ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Although a positive link between sleep spindle occurrence and measures of post-sleep recall (learning success) is often reported for humans and replicated across species, the test-retest reliability of the effect is sometimes questioned. The largest to date study could not confirm the association, however methods for automatic spindle detection diverge in their estimates and vary between studies. Here we report that in dogs using the same detection method across different learning tasks is associated with observing a positive association between sleep spindle density (spindles/minute) and learning success. Our results suggest that reducing measurement error by averaging across measurements of density and learning can increase the visibility of this effect, implying that trait density (estimated through averaged occurrence) is a more reliable predictor of cognitive performance than estimates based on single measures.
Content may be subject to copyright.

Scientic Reports | (2020) 10:22461 | 
www.nature.com/scientificreports
Averaging sleep spindle occurrence
in dogs predicts learning
performance better than single
measures
Ivaylo Borislavov Iotchev1,4*, Vivien Reicher1,3,4, Enikő Kovács1,2,4, Tímea Kovács1,
Anna Kis2,5, Márta Gácsi1,3,5 & Enikő Kubinyi1,5
Although a positive link between sleep spindle occurrence and measures of post-sleep recall (learning
success) is often reported for humans and replicated across species, the test–retest reliability of the
eect is sometimes questioned. The largest to date study could not conrm the association, however
methods for automatic spindle detection diverge in their estimates and vary between studies. Here we
report that in dogs using the same detection method across dierent learning tasks is associated with
observing a positive association between sleep spindle density (spindles/minute) and learning success.
Our results suggest that reducing measurement error by averaging across measurements of density
and learning can increase the visibility of this eect, implying that trait density (estimated through
averaged occurrence) is a more reliable predictor of cognitive performance than estimates based on
single measures.
Sleep spindles are thalamocortical transmissions1,2 observed mostly in mammalian non-REM sleep3 as brief
(0.5–5 s4) trains of symmetric waves5 in the EEG signal. Dierent propositions for their dening frequency
(waves/second) overlap in the 9–16Hz band among humans6,7, mice8, and dogs911.
e most oen reported cognitive correlate of sleep spindles in humans is a positive relationship with post
sleep-recall (learning success). However, this has almost exclusively been reported in smaller samples1223, and
thus the reliability of this eect requires a stronger conrmation. On one hand, the study using the largest to
date sample could not nd such anassociation24. Moreover, it is troublesome that dierent studies use dierent
algorithms2, since automatic spindle detection methods diverge in their estimates of spindle occurrence25. On
the other hand, invasive work in animal models has revealed putative mechanisms26 to explain how spindles
promote memory consolidation, as well as implicated causality27 where human data mostly allows only for cor-
relation. e issue thus remains controversial.
Here we report a replication analysis for the link between spindle occurrence and learning in dogs. e dog
(Canis familiaris) is a fairly new model species in sleep spindle research, but one advantage in addressing the
problem of replicability is that currently only one method for detecting canine spindles has been consolidated
across all published studies (from one single research group, Iotchev etal.911). e same method, adopted from
the human literature28 with minor alterations for use in dogs9, will also be used here to avoid the problem of
automatic detector divergence25. Moreover, the current analysis will include all unpublished data sets available
to us which are t for this analysis. is is crucial because in the human literature publication bias is suspected
to underlie eects reported for (fast) spindle density29. e goal of the analyses presented here will be to evaluate
if the relationship between sleep spindle occurrence and learning success is real or a type I error. To evaluate
this, we will look both at the prevalence of positive ndings and the conditions under which a positive or nega-
tive nding is observed. We will thereby also compare the prevalence of associations between (1) single and (2)
averaged measurements of sleep spindle density (spindles/minute) and learning, the latter being deemed more
likely to reect underlying traits and to be freer of measurement error.
OPEN
            
           
      
    
   
 *
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2020) 10:22461 | 
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Methods
Procedure. ree datasets were included in the analyses. In all datasets the basic learning paradigm (“novel
words paradigm”) was based on the one used to obtain the data for Iotchev etal.9 and Kis etal.30 (for details see
Supplementary Material). We included the dataset used in these studies for comparison (data set 0, N = 15). Data
set 1 (N = 19) originated from Reicher etal., in prep and data set 2 (N = 13) from Kovács etal. in prep. One dog
participated in data set 0 and 2, otherwise the samples did not overlap. Before sleep, dogs were required to learn
novel words (in English), and associate them with actions that they had been trained to perform to dierent ver-
bal commands before (in Hungarian). Aer sleep, the nal performance was measured as the percent of correct
trials (out of eighteen, on the re-test), and learning gain (% performance re-test minus test) was also calculated.
All three datasets were comprised of an adaptation sleep, followed by two counter-balanced, repeated-measures
conditions. Condition 1a used a supportive type of training (using both food and social reward in case of cor-
rect action and no scolding in case of incorrect action), while in condition 1b a controlling type of training was
conducted by a dierent experimenter (using only food reward without social reinforcement in the case of cor-
rect action and scolding in case of incorrect action). In condition 2a training was carried out by the owner in a
socially relevant manner (using both food reinforcement and social reward in case of correct action), in condi-
tion 2b training was carried out by an experimenter unknown to the dog in a socially irrelevant manner (using
food reinforcement but without social reward in case of correct action).
Subjects. A total of 46 dogs (23 females, age range 1–9years, 28 were purebred representatives from 16 dif-
ferent breeds) participated in the three studies (0, 1, 2). One dog was included in both study 0 and 2 (a female
golden retriever, aged 1year in study 0 and 2years in study 2). Because search for sleep spindles was restricted
to non-REM sleep as in Iotchev etal.9,10, dogs which did not reach this stage or had otherwise corrupted or miss-
ing les were assigned missing values for sleep spindle density. During adaptation (occasion 1), three dogs were
assigned missing values in study 0, ve dogs in study 1, and two dogs in study 2. Regarding data from experimen-
tal conditions (occasion 2 and 3), two missing values were assigned in condition 1a, and one in condition 2a (see
conditions below). Missing values were excluded from our analyses and the calculation of averages.
Ethical statement. According to the Hungarian regulations of animal experimentation, our non-invasive
polysomnography research does not qualify as an animal experiment. e Hungarian Scientic Ethical Commit-
tee of Animal Experiments issued a permission (under the number PE/EA/853-2/2016) approving of our non-
invasive protocol. All owners volunteered to participate in the study and were informed about the procedure
before beginning.
Electrode placement and EEG post-processing. Electrode placement (see Fig.1) followed the method
outlined by Kis etal.31 e polysomnographic recordings were manually categorized into sleep stages (wake,
drowsiness, non-REM, REM, see Supplementary for example images) according to standard criteria31 (validated
in Gergely etal.32). e traces identied as non-REM (descriptive statistics in the Supplementary) were scanned
for spindles using the frontal (Fz) and central (Cz) midline electrodes.
Spindle detection. Automatic detection was implemented as in Iotchev et al.9 on parts of the signal
marked as non-REM sleep and pre-ltered between 5 and 16Hz. Specics of the applied algorithm are detailed
in Iotchev etal.9,10 and based on similar criteria validated against visual experts on human EEG by Nonclercq
etal.28. Importantly, the algorithm invokes 2 steps, as initial detections are used to re-calculate boundaries for the
target amplitude and frequency of spindles for each dog and recording, in line with the assumption that these are
normally distributed within individuals. In the rst step the frequency is assumed to be 9–16Hz and a minimum
amplitude criterion of more than 1 standard deviation above the average of the searched signal is set. For the
individual adjustment in the second step the algorithm calculates maximum likelihood estimates for the means
and standard deviations of amplitude and frequency. e amplitudes and frequency of the nal detections have
to be within 2 standard deviations of the estimated means.
Figure1. Schematic drawing (by Vivien Reicher) of electrode placement in the dog identical in study 1 & 2
(study 0 used the same electrode placement, but without the F7 channel).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2020) 10:22461 | 
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Analysis. Since only Fz was active in data set 0, we will focus below mainly on results obtained from Fz. We
will also refer to and discuss detections from across the whole 9–16Hz frequency range, considering that many
studies looking into spindle–learning correlations did not divide spindles into slow and fast ones1214,33. We refer
to Supplementary TablesS1, S2, S4 and S5 for statistics obtained across both electrodes, and spindle sub-types
(slow and fast). All associations were tested with Pearson correlations using SPSS v25.
Results
By comparing single measurements (i.e. from one single attendance, same condition) of sleep spindle occur-
rence and learning success, we found an association between spindle density and learning gain for data sets 0
and condition 1a, but not for condition 1b and data set 2 (see Fig.2). Upon closer examination, both eects
were specic to the slow spindle type (TableS2) and for condition 1a a trend for the same eect was observed
on Cz, as well (TableS1).
Next, we compared averaged measurements (across attendances/conditions) of sleep spindle occurrence
and learning, to test how reducing measurement error could aect the relationship between spindle density and
learning in the three data sets, considering that a single measurement for each variable might be less reliable34.
Averaging across attendances/conditions was deemed valid, because one of the rationales for this analysis was to
approximate the underlying traits, rather than estimate a condition-specic expression of either spindle occur-
rence or learning performance. erefore, we averaged within each data set and for each dog spindle density
values obtained from all three recordings (in data set 0 these correspond to the adaptation, control and learning
conditions, while in data set 1 and 2 there was only an adaptation and two learning conditions; due to missing
values, for some dogs the averages were based on only two recordings, see Supplementary for further details).
We furthermore averaged learning performance variables (nal performance and learning gain) for the two
learning conditions in data sets 1 and 2 (this was not possible for data set 0 in which there was only one measure
for each). We tested associations with both (averaged) learning gain and (averaged) nal performance because
their distribution and range (TableS3) suggestedthat for each data set a dierent read-out variable might better
reect the underlying learning process.
Averaged density was positively associated with learning gain in data set 0 and average nal performance
in data set 2 (Fig.3). ese eects were also signicant for the slow sub-type and specic to Fz (TablesS4, S5).
Neither averaged learning performance variables were associated with averaged density in data set 1, but note
that in condition 1b, more than half of the dogs (57.9% or 11 out of 19 animals) worsened their performance
on the novel task aer sleep. In comparison, only 7 dogs (36.8%) did so in condition 1a. Together with a visual
inspection of the data (Fig.1) and the performance overview provided in TableS3 (lowest average values for
learning gain and nal performance), these numbers suggest a oor eect on learning success in condition 1b.
Discussion
A positive association between dogs’ spindle occurrence and learning success could be demonstrated in each
of the three data sets. As a note of caution,dierent transformations of the raw variables(like averaged scores)
and operationalization of learning (learning gain versus nal performance)were used between some of the
Figure2. Associations between spindle density (spindles/minute) and learning gain as measured over the
frontal midline electrode (Fz) in data sets 0, 1, and 2. For the already published nding in data set 09 we used
lighter colors. Two dogs did not sleep in condition 1a and one dog did not sleep in condition 2b, these animals
were excluded from the analysis. Schematic drawing (by the corresponding author) of how learning gain was
calculated during tests on the novel task.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2020) 10:22461 | 
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
comparisons,while all available samples arealsosmall (N < 20). An argument for treating the positive associations
as cumulativederives from usingthe same detection algorithm9,andthe same experimental operationalization
of learning (the “novel words paradigm” of Kis etal.30) across all experiments. Intriguingly, all signicant asso-
ciations were specic to slow and frontal spindles (see Supplementary TablesS2, S4, S5) which resembles what
is seen in humans when learning is tested with verbal material, like word-pairs13,16,23. In further support of the
spindle-learning association, Type II errors arecommonin small, and thereby likely underpowered samples35.
False negatives are also likely, considering that memory consolidation is not restricted to sleep alone in neither
humans nor dogs30,36. Also, many additional conditions are known to inuence if any eect is observed, e.g.
relative timing to ripples and slow-waves37, emotional arousal38 and exact stage of non-REM sleep14. However,
since in most animals it is hard to separate non-REM sleep stages from each other31,39 not all of these conditions
can be tested outside of humans.
Surprisingly, although sleep-dependent memory consolidation operates in the time-frame of a single
day40,41 and exposure to new information has been shown to result in direct increases in spindle occurrence
in humans15,23, rats33 and dogs9, our results for data sets 0 and 2 suggest that estimating trait density by averag-
ing across recordings might increase the visibility of spindle-learning associations. Other arguments for the
predictive utility of trait density come from reports of stable spindle occurrence across nights in humans42,43,
the heritability of sleep spindle density44, and the observation that dierent psychiatric conditions and natural
aging, each associated with memory problems, can measurably reduce spindle occurrence in humans4549 and
specically the occurrence of slow spindles in dogs10.
We conclude thatthe here examined data-sets lendadditionalsupport to thepositive association between
sleep spindle occurrence and learningobservedin dogsearlier9, but the need for further evidence is not
exhausted. Even more and larger samples will berequiredtoestablish to what extendlow poweraccounts for the
proportion of null results. Moreover, since traitoccurrenceis also associated with general mental ability50more
studies with a control condition, in which sleep is not preceded by learning demand,will beneeded in dogs to
separateif these correlationsreect memory consolidationorgeneral learning potential.
Received: 19 June 2020; Accepted: 17 December 2020
References
1. Steriade, M. & Llinás, R. R. e functional states of the thalamus and the associated neuronal interplay. Physiol. Rev. 68, 649–742
(1988).
2. Fernandez, L. M. J. & Lüthi, A. Sleep spindles: Mechanisms and functions. Physiol. Rev. https ://doi.org/10.1152/physr ev.00042
.2018 (2020).
3. Kryger, M. H., Roth, T. & Dement, W. C. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine (Saunders/Elsevier, Amsterdam, 2011).
4. Rechtschaen, A. & Kales, A. A Manual of Standardized Techniques and Scoring System for Sleep Stages of Human Subjects (D.C.
U.S. Gov. Print. O. NIH Public, Washington, 1968).
5. Dutertre, F. Catalog of the Main EEG-Patterns .Handbook of Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology (Elsevier, Amster-
dam, 1977).
6. Bódizs, R., Körmendi, J., Rigó, P. & Lázár, A. S. e individual adjustment method of sleep spindle analysis: Methodological
improvements and roots in the ngerprint paradigm. J. Neurosci. Methods 178, 205–213 (2009).
7. De Gennaro, L. et al. e electroencephalographic ngerprint of sleep is genetically determined: A twin study. Ann. Neurol. 64,
455–460 (2008).
8. Kim, D., Hwang, E., Lee, M., Sung, H. & Choi, J. H. Characterization of topographically specic sleep spindles in mice. Sleep 38,
85–96 (2015).
9. Iotchev, I. B., Kis, A., Bódizs, R., van Luijtelaar, G. & Kubinyi, E. EEG transients in the sigma range during non-REM sleep predict
learning in dogs. Sci. Rep. 7, 12936 (2017).
Figure3. Associations between trait density (estimated here by averaging across recordings) and learning
success (also averaged for data set 2, based on nal performance) over the frontal midline electrode (Fz) in data
sets 09 and 2. Averaging learning gain was not possible in data set 0 as the experiment consisted of only one
learning condition. Data from the published data set 0 is presented in lighter colors.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol.:(0123456789)
Scientic Reports | (2020) 10:22461 | 
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
10. Iotchev, I. B. et al. Age-related dierences and sexual dimorphism in canine sleep spindles. Sci. Rep. 9, 10092 (2019).
11. Iotchev, I. B., Szabó, D., Kis, A. & Kubinyi, E. Possible association between spindle frequency and reversal-learning in aged family
dogs. Sci. Rep. 10, 6505 (2020).
12. Clemens, Z., Fabó, D. & Halász, P. Twenty-four hours retention of visuospatial memory correlates with the number of parietal
sleep spindles. Neurosci. Lett. 403, 52–56 (2006).
13. Clemens, Z., Fabó, D. & Halász, P. Overnight verbal memory retention correlates with the number of sleep spindles. Neuroscience
132, 529–535 (2005).
14. Cox, R., Hofman, W. F. & Talamini, L. M. Involvement of spindles in memory consolidation is slow wave sleep-specic. Learn.
Mem. 19, 264–267 (2012).
15. Gais, S., Mölle, M., Helms, K. & Born, J. Learning-dependent increases in sleep spindle density. J. Neurosci. 22, 6830–6834 (2002).
16. Kuula, L. et al. Higher sleep spindle activity is associated with fewer false memories in adolescent girls. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.
157, 96–105 (2019).
17. Seeck-Hirschner, M. et al. Declarative memory performance is associated with the number of sleep spindles in elderly women.
Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 20, 782–788 (2012).
18. Lustenberger, C. et al. Feedback-controlled transcranial alternating current stimulation reveals a functional role of sleep spindles
in motor memory consolidation. Curr. Biol. 26, 2127–2136 (2016).
19. Yordanova, J., Kolev, V., Bruns, E., Kirov, R. & Verleger, R. Sleep spindles in the right hemisphere support awareness of regularities
and reect pre-sleep activations. Sleep 40, 1–13 (2017).
20. B arakat, M. et al. Fast and slow spindle involvement in the consolidation of a new motor sequence. Behav. Brain Res. 217, 117–121
(2011).
21. Tamaki, M., Matsuoka, T., Nittono, H. & Hori, T. Activation of fast sleep spindles at the premotor cortex and parietal areas con-
tributes to motor learning: A study using sLORETA. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 878–886 (2009).
22. Astill, R. G. et al. Sleep spindle and slow wave frequency reect motor skill performance in primary school-age children. Front.
Hum. Neurosci. https ://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum .2014.00910 (2014).
23. Schmidt, C. et al. Encoding diculty promotes postlearning changes in sleep spindle activity during napping. J. Neurosci. 26,
8976–8982 (2006).
24. Ackermann, S., Hartmann, F., Papassotiropoulos, A., de Quervain, D. J. F. & Rasch, B. No associations between interindividual
dierences in sleep parameters and episodic memory consolidation. Sleep 38, 951–959 (2015).
25. Warby, S. C. et al. Sleep-spindle detection: Crowdsourcing and evaluating performance of experts, non-experts and automated
methods. Nat. Methods 11, 385–392 (2014).
26. Rosanova, M. & Ulrich, D. Pattern-specic associative long-term potentiation induced by a sleep spindle-related spike train. J.
Neurosci. 25, 9398–9405 (2005).
27. Latchoumane, C. F. V., Ngo, H. V. V., Born, J. & Shin, H. S. alamic spindles promote memory formation during sleep through
triple phase-locking of cortical, thalamic, and hippocampal rhythms. Neuron 95, 424-435.e6 (2017).
28. Nonclercq, A. et al. Sleep spindle detection through amplitude-frequency normal modelling. J. Neurosci. Methods 214, 192–203
(2013).
29. Ujma, P. P. Sleep spindles and general cognitive ability—A meta-analysis. Sleep Spindl. Cortical Up States. https ://doi.
org/10.1556/2053.2.2018.01 (2018).
30. Kis, A. et al. e interrelated eect of sleep and learning in dogs (Canis familiaris); an EEG and behavioural study. Sci. Rep. 7,
41873 (2017).
31. Kis, A. et al. Development of a non-invasive polysomnography technique for dogs (Canis familiar is). Physiol. Behav. 130, 149–156
(2014).
32. Gergely, A. et al. Reliability of family dogs’ sleep structure scoring based on manual and automated sleep stage identication.
Animals. https ://doi.org/10.3390/ani10 06092 7 (2020).
33. Eschenko, O., Molle, M., Born, J. & Sara, S. J. Elevated sleep spindle density aer learning or aer retrieval in rats. J. Neurosci. 26,
12914–12920 (2006).
34. Reynolds, C. M., Gradisar, M. & Short, M. A. Reliability of sleep spindle measurements in adolescents: How many nights are
necessary?. J. Sleep Res. https ://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12698 (2019).
35. Schmidt, F. L. & Hunter, J. E. Methods of Meta-Analysis: Correcting Error and Bias in Research Findings (SAGE Publications, ou-
sand Oaks, 2004).
36. Korman, M. et al. Daytime sleep condenses the time course of motor memory consolidation. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1206–1213 (2007).
37. Clemens, Z. et al. Fine-tuned coupling between human parahippocampal ripples and sleep spindles. Eur. J. Neurosci. 33, 511–520
(2011).
38. Lehmann, M., Schreiner, T., Seifritz, E. & Rasch, B. Emotional arousal modulates oscillatory correlates of targeted memory reac-
tivation during NREM, but not REM sleep. Sci. Rep. https ://doi.org/10.1038/srep3 9229 (2016).
39. Genzel, L., Kroes, M. C. W., Dresler, M. & Battaglia, F. P. Light sleep versus slow wave sleep in memory consolidation: A question
of global versus local processes?. Trends Neurosci. 37, 10–19 (2014).
40. Stickgold, R., James, L. & Hobson, J. Visual discrimination learning requires sleep aer training. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1237 (2000).
41. Tse, D. et al. Schemas and memory consolidation. Science 316, 76–82 (2007).
42. Silverstein, L. D. & Levy, C. M. e stability of the sigma sleep spindle. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 40, 666–670 (1976).
43. Gaillard, J. M. & Blois, R. Spindle density in sleep of normal subjects. Sleep 4, 385–391 (1981).
44. Hori, A. Sleep characteristics in twins. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 40, 35–46 (1986).
45. Gorgoni, M. et al. Parietal fast sleep spindle density decrease in Alzheimer’s disease and amnesic mild cognitive impairment. Neural
Plast. 2016, 10 (2016).
46. Ferrarelli, F. et al. Reduced sleep spindle activity in schizophrenia patients. Am. J. Psychiatry 164, 483–492 (2007).
47. Merikanto, I. et al. ADHD symptoms are associated with decreased activity of fast sleep spindles and poorer procedural overnight
learning during adolescence. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 157, 106–113 (2019).
48. Guazzelli, N. et al. Sleep spindles in normal elderly: Comparison with young adult patterns and relation to nocturnal awakening,
cognitive function and brain atrophy. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 63, 526–539 (1986).
49. Smirne, S. et al. Sleep in presenile dementia. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 43, 4 (1977).
50. Fogel, S. M., Nader, R., Cote, K. A. & Smith, C. T. Sleep spindles and learning potential. Behav. Neurosci. 121, 1–10 (2007).
Acknowledgements
We thank our dog trainers Rita Báji and Barbara Csibra for their contributions to the work behind data set 1,
as well as all owner-dog pairswho participated in the discussed studies; Borbála Turcsan, Tamás Faragó for
useful comments to this manuscript, and Péter Ujma for inspiring debates on the reliability of spindle-learning
correlations.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol:.(1234567890)
Scientic Reports | (2020) 10:22461 | 
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Author contributions
I.I. wrote the detection algorithm, analyzed the data and wrote the initial dra; R.V., A.K., E.Ko. and T.K. collected
data and participated in the writing, A.K., M.G. and E.Ku. supervised the research, participated in planning the
experiments and in the writing process.
Funding
is work has received funding from the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon
2020 research and innovation programme (Grant Agreement No. 680040), the National Research Development
and Innovation Oce (OTKA FK128242, K132372), the BIAL Foundation (Grant No 169/16), Hungarian Acad-
emy of Sciences (F01/031) and the János Bolyai Research Scholarship of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences.
Competing interests
e authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Supplementary Information e online version contains supplementary material available at https ://doi.
org/10.1038/s4159 8-020-80417 -8.
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to I.B.I.
Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
Open Access is article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the
Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. e images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from
the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creat iveco mmons .org/licen ses/by/4.0/.
© e Author(s) 2020
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
... In another line of research, untrained family dogs were measured non-invasively in a number of different sleep EEG (e.g. [19][20][21][22] ) and awake ERP 23,24 experiments. ...
... Dogs show significant individual-level variation in the morphological features of their head musculature, skull shape and thickness 57 that might have an influence on the EEG data. To prevent a measurement error arising from these differences, absolute power was normalized by computing the relative power spectra of the delta (1-4 Hz), theta (4-8 Hz), alpha (8-12 Hz), sigma (12)(13)(14)(15)(16) and beta (16)(17)(18)(19)(20)(21)(22)(23)(24)(25)(26)(27)(28)(29)(30) Hz) bands of NREM sleep. ...
... In senior subjects it seems that the proportion of delta power activity is lower (lowest in the first sleep measurement of the Senior wolf), while the proportion of the theta, alpha, sigma and beta frequency bands are higher compared to the young subjects. The individual relative power spectrum of delta (1-4 Hz), theta (4-8 Hz), alpha (8-12 Hz), sigma (12-16 Hz) and beta (16)(17)(18)(19)(20)(21)(22)(23)(24)(25)(26)(27)(28)(29)(30) ranges are visualized in Supplementary Fig. S6 (young animals) and in Supplementary Fig. S7 (seniors). In the case of young animals, the frequency range of 16-30 Hz was not visualized as it contained less than 0.03% of the whole relative power spectra. ...
Article
Full-text available
Sleep research greatly benefits from comparative studies to understand the underlying physiological and environmental factors affecting the different features of sleep, also informing us about the possible evolutionary changes shaping them. Recently, the domestic dog became an exceedingly valuable model species in sleep studies, as the use of non-invasive polysomnography methodologies enables direct comparison with human sleep data. In this study, we applied the same polysomnography protocol to record the sleep of dog’s closest wild relative, the wolf. We measured the sleep of seven captive (six young and one senior), extensively socialized wolves using a fully non-invasive sleep EEG methodology, originally developed for family dogs. We provide the first descriptive analysis of the sleep macrostructure and NREM spectral power density of wolves using a completely non-invasive methodology. For (non-statistical) comparison, we included the same sleep data of similarly aged dogs. Although our sample size was inadequate to perform statistical analyses, we suggest that it may form the basis of an international, multi-site collection of similar samples using our methodology, allowing for generalizable, unbiased conclusions. As we managed to register both macrostructural and spectral sleep data, our procedure appears to be suitable for collecting valid data in other species too, increasing the comparability of non-invasive sleep studies.
... Due to the relatively high heritability of these traits (47), some working dog organizations have drastically reduced their incidence through selective breeding programs. For example, in a study across three different breeds, the percentage of dogs with "excellent" hip scores was increased from 34-55% to 87-94% within eight generations of selection (208). ...
... Sleep appears to be another crucial variable (206). In dogs specifically, performance in learning new commands has been shown to be enhanced by sleep-related improvement in memory consolidation (203,207,208). Given that command learning is an integral part of working dog training, these findings, along with an emerging literature on the environmental factors that affect quality and quantity of sleep (209), are of great relevance. ...
Book
Full-text available
During two retreats in 2017 and 2020, a group of international scientists convened to explore the Human-Animal Bond. The meetings, hosted by the Wallis Annenberg PetSpace Leadership Institute, took a broad view of the human-dog relationship and how interactions between the two may benefit us medically, psychologically or through their service as working dogs (e.g. guide dogs, explosive detection, search and rescue, cancer detection). This Frontiers’ Special Topic has collated the presentations into a broad collection of 14 theoretical and review papers summarizing the latest research and practice in the historical development of our deepening bond with dogs, the physiological and psychological changes that occur during human-dog interactions (to both humans and dogs) as well as the selection, training and welfare of companion animals and working dogs. The overarching goals of this collection are to contribute to the current standard of understanding of human-animal interaction, suggest future directions in applied research, and to consider the interdisciplinary societal implications of the findings.
... Research has shown that electroencephalography can be used beyond the diagnosis of idiopathic or structural epilepsy. EEG has been used to monitor the sleep quality of dogs and correlate with ease of learning [11], indicate which foals are best managed as a function of sleep depth during the day and night [12]. In the behavioral field, it has been used to monitor the degree of stress (animal welfare) in horses [13], check neurological responses associated with behavioral changes in dogs [14] and horses [15]. ...
... Today it is a standard practice in human medicine and already starting in veterinary medicine, used in the assessment of brain activity. Based on the mathematical analysis of FFT (Fast-Fourier Transformation), the waves generated in the time domain are transformed into a graph the frequency domain, reflecting the intensity of the colors in relation to the intensity of the waves at a given frequency, using generally accepted limits of: delta (0-4 Hz), theta (4-8 Hz), alpha (8)(9)(10)(11)(12)(13) and beta (13)(14)(15)(16)(17)(18)(19)(20)(21)(22)(23)(24)(25)(26)(27)(28)(29)(30). The spectrogram (graph) changes dynamically and allows instant visual interpretation, providing data for various applications such as anesthetic monitoring, sleep disorders, epileptic disorders, level of consciousness, pain, brain injuries, cognition disorders, behavioral assessment, degenerative injuries, emotion recognition, machine learning, and others [27]. ...
Article
Full-text available
DESCRIPTION The Electroencephalic dysfunctions are permeated among several etiologies, such as neoplasms, endocrine and metabolic alterations, intoxications, morphological alterations, among others. To fully understand them, it is necessary to access not only the structural alterations of the brain, but also its functional disorders. To this end, only the electroencephalogram can provide information about the neuroelectrical activity [1]. The Electro Encephalo Gram (EEG) records spontaneous cortical electrical function and can identify waves with frequencies between 0.5 and 90 Hz and amplitude between 1 and 500 microvolts. The electrical signal originates mainly from three sources: the synaptic activity of cortical neurons, changes of action potentials at the surface of glial cells, and generalized electrical activity generated by large populations of neurons, mainly from deeper neuronal layers [2]. Although electroencephalography is a diagnostic method available for many years in veterinary medicine, having been employed in cattle since the work of Golikov in 1966; dogs [3], horses [4] and cats [5], its routine use in veterinary clinical neurology has received little attention [6]. Still, recent years have shown much promise for the experimental use of this technique, as it is being recognized as essential for the verification of brain function in association with imaging examinations, which only illustrate the structural portion [7]. The International Veterinary Epilepsy Task Force (IVETF) itself strongly recommends that EEG be used as a diagnostic method for idiopathic level III epilepsies [8]. In addition, EEG has evolved greatly due to the development of new technologies for data acquisition [9] and signal processing [10]. Research has shown that electroencephalography can be used beyond the diagnosis of idiopathic or structural epilepsy. EEG has been used to monitor the sleep quality of dogs and correlate with ease of learning [11], indicate which foals are best managed as a function of sleep depth during the day and night [12]. In the behavioral field, it has been used to monitor the degree of stress (animal welfare) in horses [13], check neurological responses associated with behavioral changes in dogs [14] and horses [15]. In humans it has been used as a method for classifying depression [16] and monitoring neuronal degeneration states [17]. Another extremely promising area of EEG use in animals is the quantitative aid in pain diagnosis. From verifying the pain associated with the quality of the slaughter blade in cattle [18], to proving the presence of pain in castrated calves without anesthetic procedures [19], the electroencephalogram has been very useful in proving and understanding pain mechanisms in animals. It is important to emphasize that pain is different from nociception because it requires brain processing of nociceptive impulses for it to occur. Thus, the EEG, as a method for evaluating brain activity, becomes an interesting quantitative tool for direct assessment of pain and not only nociception [20]. Immunogenetic studies have been associated with electroencephalography exams in order to gather information about brain function and gene expression in certain cerebral areas. Thus, relevant information has been obtained for understanding mechanisms such as narcolepsy [21], epilepsy [22], hereditary neurodegenerations [23], senile neurodegenerations [24], and adverse reactions to medications [25]. The most recent method for real-time EEG analysis is computer analysis of EEG signals in the form of power spectrum analysis [26]. Today it is a standard practice in human medicine and already starting in veterinary medicine, used in the assessment of brain activity. Based on the mathematical analysis of FFT (Fast-Fourier Transformation), the waves generated in the time domain are transformed into a graph the frequency domain, reflecting the intensity of the colors in relation to the intensity of the waves at a given frequency, using generally accepted limits of: delta (0-4 Hz), theta (4-8 Hz), alpha (8-13 Hz) and beta (13-30 Hz). The spectrogram (graph) changes dynamically and allows instant visual interpretation, providing data for various applications such as anesthetic monitoring, sleep disorders, epileptic disorders, level of consciousness, pain, brain injuries, cognition disorders, behavioral assessment, degenerative injuries, emotion recognition, machine learning, and others [27]. It is created during measurements (in real time) and does not require
... Pesquisas têm mostrado que a eletroencefalografia pode ser utilizada além do diagnóstico de epilepsia idiopática ou estrutural. O EEG tem sido utilizado para monitorar a qualidade do sono de cães e correlacionar com a facilidade de aprendizagem (Iotchev et al., 2020), indicar qual o melhor manejo de potros em função da profundidade do sono durante o dia e a noite (Zanker et al., 2021). No campo comportamental, tem sido usado para monitorar o grau de estresse (bem-estar animal) de cavalos (de Camp et al., 2020), verificar respostas neurológicas associadas a alterações comportamentais em cães (Wrzosek et al., 2015) e equinos (Pickles et al., 2011). ...
... A polissonografia foi utilizada para verificar o desempenho de aprendizagem de cães. A ocorrência de ondas do tipo fuso (spindle) ocorrem em transmissões tálamo-corticais durante o sono não REM de mamíferos, com frequências entre 9 e 16Hz e humanos (Bódizs et al., 2009) e 9-11Hz em cães (Iotchev et al., 2020). Sabe-se que a ocorrência destas ondas possui correlação positiva com o desempenho de aprendizagem em humanos, sendo que tal correlação também foi confirmada para cães. ...
... Pesquisas têm mostrado que a eletroencefalografia pode ser utilizada além do diagnóstico de epilepsia idiopática ou estrutural. O EEG tem sido utilizado para monitorar a qualidade do sono de cães e correlacionar com a facilidade de aprendizagem (Iotchev et al., 2020), indicar qual o melhor manejo de potros em função da profundidade do sono durante o dia e a noite (Zanker et al., 2021). No campo comportamental, tem sido usado para monitorar o grau de estresse (bem-estar animal) de cavalos (de Camp et al., 2020), verificar respostas neurológicas associadas a alterações comportamentais em cães (Wrzosek et al., 2015) e equinos (Pickles et al., 2011). ...
... A polissonografia foi utilizada para verificar o desempenho de aprendizagem de cães. A ocorrência de ondas do tipo fuso (spindle) ocorrem em transmissões tálamo-corticais durante o sono não REM de mamíferos, com frequências entre 9 e 16Hz e humanos (Bódizs et al., 2009) e 9-11Hz em cães (Iotchev et al., 2020). Sabe-se que a ocorrência destas ondas possui correlação positiva com o desempenho de aprendizagem em humanos, sendo que tal correlação também foi confirmada para cães. ...
Article
Full-text available
RESUMO A eletroencefalografia (EEG) é um método utilizado para a análise da atividade elétrica cerebral de humanos e animais. Possui baixo custo e métodos já estudados para as principais espécies de animais domésticos, tais como cães, gatos, equinos e bovinos. É um método minimamente invasivo que, ao ser associado com exames de avaliação estrutural, tais como a ressonância magnética e tomografia, permite elaborar um cenário mais completo sobre o estado geral do tecido neural cerebral. Sua aquisição de dados ao longo do tempo permite avaliar pacientes com epilepsia idiopática, epilepsias estruturais, metabólicas, hidrocefalia congênita e realizar monitoramento anestésico. O conhecimento de pós-processamento de sinais utilizando-se de FFT abre um universo de possibilidades para o uso da EEG, incluindo a polissonografia, avaliação de distúrbios comportamentais, neuropsíquicos, dor, cognição e aprendizagem. SUMMARY Electroencephalography (EEG) is a method used to analyze the electrical activity of the brain in humans and animals. It has low cost and methods already studied for the main species of domestic animals, such as dogs, cats, horses, and cattle. It is a minimally invasive method that, when associated with structural assessment tests such as MRI and CT, allows for a more complete picture of the general state of brain neural tissue. Its data acquisition over time allows the evaluation of patients with idiopathic epilepsy, structural epilepsies, metabolic epilepsies, congenital hydrocephalus, and to perform anesthesia monitoring. The knowledge of signal post-processing using FFT opens a universe of possibilities for the use of EEG, including polysomnography, evaluation of behavioral and neuropsychiatric disorders, pain, cognition, and learning.
... These results have been further validated using EEG, demonstrating a correlation between sleep spindle (non-REM bursts of activity in the sigma range) intrinsic frequency and the number of reversal learning training trials required to reach the criterion 17 . Sleep spindles predict learning in dogs and vary with age 18,19 . Ageing appeared to affect also dogs' ability to retain and later exploit spatial information. ...
Article
Full-text available
The prolonged lifespan of companion dogs has resulted in increased behavioural and physical challenges linked to old age. The development of behavioural tests to identify and monitor age‑related differences has begun. However, standardised testing requires validation. The present study aimed to assess external validity, interobserver reliability, and test–retest reliability of an indoor test battery for the rapid assessment of age‑related behavioural differences in dogs. Two experimenters tested young dogs (N = 20, mean age ± SD = 2.7 ± 0.4 years) and old dogs (N = 18, mean age ± SD = 11.8 ± 1.3 years) in the test battery once and then again after two weeks. Our results found external validity for two subtests out of six. On both test occasions, old dogs committed more errors than young dogs in a memory subtest and showed more object avoidance when encountering a novel object. Interobserver reliability and test–retest reliability was high. We conclude that the Memory and Novel object subtests are valid and reliable for monitoring age‑related memory performance and object neophobic differences in dogs.
... Impaired sleep is also associated with impaired working memory in humans (Chee and Choo, 2004;Steenari et al., 2003), therefore sleep impairments may affect the ability of dogs with osteoarthritis to solve problems or navigate around their environment, which could alter their ability to respond to training and to engage with the environment when walking with owners. In line with these suggestions, recent dog studies show that memory improvements in command learning tasks are related to EEG spectral features and spindle density during pre-test sleep periods Iotchev et al., 2017Iotchev et al., , 2020a, and that EEG sleep spindle frequency characteristics of individual dogs are associated with their reversal learning ability (Iotchev et al., 2020b). These findings suggest that, as in humans, there are links between sleep and cognitive performance in dogs and therefore that disrupted sleep may indeed have detrimental effects on dog learning and memory. ...
Article
Full-text available
Human patients with chronic pain from osteoarthritis often report impaired sleep, but it is not yet known if sleep is also impaired in dogs with osteoarthritis. This study aimed to compare the night-time sleep behaviour of osteoarthritic (N=20) and healthy control (N=21) dogs over a 28-day period, using an actigraphic device (the FitBark activity monitor) and an owner questionnaire designed to measure sleep quality (the SNoRE). Actigraphic data were aggregated to estimate the time each dog spent resting each night, and questionnaires were completed every 7 days. Data were analysed using robust mixed-effects linear regression. The presence of clinical signs of osteoarthritis had a significant effect on actigraphic recordings, with osteoarthritic dogs spending lower proportions of the night period resting (and therefore higher proportions of the night period active) compared to control dogs (z=2.21; P=0.0268). However, there was no significant difference between the SNoRE scores of osteoarthritic and control dogs (z=-1.01, p=0.312). The actigraphic findings of this study suggest that dogs with osteoarthritis may experience impaired sleep, which could have important welfare implications and merits further study.
... These results have been further validated using EEG, demonstrating a correlation between sleep spindle (non-REM bursts of activity in the sigma range) intrinsic frequency and the number of reversal learning training trials [17]. Sleep spindles predict learning in dogs, and vary with age [18,19]. A spatial memory task where dogs had to rely on short-term memory to nd food, was also developed, showing that younger dogs (3-6 years) were more e cient than older dogs (9-11 years), committing fewer errors and nding the food more often at their rst attempt. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The prolonged lifespan of companion dogs has resulted in an increased occurrence of behavioural and physical challenges linked to old age. The development of behavioural tests for identifying and monitoring age-related differences has begun. However, standardised testing requires validation. The present study aimed to assess external validity, interobserver reliability, and test-retest reliability of an indoor test battery for the rapid assessment of age-related behavioural differences in dogs. Two experimenters tested young and old dogs on a first occasion and after two weeks. Our results found external validity for two subtests out of six. On both test occasions, old dogs committed more errors than young dogs in a memory test and showed more object avoidance when encountering a novel object. Interobserver reliability and test-retest reliability was high. We conclude that the Memory and Novel object tests are valid and reliable for monitoring age-related memory performance and object neophobic differences in dogs.
... Sleep appears to be another crucial variable (206). In dogs specifically, performance in learning new commands has been shown to be enhanced by sleep-related improvement in memory consolidation (203,207,208). Given that command learning is an integral part of working dog training, these findings, along with an emerging literature on the environmental factors that affect quality and quantity of sleep (209), are of great relevance. ...
Article
Full-text available
Dogs are trained for a variety of working roles including assistance, protection, and detection work. Many canine working roles, in their modern iterations, were developed at the turn of the 20th century and training practices have since largely been passed down from trainer to trainer. In parallel, research in psychology has advanced our understanding of animal behavior, and specifically canine learning and cognition, over the last 20 years; however, this field has had little focus or practical impact on working dog training. The aims of this narrative review are to (1) orient the reader to key advances in animal behavior that we view as having important implications for working dog training, (2) highlight where such information is already implemented, and (3) indicate areas for future collaborative research bridging the gap between research and practice. Through a selective review of research on canine learning and behavior and training of working dogs, we hope to combine advances from scientists and practitioners to lead to better, more targeted, and functional research for working dogs.
Article
Full-text available
Subjective sleep disturbances are reported by humans with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). However, no consistent objective findings related to sleep disturbances led to the removal of sleep problems from ADHD diagnostic criteria. Dogs have been used as a model for human ADHD with questionnaires validated for this purpose. Also, their sleep physiology can be measured by non-invasive methods similarly to humans. In the current study, we recorded spontaneous sleep EEG in family dogs during a laboratory session. We analyzed the association of sleep macrostructure and deep sleep (NREM) slow-wave activity (SWA) with a validated owner-rated ADHD questionnaire, assessing inattention (IA), hyperactivity/impulsivity (H/I) and total (T) scores. Higher H/I and T were associated with lower sleep efficiency and longer time awake after initial drowsiness and NREM. IA showed no associations with sleep variables. Further, no association was found between ADHD scores and SWA. Our results are in line with human studies in which poor sleep quality reported by ADHD subjects is associated with some objective EEG macrostructural parameters. This suggests that natural variation in dogs’ H/I is useful to gain a deeper insight of ADHD neural mechanisms.
Article
Full-text available
Non-invasive polysomnography recording on dogs has been claimed to produce data comparable to those for humans regarding sleep macrostructure, EEG spectra and sleep spindles. While functional parallels have been described relating to both affective (e.g., emotion processing) and cognitive (e.g., memory consolidation) domains, methodologically relevant questions about the reliability of sleep stage scoring still need to be addressed. In Study 1, we analyzed the effects of different coders and different numbers of visible EEG channels on the visual scoring of the same polysomnography recordings. The lowest agreement was found between independent coders with different scoring experience using full (3 h-long) recordings of the whole dataset, and the highest agreement within-coder, using only a fraction of the original dataset (randomly selected 100 epochs (i.e., 100 × 20 s long segments)). The identification of drowsiness was found to be the least reliable, while that of non-REM (rapid eye movement, NREM) was the most reliable. Disagreements resulted in no or only moderate differences in macrostructural and spectral variables. Study 2 targeted the task of automated sleep EEG time series classification. Supervised machine learning (ML) models were used to help the manual annotation process by reliably predicting if the dog was sleeping or awake. Logistic regression models (LogREG), gradient boosted trees (GBT) and convolutional neural networks (CNN) were set up and trained for sleep state prediction from already collected and manually annotated EEG data. The evaluation of the individual models suggests that their combination results in the best performance: ~0.9 AUC test scores.
Article
Full-text available
In both humans and dogs sleep spindle occurrence between acquisition and recall of a specific memory correlate with learning performance. However, it is not known whether sleep spindle characteristics are also linked to performance beyond the span of a day, except in regard to general mental ability in humans. Such a relationship is likely, as both memory and spindle expression decline with age in both species (in dogs specifically the density and amplitude of slow spindles). We investigated if spindle amplitude, density (spindles/minute) and/or frequency (waves/second) correlate with performance on a short-term memory and a reversal-learning task in old dogs (> 7 years), when measurements of behavior and EEG were on average a month apart. Higher frequencies of fast (≥ 13 Hz) spindles on the frontal and central midline electrodes, and of slow spindles (≤ 13 Hz) on the central midline electrode were linked to worse performance on a reversal-learning task. The present findings suggest a role for spindle frequency as a biomarker of cognitive aging across species: Changes in spindle frequency are associated with dementia risk and onset in humans and declining learning performance in the dog.
Article
Full-text available
Non-REM bursts of activity in the sigma range (9–16 Hz) typical of sleep spindles predict learning in dogs, similar to humans and rats. Little is known, however, about the age-related changes in amplitude, density (spindles/minute) and frequency (waves/second) of canine spindles. We investigated a large sample (N = 155) of intact and neutered pet dogs of both sexes, varying in breed and age, searching for spindles in segments of non-REM sleep. We recorded EEG from both a frontal midline electrode (Fz) and a central midline electrode (Cz) in 55.5% of the dogs, in the remaining animals only the Fz electrode was active (bipolar derivation). A similar topography was observed for fast (≥13 Hz) spindle occurrence as in humans (fast spindle number, density on Cz > Fz). For fast spindles, density was higher in females, and increased with age. These effects were more pronounced among intact animals and on Fz. Slow spindle density declined and fast spindle frequency increased with age on Cz, while on Fz age-related amplitude decline was observed. The frequency of fast spindles on Fz and slow spindles on Cz was linked to both sex and neutering, suggesting modulation by sexual hormones. Intact females displayed higher frequencies than males and neutered females. Our findings support the argument that sigma bursts in the canine non-REM sleep are analogous to human sleep spindles, and suggest that slow and fast spindles display different trajectories related to age, of which an increase in frontal fast spindles is unique to dogs.
Article
Full-text available
The scientific discussion of sleep spindles now routinely mentions the association between these oscillations and cognitive ability. Numerous studies have been published on the topic with various methodologies and highly divergent results. In this meta-analysis of the relevant literature (total k = 22 articles, total N = 953 subjects), it is concluded that there is evidence for a modest positive association between cognitive ability and slow ( r = .113) and fast ( r = .183) spindle amplitudes and also some evidence for an association between cognitive ability and slow spindle duration ( r = .087) but none for associations with other parameters. Evidence for publication bias was found in case of fast spindle amplitude and density, but the correlation between cognitive ability and amplitude was robust to this effect. Studies with more females reported weaker associations with slow spindle duration, but this effect was driven by a single study with an all-male sample, and no other effect size was significantly moderated by age or sex. Most studies were conducted in small data sets and did not exhaustively report all measured associations. It is recommended that future studies having access to both sleep spindle and intelligence measures report their associations, regardless of their nature, that data sets be pooled across research groups for more statistical power, and that at least a basic agreement of spindle detection and classification criteria be reached in the research community.
Article
Full-text available
Evidence of night‐to‐night variation in adolescent sleep spindle characteristics is lacking. Twelve adolescents (M = 15.8 ± 0.8 years, eight males) participated in a laboratory study involving 9 nights with 10 hr sleep opportunity. Sleep electroencephalograph was analysed and intra‐class coefficients calculated to determine the reliability of sleep spindles across multiple nights of recording. Slow spindle amplitude and fast spindle density, duration and amplitude characteristics all had acceptable reliability within a single night of sleep recording. Slow spindle density and duration measurements needed a minimum of 4 and 2 nights, respectively, for reliable estimation. Theoretical and methodological implications are discussed.
Article
Sleep spindles are burst-like signals in the electroencephalogram (EEG) of the sleeping mammalian brain and electrical surface correlates of neuronal oscillations in thalamus. As one of the most inheritable sleep EEG signatures, sleep spindles probably reflect the strength and malleability of thalamocortical circuits that underlie individual cognitive profiles. We review the characteristics, organization, regulation and origins of sleep spindles and their implication in non-rapid-eye-movement sleep (NREMS) and its functions, focusing on human and rodent. Spatially, sleep spindle-related neuronal activity appears on scales ranging from small thalamic circuits to functional cortical areas, and generates a cortical state favoring intracortical plasticity while limiting cortical output. Temporally, sleep spindles are discrete events, part of a continuous power band, and elements grouped on an infraslow time scale over which NREMS alternates between continuity and fragility. We synthetize diverse and seemingly unlinked functions of sleep spindles for sleep architecture, sensory processing, synaptic plasticity, memory formation and cognitive abilities into a unifying sleep spindle concept, according to which sleep spindles a) generate neural conditions of large-scale functional connectivity and plasticity that outlast their appearance as discrete EEG events, b) appear preferentially in thalamic circuits engaged in learning and attention-based experience during wakefulness c) enable a selective re-activation and routing of wake-instated neuronal traces between brain areas such as hippocampus and cortex. Their fine spatiotemporal organization reflects NREMS as a physiological state coordinated over brain and body and may indicate, if not anticipate and ultimately differentiate, pathologies in sleep and neurodevelopmental, -degenerative and -psychiatric conditions.
Article
Background: Sleep facilitates the extraction of semantic regularities amongst newly encoded memories, which may also lead to increased false memories. We investigated sleep stage proportions and sleep spindles in the recollection of adolescents' false memories, and their potential sex-specific differences. Methods: 196 adolescents (mean age 16.9 y; SD = 0.1, 61% girls) underwent the Deese, Roediger & McDermott (DRM) false memory procedure and overnight polysomnography, with free recall the following morning. Sleep was scored manually into stages 1, 2, 3 and REM. Stage 2 sleep spindle frequency, density, and peak amplitude were used as measures of spindle activity for slow (10-13 Hz) and fast (13-16 Hz) ranges. Results: In girls, a lower number of critical lures was associated with higher spindle frequency (p ≤ 0.01), density (p ≤ 0.01), and amplitude (p = 0.03). Additionally, girls' longer sleep duration was associated with more intrusion words (p = 0.03), but not with critical lures. These associations survived adjustment for age, pubertal status, and intelligence. No significant results emerged in boys. Conclusions: In adolescent girls, higher spindle activity was associated with fewer critical lures being falsely recalled in the DRM paradigm. Unlike studies using adult participants, we did not observe any association between slow-wave sleep and false memory recollection.
Article
ADHD and its subclinical symptoms have been associated with both disturbed sleep and weakened overnight memory consolidation. As sleep spindle activity during NREM sleep plays a key role in both sleep maintenance and memory consolidation, we examined the association between subclinical ADHD symptoms and sleep spindle activity. Furthermore, we hypothesized that sleep spindle activity mediates the effect of ADHD symptoms on overnight learning outcome in a procedural memory task. We studied these questions in a community-based cohort of 170 adolescents (58% girls, mean age = 16.9, SD = 0.1 years), who filled in the Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS-v1.1), and underwent an overnight sleep EEG coupled with a mirror tracing task before and after sleep. Elevated ADHD symptoms were associated with weaker fast sleep spindle activity, and poorer overnight learning in the procedural memory test. However, sleep spindles, contrary to the hypothesis, did not mediate the association between ADHD symptoms and overnight learning. Our results showed that a higher level of ADHD symptoms in adolescence is associated with similar alterations in sleep spindle activity as observed in many neuropsychiatric conditions and might contribute to altered synaptic connectivity and sleep fragmentation observed in ADHD.