Content uploaded by Jackson G. Lu
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Jackson G. Lu on Jan 20, 2021
Content may be subject to copyright.
RegisTeRed RepoRT
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-01007-2
People quickly and involuntarily form impressions of others
based on their facial appearance1–3. These impressions then
influence important social outcomes4,5. For example, people
are more likely to cooperate in socioeconomic interactions with
individuals whose faces are evaluated as more trustworthy6, vote for
individuals whose faces are evaluated as more competent7, and seek
romantic relationships with individuals whose faces are evaluated as
more attractive8. Facial appearance can even influence life-or-death
outcomes. For example, untrustworthy-looking defendants are
more likely to receive death sentences9. Given that such evaluations
influence profound outcomes, understanding how people evaluate
others’ faces can provide insight into a potentially important route
through which social stereotypes impact behaviour10,11.
Over the past decade, Oosterhof and Todorov’s valence–domi-
nance model12 has emerged as the most prominent account of how
we evaluate faces on social dimensions5. Oosterhof and Todorov
identified 13 different traits (aggressiveness, attractiveness, car-
ingness, confidence, dominance, emotional stability, unhappiness,
intelligence, meanness, responsibility, sociability, trustworthiness
and weirdness) that perceivers spontaneously use to evaluate faces
when forming trait impressions12. From these traits, they derived
a two-dimensional model of perception: valence and dominance.
Valence, best characterized by rated trustworthiness, was defined
as the extent to which the target was perceived as having the inten-
tion to harm the viewer12. Dominance, best characterized by rated
dominance, was defined as the extent to which the target was per-
ceived as having the ability to inflict harm on the viewer12. Crucially,
the model proposes that these two dimensions are sufficient to
drive social evaluations of faces. As a consequence, the majority of
research on the effects of social evaluations of faces has focused on
one or both of these dimensions4,5.
Successful replications of the valence–dominance model have
only been conducted in Western samples13,14. This focus on the West
is consistent with research on human behaviour more broadly, which
typically draws general assumptions from analyses of Western par-
ticipants’ responses15. Kline etal.16 recently termed this problematic
practice the Western centrality assumption and argued that regional
variation, rather than universality, is probably the default for
human behaviour.
Consistent with Kline etal.’s notion that human behaviour is
best characterized by regional variation, two recent studies of social
evaluation of faces by Chinese participants indicate that different
factors underlie their impressions17,18. Both studies reported that
Chinese participants’ social evaluations of faces were underpinned
by a valence dimension similar to that reported by Oosterhof and
Todorov for Western participants, but not by a corresponding
dominance dimension. Instead, both studies reported a second
dimension, referred to as capability, which was best characterized
by rated intelligence. Furthermore, the ethnicity of the faces rated
only subtly affected perceptions17. Research into potential cultural
differences in the effects of experimentally manipulated facial char-
acteristics on social perceptions has also found little evidence that
cultural differences in social perceptions of faces depend on the eth-
nicity of the faces presented19–21. Collectively, these results suggest
that the Western centrality assumption may be an important barrier
to understanding how people evaluate faces on social dimensions.
Crucially, these studies also suggest that the valence–dominance
model is not necessarily a universal account of social evaluations
of faces and warrants further investigation in the broadest set of
samples possible.
Although the studies described above demonstrate that the
valence–dominance model is not perfectly universal, to which spe-
cific world regions it does and does not apply are open and impor-
tant questions. Demonstrating differences between British and
Chinese raters is evidence against the universality of the valence–
dominance model, but it does not adequately address these ques-
tions. Social perception in China may be unique in not fitting the
valence–dominance model because of the atypically high general
importance placed on status-related traits, such as capability, during
social interactions in China22,23. Indeed, Tan etal.24 demonstrated
face-processing differences between Chinese participants living in
mainland China and Chinese participants living in nearby coun-
tries, such as Malaysia. Insights regarding the unique formation of
social perceptions in other cultures and world regions are lacking.
To which world regions does the valence–
dominance model of social perception apply?
Over the past 10 years, Oosterhof and Todorov’s valence–dominance model has emerged as the most prominent account of
how people evaluate faces on social dimensions. In this model, two dimensions (valence and dominance) underpin social
judgements of faces. Because this model has primarily been developed and tested in Western regions, it is unclear whether
these findings apply to other regions. We addressed this question by replicating Oosterhof and Todorov’s methodology across
11 world regions, 41 countries and 11,570 participants. When we used Oosterhof and Todorov’s original analysis strategy,
the valence–dominance model generalized across regions. When we used an alternative methodology to allow for correlated
dimensions, we observed much less generalization. Collectively, these results suggest that, while the valence–dominance
model generalizes very well across regions when dimensions are forced to be orthogonal, regional differences are revealed
when we use different extraction methods and correlate and rotate the dimension reduction solution.
Protocol registration
The stage 1 protocol for this Registered Report was accepted in principle on 5 November 2018. The protocol, as accepted by the
journal, can be found at https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.7611443.v1.
A full list of author affiliations appears at the end of the paper.
NATURE HUMAN BEHAVIOUR | VOL 5 | JANUARY 2021 | 159–169 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav 159
RegisTeRed RepoRT Nature HumaN BeHaviour
Only a large-scale study investigating social perceptions in many
different world regions can provide such insights.
To establish the world regions to which the valence–dominance
model applies, we replicated Oosterhof and Todorov’s methodol-
ogy12 in a wide range of world regions (Africa, Asia, Australia and
New Zealand, Central America and Mexico, Eastern Europe, the
Middle East, the United States and Canada, Scandinavia, South
America, the United Kingdom and Western Europe; see Table 1).
Our study is the most comprehensive test of social evaluations of
faces to date, including more than 11,000 participants. Participating
research groups were recruited via the Psychological Science
Accelerator project25–27. Previous studies compared two cultures to
demonstrate regional differences17,18. In contrast, the scale and scope
of our study allows us to generate the most comprehensive picture
of the world regions to which the valence–dominance model does
and does not apply.
We tested two specific competing predictions: (1) the valence–
dominance model applies to all world regions; and (2) the valence–
dominance model applies in Western-world regions, but not other
world regions.
Results
Analysed dataset. Following the planned data exclusions (see the
Supplementary Information for a breakdown of these exclusions;
code 1.5), the analysed dataset is summarized in Table 2.
Main analysis (principal component analysis (PCA); code 2.1).
Oosterhof and Todorov reported the results of a PCA with orthogo-
nal components, no rotation and retaining components with eigen-
values of >1. We conducted an identical analysis and report: (1)
the number of components extracted per the registered criteria; (2)
whether the first and second components had the same primary pat-
tern as Oosterhof and Todorov reported; and (3) the similarity of the
first and second factors as quantified with a congruence coefficient.
We extrac ted the same number of components (two) as Oosterhof
and Todorov in two world regions (Africa and South America)
and a different number of components (three) in the other world
regions (see Fig. 1). In the world regions where a third compo-
nent was extracted, the trait ratings of unhappy and weird tended
to have the highest loadings on that component, but those ratings
also crossloaded on the first component. We hesitate to interpret or
describe this component with any authority because it varied across
world regions, consisted of crossloaded traits and explained only a
small proportion of additional variance.
The primary pattern reported by Oosterhof and Todorov (a first
component that correlated strongly with rated trustworthiness but
not with rated dominance and a second component that correlated
strongly with rated dominance but not with rated trustworthiness)
was present in all world regions except Eastern Europe. In Eastern
Europe, dominance was correlated with the first component more
strongly than our registered criterion (i.e., that dominance would
correlate weakly with the first component; r < 0.5). Figure 1 shows
the full loading matrices for each region and Table 3 shows how
these relate to our registered criteria.
We report Tucker’s coefficient of congruence, ϕ, which quantifies
the loading similarity of Oosterhof and Todorov’s reported compo-
nent to the corresponding component we extracted. However, it is
important to interpret ϕ with caution when the numbers of compo-
nents differ across the solutions being compared. When comparing
loadings across solutions, an assumption is that the configuration of
the traits to components is the same (that is, configural invariance).
To the extent that the structures of the loading matrices differ across
solutions, the comparability of the loadings is compromised (that is,
loadings estimated from different dimensional spaces are not on the
same scale). For world regions that did not have the same configura-
tion of traits to components (that is, those with a different number
of components extracted or a different primary pattern observed), ϕ
was uninterpretable. This is because the differences in configuration
across the two solutions were conflated with the loading differences.
Our analyses indicated that the first component was equal to
the first component in Oosterhof and Todorov’s original study for
all world regions (ϕ > 0.95). The second component was equal to
(ϕ > 0.95) or fairly similar to (ϕ > 0.85) the second component
reported by Oosterhof and Todorov in all of the world regions
except Asia (ϕ = 0.848). Table 4 summarizes these results.
Together, these results suggest that the valence–dominance
model generalizes across world regions when using an identical
analysis to that used in Oosterhof and Todorov’s original study.
Thus, the results of our PCA support prediction 1 (that the valence–
dominance model will apply to all world regions) but not prediction
2 (that the valence–dominance model will apply in Western-world
regions but not other world regions). However, we note here that in
most world regions we extracted a third component not extracted
in the original study: that Eastern Europe did not demonstrate the
same primary pattern and that ϕ should be interpreted with caution
for all world regions except Africa and South America.
Robustness analyses (exploratory factor analysis (EFA); code
2.2). Following our analysis plan, we conducted additional robust-
ness analyses that directly addressed criticisms of the type of sta-
tistical analyses used by Oosterhof and Todorov (see ref. 28 for a
discussion of these criticisms). These robustness analyses employed
EFA with an oblimin rotation as the model and used parallel analy-
sis to identify the number of factors to extract. The goal of an EFA
with an oblimin rotation is to simplify the loading matrix and yield
interpretable factors.
We conducted this analysis on Oosterhof and Todorov’s original
data and found a similar result to their PCA solution: two factors
extracted, with factor 1 characterized by a high loading for trustwor-
thiness and factor 2 characterized by a high loading for dominance.
Table 1 | World regions, countries and localities of data
collection
World region Countries and localities
Africa Kenya, (Nigeria) and South Africa
Asia China, India, Malaysia, Taiwan and Thailand
Australia and New
Zealand Australia and New Zealand
Central America and
Mexico El Salvador and Mexico
Eastern Europe Hungary, Lithuania, Poland, Russia, Serbia and
Slovakia
The Middle East Iran, Israel and Turkey
United States and
Canada Canada and the United States
Scandinavia Denmark, (Finland), Norway and (Sweden)
South America Argentina, Brazil, Chile, Colombia and
Ecuador
United Kingdom England, Scotland and Wales
Western Europe Austria, Belgium, France, Germany, (Greece),
Italy, the Netherlands, Portugal, Spain and
Switzerland
We collected data from a minimum of 350 raters per world region based on the simulations
described in the Methods. Countries in parentheses were added to the list after acceptance in
principle of the stage 1 protocol. Ecuador was incorrectly classified as Central America and Mexico
in our stage 1 submission, but has been classified as South America for analyses and in our stage
2 submission.
NATURE HUMAN BEHAVIOUR | VOL 5 | JANUARY 2021 | 159–169 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav
160
RegisTeRed RepoRT
Nature HumaN BeHaviour
However, for all other world regions, we extracted more than two
factors using parallel analysis. Full EFA loading matrices for each
region and Oosterhof and Todorov’s original data are shown in
Fig. 2. The four-factor solution for the USA and Canada did not
converge. We did not register a contingency for nonconvergence,
but because parallel analysis can lead to over extraction, we reran
the EFA with one fewer than the number of suggested factors. The
model converged when estimating three factors.
In contrast with the PCA, the results of our robustness analyses
showed less evidence that the valence–dominance model generalizes
across world regions. For example, we extracted a different number
of factors than the original solution for all world regions. A summary
of the results for our replication criteria is given in Table 5.
Because the number of factors differed from the original solu-
tion in all world regions and the loading matrices were differen-
tially rotated from the original solution, it is not valid to compare
the differences in the loadings from the original solution with those
observed in the world regions reported here, as we had initially
planned. Loadings quantify the relationship of traits to a factor. To
compare loadings across samples, we must first determine whether
we extracted the same factor in each sample (that is, satisfied the
assumption of configural invariance). Our registered analyses
included the calculation of Tucker’s coefficient of congruence, ϕ in
order to compare the first factor from the original study with the
first factor we extracted in a given world region, and to compare
the second factor from the original study with the second factor
extracted in a given world region. However, because we extracted
a different number of factors from the original solution in all world
regions, it is not valid to compare the loadings across these different
factors, or to quantify their differences using ϕ.
The congruence coefficient is only appropriate to report when
we can ensure that the factors are comparable across samples. That
the number of factors extracted did not replicate the original pattern
and that the EFAs were rotated differently across world regions
negates the comparability of the loadings. Consistent with our
registered analysis code, we reported ϕ for the relationship of the
first factor from Oosterhof and Todorov to the factor with the most
explained variance in a world region, and ϕ for the relationship of
the second factor from Oosterhof and Todorov to the factor with
the second most explained variance in a world region only in the
Supplementary Information. However, we stress that these coeffi-
cients are quantifying loadings that link to different factors from
different dimensional spaces and are not necessarily comparable.
In summary, the results of our EFA support neither prediction 1
(that the valence–dominance model will apply to all world regions)
nor prediction 2 (that the valence–dominance model will apply to
Western-world regions but not other world regions).
Discussion
Our primary analyses—PCAs identical to those reported by
Oosterhof and Todorov—suggested that the valence–dominance
model of social perception of faces generalizes well across world
regions. Although most world regions showed a third component
not discussed in the original work12, this third component is actually
similar to the third component in Oosterhof and Todorov’s origi-
nal study. In Oosterhof and Todorov’s original study, they did not
interpret the third component because its eigenvalue was below 1,
whereas in our analyses the eigenvalues of the third components
in most of the regions were just above 1. Nonetheless, the third
component in each region had a factor congruence between 0.77
and 0.90 with the third component for Oosterhof and Todorov’s
data. However, we emphasize here that many of these dimensions
accounted for a relatively small proportion of the variance explained
and, thus, may be of limited theoretical importance.
In contrast with the results of our PCAs, an alternative analysis
that addressed common criticisms of the type of analysis Oosterhof
and Todorov employed showed much less generalization across
world regions. We used modern extraction techniques and EFAs
with correlated factor rotations. The correlated rotation meth-
ods aim to simplify the loading matrix with the goal of estimating
Table 2 | Number of participants per region and Cronbach’s α values following data quality checks and exclusions
Region Aggressive Attractive Caring Confident Dominant Emotionally
stable
Intelligent Mean Responsible Sociable Trustworthy Unhappy Weird
Western Europe α=0.978
n=152
α=0.991
n=147
α=0.976
n=136
α=0.985
n=156
α=0.973
n=150
α=0.981
n=141
α=0.975
n=141
α=0.969
n=120
α=0.978
n=138
α=0.988
n=188
α=0.978
n=141
α=0.983
n=140
α=0.982
n=113
United States and
Canada
α=0.983
n=248
α=0.991
n=224
α=0.986
n=257
α=0.989
n=303
α=0.977
n=246
α=0.986
n=270
α=0.979
n=239
α=0.984
n=270
α=0.984
n=269
α=0.988
n=246
α=0.984
n=263
α=0.985
n=252
α=0.987
n=226
United Kingdom α=0.879
n=16
α=0.949
n=22
α=0.936
n=34
α=0.93
n=30
α=0.886
n=34
α=0.9
n=30
α=0.911
n=34
α=0.87
n=27
α=0.892
n=37
α=0.932
n=28
α=0.92
n=27
α=0.937
n=24
α=0.899
n=18
South America α=0.948
n=97
α=0.982
n=108
α=0.944
n=112
α=0.968
n=108
α=0.957
n=121
α=0.949
n=100
α=0.938
n=110
α=0.949
n=95
α=0.937
n=117
α=0.974
n=110
α=0.952
n=107
α=0.961
n=87
α=0.973
n=116
Scandinavia α=0.95
n=48
α=0.969
n=44
α=0.949
n=46
α=0.96
n=56
α=0.941
n=49
α=0.955
n=67
α=0.958
n=54
α=0.912
n=36
α=0.915
n=37
α=0.969
n=64
α=0.949
n=58
α=0.952
n=55
α=0.952
n=39
Middle East α=0.912
n=32
α=0.949
n=32
α=0.934
n=42
α=0.943
n=39
α=0.9
n=35
α=0.903
n=33
α=0.896
n=48
α=0.901
n=36
α=0.87
n=34
α=0.944
n=41
α=0.895
n=42
α=0.943
n=57
α=0.896
n=32
Eastern Europe α=0.941
n=59
α=0.971
n=58
α=0.926
n=56
α=0.946
n=60
α=0.952
n=74
α=0.923
n=56
α=0.939
n=64
α=0.937
n=68
α=0.953
n=65
α=0.955
n=68
α=0.937
n=54
α=0.964
n=74
α=0.956
n=53
Central America
and Mexico
α=0.845
n=26
α=0.93
n=25
α=0.788
n=24
α=0.89
n=32
α=0.859
n=33
α=0.835
n=23
α=0.832
n=33
α=0.817
n=23
α=0.824
n=22
α=0.882
n=28
α=0.851
n=27
α=0.771
n=27
α=0.842
n=15
Australia and
New Zealand
α=0.956
n=77
α=0.98
n=88
α=0.964
n=90
α=0.972
n=93
α=0.936
n=66
α=0.957
n=88
α=0.951
n=81
α=0.947
n=71
α=0.937
n=68
α=0.972
n=95
α=0.953
n=72
α=0.948
n=85
α=0.962
n=70
Asia α=0.932
n=59
α=0.957
n=52
α=0.948
n=73
α=0.959
n=72
α=0.917
n=55
α=0.908
n=55
α=0.927
n=64
α=0.909
n=51
α=0.931
n=63
α=0.952
n=65
α=0.93
n=61
α=0.937
n=61
α=0.942
n=49
Africa α=0.808
n=45
α=0.873
n=38
α=0.865
n=44
α=0.805
n=31
α=0.79
n=38
α=0.779
n=38
α=0.756
n=37
α=0.889
n=51
α=0.811
n=36
α=0.819
n=34
α=0.867
n=49
α=0.795
n=43
α=0.889
n=37
NATURE HUMAN BEHAVIOUR | VOL 5 | JANUARY 2021 | 159–169 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav 161
RegisTeRed RepoRT Nature HumaN BeHaviour
interpretable factors, and in our data revealed more regional varia-
tion. These results suggest that, if the dimensions of face percep-
tion are indeed correlated, using analytical techniques that force
these dimensions to be uncorrelated may be obscuring important
regional differences in the structure of face perceptions.
A necessary next step for moving forward in person percep-
tion research is to address which analysis model (PCA or EFA)
best aligns with theory, so that models and theories can be revised
and expanded appropriately in future research. Crucially, the two
models make different assumptions about trait ratings of faces.
Table 3 | Replication criteria for the PCA for each region
Region Component 1 Component 2 Replicated
Trustworthy Dominant Dominant Trustworthy
Oosterhof and Todorov12 0.941 −0.244 0.929 −0.060 Yes
Africa 0.924 0.271 0.843 −0.065 Yes
Asia 0.922 0.370 0.863 −0.006 Ye s
Australia and New Zealand 0.943 0.257 0.907 −0.076 Yes
Central America and Mexico 0.918 0.007 0.915 −0.050 Yes
Eastern Europe 0.938 0.599 0.755 −0.113 No
Middle East 0.831 0.490 0.810 −0.382 Ye s
Scandinavia 0.953 0.392 0.881 −0.121 Yes
South America 0.898 0.309 0.905 −0.151 Ye s
United Kingdom 0.944 0.331 0.851 −0.121 Yes
United States and Canada 0.966 0.406 0.841 −0.073 Yes
Western Europe 0.957 0.357 0.875 −0.166 Yes
Oosterhof and Todorov’s valence–dominance model was judged to have been replicated in a given world region if the first component had a loading of >0.7 with trustworthiness and <0.5 with dominance,
and if the second component had a loading of >0.7 with dominance and <0.5 with trustworthiness.
–0.71
0.81
0.90
0.67
–0.24
0.93
0.72
–0.76
0.91
0.91
0.94
–0.71
–0.87
0.66
0.32
–0.29
0.65
0.93
0.19
0.13
0.55
0.11
0.20
–0.06
0.01
–0.22
0.63 0.18
–0.62
0.67
0.78
0.74
0.01
0.92
0.76
–0.75
0.85
0.84
0.92
–0.74
–0.71
0.69
0.46
–0.41
0.45
0.92
0.06
0.27
0.48
0.36
–0.14
–0.05
0.07
–0.15
0.16
–0.30
0.07
0.36
0.07
0.10
–0.28
0.14
–0.03
0.33
–0.14
–0.58
0.50
0.56 0.18 0.08
–0.60
0.71
0.76
0.79
0.31
0.95
0.77
–0.73
0.82
0.90
0.90
–0.79
–0.77
0.76
0.48
–0.55
0.50
0.91
0.02
0.24
0.64
0.18
–0.05
–0.15
0.01
–0.22
0.09
–0.21
0.12
0.31
0.08
0.09
–0.32
0.09
–0.21
0.37
–0.24
–0.57
0.42
0.59 0.21 0.08
–0.58
0.78
0.81
0.69
0.27
0.82
0.75
–0.70
0.78
0.89
0.92
–0.75
–0.82
0.75
0.34
–0.41
0.53
0.84
0.13
0.24
0.63
0.34
–0.05
–0.07
0.25
–0.10
0.57 0.19
–0.63
0.88
0.87
0.77
0.60
0.90
0.87
–0.64
0.92
0.90
0.94
–0.60
–0.81
0.74
0.23
–0.28
0.53
0.76
0.05
0.08
0.72
0.19
–0.11
–0.11
0.11
–0.12
–0.02
0.24
0.11
–0.30
–0.01
–0.28
0.21
–0.02
0.14
–0.25
0.20
0.76
–0.41
0.65 0.16 0.09
–0.62
0.73
0.87
0.78
0.33
0.89
0.80
–0.67
0.87
0.92
0.94
–0.65
–0.76
0.71
0.48
–0.31
0.41
0.85
0.06
0.15
0.63
0.24
–0.11
–0.12
0.24
–0.23
0.18
–0.31
0.12
0.39
0.23
0.18
–0.32
0.12
–0.23
0.25
–0.10
–0.66
0.46
0.60 0.18 0.10
–0.65
0.76
0.91
0.81
0.37
0.88
0.83
–0.80
0.91
0.93
0.92
–0.73
–0.76
0.63
0.51
–0.30
0.26
0.86
–0.14
0.42
0.50
0.25
–0.24
–0.01
0.36
–0.27
0.33
–0.24
0.09
0.47
0.22
0.22
–0.08
0.15
–0.05
0.16
–0.27
–0.53
0.52
0.64 0.18 0.09
–0.55
0.66
0.83
0.81
0.49
0.94
0.72
–0.44
0.77
0.87
0.83
–0.72
–0.70
0.77
0.48
–0.41
0.45
0.81
–0.06
0.52
0.84
0.39
–0.28
–0.38
0.10
–0.10
0.13
–0.38
0.06
0.26
0.11
0.04
–0.19
0.09
0.11
0.28
–0.21
–0.58
0.61
0.54 0.25 0.09
–0.70
0.72
0.89
0.81
0.41
0.95
0.83
–0.70
0.92
0.95
0.97
–0.73
–0.79
0.67
0.48
–0.30
0.42
0.84
0.01
0.17
0.68
0.13
–0.04
–0.07
0.19
–0.20
0.18
–0.27
0.14
0.37
0.16
0.16
–0.27
0.05
–0.23
0.21
–0.17
–0.62
0.45
0.66 0.17 0.08
–0.74
0.75
0.90
0.79
0.26
0.92
0.79
–0.67
0.91
0.93
0.94
–0.68
–0.77
0.62
0.44
–0.25
0.45
0.91
0.10
0.14
0.65
0.16
–0.10
–0.08
0.13
–0.13
0.13
–0.30
0.17
0.36
0.11
0.22
–0.27
0.07
–0.21
0.24
–0.19
–0.69
0.52
0.63 0.17 0.10
–0.63
0.80
0.88
0.77
0.39
0.95
0.79
–0.69
0.91
0.94
0.95
–0.74
–0.75
0.72
0.35
–0.30
0.50
0.88
0.04
0.25
0.68
0.12
–0.11
–0.12
0.07
–0.15
0.03
–0.31
0.08
0.34
0.11
0.13
–0.26
0.06
–0.12
0.20
–0.14
–0.62
0.52
0.64 0.18 0.08
–0.69
0.82
0.86
0.83
0.36
0.95
0.81
–0.64
0.90
0.93
0.96
–0.74
–0.78
0.70
0.36
–0.36
0.45
0.88
0.08
0.24
0.73
0.30
–0.19
–0.17
0.06
–0.15
0.01
0.31
0.04
–0.30
–0.15
–0.17
0.24
–0.05
0.11
–0.22
0.14
0.61
–0.50
0.65 0.19 0.08
South America United Kingdom United States and Canada Western Europe
Central America and Mexico Eastern Europe Middle East Scandinavia
Oosterhof and Todorov (2008)
Africa Asia Australia and New Zealand
Component 1 Component 2 Component 3 Component 1 Component 2 Component 3 Component 1 Component 2 Component 3 Component 1 Component 2 Component 3
Dominant
Aggressive
Mean
Unhappy
Weird
Confident
Intelligent
Attractive
Caring
Sociable
Responsible
Emotionally Stable
Trustworthy
Prop.Var
Dominant
Aggressive
Mean
Unhappy
Weird
Confident
Intelligent
Attractive
Caring
Sociable
Responsible
Emotionally Stable
Trustworthy
Prop.Var
Dominant
Aggressive
Mean
Unhappy
Weird
Confident
Intelligent
Attractive
Caring
Sociable
Responsible
Emotionally Stable
Trustworthy
Prop.Var
Fig. 1 | PCA loading matrices for each region. Positive loadings are shaded red and negative loadings are shaded blue. Darker colours correspond to
stronger loadings. The proportion of variance (Prop.Var) explained by each component is included at the top of each table.
NATURE HUMAN BEHAVIOUR | VOL 5 | JANUARY 2021 | 159–169 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav
162
RegisTeRed RepoRT
Nature HumaN BeHaviour
The PCA model does not assume that a latent factor causes the
trait ratings of the faces. The component captures linear combi-
nations of the original variables, maximized to explain variance.
Furthermore, in the original valence–dominance model, those
components were assumed to be orthogonal. In contrast, the the-
ory underlying the EFA model is that a latent factor causes the trait
ratings and any unexplained variance in that rating is measure-
ment error. Additionally, our EFA models allowed for the factors to
be correlated.
Theory can guide which model we use to analyse person per-
ception data. A person perception theory that aligns with a PCA
model would state that there are no underlying latent factors that
cause a person to rate a face in a particular way. There are, instead,
perceptual processes that vary across contexts, those doing the rat-
ing and those being rated, and the differential processes give rise
to components that can be used to reduce the data. This theory of
person perception would move forward with identifying the shared
processes across contexts, raters and ratees to see whether there are
predictable patterns in how the data are reduced.
A person perception theory that aligns with an EFA model makes
different assumptions about the processes that give rise to face rat-
ings. This theory would state that latent factors (for example, valence
or dominance) cause the trait ratings and, once we account for the
correct latent factors, any variability left in the ratings is measure-
ment error. We suggest that more careful and explicit consideration
of how theory connects to these approaches, and of which approach
is best suited to different research questions, will benefit the field.
Our study is one of several recent studies that have begun to
utilize different statistical models and to explore more dynamic
theories of trait ratings21,29,30 by exploring how the structures of
trait ratings vary systematically. This growing body of work cata-
logues variations in trait ratings by target demographic21,29,31, target
status32, target age33, perceiver knowledge34 and cultural factors17,18.
Furthermore, this growing body of work proposes dynamic theories
of person perception and more flexible statistical models for captur-
ing them21,29,30,35.
Table 4 | Factor congruence for each region’s PCA
Region Component 1 Component 2
Loading Congruence Loading Congruence
Africa 0.980 Equal 0.947 Fairly similar
Asia 0.974 Equal 0.843 Not similar
Australia and
New Zealand 0.982 Equal 0.959 Equal
Central America
and Mexico 0.992 Equal 0.935 Fairly similar
Eastern Europe 0.953 Equal 0.948 Fairly similar
Middle East 0.952 Equal 0.859 Fairly similar
Scandinavia 0.973 Equal 0.960 Equal
South America 0.976 Equal 0.953 Equal
United Kingdom 0.976 Equal 0.938 Fairly similar
United States and
Canada 0.972 Equal 0.952 Equal
Western Europe 0.975 Equal 0.936 Fairly similar
–0.32
0.86
0.68
0.92
0.23
0.94
0.67
–0.42
0.88
0.92
0.83
–0.61
–0.88
0.84
0.10
–0.50
0.47
0.97
–0.04
–0.06
0.72
–0.12
–0.02
–0.29
0.17
0.01
0.56 0.23
–0.05
0.04
0.43
0.75
0.11
0.51
0.04
–0.14
0.34
0.81
0.27
–0.93
0.11
0.90
0.19
–0.46
0.31
0.79
–0.23
–0.16
0.74
0.02
–0.14
–0.31
0.10
0.17
–0.07
0.83
0.33
0.15
0.24
0.19
0.40
–0.12
0.38
0.25
0.59
0.22
–0.76
–0.09
0.05
–0.22
0.24
0.23
0.37
0.55
–0.23
0.46
–0.25
0.06
–0.10
–0.12
0.26 0.210.23 0.11
0.91
0.17
–0.70
0.34
0.78
–0.21
–0.02
0.83
–0.07
–0.23
–0.46
0.08
0.12
–0.07
0.10
0.39
0.71
0.26
0.47
–0.02
–0.13
0.07
0.80
0.06
–0.96
0.10
–0.17
0.06
–0.05
0.21
0.28
0.41
0.80
–0.18
0.86
–0.13
0.25
–0.14
–0.27
0.03
0.82
0.19
0.23
0.28
0.16
0.12
–0.12
0.01
0.30
0.54
0.20
–0.72
0.25 0.24 0.20 0.19
–0.24
0.96
0.34
0.31
0.20
0.28
0.59
–0.17
0.76
0.68
0.79
0.07
–0.94
–0.42
–0.16
0.57
0.45
0.07
0.62
0.18
–0.65
0.05
0.28
0.21
–0.94
0.09
0.68
0.12
–0.28
0.63
0.80
0.27
0.17
0.48
0.21
–0.08
–0.13
–0.06
0.09
0.37 0.25 0.16
–0.21
0.95
0.64
0.57
0.75
0.50
0.83
–0.23
0.89
0.47
0.84
0.17
–0.92
0.85
0.02
–0.43
0.41
0.61
–0.09
–0.12
0.82
–0.02
–0.25
–0.32
0.12
0.09
–0.16
–0.03
0.12
0.54
0.15
0.54
0.03
–0.16
0.09
0.51
0.03
–1.01
0.17
0.49 0.19 0.19
–0.15
–0.01
0.64
0.67
0.25
0.47
–0.08
–0.23
0.06
0.73
0.34
–0.95
0.09
–0.08
0.82
0.48
0.13
0.27
0.03
0.31
–0.08
0.45
0.32
0.51
0.23
–0.81
0.85
0.22
–0.32
0.39
0.77
–0.09
–0.19
0.72
–0.06
–0.13
–0.30
0.07
0.06
–0.18
0.16
–0.18
0.34
0.21
0.58
0.68
–0.21
0.54
0.04
0.19
–0.06
–0.17
0.26 0.25 0.18 0.18
–0.31
0.90
0.28
0.18
0.49
0.26
0.77
–0.31
0.69
0.26
0.76
0.25
–1.01
–0.26
0.01
0.69
0.83
0.11
0.72
0.23
–0.46
0.36
0.75
0.24
–1.06
0.17
0.74
0.20
–0.29
0.41
0.79
–0.09
0.21
0.57
0.08
–0.20
–0.25
0.06
0.14
0.37 0.36 0.16
0.92
0.10
–0.63
0.21
0.60
–0.36
0.16
0.93
0.10
–0.47
–0.69
0.18
0.30
–0.06
0.07
0.45
0.80
0.50
0.54
0.27
–0.03
0.56
0.74
0.18
–0.89
0.18
–0.06
0.82
0.16
0.29
0.45
0.38
0.71
0.06
0.41
–0.01
0.44
0.21
–0.89
0.280.29 0.26
–0.32
0.86
0.31
0.35
0.49
0.47
0.80
–0.19
0.87
0.39
0.77
0.22
–0.97
–0.12
–0.00
0.54
0.76
0.26
0.57
0.07
–0.27
0.11
0.64
0.19
–1.05
0.15
0.84
0.22
–0.41
0.38
0.72
–0.12
–0.07
0.79
–0.11
–0.14
–0.30
0.13
0.08
0.41 0.28 0.19
–0.50
0.63
0.74
0.12
0.16
0.17
–0.09
–0.31
0.54
0.64
0.73
–0.00
–0.42
–0.11
–0.12
0.39
0.66
0.12
0.57
0.13
–0.19
0.06
0.50
0.06
–0.98
0.22
–0.21
0.31
–0.07
0.28
0.07
0.43
0.94
–0.25
0.44
–0.01
0.28
0.03
–0.64
0.63
0.39
–0.16
0.45
0.87
0.05
–0.06
0.64
0.11
–0.03
–0.08
0.07
–0.02
0.30 0.220.23 0.15
–0.22
0.91
0.42
0.32
0.43
0.50
0.78
–0.31
0.75
0.39
0.74
0.18
–0.98
–0.21
–0.01
0.43
0.72
0.29
0.54
0.07
–0.20
0.23
0.61
0.21
–1.04
0.22
0.80
0.15
–0.42
0.41
0.81
–0.10
0.05
0.79
–0.06
–0.23
–0.30
0.10
0.06
0.41 0.27 0.20
–0.17
0.55
–0.00
0.26
0.11
0.34
1.03
–0.13
0.58
0.03
0.40
0.02
–0.68
–0.29
–0.06
0.21
0.68
0.26
0.56
0.11
–0.18
0.22
0.57
0.16
–0.99
0.20
0.71
0.30
–0.21
0.42
0.87
0.03
–0.08
0.70
0.19
–0.10
–0.17
0.06
–0.03
–0.31
0.49
0.80
0.11
0.14
0.24
–0.18
–0.38
0.29
0.51
0.56
0.06
–0.42
0.27 0.24 0.170.24
South America United Kingdom United States and Canada Western Europe
Central America and Mexico Eastern Europe Middle East Scandinavia
Oosterhof and Todorov (2008) Africa Asia Australia and New Zealand
Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4
Dominant
Aggressive
Mean
Unhappy
Weird
Confident
Intelligent
Attractive
Caring
Sociable
Responsible
Emotionally Stable
Trustworthy
Prop.Var
Dominant
Aggressive
Mean
Unhappy
Weird
Confident
Intelligent
Attractive
Caring
Sociable
Responsible
Emotionally Stable
Trustworthy
Prop.Var
Dominant
Aggressive
Mean
Unhappy
Weird
Confident
Intelligent
Attractive
Caring
Sociable
Responsible
Emotionally Stable
Trustworthy
Prop.Var
Fig. 2 | EFA loading matrices for each region. Positive loadings are shaded red and negative loadings are shaded blue. Darker colours correspond to
stronger loadings. The proportion of variance explained by each factor is included at the top of each table.
NATURE HUMAN BEHAVIOUR | VOL 5 | JANUARY 2021 | 159–169 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav 163
RegisTeRed RepoRT Nature HumaN BeHaviour
Our results are consistent with this recent work in that they do
not provide strong evidence that there are a few generalizable latent
factors that cause the trait ratings across world regions. However,
they do suggest a dynamic process of person perception and eluci-
date the differential patterns of ratings across world regions. We can
use these data, representing impressions formed on a global scale,
to expand or refine our theories and to guide the selection of sta-
tistical models to represent those theories. Given the accumulating
evidence for variation in trait ratings, it is important that the con-
nection between the statistical models used to represent theories of
person perception are explicit and can accommodate the complexi-
ties of the impression formation process.
Methods
Ethics. Each research group had approval from their local ethics committee or
institutional review board to conduct the study, had explicitly indicated that their
institution did not require approval for the researchers to conduct this type of
face-rating task or had explicitly indicated that the current study was covered by
a pre-existing approval. Although the specics of the consent procedure diered
across research groups, all participants provided informed consent. All data were
stored centrally on University of Glasgow servers.
Procedure. Oosterhof and Todorov derived their valence–dominance model from
a PCA of ratings (by US raters) of 66 faces for 13 different traits (aggressiveness,
attractiveness, caringness, confidence, dominance, emotional stability, intelligence,
meanness, responsibility, sociability, trustworthiness, unhappiness and
weirdness)12. Using the criteria of the number of components with eigenvalues
greater than 1.0, this analysis produced two principal components. The first
component explained 63% of the variance in trait ratings, strongly correlated
with rated trustworthiness (r = 0.94) and weakly correlated with rated dominance
(r = −0.24). The second component explained 18% of the variance in trait ratings,
strongly correlated with rated dominance (r = 0.93) and weakly correlated with
rated trustworthiness (r = −0.06). We replicated Oosterhof and Todorov’s method12
and primary analysis in each world region we examined.
Stimuli in our study came from an open-access, full-colour face image set36
consisting of images of the faces of 60 men and 60 women taken under standardized
photographic conditions (Mage = 26.4 years; s.d. = 3.6 years; range = 18–35 years).
These 120 images consisted of 30 Black (15 male; 15 female), 30 White (15 male; 15
female), 30 Asian (15 male; 15 female) and 30 Latin faces (15 male; 15 female). As
reported by Oosterhof and Todorov’s study12, the individuals photographed posed
looking directly at the camera with a neutral expression, and the background,
lighting and clothing (here, a grey t-shirt) were constant across images.
In our study, adult raters were randomly assigned to rate the 13 adjectives
tested by Oosterhof and Todorov using scales ranging from 1 (not at all) to 9
(very) for all 120 faces in a fully randomized order at their own pace. Because all
researchers collected data through an identical interface (except for differences in
instruction language), data collection protocols were highly standardized across
laboratories. Each participant completed the block of 120 face-rating trials twice
so that we could report test–retest reliabilities of ratings; ratings from the first and
second blocks were averaged for all analyses (see code 1.5.5 in the Supplementary
Information).
Raters also completed a short questionnaire requesting demographic
information (sex, age and ethnicity). These variables were not considered in
Oosterhof and Todorov’s analyses but were collected in our study so that other
researchers could use them in secondary analyses of the published data. The data
from this study comprise the largest and most comprehensive open-access set of
face ratings with open stimuli from around the world, providing an invaluable
resource for further research addressing the Western centrality assumption in
person perception research.
Raters completed the task in a language appropriate for their country (see
below). To mitigate potential problems with translating single-word labels,
dictionary definitions for each of the 13 traits were provided. Twelve of these
dictionary definitions had previously been used to test for effects of social
impressions on the memorability of face photographs37. Dominance (not included
in that study) was defined as strong and important.
Participants. Simulations determined that we should obtain at least 25 different
raters for each of the 13 traits in every region (see https://osf.io/x7fus/ for
code and data). We focused on ratings of attractiveness and intelligence for the
simulations because they showed the highest and lowest agreement among the
traits analysed by Oosterhof and Todorov, respectively. First, we sampled from a
population of 2,513 raters, each of whom had rated the attractiveness of 102 faces;
these simulations showed that more than 99% of 1,000 random samples of 25
raters produced good or excellent inter-rater reliability coefficients (Cronbach’s α
values > 0.80). We then repeated these simulations, sampling from a population
of 37 raters, each of whom rated the intelligence of 100 faces, showing that 93% of
1,000 random samples of 25 raters produced good or excellent inter-rater reliability
coefficients (Cronbach’s α values > 0.80). Thus, averages of ratings from 25 or more
raters will have produced reliable dependent variables in our analyses; we planned
to test at least 9,000 raters in total.
In addition to rating the faces for the 13 traits examined by Oosterhof and
Todorov, 25 participants in each region were randomly assigned to rate the
targets’ age in light of Sutherland etal.’s results showing that a youth/attractiveness
dimension emerged from analyses of a sample of faces with a very diverse age
range38. Age ratings were not included in analyses relating to replications of
Oosterhof and Todorov’s valence–dominance model. These age ratings were
collected to allow for planned exploratory analyses including rated age, but we did
not perform these analyses.
Analysis plan. The code used for our analyses is included in the Supplementary
Information and publicly available from the Open Science Framework (https://osf.
io/87rbg/). The specific sections of code are cited below.
Ratings from each world region were analysed separately and anonymous raw
data have been published on the Open Science Framework. Our main analyses
directly replicated the PCA reported by Oosterhof and Todorov to test their
theoretical model in each region sampled (code 2.1). First, we calculated the average
rating for each face separately for each of the 13 traits (code 2.1.2). We then subjected
Table 5 | Replication criteria for the EFA for each region
Region Factor 1 Factor 2 Replicated
Trustworthy Dominant Dominant Trustworthy
Oosterhof and Todorov12 0.826 0.228 0.970 −0.288 Ye s
Africa 0.786 0.200 0.069 0.214 No
Asia 0.761 0.487 0.110 0.236 No
Australia and New Zealand 0.730 0.157 0.071 0.281 No
Central America and Mexico 0.268 0.108 0.241 0.591 No
Eastern Europe 0.843 0.750 0.609 −0.322 No
Middle East 0.177 0.502 0.600 −0.686 No
Scandinavia 0.744 0.428 0.293 0.211 No
South America −0.458 0.778 0.261 0.058 No
United Kingdom 0.338 0.249 0.265 0.510 No
United States and Canada 0.768 0.491 0.264 0.189 No
Western Europe 0.398 0.111 0.256 0.164 No
Oosterhof and Todorov’s valence–dominance model was judged to have been replicated in a given world region if the first factor had a loading >0.7 with trustworthiness and <0.5 with dominance and the
second factor had a loading >0.7 with dominance and <0.5 with trustworthiness.
NATURE HUMAN BEHAVIOUR | VOL 5 | JANUARY 2021 | 159–169 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav
164
RegisTeRed RepoRT
Nature HumaN BeHaviour
these mean ratings to PCA with orthogonal components and no rotation, as
Oosterhof and Todorov did (code 2.1.3). Using the criteria they reported, we retained
and interpreted components with eigenvalues greater than 1.0 (code 2.1.3.1).
Criteria for replicating Oosterhof and Todorov’s valence–dominance model. We
used multiple sources of evidence to judge whether Oosterhof and Todorov’s
valence–dominance model replicated in a given world region. First, we examined
the solution from the PCA conducted in each region and determined whether
Oosterhof and Todorov’s primary pattern replicated according to three criteria: (1)
the first two components had eigenvalues greater than 1.0; (2) the first component
(that is, the one explaining more of the variance in ratings) correlated strongly with
trustworthiness (r > 0.7) and weakly with dominance (r < 0.5); and (3) the second
component (that is, the one explaining less of the variance in ratings) correlated
strongly with dominance (r > 0.7) and weakly with trustworthiness (r < 0.5). If the
solution in a world region met all three of these criteria, we concluded that the
primary pattern of the model replicated in that region (code 2.1.3.3).
In addition to reporting whether the primary pattern was replicated in each
region, we also reported Tucker’s coefficient of congruence39,40. The congruence
coefficient, ϕ, ranges from −1 to 1 and quantifies the similarity between two
vectors of loadings41. It is:
ϕx;yð Þ¼
Px
i
y
i
ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi
Px
2
iPy
2
i
p
where xi and yi are the loadings of variable i (i = 1, …, n number of indicators
in the analysis) onto factors x and y, respectively. For the purposes of the current
research, we compared the vector of loadings from the first component from
Oosterhof and Todorov with the vector of loadings from the first component
estimated from each world region. We repeated this analysis for the second
component. This produced a standardized measure of component similarity for
each component in each world region that was not sensitive to the mean size of the
loadings42. Furthermore, this coefficient was fitting for the current study because
it does not require an apriori specification of a factor structure for each group, as
would be needed if we were to compare the factor structures in a multiple-group
confirmatory factor analysis. Following previous guidelines42, we concluded that
the components reported by Oosterhof and Todorov were not similar to those
estimated in a given world region if the coefficient was <0.85, were fairly similar if
it was between 0.85 and 0.94 and were equal if it was >0.95 (code 2.1.4).
Thus, we reported whether the solution had the same primary pattern that
Oosterhof and Todorov found and quantified the degree of similarity between
each component and the corresponding component from Oosterhof and Todorov’s
work. This connects to our competing predictions.
Prediction 1 (the valence–dominance model applies to all world regions) was
supported if the solution from the PCA conducted in each region satisfied all of the
criteria described above. Specifically, the primary pattern was replicated and the
components had at least a fair degree of similarity as quantified by a value of ϕ of
0.85 or greater.
Prediction 2 (the valence–dominance model applies in Western-world
regions but not other world regions) was supported if the solutions from the
PCA conducted in Australia and New Zealand, the United States and Canada,
Scandinavia, the United Kingdom and Western Europe, but not Africa, Asia,
Central America and Mexico, Eastern Europe, the Middle East or South America,
satisfied the criteria described above.
Exclusions. Data from raters who failed to complete all 120 ratings in the first
block of trials or who provided the same rating for 75% or more of the faces were
excluded from the analyses (codes 1.5.1, 1.5.3 and 1.5.5).
Data quality checks. Following previous research testing the valence–dominance
model12–14, data quality was checked by separately calculating the inter-rater
agreement (indicated by Cronbach’s α and test–retest reliability) for each trait in every
world region (code 2.1.1). A trait was only included in the analysis for that region if
the coefficient exceeded 0.70. Cases in which the coefficient did not exceed 0.70 are
reported and discussed. There were no cases in which the coefficient did not exceed
0.70. Test–retest reliability of traits was not used to exclude traits from analysis.
Power analysis. Simulations showed that we had more than 95% power to detect the
key effect of interest (that is, two components meeting the criteria for replicating
Oosterhof and Todorov’s work, as described above). We used the open data from
Morrison etal.’s replication13 of Oosterhof and Todorov’s research to generate a
variance–covariance matrix representative of typical inter-relationships among the
13 traits tested in our study. We then generated 1,000 samples of 120 faces from
these distributions and ran our planned PCA (which is identical to that reported
by Oosterhof and Todorov) on each sample (see https://osf.io/87rbg/ for code and
data). The results of >99% of these analyses matched our criteria for replicating
Oosterhof and Todorov’s findings. Thus, 120 faces gave us more than 95% power to
replicate Oosterhof and Todorov’s results.
Robustness analyses. Oosterhof and Todorov extracted and interpreted components
with an eigenvalue greater than 1.0 using an unrotated PCA. As described above,
we directly replicated their method in our main analyses but acknowledge that this
type of analysis has been criticized.
First, it has been argued that EFA with rotation, rather than an unrotated PCA,
is more appropriate when one intends to measure correlated latent factors, as was
the case in the current study43,44. Second, the extraction rule of eigenvalues greater
than 1.0 has been criticized for not indicating the optimal number of components,
as well as for producing unreliable components45,46.
To address these limitations, we repeated our main analyses using EFA with an
oblimin rotation as the model and a parallel analysis to determine the number of
factors to extract. We also recalculated the congruence coefficient described above
for these EFA results (code 2.2.2).
We used parallel analysis to determine the number of factors to extract
because it has been described as yielding the optimal number of components (or
factors) across the largest array of scenarios43,47,48 (code 2.2.1). In a parallel analysis,
random data matrices are generated such that they have the same number of cases
and variables as the real data. The mean eigenvalue from the components of the
random data is compared with the eigenvalue for each component from the real
data. Components are then retained if their eigenvalues exceed those from the
randomly generated data49.
The purpose of these additional analyses was twofold: (1) to address potential
methodological limitations in the original study; and (2) to ensure that the
results of our replication of Oosterhof and Todorov’s study are robust to the
implementation of those more rigorous analytical techniques. The same criteria for
replicating Oosterhof and Todorov’s model described above were applied to this
analysis (code 2.2.1.3).
Reporting Summary. Further information on research design is available in the
Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Data availability
Full data are publicly available at https://osf.io/87rbg/.
Code availability
Full analysis code is publicly available at https://osf.io/87rbg/.
Received: 18 May 2018; Accepted: 23 October 2020;
Published online: 4 January 2021
References
1. Olivola, C. Y. & Todorov, A. Elected in 100 milliseconds: appearance-based
trait inferences and voting. J. Nonverbal Behav. 34, 83–110 (2010).
2. Ritchie, K. L., Palermo, R. & Rhodes, G. Forming impressions of facial
attractiveness is mandatory. Sci. Rep. 7, 469 (2017).
3. Willis, J. & Todorov, A. First impressions: making up your mind aer 100 ms
exposure to a face. Psychol. Sci. 17, 592–598 (2006).
4. Olivola, C. Y., Funk, F. & Todorov, A. Social attributions from faces bias
human choices. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 566–570 (2014).
5. Todorov, A., Olivola, C. Y., Dotsch, R. & Mende-Siedlecki, P. Social
attributions from faces: determinants, consequences, accuracy, and functional
signicance. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66, 519–545 (2015).
6. Van ‘t Wout, M. & Sanfey, A. G. Friend or foe: the eect of implicit
trustworthiness judgments in social decision-making. Cognition 108,
796–803 (2008).
7. Todorov, A., Mandisodza, A. N., Goren, A. & Hall, C. C. Inferences
of competence from faces predict election outcomes. Science 308,
1623–1626 (2005).
8. Langlois, J. H. etal. Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and
theoretical review. Psychol. Bull. 126, 390–423 (2000).
9. Wilson, J. P. & Rule, N. O. Facial trustworthiness predicts extreme
criminal-sentencing outcomes. Psychol. Sci. 26, 1325–1331 (2015).
10. Todorov, A., Said, C. P., Engell, A. D. & Oosterhof, N. N. Understanding
evaluation of faces on social dimensions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12,
455–460 (2008).
11. Jack, R. E. & Schyns, P. G. Toward a social psychophysics of face
communication. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 68, 269–297 (2017).
12. Oosterhof, N. N. & Todorov, A. e functional basis of face evaluation.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 11087–11092 (2008).
13. Morrison, D., Wang, H., Hahn, A. C., Jones, B. C. & DeBruine, L. M.
Predicting the reward value of faces and bodies from social perception.
PLoS ONE 12, e0185093 (2017).
14. Wang, H., Hahn, A. C., DeBruine, L. M. & Jones, B. C. e motivational
salience of faces is related to both their valence and dominance. PLoS ONE
11, e0161114 (2016).
15. Henrich, J., Heine, S. & Norenzayan, A. e weirdest people in the world?
Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 61–83 (2010).
16. Kline, M. A., Shamsudheen, R. & Broesch, T. Variation is the universal:
making cultural evolution work in developmental psychology. Phil. Trans. R.
Soc. B 373, 20170059 (2018).
NATURE HUMAN BEHAVIOUR | VOL 5 | JANUARY 2021 | 159–169 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav 165
RegisTeRed RepoRT Nature HumaN BeHaviour
17. Sutherland, C. A. M. etal. Facial rst impressions across culture: data-driven
modeling of Chinese and British perceivers’ unconstrained facial impressions.
Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 44, 521–537 (2018).
18. Wang, H. etal. A data-driven study of Chinese participants’ social judgments
of Chinese faces. PLoS ONE 14, e0210315 (2019).
19. Han, C. etal. Cultural dierences in preferences for facial coloration. Evol.
Hum. Behav. 39, 154–159 (2018).
20. Perrett, D. I. etal. Eects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness.
Nature 394, 884–887 (1998).
21. Xie, S. Y., Flake, J. K. & Hehman, E.Perceiver and target characteristics
contribute to impression formation dierently across race and gender. J. Pers.
Soc. Psychol. 117, 364–385 (2019).
22. Li, N. P., Valentine, K. A. & Patel, L. Mate preferences in the US and
Singapore: a cross-cultural test of the mate preference priority model. Pers.
Individ. Dier. 50, 291–294 (2011).
23. Ting-Toomey, S. in e Challenge of Facework: Cross-Cultural and
Interpersonal Issues (ed. Ting-Toomey, S.) 1–14 (State Univ. New York
Press, 1994).
24. Tan, C. B. Y., Stephen, I. D., Whitehead, R. & Sheppard, E. You look familiar:
how Malaysian Chinese recognize faces. PLoS ONE 7, e29714 (2012).
25. Chartier, C., McCarthy, R. & Urry, H. e Psychological Science Accelerator
(Association for Physical Science, 2018).
26. Chawla, D. S. A new ‘accelerator’ aims to bring big science to psychology.
Science https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aar4464 (2017).
27. Moshontz, H. etal. e Psychological Science Accelerator: advancing
psychology through a distributed collaborative network. Adv. Methods Pract.
Psychol. Sci. 1, 501–515 (2018).
28. Widaman, K. F. On common factor and principal component representations
of data: implications for theory and for conrmatory replications. Struct. Equ.
Modeling 25, 829–847 (2018).
29. Hehman, E., Sutherland, C. A., Flake, J. K. & Slepian, M. L.e unique
contributions of perceiver and target characteristics in person perception.
J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 113, 513–529 (2017).
30. Sutherland, C. A., Rhodes, G., Burton, N. S. & Young, A. W.Do facial
rst impressions reect a shared social reality? Br. J. Psychol. 111,
215–232 (2020).
31. Oh, D., Dotsch, R., Porter, J. & Todorov, A.Gender biases in impressions from
faces: empirical studies and computational models. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 149,
323–342 (2020).
32. Oh, D., Shar, E. & Todorov, A. Economic status cues from clothes aect
perceived competence from faces. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 287–293 (2020).
33. Collova, J. R., Sutherland, C. A. & Rhodes, G. Testing the functional basis of
rst impressions: dimensions for children’s faces are not the same as for
adults’ faces. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 117, 900–924 (2019).
34. Stolier, R. M., Hehman, E., Keller, M. D., Walker, M. & Freeman, J. B. e
conceptual structure of face impressions. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115,
9210–9215 (2018).
35. Stolier, R. M., Hehman, E. & Freeman, J. B. A dynamic structure of social
trait space. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 197–200 (2018).
36. Ma, D. S., Correll, J. & Wittenbrink, B. e Chicago Face Database: a
free stimulus set of faces and norming data. Behav. Res. Methods 47,
1122–1135 (2015).
37. Bainbridge, W. A., Isola, P. & Oliva, A. e intrinsic memorability of face
photographs. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 142, 1323–1334 (2013).
38. Sutherland, C. A. etal. Social inferences from faces: ambient images generate
a three-dimensional model. Cognition 127, 105–118 (2013).
39. Burt, C. e factorial study of temperament traits. Br. J. Psychol. Stat. Sect. 1,
178–203 (1948).
40. Tucker, L. R. A Method for Synthesis of Factor Analysis Studies Personnel
Research Section Report No. 984 (Department of the Army, 1951).
41. Davenport, E. C. Jr Signicance testing of congruence coecients: a good
idea? Educ. Psychol. Meas. 50, 289–296 (1990).
42. Lorenzo-Seva, U. & ten Berge, J. M. F. Tucker’s congruence coecient as a
meaningful index of factor similarity. Methodology 2, 57–64 (2006).
43. Fabrigar, L. R., Wegener, D. T., MacCallum, R. C. & Strahan, E. J. Evaluating
the use of exploratory factor analysis in psychological research. Psychol.
Methods 4, 272–299 (1999).
44. Park, H. S., Dailey, R. & Lemus, D. e use of exploratory factor analysis and
principal components analysis in communication research. Hum. Commun.
Res. 28, 562–577 (2002).
45. Cli, N. e eigenvalues-greater-than-one rule and the reliability of
components. Psychol. Bull. 103, 276–279 (1988).
46. Zwick, W. R. & Velicer, W. F. Comparison of ve rules for determining the
number of components to retain. Psychol. Bull. 99, 432–442 (1986).
47. O’Connor, B. P. SPSS and SAS programs for determining the number of
components using parallel analysis and Velicer’s MAP test. Behav. Res.
Methods Instrum. Comput. 32, 396–402 (2000).
48. Schmitt, T. A. Current methodological considerations in exploratory and
conrmatory factor analysis. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 29, 304–321 (2011).
49. Courtney, M. G. R. Determining the number of factors to retain in EFA:
using the SPSS R-Menu v2.0 to make more judicious estimations. Pract.
Assess. Res. Eval. 18, 1–14 (2013).
Acknowledgements
C.L. was supported by the Vienna Science and Technology Fund (WWTF VRG13-007);
L.M.D. was supported by ERC 647910 (KINSHIP); D.I.B. and N.I. received funding from
CONICET, Argentina; L.K., F.K. and Á. Putz were supported by the European Social
Fund (EFOP-3.6.1.-16-2016-00004; ‘C omprehensive Development for Implementing
Smart Specialization Strategies at the University of Pécs’). K.U. and E. Vergauwe were
supported by a grant from the Swiss National Science Foundation (PZ00P1_154911 to E.
Vergauwe). T.G. is supported by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council
of Canada (SSHRC). M.A.V. was supported by grants 2016-T1/SOC-1395 (Comunidad
de Madrid) and PSI2017-85159-P (AEI/FEDER UE). K.B. was supported by a grant
from the National Science Centre, Poland (number 2015/19/D/HS6/00641). J. Bonick
and J.W.L. were supported by the Joep Lange Institute. G.B. was supported by the Slovak
Research and Development Agency (APVV-17-0418). H.I.J. and E.S. were supported
by a French National Research Agency ‘Investissements d’Avenir’ programme grant
(ANR-15-IDEX-02). T.D.G. was supported by an Australian Government Research
Training Program Scholarship. The Raipur Group is thankful to: (1) the University
Grants Commission, New Delhi, India for the research grants received through its
SAP-DRS (Phase-III) scheme sanctioned to the School of Studies in Life Science;
and (2) the Center for Translational Chronobiology at the School of Studies in Life
Science, PRSU, Raipur, India for providing logistical support. K. Ask was supported by
a small grant from the Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg. Y.Q. was
supported by grants from the Beijing Natural Science Foundation (5184035) and CAS
Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology. N.A.C. was supported
by the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship (R010138018). We
acknowledge the following research assistants: J. Muriithi and J. Ngugi (United States
International University Africa); E. Adamo, D. Cafaro, V. Ciambrone, F. Dolce and E.
Tolomeo (Magna Græcia University of Catanzaro); E. De Stefano (University of Padova);
S. A. Escobar Abadia (University of Lincoln); L. E. Grimstad (Norwegian School of
Economics (NHH)); L. C. Zamora (Franklin and Marshall College); R. E. Liang and R.
C. Lo (Universiti Tunku Abdul Rahman); A. Short and L. Allen (Massey University, New
Zealand), A. Ateş, E. Güneş and S. Can Özdemir (Boğaziçi University); I. Pedersen and T.
Roos (Åbo Akademi University); N. Paetz (Escuela de Comunicación Mónica Herrera);
J. Green (University of Gothenburg); M. Krainz (University of Vienna, Austria); and B.
Todorova (University of Vienna, Austria). The funders had no role in study design, data
collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.
Author contributions
Conceptualization: B.C.J., L.M.D., J.K.F., J.P.W., J.B.F., S.Á.-S., H.I., S.M.J.J., H.L.
Data curation: B.C.J., L.M.D., N.C.A., N.G.B., Y.Q., J.W.L., K.G., G.M.M., J.G.L.,
J.B.F., P.C., A.P., N.P., S.P., M.M.S., B.P., M.J.B., V.K., J.P., D.S., S.C.W., J.V.V., P.S.F.,
C.R.C., N.A.C.
Formal analysis: B.C.J., L.M.D., J.K.F., Y.Q., J.B.F.
Funding acquisition: N.C.O., Y.Q., J.W.L., C.C., J. Leongómez, O.R.S., E. Valderrama,
M.V.-A., J.G.L., M.C.P., J.B.F., J.K.O., G.K., H.I., H.D.F., T.J.S.L., E. Vergauwe, K. Ask,
K.W.T., M.I., C.L., P.S.F., C.R.C.
Investigation: B.C.J., L.M.D., M.T.L., J.A., I.L.G.N., N.G.B., S.C.L., F.F., M.L.W.,
C.P.C., M.A.V., S.A.S., N.C.O., D.P.C., A.W., Y.Q., H.M., P. Suavansri, T.R.E., J. Bonick,
J.W.L., C.C., A. Kapucu, A. Karaaslan, J. Leongómez, O.R.S., E. Valderrama, M.V.-A.,
B.A., P. Szecsi, M. Andreychik, E.D.M., C.B., C.-P.H., Q.-L.L., L.A.V., K.B., K.G., I.S., S.S.,
R.A., C.M., W.V., Z.J., Q.W., G.M.M., I.D.S., J.G.L., M.C.P., J.D.A., E.H., S.Y.X., W.J.C.,
M. Seehuus, J.P.W., E.K., M.P.-P., A.E.B.-S., A.d.-G., I.G.-S., H.-H.W., J.B.F., D.W.O., V.S.,
T.E.S., C.A.L., C.L.C., A.K.P., J. Bavolar, P. Kačmár, I. Zakharov, S.Á.-S., E.B., M.T., K.S.,
C.D.C., J.W.S., J.K.O., A.-S.L., T.D.G., J.A.O., B.J.W.D., L.M.S., G.R., M.J.B., B.J., D.R.,
G.K., V.A.F., H.L.U., S.-C.C., G.P., Z.V., D.M.B.-B., H.I., N.V.d.L., C.B.Y.T., V.K., M.F.C.,
H.D.F., D.I.B., G.G., J.P., C.S., K.A.Ś., E.M.O.K., D.S., B.S., M. Sirota, G.V.S., T.J.S.L., K.U.,
E. Vergauwe, J.S., K. Ask, C.J.J.v.Z., A. Körner, S.C.W., J. Boudesseul, F.R.-D., K.L.R.,
N.M.M., K.R.B., D.W., A.R.G.-F., M. Anne, S.M.J.J., K.M.L., T.K.N., C.K.T., J.H.Z.,
A.D.R., L.K., M. Vianello, N.I., A.C., S.L., J. Lutz, M. Adamkovic, P.B., G.B., I.R., V.C.,
K.P., N.K.S., K.W.T., C.A.T., A.M.F., R.M.C.S.H., J.V.V., N.S.C.-F., M.F.-A., J.H., A.M.,
M. Sharifian, B.F., H.L., M.I., C.L., E.P., M. Voracek, J.O., E.M.G., A.A., A.A.Ö., M.T.C.,
B.B.-D., M.A.K., C.O., T.G., J.K.M., Y.D., X.Y., S. Alper, P.S.F., C.R.C., N.A.C.
Methodology: B.C.J., L.M.D., J.K.F., S.C.L., L.A.V., M. Seehuus, S. Azouaghe, A.B.,
J.E., J.P.W., J.B.F., C.A.L., C.D.C., K.H., B.J., J.W., G.K., H.I., T.B., N.V.d.L., H.D.F., J.P.,
F.M.A.W., S.M.J.J., H.L.
Project administration: B.C.J., L.M.D., N.G.B., S.C.L., M.L.W., M.G., A.S., N.C.O.,
A.W., Y.Q., H.M., R.M.S., J. Bonick, J.W.L., C.C., A. Kapucu, A. Karaaslan, J. Leongómez,
O.R.S., E. Valderrama, M.V.-A., B.A., C.B., C.-P.H., L.A.V., K.B., K.G., I.S., S.S., I.D.S.,
M.C.P., S.Y.X., W.J.C., M. Seehuus, A.d.-G., I.G.-S., C.-C.K., J.B.F., D.W.O., C.A.L., J.
Bavolar, P. Kačmár, I. Zakharov, K.S., C.D.C., J.W.S., J.L.B., J.A.O., B.J.W.D., M.J.B., B.J.,
D.R., G.P., Z.V., E.S., N.V.d.L., V.K., M.F.C., H.D.F., J.P., C.S., K.A.S., E.M.O.K., B.S., M.
Sirota, T.J.S.L., K.U., E. Vergauwe, K. Ask, C.J.J.v.Z., S.C.W., J. Boudesseul, F.R.-D., K.L.R.,
D.W., S.M.J.J., C.K.T., J.H.Z., L.K., S.L., V.C., N.K.S., K.W.T., R.M.C.S.H., J.V.V., A.M., M.
Sharifian, B.F., H.L., C.L., E.P., M. Voracek, A.A., A.A.Ö., M.A.K., T.G., X.Y., S. Alper,
P.S.F., C.R.C., N.A.C.
NATURE HUMAN BEHAVIOUR | VOL 5 | JANUARY 2021 | 159–169 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav
166
RegisTeRed RepoRT
Nature HumaN BeHaviour
Resources: B.C.J., L.M.D., M.T.L., S.C.L., C.P.C., M.A.V., S.A.S., A.W., Y.Q.,
K. Ariyabuddhiphongs, S.J., H.M., P. Suavansri, N.T., R.M.S., C.C., A. Kapucu, J.
Leongómez, M.V.-A., N.H., C.B., L.A.V., K.B., K.G., Z.J., G.M.M., I.D.S., J.G.L., S.Y.X.,
W.J.C., M. Seehuus, S. Azouaghe, A.B., J.E., A.d.-G., C.-C.K., J.B.F., C.A.L., A.K.P., P.
Kačmár, I. Zakharov, E.B., K.S., C.D.C., J.K.O., J.L.B., B.J.W.D., D.R., W.W.A.S., S.-C.C.,
G.P., D.M.B.-B., T.B., C.B.Y.T., V.K., H.D.F., G.G., C.S., K.A.S., E.M.O.K., B.S., M. Sirota,
G.V.S., T.J.S.L., K.U., E. Vergauwe, K.J., K. Ask, J. Boudesseul, F.R.-D., N.M.M., S.M.J.J.,
C.K.T., A.D.R., F.K., Á.P., P.T., M. Vianello, A.C., S.L., J. Lutz, M. Adamkovic, P.B., V.C.,
A.M.F., R.M.C.S.H., J.V.V., N.S.C.-F., M.F.-A., A.M., M. Sharifian, H.L., C.L., M. Voracek,
E.M.G., A.A.Ö., M.A.K., C.O., X.Y., S. Alper, P.S.F., C.R.C. Software: B.C.J., L.M.D.,
J.K.F., G.M.M., I.D.S., N.P., B.P., C.D.C., H.D.F., C.S., K.R.B., R.M.C.S.H., C.R.C., N.A.C.
Supervision: B.C.J., L.M.D., J.K.F., M.T.L., S.C.L., M.L.W., N.C.O., A.W., H.M., J.W.L.,
C.C., A. Kapucu, J. Leongómez, O.R.S., E. Valderrama, M.V.-A., M. Andreychik, E.D.M.,
C.B., L.A.V., K.B., I.D.S., M.C.P., E.H., W.J.C., M. Seehuus, C.-C.K., J.B.F., C.A.L., P.
Kačmár, I. Zakharov, K.S., C.D.C., J.W.S., J.K.O., A.-S.L., J.L.B., J.A.O., B.J.W.D., M.J.B.,
H.I., V.K., M.F.C., H.D.F., J.P., C.S., E.M.O.K., D.S., B.S., M. Sirota, T.J.S.L., K.U., E.
Vergauwe, K. Ask, C.J.J.v.Z., D.W., S.M.J.J., A.C., S.L., K.P., N.K.S., K.W.T., A.M.F., J.V.V.,
M. Sharifian, M.I., C.L., M. Voracek, A.A., A.A.Ö., M.A.K., S. Alper, P.S.F., C.R.C., N.A.C.
Validation: B.C.J., L.M.D., J.K.F., C.C., Q.W., S.Y.X., M. Seehuus, C.L.C., A.K.P., I.
Zakharov, J.W.S., E.S., V.K., H.D.F., J.P., M. Sirota, E. Vergauwe, C.J.J.v.Z., P.T., J.H., M.
Voracek, M.A.K.
Visualization: B.C.J., L.M.D., J.K.F., H.D.F., M.A.K., P.S.F. Writing (original draft):
B.C.J., L.M.D., J.K.F., F.F., Y.Q., C.B., I.G.-S., J.B.F., K.S., B.J.W.D., G.K., H.L.U., H.I., H.D.F.,
D.I.B., J.P., C.S., D.S., K.L.R., S.M.J.J., A.D.R., N.K.S., J.O., A.A.Ö., M.A.K., P.S.F., N.A.C.
Writing (review & editing): B.C.J., L.M.D., J.K.F., M.T.L., J.A., I.L.G.N., S.C.L.,
F.F., M.L.W., M.A.V., A.S., D.P.C., A.W., Y.Q., K. Ariyabuddhiphongs, H.M., T.R.E., J.
Bonick, J.W.L., C.C., J. Leongómez, B.A., N.H., P. Szecsi, M. Andreychik, E.D.M., C.B.,
N.L., L.A.V., K.B., I.S., S.S., Z.J., I.D.S., M.C.P., J.D.A., E.H., S.Y.X., W.J.C., M. Seehuus,
S. Azouaghe, A.B., J.E., J.P.W., E.K., M.P.-P., A.E.B.-S., A.d.-G., J.B.F., V.S., T.E.S., C.A.L.,
C.L.C., P.C., P. Kujur, A.P., N.P., A.K.P., S.P., M.M.S., B.P., P. Kačmár, I. Zakharov, S.Á.-S.,
E.B., M.T., K.S., C.D.C., J.W.S., J.K.O., A.-S.L., J.L.B., T.D.G., J.A.O., B.J.W.D., G.R., M.J.B.,
K.H., B.J., G.K., V.A.F., H.L.U., G.P., Z.V., H.I., T.B., N.V.d.L., C.B.Y.T., V.K., M.F.C.,
H.D.F., D.I.B., G.G., C.S., E.M.O.K., D.S., B.S., M. Sirota, T.J.S.L., K.U., E. Vergauwe,
J.S., K. Ask, C.J.J.v.Z., A. Körner, K.L.R., K.R.B., D.W., A.R.G.-F., S.M.J.J., T.K.N., C.K.T.,
J.H.Z., M. Vianello, N.I., M. Adamkovic, G.B., I.R., V.C., K.P., N.K.S., K.W.T., C.A.T.,
A.M.F., R.M.C.S.H., J.V.V., B.F., H.L., C.L., E.P., M. Voracek, J.O., E.M.G., A.A., A.A.Ö.,
B.B.-D., M.A.K., T.G., J.K.M., Y.D., P.S.F., C.R.C., N.A.C.
The following people did not indicate specific contributions: A.F.D., A.C.H.,
A.D.L.R.-G., D.R.F., D.T., E.T., E.G.-S., H.I.J., I. Zettler, I.R.P., J.A.M.-R., J.D.L., L.N.,
L.F.A., M.A.C.V., M.M.A., M.L.B.-G., M.H.S., N.O.R., P.P., P.F., R.J.M., S.G., S.J.C., T.H.,
V.K.M.S., W.-J.Y.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Supplementary information is available for this paper at https://doi.org/10.1038/
s41562-020-01007-2.
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to B.C.J.
Peer review information Primary Handling Editor: Stavroula Kousta.
Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.
© The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Nature Limited 2021
Benedict C. Jones 1,156 ✉ , Lisa M. DeBruine 2,156, Jessica K. Flake3,156, Marco Tullio Liuzza 4,
Jan Antfolk 5, Nwadiogo C. Arinze 6, Izuchukwu L. G. Ndukaihe6, Nicholas G. Bloxsom7,
Savannah C. Lewis 7, Francesco Foroni 8, Megan L. Willis 8, Carmelo P. Cubillas9,
Miguel A. Vadillo 9, Enrique Turiegano 10, Michael Gilead11, Almog Simchon 11, S. Adil Saribay 12,
Nicholas C. Owsley13, Chaning Jang 13, Georgina Mburu13, Dustin P. Calvillo14, Anna Wlodarczyk 15,
Yue Qi16, Kris Ariyabuddhiphongs 17, Somboon Jarukasemthawee17, Harry Manley 17,
Panita Suavansri 17, Nattasuda Taephant17, Ryan M. Stolier 18, Thomas R. Evans 19,
Judson Bonick 20, Jan W. Lindemans 20, Logan F. Ashworth21, Amanda C. Hahn 21,
Coralie Chevallier 22, Aycan Kapucu 23, Aslan Karaaslan 23, Juan David Leongómez 24,
Oscar R. Sánchez 24, Eugenio Valderrama 24, Milena Vásquez-Amézquita 24, Nandor Hajdu 25,26,
Balazs Aczel 26, Peter Szecsi 26, Michael Andreychik 27, Erica D. Musser 28, Carlota Batres 29,
Chuan-Peng Hu 30, Qing-Lan Liu31, Nicole Legate 32, Leigh Ann Vaughn 33,
Krystian Barzykowski 34, Karolina Golik 34, Irina Schmid 35, Stefan Stieger 35, Richard Artner 36,
Chiel Mues 36, Wolf Vanpaemel 37, Zhongqing Jiang 38, Qi Wu38, Gabriela M. Marcu 39,
Ian D. Stephen 40, Jackson G. Lu 41, Michael C. Philipp 42, Jack D. Arnal 43, Eric Hehman3,
Sally Y. Xie3, William J. Chopik 44, Martin Seehuus45, Soufian Azouaghe 46,47, Abdelkarim Belhaj46,
Jamal Elouafa46, John P. Wilson 48, Elliott Kruse49, Marietta Papadatou-Pastou 50,
Anabel De La Rosa-Gómez 51, Alan E. Barba-Sánchez 51, Isaac González-Santoyo 52,
Tsuyueh Hsu 53, Chun-Chia Kung 53, Hsiao-Hsin Wang53, Jonathan B. Freeman 54,
Dong Won Oh 55, Vidar Schei 56, Therese E. Sverdrup 56, Carmel A. Levitan 57, Corey L. Cook58,
Priyanka Chandel 59, Pratibha Kujur 59, Arti Parganiha 59, Noorshama Parveen 59,
Atanu Kumar Pati 59, Sraddha Pradhan 59, Margaret M. Singh59, Babita Pande 60, Jozef Bavolar 61,
Pavol Kačmár 61, Ilya Zakharov 62, Sara Álvarez-Solas 63, Ernest Baskin 64, Martin Thirkettle 65,
Kathleen Schmidt 66, Cody D. Christopherson 67, Trinity Leonis67, Jordan W. Suchow68,
Jonas K. Olofsson 69, Teodor Jernsäther 69, Ai-Suan Lee 70, Jennifer L. Beaudry 71,
Taylor D. Gogan 71, Julian A. Oldmeadow 71, Benjamin Balas72, Laura M. Stevens73,
NATURE HUMAN BEHAVIOUR | VOL 5 | JANUARY 2021 | 159–169 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav 167
RegisTeRed RepoRT Nature HumaN BeHaviour
Melissa F. Colloff 73, Heather D. Flowe 73, Sami Gülgöz 74, Mark J. Brandt 75, Karlijn Hoyer 75,
Bastian Jaeger 75, Dongning Ren 75, Willem W. A. Sleegers75, Joeri Wissink75,
Gwenaël Kaminski 76, Victoria A. Floerke 77, Heather L. Urry 77, Sau-Chin Chen 78, Gerit Pfuhl 79,
Zahir Vally 80, Dana M. Basnight-Brown81, Hans I. Jzerman 47, Elisa Sarda47, Lison Neyroud82,
Touhami Badidi83, Nicolas Vander Linden 84, Chrystalle B. Y. Tan 85, Vanja Kovic86,
Waldir Sampaio 87, Paulo Ferreira 88, Diana Santos 88, Debora I. Burin 89, Gwendolyn Gardiner90,
John Protzko 91, Christoph Schild92, Karolina A. Ścigała92, Ingo Zettler 92, Erin M. O’Mara Kunz 93,
Daniel Storage 94, Fieke M. A. Wagemans95, Blair Saunders96, Miroslav Sirota97, Guyan V. Sloane97,
Tiago J. S. Lima 98, Kim Uittenhove 99, Evie Vergauwe99, Katarzyna Jaworska 2, Julia Stern 100,
Karl Ask 101, Casper J. J. van Zyl102, Anita Körner 103, Sophia C. Weissgerber 103,
Jordane Boudesseul 104, Fernando Ruiz-Dodobara104, Kay L. Ritchie105, Nicholas M. Michalak 106,
Khandis R. Blake 107,108, David White107, Alasdair R. Gordon-Finlayson 109, Michele Anne 110,
Steve M. J. Janssen 110, Kean Mun Lee 110, Tonje K. Nielsen111, Christian K. Tamnes 111,
Janis H. Zickfeld 112, Anna Dalla Rosa 113, Michelangelo Vianello 113, Ferenc Kocsor114,
Luca Kozma 114, Ádám Putz114, Patrizio Tressoldi 115, Natalia Irrazabal 116, Armand Chatard 117,
Samuel Lins 118, Isabel R. Pinto118, Johannes Lutz119, Matus Adamkovic 120, Peter Babincak 120,
Gabriel Baník 120, Ivan Ropovik 121,122, Vinet Coetzee 123, Barnaby J. W. Dixson 124,
Gianni Ribeiro 124, Kim Peters 124, Niklas K. Steffens 124, Kok Wei Tan125,
Christopher A. Thorstenson126, Ana Maria Fernandez 127, Rafael M. C. S. Hsu128,
Jaroslava V. Valentova 128, Marco A. C. Varella 128, Nadia S. Corral-Frías 129,
Martha Frías-Armenta 129, Javad Hatami130, Arash Monajem 130, MohammadHasan Sharifian130,
Brooke Frohlich131, Hause Lin 132, Michael Inzlicht 132, Ravin Alaei132, Nicholas O. Rule132,
Claus Lamm 133, Ekaterina Pronizius 133, Martin Voracek 133, Jerome Olsen 134,
Erik Mac Giolla 135, Aysegul Akgoz136, Asil A. Özdoğru 136, Matthew T. Crawford137,
Brooke Bennett-Day 138, Monica A. Koehn 139, Ceylan Okan140, Tripat Gill 141, Jeremy K. Miller 142,
Yarrow Dunham 143, Xin Yang 143, Sinan Alper 144, Martha Lucia Borras-Guevara145, Sun Jun Cai146,
Dong Tiantian 146, Alexander F. Danvers147, David R. Feinberg 148, Marie M. Armstrong 148,
Eva Gilboa-Schechtman149, Randy J. McCarthy150, Jose Antonio Muñoz-Reyes151, Pablo Polo151,
Victor K. M. Shiramazu 152, Wen-Jing Yan153, Lilian Carvalho 154, Patrick S. Forscher 82,
Christopher R. Chartier 7 and Nicholas A. Coles155
1School of Psychological Sciences and Health, University of Strathclyde, Glasgow, UK. 2Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology, University of Glasgow,
Glasgow, UK. 3Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Québec, Canada. 4Department of Medical and Surgical Sciences, Magna Græcia
University of Catanzaro, Catanzaro, Italy. 5Faculty of Arts, Psychology and Theology, Åbo Akademi University, Turku, Finland. 6Department of Psychology,
Alex Ekwueme Federal University Ndufu Alike, Ikwo, Nigeria. 7Department of Psychology, Ashland University, Danville, CA, USA. 8School of Behavioural
and Health Sciences, Australian Catholic University, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia. 9Department of Basic Psychology, Autonomous University of
Madrid, Madrid, Spain. 10Department of Biology, Autonomous University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain. 11Department of Psychology, Ben-Gurion University of
the Negev, Beersheba, Israel. 12Department of Psychology, Boğaziçi University, Beşiktaş, Turkey. 13Busara Center for Behavioral Economics, Nairobi, Kenya.
14Psychology Department, California State University San Marcos, San Marcos, CA, USA. 15School of Psychology, Catholic University of the North,
Antofagasta, Chile. 16Department of Psychology, Renmin University of China, Beijing, China. 17Faculty of Psychology, Chulalongkorn University, Bangkok,
Thailand. 18Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA. 19School of Psychological, Social and Behavioural Sciences, Coventry
University, Coventry, UK. 20Center for Advanced Hindsight, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA. 21Department of Psychology, Humboldt State University,
Arcata, CA, USA. 22Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives et Computationnelles, Département d’Études Cognitives, INSERM U960, École Normale
Supérieure, Paris, France. 23Psychology Department, Ege University, İzmir, Turkey. 24 Faculty of Psychology, Universidad El Bosque, Bogotá, Colombia.
25Doctoral School of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary. 26 Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest,
Hungary. 27Department of Psychology, Fairfield University, Fairfield, CT, USA. 28Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL, USA.
29Department of Psychology, Franklin and Marshall College, Lancaster, PA, USA. 30Leibniz Institute for Resilience Research, Mainz, Germany. 31Department
of Psychology, Hubei University, Wuhan, China. 32Department of Psychology, Illinois Institute of Technology, Chicago, IL, USA. 33Department of Psychology,
Ithaca College, Ithaca, NY, USA. 34Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland. 35Department of Psychology and Psychodynamics, Karl
NATURE HUMAN BEHAVIOUR | VOL 5 | JANUARY 2021 | 159–169 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav
168
RegisTeRed RepoRT
Nature HumaN BeHaviour
Landsteiner University of Health Sciences, Krems an der Donau, Austria. 36Research Group of Quantitative Psychology and Individual Differences,
Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium. 37Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Leuven, Belgium.
38Department of Psychology, Liaoning Normal University, Dalian, China. 39Department of Psychology, Lucian Blaga University of Sibiu, Sibiu, Romania.
40Department of Psychology, Macquarie University, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia. 41Sloan School of Management, Massachusetts Institute of
Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA. 42School of Psychology, Massey University, Palmerston North, New Zealand. 43Psychology Department, McDaniel
College, Westminster, CO, USA. 44Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, USA. 45Department of Psychology, Middlebury
College, Middlebury, VT, USA. 46Department of Psychology, Mohammed V University in Rabat, Rabat, Morocco. 47LIP/PC2S, Université Grenoble Alpes,
Grenoble, France. 48Psychology Department, Montclair State University, Montclair, NJ, USA. 49EGADE Business School, Monterrey Institute of Technology
and Higher Education, Monterrey, Mexico. 50School of Education, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Athens, Greece. 51School of Higher
Studies Iztacala, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico. 52Department of Psychology, National Autonomous University of
Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico. 53Department of Psychology, National Cheng Kung University, Tainan City, Taiwan. 54Department of Psychology and Center
for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY, USA. 55Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA. 56Department of
Strategy and Management, Norwegian School of Economics (NHH), Bergen, Norway. 57Department of Cognitive Science, Occidental College, Los Angeles,
CA, USA. 58Department of Psychology, Pacific Lutheran University, Tacoma, WA, USA. 59School of Studies in Life Science, Pandit Ravishankar Shukla
University, Raipur, India. 60Center for Basic Sciences, Pandit Ravishankar Shukla University, Raipur, India. 61Department of Psychology, Pavol Jozef Šafárik
University in Košice, Košice, Slovakia. 62Developmental Behavioral Genetics Lab, Psychological Institute of Russian Academy of Education, Moscow, Russia.
63Facultad de Ciencias dela Vida, Universidad Regional Amazónica Ikiam, Guayaquil, Ecuador. 64Department of Food Marketing, Saint Joseph’s University,
Philadelphia, PA, USA. 65Centre for Behavioural Science and Applied Psychology, Sheffield Hallam University, Sheffield, UK. 66School of Psychological and
Behavioral Sciences, Southern Illinois University, Carbondale, IL, USA. 67 Psychology Department, Southern Oregon University, Ashland, OR, USA. 68School
of Business, Stevens Institute of Technology, Hoboken, NJ, USA. 69Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden. 70Department of
Psychology, Sunway University, Subang Jaya, Malaysia. 71Department of Psychological Sciences, Swinburne University of Technology, Melbourne, Victoria,
Australia. 72Department of Psychology, North Dakota State University, Fargo, ND, USA. 73School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK.
74Koç University, Istanbul, Turkey. 75Department of Social Psychology, Tilburg University, Tilburg, the Netherlands. 76CLLE, Toulouse University, Toulouse,
France. 77Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, MA, USA. 78Department of Human Development and Psychology, Tzu-Chi University,
Hualien, Taiwan. 79Department of Psychology, UiT The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway. 80Department of Psychology and Counseling, United
Arab Emirates University, Abu Dhabi, United Arab Emirates. 81United States International University Africa, Nairobi, Kenya. 82Department of Psychology,
Université Grenoble Alpes, Saint-Martin-d’Hères, France. 83Department of Psychology, Université Ibn Tofail, Kénitra, Morocco. 84Center for Social and
Cultural Psychology, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium. 85Department of Community and Family Medicine, Universiti Malaysia Sabah, Kota
Kinabalu, Malaysia. 86Department of Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy, University of Belgrade, Belgrade, Serbia. 87Universidade Federal de São Carlos, São
Paulo, Brazil. 88Universidade Federal da Grande Dourados, Dourados, Brazil. 89Instituto de Investigaciones, Facultad de Psicologia, Universidad de Buenos
Aires, Buenos Aires, Argentina. 90Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, Riverside, CA, USA. 91Department of Psychological and
Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA, USA. 92Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen,
Denmark. 93Department of Psychology, University of Dayton, Dayton, OH, USA. 94Department of Psychology, University of Denver, Denver, CO, USA.
95Institute for Socio-Economics, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany. 96School of Social Sciences, University of Dundee, Dundee, UK.
97Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Colchester, UK. 98Department of Social and Work Psychology, University of Brasília, Brasília, Brazil.
99Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland. 100Department of Psychology, University of Goettingen,
Goettingen, Germany. 101Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden. 102Department of Psychology, University of
Johannesburg, Johannesburg, South Africa. 103Department of Psychology, University of Kassel, Kassel, Germany. 104Institute of Scientific Research, Faculty
of Psychology, University of Lima, Lima, Peru. 105School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Lincoln, UK. 106Department of Psychology, University of
Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA. 107Evolution and Ecology Research Centre, University of New South Wales Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia.
108Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia. 109Faculty of Health, Education and Society,
University of Northampton, Northampton, UK. 110School of Psychology, University of Nottingham Malaysia, Semenyih, Malaysia. 111Department of
Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway. 112Department of Management, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark. 113Department of Philosophy, Sociology,
Education and Applied Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy. 114Institute of Psychology, University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary. 115Department of
General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy. 116Faculty of Social Sciences, University of Palermo, Buenos Aires, Argentina. 117Psychology
Department, University of Poitiers, Poitiers, France. 118Department of Psychology, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal. 119Department of Psychology,
University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany. 120Institute of Psychology, Faculty of Arts, University of Presov, Presov, Slovakia. 121Faculty of Education,
University of Presov, Presov, Slovakia. 122Institute for Research and Development of Education, Faculty of Education, Charles University, Prague, Czechia.
123Department of Biochemistry, Genetics and Microbiology, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa. 124School of Psychology, University of Queensland,
Brisbane, Queensland, Australia. 125School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading Malaysia, Johor, Malaysia. 126Department of
Clinical and Social Sciences in Psychology, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA. 127School of Psychology, University of Santiago, Chile, Santiago,
Chile. 128Institute of Psychology, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil. 129Department of Psychology,
University of Sonora, Hermosillo, Mexico. 130Department of Psychology, University of Tehran, Tehran, Iran. 131Department of Psychology, University of
Tennessee, Knoxville, Knoxville, TN, USA. 132Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada. 133Department of Cognition,
Emotion, and Methods in Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria. 134Department of Applied Psychology: Work, Education
and Economy, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria. 135Department of Behavioral Sciences, University West, Trollhättan, Sweden.
136Department of Psychology, Üsküdar University, Istanbul, Turkey. 137School of Psychology, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand.
138Department of Psychology, Wesleyan College, Middletown, CT, USA. 139Discipline of Psychology, Faculty of Health, University of Canberra, Canberra,
Australian Capital Territory, Australia. 140School of Social Science and Psychology, Western Sydney University, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia.
141Lazaridis School of Business and Economics, Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada. 142Department of Psychology, Willamette University,
Salem, OR, USA. 143Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA. 144Department of Psychology, Yasar University, Izmir, Turkey.
145University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, UK. 146Qufu Normal University, Jining, China. 147University of Oklahoma, Norman, OK, USA. 148Department of
Psychology, Neuroscience, and Behaviour, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada. 149Bar-Ilan University, Tel Aviv, Israel. 150Northern Illinois
University, DeKalb, IL, USA. 151Playa Ancha University of Educational Sciences, Valparaiso, Chile. 152Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, Rio Grande
do Norte, Brazil. 153Wenzhou University, Wenzhou, China. 154FGV/EAESP, Sao Paulo, Brazil. 155Harvard Kennedy School, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA,
USA. 156These authors contributed equally: Benedict C. Jones, Lisa M. DeBruine, Jessica K. Flake. ✉e-mail: psysciacc.001@gmail.com
NATURE HUMAN BEHAVIOUR | VOL 5 | JANUARY 2021 | 159–169 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav 169
1
nature research | reporting summary April 2020
Corresponding author(s): Benedict Jones
Last updated by author(s): 2020 -05-29
Reporting Summary
Nature Research wishes to improve the reproducibility of the work that we publish. This form provides structure for consistency and transparency
in reporting. For further information on Nature Research policies, see our Editorial Policies and the Editorial Policy Checklist.
Statistics
For all statistical analyses, confirm that the following items are present in the figure legend, table legend, main text, or Methods section.
n/a Confirmed
The exact sample size (n) for each experimental group/condition, given as a discrete number and unit of measurement
A statement on whether measurements were taken from distinct samples or whether the same sample was measured repeatedly
The statistical test(s) used AND whether they are one- or two-sided
Only common tests should be described solely by name; describe more complex techniques in the Methods section.
A description of all covariates tested
A description of any assumptions or corrections, such as tests of normality and adjustment for multiple comparisons
A full description of the statistical parameters including central tendency (e.g. means) or other basic estimates (e.g. regression coefficient)
AND variation (e.g. standard deviation) or associated estimates of uncertainty (e.g. confidence intervals)
For null hypothesis testing, the test statistic (e.g. F, t, r) with confidence intervals, effect sizes, degrees of freedom and P value noted
Give P values as exact values whenever suitable.
For Bayesian analysis, information on the choice of priors and Markov chain Monte Carlo settings
For hierarchical and complex designs, identification of the appropriate level for tests and full reporting of outcomes
Estimates of effect sizes (e.g. Cohen's d, Pearson's r), indicating how they were calculated
Our web collection on statistics for biologists contains articles on many of the points above.
Software and code
Policy information about availability of computer code
Data collection DeBruine. L.M. (2019, April 9). Experimentum: Beta release 1 (Version v.0.1). Zenodo. http://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.2634356
Data analysis https://osf.io/87rbg/
For manuscripts utilizing custom algorithms or software that are central to the research but not yet described in published literature, software must be made available to editors and
reviewers. We strongly encourage code deposition in a community repository (e.g. GitHub). See the Nature Research guidelines for submitting code & software for further information.
Data
Policy information about availability of data
All manuscripts must include a data availability statement. This statement should provide the following information, where applicable:
- Accession codes, unique identifiers, or web links for publicly available datasets
- A list of figures that have associated raw data
- A description of any restrictions on data availability
https://osf.io/87rbg/
2
nature research | reporting summary April 2020
Field-specific reporting
Please select the one below that is the best fit for your research. If you are not sure, read the appropriate sections before making your selection.
Life sciences Behavioural & social sciences Ecological, evolutionary & environmental sciences
For a reference copy of the document with all sections, see nature.com/documents/nr-reporting-summary-flat.pdf
Behavioural & social sciences study design
All studies must disclose on these points even when the disclosure is negative.
Study description Quantitative analysis of ratings data
Research sample Student-type samples collected in eleven different world regions
Sampling strategy Opportunistic sampling by individual research groups
Data collection Rating of faces on one of thirteen randomly determined traits
Timing Throughout 2019
Data exclusions All data exclusions were described in the stage one protocol and in the analysis code
Non-participation These data are given in the analysis code and output
Randomization Participants were randonly allocated to rate one triat
Reporting for specific materials, systems and methods
We require information from authors about some types of materials, experimental systems and methods used in many studies. Here, indicate whether each material,
system or method listed is relevant to your study. If you are not sure if a list item applies to your research, read the appropriate section before selecting a response.
Materials & experimental systems
n/a Involved in the study
Antibodies
Eukaryotic cell lines
Palaeontology and archaeology
Animals and other organisms
Human research participants
Clinical data
Dual use research of concern
Methods
n/a Involved in the study
ChIP-seq
Flow cytometry
MRI-based neuroimaging
Human research participants
Policy information about studies involving human research participants
Population characteristics Raters from a range of geographic regions and countries. Region and country information are reported.
Recruitment Opportunistic recruitment by individual labs.
Ethics oversight main ethics approval was from University of Glasgow, although some individual groups also obtained their own thicas
approvals.
Note that full information on the approval of the study protocol must also be provided in the manuscript.