suffixes
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1838
K.Walter et al.
1 3
gain scores were calculated, these were analyzed using two-way analyses of variance
(ANOVAs). Second, for measures with no maximum score (CBM-W productivity
measures of TWW, CS, and WiCS), t1 performance was entered as a covariate in a
series of one-way analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs). Hedge’s g was used to estab-
lish the size of the effect for each analysis; effect sizes exceeding 0.40 are reported
in the text. Finally, for those measures with significant group-level differences,
exploratory regression analyses were conducted to explore the role of oral language,
spelling and reading skills in SWs responsiveness to the interventions.
Results
The results are presented in two sections; the first examines the impact of the inter-
vention on sentence-combining, compositional quality, spelling and the CBM-W
accuracy and productivity variables. The second section uses exploratory regression
analyses to examine the role of oral language, spelling, and reading skills in chil-
dren’s response to the interventions for measures where the SC intervention group
showed greater gains than the MS or WLC groups.
Intervention effectiveness
To examine differential progress across the three groups between t1 and the t2 and
t3 post-tests normalised gain scores were used for sentence combining, spelling,
compositional quality (for raw scores see Table2), and all the CBM-W accuracy
variables (for raw scores see Online Materials 1).
WIAT‑II sentence combining
Results from the mixed-measures ANOVA (Table5) for WIAT-II sentence combin-
ing revealed a significant main effect of the intervention group (F (2, 62) = 5.81,
p = 0.005,
𝜂2
𝜌
= 0.16). Post hoc comparisons (LSD adjustment) revealed the SC group
showed greater gains than the MS group who in turn showed greater gains than the
WLC group (see Fig. 3). There was no significant main effect of time from t2 to
t3 (F (1, 62) = 1.57, p = 0.215), and no significant group by time interaction (F (2,
62) = 0.05, p = 0.949).
Further analyses, using a Bonferroni correction (α = 0.025), show the difference
between groups was present at both t2 (F (2, 62) = 4.54, p = 0.014) and t3 (F (2,
62) = 4.67, p = 0.013). Post hoc tests (LSD adjustment) showed the SC group had
greater gains, with moderate to large effect sizes, than both the MS (g = 0.78) and
WLC (g = 0.76) groups at t2 (see Table 5). At t3, the MS group had made some
small gains, though not enough to reach significance. These gains meant the dif-
ference between the SC and MS groups at t3 was no longer significant, while still
maintaining a moderate effect size (g = 0.61). The significant difference between SC
and WLC groups was maintained at t3, with a large effect size (g = 0.84).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1839
1 3
A sentence‑combining intervention forstruggling writers:…
Compositional quality
The mixed-measures ANOVA for compositional quality (Table 5) revealed no sig-
nificant main effect of group (F (2, 62) = 0.61, p = 0.548) or time (F (1, 62) = 1.21,
p = 0.277), and no significant group by time interaction (F (2, 62) = 0.76, p = 0.471).
Despite the non-significant main effect, it is worth noting the moderate effect size
(g = 0.48) present when comparing the SC and WLC groups at t3 (see Table5), this
suggests meaningful improvements, which did not reach significance, for children in
the SC group compared to the WLC group at t3.
Table 5 Mean normalised
gain scores, effect sizes at
each time point for sentence
combining, spelling ability and
compositional quality
SC = Sentence−combining intervention group, MS = Morphologi-
cal Spelling Intervention Group, WLC = Waiting List Control Group
SC MS WLC
N Mean (SD) N Mean (SD) N Mean (sSD)
WIAT-II sentence combining
t2 23 0.20 (0.29) 26 0.01 (0.23) 22 -0.10 (0.47)
t3 23 0.23 (0.31) 23 0.06 (0.23) 19 -0.06 (0.37)
Compositional quality
t2 21 0.13 (0.32) 24 0.14 (0.24) 17 0.08 (0.28)
t3 21 0.15 (0.26) 22 0.05 (0.29) 18 0.02 (0.27)
BAS spelling ability
t2 23 0.05 (0.06) 23 0.05 (0.06) 18 0.03 (0.07)
t3 23 0.06 (0.09) 23 0.07 (0.06) 18 0.06 (0.07)
Fig. 3 Normalised gain scores for WIAT-II Sentence Combining for sentence combining (SC) interven-
tion group compared to the morphological spelling (MS) and waiting list control groups (WLC)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1840
K.Walter et al.
1 3
BAS spelling
The mixed-measures ANOVA for BAS Spelling ability (Table5) revealed no signifi-
cant main effect of group (F (1, 61) = 0.31, p = 0.735), a significant main effect of
time (F (1, 61) = 4.44, p = 0.039), with an increase in from t2 to t3 and no significant
group by time interaction (F (2, 61) = 0.17, p = 0.845).
CBM‑W accuracy
Results for the CBM-W accuracy measures are presented in Table6.
The sample size is smaller for some measures as normalised gain scores could
not be calculated for children who had the maximum score at t1.
The mixed-measures ANOVA for proportion of Words Spelled Correctly (WSC)
revealed no significant main effect of group (F (2, 55) = 1.04, p = 0.360) or time
(F (1, 55) = 0.14, p = 0.715), and no significant group by time interaction (F (2,
55) = 0.13, p = 0.884).
The mixed-measures ANOVA on proportion of Correct Word Sequences
(CWS) revealed no significant main effect of intervention group (F (2, 55) = 1.04,
p = 0.360). Despite the non-significant main effect for the intervention group, there
are moderate effect sizes present when comparing the SC to both the MS (g = 0.56)
and WLC (g = 0.69) groups at t2 (see Table 6). These effect sizes were not main-
tained at t3. There was a significant main effect of time (F (1, 55) = 5.07, p = 0.028,
𝜂2
𝜌
= 0.08) where all groups showed greater gains at t3 than at t2 and no significant
group by time interaction (F (2, 55) = 1.77, p = 0.180).
The mixed-measures ANOVA for the lexical diversity measure, TTR, revealed
the main effect of intervention group was approaching significance (F (2, 46) = 2.83,
p = 0.070), no significant main effect of time (F (1, 46) = 0.84, p = 0.364) and no
significant group by time interaction (F (2, 46) = 2.95, p = 0.062). Post-hoc tests
(LSD adjustment) found the MS group made significantly more gains in TTR than
the WLC group with a very large effect size (g = 1.01) at t3. There was also a mod-
erate effect size for the SC group as compared to the WLC group at t3 (g = 0.51).
Table 6 Means, SD and effect sizes for the normalised gain scores of CBM-W proportion of words
spelled correctly, proportion correct word sequences and TTR
SC = Sentence-combining intervention group, MS = Morphological Spelling Intervention Group,
WLC = Waiting List Control Group
Normalised gain variables SC Intervention MS Intervention WLC
N Mean (SD) N Mean (SD) N Mean (SD)
Proportion of words spelled correctly t2 19 0.31 (0.43) 23 0.08 (0.65) 16 0.07 (0.78)
t3 19 0.28 (0.45) 23 -0.06 (1.33) 16 0.08 (0.65)
Proportion of correct word sequences t2 21 0.21 (0.56) 21 -0.27 (1.05) 16 -0.49 (1.38)
t3 21 0.17 (1.00) 21 0.16 (0.84) 16 0.07 (0.64)
Lexical Diversity (TTR) t2 16 -0.48 (0.57) 18 -0.16 (0.49) 15 -0.41 (0.72)
t3 16 -0.25 (0.54) 18 -0.01 (0.35) 15 -0.59 (0.74)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1841
1 3
A sentence‑combining intervention forstruggling writers:…
Although not significant, there were moderate effect sizes for the MS group as com-
pared to the SC group at both t2 (g = 0.59) and t3 (g = 0.52).
CBM‑W productivity
A series of mixed-measures ANCOVAs, with t1 performance as a covariate, was
conducted to explore the performance over time and by intervention group on the
CBM-W productivity variables: TWW, CS, and WiCS. There were no significant
main effects or interactions. For further details, see Online Materials 2.
Summary
To summarise, results for intervention effectiveness found the SC intervention group
made significant gains, with moderate to large effect sizes, in comparison to the
MS and WLC groups, on the WIAT-II sentence combining task at t2. Differences
between the SC and WLC groups were maintained at t3. There was indicative evi-
dence that children in the MS group showed more gains in lexical diversity than
those in the WLC group. There were no other group-level differences on the other
writing measures (compositional quality, spelling, or CBM-W accuracy and produc-
tivity measures).
Exploratory analysis fortherole oforal language, reading andspelling.
To explore the potential impact of oral language, reading and spelling on interven-
tion efficacy, exploratory hierarchical regression analyses were conducted on WIAT-
II sentence-combining gain scores where group group-level differences were signifi-
cant. Children’s oral language, spelling and reading abilities along with t1 scores for
WIAT-II sentence-combining, were entered into the model first. The two dummy
coded intervention variables (with the SC intervention as the reference group) were
added to the second model. There were no issues of multicollinearity.
At t2, the results of the first model were significant (see Table 7). Children with
lower t1 sentence combining scores, lower reading ability and better spelling ability
showed greater gains at t2. The results of the second model, adding the intervention
groups, significantly improved the model. In addition to reading, t1 sentence com-
bining and spelling ability predictors, both the MS and WLC dummy coded vari-
ables predicted children’s normalised gain scores on the sentence combining task at
t2 suggesting sentence combining groups showed the most gains.
At t3, the relationships between the predictor variables and children’s normalised
gain scores changed (see Table8). At this point, the models remained significant, but
the second model did not significantly improve on the first model. Single-word read-
ing was no longer a significant predictor of children’s gain scores for the sentence
combining task. Instead, listening comprehension, spelling ability and t1 sentence
combining predicted gains at t3. Furthermore, at t3, MS intervention was catching
up with the SC intervention as this predictor was no longer significant; those in the
SC group were still doing better than those in the WLC condition.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1842
K.Walter et al.
1 3
Table 7 Hierarchical multiple regression for sentence combining normalised gain scores at t2
* p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001
Predictors t p Standardised β SE F df p Adj. R2R2 change
Model 1
Overall model 8.68 5, 66 < .001 .37 .42
Single word reading standard score − 2.17 .034* − 0.01 0.00
Listening comprehension standard score 1.25 .217 0.00 0.00
Oral expression standard score − 1.51 .135 − 0.01 0.00
BAS spelling ability 3.28 .002** 0.01 0.00
t1 sentence combining − 5.72 < .001*** − 0.12 0.02
Model 2
Overall model 7.63 7, 66 < .001 .42 .06
Single word reading standard score − 2.57 .013* − 0.01 0.00
Listening comprehension standard score 1.44 .154 0.00 0.00
Oral expression standard score − 1.60 .115 − 0.01 0.00
BAS spelling ability 3.23 .002** 0.01 0.00
t1 sentence combining − 5.38 < .001*** − 0.11 0.02
Morphological spelling group − 2.25 .028* − 0.19 0.08
Waiting list control group − 2.22 .030* − 0.20 0.09
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1843
1 3
A sentence‑combining intervention forstruggling writers:…
Table 8 Hierarchical Multiple Regression for sentence combining normalised gain scores at t3
* p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001
Predictors t p Standardised β SE F df p Adj. R2R2 change
Model 1
Overall model 10.66 5, 60 < .001 .45 .49
Single word reading standard score − 0.74 .464 0.00 0.00
Listening comprehension standard score 2.19 .033* 0.01 0.00
Oral expression standard score − 1.29 .204 0.00 0.00
BAS spelling ability 4.39 < .000*** 0.01 0.00
t1 sentence combining − 6.33 < .000*** − 0.12 0.02
Model 2
Overall model 9.02 7, 60 < .001 .48 .05
Single word reading standard score − 1.06 .295 0.00 0.00
Listening comprehension standard score 2.42 .019* 0.01 0.00
Oral expression standard score − 1.27 .208 0.00 0.00
BAS spelling ability 4.34 < .000*** 0.01 0.00
t1 sentence combining − 5.79 < .000*** − 0.11 0.02
Morphological spelling group − 1.83 .073 -0.13 0.07
Waiting list control group − 2.29 .026* − 0.19 0.08
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1844
K.Walter et al.
1 3
Discussion
The current study aimed to establish the effectiveness of a sentence-level, SC inter-
vention in comparison to a word-level MS intervention and a business as usual WLC
group and to capture the impact of oral language and spelling skills on the effective-
ness of the intervention. It was predicted that the sentence-combining intervention
would improve sentence combining ability, compositional quality and measures of
productivity and accuracy captured by the CBM-W and that children with poorer
language and spelling skills would be more resistant to change. The MS intervention
was predicted to improve spelling accuracy within the text.
As predicted, the SC intervention was more effective at improving the sentence
combining ability of SWs in comparison with the MS and WLC control groups at
t2. The difference between the SC and WLC groups was maintained at t3. However,
contrary to expectation, the difference between the SC and MS groups on sentence
combining was no longer significant by t3. Contrary to predictions, there was no
significant group effect of the interventions on compositional quality. The MS inter-
vention did not lead to significant improvements in children’s standardised test spell-
ing scores, but there was weak evidence that the children were producing texts with
greater lexical diversity than those in the WLC groups. These results are discussed
below.
Intervention effectiveness
Consistent with previous research (Andrews et al., 2004; Berninger et al., 2011;
Saddler, 2012; Saddler, Asara, etal., 2008; Saddler & Graham, 2005), SWs who
received the SC intervention learned to combine sentences more effectively than
those in either the MS or WLC groups. Effect sizes comparing SC and WLC
(g = 0.48–0.84) for both sentence-combining and compositional quality measures
are comparable to previous research. For example, in their meta-analysis, Graham
and Perin (2007) found an average ES = 0.50, which was based on five articles with
ES ranging from 0.21–0.66.
However, the lack of wider comparative improvements in SWs written texts (in
quality, productivity and accuracy) suggests that the focus on sentence combining
alone was not appropriate for these SWs. This may reflect either the length of the
intervention or the children’s baseline skills. For example, in the current study, SWs
received an average of 400min teaching in groups of 4 to 6. By contrast, children
in Saddler and Graham’s (2005) Tier 3 intervention study received nearly twice the
amount of instruction (750min) in pairs. Further studies are needed to capture the
effects of intervention dosage for generalization to other aspects of text production.
Dosage and children’s baseline skills likely interact. Better spellers showed greater
gains on the sentence-combining measure suggesting that they had a greater capac-
ity to benefit from the intervention (McCutchen, 1996).
The weak evidence for greater lexical diversity in the texts of children in the MS
group, as compared to the SC and WLC groups, suggests the spelling intervention
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1845
1 3
A sentence‑combining intervention forstruggling writers:…
may have begun to make some text-level impacts. Differences in lexical diversity
may result from increased confidence to attempt unfamiliar words (Sumner etal.,
2014), or due to exposure to new words during the intervention. Alternatively, the
measures used may have been insensitive to developmental change, as a result of the
short time between testing points in the current study.
Exploring theroles oforal language andreading
It was expected that children’s oral language, reading and spelling abilities would
moderate children’s response to the interventions. Many participants had poor oral
language (listening comprehension and oral expression), reading and spelling skills.
The exploratory hierarchical regressions showed that reading, spelling and baseline
t1 sentence combining scores best predicted gains for sentence combining at t2.
However, longer-term gains in sentence combining at t3 were predicted by listen-
ing comprehension, spelling and t1 sentence combining at baseline and confirm the
importance of oral language to maintaining learning from this intervention.
Poor readers with a reasonable level of listening comprehension seem to have
responded better to the intervention on the sentence combining measure while those
with poor oral language skills may be resistant to intervention (Al Otaiba & Fuchs,
2002). Those with poor oral language skills experience difficulties with both word-
and sentence-level skills (Graham etal., 2020) and this may explain their resistance
to intervention as compared to poor readers. Reading skills are thus important for
the initial response to the sentence combining intervention, and there was, in fact,
some small gains to reading decoding made by participants perhaps as a side effect
of the reading exposure undertaken during the intervention. However, to maintain
longer-term gains the distal, but important, language skills measured by listening
comprehension was a stronger predictor, and this conforms to other studies on writ-
ing growth in children (Dockrell, Connelly, & Arfé, 2019) and the importance of
comprehension for sentence combining (Saddler, Ellis-Robinson & Asaro-Saddler
2018).
The consistent influence of the proximal factor of spelling (Dockrell, Connelly,
& Arfé, 2019) at t2 and t3 suggests sentence combining interventions may be more
appropriate for children without significant spelling difficulties. For poor spellers, it
may be more beneficial to use a combined intervention, targeting multiple skills, to
help develop their spelling skills alongside other aspects of written text production.
The role of handwriting as a pre-, or co-, requisite for spelling development could be
a factor depressing spelling levels. However, no measure was taken of handwriting
fluency, and this could be examined in future studies.
Limitations
The small sample sizes, mean these analyses are underpowered, so more work is
needed to confirm the roles of oral language and reading in children’s response to
the interventions. Furthermore, the measure used to assess growth in sentence-com-
bining ability was relatively short in items in comparison to previous studies. For
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1846
K.Walter et al.
1 3
example, Saddler and Graham (2005) used standard scores from a 20-item stand-
ardised measure and found their SC intervention was more effective than a grammar
instruction intervention. The brevity of the sentence-combining measure used in the
current study gives a minimal opportunity for students to demonstrate growth. In the
future, a longer measure, such as that used by Saddler and Graham (2005) should be
used.
Sentence combining is also, in part, a technique that can be used during revision
(B. Saddler, 2012); however, the current study did not encourage children to revise
or edit their work. Therefore, adapting the intervention to provide opportunities for
children to practice these skills during writing, or revision may improve the current
sentence-combining intervention’s effectiveness. As SWs commonly also neglect to
revise or edit their work (Flower & Hayes, 1980), this sort of alteration may be best
combined with an intervention aimed at also developing revision skills.
Implications andfuture research
The inhibiting role of spelling in the sentence-combining models may help explain
why gains were not seen in children’s compositional quality, as it suggests difficul-
ties with spelling may have limited the ability of these children to access the higher-
level skills needed for written text production. A combined intervention approach,
targeting spelling and sentence combining, may be more beneficial for these chil-
dren and so should be explored in future research.
Despite the similarities seen in texts of children with a range of co-morbid dif-
ficulties (Connelly & Dockrell, 2016), using a complete literacy profile (including
oral language, reading, spelling and writing skills) may help with identifying which
intervention(s) may be most effective. Given that, in the current study, longer-term
gains in sentence-combining ability were predicted by baseline levels of spelling
and listening comprehension it may not be appropriate to use this intervention with
children who are struggling with oral language and have not reached a sufficient
level of spelling to allow them to write sentences easily. Researchers should con-
tinue to identify which interventions are most effective for which children to enable
educators to adapt or combine effective interventions using techniques such as those
suggested by Al Otaiba etal., (2018), within a RTI framework.
Summary
The current study adds to the growing body of evidence that sentence-combining
interventions can be an effective tool for improving children’s writing regarding sen-
tence combining. However, they may require a long and intensive course of instruc-
tion before they impact text-level measures. Findings suggest children may need
competence in single word spelling to benefit from sentence combining. Further
research is needed to verify this finding and determine the level of single word spell-
ing that is needed. Furthermore, practitioners may find that sentence combining is
more appropriate for SWs whose primary area of difficulty is reading, rather than
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1847
1 3
A sentence‑combining intervention forstruggling writers:…
poor spelling or oral language. The findings suggest researchers should consider lan-
guage profiles when devising interventions for SWs.
Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at https ://doi.
org/10.1007/s1114 5-021-10135 -8.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License,
which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as
you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Com-
mons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article
are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is
not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission
directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creat iveco mmons .org/licen
ses/by/4.0/.
References
Abbott, R. D., & Berninger, V. W. (1993). Structural equation modeling of relationships among develop-
mental skills and writing skills in primary-and intermediate-grade writers. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 85(3), 478.
Al Otaiba, S., & Fuchs, D. (2002). Characteristics of Children Who Are Unresponsive to Early Literacy
Intervention. Remedial and Special Education, 23(5), 300–316. https ://doi.org/10.1177/07419 32502
02300 50501 .
Al Otaiba, S., Rouse, A. G., & Baker, K. (2018). Elementary grade intervention approaches to treat spe-
cific learning disabilities, including dyslexia. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,
49(4), 829–842.
Andrews, R., Torgerson, C., Beverton, S., Freeman, A., Locke, T., Low, G., etal. (2004). The effect of
grammar teaching (sentence combining) in English on 5 to 16 year old’s accuracy and quality in
written composition: review summary. York, UK: University of York.
APA. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: Author.
Applebee, A. N., & Langer, J. A. (2011). “EJ” Extra: A snapshot of writing instruction in middle school
and high schools. The English Journal, 100(6), 14–27.
Babayiğit, S., & Stainthorp, R. (2010). Component processes of early reading, spelling, and narrative
writing skills in Turkish: a longitudinal study. Reading and Writing, 23(5), 539–568. https ://doi.
org/10.1007/s1114 5-009-9173-y.
Berninger, V. W., & Amtmann, D. (2003). Preventing writing expression disabilities through early and
continuing assessment and intervention for handwriting and/or spelling problems: research into
practice. In H. Swanson, K. R. Harris, & S. Graham (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp.
345–363). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.
Berninger, V. W., Nagy, W., & Beers, S. (2011). Child writers’ construction and reconstruction of sin-
gle sentences and construction of multi-sentence texts: contributions of syntax and transcription to
translation. Reading and Writing, 24(2), 151–182. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s1114 5-010-9262-y.
Berninger, V. W., & Winn, W. D. (2006). Implications in advancements in brain research and technol-
ogy for writing development, writing instruction and educational evolution. In C. A. MacArthur, S.
Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of Writing Research (pp. 96–114). New York: Guilford
Press.
Berninger, V. W., Winn, W. D., Stock, P., Abbott, R. D., Eschen, K., Lin, S. J., etal. (2008). Tier 3 spe-
cialized writing instruction for students with dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 21(1–2), 95–129. https
://doi.org/10.1007/s1114 5-007-9066-x.
Bew, P. (2011). Independent review of key stage 2 testing, assessment and accountability. TSO.
Bryant, P., & Nunes, T. (2000). Phonology and morphology in learning to read and to spell. International
Journal of Psychology, 35(3–4), 4–4.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1848
K.Walter et al.
1 3
Caravolas, M., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2001). The foundations of spelling ability: evidence
from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 45(4), 751–774. https ://doi.
org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2785.
Connelly, V., & Dockrell, J. (2016). Writing development and instruction for students with learning dis-
abilities: using diagnostic categories to study writing difficulties. In C. A. MacArthur, S. Graham, &
J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research, (Second ed., pp. 349–363). The Guilford Press.
Datchuk, S. M., & Kubina, R. M. (2013). A review of teaching sentence-level writing skills to students
with writing difficulties and learning disabilities. Remedial and Special Education, 34(3), 180–192.
DfE. (2019). National curriculum assessments at key stage 2 in England, 2019 (interim). London:
Department for Education. https ://www.gov.uk/gover nment /publi catio ns/natio nal-curri culum -asses
sment s-key-stage -2-2019-inter im/natio nal-curri culum -asses sment s-at-key-stage -2-in-engla nd-
2019-inter im#attai nment -in-readi ng-writi ng-and-maths
Dockrell, J. E., & Connelly, V. (2015). The role of oral language in underpinning the text genera-
tion difficulties in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Research in Reading,
38(1), 18–34. https ://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2012.01550 .x.
Dockrell, J., Connelly, V., & Arfè, B. (2019). Struggling writers in elementary school: capturing driv-
ers of performance. Learning and Instruction, 60, 75–84.
Dockrell, J., Connelly, V., Walter, K., & Critten, S. (2015). Assessing children’s writing products: the
role of curriculum based measures. British Educational Research Journal, 41(4), 575–595. https
://doi.org/10.1002/berj.3162.
Dockrell, J., Connelly, V., Walter, K., & Critten, S. (2017). The role of curriculum based measures in
assessing writing products. In: Writing Development in Struggling Learners (pp. 182–197). Brill.
Dockrell, J. E., Marshall, C. R., & Wyse, D. (2016). Teachers’ reported practices for teaching writing
in England. Reading and Writing, 29(3), 409–434. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s1114 5-015-9605-9.
Dunsmuir, S., Kyriacou, M., Batuwitage, S., Hinson, E., Ingram, V., & O’Sullivan, S. (2015). An
evaluation of the writing assessment measure (WAM) for children’s narrative writing. Assessing
Writing. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.asw.2014.08.001.
Elliott, C. D., Smith, P., & McCulloc, K. (1997). British ability scales II (BAS II). Windsor: NFER
Nelson.
Flower, L., & Hayes, J. R. (1980). The dynamics of composing: making plans and juggling con-
straints. In L. W. Gregg & E. R. Steinberg (Eds.), Cognitive Processes in Writing (pp. 31–50).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gansle, K. A., Noell, G. H., VanDerHeyden, A. M., Naquin, G. M., & Slider, N. J. (2002). Moving
beyond total words written: The reliability, criterion validity, and time cost of alternate measures
for curriculum-based measurement in writing. School Psychology Review, 31(4), 477–497.
Graham, S., Bollinger, A., Olson, C. B., D’Aoust, C., MacArthur, C., McCutchen, D., & Olinghouse,
N. (2012). Teaching elementary school students to be effective writers: a practice guide. NCEE
2012–4058. What Works Clearinghouse.
Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (2009). Evidence-based writing practices: Drawing recommendations
from multiple sources. In: BJEP Monograph Series II, Number 6-Teaching and Learning Writing
(Vol. 95, pp. 95–111): British Psychological Society.
Graham, S., Harris, K. R., & Hebert, M. (2011). Informing writing: the benefits of formative assess-
ment. a report from carnegie corporation of New York. Carnegie Corporation of New York.
Graham, S., Harris, K. R., Mason, L., Fink-Chorzempa, B., Moran, S., & Saddler, B. (2008). How
do primary grade teachers teach handwriting? A national survey. Reading and Writing, 21(1–2),
49–69. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s1114 5-007-9064-z.
Graham, S., Hebert, M., Fishman, E., Ray, A. B., & Rouse, A. G. (2020). Do Children Classified with
specific language impairment have a learning disability in writing? A meta-analysis. Journal of
learning disabilities, 53(4), 292–310.
Graham, S., Liu, X., Bartlett, B., Ng, C., Harris, K. R., Aitken, A., et al. (2018). Reading for writ-
ing: a meta-analysis of the impact of reading interventions on writing. Review of Educational
Research, 88(2), 243–284. https ://doi.org/10.3102/00346 54317 74692 7.
Graham, S., & Perin, D. (2007). Writing next: effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in
middle and high schools: a report to carnegie corporation of New York. Washington, DC: Alli-
ance for Excellent Education.
Jimerson, S., Burns, M. K., & VanDerHeyden, A. (2007). Handbook of response to intervention: the
science and practice of assessment and intervention. New York: Spriner.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1849
1 3
A sentence‑combining intervention forstruggling writers:…
Kim, Y. S., Al Otaiba, S., Folsom, J. S., Greulich, L., & Puranik, C. (2014). Evaluating the dimension-
ality of first-grade written composition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research.
Kim, Y. S., & Schatschneider, C. (2017). Expanding the developmental models of writing: a direct
and indirect effects model of developmental writing (DIEW). Journal of Educational Psychol-
ogy, 109(1), 35.
Kirkup, C., Reardon, T., & Sainsbury, M. (2006). Progress in English 9 (2nd ed.). London: GL
Assessment Limited.
McCutchen, D. (1996). A capacity theory of writing: Working memory in composition. Educational
Psychology Review, 8(3), 299–325. https ://doi.org/10.1007/bf014 64076 .
McCutchen, D., & Stull, S. (2015). Morphological awareness and children’s writing: accuracy,
error and invention. Reading and Writing, 28(2), 271–289. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s1114
5-014-9524-1.
McCutchen, D., Stull, S., Herrera, B. L., Lotas, S., & Evans, S. (2014). Putting Words to work: effects
of morphological instruction on children’s writing. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 47(1), 86–97.
https ://doi.org/10.1177/00222 19413 50996 9.
NCES. (2012). The nation’s report card: writing 2011. Washington: National Center for Education Statis-
tics. https ://nces.ed.gov/natio nsrep ortca rd/pdf/main2 011/20124 70.pdf
Nunes, T., & Bryant, P. (2006). Improving literacy by teaching morphemes. London: Routledge.
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Olsson, J. (2003). Learning morphological and phonological spelling rules: an
intervention study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7(3), 289–307. https ://doi.org/10.1207/s1532 799xs
sr070 3_6.
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Olsson, J. (2009). Learning morphological and phonological spelling rules:
an intervention study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7(3), 289–307. https ://doi.org/10.1207/S1532
799XS SR070 3_6.
Olinghouse, N. G., & Leaird, J. T. (2009). The relationship between measures of vocabulary and narrative
writing quality in second- and fourth-grade students. Reading and Writing, 22(5), 545–565. https ://
doi.org/10.1007/s1114 5-008-9124-z.
Olive, T. (2014). Toward a parallel and cascading model of the writing system: a review of research on
writing process coordination. Journal of Writing Research, 6(2), 173–194. https ://doi.org/10.17239
/jowr-2014.06.02.4.
O’Rourke, L., Connelly, V., & Barnett, A. (2018). Understanding writing difficulties through a model of
the cognitive processes involved in writing. In B. Miller, P. McCardle, & V. Connelly (Eds.), Writ-
ing development in struggling learners: understanding the needs of writers across the lifecourse
(pp. 11–28). Leiden: Brill.
Saddler, B. (2012). Teacher’s guide to effective sentence writing. New York: Guildford Publications Inc.
Saddler, B., Asaro, K., & Behforooz, B. (2008a). The effects of peer-assisted sentence-combining prac-
tice on four young writers with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities: A Contemporary Jour-
nal, 6(1), 17–31.
Saddler, B., Behforooz, B., & Asaro, K. (2008b). The effects of sentence-combining instruction on the
writing of fourth-grade students with writing difficulties. Journal of Special Education, 42(2),
79–90. https ://doi.org/10.1177/00224 66907 31037 1.
Saddler, B., Ellis-Robinson, T., & Asaro-Saddler, K. (2018). Using sentence combining instruction to
enhance the writing skills of children with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities: A Contempo-
rary Journal, 16(2), 191–202.
Saddler, B., & Graham, S. (2005). The effects of peer-assisted sentence-combining instruction on the
writing performance of more and less skilled young writers. Journal of Educational Psychology,
97(1), 43–54. https ://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.97.1.43.
Santangelo, T., & Olinghouse, N. G. (2009). Effective writing instruction for students who have writing
difficulties. Focus on exceptional children, 42(4).
Savage, R., Kozakewich, M., Genesee, F., Erdos, C., & Haigh, C. (2017). Predicting writing development
in dual language instructional contexts: exploring cross-linguistic relationships. Developmental Sci-
ence, 20(1), e12406.
Sénéchal, M., Hill, S., & Malette, M. (2018). Individual differences in grade 4 children’s written compo-
sitions: the role of online planning and revising, oral storytelling, and reading for pleasure. Cogni-
tive Development, 45, 92–104.
Sumner, E. (2013). Children with dyslexia: spelling as a constraint on writing development. (Doctor of
Philosophy). Oxford Brookes University, Oxford.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1850
K.Walter et al.
1 3
Sumner, E., Connelly, V., & Barnett, A. L. (2014). The influence of spelling ability on vocabulary
choices for children with dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 49(3), 293–304. https ://doi.
org/10.1177/00222 19414 55201 8.
Wechsler, D. (2005). Wechsler Individual achievement test, Second UK Edition (WIAT-II UK). London,
UK: Harcourt Assessment.
Weissenburger, J. W., & Espin, C. A. (2005). Curriculum-based measures of writing across grade levels.
Journal of School Psychology, 43(2), 153–169.
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published
maps and institutional affiliations.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center
GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers
and authorised users (“Users”), for small-scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all
copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By accessing,
sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of
use (“Terms”). For these purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and
students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and
conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal subscription. These Terms will prevail over any
conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription (to
the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of
the Creative Commons license used will apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may
also use these personal data internally within ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share
it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not otherwise
disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies
unless we have your permission as detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial
use, it is important to note that Users may not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale
basis or as a means to circumvent access control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any
jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association
unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a
systematic database of Springer Nature journal content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a
product or service that creates revenue, royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as
part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal content cannot be
used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large
scale into their, or any other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not
obligated to publish any information or content on this website and may remove it or features or
functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature may revoke
this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content
which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or
guarantees to Users, either express or implied with respect to the Springer nature journal content and
all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law, including
merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published
by Springer Nature that may be licensed from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a
regular basis or in any other manner not expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer
Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com