ArticlePDF Available

Staying connected during stay‐at‐home: Communication with family and friends and its association with well‐being

Authors:

Abstract

COVID‐19 and the resulting stay‐at‐home orders issued to reduce the spread of the virus created a novel social situation in which people could not spend in‐person time with their family and friends. Thus, emerging technologies like video calling and other forms of mediated communication like voice calling and text messaging became important resources for people to stay in touch. The purpose of this study was threefold. First, we wanted to test whether people would use more mediated communication (video calls, voice calls, text messaging) to stay in touch during the stay‐at‐home order. Second, we wanted to see if increased mediated communication would be positively associated with well‐being. Finally, we explored whether mediated communication was related to age. To answer these questions, we surveyed 2090 participants who answered questions online about how their use of video calls, voice calls, and text messaging and their well‐being had changed since the stay‐at‐home order. Our results show that people increased their use of mediated communication, particularly video calling; and increases in mediated communication with close others, particularly friends, was related to higher levels of well‐being. Finally, we found that age was related only to the use of video calling; younger people tended to use more video calling. These findings support the compensatory theory of technology use, that people use technologically mediated communication to maintain contact with their close friends and family when in‐person contact is not possible, and that this form of contact, when in‐person interaction is unavailable, is associated with positive outcomes.
EMPIRICAL ARTICLE
Staying connected during stay-at-home: Communication with
family and friends and its association with well-being
Genavee Brown
1
| Patricia M. Greenfield
2
1
Department of Psychology, Northumbria
University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK
2
Department of Psychology, University of
California, Los Angeles, California
Correspondence
Genavee Brown, Department of Psychology,
Northumbria University, Newcastle upon
Tyne, UK.
Email: genavee.brown@northumbria.ac.uk
Abstract
COVID-19 and the resulting stay-at-home orders issued to reduce the spread of the
virus created a novel social situation in which people could not spend in-person time
with their family and friends. Thus, emerging technologies like video calling and other
forms of mediated communication like voice calling and text messaging became
important resources for people to stay in touch. The purpose of this study was three-
fold. First, we wanted to test whether people would use more mediated communica-
tion (video calls, voice calls, text messaging) to stay in touch during the stay-at-home
order. Second, we wanted to see if increased mediated communication would be
positively associated with well-being. Finally, we explored whether mediated commu-
nication was related to age. To answer these questions, we surveyed 2092 partici-
pants who answered questions online about how their use of video calls, voice calls,
and text messaging and their well-being had changed since the stay-at-home order.
Our results show that people increased their use of mediated communication, partic-
ularly video calling; and increases in mediated communication with close others, par-
ticularly friends, was related to higher levels of well-being. Finally, we found that age
was related only to the use of video calling; younger people tended to use more
video calling. These findings support the compensatory theory of technology use,
that people use technologically mediated communication to maintain contact with
their close friends and family when in-person contact is not possible, and that this
form of contact, when in-person interaction is unavailable, is associated with positive
outcomes.
KEYWORDS
age, close relationships, COVID-19, emotions, mediated communication, social isolation, text
messages, video calls, voice calls, well-being
1|INTRODUCTION
The COVID-19 pandemic has led to extreme changes in daily life due
to the need to avoid physical contact with other human beings in
order to prevent the spread of the virus. Governments around the
world made the difficult decision to order their populations to isolate
in their homes and avoid going out to see other people. These
extreme restrictions led to a loss of in-person contact with family or
friends who did not live in the same residence. We know that humans
are social beings, are hardwired to crave contact with others, and
indeed feel unfulfilled without it (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Ryan &
Deci, 2017).
Even more troubling in the context of COVID-19 stay-at-home
orders is the fact that it was a period of extreme stress and one main
coping mechanism for dealing with stress is receiving social support
from others (Cohen & Wills, 1985). Fortunately, technology provided
multiple avenues for staying connected during the stay-at-home
order. Individuals with a mobile phone and an Internet connection or
Received: 14 August 2020 Revised: 9 November 2020 Accepted: 14 November 2020
DOI: 10.1002/hbe2.246
Hum Behav & Emerg Tech. 2021;3:147156. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/hbe2 © 2021 Wiley Periodicals LLC 147
cellular data could use video calls, telephone calls, and text-based
messaging in the form of texts, emails, or Instant Messages via differ-
ent applications (WhatsApp, Facebook Messenger, etc.) to contact
family and friends.
The question in the current study was threefold: First, would indi-
viduals increase their usage of these technologies to maintain contact
with family and friends when they could not see them in-person? Sec-
ond, would using these avenues to stay in touch be beneficial for their
psychological well-being? Third, would age be associated with using
different types of mediated communication? We hope that answers
to these questions will shed light on how humans creatively combat
the social isolation created by COVID-19.
1.1 |Stimulation, displacement, or compensation?
In the early 2000s research on communication technologies centered
on resolving the question of whether online communication stimu-
lated (i.e., increased) or displaced (i.e., decreased) in-person contact
with others (Valkenburg & Peter, 2007). In general, these studies
tended to show that greater amounts of online communication were
associated with greater in-person contact. However, one flaw in this
comparison between stimulation versus displacement in cross-
sectional studies or even short longitudinal studies, is that they do not
take into account the changing societal context that has accompanied
increases in communication technology use. Sociological and psycho-
logical studies show that people are spending less time in the physical
company of others across the lifespan than they did previously. For
example, in adolescence, relatively recent concerns about safety have
meant that American adolescents are allowed less freedom to see fri-
ends in person (boyd, 2014). Concerning adults, authors have also
noted that internal migration in the U.S. is frequent, and people no
longer live their entire lives in the same town, making frequent in-
person communication impossible (Molloy, Smith, & Wozniak, 2011).
Even if or when people settle in a location, there seems to be
decreased investment in democratic life, including decreased in-
person contact with others, such as in bowling leagues
(Putnam, 2000). Finally, the lack of multigenerational living in the
United States has also led to a larger percentage of elderly people
feeling isolated and lonely (Cohen-Mansfield, Hazan, Lerman, &
Shalom, 2016). Taken together, this research shows that people were
already spending less in-person time with others before COVID-19
caused stay-at-home orders to be issued.
Some research suggests that computer-mediated communication
has become a way to compensate for this lack of in-person communi-
cation. For example, in a recent daily diary study, researchers found
that adolescents were spending more time communicating online with
friends on days they did not see them in person (Manago, Brown,
Lawley, & Anderson, 2019). This finding suggests that teens are
substituting computer-mediated communication for in-person com-
munication when it is not possible to hang out with friends in person.
Additionally, young adults attending university have been found to
use computer-mediated communication to stay in touch with friends
from high school when seeing them frequently in-person is not possi-
ble (Brown & Michinov, 2017; Yang & Brown, 2013). Furthermore,
Chinese international students use technology to stay in touch with
both family and friends when they move to the U.S. and visiting home
is not feasible (Cemalcilar, Falbo, & Stapleton, 2005; Kline &
Liu, 2005). Adults also use technology to stay in touch with their close
ties when in-person contact is not possible. For example, immigrants
report maintaining family relationships in their countries of origin
through technology (Chen & Choi, 2011). Finally, older adults who
may be unable to get out to see family and friends, use technology
such as video and voice chat to stay in touch (Xie, 2008; Zamir,
Hennessy, Taylor, & Jones, 2018).
The current context provides us with the opportunity to observe
whether people will increase their mediated communication to main-
tain contact with family and friends when a governmental stay-at-
home order almost completely prevents in-person contact. We
hypothesized that a large portion of the population would engage in
more mediated communication than they did before the stay-at-home
orders were issued as a way to compensate for decreased in-person
contact. Because women engage in more mediated communication
than men (Kimbrough, Guadagno, Muscanell, & Dill, 2013), we have
explored this issue separately for each gender, as well as for the sam-
ple as a whole.
We acknowledge, however, that certain aspects of this study are
exploratory and therefore make no specific predictions about the
types of mediated communication that individuals may prefer to use,
or whether there will be differences in the amounts of mediated com-
munication used with family versus friends. We will however investi-
gate these differences in order to explore people's communication
preferences when in-person contact is not possible.
1.2 |Computer-mediated communication with
close ties: Association with well-being
People need close bonds with family and friends to help maintain their
happiness and self-esteem (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). Close bonds
are maintained through communication (Hartup & Stevens, 1999;
Segrin & Flora, 2005). Typically, elements of communication that are
key to developing emotional closeness, such as self-disclosure, emo-
tion sharing, and offering social support, have been studied in con-
texts where people are in the same physical space. Close ties may be
especially important in the current crisis as individuals may need extra
emotional and instrumental social support during this stressful period
(Taylor et al., 2020; WHO, 2020); but, due to the stay-at-home order,
individuals can only access social support through technological
means. Fortunately, some research suggests that online communica-
tion can provide social support and is associated with positive well-
being.
The positive effects of computer-mediated communication on
well-being have been demonstrated across the lifespan. Starting in
adolescence, teens who use computer-mediated communication to
fulfill their relational needs (Ang, Talib, Tan, Tan, & Yaacob, 2015) and
148 BROWN AND GREENFIELD
improve friendship quality (Valkenburg & Peter, 2007) tend to have
greater satisfaction with life. Young adults at university who engaged
in more self-disclosure through online communication had higher sat-
isfaction with life (Schiffrin, Edelman, Falkenstern, & Stewart, 2010).
In a random community sample of adults aged 1870, researchers
found that using more communication modalities, such as in-person
conversations, phone calls, video calls, texting, and email, with family
and friends was associated with greater well-being (Chan, 2015).
Finally, in a qualitative study of older adults who used chat rooms and
online voice calls to stay in touch with friends, researchers found that
they felt less lonely and received greater social support (Xie, 2008).
Thus, we hypothesized that individuals who use more mediated com-
munication with friends and family during the stay-at-home orders
would experience greater satisfaction with life, more positive emo-
tions, and fewer negative emotions. We made no specific predictions
about whether communication with friends or family would be more
beneficial to well-being, but planned to test them in the same model
to see if they are equally beneficial for well-being.
1.3 |Current study
In order to answer our research questions, we recruited participants
from two U.S. states: California and Rhode Island. In both states, resi-
dents had lived under a stay-at-home order for 34 days when the sur-
vey began; in both states; residents were still under stay-at-home
orders 7 days later when the survey ended. We used an online survey
to collect data on individuals' use of video calls, phone calls, and text
messages to stay in touch with family and friends. We also measured
shifts in satisfaction with life, positive emotions, and negative emo-
tions during COVID-19 as measures of well-being. Our hypotheses
and questions fell into three areas:
1. We hypothesized that individuals would use more mediated com-
munication with both family and friends during the stay-at-home
order than they did before the order. We explored whether this
predicted increase would take place in all media types: phone
calls, video calls, and text messages or would be concentrated in
the new, emerging media of video and text. We also explored
whether individuals would tend to be consistent across media
types, such that increased use would take place in all types of
communication, or whether individuals would tend to be selective,
increasing their use of one particular communication medium, but
not others.
2. We hypothesized that greater use of mediated communication
with family and friends during the stay-at-home order would be
associated with greater well-being. We also explored whether
communicating with family or friends would have a greater con-
nection to well-being.
3. We explored age effects: Would older adults increase their use of
the phone calls, an older technology? Would younger adults
increase their use of video and text-based communication, newer
technologies?
2|METHOD
2.1 |Participants
We recruited 2092 participants (M
age
= 59.15, SD
age
= 13.56) com-
posed of 476 men, 1,583 women, 29 participants who self-identified
as other genders, and 4 with unreported gender. One reason for this
gender imbalance is that we recruited participants via Facebook and a
higher percentage of American women use Facebook (83%), than
American men (75%) (Greenwood, Perrin, & Duggan, 2016).
Participants were recruited from two states in the U.S., California
(n= 1,137) and Rhode Island (n= 955). They were recruited using
Facebook advertisements with links to the survey.
Our aim was to control for the time after the stay-at-home order
was issued; and since these orders were issued by state, we recruited
from each state individually. Participants had been under stay-at-
home orders a little more than a month (34 days) when the survey
began; it was available for a week in each state. We hoped that this
amount of time would have allowed participants to settle into their
new patterns of living during the stay-at-home order and to have
developed new patterns of communication. In California, data collec-
tion took place from April 22 to April 29, 2020. In Rhode Island data
collection took place from May 1 through May 8, 2020. Ninety-two
percent of participants reported complying with the stay-at-home
order and those who respected the order were at home for an aver-
age of 41.87 days (SD = 13.13) at the time of the survey. Our sample
was mainly composed of European Americans (80.4%) with small per-
centages of LatinX (2.2%), African American (1.1%), Asian American
(1.8%), Native American (1%), and Pacific Islander (0.3%) participants.
11.5% identified as "other;"1.7% did not respond.
2.2 |Procedure
Participants were asked to participate in a study about how their lives
had changed since the coronavirus outbreak and the stay-at-home
order. If participants, clicked on the link, they were directed to Qualtrics,
presented with further information about the study, and asked for
informed consent to participate. The survey instrument included ques-
tions on the types of communication people use to stay in touch with
family and friends during the stay-at-home order and questions about
their emotions and satisfaction with life during the stay-at-home order.
Finally, participants were asked demographic questions. This study was
given ethical approval by the IRB at UCLA. All participants gave
informed consent before taking part in the current research project.
3|MEASURES
3.1 |Communication with family and friends
There were six mediated communication questions, three concerning
communication with family and three concerning communication with
BROWN AND GREENFIELD 149
friends. Participants first read the prompt, The following questions
are about how the type of communication you now use with your
(family/friends) living outside of your household has changed since
the stay-at-home order.Participants were then asked if the amount
of time they spend communicating with them via phone calls, video
calls, and textual messages, had changed. For example, Phone calls
have becomePossible answer choices were less frequent, no change,
more frequent. We coded the response less frequent as 1, no change
as 0, and more frequent as +1. For each participant, answers on the
phone call, video call, and textual message items were summed to cre-
ate a composite variable for family and friends separately. Scores
could range from 3 (decrease in all three media) to +3 (increase in all
three media). Positive scores indicate an increase since coronavirus
and stay-at-home; negative scores indicate a decrease. Because plus
and minus signs were used in calculating shifts, these scores indicate
net change. For example, a decrease in phone communication (1),
along with an increase in video calls (+1) and texting (+1) would sum
to +1. Means for the sample show that mediated communication with
family (M = 1.49, SD = 1.32) and mediated communication with fri-
ends (M = 1.24, SD = 1.37) were above zero which indicates a net
increase.
3.2 |Satisfaction with life
Satisfaction with life (SWL) was measured with one item, Compared
with before coronavirus, my life since the stay-at-home order is...
Response options were more satisfying, equally satisfying, less satisfy-
ing. More satisfying was coded as +1, equally satisfying as 0, and less
satisfying as 1. Single item measures of SWL are commonly used in
the literature and have been shown to be equally reliable and as valid
as measures with more items (Cheung & Lucas, 2014).
3.3 |Positive and negative emotions
Participants were presented with the following emotion-relevant
questions. For each emotion, they were asked to compare how they
felt before coronavirus with how they were now feeling during the
time of coronavirus and the stay-at-home order:
Compared with before coronavirus, is your life since stay at home
(Check all that apply)
i. Calmer.
ii. More anxiety-provoking.
iii. More depressing.
iv. More lonely.
Compared with before coronavirus, I have become (check all that
apply)
i. More peaceful.
ii. More content.
iii. More frazzled and distracted.
In these two emotion lists, there were three positive emotions
(calm, peaceful, content) and four negative emotions (anxious,
depressed, lonely, and frazzled/distracted).
The positive-emotion score consisted of the number of positive
emotions out of three for which a participant reported an increase
since coronavirus; hence, the possible range of individual scores was
zero to three. The mean number of positive emotions for which the
sample reported an increase was 0.75 (SD = 01.05). This mean signifies
that, on average, three-quarters of the total sample reported experienc-
ing a net increase in one positive emotion each during coronavirus.
The negative emotion score consisted of the number of negative
emotions out of four for which a participant reported an increase
since coronavirus; hence, the possible range of individual scores was
zero to four. The mean number of negative emotions for which the
total sample reported an increase was 1.03 (SD = 0.95). This mean sig-
nifies that, on average, sample participants reported experiencing a
net increase in about one negative emotion each during coronavirus.
3.4 |Demographics
We asked participants to report several demographic variables includ-
ing age, gender, and ethnicity. We also asked them to report if they
had been adhering to the stay-at-home order in their state and if so,
for how many days they had been at home.
3.5 |Data analysis
Due to the large sample size in our study, we have chosen to
set alpha at a conservative .001 level. To examine differences in medi-
ated communication before and during the pandemic, we use one-
sample ttests and test the significance of change against zero
(no change). To examine associations between usage of video calls,
phone calls, and textual messages, we use chi-square tests. In order to
examine differences between mediated communication with friends
and family, we conduct a paired-samples ttest. To examine the rela-
tionship between mediated communication and age we use Pearson
correlations. For all of these statistics, we also report effect sizes. To
examine the association between mediated communication and the
well-being variables, we use the composite communication variables
to predict well-being indicators with multiple regression analyses.
4|RESULTS
4.1 |Communication changes during stay-at-home
order
Seventy-nine percent of people reported increasing at least one form of
mediated communication with family; 76% reported increasing at least
150 BROWN AND GREENFIELD
one form of mediated communication with friends. Both men and
women showed the same pattern: 69.6% of men and 83.2% of women
increased at least one form of mediated communication with family;
64% of men and 79.6% women increased at least one form of mediated
communication with friends. Chi-square tests indicated that more
women than men increased communication with family (χ
2
[1] = 42.733,
p< .001) and with friends (χ
2
[1] = 48.338. p<.001); but effect sizes for
these gender differences were small (family: Φ= .14; friends: Φ=.15)
We ran one-sample ttests for each of the six communication
items with 0, no change, as the test value. We found that all types of
communication with both family and friends increased significantly
during the stay home order. Phone use with friends was the type of
communication that increased the least. For both friends and family,
textual communication increased the most compared with the other
types of communication. Effect sizes for all tests were large (Cohen,
1998). ttest values, effect sizes, and pvalues are reported in Table 1.
We carried out the same statistical analyses separately for men and
women. The pattern of results was the same for both genders: with
both family and friends, mediated communication in all modalities had
increased for both men and women, p< .001.
We were also interested in knowing whether people who
increased one form of computer mediated communication would also
increase other forms of mediated communication. To examine this
question, we ran two three-dimensional chi-square tests examining
the relationship between phone calls, video calls, and textual commu-
nication. One test examined this issue with respect to communicating
with family; the other test examined this issue with respect to com-
municating with friends. For both family and friends, the chi-square
values were highly significant (family: χ
2
(4) = 656.109, p< .001; fri-
ends: χ
2
(4) = 757.298, p< .001). Effect sizes were medium (family:
Φ= .56; friends: Φ= .60.)
The significant chi-square values indicated consistency in
response to stay-at-home across communication types. An examina-
tion of the italicized figures in Table 2 shows that people who
increased one type of mediated communication with family members
also tended to increase the other types as well. Similarly, people who
decreased one type of mediated communication also tended to
decrease the other types; and people who did not change their use
of one type of mediated communication tended to not change
their use of other types. We found a similar pattern for mediated
communication with friends. This pattern is demonstrated in the
-italicized figures in Table 3.
4.2 |Mediated communication and well-being
To explore the relationship between well-being and communication
we conducted Pearson's correlations on shifts in the three well-being
variables (SWL, positive emotions, and negative emotions) and shifts
in the composite communication variables for family and friends (com-
bining all three types of communication: phone, text, and video calls).
The bivariate Pearson's correlations reported in Table 4 show that
increased communication with both family and friends is positively
correlated with increased life satisfaction and augmented positive
emotions during coronavirus. There is no correlation between nega-
tive emotional shift and communication with family or friends. In
order to examine whether increased mediated communication with
family or friends was more important for well-being, we ran two
regressions on the indicators of well-being that were significantly cor-
related with communication: increased satisfaction with life (SWL)
and increased positive emotions experienced during the stay-at-home
order. The predictors were the composite variables for communica-
tion with family and with friends.
Concerning satisfaction with life during the stay-at-home order,
we found that model fit with the two predictors was significant, F
(2,1966) = 13.959, p< .001. Including shifts in mediated communica-
tion with friends and with family in the model explained 1.4% of the
variance in changing satisfaction with life. Examining the individual
predictors, we found that an increase in mediated communication
with family did not significantly predict increased satisfaction with life,
but an increase in mediated communication with friends was posi-
tively associated with increased satisfaction with life, β= .104,
t= 3.713, p< .001. For changes in positive emotion experienced dur-
ing the stay-at-home order, we found a similar pattern. Model fit with
the two predictors was significant, F(2,2005) = 21.592, p< .001. The
mediated communication with family and friends explained 2.1% of
the variance in positive emotion change during the pandemic. Only an
increase in mediated communication with friends was a significant
predictor in the equation, β= .122, t= 4.401, p< .001. In summary,
increased levels of mediated communication with friends during the
TABLE 1 One-sample ttests on
mean net change in the different types of
mediated communication for family and
friends
t-value df Mean SD Cohen's D
Family
Video calling 41.533 2058 0.511 0.559 0.914
Phone 30.691 2062 0.429 0.634 0.677
Text 44.337 2061 0.551 0.564 0.977
Friends
Video calling 32.919 2066 0.419 0.579 0.724
Phone 22.136 2068 0.306 0.629 0.486
Text 39.969 2072 0.517 0.589 0.878
Note: For all ttests, p< .001.
BROWN AND GREENFIELD 151
stay-at-home order were associated with increases in life satisfaction
and increases in positive emotions during the pandemic. Negative
emotional shifts were not associated with changes in mediated com-
munication with family or friends.
4.3 |Age and shifts in mediated communication
during stay-at-home
We tested the relationship between age and the use of different com-
munication media with friends and family, using Pearson's correla-
tions. As expected, age was negatively associated with the use of
video calls for communicating with both family (r=.115, p< .001)
and friends (r=.118, p< .001). This finding suggests that the youn-
ger participants were, the more frequently they tended to increase
their use of video calls to stay in touch during the stay-at-home
orders; however, effect size is small. Contrary to our prediction that
older participants would augment their use of voice calls during coro-
navirus and stay-at-home more than younger participants, age was
not associated with shifts in using traditional voice calls to stay in
touch with family and friends during stay-at-home. Changes in text
messaging with friends and family were also not significantly corre-
lated with age, indicating that people of all ages changed their use of
texting in similar ways during the stay-at-home order.
5|DISCUSSION
Our study surveyed a large sample of participants from two different
U.S. states a little over a month into the stay-at-home order. We
found that voice calls, video calls, and text-based messages increased
with friends and family during the stay-at-home order. The pattern
held for both genders, although it was slightly stronger for women.
We also found that participants who increased one form of mediated
communication tended to increase their use of the other forms of
mediated communication. Additionally, we found that an increase in
mediated communication with both family and friends was beneficial
for well-being, but that an increase in mediated communication with
friends was the more important factor. Interestingly, we found that
while younger participants increased their use of video calls more
than older ones, there was no association between age and voice calls
or text messages.
5.1 |Increased use of mediated communication for
relationship maintenance
We found that most of our participants (92%) were respecting the
stay-at-home order which meant that they could not have in-person
contact with family and friends outside of household members. Fortu-
nately, many people adapted to this situation by increasing their tech-
nology usage. Indeed, our results showed that people increased their
use of voice and video calls and text-based messages, although for
both family and friends phone calls increased the least. Perhaps this is
because if one has the time and capability to make a phone call on a
mobile phone, they could also use a video call. Video calls may be pre-
ferred because they offer richer cues. Indeed, video calls are the clos-
est form of communication to in-person conversations because many
of the elements of in-person communication such as body language
TABLE 2 Cross-tab chart of mediated communication with family
Video calls
Phone calls Less No change More
Less texting Less 28 10 6
No change 3 9 1
More 1 2 13
No change texting Less 5 13 14
No change 3 371 137
More 1 91 134
More texting Less 17 15 53
No change 2 145 174
More 4 214 570
Note: Numbers in bold are the participants who reported less of all
mediated communication types, no change in all mediated communication
types, or more of all mediated communication types.
TABLE 3 Cross-tab chart of mediated communication with
friends
Video calls
Phone calls Less No change More
Less texting Less 51 97
No change 0 13 6
More 1 2 10
No change texting Less 7 21 9
No change 6 451 145
More 0 75 76
More texting Less 17 20 46
No change 3 209 216
More 10 200 438
Note: Numbers in bold are the participants who reported less of all
mediated communication types, no change in all mediated communication
types, or more of all mediated communication types.
TABLE 4 Pearson correlations between mediated communication
and well-being variables
12 3 4
1 MC with family
2 MC with friends .601*
3 SWL .080* .118*
4 positive emotions .106* .142* .529*
5 negative emotions .030 .028 .435* .486*
Abbreviations: MC, mediated communication; SWL, satisfaction with life.
*p< .001.
152 BROWN AND GREENFIELD
and facial expressions are present in video calls. Furthermore, video
calls have been found to promote more bonding between friends than
voice calls or text messages (Sherman, Michikyan, & Greenfield, 2013).
Interestingly, although video calls may be a preferred method of
communication when in-person conversations are not possible, peo-
ple still increased their use of voice calls and text-based messages.
Indeed, we found that people who increased one form of mediated
communication were also likely to also increase their use of other
forms of mediated communication. This finding is an example of
media multiplexity theory which suggests that people who are emo-
tionally close tend to use multiple communication channels for staying
in touch with one another (Haythornthwaite, 2005). For example, fri-
ends might text to organize a lunch together, interact in-person during
the lunch, and post pictures of their lunch on social media and com-
ment on them later. Thus, since we asked specifically about close rela-
tionships, it is perhaps unsurprising that people tended to use multiple
forms of mediated communication to stay in touch.
Our findings also provide novel evidence for the compensatory
hypothesis of mediated communication use: that individuals use medi-
ated communication as a substitute when in-person interaction is not
possible. The novelty of our study is that it is a natural experiment
where a large proportion of the population was restricted from seeing
their family and friends in-person. Under this condition, people
increased their use of technology in order to stay in touch with their
close ties and this had important implications for well-being. Further
study needs to be conducted to examine the compensatory hypothe-
sis, but some evidence already exists that when people cannot see
each other in person due to immigration (Chen & Choi, 2011), study
abroad (Cemalcilar et al., 2005; Kline & Liu, 2005), or parental restric-
tions on movement (Manago et al., 2019) they adopt mediated com-
munication as an important tool for staying in touch. One way to test
the hypothesis, would be to look for reductions in mediated communi-
cation once the COVID-19 crisis has subsided and people can move
about freely and interact in person with friends and family.
5.2 |Mediated communication and well-being
Overall, we found that an increase in mediated communication was
positively related to increases in two indicators of well-being: satisfac-
tion with life and positive emotions. This finding mirrors work in the
offline communications literature showing that having numerous
social contacts, more social capital, and more social interactions con-
tribute to well-being and happiness (Cacioppo, Hawkley, &
Thisted, 2010; Diener & Seligman, 2002; Rodríguez-Pose & Von
Berlepsch, 2014). Research on computer-mediated communication
conducted outside of the pandemic also tends to show positive
effects of computer-mediated communication with friends and family
(Manago et al., 2019; Shen et al., 2017; Valkenburg & Peter, 2007).
Our results are also supported by another research project conducted
in the U.S. during COVID-19 showing that feelings of relatedness
were positively associated with well-being and lower levels of stress
(Cantarero, Tilburg, & Smoktunowicz, 2020); however, these
researchers only asked about perceptions of relatedness, not the
methods of communication people were using to stay in touch with
others. Our study is novel in its large sample size from two U.S. states
and measurement of use of mediated communication with friends and
family rather than global measures of relatedness.
Interestingly we found no relationship of changes in mediated
communication with shifts in negative emotions. Shifts in mediated
communication were not associated with increases or decreases in
negative emotions. There are several possible explanations for this.
One is that negative emotions may promote seeking out social sup-
port (Barbee & Cunningham, 1995); but once this social support is
obtained, then negative emotions decrease (Burleson, 1990;
Leatham & Duck, 1990). Thus, the relationship between increased
negative emotions and mediated communication would be positive at
the beginning of this cycle with more negative emotion leading to
more mediated communication with the goal of obtaining social sup-
port. Once the support was obtained the association between medi-
ated communication and negative emotions would be reversed, more
mediated communication would be associated with decreases in nega-
tive emotion. Thus, we may have had participants at different stages
in this process which made the effect average out overall. Another
possible explanation is that negative emotions from the situation sur-
rounding COVID-19 were so strongly tied to external factors (worries
about economic and health insecurity) that social support provided by
family or friends through mediated communication would not be able
to alleviate these negative emotions. Interestingly a previous longitu-
dinal study on disaster victims of the Mount St. Helens volcanic erup-
tion found that social support 1 year after the disaster did not
decrease distress felt by the victims 3 years after the event
(Murphy, 1988). Thus, negative emotions tied to extreme, external,
uncontrollable events, may not be responsive to the provision of
social support, however, more study is required to examine specifi-
cally which types of negative emotion and under what conditions.
Increases in mediated communication with both friends and fam-
ily were positively associated with increased well-being; however,
increases in mediated communication with friends were more strongly
related to increases in well-being than increases in mediated commu-
nication with family. There is some evidence that supports this finding
in the literature. For example, a large, multicounty survey study
showed that frequent in-person interactions with friends were more
strongly positively correlated with well-being than in-person interac-
tion with family (Helliwell & Putnam, 2004). Similarly, in a study on
retirees, researchers found that, although family members provide the
majority of social support for older adults, friendships have a stronger
influence on well-being (Larson, Mannell, & Zuzanek, 1986). There
may be several reasons why friends provide more effective social sup-
port and contribute more to well-being than family. First, as the adage
goes, you can pick your friends but not your family. Thus, negative
relationships with family may be maintained while unfulfilling friend-
ships can be discontinued (Pinquart & Sörensen, 2000). Second,
friendship interactions seem to be qualitatively different from family
interactions, with friends providing an escape from discussions of
daily needs and positive feedback that family members may not
BROWN AND GREENFIELD 153
provide (Larson et al., 1986). Finally, some research suggests that sup-
port from family may be more stable over time and therefore have
less impact in times of stress whereas support from friends may
increase during periods of stress (Dahlem, Zimet, & Walker, 1991;
Zimet, Dahlem, Zimet, & Farley, 1988). Governments as well as mental
health practitioners may want to consider promoting more online
communication between friends as a way for individuals to maintain
their psychological well-being during periods of social or physical
isolation.
5.3 |Age and mediated communication
Younger age was associated with increased video calling during the
stay-at-home order. Phone calls did not have any relationship with
age. Additionally, texting was not associated with age. One reason
that text messaging and age may not be related is because texting is
an accessible form of communication to older adults. While video call-
ing may require learning new skills and also may be difficult for hear-
ing impaired older individuals (Zamir et al., 2018), text messaging,
whether via a computer or phone, is more accessible. For older adults
who are visually impaired, they can enlarge text to make it easier to
read and for those who are hearing impaired, reading text may be eas-
ier than communicating via video calls or phone calls. Furthermore,
many seniors still do not use smartphones which can facilitate video
calls, but more of them do have home Internet access to send text
messages via a computer (Anderson & Perrin, 2017). Thus, different
types of mediated communication may be preferred by different age
groups due to their accessibility.
5.4 |Limitations and future directions
One limitation of our study is that the effect sizes on well-being vari-
ables that we found were small. In other words, increases in mediated
communication with friends and family only explained a small amount
of the observed variability in shifts in satisfaction with life and in posi-
tive emotions. However, any improvement in people's lives and emo-
tions during a period of confinement due to COVID-19 or any other
reason, should be considered important, particularly when the behav-
iors in this case (mediated communication) were so easy to engage
with and relatively cost effective as long as people had an Internet
connection or phone line. Government leaders should consider pro-
moting greater amounts of mediated communication, during periods
when in-person contact is not possible, to help their citizens maintain
their psychological well-being.
A further line of study may interest industry leaders. The current
study focused only on individual well-being, but our results may be
extended to work contexts. Future studies could examine whether
increases in mediated communication with co-workers during home
working can increase satisfaction with work and productivity. These
studies could also address which types of mediated communication
are most effective for different work tasks.
6|CONCLUSION
The current study shows how people have adapted to social isolation
due to stay-at-home orders issued to combat the COVID-19 out-
break. It shows that people compensate for a lack of in-person con-
tact by using emerging technologies like video calls and other forms
of mediated communication. Furthermore, our study shows that
increased mediated communication was associated with increases in
subjective well-being even when people were,physically, socially iso-
lated. This finding demonstrates the importance of technologically
mediated communication during periods of crisis as a way to promote
well-being through social connection.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We would like to acknowledge the help of Sanya Obsivac for rec-
ruiting participants for this study by designing and placing
Facebook ads.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
PEER REVIEW
The peer review history for this article is available at https://publons.
com/publon/10.1002/hbe2.246.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
Data are available on https://osf.io/mjnpv/.
ORCID
Genavee Brown https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3169-9067
REFERENCES
Anderson, M., & Perrin, A. (2017). Technology use among seniors.
Washington, DC: Pew Research Center for Internet & Technology.
Ang, C.-S., Talib, M. A., Tan, K.-A., Tan, J.-P., & Yaacob, S. N. (2015).
Understanding computer-mediated communication attributes and life
satisfaction from the perspectives of uses and gratifications and self-
determination. Computers in Human Behavior,49,2029.
Barbee, A. P., & Cunningham, M. R. (1995). An experimental approach to
social support communications: Interactive coping in close relation-
ships. Annals of the International Communication Association,18(1),
381413.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for
interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psy-
chological Bulletin,117(3), 497529.
boyd, d. (2014). It's complicated: The social lives of networked teens, New
Haven, CT: . Yale University Press.
Brown, G., & Michinov, N. (2017). Cultural differences in garnering social
capital on Facebook: French people prefer close ties and Americans
prefer distant ties. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research,46
(6), 579593.
Burleson, B. R. (1990). Comforting as social support: Relational conse-
quences of supportive behaviors. In: S. Duck (ed.), Personal Relation-
ships and Social Support, (pp. 6682). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., & Thisted, R. A. (2010). Perceived social iso-
lation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of loneliness and
depressive symptomatology in the Chicago health, aging, and social
relations study. Psychology and Aging,25(2), 453463.
154 BROWN AND GREENFIELD
Cantarero, K., van Tilburg, W. A., & Smoktunowicz, E. (2020). Affirming
basic psychological needs promotes mental well-being during the
COVID-19 outbreak. Social Psychological and Personality Science.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550620942708.
Cemalcilar, Z., Falbo, T., & Stapleton, L. M. (2005). Cyber communication:
A new opportunity for international students' adaptation? International
Journal of Intercultural Relations,29(1), 91110.
Chan, M. (2015). Multimodal connectedness and quality of life: Examining
the influences of technology adoption and interpersonal communica-
tion on well-being across the life span. Journal of Computer-Mediated
Communication,20(1), 318.
Chen, W., & Choi, A. S. K. (2011). Internet and social support among
Chinese migrants in Singapore. New Media & Society,13(7),
10671084.
Cheung, F., & Lucas, R. E. (2014). Assessing the validity of single-item life
satisfaction measures: Results from three large samples. Quality of Life
Research,23(10), 28092818.
Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering
hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin,98(2), 310357.
Cohen, S. (1988). Psychosocial models of the role of social support in the
etiology of physical disease. Health Psychology,7(3), 269.
Cohen-Mansfield, J., Hazan, H., Lerman, Y., & Shalom, V. (2016). Correlates
and predictors of loneliness in older-adults: A review of quantitative
results informed by qualitative insights. International Psychogeriatrics,
28(4), 557576.
Dahlem, N. W., Zimet, G. D., & Walker, R. R. (1991). The multidimensional
scale of perceived social support: A confirmation study. Journal of Clin-
ical Psychology,47(6), 756761.
Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. (2002). Very happy people. Psychological Sci-
ence,13(1), 8184.
Greenwood, S., Perrin, A., & Duggan, M. (2016). Social media update 2016.
Pew Research Center,11(2), 118.
Hartup, W. W., & Stevens, N. (1999). Friendships and adaptation across
the life span. Current Directions in Psychological Science,8(3), 7679.
Haythornthwaite, C. (2005). Social networks and internet connectivity
effects. Information, Community & Society,8(2), 125147.
Helliwell, J. F., & Putnam, R. D. (2004). The social context of wellbeing.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Bio-
logical Sciences,359(1449), 14351446.
Kimbrough, A. M., Guadagno, R. E., Muscanell, N. L., & Dill, J. (2013). Gen-
der differences in mediated communication: Women connect more
than do men. Computers in Human Behavior,29(3), 890900.
Kline, S. L., & Liu, F. (2005). The influence of comparative media use on
acculturation, acculturative stress, and family relationships of Chinese
international students. International Journal of Intercultural Relations,
29(4), 367390.
Larson, R., Mannell, R., & Zuzanek, J. (1986). Daily well-being of older
adults with friends and family. Psychology and Aging,1(2), 117126.
Leatham, G., & Duck, S. (1990). Conversations with friends and the dynam-
ics of social support. Personal Relationships and Social Support,5,129.
Manago, A. M., Brown, G., Lawley, K. A., & Anderson, G. (2019). Adoles-
cents' daily face-to-face and computer-mediated communication:
Associations with autonomy and closeness to parents and friends.
Developmental Psychology,56(1), 153164.
Molloy, R., Smith, C. L., & Wozniak, A. (2011). Internal migration in the
United States. Journal of Economic Perspectives,25(3), 173196.
Murphy, S. A. (1988). Mental distress and recovery in a high-risk bereave-
ment sample three years after untimely death. Nursing Research,37(1),
3035.
Pinquart, M., & Sörensen, S. (2000). Influences of socioeconomic status,
social network, and competence on subjective well-being in later life:
A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging,15(2), 187224.
Putnam, R. D. (2000). Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American
community. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.
Rodríguez-Pose, A., & Von Berlepsch, V. (2014). Social capital and individ-
ual happiness in Europe. Journal of Happiness Studies,15(2), 357386.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory: Basic psychologi-
cal needs in motivation, development, and wellness. New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Schiffrin, H., Edelman, A., Falkenstern, M., & Stewart, C. (2010). The asso-
ciations among computer-mediated communication, relationships, and
well-being. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking,13(3),
299306.
Segrin, C., & Flora, J. (2005). Family interaction and mental health. In: C.
Segrin & J. Flora, (eds.), Family Communication, (pp. 301323).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Shen, C., Wang, M. P., Chu, J. T., Wan, A., Viswanath, K., Chan, S. S. C., &
Lam, T. H. (2017). Sharing family life information through video calls
and other information and communication technologies and the asso-
ciation with family well-being: Population-based survey. JMIR Mental
Health,4(4), e57.
Sherman, L. E., Michikyan, M., & Greenfield, P. M. (2013). The effects of
text, audio, video, and in-person communication on bonding between
friends. Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyber-
space,7(2). https://doi.org/10.5817/CP2013-2-3.
Taylor, S., Landry, C., Paluszek, M., Fergus, T. A., McKay, D., &
Asmundson, G. J. (2020). Development and initial validation of the
COVID stress scales. Journal of Anxiety Disorders,72, 102232.
Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2007). Online communication and adoles-
cent well-being: Testing the stimulation versus the displacement
hypothesis. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication,12(4),
11691182.
WHO. (2020). Mental health and psychosocial considerations during the
COVID-19 outbreak, 18 March 2020.
Xie, B. (2008). Multimodal computer-mediated communication and social
support among older Chinese internet users. Journal of Computer-
Mediated Communication,13(3), 728750.
Yang, C.-c., & Brown, B. B. (2013). Motives for using Facebook, patterns of
Facebook activities, and late adolescents' social adjustment to college.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence,42(3), 403416.
Zamir, S., Hennessy, C. H., Taylor, A. H., & Jones, R. B. (2018). Video-calls
to reduce loneliness and social isolation within care environments for
older people: An implementation study using collaborative action
research. BMC Geriatrics,18(1), 62.
Zimet, G. D., Dahlem, N. W., Zimet, S. G., & Farley, G. K. (1988). The multi-
dimensional scale of perceived social support. Journal of Personality
Assessment,52(1), 3041.
AUTHOR BIOGRAPHIES
Dr. Genavee Brown is a Lecturer in Psychol-
ogy at Northumbria University, UK. She
earned her PhD in social psychology at Uni-
versity Rennes 2, France. She then completed
a postdoc examining the potential for pro-
moting creativity and social skills through an
online platform in the project ProFan
financed by the French Ministry of Education. Her research
focuses on how communication technologies like mobile phones
and social media influence social relationships. She her work also
examines how technology use is shaped by social ecologies and
culture. Recent publications have included work on how conspir-
acy beliefs discourage social distancing behaviors during the
BROWN AND GREENFIELD 155
COVID-19 pandemic and research on how teens use mobile
phones to promote intimacy with friends and parents.
Patricia Marks Greenfield received her Ph. D.
from Harvard University. She is Distinguished
Professor of Psychology at UCLA and mem-
ber of the American Academy of Arts and Sci-
ences. She directs Children's Digital Media
Center at Los Angeles, a collaboration
between UCLA and California State Univer-
sity, Los Angeles. She received the 2019 Outstanding Contribu-
tions to Cultural Psychology Award, given by the Advances in
Cultural Psychology Preconference, Society for Personality and
Social Psychology, and the Boesch Prize from the German Society
of Cultural Psychology. Her books include Mind and Media: The
Effects of Television, Video Games, and Computers, which was trans-
lated into nine languages and appeared in 2015 as a 30th anniver-
sary classic edition. With Noah and Gabriel Evers, she has
coauthored a second article in this special issue: COVID-19 shifts
mortality salience, activities, and values in the United States: A big
data analysis ofonline adaptation.
How to cite this article: Brown G, Greenfield PM. Staying
connected during stay-at-home: Communication with family
and friends and its association with well-being. Hum Behav &
Emerg Tech. 2021;3:147156. https://doi.org/10.1002/
hbe2.246
156 BROWN AND GREENFIELD
... Parents reported greater work-family stress, changes in work hours, arrangements and employment status, stress of fulfilling a teaching role for their child, and fear of the child or other family members contracting the virus (Al-Balushi & Essa, 2020;Shah et al., 2020). Children and their parents were often separated from extended family members, including grandparents, and had to find new ways to connect such as through video chat programs (Brown & Greenfield, 2021;Strouse et al., 2021). These family experiences were impacted by the changing landscape of local and regional infection rates, availability of testing kits and healthcare access, and safety measures and policies established within local communities (Prime et al., 2020;Quetsch et al., 2022). ...
... When a major event such as the COVID-19 pandemic impacts one or more of children's contexts, it's important to examine resulting changes in children's well-being (Bates et al., 2020;Bhatia, 2020;Brown & Greenfield, 2021;Evans et al., 2020;de Figueiredo et al., 2021;). Although still somewhat limited, prior studies and policy recommendations have represented the views of parents (e.g., Al-Balushi & Essa, 2020;Quetsch et al., 2022), educators (e.g., Dorner, et al., 2022), medical and mental health professionals (e.g., Gupta & Jawanda, 2020;Shah et al., 2020), and policymakers and child advocates (e.g., Fore, 2020;Masonbrink & Hurley, 2020). ...
... One, they turned to media and electronics to connect with peers and classmates. Previous studies have found that media use can positively contribute to children's well-being when it connects them to others and supports relationship-building (Brown & Greenfield, 2021;Newland et al., 2018). The other adaptation that they made was heading outside, when possible, to connect with nature, enjoy physical activities, manage their emotions, and deal with boredom. ...
Article
Full-text available
Children in rural areas are more likely to experience a variety of risk factors that increase their vulnerability to physical and mental health disparities. Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model (1986) was used as a framework for understanding rural children’s perceptions and well-being within multiple interactive contexts during the COVID-19 pandemic. This phenomenological study was designed to explore rural children’s perceptions of their well-being and the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on their contexts and well-being. This sub-study of the Children’s Understandings of Well-Being project followed the standard qualitative interview protocol with additional prompts related to the pandemic. Rural children (age 8 to 18, N = 72) from the Midwestern United States participated from March 2020 to November 2021 via teleconferencing. Phenomenological analyses of transcripts focused on the essence of children’s understanding of well-being and their perception of the impact of the pandemic on their contexts and well-being. Each transcript was coded by author 1 and verified by author 2, and discrepancies were identified, discussed, and resolved. The third author served as an external auditor to enhance trustworthiness. First-cycle coding focused on children’s specific references to well-being experiences during COVID-19. Second-cycle selective coding focused on specific well-being experiences and contexts that were impacted by COVID-19. These codes were used to develop two broad themes, “Well This Kinda Stinks, But We Just Adapt” and “Safety Means Something Different to Me Now.” The meaning of themes and subthemes are explored, with implications for researchers, practitioners, and policymakers.
... With the start of lockdown during the beginning of COVID-19, it was uncertain how long the significant changes that governments made would last. Those changes brought grand challenges to life, such as preventing the virus from spreading around the world (e.g., wearing a mask) (1), staying at home to protect children and vulnerable adults (2)(3)(4), and staying connected when physically apart (5)(6)(7). The World Health Organization reported that the global prevalence of anxiety and depression increased by a massive 25% in the first year of COVID-19 (8). ...
Article
Full-text available
The COVID-19 pandemic is causing an epidemic of loneliness. Previous studies have shown the differences in positive and negative experiences of lonely and non-lonely people in a non-pandemic setting. However, it is unclear how the drastic alteration of the COVID-19 pandemic may influence peoples' reactions and beliefs, especially among those who feel lonely. Our study aims to examine the positive and negative experiences among lonely and non-lonely people. We undertook a cross-sectional online survey of the general population in Germany (N = 1,758) from May 2020 to May 2022. We assessed their feelings of loneliness with the short eight-item UCLA Loneliness Scale (ULS-8), their positive and negative experience of living in the COVID-19 pandemic as well as their psychological distress regarding the pandemic with the COVID-19 Peritraumatic Distress Index (CPDI). We found lonely individuals (ULS-8 score ≥ 16) reported fewer positive experiences of living in the COVID-19 pandemic, for example, less time with loved ones [z(1, 756) = −2.5, p = 0.012] and less sense of togetherness [z(1, 756) = −2.39, p = 0.017] as compared to non-lonely individuals. Meanwhile, they experienced more negative experiences, for example, worry and fear [z(1, 756) = 6.31, p < 0.001] compared with non-lonely individuals. Interestingly, lonely people were less likely to view the pandemic as a conspiracy than non-lonely people were [z(1, 756) = −3.35, p < 0.001]. Our results may give insight into attribution bias and the negative affect of lonely people during the COVID-19 pandemic as well as highlight the experience of non-lonely people and raise the question of differences in conspiracy beliefs. For pandemic preparedness and response, decision-makers may focus on interventions to foster social cohesion, empower people, build resilience, and most importantly provide timely social care.
... potrebu stretávať sa alebo spoločne tráviť čas) najmä medzi staršími generáciami. Online prostredie v podstate poskytlo stimuláciu, náhradu a kompenzáciu priameho kontaktu zoči-voči (Brown & Greenfield, 2021). Tento spôsob komunikácie a "stretávania sa" však nedokázal plne nahradiť rodinné stretnutia spred pandémie. ...
Book
Full-text available
The period in which we live is characterised by tremendous dynamism. Everything is changing at a rapid pace, not least the content and form of education. Both the Slovak and Czech education systems have changed with the adoption of new school laws. We are feeling strong pressure from European structures to increase the proportion of university-educated people up to the level of 40%. Employers' expectations in relation to standards of educational results and professional competences are changing. We are facing the challenge of curricular transformation as the volume of 'core' knowledge continues to grow and we are trying to weave it into the content of education. We are facing the consequences of the decentralisation of education, but this does not prevent us from centralising the examination of knowledge in primary and secondary schools. There are critical voices saying that the current education system will soon fail because it is failing at its core, which is preparation for the future profession. These are just some of the current problems of education and its dynamics. Among the bulging issues that are opening up debate and strongly polarising the public are technological and electronic innovations. The use of the Internet, the flipping of social relationships into online identities, and the mass use of "smart" devices have all entered our lives significantly in the last 25 years. What was considered at the end of the 20th century to be a dependency on the Internet and technology is now the gold standard for interactions, but also for ways of self-presentation (e.g. a smartphone with a permanent Internet connection, whose hardware rivals gaming computers and high-end cameras). There is simply an intergenerational shift in how we perceive the necessity of using technology and the extent to which we use it in everyday life. People from the "Baby Boomers", "Husak's Children" and probably even "Millennials" can easily imagine life without technology and the Internet because they have had this experience. But Generation Y and α have not experienced such a life. The Internet and technology have been with them since birth. Chances are that at two years old, they knew the functions of a smartphone better than their parents. For many of them, the smartphone was a "nanny". And perhaps right now they use it for leisure, to communicate with friends and girlfriends, to look up information or pay for goods. Inevitably, there are two camps of people. In one are the opponents of mass use of technology and the Internet, including those seeking a technology detox and those who have unplugged. In the other are proponents of their use, including technology producers, app makers and distributors. Both sides have strong arguments that they use to support their position. The "against" side talks about the change in the neurophysiological qualities of the brain that causes us to be stupid. Some talk about "debilitation." There is a decline in our key abilities, such as the ability to communicate correctly, inhibit behavior, or expand the range of short-term memory. The 'pro' side talks about inevitable progress, the availability of information, networking, faster learning or the possibility of linking human consciousness with technology. As it happens, the truth will be somewhere in the middle. And some research carried out in the Czech Republic has indeed shown that as long as we use technology and the Internet in a reasonable way, it does not have to have negative consequences for us and we can use it for our development. In relation to the massiveness of the aforementioned phenomenon, we have no choice but to accept that the Internet and technology are an integral part of our lives, for current and future adolescents. If we look at the recent period, roughly defined as 03/2020 to 03/2022, associated with the Covid-19 pandemic and its impact on education, regardless of our attitude towards technology and the Internet, we must admit that they have been a lifesaver for us if we wanted to protect health and, at the same time, continue to educate children and adolescents. Teachers were confronted with new challenges from one day to the next, which consisted in changing the form of education and were associated with a number of technical and competence problems. Online education had been in operation for almost two years and computers, tablets, telephones, various communication applications were the common means of conveying information. Last but not least, educational videos gained popularity. Many older ones have been dusted off and a huge number of new ones have been created to make it easier for children and adolescents to learn educational content. They have their drawbacks, such as the lack of social interaction and the opportunity to ask questions. But they also have their upsides, such as greater clarity, the ability to proceed at one's own pace or to retrace previous steps. They appear to be a suitable complementary method that could find a firm place in both institutionalised and non-institutionalised education. We have addressed this content in our publication. We were interested in how adolescents were educated during the Covid-19 pandemic, how they used educational videos, whether their use was related to changes in the form of education, and how they perceived them.
... Second, we aimed to assess four niche construction processes correspondingly. Two are organized around the person's striving for agency and communion [35] that shape the place experience. The two other processes entail emotional states as feedback loops in the process of self-regulation [36]; they include self-recovery and distress, as described by Korpela and colleagues [28]. ...
Article
Full-text available
In challenging times, home is frequently the primary basis of environmental self-regulation processes, individual and relational coping, and well-being. This study aimed to identify multiple types of security experiences at home during the first lockdown period of the COVID-19 pandemic. We used data from 757 Hungarian adults who completed the online, modified form of the Emotional Map of the Home Interview method in 2020 after the outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic. Participants imagined their homes, chose the place of security in their homes and rated their personal experiences (i.e., experiences of agency, communion, self-recovery, and distress) related to these places. Latent profile analysis of personal experiences revealed four types of relational-environmental self-regulation in secure places: “security in active self-recovery,” “security in detachment,” “security in doing and feeling good enough,” and “security in stress and compensation.” Profile membership was predicted by age, gender, and indices of psychological support and well-being. Results suggest that finding psychological security in the home is a multifaceted phenomenon that may be partly affected by the perception of the broader social-ecological context. Identifying subpopulations vulnerable to the challenges of the pandemic may help researchers and practitioners provide better support in times of local and global crises.
... Forward. The COVID-19 pandemic changed the landscape of daily life [51]. While much attention was paid to its implications for work, education, and childcare, less attention was paid to what a global lockdown and forced isolation meant for grandparents and family systems. ...
Article
Full-text available
Many grandparents today are physically separated from their families. Given that maintaining close family relationships (with both adult children and grandchildren) is associated with increased physical, mental, and emotional health across generations, it is important to determine how families can maintain close relationships with grandparents when physically separated. Technology offers one potential support: the proliferation of video chat. Recent work suggests that the frequency of video chat and the variety of behaviors engaged in during video calls predicts family closeness and enjoyment of using this communication method, regardless of the physical distance between parties. However, the frequency of grandparent-grandchild video chat varies across families. Here, we explore how demographic, physical (e.g., distance), technological (e.g., number of devices and barriers), and social factors (e.g., children are distracted) predicted: (1) whether or not video chat was used by grandparent-grandchild dyads, and (2) the frequency of video chat in the families using the technology. This work suggests that geographical distance, having met in person, and the number of devices owned were positive predictors of grandparents and grandchildren having ever video chatted. However, the factors associated with the frequency of video chat were different in the parent and grandparent models and included grandparents’ comfort with technology and the type of device used by the parent and child. These findings not only have implications for supporting grandparent-grandchild relationships but also for all family members separated by distance, immigration, incarceration, health emergencies, and displacement.
... Complexities around the role of technology in literacy and social-emotional development have come to the fore over the past decade, and most dramatically during mass quarantining in response to COVID-19. During the pandemic, children across America relied on a digital device as their only connection to school and classmates, and longduration use of such devices increased significantly (Brown & Greenfield, 2021). Quality of reading instruction available to youth varied substantially during COVID-19 lockdowns, in part due to technological constraints and access (Chamberlain et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Current conversations in literacy research call for the need to consider children’s social-emotional development and academic learning in an integrated way that honors and supports the whole child in their cultural context. Here we review available literatures on the cognitive, linguistic, affective, social and cultural dimensions of typical and atypical reading acquisition and what is known of their neural correlates. From this review, and in consultation with neuro- and psychological scientists and expert educators working in a range of contexts, we derive a series of interdisciplinary Learnings. These describe what is known about how diverse children grow into readers and how literacy development can be optimally supported in school, home and community contexts across the first decades of life. The Learnings together provide a critical cross-disciplinary synthesis of how literacy and social-emotional development are coordinated, individually variable, dynamic and sensitive to contextual influences. They reveal how evidence-based, culturally inclusive, socially attentive and developmentally appropriate instruction is an essential piece of helping every child organize their brain and mind for literacy. Such comprehensive instruction is necessary to address inequities in general and special education, and to help children become fully literate—a designation that goes beyond simply decoding and comprehending text. Impacts of digital technology, and other timely issues and open questions, are also discussed.
... Academics reported using technology to stay connected to colleagues. Other studies report the importance of connection and use of technology for people to stay in contact (Brown and Greenfield, 2021). ...
Article
Background The COVID-19 global pandemic was declared in March 2020. By June 2022, the total deaths worldwide attributed to COVID-19 numbered over 6.3 million. Health professionals have been significantly impacted worldwide primarily those working on the frontline but also those working in other areas including nursing, midwifery, and paramedic higher education. Studies of occupational stress have focused on the clinical health professional roles but scant attention has been drawn to the pressures on university-based academic staff supporting and preparing professionals for frontline health work. Design and objectives This qualitative study sought to explore the challenges experienced by health academics (nurses, midwives and paramedics), during COVID-19 and identify strategies enlisted. Setting and participants Six Australian and two United Kingdom universities collaborated, from which 34 health academics were individually interviewed via video or teleconference, using six broad questions. Ethical approval was obtained from the lead site and each participating University. Data analysis Thematic analysis of the data was employed collaboratively across institutions, using Braun and Clarke's method. Results Data analysis generated four major themes describing academics': Experiences of change; perceptions of organisational responses; professional and personal impacts; and strategies to support wellbeing. Stress, anxiety and uncertainty of working from home and teaching in a different way were reported. Strategies included setting workday routine, establishing physical boundaries for home-working and regular online contact with colleagues. Conclusions The ability of nursing, midwifery and, paramedic academic staff to adapt to a sudden increase in workload, change in teaching practices and technology, while being removed from their work environment, and collegial, academic and technological supports is highlighted. It was recognised that these changes will continue post-COVID and that the way academics deliver education is forever altered.
... Similarly, it has been suggested, that digital technology may assist in reducing loneliness and mitigate the negative effects of social distancing 45,46 . For example, recent studies have shown that increased virtual social interactions with close others during the pandemic were linked to higher levels of wellbeing in both younger and older adults 47,48 . Moreover, the Zoom platform allowed forming and maintaining rapports between researcher and participant in a recent study 49 . ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Although the link between synchrony and emotional contagion has been studied extensively during face-to-face interaction, the question of whether this association also exists in virtual settings has remained unanswered. Here, we examined whether this link exists during virtual social interactions and whether pro-social effects will be induced during those interactions. To this end, two strangers shared difficulties they have experienced due to the COVID-19 pandemic during a virtual social interaction. The findings revealed that spontaneous motor synchrony and emotional contagion can arise during a virtual social interaction between two strangers and that this kind of interaction has pro-social effects. It can thus be presumed that virtual social interactions may share similar characteristics and social effects with face-to-face interactions. Considering the tremendous changes COVID-19 epidemic has caused regarding social communication, these findings may provide grounds for developing new intervention protocols aiming at dealing with the consequences of social distancing.
Article
Full-text available
The COVID-19 pandemic has put more than just our physical health at risk. Due to containment measures, people have become increasingly isolated and have drastically reduced their daily social interactions. Many studies have already shown the negative effects of these measures, including fatalism. However, research linking fatalism during COVID-19 to well-being indicators is still limited. The goal of this study is to examine the relationship between COVID-19-related fatalism and well-being indicators, as well as the role of loneliness in moderating this relationship. Data was collected from 1,036 adults in Peru through an online survey that included the Quality-of-Life Index, the Fatalism Facing COVID-19 Scale, the Loneliness Scale, and the Mood Assessment Scale. Three models were tested using linear regression and ordinary least squares with bias-corrected bootstrapping. The results indicate that fatalism has a negative impact on quality of life and a positive effect on negative affect, and loneliness moderates both relationships, supporting the conclusion that fatalism exacerbates the effect of well-being indicators and negative affect.
Article
Full-text available
We tested if challenges to basic psychological needs (BPN) for autonomy, competence, and relatedness during the COVID-19 pandemic undermine people’s mental well-being. Furthermore, we tested if an intervention, affirmation of these psychological needs, enhances mental well-being. Results of Study 1 ( N = 153) showed that higher levels of satisfaction of BPN were related to higher well-being during the COVID-19 outbreak. In Study 2 ( N = 215), we employed an online intervention enhancing these BPN. We found increased mental well-being through bolstered relatedness in particular. The intervention also decreased perceived stress. Both studies showed that mental well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic is positively related to the ability to work as usual and the number of people contacted via phone or internet but not in person.
Article
Full-text available
Research and clinical observations suggest that during times of pandemic many people exhibit stress- or anxiety-related psychopathology that include fear of becoming infected, fear of coming into contact with possibly contaminated objects or surfaces, fear of foreigners who might be carrying infection (i.e., disease-related xenophobia), fear of the socio-economic consequences of the pandemic, compulsive checking and reassurance-seeking regarding possible pandemic-related threats, and traumatic stress symptoms about the pandemic (e.g., nightmares, intrusive thoughts). We developed the 36-item COVID Stress Scales (CSS) to measure these features, as they pertain to COVID-19. The CSS were developed to better understand and assess COVID-19-related psychopathology. The scales were intentionally designed so they could be readily adapted for future pandemics. The CSS were developed and initially validated in population-representative samples from Canada (N = 3,479) and the United States (N = 3,375). A stable 5-factor solution was identified, corresponding to scales assessing COVID-related stress and anxiety symptoms: (1) Danger and contamination fears, (2) fears about economic consequences, (3) xenophobia, (4) compulsive checking, and (5) traumatic stress symptoms about COVID-19. The scales performed well on various indices of reliability and validity. The scales were intercorrelated, providing evidence of a COVID Stress Syndrome. The scales offer promise as tools for better understanding the psychopathology associated with COVID-19 and for identifying people in need of mental health services.
Article
Full-text available
The amount of time adolescents spend communicating via digital technologies such as smartphones has led to concerns that computer-mediated communication (CMC) is displacing face-to-face (FtF) interactions and disrupting social development. Although many studies have examined CMC in adolescents' relationships with friends, few studies have examined the role of CMC in adolescents' renegotiation of closeness and autonomy with parents. To examine this issue, we administered an online daily diary with 169 U.S. adolescents to estimate the time they spend in CMC and FtF interactions and the number of texts they exchange with friends and parents. On the last day of the survey, we asked adolescents about their emotional closeness to friends and parents and their balance of closeness and volition with parents. Overall, we found more evidence for social stimulation than displacement effects of CMC. Texts and CMC time with friends predicted friend closeness after accounting for FtF time with friends; texts with parents predicted parent closeness after accounting for FtF time with parents. We also found support for our hypothesis that CMC would be associated with greater adolescent volition. CMC time with parents predicted greater volitional dependence (volition plus closeness) whereas texts with friends predicted greater independent decision-making (volition plus distance). We discuss how communication technologies are integrated into U.S. adolescents' relationships with friends and parents and how CMC can facilitate, rather than stifle, adolescents' adjustment of autonomy-relatedness with parents and their construction of emotional closeness with friends. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Full-text available
Background Older people in care may be lonely with insufficient contact if families are unable to visit. Face-to-face contact through video-calls may help reduce loneliness, but little is known about the processes of engaging people in care environments in using video-calls. We aimed to identify the barriers to and facilitators of implementing video-calls for older people in care environments. MethodsA collaborative action research (CAR) approach was taken to implement a video-call intervention in care environments. We undertook five steps of recruitment, planning, implementation, reflection and re-evaluation, in seven care homes and one hospital in the UK. The video-call intervention ‘Skype on Wheels’ (SoW) comprised a wheeled device that could hold an iPad and handset, and used Skype to provide a free video-call service. Care staff were collaborators who implemented the intervention within the care-setting by agreeing the intervention, recruiting older people and their family, and setting up video-calls. Field notes and reflective diaries on observations and conversations with staff, older people and family were maintained over 15 months, and analysed using thematic analysis. ResultsFour care homes implemented the intervention. Eight older people with their respective social contacts made use of video-calls. Older people were able to use SoW with assistance from staff, and enjoyed the use of video-calls to stay better connected with family. However five barriers towards implementation included staff turnover, risk averseness, the SoW design, lack of family commitment and staff attitudes regarding technology. Conclusions The SoW intervention, or something similar, could aid older people to stay better connected with their families in care environments, but if implemented as part of a rigorous evaluation, then co-production of the intervention at each recruitment site may be needed to overcome barriers and maximise engagement.
Article
Full-text available
Background: The use of information and communication technologies (ICTs) for information sharing among family members is increasing dramatically. However, little is known about the associated factors and the influence on family well-being. Objective: The authors investigated the pattern and social determinants of family life information sharing with family and the associations of different methods of sharing with perceived family health, happiness, and harmony (3Hs) in Hong Kong, where mobile phone ownership and Internet access are among the most prevalent, easiest, and fastest in the world. Methods: A territory-wide population-based telephone survey was conducted from January to August 2016 on different methods of family life information (ie, information related to family communication, relationships with family members, emotion and stress management) sharing with family members, including face-to-face, phone, instant messaging (IM), social media sites, video calls, and email. Family well-being was assessed by three single items on perceived family health, happiness, and harmony, with higher scores indicating better family well-being. Adjusted prevalence ratios were used to assess the associations of sociodemographic factors with family life information sharing, and adjusted beta coefficients for family well-being. Results: Of 2017 respondents, face-to-face was the most common method to share family life information (74.45%, 1502/2017), followed by IM (40.86%, 824/2017), phone (28.10%, 567/2017), social media sites (11.91%, 240/2017), video calls (5.89%, 119/2017), and email (5.48%, 111/2017). Younger age and higher education were associated with the use of any (at least one) method, face-to-face, IM, and social media sites for sharing family life information (all P for trend <.01). Higher education was most strongly associated with the use of video calls (adjusted prevalence ratio=5.61, 95% CI 2.29-13.74). Higher household income was significantly associated with the use of any method, face-to-face, and IM (all P for trend <.05). Sharing family life information was associated with a higher level of perceived family well-being (beta=0.56, 95% CI 0.37-0.75), especially by face-to-face (beta=0.62, 95% CI 0.45-0.80) and video calls (beta=0.34, 95% CI 0.04-0.65). The combination of face-to-face and video calls was most strongly associated with a higher level of perceived family well-being (beta=0.81, 95% CI 0.45-1.16). Conclusions: The differential use of ICTs to share family life information was observed. The prevalence of video calls was low, but associated with much better family well-being. The results need to be confirmed by prospective and intervention studies to promote the use of video calls to communicate and share information with family, particularly in disadvantaged groups.
Article
Full-text available
The link between social capital (SC) and Facebook has been widely studied in the U.S., and less is known about how students from different cultures use the site to garner SC. We measured network composition, communication on Facebook, and SC via questionnaires in France and the U.S. We found that American students have a greater proportion of distant to close ties in their networks and higher levels of bridging SC than French, the latter preferring bonding SC. A stronger relationship between SC and communication via Facebook was observed in the U.S. These findings are explained by cultural differences in relatedness.
Article
This review covers four major topics. First, the authors discuss previous studies on social support that document the content of supportive communications. Next, the discussion turns to the development and validation of the Interactive Coping Behavior Coding System and its converse, the Support Activation Behavior Coding System. A third focus is research stemming from sensitive interaction systems theory, which makes predictions, based on numerous variables, concerning whether an interaction will be ameliorative or harmful. The final section presents findings on the effects of interactive coping variables on relationship maintenance.