ArticlePDF Available

Creativity and the Brain: An Editorial Introduction to the Special Issue on the Neuroscience of Creativity

Authors:
NeuroImage 231 (2021) 117836
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
NeuroImage
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuroimage
Creativity and the brain: An editorial introduction to the special issue on
the neuroscience of creativity
The creative ability of the human brain, among the newest prod-
ucts of 3.8 billion years of evolution on Earth, may be humanity’s most
identity-defining feature in the age of artificial intelligence. Many of
the most complex things humans do are now done —or soon will be
done —far better by computers. Creativity projects to be the greatest
exception. There has never been more widespread recognition that un-
derstanding and fostering human creativity is a priority for scientific
research. The capacity to generate ideas that are both divergent and
useful is widely recognized as valuable for learning and practice in the
arts and sciences, and as a driver of the modern innovation economy.
This value will only increase in the foreseeable future. Because creativ-
ity has such broad and diverse impacts, the neuroscience of creativity
is being pursued by a diverse set of researchers. As is generally true
in the early stages of a field, research endeavors into creativity neu-
roscience have often been undertaken separately by researchers siloed
within sub-disciplines of psychology, education, industry, and clinical
neuroscience. For the neuroscience of creativity to fulfill its considerable
potential, it is important to develop greater mutual awareness and cohe-
sion among researchers, and communication with educators and other
stakeholders, so that priority directions can be identified and pursued.
Meeting this need is a primary objective of the Society for the Neuro-
science of Creativity (SfNC). This special issue (SI) on the neuroscience
of creativity, guest-edited by a group of us who serve on the SfNC Exec-
utive Committee, is aimed at bringing together both expository and new
empirical work from creativity neuroscience labs across the globe. We
hope that this SI can contribute to (1) mapping the diversity of creativ-
ity neuroscience to increase mutual awareness within the field, while
increasing awareness of creativity neuroscience across the broader cog-
nitive neuroscience community; and (2) highlighting promising research
directions toward stronger coalescence around methods and questions
that have potential to catalyze basic understanding of how creativity
occurs in the brain and how to enhance it. In this editorial, we attempt
to summarize the results and theories reported in this SI, situate them
within a larger cognitive neuroscience framework, and provide a modest
list of research priorities for the field.
Overview of special issue
This SI attempts to provide a snapshot of current research in the
neuroscience of creativity, outlining recent advances in the field. Sev-
eral studies included in this SI address novel questions related to pos-
itive and negative influences on creative performance, including the
impacts of stress ( Nair et al. 2020 ) and disease ( Paulin et al. 2020 ;
Gross et al. 2019 ), as well as influences of mindset ( Wang et al. 2019 ),
cognitive reappraisal ( Wu et al. 2019 ), and pharmacological interven-
tion ( Baas et al. 2020 ). The development of the creative brain is ex-
plored ( Saggar et al. 2019 ) as are particular attributes of brain morphol-
ogy and function found in eminent creative achievers ( Chrysikou, et al.
2020 a; Barrett et al. 2020 ). Innovative studies examined team cre-
ativity using fNIRS hyper-scanning ( Mayseless et al., 2019 ; Lu et al.,
2020 ), and how idea generation takes root in semantic, associative and
mnemonic neurocognition ( Paulin et al. 2020 ; Beaty et al. 2020 ). The
neural correlates of musical creativity were investigated along multi-
ple lines ( Zioga et al. 2020 ; Belden et al. 2020 ; Rosen et al. 2020 ;
Bashwiner et al. 2020 ). Other studies demonstrated the effective ap-
plication of methods that have been thus far underutilized in the field,
including neurogenetics ( Si et al. 2020 ), network science ( Kenett et al.,
2020 a; Saggar et al. 2019 ), oculometric signatures ( Salvi et al. 2020 ),
7-Tesla MRI ( Schuler et al., 2019 ), and machine learning ( Stevens and
Zabelina 2020 ). Several studies also revealed novel morphometric
( Sunavsky and Poppenk 2020 ; Wertz et al.,. 2020 a; Chrysikou, et al.
2020 b; Vartanian et al. 2020 ), intrinsic ( Schuler et al. 2019 ;
Marron et al. 2020 ), task-evoked ( Chen et al. 2019 ; Wang et al. 2019 ;
Becker et al., 2020 ; Agnoli et al. 2020 ; Rominger et al. 2020 ;
Benedek et al. 2020 ; Hartung et al. 2020 ; Oh et al. 2020 ; Roberts et al.,
2020 ; Takeuchi et al., et al. 2020 b), and structural connectivity
( Wertz et al. 2020 b; Takeuchi et al., 2020 a) characteristics associ-
ated with individual differences in creative thinking, including con-
nectomic analysis of the novel construct of creativity-specific anxiety
( Ren et al. 2021 ).
Three theoretical/review papers ( Zhang et al., 2020 ; Girn et al. 2020 ;
Matheson and Kenett 2020) and one meta-analytically-based proof of
concept for neurally-informed ontologies ( Kenett et al. 2020 b) helped
capture how cognitive neuroscience researchers conceive of creativity
and how the constructs we use can be mapped onto and constrained
by brain function. The review articles address original questions in-
cluding the role of metacognition and mental states in creativity. The
meta-analysis explores the important issue of how best to operationally
capture cognitive constructs related to creativity with a set of experi-
mental tasks, leveraging the extant pool of neuroimaging data toward a
new method for ontological development that has promise for creativity
research and for all fields of cognitive neuroscience.
Many studies employed neuroimaging to examine task-induced brain
activity and/or brain connectivity (10 studies employed fMRI; 3 stud-
ies used fNIRS), or explore how interindividual differences in creative
abilities relate to white and gray matter regional structure ( n = 7),
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117836
Available online 5 February 2021
1053-8119/© 2021 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license
( http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ )
NeuroImage 231 (2021) 117836
Fig. 1. Summary statistics as a graph for the 42 articles included in this special issue. The graph represents similarity across the included articles. The similarity
was assessed by comparing keywords provided by the study authors for each article. Different annotations (node coloring schemes) were used to depict different
aspects of each article. The left graph is annotated by the type of neuroimaging modality used, with node coloring as follows: fMRI (dark green), fNIRS (orange),
structural (purple), eye-tracking (pink), pharmacological (light green), genetics (yellow), EEG (brown), and review articles (gray). The middle graph is annotated
by task-type-intrinsic (at rest) vs. evoked task design, with node coloring as follows: resting-state (dark green) and task-based (orange). Lastly, the right graph is
annotated by the continent of the senior author’s lab, with node coloring as follows: Europe (dark green), Asia (orange), North America (purple), Australia (pink),
and South America (light green). In each graph the size of node represents the number of participants used in each study.
or to measure intrinsic functional connectivity during rest ( n = 7).
Reflective of the field of creativity research more broadly, creative
cognition was mainly assessed in the verbal domain (21 studies used
only verbal tasks; 5 studies used figural tasks; 4 used a combination
of assessments in different domains; and 4 used questionnaires to es-
timate creative achievements in real life). Creativity assessment was
most frequently operationalized via divergent thinking tasks ( n = 21),
among which the Alternative Uses Task was predominant (used in 15
studies). Attempts to better capture the diversity of creative cognition
are noteworthy in 4 studies ( Baas et al. 2020 ; Benedek et al. 2020 ;
Chrysikou et al. 2020 a; Sunavsky and Poppenk 2020 ) that used a combi-
nation of measures from distinct frameworks (divergent and convergent
thinking, creative achievement, openness to experience). In addition, 5
studies ( Barrett et al. 2020 ; Bashwiner et al. 2020 ; Belden et al. 2020 ;
Rosen et al. 2020 ; Zioga et al. 2020 ) represent the substantial and
methodologically diverse presence of musical creativity within the field.
To better understand the scope of research covered by this special
issue in terms of neuroimaging modality, demographics, and experimen-
tal design, we embedded all 42 articles in a low-dimensional space and
examined the similarity between them ( Fig. 1 ). To embed each article,
we applied Google’s Universal Sentence Encoder ( Cer et al. 2018 ) to
keywords submitted with each article. The similarity across articles was
then assessed using Euclidean distance. The resulting similarity matrix
was visualized in a 2-D force layout as a graph, where nodes represent ar-
ticles and edges represent similarity. We annotated the generated graph
using different meta-information: modality used, experimental design,
and geographical location of the lab. Fig. 1 left graph illustrates the clear
prevalence of fMRI among modalities for exploring brain bases for cre-
ativity, followed by structural morphometric, and diffusion-based stud-
ies. Largest node-sizes represent consortium-level studies with > 1000
participants enrolled. As shown in Fig. 1 middle graph, evoked or task-
based studies were most strongly represented within this special issue,
compared with examinations of intrinsic or spontaneous correlates of
creative thinking. Lastly, Fig. 1 right graph represents geographic lo-
cation of the labs contributing to the SI (based on senior author affili-
ations). Labs in North America are most frequently represented in the
SI, followed by labs in Asia. Overall, these analyses indicate the breadth
and diversity of the selection of articles included in this SI, which re-
flect our current understanding of the cognitive and neural mechanisms
of creative thought.
Outlook and future directions for the neuroscience of creativity
Ten years ago, having a special issue on the neuroscience of cre-
ativity in a mainstream neuroimaging journal, with 42 outstanding
contributions selected from a much larger set of high-quality submis-
sions, would not have appeared likely. Even just a decade ago, under-
standing the neural bases of creative thinking was at the outskirts of
cognitive neuroscience research. Much has changed since then. A key
drivers of the movement of creativity neuroscience toward a more cen-
tral position within cognitive neuroscience has been the commitment of
creativity researchers to situate and examine creative thinking within
better-established aspects of cognition, such as semantic and autobio-
graphical memory, attention, mentalization, and cognitive control, e.g.,
( Beaty et al. 2016 ; Chrysikou 2018 ; 2019 ; Kenett et al. 2018 ; Volle 2018 ;
Zabelina and Andrews-Hanna 2016 ; Xie et al. 2021 ; Abraham 2014 ). Un-
derstanding creativity necessitates understanding how these processes
take place in the context of creative thinking tasks. On the other hand,
creativity is more than the sum of its ‘cognitive parts’: A comprehen-
sive understanding of how new ideas can come about from already ex-
isting knowledge requires a synthesis of extant findings toward a work-
ing theoretical framework of creativity neuroscience. Although pieces of
this framework are evident across the excellent research featured in this
SI, future work toward theoretical unification will be essential. Addi-
tionally, questions of the where , and —critically when and how creativ-
ity happens within and between key neural networks, and in conjunc-
tion with activity throughout the brain still remain. Are these processes
consistent across creative domains? Does the current evidence on task-
evoked creativity neuroscience, much of which is featured in this SI, gen-
eralize to long-term (e.g., multi-year) creative endeavors? Critically, can
creativity be enhanced by enhancing activity in the identified brain sys-
tems using non-invasive brain stimulation (e.g., Chrysikou et al. 2013 ;
Green et al. 2017 ; Lucchiari et al., 2018 ; Radel et al. 2015 ) or domain-
general training ( Saggar et al. 2017 )?
The rise of creativity neuroscience research holds strong potential to
advance our understanding of more traditional cognitive neuroscience
domains. By examining how memory, attention, cognitive control, and
social cognition processes, among others, contribute and interact within
creative cognition, we can test the validity of well-established knowl-
edge in these subfields. Parallels between creativity research and re-
search examining cognitive and behavioral flexibility are also beginning
2
NeuroImage 231 (2021) 117836
to emerge ( Uddin, 2021 ). Creativity neuroscience thus presents a unique
testbed for theories across all cognitive neuroscience research. Never-
theless, because fundamental research and methodologies within these
domains are advancing rapidly, increased interdisciplinary collabora-
tions among creativity neuroscientists and experts from other cognitive
neuroscience domains will be required to advance knowledge.
As evident in the multiple and complementary methods employed
in the studies featured in this SI, creativity neuroscience has pro-
gressed from simple ‘activation-based’ fMRI studies to complex net-
work analytical paradigms. Much knowledge has been gained from
these methods, and cognitive neuroscience methods continue to ad-
vance with respect to resolution and multidimensionality. Methodologi-
cal advances notwithstanding an emerging research gap concerns the
relationship between how we study creativity in the lab and how
creativity happens in the real world. Although several studies fea-
tured in this SI examined aspects of ‘real world’ creativity such as
team problem solving (e.g., Mayseless et al., 2020), musical creativ-
ity (e.g., ( Zioga et al. 2020 ; Belden et al. 2020 ; Rosen et al. 2020 ;
Bashwiner et al. 2020 ), and real-life high creative achievers (e.g.,
Chrysikou et al., 2020 a; Chrysikou et al., 2020 b), much additional work
using ecologically valid, real-world tasks will be required to ensure
broad generalizability of creativity neuroscience findings.
For both lab-based and real-world creativity measures, a key future
direction for creativity neuroscience is the development of a clearer and
more uniform ontology of creativity constructs and measurement. In
order for studies to inform each other, researchers must agree on a vo-
cabulary so that the same terms refer to the same constructs and, most
importantly, use consistent measurement instruments/tasks to opera-
tionalize these constructs. In this SI, ( Kenett et al., 2020 b) demonstrate
proof-of-concept for a novel approach to deriving a neurally-informed
ontology of creativity measurement that leverages meta-analytic neu-
roimaging data in combination with representational similarity analy-
sis. Approaches such as this one that leverage the ever-growing body of
creativity neuroimaging data to empirically optimize the fit of tasks to
constructs are promising for the future of creativity measurement.
Across these promising directions for future research, creativity neu-
roscience has substantial opportunity to benefit from, and contribute to,
the momentum toward open science that has developed in the broader
fields of neuroimaging and cognitive neuroscience ( Poldrack and Gor-
golewski 2014 ). To reduce publication bias, preregistering a study plan
with details about data acquisition, exclusion criteria, and data analysis
before any data have been acquired should be encouraged when prac-
ticable ( Gorgolewski and Poldrack 2016 ; Open Science Collaboration
2016 ). Further, data sharing irrespective of the sample size of the study
should also be encouraged. It has been convincingly argued that greater
availability of data from small-sample studies could help with failing
faster, developing innovative methods, improving statistical power for
future studies ( Mumford, 2012 ), as well as validating older results on
newer datasets ( Saggar and Uddin 2019 ).
There is no lack of enthusiasm for creativity neuroscience, but the
growth of the field depends greatly on how effectively that energy can
be harnessed. In these early days, individual studies are not always
clearly contextualized in relation to existing studies, and there are in-
stances of crosstalk and redundancy that cloud interpretation and slow
progress. Scientific societies play a crucial role in the development of
a field by providing platforms for sharing new ideas, establishing stan-
dards for methodological rigor, and fostering cohesion and collaboration
to achieve a force multiplier-effect. SfNC was formed with the academic
charter to support interdisciplinary research on the neural and cognitive
bases of creativity and related processes, and to provide an inclusive fo-
rum for communicating this research so that it has maximal impacts for
education, health, innovation, and artistic performance. SfNC and other
organizations focused on the rigorous empirical study of creativity, and
projects such as this SI that present the field both to itself and to the
broader neuroscientific community, are essential for combining the en-
ergy sources surrounding creativity neuroscience to advance the field in
productive directions.
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to the authors for submitting their original research
to this special issue. We are also indebted to the ‘unsung heroes’ –re-
viewers, who have devoted substantial amounts of their time and exper-
tise to improving submitted articles with their constructive feedback. We
also thank the staff at the NeuroImage editorial office for their unending
support and guidance throughout the development of the special issue.
Manish Saggar
Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University,
Stanford, CA, USA
Emmanuelle Volle
Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Épinière (ICM), Sorbonne Université,
Paris, France
Lucina Q. Uddin
Department of Psychology, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL, USA
Evangelia G. Chrysikou
Department of Psychology, Drexel University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
Adam E. Green
Department of Psychology, Georgetown University, Washington, DC, USA
Corresponding author.
E-mail address: l.uddin@miami.edu (L.Q. Uddin)
References
Abraham, A., 2014. Creative thinking as orchestrated by semantic processing vs.
Cognitive control brain networks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 95. doi: 10.3389/fn-
hum.2014.00095 .
Agnoli, S., Zanon, M., Mastria, S., Avenanti, A., Corazza, G.E., 2020. Predicting re-
sponse originality through brain activity: an analysis of changes in EEG al-
pha power during the generation of alternative ideas. NeuroImage 207, 116385.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116385 .
Baas, M., Boot, N., Gaal, S.V., Dreu, C.K W d., Cools, R., 2020. Methylphenidate does
not affect convergent and divergent creative processes in healthy adults. NeuroImage
205, 116279. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116279 .
Barrett, K.C., Barrett, F.S., Jiradejvong, P., Rankin, S.K, Landau, A.T, Limb, C.J,
2020. Classical creativity: a functional magnetic resonance imaging (FMRI) inves-
tigation of pianist and improviser Gabriela Montero. NeuroImage 209, 116496.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116496 .
Bashwiner, D.M, Bacon, D.K, Wertz, C.J, Flores, R.A, Chohan, M.O, Jung, R.E, 2020.
Resting state functional connectivity underlying musical creativity. NeuroImage 218,
116940. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116940 .
Beaty, R.E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P.J., Schacter, D.L., 2016. Creative cognition and brain
network dynamics. Trends Cognit. Sci. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.004 .
Beaty, R.E, Chen, Q., Christensen, A.P, Kenett, Y.N, Silvia, P.J, Benedek, M., Schacter, D.L,
2020. Default network contributions to episodic and semantic processing during di-
vergent
creative thinking: a representational similarity analysis. NeuroImage 209,
116499. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116499 .
Becker, M., Sommer, T., Kühn, S., 2020. Inferior frontal gyrus involvement during search
and solution in verbal creative problem solving: a parametric FMRI study. NeuroImage
206, 116294. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116294 .
Belden, A., Zeng, T., Przysinda, E., Anteraper, S.A., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Loui, P.,
2020. Improvising at rest: differentiating jazz and classical music train-
ing with resting state functional connectivity. NeuroImage 207, 116384.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116384 .
Benedek, M., Jurisch, J., Koschutnig, K., Fink, A., Beaty, R.E, 2020. “Ele-
ments of creative thought: investigating the cognitive and neural corre-
lates of association and bi-association processes. NeuroImage 210, 116586.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116586 .
Cer, D., Yang, Y., Kong, S.Y., Hua, N., Limtiaco, N., John, R.S., Constant, N., et al., 2018.
Universal sentence encoder for english. In: Proceedings of the EMNLP 2018 - Confer-
ence on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing: System Demonstrations,
Proceedings doi: 10.18653/v1/d18-2029 .
Chen, Q., Beaty, R.E, Cui, Z., Sun, J., He, H., Zhuang, K., Ren, Z., Liu, G., Qiu, J., 2019.
Brain hemispheric involvement in visuospatial and verbal divergent thinking. Neu-
roImage 202, 116065. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116065 .
3
NeuroImage 231 (2021) 117836
Chrysikou, E.G., 2018. The costs and benefits of cognitive control for cre-
ativity. The Cambridge Handbook of the Neuroscience of Creativity
doi: 10.1017/9781316556238.018 .
Chrysikou, E.G., 2019. Creativity in and out of (Cognitive) Control. Curr. Opin. Behav.
Sci. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.09.014 .
Chrysikou, E.G., Hamilton, R.H., Branch Coslett, H., Datta, A., Bikson, M., Thompson-
Schill, S.L., 2013. Noninvasive transcranial direct current stimulation over the left
prefrontal cortex facilitates cognitive flexibility in tool use. Cognit. Neurosci. 4 (2).
doi: 10.1080/17588928.2013.768221 .
Chrysikou, E.G, Jacial, C., Yaden, D.B, Dam, W.V., Kaufman, S.B., Conklin, C.J, Winter-
ing, N.A, Abraham, R.E, Jung, R.E, Newberg, A.B, 2020a. Differences in brain activity
patterns during creative idea generation between eminent and non-eminent thinkers.
NeuroImage 220, 117011. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117011 .
Chrysikou, E.G, Wertz, C., Yaden, D.B, Kaufman, S.B., Bacon, D., Wintering, N.A,
Jung, R.E, Newberg, A.B, 2020b. Differences in brain morphometry associated with
creative performance in high- and average-creative achievers. NeuroImage 218,
116921. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116921 .
Girn, M., Mills, C., Roseman, L., Carhart-Harris, R.L, Christoff, K., 2020. Updating the dy-
namic framework of thought: creativity and psychedelics. NeuroImage 213, 116726.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116726 .
Gorgolewski, K.J., Poldrack, R.A., 2016. A practical guide for improving transparency
and reproducibility in neuroimaging research. PLoS Biol.
14 (7). doi: 10.1371/jour-
nal.pbio.1002506 .
Green, A.E., Spiegel, K.A., Giangrande, E.J., Weinberger, A.B., Gallagher, N.M.,
Turkeltaub, P.E., 2017. Thinking cap plus thinking zap: Tdcs of frontopolar cortex im-
proves creative analogical reasoning and facilitates conscious augmentation of state
creativity in verb generation. Cereb. Cortex 27 (4). doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw080 .
Gross, M.E, Araujo, D.B, Zedelius, C.M, Schooler, J.W, 2019. Is perception the missing
link between creativity, curiosity and schizotypy? Evidence from spontaneous eye-
movements and responses to auditory oddball stimuli. NeuroImage 202, 116125.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116125 .
Hartung, F., Kenett, Y.N, Cardillo, E.R, Humphries, S., Klooster, N., Chatterjee, A., 2020.
Context matters:
novel metaphors in supportive and non-supportive contexts. Neu-
roImage 212, 116645. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116645 .
Kenett, Y.N., Medaglia, J.D., Beaty, R.E., Chen, Q., Betzel, R.F., Thompson-
Schill, S.L., Qiu, J., 2018. Driving the brain towards creativity and in-
telligence: a network control theory analysis. Neuropsychologia 118.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.01.001 .
Kenett, Y.N, Betzel, R.F, Beaty, R.E, 2020a. Community structure of the creative brain at
rest. NeuroImage 210, 116578. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116578 .
Kenett, Y.N, Kraemer, D.J M, Alfred, K.L, Colaizzi, G.A, Cortes, R.A, Green, A.E, 2020b. De-
veloping a neurally informed ontology of creativity measurement. NeuroImage 221,
117166. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117166 .
Lu, K., Yu, T., Hao, N., 2020. Creating while taking turns, the choice
to unlocking group creative potential. NeuroImage 219, 117025.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117025 .
Lucchiari, C., Sala, P.M., Vanutelli, M.E., 2018. Promoting creativity through transcra-
nial direct current stimulation (TDCS). A critical review. Front. Behav. Neurosci.
doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00167 .
Marron, T.R, Berant, E., Axelrod, V., Faust, M., 2020. Spontaneous cognition and its re-
lationship to human creativity: a functional connectivity study involving a chain free
association task. NeuroImage 220, 117064. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117064 .
Mayseless, N., Hawthorne, G., Reiss, A.L, 2019. Real-life creative problem solv-
ing in teams: FNIRS based hyperscanning study. NeuroImage 203, 116161.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116161 .
Mumford, J.A., 2012. A Power Calculation Guide for FMRI Studies. Social Cognitive and
Affective Neuroscience 7 (6). doi: 10.1093/scan/nss059 .
Nair, N., Hegarty, J.P, Ferguson, B.J, Hecht, P.M, Tilley, M., Christ, S.E, Beversdorf, D.Q,
2020. Effects of stress on functional connectivity during problem solving. NeuroImage
208, 116407. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116407 .
Oh, Y., Chesebrough, C., Erickson, B., Zhang, F., Kounios, J., 2020.
An insight-related neural reward signal. NeuroImage 214, 116757.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116757 .
Open Science Collaboration, 2016. Psychological science under scrutiny: recent challenges
and proposed solutions. Psychol. Sci. Under Scrut. Recent Chall. Propos. Solut. .
Paulin, T., Roquet, D.,
Kenett, Y.N, Savage, G., Irish, M., 2020. The effect of semantic
memory degeneration on creative thinking: a voxel-based morphometry analysis. Neu-
roImage 220, 117073. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117073 .
Poldrack, R.A, Krzysztof, J.G., 2014. Making big data open: data sharing in neuroimaging.
Nat. Neurosci. 17 (11), 1510–1517. doi: 10.1038/nn.3818 .
Radel, R., Davranche, K., Fournier, M., Dietrich, A., 2015. The role of (Dis)Inhibition
in creativity: decreased inhibition improves idea generation. Cognition 134.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.09.001 .
Ren, Z., Daker, R.J, Shi, L., Sun, J., Beaty, R.E, Wu, X., Chen, Q., et al., 2021.
Connectome-based predictive modeling of creativity anxiety. NeuroImage 225,
117469. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117469
.
Roberts, R.P, Grady, C.L, Addis, D.R., 2020. Creative, internally-directed cogni-
tion is associated with reduced BOLD variability. NeuroImage 219, 116758.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116758 .
Rominger, C., Papousek, I., Perchtold, C.M, Benedek, M., Weiss, E.M, Weber, B., Schwerdt-
feger, A.R, Eglmaier, M.T W, Fink, A., 2020. Functional coupling of brain networks
during creative idea generation and elaboration in the figural domain. NeuroImage
207, 116395. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116395 .
Rosen, D.S, Oh, Y., Erickson, B., Zhang, F.(Z.), Youngmoo, E.K., Kounios, J., 2020. Dual-
process contributions to creativity in jazz improvisations: an SPM-EEG study. Neu-
roImage 213, 116632. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116632 .
Saggar, M., Quintin, E.-M., Bott, N.T., Kienitz, E., Chien, Y.-H., Hong, D.W.-C., Liu, N.,
Royalty, A., Hawthorne, G., Reiss, A.L., 2017. Changes in Brain Activation Associated
with Spontaneous
Improvization and Figural Creativity After Design-Thinking-Based
Training: A Longitudinal FMRI Study. Cereb. Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991) 27 (7),
3542–3552. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw171 .
Saggar, M., Uddin, L.Q., 2019. Pushing the boundaries of psychiatric neuroimaging to
ground diagnosis in biology. ENeuro 6 (6). doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0384-19.2019 .
Saggar, M., Xie, H., Beaty, R.E., Stankov, A.D., Schreier, M., Reiss, A.L., 2019.
Creativity slumps and bumps: examining the neurobehavioral basis of creativ-
ity development during middle childhood. NeuroImage 196 (August), 94–101.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.080 .
Salvi, C., Simoncini, C., Grafman, J., Beeman, M., 2020. Oculometric signature
of switch into awareness? Pupil size predicts sudden insight whereas mi-
crosaccades predict problem-solving via analysis. NeuroImage 217, 116933.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116933 .
Schuler, A.L., Tik, M., Sladky, R., Luft, C.D.B., Hoffmann, A., Woletz, M., Zioga, I.,
Bhattacharya, J., Windischberger, C., 2019. Modulations in resting state net-
works of subcortical structures linked to creativity. NeuroImage 195, 311–319.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.017 .
Si, S., Su, Y., Zhang, S., Zhang, J., 2020. Genetic susceptibility to parent-
ing style: DRD2 and COMT influence creativity. NeuroImage 213, 116681.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116681 .
Stevens, C.E, Zabelina, D.L, 2020. Classifying creativity: applying machine learn-
ing techniques to divergent thinking EEG data. NeuroImage 219, 116990.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116990 .
Sunavsky, A., Poppenk,
J., 2020. Neuroimaging predictors of creativity in healthy adults.
NeuroImage 206, 116292. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116292 .
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Matsudaira, I., Ikeda, S., Kelssy, H.d., Kawata, S., Nouchi, R.,
Sakaki, K., et al., 2020a. Convergent creative thinking performance is associated
with white matter structures: evidence from a large sample study. NeuroImage 210,
116577. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116577 .
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Nouchi, R., Yokoyama, R., Kotozaki, Y., Nakagawa, S.,
Sekiguchi, A., et al., 2020b. Originality of divergent thinking is associated with work-
ing memory–related brain activity: evidence from a large sample study. NeuroImage
216, 116825. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116825 .
Uddin, L. Q. ,
2021. Cognitive and behavioural flexibility: neural mechanisms and clinical
considerations. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. In press .
Vartanian, O., Smith, I., Lam, T.K, King, K., Lam, Q., Beatty, E.L, 2020. The relationship
between methods of scoring the alternate uses task and the neural correlates of diver-
gent thinking: evidence from voxel-based morphometry. NeuroImage 223, 117325.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117325 .
Volle, E., 2018. Associative and controlled cognition in divergent thinking: theoretical,
experimental, neuroimaging evidence, and new directions. The Cambridge Handbook
of the Neuroscience of Creativity doi: 10.1017/9781316556238.020 .
Wang, X., He, Y., Lu, K., Deng, C., Qiao, X., Hao, N., 2019. How does
the embodied metaphor affect creative thinking? NeuroImage 202, 116114.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116114 .
Wertz, C.J, Chohan, M.O, Flores, R.A, Jung, R.E, 2020a. Neuroanatomy of creative
achievement. NeuroImage 209, 116487. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116487 .
Wertz, C.J, Chohan, M.O, Ramey, S.J, Flores, R.A, Jung, R.E, 2020b. White matter
correlates of creative cognition in a normal cohort. NeuroImage 208, 116293.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116293 .
Wu, X., Guo, T., Tan, T., Zhang, W., Qin, S., Fan, J., Luo, J., 2019. Superior emo-
tional regulating effects of creative cognitive reappraisal. NeuroImage 200, 540–551.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.06.061 .
Xie, H. , Beaty, R. , Jahanikia, S. , Geniesse, C. , Sonalkar, N.
, Saggar, M. , 2021. Spontaneous
and deliberate modes of creativity: multitask Eigen-connectivity analysis captures la-
tent cognitive modes during creative thinking. BioRxiv .
Zabelina, D.L., Andrews-Hanna, J.R., 2016. Dynamic network interac-
tions supporting internally-oriented cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.
doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2016.06.014 .
Zhang, W., Sjoerds, Z., Hommel, B., 2020. Metacontrol of human creativity: the neurocog-
nitive mechanisms of convergent and divergent thinking. NeuroImage 210, 116572.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116572 .
Zioga, I., Harrison, P.M C, Pearce, M.T, Bhattacharya, J., Luft, C.D.B., 2020. From
learning to creativity: identifying the behavioural and neural correlates of learn-
ing to predict human judgements of musical creativity. NeuroImage
206, 116311.
doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116311 .
4
... These characteristics encompassed local grey matter and white matter predictors within the IFG, the superior longitudinal fasciculus that connects these regions, and the functional connectivity between the IFG and IPL. According to the study [97], there were identifiable patterns in the development of children's creative thinking ability during middle childhood. The research also revealed that enhanced segregation or functional specialization in the right lateral frontal region of the brain corresponded with increases in creative thinking capacity over time. ...
... The correlation between creativity and academic achievement is a topic that has generated considerable debate. In the scholarly discourse, a study [97] draws attention to divergent findings in the existing literature concerning the correlation between creativity and academic attainment. Nevertheless, most of the studies examined in this study substantiate the existence of a favorable correlation between creativity and academic performance. ...
Article
Full-text available
The present study provides an in-depth review of the neurocognitive aspects of creativity and its association with academic achievement in children. The outcomes of this study indicate a considerable positive relationship between creativity and academic ability, namely in reading, comprehension, and written tasks. Neurocognitive processes, including associative thinking, divergent thinking, executive functions, and predictive representations, play a significant role in shaping the characteristics associated with creativity. Although creativity may lead to potential adverse outcomes, extensive study has firmly established its significance in academic accomplishment. This systematic review includes studies conducted between the years 2016 and 2022. The primary selection criteria employed for the inclusion of articles in the research encompassed the following aspects: articles written in the English language, articles directly relevant to the subject matter of the study, articles featuring a sample population comprising students, displaying regular and typical developmental patterns, articles adopting a descriptive, exploratory, or longitudinal study design. The research promotes the cultivation of creativity within educational settings, emphasizing the utilization of cultural resources, the alignment of teachers’ attitudes with the promotion of creativity, and the comprehension of neurocognitive factors influencing creative processes. There is a significant focus on the necessity for more investigation into the correlation between creativity and academic achievement. Further research is needed to gain a comprehensive understanding of how creativity is incorporated within educational settings to achieve academic performance.
... Divergent thinking is crucial for generating a variety of alternative solutions or viewpoints during task completion [22]. It involves the development of interconnected ideas related to a specific topic or problem, helping individuals create a wide range of solutions [23,24]. Divergent thinking focuses on seeking answers from various angles [25][26][27]. ...
Article
Full-text available
As a translation strategy, mind mapping has a positive influence on the development of students’ divergent thinking skills. However, research on how mind mapping affects divergent thinking has primarily relied on traditional testing methods, which mainly focus on completed translation tasks. There is still a lack of research using dynamic and procedural experimental methods. An electroencephalogram (EEG) is one of the most effective tools for identifying and analyzing cognitive processing. In this study, we utilized EEG data to explore the effects of mind mapping on divergent thinking skills, with a primary focus on the dynamics of the alpha band power, which is closely related to divergent thinking. The participants were university students learning foreign languages in Dalian, China. One group received foreign language translation teaching integrated with mind mapping training (experimental group), while the other group received regular foreign language translation teaching (control group). The task-related alpha band power of participants engaged in divergent thinking tasks shows that participants in the experimental group exhibited superior divergent thinking skills, as evidenced by greater elevations in alpha band power within the anterior frontal lobe (M = 0.582, SD = 0.301), frontal lobe (M = 0.312, SD= 0.103), frontocentral lobe (M = 0.183, SD = 0.256), and temporal lobe (M = 0.205, SD = 0.176). In addition, they displayed significantly greater alpha band activity in the right cerebral hemisphere (M = 0.418, SD = 0.271), further highlighting the unique neural patterns linked with divergent thinking. The empirical results indicate that mind mapping training is an effective method for enhancing students’ divergent thinking skills.
... In recent years, researchers have increasingly utilised non-embedded EEG detection devices to investigate brain activity patterns during creative activities (Saggar et al., 2021;Stevens & Zabelina, 2020). Empirical evidence suggests a strong correlation between creativity and heightened attentional focus in the user's brain, with this concentrated cognitive state enabling more effective processing of relevant information (Frith et al., 2021;Takeuchi et al., 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
Contemporary technological advancements offer new possibilities for enhancing user creativity. We aimed to explore how technology can boost student creativity to meet the twenty-first century’s demand for innovative talent. Based on the 4P model of creativity (person, process, product, and press) and constructivist theory, a virtual reality (VR) creative enhancement system was designed, developed, and evaluated. The study recruited 47 participants, randomly assigned to either the experimental group (23 participants) using a VR system with haptic vibration feedback via electroencephalography (EEG) or the control group (24 participants) using a standard VR system. Participants’ creative performance, attention level, motivation, and cognitive load were assessed. The data were analysed using analysis of variance (ANOVA) as the statistical approach. The findings revealed that participants in the experimental group exhibited superior outcomes compared with those in the control group in terms of creative performance, attention level, and cognitive load. However, no significant difference was observed in motivation, with the control group scoring slightly higher. Our findings suggest that changing the learning environment, improving attention, reducing cognitive load, and setting up activities involving invention can effectively enhance students’ creative performance. This study provides a new technologically supported approach to creativity education, with potential applications for cultivating innovative talent in higher education.
... The curriculum's innovative edge is further enhanced by the integration of a NeuroDesign approach, merging cognitive neuroscience with EDT principles to explore the neural foundations of learning and creativity [14][15][16]. This transdisciplinary approach optimizes teaching methods by aligning educational experiences with natural learning processes. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
This study explores the integration of a NeuroDesign approach with an Empathy Design Thinking (EDT) curriculum to enhance creativity and empathy among primary school students in China. Utilizing functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), the research examines the neuroscientific enhancement of educational practices. Quantitative analysis indicated trends toward improved cognitive performance, although these were not statistically significant. In contrast, qualitative data from classroom observations and interviews demonstrated notable improvements in students' empathy and creative abilities. These findings underscore the potential of NeuroDesign to significantly enrich primary education by cultivating key 21st-century skills, setting a robust groundwork for further exploration in this emergent field.
... These platforms enable students to engage in experiential learning experiences that are both engaging and educational. The investigations of Saggar et al. (2017Saggar et al. ( , 2021 into the neuroscientific underpinnings of learning environments underscore the profound impact such environments can have on activating brain regions associated with creativity, offering robust scientific support for the methodologies employed within the EDT framework. This research emphasizes the significance of engaging and empathetic learning experiences in stimulating creative thought processes, highlighting its effectiveness in fostering an educational environment that fosters creativity. ...
Article
Full-text available
This research explores the application of an Empathy Design Thinking (EDT) curriculum within primary education, guided by the principles of Experience, Empathy, Exploration, and Evaluation, to examine its effect on fostering student creativity in a Chinese context. The curriculum was redesigned into a streamlined, modular format for ease of integration into existing educational frameworks. Findings demonstrate a positive impact on students' creative thinking abilities, particularly in enhancing fluency and flexibility, with notable mentions of empathy's vital role in the educational process. This study aligns with global educational trends emphasizing the need for empathetic and comprehensive learning experiences, offering insights into the potential of EDT to enrich creative education for primary students, educators, curriculum developers, and policymakers.
... Sin embargo, es posible conocer la neurotecnología de medios digitales y artes mediales para proporcionar nuevas perspectivas sobre la experiencia humana y la interacción con el arte multimedial, tales como los instrumentos tecnológicos de investigación en neurociencia. Algunas técnicas de diagnóstico modernas como PET (tomografía por emisión de positrones), MRI (imagen por resonancia magnética), fTCD (ultrasonido Doppler transcraneal funcional) y fMRI (imagen por resonancia magnética funcional) han permitido identificar la observación en diferentes tipos de composiciones y artísticos, así como la interacción de la actividad motora, sensorial, cognitiva y emocional para dominar el hemisferio cerebral, lo que ha llevado a posibilidades de utilizar la música, la pintura y la danza como ayuda en el tratamiento de trastornos somáticos, neurológicos o psiquiátricos (Bosnar-Puretić, et al., 2009;Chatterjee, 2011;Dale, et al., 2007;Horváth, 2018;Lundy, et al, 2022;Saggar et al,, 2021). Esto se transforma el neuroarte. ...
Article
Full-text available
El presente ensayo se indaga en el campo interdisciplinario que combina la neurotecnología y la investigación artística para incorporar las obras actuales de la última generación. El objetivo general es explorar la conexión entre el cerebro y la experiencia humana a través del arte de los nuevos medios, utilizando una metodología basada en la revisión documental y el análisis de obras de arte contemporáneo en el ámbito del neuroarte y las artes mediales. La característica clave de la neurotecnología en artes mediales, otorgando importancia a la relación entre la obra de arte y el cerebro humano. La neurotecnología en las artes mediales se basó en algunos resultados de obras de arte de Dikker y Oostrik, y Lieberman. Se puede tener un impacto significativo en la comprensión del cerebro y la mente. En conclusión, es fundamental para mejorar la creación y la colaboración interdisciplinaria.
... Toward that objective, the present paper was developed as a component of ongoing efforts of The Society for The Neuroscience of Creativity via the SfNC Ontology Initiative (e.g., Kenett et al., 2020, Saggar, Volle, Uddin, Chrysikou, & Green, 2021. This initiative seeks to engage theory alongside neural and cognitive data to achieve greater clarity in the ontology of creativity. ...
Article
The “standard” definition of creativity as novel and useful describes creative products, but creativity is constituted by processes. This misalignment contributes to the oft-noted challenges of operationalizing creativity. Here, we distinguish creativity as a process from creativity as an attribute (i.e. “creative-ness”). Operating from a priori premises of creativity theory, we develop a process definition of creativity. Specifically, creativity is defined as internal attention constrained by a generative goal. This definition comprises three criteria: 1) attention is directed internally (toward mental representations); 2) attentional operations (e.g. search, manipulation) are constrained to fit parameters of a to-be-achieved goal state (whether or not a goal is actually achieved); and 3) the goal state is generative (not already precisely held in memory). We illustrate how these three definitional process elements align with insights yielded by creativity neuroscience, clarify ontological distinctions (e.g. from mind-wandering and retrieval), and relate the process definition to process-based models. The process definition provides minimal necessity and sufficiency criteria for whether a process should be considered creativity, but does not exclude the many other perceptual, emotional, etc. elements that can contribute to creativity processes. Researchers should specify whether they are studying creativity-as-process vs. “creative-ness,” and consider including process-focused assessments.
Article
Coming up with creative ideas is not easy. In this conceptual article, we integrate organizational behavior, cognitive psychology, and neuroscience literatures to propose that different forms of attention may be a key to maximizing creative usefulness and novelty. Specifically, we develop a neurocognitive framework of attentional control to propose differential pathways from creative goal‐directed attention (a narrow and selective focus) to deliberate information processing, and from undirected attention (a wide and unconstrained focus) to spontaneous information processing. These propositions have implications for creative usefulness and novelty, respectively—namely, that creative goal‐directed attention should facilitate the usefulness of creative outputs to a greater extent than their novelty, whereas undirected attention should promote the novelty of creative outputs to a greater extent than their usefulness. Our framework further suggests that time spent experiencing creative goal‐directed attention followed by undirected attention is the optimal sequence for maximizing both the usefulness and novelty of creative outputs. In combination, our framework advances theoretical understanding of attentional pathways to creative outcomes and offers practical implications for maximizing creative potential at work.
Article
Full-text available
Creative cognition is the driving force behind all cultural and scientific progress. In recent years, the field of neurocognitive creativity research (NCR) has made considerable progress in revealing the neural and psychological correlates of creative cognition. However, a detailed understanding of how cognitive processes produce creative ideas, and how these processes interact differently across tasks and individuals, remains elusive. In this article, we argue that the increased adoption of computational modeling can help greatly in achieving this goal. While the verbal theories guiding NCR have evolved from broader accounts into more specific descriptions of neurocognitive processes, they remain more open to interpretation and harder to falsify than formal models. Translating theories into computational models can make them more concrete, accessible, and easier to compare, and helps researchers to develop causal hypotheses for how variation in cognitive factors leads to variation in creative outcomes. Currently, however, computational modeling of creativity is conducted almost entirely separately from NCR, and few attempts have been made to embody the cognitive theories of NCR in models that can simulate performance on common lab-based tasks. In this article, we discuss theories of creative cognition and how they might benefit from the wider adoption of formal modeling. We also examine recent computational models of creativity and how these might be improved and better integrated with NCR. Finally, we describe a pathway toward a mechanistic understanding of creative cognition through the integration of computational modeling, psychological theory, and empirical research, outlining an example model based on dual-process accounts.
Article
Full-text available
While a recent upsurge in the application of neuroimaging methods to creative cognition has yielded encouraging progress toward understanding the neural underpinnings of creativity, the neural basis of barriers to creativity are as yet unexplored. Here, we report the first investigation into the neural correlates of one such recently identified barrier to creativity: anxiety specific to creative thinking, or creativity anxiety (Daker et al., 2019). We employed a machine-learning technique for exploring relations between functional connectivity and behavior (connectome-based predictive modeling; CPM) to investigate the functional connections underlying creativity anxiety. Using whole-brain resting-state functional connectivity data, we identified a network of connections or “edges” that predicted individual differences in creativity anxiety, largely comprising connections within and between regions of the executive and default networks and the limbic system. We then found that the edges related to creativity anxiety identified in one sample generalize to predict creativity anxiety in an independent sample. We additionally found evidence that the network of edges related to creativity anxiety were largely distinct from those found in previous work to be related to divergent creative ability (Beaty et al., 2018). In addition to being the first work on the neural correlates of creativity anxiety, this research also included the development of a new Chinese-language version of the Creativity Anxiety Scale, and demonstrated that key behavioral findings from the initial work on creativity anxiety are replicable across cultures and languages.
Article
Full-text available
Divergent thinking tests have been used extensively in neuroscientific studies of creativity. However, output from tests of divergent thinking can be scored in different ways, and those scores can influence assessments of divergent thinking performance and its relationship with brain activation. Here we sought to investigate the relationship between various methods of scoring the Alternate Uses Task (AUT)—a well-known test of divergent thinking—and regional grey matter volume (GMV) using voxel-based morphometry (VBM). We assessed AUT performance based on (a) traditional approaches that involve scoring participants’ output on fluency, flexibility, originality, and elaboration, (b) a subjective approach that involves scoring output directly on “snapshot” creativity, and (c) the definitional approach that involves scoring output separately on novelty and usefulness—the two criteria deemed necessary and jointly sufficient to categorize an idea as creative. Correcting for age, sex, intracranial volume, verbal IQ and working memory capacity, we found negative correlations between regional GMV in the left inferior temporal gyrus (ITG) and novelty and usefulness scores, but no correlation involving other scoring approaches. As part of the brain's core semantic system, this region is involved in concept retrieval and integration. We discuss the implications of these findings for our understanding of the neural bases of divergent thinking, and how ITG could be related to the generation of novel and useful responses.
Article
Full-text available
A central challenge for creativity research—as for all areas of experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience—is to establish a mapping between constructs and measures (i.e., identifying a set of tasks that best captures a set of creative abilities). A related challenge is to achieve greater consistency in the measures used by different researchers; inconsistent measurement hinders progress toward shared understanding of cognitive and neural components of creativity. New resources for aggregating neuroimaging data, and the emergence of methods for identifying structure in multivariate data, present the potential for new approaches to address these challenges. Identifying meta-analytic structure (i.e., similarity) in neural activity associated with creativity tasks might help identify subsets of these tasks that best reflect the similarity structure of creativity-relevant constructs. Here, we demonstrated initial proof-of-concept for such an approach. To build a model of similarity between creativity-relevant constructs, we first surveyed creativity researchers. Next, we used NeuroSynth meta-analytic software to generate maps of neural activity robustly associated with tasks intended to measure the same set of creativity-relevant constructs. A representational similarity analysis-based approach identified particular constructs—and particular tasks intended to measure those constructs—that positively or negatively impacted the model fit. This approach points the way to identifying optimal sets of tasks to capture elements of creativity (i.e., dimensions of similarity space among creativity constructs), and has long-term potential to meaningfully advance the ontological development of creativity research with the rapid growth of creativity neuroscience. Because it relies on neuroimaging meta-analysis, this approach has more immediate potential to inform longer-established fields for which more extensive sets of neuroimaging data are already available.
Article
Full-text available
Resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) between various brain regions is thought to be associated with creative abilities. Extensive research correlating RSFC with performance on creativity tasks has revealed some of the RSFC patterns characterizing ‘the creative brain’. Yet, our understanding of the neurocognitive processes underlying creative thinking still remains limited. This limitation results, in part, from the fact that standard creativity tasks used in these studies do not distinguish between the different modes of cognitive processing that are critical in creative cognition (e.g., spontaneous cognition vs. controlled cognition). In the present fMRI research we address this limitation by using a chain free association task − a task that we have recently refined and validated for the purpose of isolating measures of spontaneous cognition that are relevant for creative thinking (referred to as associative fluency and associative flexibility). In our study, 27 female participants completed standardized creativity tasks, a chain free association task, and an fMRI scan in which RSFC was measured. Our results indicate that higher scores on associative fluency are associated with stronger positive RSFC within the default mode network (DMN; i.e., between DMN regions). Critically, we provide evidence that the previously-identified relationship between performance on creativity tasks and connectivity within the DMN is partially mediated by associative fluency. Thus, our observations suggest that the heightened DMN connectivity observed in ‘the creative brain’ can be explained, at least to some extent, by spontaneous cognition. Overall, our study identifies unique RSFC patterns that are related specifically to spontaneous cognitive processes involved in creative ideation, thus shedding new light on mechanisms of creative processing.
Article
Full-text available
Increasing attention is being directed towards explicating the neurocognitive mechanisms of divergent thinking. While neuroimaging studies have tended to dominate the contemporary creativity literature, lesion studies provide important converging evidence by revealing the regions that are not only implicated in, but essential for, task performance. Here we explored the capacity for divergent thinking in semantic dementia (SD), a neurodegenerative disorder characterised by the progressive degeneration of the conceptual knowledge base. The performance of 10 SD patients on a divergent thinking task was contrasted with that of 15 patients with the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) and 20 healthy control participants. In addition, all participants underwent neuropsychological testing and structural MRI. Relative to controls, both patient groups generated significantly fewer responses on the divergent thinking task, with disproportionate impairment in the SD group. Further, the responses generated by patient groups were less original and reflected less flexible thinking when compared with controls. For SD patients, fluency of responses correlated with performance on a measure of semantic association, and originality of responses correlated with semantic naming and comprehension ability. In bvFTD, originality of ideas correlated with letter fluency and response inhibition. Voxel-based morphometry analyses revealed two grey matter clusters consistently associated with diminished Fluency of ideas, namely a left medial temporal lobe cluster centred on the left anterior hippocampus, and a left middle frontal gyrus cluster. Our study highlights the importance of distinct temporal and prefrontal contributions to divergent thinking via a lesion approach, and underscores the pivotal role of semantic processes in creative cognition.
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to examine how communication modes affect creative idea generation in groups. Three communication mode conditions were created: natural (N), turn-taking (T), and electronic brainstorming (E). Participants were randomly recruited and grouped in dyads to solve one alternative uses task (AUT) in each condition, during which functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS)-based hyperscanning was used to record interpersonal neural responses. No difference was observed in AUT fluency across the three conditions, but AUT uniqueness was higher in the T condition than in the E condition. In addition, AUT uniqueness, AUT fluency, and perspective-taking behaviours increased faster in the T condition than in the other conditions. The T condition also showed higher perspective-taking behaviours than did the other conditions. Moreover, fNIRS data showed higher interpersonal brain synchronisation (IBS) increments at the right angular gyrus in the T condition than in the other conditions, which positively predicted perspective-taking behaviours between individuals during group creativity tasks. These findings indicate that when group members create together while taking turns, both creative performance and interpersonal interaction processes can be stimulated.
Article
Full-text available
An influential model of the neural mechanisms of creative thought suggests that creativity is manifested in the joint contributions of the Default Mode Network (DMN; a set of regions in the medial PFC, lateral and medial parietal cortex, and the medial temporal lobes) and the executive networks within the dorsolateral PFC. Several empirical reports have offered support for this model by showing that complex interactions between these brain systems account for individual differences in creative performance. The present study examined whether the engagement of these regions in idea generation is modulated by one’s eminence in a creativity-related field. Twenty (n = 20) healthy eminent creators from diverse fields of expertise and a ‘smart’ comparison group of sixteen (n = 16) age- and education-matched non-eminent thinkers were administered a creative generation task (an adaptation of the Alternative Uses Task) and a control perceptual task, while undergoing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The participants’ verbal responses were recorded through a noise-canceling microphone and were later coded for fluency and accuracy. Behavioral and fMRI analyses revealed commonalities between groups, but also distinct patterns of activation in default mode and executive brain regions between the eminent and the non-eminent participants during creative thinking. We interpret these findings in the context of the well-documented contributions of these regions in the generation of creative ideas as modulated, in this study, by participants’ creative eminence.
Article
Full-text available
Prior research has shown that greater EEG alpha power (8-13 Hz) is characteristic of more creative individuals, and more creative task conditions. The present study investigated the potential for machine learning to classify more and less creative brain states. Participants completed an Alternate Uses Task, in which they thought of Normal or Uncommon (more creative) uses for everyday objects (e.g., brick). We hypothesized that alpha power would be greater for Uncommon (vs. Common) uses, and that a machine learning (ML) approach would enable the reliable classification data from the two conditions. Further, we expected that ML would be successful at classifying more (vs. less) creative individuals. As expected, alpha power was significantly greater for the Uncommon than for the Normal condition. Using spectrally weighted common spatial patterns to extract EEG features, and quadratic discriminant analysis, we found that classification accuracy for the two conditions varied widely among individuals, with a mean of 63.9%. For more vs. less creative individuals, 82.3% classification accuracy was attained. These findings indicate the potential for broader adoption of machine learning in creativity research.
Article
Despite substantial progress in the quest of demystifying the brain basis of creativity, several questions remain open. One such issue concerns the relationship between two latent cognitive modes during creative thinking, i.e., deliberate goal-directed cognition and spontaneous thought generation. Although an interplay between deliberate and spontaneous thinking is often implicated in the creativity literature (e.g., dual-process models), a bottom-up data-driven validation of the cognitive processes associated with creative thinking is still lacking. Here, we attempted to capture the latent modes of creative thinking by utilizing a data-driven approach on a novel continuous multitask paradigm (CMP) that widely sampled a hypothetical two-dimensional cognitive plane of deliberate and spontaneous thinking in a single fMRI session. The CMP consisted of eight task blocks ranging from undirected mind wandering to goal-directed working memory task, while also included two widely-used creativity tasks, i.e., alternate uses task (AUT) and remote association task (RAT). Using eigen-connectivity (EC) analysis on the multitask whole-brain functional connectivity (FC) patterns, we embedded the multitask FCs into a low-dimensional latent space. The first two latent components, as revealed by the EC analysis, broadly mapped onto the two cognitive modes of deliberate and spontaneous thinking, respectively. Further, in this low-dimensional space, both creativity tasks were located in the upper right corner of high deliberate and spontaneous thinking (creative cognitive space). Neuroanatomically, the creative cognitive space was represented by not only increased intra-network connectivity within executive control and default mode network, but also by higher coupling between the two canonical brain networks. Further, individual differences reflected in the low-dimensional connectivity embeddings were related to differences in deliberate and spontaneous thinking abilities. Altogether, using a continuous multitask paradigm and a data-driven approach, we provide initial empirical evidence for the contribution of both deliberate and spontaneous modes of cognition during creative thinking.
Article
Cognitive and behavioural flexibility permit the appropriate adjustment of thoughts and behaviours in response to changing environmental demands. Brain mechanisms enabling flexibility have been examined using non-invasive neuroimaging and behavioural approaches in humans alongside pharmacological and lesion studies in animals. This work has identified large-scale functional brain networks encompassing lateral and orbital frontoparietal, midcingulo-insular and frontostriatal regions that support flexibility across the lifespan. Flexibility can be compromised in early-life neurodevelopmental disorders, clinical conditions that emerge during adolescence and late-life dementias. We critically evaluate evidence for the enhancement of flexibility through cognitive training, physical activity and bilingual experience.