BookPDF Available

Explicit Learning in the L2 Classroom

Authors:
Explicit Learning in the L2 Classroom offers a unique five-prong (theoretical,
empirical, methodological, pedagogical, and model-building) approach to the
issue of explicit learning in the L2 classroom from a student-centered perspective.
To achieve this five-prong objective, the book reports the theoretical underpin-
nings, empirical studies, and research designs employed in current research to
investigate the constructs of attention and awareness in SLA, with the objectives
to (1) propose a model of the L2 learning process in Instructed SLA that accounts
for the cognitive processes employed during this process and (2) provide peda-
gogical and curricular implications for the L2 classroom. The book also pro-
vides a comprehensive treatise of research methodology that is aimed at not only
underscoring the major features of conducting robust research designs with high
levels of inter n a l va lid ity but also preparing teacher s to become crit ica l readers of
published empirical research.
Ronald P. Leow is Professor of Applied Linguistics and Director of Spanish
Language Instruction in the Department of Spanish and Portuguese at George-
town University. His areas of expertise include language curriculum develop-
ment, teacher education, SLA, psycholinguistics, cognitive processes in language
learning, research methodology, and CALL.
EXPLICIT LEARNING IN THE
L2 CLASSROOM
6241-0749-0FM.indd i6241-0749-0FM.indd i 1/29/2015 10:13:12 AM1/29/2015 10:13:12 AM
Monographs on Theoretical Issues:
Schachter/Gass Second Language Classroom Research: Issues and Opportunities
(1996)
Birdsong Second Language Acquisition and the Critical Period Hypotheses (1999)
Ohta Second Language Acquisition Processes in the Classroom: Learning Japanese
(2001)
Major Foreign Accent: Ontogeny and Phylogeny of Second Language Phonology
(2001)
Va n P a t t e n Processing Instruction: Theory, Research, and Commentar y (2003)
Va n P at t e n / W i l lia m s / Rot t/Ove rs treet Form-Meaning Connections in Second
Language Acquisition (2004)
Bardovi-Harlig/Hartford Interlanguage Pragmatics: Exploring Institutional
Talk (2005)
Dörnyei The Psychology of the Language Learner: Individual Differences in
Second Language Acquisition (2005)
Long Problems in SLA (2007)
Va n P at t e n / W i l lia m s Theories in Second Language Acquisition (2007)
Second Language Acquisition Research Series :
Theoretical and Methodological Issues
Susan M. Gass and Alison Mackey, Editors
6241-0749-0FM.indd ii6241-0749-0FM.indd ii 1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM
Ortega/Byrnes The Longitudinal Study of Advanced L2 Capacities (2008)
Liceras/Zobl/Goodluck The Role of Formal Features in Second Language
Acquisition (2008)
Phi lp/Adams/Iwashita Peer Interaction and Second Language Learning (2013)
Va n P at t e n / W i l lia m s Theories in Second Language Acquisition, Second Edition
(2014)
Leow Explicit Learning in the L2 Classroom (2015)
Monographs on Research Methodology:
Ta r o n e /Gass/C o h e n Research Methodology in Second Language Acquisition
(1994)
Yu l e Referential Communication Tasks (1997)
Gass/Mackey Stimulated Recall Methodolog y in Second Language Research
(2000)
Markee Conversation Analysis (2000)
Gass/Mackey Data Elicitation for Second and Foreign Language Research (2007)
Duf f Case Study Research in Applied Linguistics (2007)
McDonough/Trofimov ich Using Priming Methods in Second Language
Research (2008)
Larson-Hall A Guide to Doing Statistics in Second Language Research Using
SPSS (2009)
Dörnyei/Taguchi Questionnaires in Second Language Research: Construction,
Administration, and Processing, 2nd Edition (2009)
Bowles The Think-Aloud Controversy in Second Language Research (2010)
Jiang Conducting Reaction Time Research for Second Language Studies (2011)
Barkhuizen/Benson/Chik Narrative Inquiry in Language Teaching and
Learning Research (2013)
Jegerski/VanPatten Research Methods in Second Language Psycholinguistics
(2013)
6241-0749-0FM.indd iii6241-0749-0FM.indd iii 1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM
Of Related Interest:
Gass Input, Interaction, and the Second Language Learner (1997)
Gass/Sorace/Selinker Second Language Learning Data Analysis, Second Edition
(1998)
Mackey/Gass Second Language Research: Methodology and Design (2005)
Gass/Selinker Second Language Acquisition: An Introductory Course, Third
Edition (2008)
6241-0749-0FM.indd iv6241-0749-0FM.indd iv 1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM
EXPLICIT LEARNING IN
THE L2 CLASSROOM
A Student-Centered Approach
Ronald P. Leow
Georgetown University
6241-0749-0FM.indd v6241-0749-0FM.indd v 1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM
First published 2015
by Routledge
711 Third Avenue, New York, NY 10017
and by Routledge
2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abi ngdon, Oxon, OX14 4RN
Routledge i s an impr int of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business
© 2015 Taylor & Francis
The r ight of Rona ld P. Leow to be identif ied as author of this work
has been asser ted by hi m in accord ance with sections 77 and 78 of the
Copyright, Desig ns and Patents Act 1988.
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced
or utilized in any form or by any electron ic, mechanical, or other
means, now k nown or hereafter invented, including photocopying and
recording, or in any i nformation storage or retrieval system, without
permission in writ ing from the publishers.
Tra d e m a rk n o t i c e : Product or corporate na mes may be trademarks
or registered tr adema rks, and are used only for ident if ication a nd
explan ation without intent to infringe.
Library of Congress Cataloging- in-Publication Data
Leow, Ronald P. (Ronald Phi lip), 1954–
Explicit learning i n the L2 classroom : a student-centered approach /
Ronald P. Leow, Georgetown Universit y.
pages cm. — (Second lang uage acquisit ion research series)
Includes bibliographical references and index.
1. Language and languages—Study and teaching. 2. Second lang uage
acquisition. I. Title.
P51.L4968 2015
418.00 71— d c23
2014036365
ISBN: 978-0-415-70705-3 (hbk)
ISBN: 978-0-415-70706-0 (pbk)
ISBN: 978-1-315-88707-4 (ebk)
Typeset in Bembo
by Apex CoVantage, LLC
6241-0749-0FM.indd vi6241-0749-0FM.indd vi 1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM
To Deborah, Philip, and Stephanie
6241-0749-0FM.indd vii6241-0749-0FM.indd vii 1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM
6241-0749-0FM.indd viii6241-0749-0FM.indd viii 1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM
Preface xi
Acknowledgments xviii
1 Introduction, or Strolling Down Memory Lane: Raising
Your Awareness 1
SECTION 1
Theoretical Foundations 13
2 A Preliminary Theoretical Framework for the L2
Learning Process in SLA 15
3 Theoretical Foundations for the Role of Attention in
Learning from Non-SLA Fields 23
4 Theoretical Foundations for the Role of Awareness in
Learning from Non-SLA Fields 48
5 Theoretical Foundations for the Roles of Attention and
Awareness in L2 Learning in SLA 68
SECTION 2
Research Methodology 107
6 Methodological Issues in Research on the Relationships
between Attention, Awareness, and L2 Learning in SLA 109
CONTENTS
6241-0749-0FM.indd ix6241-0749-0FM.indd ix 1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM
x Contents
7 Deconstructing the Construct of Learning 123
8 Location, Location, Location: Probing Inside the Box 136
SECTION 3
Empirical Research Investigating the Role of Attention/
Noticing in L2 Development 157
9 Your Attention, Please 159
10 Learning Explicitly or Implicitly: That Is the Question 184
11 Depth of Processing in L2 Processing 203
SECTION 4
Model Building 237
12 Toward a Model of the L2 Learning Process in Instructed SLA 239
SECTION 5
Pedagogy 251
13 Toward the Development of Psycholinguistics-Based
E-Tutors 253
14 Conclusion: The Changing L2 Classroom, and Where Do
We Go From Here? 270
Index 279
6241-0749-0FM.indd x6241-0749-0FM.indd x 1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM
To begin at the beginning: Let us begin with two agreements, one professional,
one theoretical. Professionally, no one will disagree with the statement that
teaching is one of the most rewarding and, at the same time, potentially one
of the most frustrating professions to undertake (yes, I am going to wear two
hats in this book: Teacher’s, based on four decades of teaching experience, and
researcher’s, based on my research in the second language acquisition (SLA) field
over the last two decades). It is rewarding when our students perform according
to our objectives and “master/mistress” plans, and frustrating when, despite our
efforts to facilitate their learning by providing them with an appropriate
environment, and adequate exposure to and practice with the second or foreign
(L2) language (accompanied by much love), they apparently fail to grasp even
the “simplest(that is, from our perspective) grammatical rule we teach them
or to which we expose them. What can explain this apparent contradiction?
To answer this question, let us discuss some important processes in language
learning. First, did you pay attention to or notice the quotation marks around
the descriptors “appropriate” and “simplest”? I even bolded them to draw your
attention to them, with the hope of making you process them at a deeper level
(this, incidentally, is the argument behind any effort to make specific aspects of
the written L2 (and even oral L2 via pauses, intonation, funny facial expressions,
etc.) more salient to L2 learners, and falls in the research strand of input/textual
enhancement, which I will discuss later). Were you aware (pun intended) that
there are deeper connotations embedded in these words? You could have paid
more attention to these words but perhaps did not process them further—but
that is getting into your heads, an internal process. “Simplest” refers to a teacher-
centered perspective of what constitutes a simple rule for the teacher and not
necessarily from a student-centered perspective. “Appropriate” is based on our
PREFACE
6241-0749-0FM.indd 11 1/29/2015 11:06:29 AM
xii Preface
perception of language learning and teaching. Take a pause and contemplate this
simple but challenging question: What is your perception of language learning
and teaching ? In other words, how do we think an L2 is learned (or acquired),
and how should it be taught or presented? This is what drives all of us in the
“classroom” setting (OK, these quotation marks around “classroom” refer to the
fact that nowadays we have the hybrid curriculum in which the classroom can
be physical or virtual/electronic or both).
There are many responses to this simple but challenging question, some based
on personal experience or attendance at teacher education courses, some based
on the SLA and/or non-SLA (e.g., cognitive psychology or cognitive science,
cognitive neuroscience, etc.) literatures, some based on a combination of two
or more, and so on. However, regardless from where our perception may be
derived, the fact remains that we, however hard we try, cannot learn for our
students. In other words, learning is an internal process that may or may not be
manipulated by external factors and, as we know, learning may be explicit, that
is, with awareness, or implicit, that is, without awareness.
What, then, is awareness? Are you aware that the literature in both SLA and
non-SLA fields is literally littered with the role of awareness being explicitly or
implicitly subsumed within a remarkable number of variables? We have type of
learning (e.g., subliminal, incidental, implicit, explicit), type of learning con-
dition (e.g., implicit, explicit), type of instructional condition (e.g., implicit,
explicit, inductive, deductive), type of awareness (e.g., language, metacognitive,
phenomenal, situational, self, conscious, unconscious) and so on. We also have
constructs such as noticing (attention with awareness), detection (attention, cog-
nitive registration without awareness), perception (with or without awareness),
consciousness (even conscious awareness! So we can assume there is unconscious
awareness?), and the list goes on. There are quite a large number of definitions,
but here are two: “[T]he function of the interpretation of the nature of the
encoding and retrieval processes required by the task” (Robinson, 1995: 301),
and “a particular state of mind in which an individual has undergone a specific
subjective experience of some cognitive content or external stimulus” (Tomlin &
Villa, 1994: 193). For now, simply note that the construct of awareness as defined
refers to something taking place in our brain as we process language.
Now we can go to the theoretical agreement: We are still not sure which type
of learning (explicit, that is, with awareness, or implicit, that is, without aware-
ness) works better for our students in the L2 classroom. In other words, the role
awareness plays in the learning process is a theoretically valid question and it plays
out every day in our classrooms (with or without our own awareness).
Yet from a practitioner’s perspective, we “know” (as in “intuition” that comes
from vast experience) that raising our students’ awareness of grammatical rules,
learning strategies, etc., is the way to go. Many if not all of us even do it today in
our classrooms, since it feels so natural and instinctive! Take a look at every sin-
gle foreign language textbook. Did you find one WITHOUT any grammatical
6241-0749-0FM.indd xii6241-0749-0FM.indd xii 1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM
Preface xiii
ru les? It i s not going to sel l becaus e we t eacher s (a nd student s) exp ect such g r am-
matical explanations, at least in some form, be it traditional—for example, com-
plete verbal paradigms—or a little more progressive, as in partial paradigms that
only address the persons involved (e.g., you and I , y yo , je et tu , etc.). For those
teachers who have been in the profession as long as I have been, I am sure you
will fondly remember the good old days of the Grammar Translation Method,
when we taught the grammatical rules and then told our students to apply the
rules clearly (from our perspective), with a high level of grammatical awareness,
in translations. For our slightly younger teachers, you have likely been exposed
to myriad methods and theoretical perceptions that view language learning from
either a formal or traditional perspective (grammar comes first), an informal
perspective (grammar is embedded in the L2 input, so you are wasting your time
teaching it), or a combination of the two. Any way we look at all these perspec-
tives with regard to grammar instruction or exposure, and their relationships
with the role of awareness of these grammar rules, the focus has clearly shifted
from a teacher-centered perspective to a more learner-centered one (at least for
mo st o f us), albe it w ith one cavea t : Do e slea r n er-cente r ednes s” mean t h at (1) we
make our students more active by participating in more activities, etc., but we
still provide the essential grammatical information based on our individual per-
ception of how the learning process operates, or (2) we make our students more
responsible for the learning process obviously premised on a better understand-
ing of the internal processes involved in language learning? The irony of the
current status of language learning and teaching, however, may lie in the absence
of a clear treatise on the roles of many important variables postulated to promote
language learning that not only are theoretically and empirically supported but
also offer solid pedagogical implications for the L2 classroom.
Explicit Learning in the L2 Classroom: A Student-Centered Approach , then, is a
book that provides a theoretically grounded and empirically supported approach
to the promotion of explicit learning, that is, learning with awareness, in L2
development with a direct connection to learning in the classroom setting. Put
another way, it is not a book on justifying the teaching of grammatical rules in
the second/foreign language (L2) classroom. It is also not a book on the acquisition
of an L2 in an L2 setting since the author believes that this kind of formal setting
does not lend itself to the natural acquisition process that is usually associated
with a process similar to that of a child acquiring his/her f irst language (e.g., L2
classroom contexts are often characterized by impoverished input or L2, an inad-
equate amount of exposure and interaction, and practice and homework focused
on grammar, be it by the teacher or the textbook, etc.). It is a book on learning ,
a process that involves quite a lot of processing and potential learner awareness
while interacting with the L2 inside and outside the classroom setting. I shall elab-
orate on, or, more specifically, deconstruct this construct of learning in Chapter 7 .
Now that I have gotten your attention ( I am as su m ing th at si nce you have read
this far you must have been paying some attention to the message and, in some
6241-0749-0FM.indd xiii6241-0749-0FM.indd xiii 1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM
xiv Preface
cases, processing and interacting with the content from a personal viewpoint),
the main purpose of this book is to raise your awareness (pun intended) of many
important variables that contribute to language learning and, ultimately, to lan-
guage teaching. We are going to take a closer and critical look at internal cog-
nitive processes and, more specifically, constructs such as attention, awareness,
and working memory, together with activation of prior knowledge and depth of
processing postulated to play important roles in language learning, from both a
theoretical and empirical perspective, in order to better inform ourselves as we
teach in the L2 “classroom” (think hybrid).
For those readers who were processing the above information with some level
of awareness and making inferences with respect to the title of this book, it
will come as no surprise that the overwhelming number of studies that have
empirically and directly or indirectly investigated the construct of awareness or
lack thereof in L2 development (e.g., de la Fuente, 2015; Bowles, 2003; Faretta-
Stutenberg & Morgan-Short, 2011; Hama & Leow, 2010; Hsieh, Moreno, &
Leow, 2015; Leow, 1997, 1998, 2000, 2001, 2015; Leung & Williams, 2011, 2012;
Martínez-Fernández, 2008; Medina, 2015; Robinson, 1996; Rosa & Leow, 2004;
Rosa & O’Neill, 1999; Sachs & Suh, 2007; Williams, 2005) have reported sub-
stantial and robust support for its role in the learning process, that is, explicit learn-
ing in the classroom setting viewed from an internal perspective rocks ! However,
please do not take my word since, as a human being, I may be biased, given that
the bulk of my published research lies in the attentional and awareness strands of
research. There is absolutely no need on my part to mention that the classroom
setting is far more conducive to explicit learning than implicit learning, for
obvious reasons. So what I propose to offer in this book is simply a presentation
of the facts—from an unbiased perspective—and you, my dear readers, will be
the judge of the substance and purpose of the book: Promoting explicit learning,
that is, learning with learner awareness in the L2 classroom.
To this end, I shall present the relevant data from a five-prong approach (theo-
retical, empirical, methodological, model-building, and pedagogical). Section 1
is all theoretical. Without theory, everything is explained in an ad hoc way, which
is not very scholarly—plus I sound smart. Chapter 1 takes a stroll down memory
lane to situate the construct of awareness in the SLA literature. To situate the the-
oretical foundations for explicit learning in both SLA and non-SLA, Chapter 2
presents, from a psycholinguistic perspective, a coarse-grained theoretical frame-
work of the L2 learning process in SLA, to which we shall refer several times
throughout the book. Chapters 3 and 4 then present relatively broad overviews
of several major theoretical underpinnings postulated for the roles of attention
and awareness in learning in non-SLA fields that have influenced those in SLA.
Chapter 5 provides a more in-depth discussion of the theoretical underpinnings
postulated for the roles of attention and awareness in language learning , together
with a summary of their major tenets postulated to account for the learning
process in SLA.
6241-0749-0FM.indd xiv6241-0749-0FM.indd xiv 1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM
Preface xv
Before we discuss the empirical research, we need to have a relatively good idea
of what constitutes a robust research design that produces findings upon which
we can place a high level of confidence. Let’s get critical! To this end, Section 2
(Research Methodology) provides an in-depth report of the heart of any empirical
study, namely the research design, and its corresponding level of internal validity.
Chapter 6 focuses on the methodological issues surrounding the investigation of
the relationship between the roles of attention and awareness, or lack thereof, and
learning in the SLA field. Chapter 7 presents a tri-dimensional perspective of the
construct of learning to address the potential terminological confusion as to what
comprises “learning” in SLA. Chapter 8 addresses three major concurrent data
elicitation procedures (reaction time, eye-tracking, and think aloud) employed to
gather data on learner processes while they are interacting with L2 data, and their
benefits to further research on learner processes. Related methodological issues
such as reactivity and veridicality are also discussed.
Section 3 (Empirical Research) then provides synopses of the empirical research
conducted in SLA on the roles of attention/noticing ( Chapter 9 ) and awareness or
lack thereof ( Chapter 10 ) in L2 development. As I mentioned above, Section 2 will
assist you in being critical of published work. Chapter 11 discusses the concept of
depth of processing in the L2 learning process and suggests that we centralize the
process of learning not on the construct of attention, which clearly plays a crucial
role in L2 learning, but on the notion of how L2 learners process the L2 data.
Theoretical, methodological, empirical, and pedagogical benef its are discussed.
Section 4 (Model Building) presents in Chapter 12 a proposed model of the
L2 learning process in Instructed SLA that draws from previous theoretical
underpinnings in both the SLA and non-SLA fields, and attempts to capture the
important roles several cognitive processes play along the L2 learning process
from input to output. Learning will be represented in this model as being both
a process and a product (knowledge), and special emphasis will be placed on the
potential roles attention, depth of processing, (levels of ) awareness, and activa-
tion of prior knowledge play along several stages in the learning process.
Section 5 (Pedagogy) is based exclusively on the previous chapters. Chapter 13
provides suggestions for the development of psycholinguistics-based tasks and
e-tutors designed to enhance robust learning, especially of problematic gram-
matical points in the L2. Specific examples are provided. Finally, Chapter 14
discusses some conclusions and questions gleaned from the previous chapters,
reports on the inroads technology is currently making at both the curricular and
instructional level leading to the changing dynamics of the L2 classroom, and
provides one feasible curricular suggestion, namely, a partial hybrid curriculum,
to embrace the changing format of the traditional L2 classroom, the role of tech-
nology, and Instructed SLA research.
It is hoped that after reading this book, readers’ awareness of several impor-
tant variables postulated to contribute to language learning (and teaching) will
be raised, together with more creative ways to enhance and stimulate students’
6241-0749-0FM.indd xv6241-0749-0FM.indd xv 1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM
xvi Preface
explicit learning, that is, learning with awareness, in the L2 classroom. While
Baars (1997) wrote, “Paying attention—becoming conscious of some material—
seems to be the sovereign remedy for learning anything applicable to many very
different kinds of information. It is the universal solvent of the mind” (sec. 5,
p. 304), I personally like to think that the depth of processing, with its potential
of raising the level of awareness, is an important step to potential change in the
learning process.
References
Baars, B. J. (1997). In the theater of consciousness: The workspace of the mind. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Bowles, M. A. (2003). The effects of textual input enhancement on language learning:
An online/off line study of fourth-semester Spanish students. In P. Kempchinsky &
C. E. Piñeros (Eds.), Theory, practice, and acquisition: Papers from the 6th Hispanic Lin-
guistics Symposium and the 5th Conference on the Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese (pp.
395–411). Somer ville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
De la Fuente, M. (2015). Explicit cor rective feedback and computer-based, form-focused
instruction: The role of L1 in promoting awareness of L2 forms. In R. P. Leow,
L. Cerezo, & M. Baralt (Eds.), A psycholinguistic approach to technology and language
learning. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.
Faretta-Stutenberg, M., & Morgan-Short, K. (2011). Learning w ithout awareness recon-
sidered: A replication of Williams (2005). In Granena, G., Koeth, J., Lee-Ellis, S.,
Lukyanchenko, A., Prieto Botana, G., & Rhoades, E. (Eds.), Selected proceedings of the
2010 Second Language Research Forum: Reconsidering SLA research, dimensions, and direc-
tions (pp. 18–28). Somerville, MA: Cascadil la Proceedings Project.
Hama, M., & Leow, R. P. (2010). Learning without awareness revisited: Extending Wil-
liams (2005). Studies in Second Language Acquisition , 32 , 465–491.
Hsieh, H-C., Moreno, N., & Leow, R. P. (2015). A comparison of level of awareness and
depth of processing in two types of instructional media (C-FTF vs. CAI): Revisiting
Hsieh (2008). In R. P. Leow, L. Cerezo, & M. Baralt (Eds.), A psycholinguistic approach
to technology and language learning. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.
Leow, R. P. (1997). Attention, awareness, and foreign language behavior. Language Learn-
ing, 47 , 467–506.
Leow, R. P. (1998). Toward operationa lizing the process of attention in second lang uage
acquisition: Evidence for Tomlin and Villa’s (1994) fine-grained analysis of attention.
Applied Psycholinguistics, 19 , 133–159.
Leow, R. P. (2000). A study of the role of awareness in foreign language behavior: Aware
versus unaware learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition , 22 , 557–584.
Leow, R. P. (2001). Attention, awareness and foreign language behavior. Language Learn-
ing , 51 , 113 –155 .
Leung, J. H. C., & Williams, J. N. (2011). The implicit learning of mappings bet ween
forms and contextually derived meanings. Studies in Second Language Acquisition , 33 ,
33–55.
Leung, J. H. C., & Williams, J. N. (2012). Constraints on implicit learning of grammati-
cal form-meaning connections. Language Learning , 62 , 634–662.
6241-0749-0FM.indd xvi6241-0749-0FM.indd xvi 1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM
Preface xvii
Martínez-Fernández, A. (2008). Revisiting the involvement load hypothesis: Awareness,
type of task and type of item. In M. Bowles, R. Foote, S. Perpiñán, & R. Bhatt (Eds.),
Selected proceedings of the 2007 Second Language Research Forum (pp. 210–228). Somer-
ville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.
Medina, A. (2015). The variable effects of level of awareness and CALL versus non-
CALL textual modification on adult L2 readers’ input comprehension and learning.
In R. P. Leow, L. Cerezo, & M. Baralt (Eds.), A psycholinguistic approach to technology
and language learning. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.
Robinson, P. (1995). Review article: Attention, memory and the ‘noticing’ hypothesis.
Language Learning , 45 , 283–331.
Robinson, P. (1996). Learning simple and complex second language rules under implicit,
incidental, rule-search and instructed conditions. Studies in Second Language Acquisi-
tion , 18 , 27–68.
Rosa, E., & O’Neill, M. D. (1999). Explicitness, intake, and the issue of awareness. Studies
in Second Language Acquisition , 21 , 511–5 56.
Rosa, E. M., & Leow, R. P. (2004). Awareness, different learning conditions, and second
language development. Applied Psycholinguistics , 25 , 269–292.
Sachs, R., & Suh, B-R. (2007). Textually enhanced recasts, learner awareness, and L2 out-
comes in synchronous computer-mediated interaction. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversa-
tional interaction in second-language acquisition: A series of empirical studies (pp. 197–227).
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Tomlin, R. S., & Villa, V. (1994). Attention in cognitive science and second language
acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition , 16 , 18 3 –2 03 .
Williams, J. N. (2005). Learning without awareness. Studies in Second Language Acquisi-
tion , 27 , 269–304.
6241-0749-0FM.indd xvii6241-0749-0FM.indd xvii 1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM
I would like to thank all my previous teachers from pre-kindergarten to col-
lege (especially James F. Lee, my mentor, and Bill VanPatten); my current and
former students/colleagues who have, in so many ways, contributed to the
content of this book; and especially the think aloud/awareness/CALL group
(Elena Rosa, Miguel Angel Novella, Maite Camblor, Melissa Bowles, Ana-María
Nuevo, Ya-Chin Tsai, Takako Egi, Maite Camblor, Almitra Medina, Claudia
Guidi, Laura Gurzynski-Weiss, Rebecca Sachs, YunDeok Choi, Maymona Khalil
Al Khalil, Marisa Filgueras Gómez, de la Fuente), the research methodological
group (Kara Morgan-Short, Bo-Ram Suh, Melissa Baralt, Luis Cerezo, Mika
Hama, Ellen Johnson Sarafini, Germán Zárate-Sández, Sarah Grey, Silvia Mari-
juan, Colleen Moorman), the more recent depth-of-processing group (Hui-
Chen Hsieh, Nina Moreno, Ana Martínez-Fernández, Annie Calderón, Sergio
Andrada, Johnathan Mercer), and my two co-creators of the criteria for coding
depth of processing (Annie Calderón and Ellen Johnson Sarafini) in Chapter 11.
Thanks also to the Initiative on Technology-Enhanced Learning (ITEL) grant
(coordinated by Peter Janssens) awarded by Georgetown University’s Center for
New Designs in Learning and Scholarship (CNDLS) that permitted me to cre-
ate the Gustar Maze that was designed by Bill Garr and extended by Allison
Caras. A special thanks to my graphic designer Steven Mercer for the model
of the L2 learning process in Instructed SLA, Johnathan Mercer (yes, they are
siblings) for contributing to the visual conceptualization of the model, and Celia
Zamora for the indexing. Finally, much appreciation to my two editors (Susan
Gass and Alison Mackey) who have been, without their awareness, sources of
inspiration to me, and to Renata Corbani and Carmen Baumann for making the
publishing process enjoyable.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
6241-0749-0FM.indd xviii6241-0749-0FM.indd xviii 1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM1/29/2015 10:13:13 AM
Before I jump into the nitty-gritty of the book components, I would like f irst to
broadly describe what comprises L2 learning in this book and where this book is
situated, and then I would like to take a quick stroll down memory lane regard-
ing the many changes pertaining to our students’ role in L2 learning in the SLA
field, dating back to the theoretical and pedagogical approaches to learning in
the 1960s to the current empirical focus on implicit learning in SLA.
L2 Learning and Setting
L2 learning, for now ( Chapter 7 elaborates on this construct), can be described
broadly as a process in which many changes take place in L2 learners’ cogni-
tion as they try to create new representations for the L2 grammar, internalize
such data, and restructure if necessary, all the while developing their ability to
comprehend and produce the L2, either orally or written, in real time. It takes
place in a setting in which the L2 is either viewed as a foreign language (as in
English speakers t aking the foreig n lan guag e requ i rement i n an L1 env ironment)
or a second language (as in Japanese speakers taking English classes in an L2
environment, for example, in the USA). In either setting, L2 learners are exposed
to naturally occurring languages and are interacting with the language, be it
communicatively or performing a task of some ecological validity. In the typical
formal classroom setting, the L2 is taught by an instructor, and students learn
the so-called traditional four skills of listening, reading, writing, and speaking.
There is a curriculum that provides information on, for example, the grading
criteria, attendance policy, percent weight of each section of the curriculum,
objectives for all four skills, and a syllabus that provides a guideline for each class
session. Homework is usually assigned and students follow a prescribed textbook.
1
INTRODUCTION, OR STROLLING
DOWN MEMORY LANE
Raising Your Awareness
6241-0749-001.indd 16241-0749-001.indd 1 1/29/2015 10:48:29 AM1/29/2015 10:48:29 AM
2 Introduction
The amount of time spent in this formal setting varies, but the minimum is usu-
ally around one hour either daily (for intensive classes) or three or four times a
week (for non-intensive classes, depending upon the language program). SLA
research that seeks to probe into learner cognition, then, needs to focus on the
identification and explanation of the cognitive processes employed by L2 learners
as they learn the L2 in these two settings.
Memory Lane
Now, let us situate this book’s perspective regarding our students’ role in L2
learning by taking a quick stroll down memory lane over the many selected
changes in focus, both theoretical and empirical, toward what Omaggio (1993)
calls the “presumed locus of control of the process of language acquisition” (p. 43)
over the last several decades.
First, a quick pop quiz: How many of you are aware that the construct of
awareness has always been subsumed in the teaching profession? Isn’t it true that
we language teachers—well, most of us—have this innate desire to promote our
students “knowledge” of what they are learning or their awareness of what they
are producing? Be it theoretically, empirically, or pedagogically driven, we, as
teachers of L2 lang uages, do incorporate activities or tasks that require some role
of awareness or lack thereof on our students’ part. Now that we are on the same
page, let us proceed to some previous theoretical perspectives regarding the L2
learning process.
As many of us will recall, the two dominant theoretical approaches to L2
learning in the 50s and early 60s were the behaviorist/empiricist (e.g., Hilgard,
1962; Skinner, 1957) versus the rationalist/mentalist/nativist (e.g., Chomsky,
1957) perspective of learning. The former postulated that learning was literally
teacher-centered, that is, it was the teacher who was responsible for providing the
appropriate stimuli or grammatical data. The student was like the little baby (or
Pavlovian puppy) conditioned to absorb all this important information without
any personal input or cognitive processes involved, but being rewarded when
following instructions correctly (which may explain why many teachers do have
jars of candy in their off ices). Thus, we had our pedagogical repertoire of repeti-
tion exercises or rote memory that was relatively divorced from a relationship
with meaning or even real communication (cf. the well-known Audio-lingual
Methodology in the 60s). The latter theoretical approach viewed the student as
a more active participant in the learning process (after all, s/he already possessed
innately Chomsky’s famous language acquisition device (LAD), also referred to
as “ the black box ), so teachers shifted more responsibilities to the students for
the learning process, and in doing so subtly acknowledged the role of cognitive
processes in L2 learning. However, like in real life, the rationalist/mentalist/
nativist approach to L2 learning led to several interpretations of this new learner-
centered perspective of the lea r ning proces s. This resu lted i n severa l variation s of
6241-0749-001.indd 26241-0749-001.indd 2 1/29/2015 10:48:29 AM1/29/2015 10:48:29 AM
Introduction 3
teaching practices, depending on one’s personal perception of language learning
and teaching within this approach: Witness, for example, the Grammar Transla-
tion Method (50s), the Cognitive Code Method that placed a premium on formal
instruction of grammar before practice, the humanistic perception as evident in
the 70s in The Silent Way, Community Language Learning, and Suggestope-
dia, and the focus on communication first as in the Direct Method (60s), the
Total Physical Response (70s), the Communicative Approach (70s), the Natural
Approach (80s), Task-based Learning (current) and so on. I am proud to say
that I have tried, during my four decades of teaching, most of these methods,
approaches, techniques, etc., as bandwagons came and rode off into the sunset.
Interestingly, the two major publications, namely Corder (1967) and Selinker
(1972), that provided the foundation for current research in SLA were both
learner-centered and repudiations against a behaviorist approach to language
learning. For language instruction, Corder suggested the need to seriously
address what L2 learners bring to the task of learning an L2, which he called
their “internal syllabus.” In addition, he coined the term “intake” that sought to
differentiate what learners are exposed to, for example, the L2 (the input), and
what they take in . Theoretically, it is assumed that not everything that learners
pay attention to in the input is automatically “taken in” or processed, most likely
due to processing demands and attentional constraints. Selinker suggested that
we acknowledge the internal system, which he calls “interlanguage,” that L2
learners possess as they develop their ability to learn the L2. As the term con-
notes, interlanguage is a system that is somewhere between the f irst language
(L1) and the L2. Given the status of interlanguage being a system with its own
rules, doesn’t it make you wonder whether errors produced by our students are
really systematically “correct” according to their own interlanguage system, but
are being graded from a native or near-native speaker’s perspective? Put another
way, perhaps they are “right” and we are “wrong.”
The 70s witnessed several empirical efforts to address Corder’s and Selinker’s
calls for more focus on the learner’s involvement in the learning process. Many
of these studies were essentially based on L1 research conducted within an L2
context and provided quite a contrast in their pursuits. On the one hand were
the acquisition order studies (do some of you recall the famous morpheme stud-
ies by, for example, Dulay and Burt (1973, 1974) that attempted to equate the
L1 acquisitional process with that of the L2 based on an apparent natural order
of morphemes?), while on the other hand we had the error analysis studies that
sought to prove otherwise, that is, that the L1 transfer process may not be entirely
similar to the L2 (cf. Corder, 1967; Schachter, 1974).
The 80s, in my opinion, began a fruitful period of research in the L2 learn-
ing process both from an empirical and theoretical perspective. Even though
the shift from a strict behaviorist perspective of language learning was relatively
accepted in the SLA field, early empirical research still began to focus on the
external features of the L2 input (cf. studies on simplification such as Blau, 1982;
6241-0749-001.indd 36241-0749-001.indd 3 1/29/2015 10:48:29 AM1/29/2015 10:48:29 AM
4 Introduction
Davies, 1984; Parker & Chaudron, 1987) and the role of interaction in L2 lan-
guage learning, mostly from a descriptive perspective (cf. Hatch, Shapira, &
Wagner-Gough, 1978; Henzl, 1979). At the same time, there were some impor-
tant theoretical underpinnings that began to focus more closely on learners’
internal processes in relation to the role of awareness. Fir st, the ter m “conscious-
ness-raising” (Sharwood Smith, 1981) came into being with a direct relationship
to students’ internal processes. If we can raise our students’ consciousness of the
underlying grammatical rules, this will greatly facilitate their learning (by the
way, the Grammar Translation Method could be credited for doing this, though
learning was defined as the ability to write or translate instead of the ability to
speak). However, Sharwood Smith came to realize ( became aware ) that he was
dealing with an internal process and, consequently, modified his term to “input
enhancement” (Sharwood Smith, 1993), arguing that this term was more appro-
priate in depicting exactly what was being proposed, namely enhancing the L2
input via, for example, grammatical rules, additional emphasis, or anything that
could potentially draw students’ attention to the enhanced aspect of the L2 input.
Needless to say, this strand of research exploded in the 90s, given its relatively
broad definition of what comprises input enhancement (cf. Leow, 2009, for a
more elaborated and critical discussion of this issue), and is still current today.
In my opinion, there are some milestones along the theoretical and empirical
routes to current studies that have gone beyond investigating the role of awareness
in L2 learning to addressing whether the absence of awareness also plays a role in
L2 learning (cf. Chan & Leung, 2014; Hama & Leow, 2010; Leow, 2000; Leung &
Williams, 2011, 2014; Williams, 2005). The first milestone was Krashen’s (1982)
Monitor Theory, with its pedagogical sidekick the Natural Approach that ini-
tiated and maximally contributed to this theoretical and empirical impetus on
internal processes. The Monitor Theory, premised on children’s first language
acquisition, was the first theoretical underpinning to raise the issue of the role of
the construct of awareness (termed “consciousness” in those days and also today
with some researchers) in the L2 learning process and to distinguish between
learning (with consciousness), resulting in learned/explicit knowledge, and acquir-
ing (without consciousness), resulting in acquired/implicit knowledge. Krashen
also argued that there was no interface (connection) between implicit (acquired)
and explicit (learned) knowledge, which led to quite a discussion of whether
there exists in SLA a weak interface; for example, explicit knowledge can lead to
im plicit knowled g e (e.g., R. Ellis, 2006), or i mpl icit k n owled ge m ay b e assist ed by
explicit knowledge (e.g., N. Ellis, 2005). A strong interface (e.g., DeKeyser, 2007)
derived from skill acquisition theory in cognitive psychology (cf. Anderson, 1982)
postulates that SLA is largely a conscious process, so we begin the learning process
with declarative knowledge that can then become procedural knowledge (after
much practice), or none at all (Krashen, 1982; Paradis, 2009).
The interesting aspect of this interface debate that rears its head every now
and then is that while type of knowledge (a product) is under consideration,
6241-0749-001.indd 46241-0749-001.indd 4 1/29/2015 10:48:29 AM1/29/2015 10:48:29 AM
Introduction 5
by attaching the dichotomies implicit versus explicit or acquired versus learned or
conscious versus subconscious to the term knowledge, we have shifted the product
knowledge to include a process of learning, that is, learning with or without
awareness. So the theoretical question is not only whether knowledge can be
identified as implicit or explicit but also how such knowledge got to be explicit
or implicit. In other words, the end result (product) may not ref lect the process
of how the knowledge made its way into the internal system, and in order to
address adequately the interface issue, concurrent or online data on learners’
processes need to be gathered instead of making extrapolations based on non-
concurrent or off line data. See how convoluted the issue can become, and yet it
remains charmingly challenging and stimulating to research?
In addition to other postulations, Krashen’s theory equated L2 “acquisition”
with L1 acquisition and also postulated that acquisition followed a predictable
order. In addition to the obvious critique of the inability to test his theory of L2
learning ( “hmm, comprehensible input , to whom? input comprehensible to you
may not be comprehensible to me; hmm, i plus 1, where to locate each student’s i, GPS, any-
one? and what is the 1 again?” ), serious questions such as, “Do we treat our adult
students like babies following a first language (L1) acquisitional trajectory?”
(Krashen: look at the evidence of an apparent unchangeable acquisitional sequence, albeit
based on morphemes, those little pieces that make up a word ), or “Do we intervene
in an ‘appropriate’ way ( we need to consider the psycholinguistic or sociocultural factors
involved in learning ) in their learning process?” ( let us provide feedback at an appropri-
ate point during interaction or let us put them into collaborative groups and learning will
take place ), still need to be more fully addressed.
As I mentioned above, Krashen’s scholarly contribution to the SLA field via
his Monitor Model ranks very high in my estimation. While we do have the
phenomenon called “Krashen bashin’” (cf. for example, Gregg, 1984; McLaugh-
lin, 1978 and others who took him to task), without his theoretical postulations
serious research on learners’ internal processes would most likely have taken
place at a later date. When you publish a study or postulate a theory or model and
subsequently encourage a whole string of further investigation into the issue(s)
you initiated, you have my highest respect, irrespective of any potential bash-
ing you may receive, because in a weird sense you have contributed to a better
understanding of the learning process by stimulating further and, ideally, more
robust research.
Around the late 80s, both teachers and researchers were beginning to con-
template the use of two types of instruction: Explicit versus implicit (cf. e.g.,
Sc o t t , 1989; Sh a ffe r, 1989 ) . Explicit (also referred to as “deductive”) instruction
laid the responsibility on the teacher to explain the grammatical rules first, keep-
ing their f ingers crossed that students did understand the explanation, before
allowing them to practice the rules. Implicit (also referred to asinductive)
instruction exposed the students to the L2 with many of the targeted grammatical
or lexical items embedded in the input, and teachers also kept their fingers (and
6241-0749-001.indd 56241-0749-001.indd 5 1/29/2015 10:48:29 AM1/29/2015 10:48:29 AM
6 Introduction
toes) crossed that students would induce these grammatical features all by them-
selves (given the impoverished environment of the classroom and the paucity of
extensive exposure to and interaction with the L2, tough luck). Interestingly,
the two studies cited above (Scott, 1989; Shaffer, 1989) actually defined induc-
tive instruction differently. While Scott followed the definition of inductive
instruction as described above, Shaffer actually oriented her participants to pay
attention to the targeted linguistic items in the input, which, in my opinion,
may be best described as a partially combined deductive/inductive def inition.
Two other terms that arose and were employed very loosely in this period were
the constructs of explicit learning , that is, learning with awareness, and implicit
learning , that is, learning without awareness, although at that time the construct
of awareness was not independently operationalized or measured. Both terms
are now playing a prominent role in current SLA research, as mentioned above.
With regard to a focus on learners internal processes, McLaughlin (1987)
posited his Cognitive Theory based on cognitive psychology tenets on the role of
attention, limited attentional capacity, and types of processing (controlled versus
automatic) assumed to play a role in input processing during the early stages of
the learning process. The next year witnessed the first attempt to capture the L2
learning process from input (called ambient speech) > apperceived input > com-
prehended input > intake > integration > output (Gass, 1988, which has been
refined and updated in Gass, 1997, and Gass & Selinker, 2008).
The 90s began with Schmidt’s (1990) seminal article on his noticing hypoth-
esis that brought into the SLA field a theoretical postulation that the roles of
focal attention and awareness were isomorphic (two sides of the same coin) and
crucial in any L2 development, and more specifically during the early stage of
the learning process, namely the input-to-intake stage. This is the second mile-
stone along the route to current unawareness or implicit learning studies. The
noticing hypothesis immediately became arguably the most influential theoreti-
cal underpinning of many strands of SLA research that include input enhance-
ment, interaction, learning conditions, output hypothesis, and so on. Making the
attentional strand of research even more interesting and debatable was Tomlin
and Villa’s (1994) model of input processing in SLA derived from a cognitive
neuroscience perspective that did not posit any role for awareness in the initial
stages of the learning process (intake), soon followed by Robinson’s (1995) model
of the relationship between attention and memory that attempted to reconcile
these two theoretical perspectives in addition to elaborating on the important
role of memory.
Appearing also were theoretical models (e.g., Gass, 1997, an update of Gass,
1988; VanPatten, 1996, updated in 2004 and 2007) of the L2 learning process
that went beyond the initial stage of the learning process to include other stages
postulated to occur along this process, for example, beyond intake (internal-
ization) and output (production) (cf. Gass, 1988), and a modular framework
(Truscott & Sharwood Smith, 2004, updated in 2011) that attempts to integrate
6241-0749-001.indd 66241-0749-001.indd 6 1/29/2015 10:48:29 AM1/29/2015 10:48:29 AM
Introduction 7
language acquisition accounts with proposals of how language, and more spe-
cifically phonology and (morpho)syntax, and cognition interact. At this point,
methodologically, empirical studies were still gathering data after experimental
exposures or treatments. Thus, while theoretically the focus was on the learning
process and not on external input or output features, product data were being
employed to account for internal processes. Here was (and still is for many cur-
rent studies) the classic research design: Pretest (to establish that experimental
and control groups were statistically similar in ability to perform some task) >
treatment or exposure (to the targeted linguistic item in the input) > immedi-
ate posttest (e.g., a recognition and/or production test, etc.). Usually the raw
scores of the groups were entered into a statistical program, and if a significant
difference in performance or between the means of the groups was revealed (ide-
ally, the experimental group outperforming the control group), the researcher
would jump up and proclaim to everyone that it was the treatment or type of
exposure that contributed to the results, which boils down to the equivalent of
assuming that participants did exactly what was expected of them during the
experiment and that all variables that could have potentially affected the results
were well controlled. In other words, it was inherently assumed that there was
high internal validity in the study, that we could safely place our confidence in
the findings, and that we could, from a pedagogical perspective, incorporate the
findings into our classroom activities, etc.
It was only in the latter part of the 90s that the constructs of attention and
awareness began to be addressed both methodologically and empirically in
an effort to investigate their effects on L2 development. While some studies
employed offline measures, that is, after exposure (e.g., Robinson, 1996, 1997),
other studies first methodologically established the constructs of attention and
awareness before submitting the data to statistical analyses (e.g., Alanen, 1995;
Leow, 1997). Data elicitation procedures such as online verbal reports or think-
aloud protocols (in which participants were requested to say aloud whatever came
into their head as they processed the L2 data, that is, while they interacted with
the new grammatical or lexical information in the L2) began to be employed
in many studies in an effort to gather online or concurrent insights into stu-
dents’ cognitive processes. These cited studies also addressed levels of awareness.
Similarly, in the Vygotskyian sociocultural strand of research, a closely related
phenomenon to think alouds was called inner speech (e.g., Vocate, 1994), and
currently languaging, defined as “the process of making meaning and shap-
ing knowledge and experience through language” (Swain 2006: 89), As can
be seen, then, the use of concurrent data elicitation procedures in the research
methodology signaled a distinct methodological shift in gathering both process
(online) and product (off line) data in an effort to gather important informa-
tion during and not only after the learning process. The outcome of this new
methodological approach to operationalize and measure the constructs of atten-
tion and awareness are data that allow us to peek into the internal processes
6241-0749-001.indd 76241-0749-001.indd 7 1/29/2015 10:48:29 AM1/29/2015 10:48:29 AM
8 Introduction
learners employed while interacting with the L2 input, without having to rely on
researchers’ assumptions of what actually took place based on the results of post-
exposure assessment tasks. Should I mention that this use of concurrent verbal
reports triggered another strand of research that methodologically addressed and
is still addressing whether asking participants to think aloud could potentially
affect their thought processes? The buzz word is reactivity (thinking aloud poten-
tially affecting learner primar y processes) for concurrent think-aloud protocols
and veridicality (memory decay) for post-exposure, off line stimulated recalls that
ask participants to try to recall either what they were thinking at specific points
during an interaction, via a video of the interaction or verbal reports that ask
participants to provide an underlying rule embedded in the input.
Now, with this background in mind, let us return to the issues of implicit and
explicit learning, which are both internal processes. Several studies (e.g., de la
Fuente, 2015; Hsieh et al., 2015; Leow, 2000; Martínez-Fernández, 2008; Medina,
2015; Rosa & Leow, 2004; Rosa & O’Neill, 1999; Sachs & Suh, 2007) that began
to explore the role of awareness in L2 development initiated the opportunity to
investigate both theoretically and empirically the role of explicit learning in the
SLA field. The 2000s began with attempts to address the construct of unawareness
in L2 development (e.g., Leow, 2000), followed by at least seven other studies pur-
porting to do the same (cf. Chan & Leung, 2014; Chen, Guo, Tang, Zhu, Yang, &
Dienes, 2011; Faretta-Stutenberg & Morgan-Short, 2011, Hama & Leow, 2010;
Leung & Williams, 2011, 2012, 2014). Currently, like life (and politics), the studies
are divided in their support or lack thereof regarding the role of implicit learning
in L2 development. I will elaborate a bit more on this debate later in Chapter 10 .
Conclusion
I hope you enjoyed the stroll down “awareness lane.” We shall leave this chapter
with one empirically supported finding: Explicit learning is highly related to L2
development.
References
Alanen, R. (1995). Input enhancement and rule presentation in second language acquisi-
tion. In R. Schmidt, (Ed.), Attention and awareness in foreign language learning and teach-
ing (pp. 395–411). Honolulu: University of Hawai’i Press.
Anderson, J. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89 , 369–406.
Blau, E. (1982). The effect of syntax on readability for ESL students in Puerto Rico.
TESOL Quarterly , 16 , 517– 527.
Chan, R., & Leung, J. (2014). Implicit lear ning of L2 natural language stress rules. Second
Language Research , 30 , 463–484.
Chen, W., Guo, X., Tang, J., Zhu, L., Yang, Z., & Dienes, Z. (2011). Unconscious str uc-
tural knowledge of form-meaning connections. Consciousness and Cognition, 20 ,
1751–1760.
6241-0749-001.indd 86241-0749-001.indd 8 1/29/2015 10:48:29 AM1/29/2015 10:48:29 AM
Introduction 9
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: The Netherlands Mouton and
Company.
Corder, S. (1967). The significance of learners’ errors. International Review of Applied Lin-
guistics , 5 , 161–170.
Davies, A. (1984). Simple, simplified and simplification: What is authentic? In J. Alder-
son & A. Urquhart (Eds.), Reading in a foreign language (pp. 181–195). New York:
Longman.
DeKeyser, R. (2007). Skill acquisition theory. In B. VanPatten & J. Williams (Eds.), Theo-
ries in second language acquisition (pp. 97–113). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.