ArticlePDF Available
Correspondence: On the state of
knowledge concerning the effects
of temporal light modulation
Temporal light modulation (TLM) is a change
in the luminous quantity or spectral distribu-
tion of light with respect to time of either a
light source or a lighting system. These
changes arise because of the device or system
design, including drivers and control gear, and
because of fluctuations in the electrical supply.
In former times, families of lighting products
all exhibited the same (or approximately the
same) TLM characteristics: For example, AC-
powered incandescent lamps shared the prop-
erty of TLM in a sinusoidal wave at twice the
mains frequency and 4%–10% modulation
depth. T8 fluorescent lighting systems with
electronic ballasts operated with a dominant
frequency between 20 kHz and 40 kHz and
little modulation depth. Previously, knowing
the lighting technology provided sufficient
information about the TLM properties of a
lighting system; today, this is impossible with-
out measuring the system directly.
Light-emitting diode (LED) light sources
and lighting systems exhibit a very wide
variety of TLM characteristics because of
the variety of ways in which the products can
be designed and because LEDs inherently
respond very quickly to the driving current.
1,2
Moreover, TLM is sometimes deliberately
introduced to LED lighting systems through
the use of pulse-width modulation (PWM) to
control the light intensity. When using PWM,
the light output exhibits a 100% modulation
depth at constant frequency but with variable
duty cycle depending on the desired intensity.
Most systems of which we are aware use
PWM at frequencies higher than twice the
mains frequencies (e.g., 300–400 Hz).
The importance of this phenomenon rests
in the fact that TLM can be a cause of adverse
visual, behavioural and health effects on
viewers.
2–4
The undesirable visual perceptions
are flicker, the stroboscopic effect and the
phantom array effect; these are collectively
known as temporal light artefacts.
1
Among
the unanswered questions is the relative
importance of the visual effects of TLM, as
compared to effects on health or task per-
formance.
5
Some argue that the visual effects
in common interiors are little more than an
annoyance provided that the light source does
not exhibit flicker; others argue that these
visual perceptions relate to more serious
phenomena, at least for some sensitive
people.
6
In 2015, the IEEE published a recom-
mended practice that included an in-depth
risk assessment of possible heath and behav-
ioural outcomes, taking into account both the
severity of the outcome and the probability of
its occurrence.
2
The risk assessment compo-
nent of the hazard analysis took into account
the strength of the evidence; notably, many of
the outcomes were judged to be in need of
more evidence to support evidence-based
guidance on limits for TLM. This also was
the conclusion drawn by the 2017 CIE stake-
holder workshop
3
and the 2019 ANSES
report.
4
Researchers have risen to these challenges
with several recent publications in this journal
and others, which is most welcome; nonethe-
less, there remains disagreement concerning
the meaning of these findings for recommen-
dations and regulations. We comment here,
Lighting Res. Technol. 2021; 53: 89–92
ßHer Majesty the Queen in Right of Canada. National Research Council of Canada, 2020 10.1177/1477153520959182
very briefly, on the state of knowledge, focus-
ing primarily on publications from Lighting
Research and Technology, and propose a few
priority topics for research attention.
It has long been the case that the visual
effects of TLM receive the most research
attention, and these were the basis for the
limits in the only institutional recommended
practice on the topic to date.
2
These effects
occur after very short exposures and are
therefore easy to test.
1
For example, Perz
et al. developed the Stroboscopic Visibility
Measure
7
to predict the stroboscopic effect
from a metric derived from the measured light
source TLM. This metric is founded on the
principle of threshold visibility and is normal-
ized so that the value of the SVM for threshold
visibility (50% likelihood of seeing the strobo-
scopic effect) of the average person is 1.0.
5,7
According to Scopus, the eighth-most-cited
paper from this journal (cited 141 times as of
this writing) is the 1989 paper by Wilkins,
Nimmo-Smith, Slater and Bedocs
8
in which
fluorescent lighting in an office was changed
between magnetic ballasts and high-frequency
ballasts. Complaints of headaches and eye-
strain were reduced by the high-frequency
operation of the linear fluorescent lamps, but
the effect was pronounced only for those with
a tendency to experience headaches and eye-
strain. That is, on average across the whole
sample the effects were marginal, but there
appears to have been a subpopulation of
sensitive individuals who experienced the
adverse effects of the TLM more powerfully.
This outcome occurred despite the large win-
dows in the offices, which would have the
effect of reducing the influence of the electric
lighting conditions during daylight hours.
That is, in the general population it can be
difficult to detect the effects of TLM. Veitch
and McColl
9
observed an effect of TLM (also
manipulated using fluorescent lighting bal-
lasts) only on a visually difficult task in a
repeated-measures experiment under restrict-
ive viewing conditions, and with young adult
participants. Sekulovski et al.
10
conducted a
field intervention study with conceptual simi-
larity to the original Wilkins et al.
8
paper, but
did not find that there was any difference in
headache incidence between LED lighting
with an SVM value of nominally 1.34 and
one of 0.47. They appear not to have had data
concerning the participants’ individual differ-
ences in sensitivity, so it is unknown whether
the most-sensitive individuals might have had
a different experience than the group overall.
Moreover, the experimental space had large
windows and included desks at varying dis-
tances from the window, so that there was
considerable variability in the effective SVM
of the conditions to which participants were
exposed both across the space and during the
day as well as an unknown additional vari-
ability in the SVM experience of individuals
as they moved throughout the space during
the day. Thus, there are several reasons why
the expected effect of SVM on headache
incidence was not observed.
Veitch and Martinsons
11
studied strobo-
scopic visibility and the acceptability of con-
ditions for a set of commercially available
lamps varying in SVM, focusing on young
adults (thought to be more sensitive) and
including a measure of sensitivity to visual
discomfort. The relationship between the light
source’s SVM value and participants’ report-
ing of stroboscopic visibility (namely, a rapid,
non-linear increase in stroboscopic visibility
as SVM increased above 0.4) were the same
for groups high and low in sensitivity to visual
discomfort, but the high-sensitivity group did
report that the conditions were more annoy-
ing than the low-sensitivity group when the
SVM value of the light source was 1.4 or 3.0.
Given the very short exposures, Veitch and
Martinsons argued that this finding merits a
greater focus in future research upon more
sensitive individuals. Individuals who are
more sensitive to visual discomfort also have
a higher TLM frequency threshold for detect-
ing the phantom array effect than those who
90 JA Veitch et al.
Lighting Res. Technol. 2021; 53: 89–92
are less sensitive (up to 11 kHz, vs. the 6 kHz
average threshold).
12
The renewed focus on effects beyond visual
perception seen in Sekulovski et al.
10
is
important. Zhao et al.,
13
with a small
sample size, examined brain activity, eye
movement, the stroboscopic effect and cogni-
tive performance in a repeated-measures
experiment with nine TLM conditions
chosen in relation to the IEEE 1789-2015
recommendations.
2
They found that the con-
ditions identified as being high-risk condi-
tions in IEEE 1789-2015 caused greater
cortical arousal and, in some cases, higher
ratings of task difficulty even when cognitive
performance was unaffected.
The rapid adoption of LED lighting has
created pressure to develop recommendations
to limit the risk of such adverse effects.
Whether in the form of recommendations
2
or regulations,
14
these are invariably contro-
versial.
5,15
Debate and discussion of these
various approaches to establishing the suit-
able metrics and the limit values on them are
important and necessary, of course; but more
and better information on which to base these
discussions would prevent a continual revisit-
ing of old disagreements. Specifically, the
world would benefit from more information
to address the following open questions,
among others:
What are the effects of TLM in the popu-
lation subgroup of people who are suscep-
tible to visual stress?
What are the effects of varying TLM
conditions on outcomes beyond visual per-
ception (i.e., physiological, behavioural and
health effects)?
Given that much emphasis has been placed
on dominant frequency and modulation
depth as predictors of outcomes, what are
the effects of other possibly influential par-
ameters, such as duty cycle and waveform?
It should go without saying that strong
research designs, adequate sample sizes, and
clean measurements of both stimulus condi-
tions and outcome measures will be required
to provide the strong evidence base upon
which to build future recommendations,
standards and regulations. Exactly what
those documents ought to say will be the
result of consensus processes in various
communities,
3
but more and better research
is needed as inputs to the discussions.
ORCID iDs
Jennifer A Veitch https://orcid.org/0000-
0003-3183-4537
Christophe Martinsons https://orcid.org/
0000-0002-2286-5991
Carsten Dam-Hansen https://orcid.org/
0000-0001-7518-4025
References
1 Commission Internationale de l’Eclairage
(CIE). Visual aspects of time-modulated
lighting systems – Definitions and measure-
ment models. TN 006-2016. Vienna, Austria:
CIE, 2016. Retrieved 2 September 2020, from
http://files.cie.co.at/883_CIE_TN_006-2016.
pdf.
2 IEEE Power Electronics Society. IEEE rec-
ommended practices for modulating current in
high-brightness LEDs for mitigating health risks
to viewers. S1789-2015. New York, NY:
Institute for Electrical and Electronics
Engineers, 2015. Retrieved 2 September 2020,
from https://standards.ieee.org/standard/1789-
2015.html.
3 Commission Internationale de l’Eclairage
(CIE). Final report CIE stakeholder workshop
for temporal light modulation standards for
lighting systems. CIE TN 008:2017. Vienna,
Austria: CIE, 2017. Retrieved 2 September
2020, from http://files.cie.co.at/943_CIE_TN_
008-2017.pdf.
4 Martinsons C, Attia D, Behar-Cohen F, Carre
´
S, Enouf O, Falco
´n J, et al.. Correspondence:
On the state of knowledge concerning the effects of TLM 91
Lighting Res. Technol. 2021; 53: 89–92
An appraisal of the effects on human health
and the environment of using light-emitting
diodes. Lighting Research & Technology 2019;
51: 1275–1276.
5 Perz M. Opinion: Setting the stroboscopic
visibility threshold. Lighting Research &
Technology 2020; 52(3): 322.
6 Boyce PR, Wilkins A. Visual discomfort
indoors. Lighting Research & Technology 2018;
50(1): 98–114.
7 Perz M, Vogels IMLC, Sekulovski D, Wang
LL, Tu Y, Heynderickx IEJ. Modeling the
visibility of the stroboscopic effect occurring in
temporally modulated light systems. Lighting
Research and Technology 2015; 47(3): 281–300.
8 Wilkins AJ, Nimmo-Smith I, Slater AI, Bedocs
L. Fluorescent lighting, headaches and eye-
strain. Lighting Research and Technology 1989;
21(1): 11–18.
9 Veitch JA, McColl SL. Modulation of fluor-
escent light: Flicker rate and light source
effects on visual performance and visual com-
fort. Lighting Research and Technology 1995;
27(4): 243–256.
10 Sekulovski D, Poort S, Perz M, Waumans L.
Effects of long-term exposure to stroboscopic
effect from moderate-level modulated light.
Lighting Research & Technology. Epub ahead
of print 19 November 2019. DOI:
1477153519881473.
11 Veitch JA, Martinsons C. Detection of the
stroboscopic effect by young adults varying in
sensitivity. Lighting Research & Technology.
Epub ahead of print 12 February 2020. DOI:
1477153519898718.
12 Brown E, Foulsham T, Lee C-S, Wilkins A.
Research note: Visibility of temporal light
artefact from flicker at 11 kHz. Lighting
Research & Technology 2020; 52(3): 371–376.
13 Zhao X, Hou D, Lin Y, Xu W. The effect of
stroboscopic effect on human health
indicators. Lighting Research & Technology
2020; 52(3): 389–406.
14 European Commission. Commission
Regulation (EU) 2019/2020 of 1 October 2019
laying down ecodesign requirements for light
sources and separate control gears pursuant to
Directive 2009/125/EC of the European
Parliament and of the Council and repealing
Commission Regulations (EC) No 244/2009,
(EC) No 245/2009 and (EU) No 1194/2012.
Official Journal of the European Union. 2019;
62:L315/209–240. Retrieved 2 September 2020,
from https://eur-lex.europa.eu/legal-content/
EN/TXT/?uri¼uriserv:OJ.L_.2019.315.01.
0209.01.ENG&toc¼OJ:L:2019:315:TOC.
15 National Electrical Manufacturers Association
(NEMA) Lighting Systems Division. Temporal
light artifacts (flicker and stroboscopic effects)
Rosslyn, VA: National Electrical
Manufacturers Association, 2015. Retrieved 2
September 2020, from https://www.nema.org/
Standards/Pages/Temporal-Light-Artifacts-
Flicker-and-Stroboscopic-Effects.aspx.
Jennifer A Veitch
a
,
Christophe Martinsons
b
, Steve Coyne
c
and
Carsten Dam-Hansen
d
a
National Research Council of Canada,
Ottawa, ON, Canada
b
Centre Scientifique et Technique du Ba
ˆtiment,
Saint-Martin-d’He
`res, France
c
Light Naturally, Brisbane, Australia
d
DTU Fotonik, Roskilde, Denmark
Address for correspondence: Jennifer A Veitch,
National Research Council of Canada, Ottawa,
ON K1A 0R6, Canada.
Email: jennifer.veitch@nrc-cnrc.gc.ca
92 JA Veitch et al.
Lighting Res. Technol. 2021; 53: 89–92
... The light emissions of LEDs can follow the driving current much faster than incandescent lamps which have a thermal inertia. This leads to temporal light modulation (TLM) which can cause effects in the recognition of the light by a human observer which are named temporal light artefacts (TLA) [1]. ...
... The next parameter is the number of samples n S that are required to virtually fill the interval between two samples of a burst without gaps, which is given by the ratio of the bursttrigger interval 1 fT and the integration time t int , rounded up to the next integer value: ...
Article
Full-text available
Modern LED-based light sources often use variants of pulse-width modulation to control the operation current in the LEDs and by this the light output and colour of the lamp. The light emission of the LEDs follows this variable driving current which results in a temporal light modulation (TLM). These can lead to effects in the light’s perception by a human observer, e.g. stroboscopic effects, which can range from annoying to harmful. Therefore, the assessment of TLM is needed in the design phase of lighting installations like indoor and outdoor lighting or in automotive context. This work explores the possibility to use cameras for the spatially resolved measurement of TLM. To deal with the insufficient sampling rate and the need for low-pass filtering in the time domain, an approach for periodic signals to use equivalent-time sampling is chosen. Two variants of this method are explained in detail and exemplary measurements of TLM waveforms are presented. The measurement of waveforms with a cut-off frequency of about 20 kHz is demonstrated. Despite the used demonstrator setup did not implement a V(λ)-matching, it allowed to gain insights on the TLM of sources in a scene. The main limitation is the relatively small dynamic range that can be measured without manual adjustments to the device settings. This will require some future development to add some automatic attenuators.
... It is due to these fluctuations that a phenomenon known as temporal light artefacts (TLAs) appears, as well as flickering, strobe effects and phantom array effects. Specifically, (Veitch, Martinsons, Coyne and Dam-Hansen, 2021) explain that flicker and stroboscopic effects are the result of observers in a static environment. Flickering occurs when observers in a non-static environment change the perception of moving objects, while the phantom array effect occurs when non-static observers are present in a static environment, which causes changes to perceived shapes and spatial positions of objects. ...
... In accordance with the work of the Commission Internationale de l'Éclairage (CIE), flicker is considered a visual perception effect. There have been many studies that have looked at flicker as a behavioral phenomenon, but it has also been investigated as a health hazard at fluctuation frequencies above the threshold at which it is still perceived by human eyes (Veitch, Martinsons, Coyne, and Dam-Hansen, 2021 Heine, and Franssen, 2007). This results in disability glare, which causes impaired or, to a degree, even temporary blindness. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Human performance is influenced by lighting in many situations. Many studies have investigated how lighting impacts academic performance, focus, and motivation. The issue of color rendering and temperature is becoming increasingly important as high efficiency lamps and government regulations are enforced. For decades, fluorescent lamps have been used in educational systems to provide high-quality, efficient lighting. Fluorescent lamps can negatively affect the built environment as well as the surrounding environment. Several studies have been conducted to show that LED lamps have positive effects in the built environment. Several studies show that LED lamps with high color temperature correlation affect perceptions and actions positively in the classroom and workplace. In the workplace and in school, it can increase engagement and on-task behavior. This study develops experimental research for effects of LED, fluorescent, and full spectrum lighting on classroom’s children. To assess various effects related to classroom lighting on children between the ages of four and seven, an online survey was developed. During the Mother's Day morning tea celebration, a survey was conducted among three classrooms of Victoria International School's Early Learning Center. Results of the study indicate that 5000K or higher LED lighting has a significant impact on perceptions of attitudes and behaviors associated with alertness, focus, and performance in a classroom. Further experimental studies are required to evaluate attitudes as well as behaviors related to classroom illumination and how dynamic lighting affects students.
... Research has linked migraine with sensitivity to TLM for decades. [12][13][14][15] Proposed explanations include abnormal visual evoked potential in response to intermittent light, 16 a hyperexcitable cortex in individuals with migraine, 17 and cortical mechanisms such as hemodynamic response that explain photophobia from aversive stimuli such as bright light, flicker, and patterns. 18 However, no research has investigated if or how PAE sensitivity varies between migraine groups and non-migraine groups. ...
Article
Full-text available
The phantom array effect (PAE) is a series of repeated images that may be perceived when a person moves their eyes in large saccades across a light source (or a specular reflection of that light source) that is modulating in output over time. Fifty-five people, including a group of 25 who experience migraine, evaluated the visibility and annoyingness of phantom arrays produced by 85 unique temporal light modulation waveforms (including sine, rectangular, complex and DC waveforms) generated using an LED placed against a black background. Those with migraine exhibited higher average visibility compared to those without migraine ( p = 0.019) and were relatively more sensitive at higher frequencies ( p < 0.001). Younger participants also found more stimuli to be visible ( p < 0.001). The threshold sensitivity function was similar to that developed for the phantom array visibility measure (PAVM), and PAVM was effective in predicting visibility ( R ² = 0.87 for the relevant region of PAVM < 3). While those in the migraine group did not report seeing the PAE more often in everyday life at a statistically significant level, they reported being more annoyed by it and having more unwanted physiological responses (headaches, eye fatigue and distraction/disorientation). Members of the migraine group were also more likely to have changed their behaviour in architectural spaces (such as leaving a restaurant with ‘flickering’ lights). In the four hours after completing the experiment, 64% of the migraine group (vs. 19% of the non-migraine group) reported experiencing discomfort or an adverse reaction. In particular, 41% reported experiencing a headache (vs. 8% for those in the non-migraine group).
... Headaches, migraines, and eyestrain have been commonly reported, studied, and confirmed in the scientific literature. However, these negative symptoms seem to occur more frequently and more intensely for a fraction of the general population which is yet to be rigorously identified [37]. For these well-known effects, more research should be done to provide dose-response relationships to be able to improve health and safety requirements for sensitive people. ...
Article
Temporal light modulation (TLM) is defined as a temporal change in luminous or color quantities of a light source, resulting from fluctuations of the power supply. Research studies published during the past decade helped define new metrics describing well-known visual effects of TLM such as flicker and the stroboscopic effect. Other visual artifacts induced by TLM such as the phantom array effect are still under investigation to better understand their occurrence and visibility, especially when viewing modern types of LED modules and power supplies, used for instance in automotive lighting and in color-tunable decorative or entertainment lighting. With the advent of solid-state lighting, high levels of temporal light modulation and new types of waveforms exhibited by some LED lamps and luminaires have generated concerns among users and regulators, thereby motivating the establishment of new research, metrics, and standards. Limits have been set in regulations to protect people from undesirable effects. Below these limits, TLM can be used as a useful optical signature to facilitate remote sensing measurements of lighting systems and their obtrusive light components by lock-in detection techniques
... In addition, a study of longterm exposure to stroboscopic effects did not find significant effects in health and well-being effects (like eyestrain and headaches) between the higher SVM = 1.34 and SVM = 0.47 conditions (Sekulovski et al. 2020). Subsequently, it was pointed out that Sekulovski et al.'s study did not account for individual differences in sensitivity, and the large variability in SVM levels across the experimental space and over time may have contributed to the lack of expected results (Veitch et al. 2021). Therefore, the linking the TLM stimuli with the possible health effects based on the SVM metric is inconclusive. ...
Book
This report is a comprehensive review of the scientific knowledge acquired over the last ten years on the effects of solid state lighting (SSL) on human health. The report considers issues that concern both large fractions of the population as well as small sensitive groups of people. The report is primarily intended to support policymakers, offering guidance on lighting and health, as they consider product regulatory requirements, procurement specifications, programmes to raise public awareness, and other initiatives. However, the report will also be useful to the entire lighting community, including scientists engaged in fundamental and applied lighting research, illumination engineers, manufacturers, and lighting practitioners. This report is available at https://www.iea-4e.org/ssl/news/new-comprehensive-report-on-health-effects-of-solid-state-lighting/
Article
Solid-state light sources can be more prone to larger temporal light modulation (TLM) than conventional sources. TLM visibility depends on wave shape, frequency, modulation depth and duty cycle, and is affected by the sensitivity of the observer. TLM can be visible well above the critical flicker fusion frequency, when there is relative movement between the observer’s eyes and light source, lighted space or moving objects in the field of view. This human subjects experiment explored visibility of the stroboscopic effect (SE) versus the phantom array effect (PAE) with targeted tasks under 74 TLM waveforms. The results showed the SE visibility peaks between 90 Hz and 120 Hz, while the PAE visibility peaks between 500 Hz and 1000 Hz. The phantom array is visible to sensitive participants at 6000 Hz. Both effects are more visible under rectangular versus sinusoidal TLM, higher modulation, and when duty cycles are 10% or 30% versus 50%. Higher sensitivity participants, differentiated using the Leiden Visual Sensitivity Scale, rated TLM waveforms as more visible, especially those inherently harder to see. This work lays a foundation for a PAE metric and guides driver and dimmer designers toward electronic circuits that minimize the visibility of TLM in LED products.
Article
Flicker has been an important lighting system consideration for over a century. More precise terms are temporal light modulation (TLM) as the stimulus, and responses to TLM as the unwanted visual, cognitive, or physiological consequences. As lighting technology evolved, different forms of TLM emerged, and so did responses to them. Today, some LED systems – encompassing the LED, driver, and control – can result in TLM causing severe unwanted effects, while other LED systems produce no unwanted effects at all. LED systems can deliver a much wider range of luminous waveforms than conventional lighting systems, some exhibiting very high modulation depths. More than any light source before, they can elicit perceptions of the phantom array. Direct flicker effects at modulation frequencies less than about 80 Hz and the stroboscopic effect at frequencies greater than 80 Hz are fairly well understood, but the phantom array effect needs more exploration and characterisation. This review focuses on the technology and research history that led to current metrics for quantifying TLM and human responses to TLM. Visually impaired individuals may exhibit alterations in their response to TLM, but such a discussion is beyond the intent of this review. Thus, the focus is on individuals with normal visual function.
Article
Full-text available
The advent of LED lighting has renewed concern about the possible visual, neurobiological, and performance and cognition effects of cyclic variations in lighting system luminous flux (temporal light modulation). The stroboscopic visibility measure (SVM) characterises the temporal light modulation signal to predict the visibility of the stroboscopic effect, one of the visual perception effects of temporal light modulation. A SVM of 1 means that the average person would detect the phenomenon 50% of the time. There is little published data describing the population sensitivity to the stroboscopic effect in relation to the SVM, and none focusing on people subject to visual stress. This experiment, conducted in parallel in Canada and France, examined stroboscopic detection for horizontal and vertical moving targets when viewed under commercially available lamps varying in SVM conditions (SVM: ∼0; ∼0.4; ∼0.9; ∼1.4; ∼3.0). As expected, stroboscopic detection scores increased with increasing SVM. For the horizontal task, average scores were lower than the expected 4/8 at ∼0.90, but increased non-linearly with higher SVMs. Stroboscopic detection scores did not differ between people low and high in pattern glare sensitivity, but people in the high-pattern glare sensitivity group reported greater annoyance in the SVM ∼1.4 and ∼3.0 conditions.
Article
Full-text available
An appraisal of the effects on human health and the environment of LEDs by the authors of the report of the French health agency ANSES published in 2019.
Article
Full-text available
The effect of a 13-week exposure to moderate levels of light modulation resulting in visible, but not irritable, stroboscopic effect was studied. Over the course of three months, two sets of participants working in an office environment filled in a questionnaire about their health and wellbeing at the start and the end of each working day. Using a schedule of changes between two light settings differing only in the amount of temporal modulation, it was shown that the higher temporal modulation light did not significantly increase the occurrence of any health and wellbeing parameters (like eyestrain and headaches) tested. Furthermore, even though there was a large variation in the individual probability of complaints, there was no interaction effect between the individual level of complaints and the amount of light modulation. Using power analysis, we demonstrate that the increase of unwanted effects of 5% or more has a probability of less than 5%.
Article
Full-text available
Visual discomfort refers to discomfort or pain in or around the eyes, often associated with headache and/or nausea, and sometimes accompanied by signs such as red, itchy or watering eyes. The lighting conditions likely to cause visual discomfort are insufficient light for the task at hand, dramatic differences in illuminance around the task, shadows, veiling reflections, glare and flicker. To date, research on visual discomfort has been largely reactive, i.e. in response to complaints, but there is now proactive work that explores why discomfort occurs. The hypothesis underlying this work is that the human visual system has evolved to extract information from the natural world efficiently so that when the visual environment departs from the temporal, spatial or chromatic characteristics of the natural world, discomfort is likely because of inefficient neural processing. An important implication is that visual discomfort therefore depends on both the lighting and the décor of a space. Until this approach becomes more established, visual discomfort can be minimised by following carefully developed standards and guidance, by using products that meet appropriate standards, by paying attention to both lighting and décor and by being aware of the wide variation in individual sensitivity.
Article
Full-text available
The weekly incidence of headaches among office workers was compared when the offices were lit by fluorescent lighting where the fluorescent tubes were operated by (a) a conventional switch-start circuit with choke ballast providing illumination that pulsated with a modulation depth of 43-49% and a principal frequency component at 100 Hz; (b) an electronic start circuit with choke ballast giving illumination with similar characteristics; (c) an electronic ballast driving the lamps at about 32 kHz and reducing the 100 Hz modulation to less than 7%. In a double-blind cross-over design, the average incidence of headaches and eyestrain was more than halved under high-frequency lighting. The incidence was unaffected by the speed with which the tubes ignited. Headaches tended to decrease with the height of the office above the ground and thus with increasing natural light. Office occupants chose to switch on the high-frequency lighting for 30% longer on average.
Article
Full-text available
The effects of fluorescent light spectral composition and flicker rate on visual performance and visual comfort were studied on 48 undergraduate students using two different rates of flicker: conventional low-frequency flicker (120 Hz) and high frequency flicker(between 20-60 kHz); and three different light sources; full-spectrum lamps, cool-white lamps, and filtered-cool-white lamps. The design was a 2×3 (Flicker rate × Light source) mixed within-between ANOVA. Visual performance and time on visual performance task were assessed using a Landolt ring task. Visual comfort was assessed by self-report after a period of reading difficult text. Visual performance scores of 18-24-year-old male and female university students were significantly higher in the high-frequency flicker condition than the low-frequency flicker condition. There were no other statistically significant effects. Health status was unrelated to visual performance. Neurophysiological explanations are discussed. The finding that an energy-efficient means of driving fluorescent lamps also can improve visual performance provides added impetus to adopt this new technology.
Article
The stroboscopic effect from LED light sources can become considerable in working environments. Therefore, this study aims to explore the short-term health effect of a temporal light artefact. The experiment was carried out featuring 10 university students. Three frequencies and three modulation depths were assessed. Psychological reaction was evaluated through subjective scales, while physiological parameters were also collected for mutual validation and analysis. It was found that when the conditions are in the high-risk zone defined by IEEE Standard 1789-2015, subjects considered these conditions to be unacceptable and reported discrete spatial movement and higher visual fatigue levels. Supported by psychological and physiological evidence, it is suggested that such fatigue is caused by a higher chance of flicker. Invisible flicker also significantly affected alpha and beta wave power density, suggesting that a strobe of low frequency could potentially decrease drowsiness and increase cortical arousal. Some limitations to the work performance of this study are also discussed.
Article
A flickering light can be seen during a saccadic eye movement as a pattern of contours known as a phantom array. On repeated pairs of trials, observers made saccades across a narrow (1 arc minutes), bright (10−4 cd/m²) source of flickering light and were required to detect the phantom array. On one of each pair of trials, chosen at random, the light flickered at 60 kHz and on the other at a frequency chosen in the range 1–11 kHz. In two such studies, a few observers were reliably able to discriminate 11 kHz from 60 kHz on the basis of the visibility of the phantom array. The average threshold at which the array was visible was about 6 kHz and therefore double that previously obtained with larger targets. Those observers who were able to see the phantom array tended reliably to report more symptoms of visual discomfort in everyday life.
Article
Three perception experiments were conducted to develop a measure for predicting the visibility of the stroboscopic effect occurring in temporally modulated light systems. In the first experiment, different methodologies were evaluated for their measurement error. In the second experiment, the visibilities of the stroboscopic effect for square wave and sine wave light modulations were measured and the results were found to be consistent with previous findings for flicker perception. In the third experiment, specifically crafted, complex waveforms were used to test the theory of frequency summation. Based on the results of these three experiments, a new measure for the visibility of the stroboscopic effect was developed, consisting of a summation of the energy in all frequency components, normalized for human sensitivity.