Content uploaded by Patti M. Valkenburg
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Patti M. Valkenburg on Apr 30, 2022
Content may be subject to copyright.
Social Media Browsing and Adolescent
Well-Being: Challenging the “Passive Social Media
Use Hypothesis”
Patti M. Valkenburg
1
, Ine Beyens
1
, J. Loes Pouwels
1
, Irene I. van Driel
1
,&
Loes Keijsers
2
1
Amsterdam School of Communication Research, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands
2
Department of Psychology, Education, and Child Studies, Erasmus University, The Netherlands
A recurring hypothesis in the literature is that “passive” social media use (browsing) leads to neg-
ative effects on well-being. This preregistered study investigated a rival hypothesis, which states
that the effects of browsing on well-being depend on person-specific susceptibilities to envy, inspi-
ration, and enjoyment. We conducted a three-week experience sampling study among 353 adoles-
cents (13–15 years, 126 assessments per adolescent). Using an advanced N¼1 method of analy-
sis, we found sizeable heterogeneity in the associations of browsing with envy, inspiration, and
enjoyment (e.g., for envy ranging from b¼".44 to b¼þ.71). The Passive Social Media Use
Hypothesis was confirmed for 20% of adolescents and rejected for 80%. More adolescents with
browsing-induced envy experienced negative effects on affective well-being (25%) than adolescents
with no browsing-induced envy (13%). Conversely, more adolescents with browsing-induced en-
joyment experienced positive effects on affective well-being (47%) than adolescents with no
browsing-induced enjoyment (9%).
Lay Summary
‘A recurrent question among academics is whether social media browsing negatively affects teens’
well-being. Some scholars believe that this browsing can lead to envy and declines in well-being.
Others think that it can also lead to inspiration and enjoyment, and to increases in well-being.
To investigate which scholars are right, we asked 353 teens to report six times a day for a period
of three weeks how often they had browsed Instagram and Snapchat in the past hour.
Furthermore, we asked them how much envy, inspiration, and enjoyment they had experienced
in that hour, and how well they felt at that moment. We found sizeable differences among teens
in the effects of browsing on well-being at that moment. While one group of teens (20%) felt worse
after browsing, another group (17%) felt better. More teens who had felt envy while browsing, ex-
perienced negative effects on well-being than teens who did not feel such envy while browsing.
Conversely, more teens who had felt enjoyment while browsing, experienced positive effects on
well-being than teens who had not felt such enjoyment.
Corresponding author: Patti M. Valkenburg; e-mail: p.m.valkenburg@uva.nl
Editorial Record: First manuscript received on 8 January 2021; Revisions received on 4 November 2020; 12 May 2021;
Accepted by Andrew Flanagin on 9 August 2021; Final manuscript received on 3 February 2021
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19 V
CThe Author(s) 2021. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf
of International Communication Association. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons
Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in
any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
1
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021
Open Science Framework awards
Open Materials
The components of the research methodology needed to reproduce the reported procedure and analy-
sis are publicly available for this article.
Open Data
Digitally shareable data necessary to reproduce the reported results are publicly available for this
article.
Preregistered
Research design was preregistered.
The past decade has witnessed a burgeoning empirical literature into the relation of adolescents’ social
media (SM) use with indicators of well-being, such as life satisfaction (Orben et al., 2019), depressed
mood (Frison & Eggermont, 2017), or affective well-being (Beyens et al., 2020). In addition, several
narrative reviews have attempted to integrate the results of these studies (e.g., Dienlin & Johannes,
2020;Verduyn et al., 2017). An important hypothesis forwarded in these reviews is that passive SM
use may lead to decreases in well-being, because it induces upward social comparison and envy,
whereas active SM use may elicit positive feedback and social connectedness, which subsequently lead
to increases in well-being (Verduyn et al., 2017,2020).
Although appealing at first sight, these opposite hypotheses for active and passive SM use create a
dichotomy that has long been challenged by communication theories (e.g., Hall, 1980). In fact, both
selective exposure and media effects theories reject the notion of “passive” media use. These theories
consider the reception of media messages as active, in the sense that individual recipients have auton-
omy over the way they select, process, and interpret media messages. These theories argue that (a)
people can only attend to a limited number of media messages out of the constellation of messages
that can potentially attract their attention, (b) they select media messages in response to psychologi-
cal, social, and situational antecedents that differ from person to person, and (c) because only those
messages they select have the potential to influence them, the effects of these messages also differ
from person to person (for a review, see Valkenburg et al., 2016).
The Passive Social Media Use Hypothesis is not only theoretically disputable, but also difficult to
verify empirically, at least among adolescents. Whereas several studies have indeed found small over-
all negative effects of browsing on well-being at the aggregate level (e.g., Escobar-Viera et al., 2018;
Orben & Przybylski, 2019), two recent studies adopting a person-specific within-person (N¼1) ap-
proach suggest that the Passive Social Media Use Hypothesis only holds for a small subset of youth.
In a first experience sampling (ESM) study by Beyens et al. (2020), only 10% of adolescents experi-
enced the hypothesized negative effect of passive use on affective well-being, whereas the remaining
adolescents experienced no effect or even a positive effect. In a subsequent study, which was based on
the same data set as used in the current study, Beyens et al. (2021) found that among 11% of adoles-
cents both active and passive use led to negative effects on affective well-being. Conversely, among
12% of adolescents both active and passive use led to positive effects on affective well-being. Perhaps
most remarkable, though, was their finding that less than one percent of adolescents experienced the
hypothesized positive effect of active use and the hypothesized negative effect of passive use on well-
being.
The results of the two studies of Beyens et al. (2020,2021)concurwiththeobservationsfrom
several authors who have recently criticized the active & passive dichotomy in SM research (e.g.,
Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being P. M. Valkenburg et al.
2Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021
Bayer et al., 2020;Ellison et al., 2020;Kross et al., 2020;Valkenburg et al., 2021). The aim of the cur-
rent study was to further build on the studies by Beyens et al., by focusing on the question why ado-
lescents may respond differently to passive SM use, hereafter called browsing. To do so, we
investigated a rival hypothesis, which argues that the positivity or negativity of the effects of brows-
ing on well-being does not depend on passive SM use per se, but on person-specific susceptibilities
to how SM messages are processed. In fact, we expected that three important mechanisms identified
in earlier studies may lead to differential effects of browsing on well-being: envy, inspiration, and
enjoyment (Krasnova et al., 2015;Meier et al., 2020). Specifically, we expected that some adoles-
cents are more susceptible than others to the negative effects of browsing on well-being, because
they experience more envy, less inspiration, and less enjoyment during browsing than their peers.
Conversely, we expected that other adolescents are more susceptible to the positive effects of brows-
ing on well-being, because they experience less envy, more inspiration, and more enjoyment during
browsing than their peers.
To investigate our rival hypothesis, we conducted an ESM study among 353 middle adolescents
(13–15 years), whom we surveyed six times a day for three weeks (126 measurements per person,
32,755 assessments in total). We investigated early and middle adolescents because this is the age
group that is especially prone to envy and to situationally induced fluctuations in well-being
(Maciejewski et al., 2015). We focused on the two platforms that Dutch adolescents most often use
for browsing: Instagram and Snapchat (van Driel et al., 2019). Although both platforms also afford
private interactions, in this study we focused on their affordance to browse (semi-)public posts and
stories of fellow users. Such (semi-)public posts and stories are subject to a “positivity bias”
(Reinecke & Trepte, 2014, p. 95), the tendency of SM users to share more positive messages and
emotions in (semi-)public SM interactions than in private SM interactions and in offline public
interactions (Reinecke & Trepte, 2014;Waterloo et al., 2018). Browsing such (semi-)public SM mes-
sages may provide adolescents with ample opportunity to experience envy, inspiration, and/or
enjoyment.
Like most earlier ESM studies on SM use (e.g., Bayer et al., 2018;Verduyn et al., 2015;
Wenninger et al., 2019), we investigated affective well-being as the outcome, defined as an adoles-
cent’s current feelings of happiness (e.g., Tov, 2018,hereaftercalledwell-being).Inaddition,wefo-
cused solely on browsing, firstly because the two recent studies of Beyens et al. (2020,2021)already
compared the effects of both active and passive SM use on affective well-being, and secondly, be-
cause envy and inspiration, two of our investigated mechanisms, are hypothesized to be induced by
browsing and not by active SM use. To capture adolescents’ person-specific susceptibilities to the
effects of browsing on envy, inspiration, enjoyment, and well-being, we employed Dynamic
Structural Equation Modeling (DSEM), an advanced method of analysis that combines the
strengths of N¼1 time-series analysis with multilevel and Structural Equation Modeling
(Asparouhov, Hamaker, & Muthe´n, 2018).
Social Media Browsing, Envy, and Well-Being
The Passive Social Media Use Hypothesis (e.g., Verduyn et al., 2017) is based on two successive mech-
anisms: It firstly assumes that browsing induces upward social comparison, which, subsequently, pro-
vokes envy. Envy is a painful and hostile emotion that arises when a person “lacks another’s superior
quality, achievement, or possession and either desires it or wishes that the other lacked it” (Parrott &
Smith, 1993, p. 908). In some SM studies, a distinction is made between benign and malicious envy
P. M. Valkenburg et al. Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19 3
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021
(e.g., Meier et al., 2020;Wu & Srite, 2021), while others have focused only on malicious envy (e.g., Lin
et al., 2018). Whereas benign envy motivates us to try to achieve similar achievements or possessions
as the person we envy, malicious envy makes us wish that the envied person lacks or does not deserve
these achievements or possessions (van de Ven et al., 2009). Because benign envy is strongly corre-
lated with inspiration (e.g., Meier et al., 2020), and because our aim was to compare the effects of SM-
induced envy on well-being with those of SM-induced inspiration and enjoyment, we focused on ma-
licious envy, hereafter simply called envy.
Some studies have investigated the indirect effect of browsing on envy, via upward social compari-
son. Most of these studies have yielded a positive indirect effect, meaning that browsing elicited upward
comparison, which in turn resulted in envy (e.g., Noon & Meier, 2019). Other studies have (only or
also) examined the direct association of browsing with envy, yielding a nonsignificant (e.g., Scherr et al.,
2019) or a positive effect (e.g., Wang et al., 2019) of browsing on envy. In the current study, we also fo-
cused on the direct effects of browsing on envy, which allowed us to compare these effects with those on
inspiration and enjoyment, which do not necessarily result from social comparison.
A possible explanation for the mixed findings in earlier studies into the effects of browsing on
envy may be that many prior studies have used suboptimal methods of analysis to establish these
effects. Most of these studies, including our own early work, have investigated the overall between-
person associations of browsing with envy (e.g., do frequent browsers report less envy than infre-
quent browsers). However, such between-person methods of analysis overlook an important propo-
sition of media effects theories, namely that media effects are conceptualized as intra-individual
changes in cognitions, emotions, or behavior within persons as a result of their media use
(Valkenburg et al., 2016). Therefore, the effects of browsing on envy (and any other outcome) could
best be investigated by identifying potential changes within persons due to their browsing.
In adjacent disciplines, it has been recognized for quite a while that changes due to individuals’
interactions with their environment should be conceptualized as an idiographic process, that is, a
within-person process that differs across individuals (Lerner & Lerner, 2019;Molenaar, 2004). Just
like in media effects research, in these disciplines it has long been assumed that nomothetic (i.e., ag-
gregate between-person) results are generalizable to idiographic (i.e., person-specific within-person)
results. However, more recently, it has been demonstrated that this assumption is only valid under
two very strict conditions that are highly improbable in the social sciences (for a discussion of these
conditions, see Molenaar, 2004;Molenaar & Campbell, 2009). It is therefore likely that the person-
specific within-person associations of browsing with envy and/or well-being are unrelated to or even
opposite to their analogue between-person associations. However, because the effects of SM involve
within-person changes, they should preferably be investigated by idiographic methods of analysis (cf.
Molenaar & Campbell, 2009).
Investigating the idiographic associations of browsing with envy is consistent with theories of
envy (Cohen-Charash, 2009), which argue that some individuals may experience no or little envy dur-
ing or after an envy-inducing experience, whereas others may experience much envy during or after
the same experience. It is also consistent with recent media effects studies that adopted an idiographic
approach, which have demonstrated that the effect of browsing on well-being differs strongly from
adolescent to adolescent (Beyens et al., 2020,2021). Based on envy theory (Cohen-Charash, 2009)
and these earlier results, it is conceivable that the association of browsing with envy also differs con-
siderably from adolescent to adolescent. Therefore, we investigated the following research question:
(RQ1). To what extent does the within-person association of adolescents’ browsing in the past
hour with envy in that hour differ from adolescent to adolescent?
Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being P. M. Valkenburg et al.
4Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021
While the Passive Social Media Use Hypothesis assumes that passive SM use leads to decreases in
well-being, our rival hypothesis argues that the effects of browsing on well-being depend on person-
specific susceptibilities to SM-induced envy. Based on this hypothesis, we expected that especially
adolescents who are susceptible to envy while browsing experience negative effects on well-being.
Moreover, while envy can be situationally induced during browsing, it has also been conceptualized
as a relatively stable disposition that differs across individuals (Smith & Kim, 2007). Based on situa-
tional (Cohen-Charash, 2009) and dispositional theories of envy (Smith & Kim, 2007), it is therefore
possible that the person-specific within-person effects of browsing on well-being do not only depend
on differences in SM-induced envy but also on differences in dispositional envy:
(H1). Adolescents with SM-induced envy (H1a) or higher dispositional envy (H1b) experi-
ence more negative within-person effects of browsing on well-being than adolescents without
SM-induced envy or lower dispositional envy.
Social Media Browsing, Inspiration, Enjoyment, and Well-Being
Although the conclusions of most reviews confirm the Passive Social Media Use Hypothesis, several
recent studies have demonstrated that browsing cannot only lead to envy, but also to inspiration
(Meier et al., 2020) and enjoyment (Krasnova et al., 2015), which can both lead to positive rather than
negative effects on well-being (Meier et al., 2020;Schueller & Seligman, 2010). In fact, SM-induced in-
spiration and enjoyment seem to be at least as common as SM-induced envy. For example, Krasnova
et al. (2015) found that college-aged Facebook users experienced more positive than negative emo-
tions in response to the self-presentation of their Facebook friends: About 25% experienced envy in
response to these self-presentations, whereas about half of them experienced pleasure and enjoyment,
and another 6% inspiration and admiration. It is therefore surprising that the literature on the effects
of browsing on inspiration and enjoyment is so much scarcer than that on envy.
Inspiration is an experience of enthusiasm that is often evoked spontaneously by someone or
something, and which may elicit a moment of clarity or a sudden insight (Thrash et al., 2014).
Although inspiration is not universally regarded as an emotion, it does involve emotions such as ad-
miration and awe (Thrash et al., 2014). Because inspiration is included in the Positive and Negative
Affect Scale (PANAS, Watson et al., 1988), it is assumed to fluctuate within persons. Although enjoy-
ment is also not universally viewed as an emotion, in our study we follow Ekman (1992), who consid-
ers enjoyment as one of the seven basic emotions. Based on Vorderer et al. (2004), we define
enjoyment as a pleasurable emotional response to an enjoyment-inducing message.
Like envy, inspiration (Thrash et al., 2014)andenjoyment(Watson et al., 1988)canbesituationally
induced as well as dispositional, and both types of inspiration and enjoyment can differ across individuals.
It is therefore conceivable that the effects of browsing on inspiration and enjoyment differ from adolescent
to adolescent, meaning that some adolescents experience more inspiration or enjoyment during browsing
than other adolescents do. Moreover, it is likely that the effects of browsing on well-being differ for adoles-
cents who vary in SM-induced and dispositional inspiration and enjoyment. Therefore, for both inspira-
tion and enjoyment, we investigated the same research question and hypothesis as we did for envy:
(RQ2). To what extent do the within-person associations of adolescents’ browsing in the past
hour with inspiration (RQ2a) and enjoyment in that hour (RQ2b) differ from adolescent to
adolescent?
(H2). Adolescents with SM-induced inspiration (H2a) or SM-induced enjoyment (H2b) and
with higher dispositional inspiration (H2c) or higher dispositional enjoyment (H2d)
P. M. Valkenburg et al. Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19 5
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021
experience more positive within-person effects of browsing on well-being than adolescents
without SM-induced inspiration or enjoyment and lower dispositional inspiration or
enjoyment.
Method
This preregistered (https://osf.io/eahrx/) ESM study is part of a larger project on the psychosocial
consequences of SM use for adolescents. It is based on a three-week ESM wave of this project, which
was fielded in December 2019. It builds upon an earlier study by Beyens et al. (2021), which used data
of the same ESM wave. Beyens et al. (2021) already demonstrated sizeable heterogeneity in the
within-person effects of browsing (i.e., named passive public SM use) on well-being. The current ESM
study extends that of Beyens et al. (2021) in that it focuses on the person-specific effects of browsing
on envy, inspiration, and enjoyment. Moreover, it investigates to what extent the person-specific
effects of browsing on well-being could be explained by SM-induced and dispositional envy, inspira-
tion, and enjoyment.
Data Availability
The data set underlying this study is available at Figshare: https://doi.org/10.21942/uva.13547441.
Participants
The sample of this study consisted of 353 Instagram and/or Snapchat users (58% girls, M
age
¼14.11,
SD ¼0.68) from a large secondary school in the southern part of the Netherlands. The initial sample
consisted of 387 early and middle adolescents. Because the current study focused on the effects of
Instagram and Snapchat browsing, it only included the 92% of adolescents who used these platforms
more than once a week. For more details about the sample and its representativeness, see Beyens et al.
(2021).
Sampling Scheme and Compliance
In total, participants received 126 ESM surveys (i.e., 21 days * 6 assessments a day) at random time
points within fixed intervals. Our notification scheme with the response windows is uploaded on the
Open Science Framework (OSF; https://osf.io/tbdjq/). On average, participants completed 92.79 ESM
surveys (SD ¼22.59). We reached a compliance rate of 74%, which is high in comparison with previ-
ous ESM studies among adolescents (van Roekel et al., 2019). For more details about our procedure,
monitoring and incentives plan, a priori power analyses, and methods of analysis, see Supplement 1
and Beyens et al. (2021).
Measures
Browsing
Browsing was measured by the following two items: “How much time in the past hour have you spent
viewing posts/stories of others on Instagram?” and “viewing stories of others on Snapchat?”.
Response options ranged from 0 to 60 min, with 1-minute intervals.
Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being P. M. Valkenburg et al.
6Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021
Well-being
In each ESM survey, adolescents were asked to respond to the question “How happy do you feel right
now?” using a 7-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 6 (completely) with 3 (a little) as the mid-
point. This single item has been reliably used in previous ESM studies and has high convergent valid-
ity (Beyens et al., 2020).
Envy, inspiration, and enjoyment
Envy, inspiration, and enjoyment were measured with the following three items: “How jealous of
someone did you feel in the past hour?” “How inspired by someone did you feel in the past hour?”
and “How much did you like what you did in the past hour?”. For each item, we used a 7-point scale
ranging from 0 (not at all) to 6 (completely), with 3 (a little) as the midpoint. SM-induced envy, inspi-
ration, and enjoyment were operationalized as the person-specific associations of browsing with envy,
inspiration, and enjoyment. Dispositional envy, inspiration, and enjoyment were operationalized as
the latent person means of the 126 ESM assessments. Emotions like envy and enjoyment are typically
fleeting, and therefore especially prone to recall bias (Diener, 1990). Therefore, investigating the aver-
age of momentary measures of these emotions may be more reliable and valid than one-shot trait sur-
vey measures (Fleeson, 2001).
In everyday Dutch language, no distinction is made between envy and jealousy, and certainly not
among middle adolescents (cf. Lennarz et al., 2017). Moreover, even in the top Dutch dictionary (Van
Dale), envy and jealousy are treated as synonyms. We therefore used the Dutch term for jealousy to
measure envy because this term is more ubiquitously used, and certainly in the context of SM
browsing.
Method of Analysis
We employed Dynamic Structural Equation Modeling (DSEM) for intensive longitudinal data in
Mplus Version 8.4 (Asparouhov et al., 2018). We first ran a concurrent DSEM model (Model 1) to in-
vestigate person-specific differences in SM-induced envy, inspiration, and enjoyment. At the within-
person level of this model, we modeled envy, inspiration, and enjoyment as the outcomes, and brows-
ing as the time-varying covariate (i.e., predictor). At the between-person level, we investigated the
associations between the latent person means of browsing with the latent person means of envy, inspi-
ration, and enjoyment. In addition, we added random slopes to investigate whether the person-
specific within-person associations of browsing with envy, inspiration, and enjoyment would differ
across adolescents (to investigate RQ1 and RQ2).
In a second DSEM model (Model 2), we modeled well-being as the outcome, controlled for the
autoregressive effect of well-being, and predicted by browsing as the time-varying covariate. To inves-
tigate the extent to which the person-specific effects of browsing on well-being depend on adolescents’
SM-induced envy, inspiration, and enjoyment (H1a, H2a, H2b) and their dispositional envy, inspira-
tion, and enjoyment (H1b, H2c, H2d), we included adolescents’ SM-induced envy, inspiration, and
enjoyment (i.e., the person-specific slopes of model 1), as well as their dispositional envy, inspiration,
and enjoyment (i.e., person-mean scores) as time-invariant covariates of the person-specific effects of
browsing on well-being. A more elaborate description of our analyses, including our inference criteria
for our within- and between-person effects, can be found in our preregistration and Supplement 1.
P. M. Valkenburg et al. Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19 7
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021
Results
Descriptives and Correlations
The range, means, standard deviations, within-person, between-person, and intraclass correlations of
all variables in the study are included in Supplement 2. As the Table in Supplement 2 shows, the
means for envy and inspiration were around 1.00 (range 0–6) and those for enjoyment and well-
being around 4.00 (range 0–6). Enjoyment was the most common emotion among participants.
During 94% of all ESM observations, adolescents experienced some enjoyment (rating $1), during
30% some envy, and during 33% some inspiration.
At both the between- and within-person level, browsing was positively associated with envy
(b: r¼.40, w: r¼.09) and inspiration (b: r¼.38, w: r¼.08), but not with enjoyment (b: r¼
".09, w: r¼.01). Contrary to expectations, envy and inspiration were positively related at both
the between- (r¼.86) and within-person level (r¼.40). Finally, enjoyment was negatively associ-
ated with envy at the between- and within-person level (b: r¼".26; w: r¼".07), and negatively
associated with inspiration at the between-person level (r¼".16), but positively at the within-
person level (r¼.07).
Investigating Person-Specific Effects (RQ1 and RQ2)
Our first DSEM model, with envy, inspiration, and enjoyment as the outcomes and browsing as the
predictor converged well before 5,000 iterations: The Potential Scale Reduction (PSR) value was
close to 1 (PSR ¼1.001). Table 1 presents the results of the DSEM analysis. Our first and second re-
search question asked to what extent the within-person effects of browsing on envy, inspiration,
and enjoyment would differ from adolescent to adolescent. The significant random effects of brows-
ing on envy, inspiration, and enjoyment revealed striking differences in person-specific effects. As
Figure 1 shows, the person-specific standardized effects (betas) ranged from b¼".44 to b¼þ.71
for envy, from b¼".45 to b¼þ.67 for inspiration, and from b¼".16 to b¼þ.20 for
enjoyment.
Explaining Person-Specific Effects (H1 and H2)
Our second DSEM model also converged well before 5,000 iterations (PSR ¼1.005). Table 2 summa-
rizes the results of the DSEM analysis with well-being as the outcome, browsing as the predictor, and
SM-induced and dispositional envy, inspiration, and enjoyment as the six hypothesized time-
invariant moderators. As Table 2 shows, our second DSEM model yielded a negative between-person
association of browsing with well-being (b¼".12), as has already been reported in Beyens et al.
(2021). This association meant that adolescents who spent more time on browsing than their peers
across the three weeks, reported lower average levels of well-being than these peers.
The model did not yield an overall within-person effect of browsing on well-being (b¼".004),
which was also already reported in (Beyens et al., 2021). This latter result meant that, overall, the time
adolescents spent on browsing in the previous hour did not lead to increases or decreases in momen-
tary well-being (compared to their average level of well-being). However, the significant random effect
indicated that the effect of browsing on well-being differed considerably from adolescent to adoles-
cent. The person-specific effects of browsing on well-being ranged from b¼".17 to b¼þ.27 (see
Figure 2, lower left histogram). Expressed in percentages (based on our preregistered inference
Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being P. M. Valkenburg et al.
8Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021
Table 1 Main Outcomes DSEM Model of Browsing With Envy, Inspiration, and Enjoyment
Envy: Fixed effects bbp95% CI
Within-person effect
Browsing !Envy (¼SM-I. envy)
.103 .079 .000 [ .063, .096]
Between-person associations
Browsing & Disp. Envy .327 .411 .000 [ .316, .496]
Browsing & SM-I. envy .000 ".003 .484 [".124, .120]
Disp. envy & SM-I. envy .012 .050 .222 [".075, .177]
Envy: Random effects r
2
p95% CI
Browsing !Envy (RQ1) 0.056 .000 [ 0.044, 0.072]
a
Inspiration: Fixed effects bbp95% CI
Within-person effect
Browsing !Inspiration (¼SM-I. inspiration) .113 .081 .000 [ .064, .098]
Between-person associations
Browsing & Disp. Inspiration .330 .388 .000 [ .293, .476]
Browsing & SM-I. inspiration ".007 ".035 .286 [".157, .089]
Disp. inspiration & SM-I. inspiration .003 .013 .421 [".112, .144]
Inspiration: Random effects r
2
p95% CI
Browsing !Inspiration (RQ2a) 0.063 .000 [ 0.048, 0.080]
a
Enjoyment: Fixed effects bbp95% CI
Within-person effect
Browsing !Enjoyment (SM-I. enjoyment) .023 .013 .045 [".002, .028]
Between-person associations
Browsing & Disp. Enjoyment ".061 ".094 .042 [".200, .013]
Browsing & SM-I. enjoyment ".003 ".026 .362 [".172, .121]
Disp. enjoyment & SM-I. enjoyment ".019 ".138 .074 [".310, .052]
Enjoyment: Random effects r
2
p95% CI
Browsing !Enjoyment (RQ2b) 0.027 .000 [ 0.018, 0.040]
a
Other between-person associations bbp95% CI
SM-I. envy & SM-I. inspiration .043 .723 .000 [ .609, .814]
SM-I. envy & SM-I. enjoyment ".009 ".227 .025 [".434, .000]
SM-I. enjoyment & SM-I. inspiration .000 ".001 .497 [".230, .230]
Notes:bs are unstandardized and bs are standardized betas; p-values are one-tailed Bayesian p-values
(McNeish & Hamaker, 2020); Significant effects are depicted in bold; SM-I.¼SM-induced;
Disp.¼Dispositional.
a
The 95%Credible Interval of the variances around the within-person effect of browsing on envy,
inspiration, and enjoyment signifies that these effects differed among adolescents.
P. M. Valkenburg et al. Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19 9
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021
criteria, see Supplement 1), we found that 20% of adolescents experienced a negative effect of brows-
ing on well-being (i.e., b<".05), whereas another 17% experienced a positive effect (i.e., b>.05).
Our first and second hypotheses predicted that the person-specific effects of browsing on well-
being would be more negative for adolescents with SM-induced envy (H1a) or higher dispositional
envy (H1b), and more positive for adolescents with SM-induced inspiration (H2a) and enjoyment
(H2b), and for adolescents with higher dispositional inspiration (H2c) or enjoyment (H2d). These hy-
potheses were partly supported. Contrary to hypotheses H1b, H2c, and H2d, dispositional envy, dis-
positional inspiration, and dispositional enjoyment could not explain the differences in person-
specific effects of browsing on well-being, and neither could SM-induced inspiration (H2a). This
meant that the effects of browsing on well-being did not depend on adolescents’ trait-like envy, inspi-
ration, and enjoyment, and neither on SM-induced inspiration.
The effects of browsing on well-being could be explained by SM-induced envy (H1a, b¼".13)
and SM-induced enjoyment (H2b, b¼.43). Although the credibility intervals for the moderating ef-
fect of SM-induced envy contained zero, the effect size was below the b%".10 threshold, and, there-
fore, according to our preregistered inference criteria, we interpreted it as a negative association.
Doing so enabled us to compare the small moderating effect of SM-induced envy with the strong one
of SM-induced enjoyment. As Figure 2 shows, more adolescents with SM-induced envy (i.e., a
person-specific within-person effect of b¼.05 or stronger) experienced negative effects of browsing
on well-being (25%, see upper right plot) than adolescents without SM-induced envy (13%, see upper
left plot). Conversely, more adolescents with SM-induced enjoyment experienced positive effects of
browsing on well-being (47%, lower right plot) than adolescents without SM-induced enjoyment (9%,
lower middle plot).
Figure 1 The ranges of the person-specific effect sizes of browsing on envy, inspiration, and
enjoyment.
Notes. The y-axes represent the number of adolescents; the x-axes represent the person-specific effect sizes of browsing
on envy, inspiration, and enjoyment, which ranged from b¼".44 to b¼þ.71 for envy (upper left plot), from b¼".45
to b¼þ.67 for inspiration (upper right plot), and from b¼-.16 to b¼þ.20 for enjoyment (bottom plot). The vertical
black lines are the average within-person effects of browsing on envy (b¼.08), inspiration (b¼.08), and enjoyment (b
¼.01).
Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being P. M. Valkenburg et al.
10 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021
Sensitivity Analysis
As preregistered, we conducted a sensitivity analysis to examine whether our findings were robust
against outliers and potentially untrustworthy answer patterns based on inconsistency of response
patterns, outliers, and unserious responses to the open question in the ESM study. These criteria per-
tained to five participants. We reran the DSEM analysis without these participants. The results did
not deviate from those of the full sample.
Discussion
The aim of this study was to extend the work of Beyens et al. (2020,2021) by investigating why ado-
lescents may respond differently to the effects of passive social media use, and why the Passive Social
Media Hypothesis may apply to only a minority of adolescents. By adopting an N¼1 time-series
Table 2 Main Outcomes DSEM Model of Browsing on Well-being With Envy, Inspiration, and
Enjoyment as N¼1 Moderators
Fixed effects bbp95% CI
Within-person effects
Browsing !Well-being (Beta) ".007 ".004 .319 [".018, .012]
Well-Being (t-1) !Well-being (t) (Phi) .278 .278 .000 [.264, .293]
Between-person associations
Browsing & Well-being ".101 ".119 .013 [".228, ".013]
Beta & SM-I. envy (H1a) ".002 ".134 .065 [".307, .038]
Beta & Disp. envy (H1b) ".009 ".069 .201 [".228, .088]
Beta & SM-I. inspiration (H2a) .000 .007 .472 [".166, .179]
Beta & Disp. inspiration (H2c) ".006 ".042 .312 [".205, .117]
Beta & SM-I. enjoyment (H2b) .003 .432 .000 [ .284, .564]
Beta & Disp. enjoyment (H2d) ".006 ".052 .291 [".228, .126]
Beta & Well-being ".026 ".183 .020 [".347, ".009]
Beta & Browsing ".004 ".042 .280 [".183, ".101]
Random effects r
2
p95% CI
Browsing !Well-being (Beta) 0.018 .000 [0.012, 0.026]
a
Well-Being (t"1) !Well-being (t) (Phi) 0.053 .000 [0.044, 0.064]
Variances Predictor and Outcome
Browsing (within-person) 0.717 .000 [0.706, 0.728]
Browsing (between-person) 0.653 .000 [0.562, 0.770]
Well-being (within-person, residual) 1.093 .000 [1.077, 1.112]
Well-being (between-person) 1.113 .000 [0.955, 1.300]
Notes:bs are unstandardized and bs are standardized betas; p-values are one-tailed Bayesian p-values;
Significant fixed effects are depicted in bold; SM-I.¼SM-induced; Disp.¼Dispositional.
a
The 95%Credible Interval of the variance around the effect of browsing on well-being (Beta) indi-
cates that the within-person effect of browsing on well-being differed among adolescents.
P. M. Valkenburg et al. Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19 11
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021
approach, we investigated firstly to what extent adolescents differed in their SM-induced envy, inspi-
ration, and enjoyment (RQ1 & RQ2), and, secondly, to what extent the effects of browsing on well-
being depended on these person-specific differences (H1 & H2). We indeed found remarkably strong
differences in the person-specific effects of SM browsing on envy, inspiration, and enjoyment (RQ1 &
RQ2, see Figure 1). Contrary to expectations, none of the dispositional variants of envy (H1b),
Figure 2 The ranges of the person-specific effects of browsing on well-being for all participants, and
for adolescents with SM-induced and no SM-induced envy and enjoyment.
Notes. The x-axes represent the person-specific effect sizes of browsing on well-being for all adolescents (lower left
plot), for adolescents with SM-induced envy (b>þ.05) and no SM-Induced envy (b¼%þ.05), and for adolescents with
SM-enjoyment and no SM-induced enjoyment. The effects of browsing on well-being ranged from b¼".17 to b¼þ.27
(lower left histogram). More adolescents with SM-induced envy experienced a negative effect of browsing on well-be-
ing (25%, upper right plot) than adolescents without SM-induced envy (13%, upper left plot). Conversely, more adoles-
cents with SM-induced enjoyment experienced positive effects of browsing on well-being (47%, lower right plot) than
adolescents without SM-induced enjoyment (9%, lower middle plot).
Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being P. M. Valkenburg et al.
12 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021
inspiration (H2c), and enjoyment (H2d) moderated the person-specific effects of browsing on well-
being, and neither did SM-induced inspiration (H2a). However, in support of H1a and H2b, adoles-
cents with SM-induced envy were more likely to experience negative effects of browsing on well-being
than their peers without SM-induced envy, whereas adolescents with SM-induced enjoyment were
more likely to experience positive effects of browsing on well-being than those who experienced no
enjoyment.
Comparing Between-Person and Within-Person Associations
In recent years, due to new methods of analysis, such as random-intercept cross-lagged panel models
and multi-level models, it has become possible to assess media effects as within-person processes that
differ across individuals (McNeish & Hamaker, 2020;Thomas et al., 2021). Therefore, the implica-
tions of the current study can best be understood by successively discussing and, when opportune,
comparing our between-person results, within-person results, and N¼1 moderation results. As for
the between-person results, we found a small negative association (b¼-.12) of browsing with well-
being, which is consistent with prior studies that also investigated between-person associations (e.g.,
Escobar-Viera et al., 2018;Pang, 2021) and the meta-analysis of Liu et al. (2019). This negative
between-person correlation meant that adolescents who spent more time on browsing across the
three-week period felt less happy than their peers who spent less time on browsing. At first sight, this
negative association seems in line with the Passive Social Media Use Hypothesis.
Establishing a between-person association of browsing with well-being is important, for example
if one wants to identify whether high or low SM users experience the lowest levels of well-being.
Between-person studies can reveal that adolescents differ from each other, both in well-being and
browsing, and that these differences are correlated. However, consistent with earlier studies (e.g.,
Beyens et al., 2020;Coyne et al., 2020) and the seminal account of Molenaar (2004), we indeed found
that the average within-person effect of browsing on well-being deviated from the overall between-
person association (b¼".004 vs. b¼".12). More importantly, our person-specific within-person
results showed that the Passive Social Media Use Hypothesis was rejected for 80% of adolescents.
In all, a comparison of our between-person and person-specific within-person results indicates
that the negative between-person associations of browsing with well-being reported in earlier studies,
including our own early work, must be interpreted in light of what they are: differences between per-
sons. Some caution is warranted when such between-person results are used to study the effects of
media, because media effects involve within-person processes that may differ across individuals
(Valkenburg et al., 2016).
N51 Moderation Approach
We introduced an N¼1 moderation approach in this study. An important advantage of such an ap-
proach is that it does not only enable researchers to demonstrate that the effects of SM on well-being
differ from person to person, but also that the conditions to explain these differences can differ from
person to person. Whereas traditional moderation approaches (e.g., by means of an interaction term)
can reveal group-level differences based on moderating variables (e.g., gender, age), our N¼1 moder-
ation approach can reveal for how many persons within these groups the hypothesized moderation ef-
fect holds (and does not hold).
Our N¼1 moderation approach has yielded unique insights into the effects of SM on well-being.
As reported, we found a small negative association of SM-induced envy and a strong positive associa-
tion of SM-induced enjoyment with the browsing-well-being effect. The small negative moderating
P. M. Valkenburg et al. Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19 13
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021
effect of SM-induced envy meant that 25% of adolescents with SM-induced envy experienced the hy-
pothesized negative effect of browsing on well-being, which is consistent with H1a. However, 75% of
adolescents with SM-induced envy experienced no or even positive effects of browsing on well-being.
The attentive reader could of course argue that these null or opposite effects may be due to the small
magnitude of this moderating effect. But remarkably, such opposite effects also applied to the strong
moderator, SM-induced enjoyment: 47% of adolescents with SM-induced enjoyment experienced the
hypothesized positive effects of browsing on well-being (H2b). However, 9% of adolescents with SM-
induced enjoyment experienced a negative effect of browsing on well-being, whereas 44% experienced
no effect. This means that even for a strong moderator a considerable percentage of respondents
within a presumedly homogenous subgroup experienced no or even opposite effects than
hypothesized.
Strengths, Limitations, and Avenues for Future Research
Our study was one of the first to respond to recent calls for “causal effect heterogeneity,” the proposi-
tion that individuals differ in how they respond to environmental influences (Bolger et al., 2019, p.
601). Most studies are still designed to answer questions about average group differences or average
associations between variables. But an additional important question is: “How many people in my
study behaved or responded in a manner consistent with theoretical expectation?” (Grice et al., 2020,
p. 1). Our causal effects heterogeneity approach allowed us to show that a seemingly established hy-
pothesis in the literature, the Passive Social Media Use Hypothesis, may be less universal than previ-
ously assumed. And it also allowed us to demonstrate for how many individuals weak and strong
moderation effects hold and do not hold.
Despite its strengths, our study also has weaknesses. Consistent with theories on inspiration
(Thrash et al., 2014)andaffect(Watson et al., 1988), we conceptualized inspiration as a positive
and envy as a negative experience/emotion. Because we conceptualized envy as malicious envy, it
should result in a negative association with inspiration. However, we found a counterintuitive pos-
itive correlation between envy and inspiration. Therefore, we cannot exclude the possibility that
some adolescents may have perceived envy as benign envy, which may explain why among 14% of
the adolescents, SM-induced envy led to a positive association of browsing on well-being. We also
cannot exclude the possibility that our participants failed to understand the Dutch translation of
inspired. Although the word “inspired” is included as an emotion in the PANAS, this may not nec-
essarily mean that Dutch adolescents understand the word “geı¨n spi ree rd” i n the s ame w ay as
English-speaking adolescents would understand the word inspired. Our inspiration item may have
also been too narrowly defined. We asked adolescents whether they were inspired by someone.But
inspiration can also result from other stimuli, such as aesthetic design or nature (Thrash et al.,
2014). The aesthetic visual content on Instagram and Snapchat can easily be a source of inspira-
tion. Future research may need to rethink whether and how to operationalize envy and inspiration
in an ESM study, and further explore our unexpected positive association between envy and
inspiration.
Affective Versus Cognitive Well-being. Like earlier ESM studies (e.g., Bayer et al., 2018;
Verduyn et al., 2015;Wenninger et al., 2019), we focused on affective well-being, and did not include
cognitive well-being measures, such as life satisfaction. Unfortunately, the trait-like capacities of cog-
nitive well-being measures render them less suitable to employ in intensive ESM studies. What is cru-
cial in any study design is to carefully match the pace in which a specific media effect takes place with
the measurement frequency of this effect. Affective well-being fluctuates both across short- (e.g.,
Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being P. M. Valkenburg et al.
14 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021
hourly) and longer-term time intervals (e.g., monthly, or yearly). Therefore, the effect of browsing on
affective well-being may occur both in the short- and longer-term. However, investigating effects of
SM use on cognitive well-being requires wider measurement intervals, because cognitive well-being
measures only show longer-term within-person changes (e.g., Dienlin & Johannes, 2020). Therefore,
what our field really needs are designs that combine the investigation of short- and longer-term
effects, because only such designs can teach us whether, how, when, and for whom potential short-
term media effects accumulate into longer-term effects.
Well-Being Versus Ill-Being.ArecentscopingreviewofValkenburg et al. (2021) found that
about half of the existing studies into the associations of browsing or active SM use focused on well-
being (e.g., life satisfaction, affective well-being, positive affect), whereas the other half focused on
ill-being (e.g., depression, depressed mood, negative affect). In a recent meta-analysis on SM use
and well-being, the measures of ill-being were reversed to collapse them with measures of well-
being (Liu et al., 2019, p. 267). However, it is progressively acknowledged that well-being is not sim-
ply the flip-side of ill-being (Ryff et al., 2006). A low level of ill-being does not necessarily imply a
high level of well-being, whereas a low level of well-being does not equal a high level of ill-being
(e.g., Ryff et al., 2006). Therefore, there is a need for future ESM studies to investigate and compare
the effects of SM use on affective well-being (e.g., happiness) with those on affective ill-being (e.g.,
sadness).
Heterogenous Content as an Explanation. Like all earlier survey and ESM studies on SM use
and well-being, our study investigated adolescents’ time spent on browsing (Valkenburg et al., 2021).
We did not investigate how SM content encountered during browsing may have played a role in the
sizeable heterogeneity in adolescents’ responses. Some adolescents may have more often browsed
cheerful content, such as funny memes, during the three-week ESM study whereas others may have
more often browsed negative content or harmful content. But differences in encountered content can-
not fully explain our results. After all, both adolescents and adults also (or even) display striking dif-
ferences in responses to the same experimental content or treatments (Bolger et al., 2019;Thomaes et
al., 2010). Therefore, future studies should try to measure both heterogeneity in content and heteroge-
neity in reception. In experiments, this can best be done by adopting a “causal effect heterogeneity
approach” as proposed by Bolger et al. (2019). In survey research, this can best be realized by linking
survey data to additional data collection methods, such as random screenshots (Reeves et al., 2019) or
SM data downloads (Boeschoten et al., 2020).
Conclusion
Along with the studies of Beyens et al. (2020,2021), the current study demonstrated that it is time to
reconsider the Passive Social Media Use Hypothesis (cf. Burnell et al., 2020;Ellison et al., 2020;Kross
et al., 2020;Valkenburg et al., 2021), by acknowledging that (a) passive use might not be the right
term to describe the variety of active intra-individual processes that may occur during browsing, and
(b) social media browsing may lead to positive, negative, or no effects among individuals, depending
on a variety of dispositional, developmental, social, and situational antecedents, mediators, and
moderators.
Acknowledgements
This preregistered study was funded by an NWO Spinoza Prize and a Gravitation grant (NWO Grant
024.001.003; Consortium on Individual Development) awarded to P.V. by the Dutch Research
P. M. Valkenburg et al. Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19 15
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021
Council (NWO). Additional funding was received from a VIDI grant (NWO VIDI Grant 452.17.011)
awarded to L.K.
Conflict of Interest
None of the authors have any conflicts of interest.
References
Asparouhov, T., Hamaker, E. L., & Muthe´n, B. (2018). Dynamic structural equation models.
Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 25(3), 359–388. 10.108
0/10705511.2017.1406803
Bayer, J. B., Ellison, N., Schoenebeck, S., Brady, E., & Falk, E. B. (2018). Facebook in context(s):
Measuring emotional responses across time and space. New Media & Society, 20(3), 1047–1067.
10.1177/1461444816681522
Bayer, J. B., Tri^
e
:u, P., & Ellison, N. B. (2020). Social media elements, ecologies, and effects. Annual
Review of Psychology, 71, 471–497. 10.1146/annurev-psych-010419-050944
Beyens, I., Pouwels, J. L., van Driel, I. I., Keijsers, L., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2020). The effect of
social media on well-being differs from adolescent to adolescent. Scientific Reports,10,10763.
10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7
Beyens, I., Pouwels, J. L., van Driel, I. I., Keijsers, L., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2021). Social media use and
adolescents’ well-being: Developing a typology of person-specific effect patterns. Communication
Research. 10.1177/00936502211038196
Boeschoten, L., Ausloos, J., Moeller, J., Araujo, T., & Oberski, D. L. (2020). [Pre-print] Digital trace
data collection through data donation. arXiv preprint arXiv:2011.09851.
Bolger, N., Zee, K., Rossignac-Milon, M., & Hassin, R. (2019). Causal processes in psychology are
heterogeneous. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,148,601–618.https://doi.org/10.
1037/xge0000558
Burnell, K., George, M. J., & Underwood, M. K. (2020). Browsing different Instagram profiles and
associations with psychological well-being. Frontiers in Human Dynamics,2,6.https://doi.org/
10.3389/fhumd.2020.585518
Cohen-Charash, Y. (2009). Episodic envy. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 39(9), 2128–2173.
10.1111/j.1559-1816.2009.00519.x
Coyne, S. M., Rogers, A. A., Zurcher, J. D., Stockdale, L., & Booth, M. (2020). Does time spent using so-
cial media impact mental health? An eight year longitudinal study. Computers in Human Behavior,
104, Article 106160. 10.1016/j.chb.2019.106160
Diener, E. (1990). Memory accuracy in the recall of emotions [10.1037/0022-3514.59.2.291]. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 291–297. 10.1037/0022-3514.59.2.291
Dienlin, T., & Johannes, N. (2020). The impact of digital technology use on adolescent well-being.
Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience,22(2),135–142.10.31887/DCNS.2020.22.2/tdienlin
Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 6(3–4), 169–200.
https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939208411068
Ellison, N. B., Tri^
e
:u, P., Schoenebeck, S., Brewer, R., & Israni, A. (2020). Why we don’t click: Interrogating
the relationship between viewing and clicking in social media contexts by exploring the “non-click”.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication,25(6),402–426.https://doi.org/10.1093/jcmc/zmaa013
Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being P. M. Valkenburg et al.
16 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021
Escobar-Viera, C. G., Shensa, A., Bowman, N. D., Sidani, J. E., Knight, J., James, A. E., & Primack, B. A. (2018).
Passive and active social media use and depressive symptoms among United States adults. Cyberpsychology,
Behavior, and Social Networking,21(7),437–443.https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2017.0668
Fleeson, W. (2001). Toward a structure-and process-integrated view of personality: Traits as density distri-
butions of states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,80(6),1011.https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-
3514.80.6.1011
Frison, E., & Eggermont, S. (2017). Browsing, posting, and liking on Instagram: The reciprocal rela-
tionships between different types of Instagram use and adolescents’ depressed mood.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 20(10), 603–609. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.
2017.0156
Grice, J. W., Medellin, E., Jones, I., Horvath, S., McDaniel, H., O’lansen, C., & Baker, M.
(2020). Persons as effect sizes. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science.
10.1177/2515245920922982
Hall, S. (1980). Encoding/decoding. In S. Hall, D. Hobson, A. Lowe, & P. Willis (Eds.), Culture, media,
language: Working papers in cultural studies (pp. 128–138). London: Hutchinson.
Krasnova, H., Widjaja, T., Buxmann, P., Wenninger, H., & Benbasat, I. (2015). Why following friends can
hurt you: An exploratory investigation of the effects of envy on social networking sites among
college-age users. Information Systems Research,26(3),585–605.https://doi.org/10.1287/isre.2015.0588
Kross, E., Verduyn, P., Sheppes, G., Costello, C. K., Jonides, J., & Ybarra, O. (2020). Social media
and well-being: Pitfalls, progress, and next steps. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.tics.2020.10.005
Lennarz, H. K., Lichtwarck-Aschoff, A., Finkenauer, C., & Granic, I. (2017). Jealousy in adolescents’
daily lives: How does it relate to interpersonal context and well-being? Journal of Adolescence, 54,
18–31. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.09.008
Lerner, R. M., & Lerner, J. V. (2019). An idiographic approach to adolescent research: Theory, method,
and application. In L. B. Hendry & M. Kloep (Eds.), Reframing Adolescent Research (pp. 25–38).
Routledge.
Lin, R., van de Ven, N., & Utz, S. (2018). What triggers envy on Social Network Sites? A comparison
between shared experiential and material purchases. Computers in Human Behavior, 85, 271–281.
10.1016/j.chb.2018.03.049
Liu, D., Baumeister, R. F., Yang, C.-C., & Hu, B. (2019). Digital communication media use and psycho-
logical well-being: A meta-analysis. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 24, 259–274.
10.1093/ccc/zmz013
Maciejewski, D. F., van Lier, P. A. C., Branje, S. J. T., Meeus, W. H. J., & Koot, H. M. (2015). A 5-Year
longitudinal study on mood variability across adolescence using daily diaries. Child Development,
86(6), 1908–1921. 10.1111/cdev.12420
McNeish, D., & Hamaker, E. L. (2020). A primer on two-level dynamic structural equation models for in-
tensive longitudinal data in Mplus. Psychological Methods,25(5),610–635.https://doi.org/10.1037/
met0000250
Meier, A., Gilbert, A., Bo¨rner, S., & Possler, D. (2020). Instagram inspiration: How upward comparison
on social network sites can contribute to well-being. Journal of Communication, 70(5), 721–743.
10.1093/joc/jqaa025
Molenaar, P. C. M. (2004). A manifesto on psychology as idiographic science: Bringing the person
back into scientific psychology, this time forever. Measurement 2(4), 201–218. 10.1207/s1
5366359mea0204_1
P. M. Valkenburg et al. Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19 17
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021
Molenaar, P. C. M., & Campbell, C. G. (2009). The new person-specific paradigm in psychology. Current
Directions in Psychological Science,18(2),112–117.10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01619.x
Noon, E. J., & Meier, A. (2019). Inspired by friends: Adolescents’ network homophily moderates the re-
lationship between social comparison, envy, and inspiration on Instagram. Cyberpsychology,
Behavior, and Social Networking, 22(12), 787–793. 10.1089/cyber.2019.0412
Orben, A., Dienlin, T., & Przybylski, A. K. (2019). Social media’s enduring effect on adolescent life sat-
isfaction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(21), 10226–10228. https://doi.org/
10.1073/pnas.1902058116
Orben, A., & Przybylski, A. K. (2019). The association between adolescent well-being and digital tech-
nology use. Nature Human Behaviour. 10.1038/s41562-018-0506-1
Pang, H. (2021). Unraveling the influence of passive and active WeChat interactions on upward social
comparison and negative psychological consequences among university students. Telematics and
Informatics, 57, 101510. 10.1016/j.tele.2020.101510
Parrott, W. G., & Smith, R. H. (1993). Distinguishing the experiences of envy and jealousy. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 64(6), 906–920. 10.1037/0022-3514.64.6.906
Reeves, B., Ram, N., Robinson, T. N., Cummings, J. J., Giles, C. L., Pan, J., Chiatti, A., Cho, M.,
Roehrick, K., Yang, X., Gagneja, A., Brinberg, M., Muise, D., Lu, Y., Luo, M., Fitzgerald, A., &
Yeykelis, L. (2019). Screenomics: A framework to capture and analyze personal life experiences
and the ways that technology shapes them. Human–Computer Interaction,1–52.10.1
080/07370024.2019.1578652
Reinecke, L., & Trepte, S. (2014). Authenticity and well-being on social network sites: A two-wave lon-
gitudinal study on the effects of online authenticity and the positivity bias in SNS communication.
Computers in Human Behavior, 30, 95–102. 10.1016/j.chb.2013.07.030
Ryff, C. D., Dienberg Love, G., Urry, H. L., Muller, D., Rosenkranz, M. A., Friedman, E. M.,
Davidson, R. J., & Singer, B. (2006). Psychological well-being and ill-Being: Do they have dis-
tinct or mirrored biological correlates? Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics,75(2),85–95.10.115
9/000090892
Scherr, S., Toma, C. L., & Schuster, B. (2019). Depression as a predictor of Facebook surveillance and
envy. Journal of Media Psychology, 31, 196–202. 10.1027/1864-1105/a000247
Schueller, S. M., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2010). Pursuit of pleasure, engagement, and meaning:
Relationships to subjective and objective measures of well-being. The Journal of Positive Psychology,
5(4), 253–263. 10.1080/17439761003794130
Smith, R. H., & Kim, S. H. (2007). Comprehending envy [doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.46].
Psychological Bulletin, 133, 46–64. 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.46
Thomaes, S., Reijntjes, A., Orobio de Castro, B., Bushman, B. J., Poorthuis, A., & Telch, M. J. (2010). I
like me if you like me: On the interpersonal modulation and regulation of preadolescents’ state
self-esteem. Child Development, 81(3), 811–825. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01435.x
Thomas, F., Shehata, A., Otto, L. P., Mo¨ller, J., & Prestele, E. (2021). How to capture reciprocal com-
munication dynamics: Comparing longitudinal statistical approaches in order to analyze within-
and between-person effects. Journal of Communication. 10.1093/joc/jqab003
Thrash, T. M., Moldovan, E. G., Oleynick, V. C., & Maruskin, L. A. (2014). The psychology of
inspiration. Social and Personality Psychology Compass,8(9),495–510.doi.org/10.1111/
spc3.12127
Tov, W. (2018). Well-being concepts and components. In E. Diener, S. Oishi, & L. Tay (Eds.),
Handbook of well-being. DEF Publishers. nobascholar.com
Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being P. M. Valkenburg et al.
18 Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021
Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J., & Walther, J. B. (2016). Media effects: Theory and research. Annual
Review of Psychology, 67, 315–338. 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033608
Valkenburg, P. M., van Driel, I. I., & Beyens, I. (2021). The associations of active and passive social me-
dia use with well-being: A critical scoping review. PsyArXiv: 10.31234/osf.io/j6xqz
van de Ven, N., Zeelenberg, M., & Pieters, R. (2009). Leveling up and down: The experiences of benign
and malicious envy. Emotion, 9(3), 419–429. 10.1037/a0015669
van Driel, I. I., Pouwels, J. L., Beyens, I., Keijsers, L., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2019). Posting, scrolling,
chatting, and Snapping: Youth (14-15) and social media in 2019. Center for Research on Children,
Adolescents, and the Media (CcaM).
van Roekel, E., Keijsers, L., & Chung, J. M. (2019). A review of current ambulatory assessment studies
in adolescent samples and practical recommendations. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 29(3),
560–577. 10.1111/jora.12471
Verduyn, P., Gugushvili, N., Massar, K., Ta¨ht, K., & Kross, E. (2020). Social comparison on social net-
working sites. Current Opinion in Psychology, 36, 32–37. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.04.002
Verduyn, P., Lee, D. S., Park, J., Shablack, H., Orvell, A., Bayer, J., Ybarra, O., Jonides, J., & Kross, E.
(2015). Passive Facebook usage undermines affective well-being: Experimental and longitudinal ev-
idence. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 144, 480–488. 10.1037/xge0000057
Verduyn, P., Ybarra, O., Re´sibois, M., Jonides, J., & Kross, E. (2017). Do social network sites enhance
or undermine subjective well-being? A critical review. Social Issues and Policy Review, 11(1),
274–302. 10.1111/sipr.12033
Vorderer, P., Klimmt, C., & Ritterfeld, U. (2004). Enjoyment: At the heart of media entertainment.
Communication Theory, 14(4), 388–408. 10.1111/j.1468-2885.2004.tb00321.x
Wang, H.-Z., Yang, T.-T., Gaskin, J., & Wang, J.-L. (2019). The longitudinal association between
passive social networking site usage and depressive symptoms: The mediating role of envy and
moderating role of life satisfaction. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 38(3), 181–199.
https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2019.38.2.181
Waterloo, S. F., Baumgartner, S. E., Peter, J., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2018). Norms of online expressions
of emotion: Comparing Facebook, Twitter, Instagram, and WhatsApp. New Media & Society,
20(5), 1813–1831. 10.1177/1461444817707349
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of posi-
tive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6),
1063–1070.
Wenninger, H., Krasnova, H., & Buxmann, P. (2019). Understanding the role of social networking sites
in the subjective well-being of users: A diary study. European Journal of Information Systems, 28(2),
126–148. 10.1080/0960085X.2018.1496883
Wu, J., & Srite, M. (2021). Envy on social media: The good, the bad and the ugly. International Journal
of Information Management, 56, 102255. 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2020.102255
P. M. Valkenburg et al. Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 00 (2021) 1–19 19
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jcmc/advance-article/doi/10.1093/jcmc/zmab015/6413702 by guest on 29 October 2021