ArticlePDF Available

Interpersonal behavior in women: Association with prenatal exposure to sex hormones

Authors:

Abstract

The ratio between the second and fourth digit of the hand (2D:4D ratio) has been used as a marker of prenatal exposure to sex hormones. A previous study found that men with smaller 2D:4D ratios, relative to men with larger ratios, were more agreeable and less quarrelsome with women than with men. We aimed to replicate this study, using a similar procedure for sampling interpersonal behaviors but including participants who were first-year students (n = 81) rather than working adults in a long-term romantic relationship. Event-contingent recording of everyday social interactions yielded data on agreeableness, quarrelsomeness, dominance, and submissiveness in different social contexts (e.g., with male vs. female interaction partners). While we did not replicate the findings previously obtained in men, this might be due to the relatively low number of male participants. Female participants with larger 2D:4D ratios were more agreeable with women than with men; this was not found among women with smaller ratios. Future studies on the link between the 2D:4D ratio and interpersonal behavior should consider with whom interactions take place and examine additional factors such as participants' relationship status and hormonal contraceptive use.
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Personality and Individual Differences
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid
Short Communication
Interpersonal behavior in women: Association with prenatal exposure to sex
hormones
Michelle Spix
a,b,
, Marije aan het Rot
a
a
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Behavioural and Social Sciences, University of Groningen, Netherlands
b
Department of Clinical Psychological Science, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Netherlands
ARTICLE INFO
Keywords:
2D:4D ratio
Agreeableness
Estrogen
Event-contingent recording
Interpersonal behavior
Sex hormones
Social interaction
Testosterone
ABSTRACT
The ratio between the second and fourth digit of the hand (2D:4D ratio) has been used as a marker of prenatal
exposure to sex hormones. A previous study found that men with smaller 2D:4D ratios, relative to men with
larger ratios, were more agreeable and less quarrelsome with women than with men. We aimed to replicate this
study, using a similar procedure for sampling interpersonal behaviors but including participants who were first-
year students (n= 81) rather than working adults in a long-term romantic relationship. Event-contingent re-
cording of everyday social interactions yielded data on agreeableness, quarrelsomeness, dominance, and sub-
missiveness in different social contexts (e.g., with male vs. female interaction partners). While we did not re-
plicate the findings previously obtained in men, this might be due to the relatively low number of male
participants. Female participants with larger 2D:4D ratios were more agreeable with women than with men; this
was not found among women with smaller ratios. Future studies on the link between the 2D:4D ratio and
interpersonal behavior should consider with whom interactions take place and examine additional factors such
as participants' relationship status and hormonal contraceptive use.
1. Introduction
Prenatal exposure to sex hormones shapes brain development
(McEwen & Milner, 2017). An easy-to-measure marker of prenatal
testosterone relative to estrogen exposure is the 2D:4D ratio, re-
presenting the length of the second digit relative to the length of the
fourth digit. Men generally have smaller ratios than women, indicating
higher prenatal testosterone exposure (Manning, 2011).
Besides, within-gender variation in the 2D:4D ratio has been linked
to within-gender variation in interpersonal functioning. Moskowitz
et al. (2015) found that while men were generally more agreeable and
less quarrelsome towards women than towards other men, this was
particularly true for men with smaller 2D:4D ratios. Associations be-
tween 2D:4D ratios and interpersonal behaviors were not significant in
women. It was suggested that men exposed to more testosterone pre-
natally are more masculine and hence more motivated to connect with
women. In the present study, we aimed to replicate these findings.
While other studies have only found only weak associations be-
tween the 2D:4D ratio and psychological functioning (Fink et al., 2004;
Hönekopp & Watson, 2011), many used standard questionnaires, which
only allow for examining between-person differences. In contrast,
Moskowitz et al. (2015) used event-contingent recording of social
interactions. As this involves intensive repeated measures across mul-
tiple social contexts, sources of within-person variation in interpersonal
behavior (e.g., the gender of interaction partners) can be considered as
well.
Unlike Moskowitz et al. (2015), whose participants were working
adults in long-term heterosexual relationships, we studied first-year
students who varied in their relationship status. Nonetheless, we ex-
pected 2D:4D ratios to be associated with agreeableness and quarrel-
someness in men but not in women.
2. Method
2.1. Participants
First-year students participated in exchange for course credit
(n= 110). Twenty-nine participants were excluded (4 did not identify
as heterosexual, 4 provided no hand scans, and 21 did not complete the
event-contingent recording), leaving 81 participants for data analyses.
Descriptive data are in Table 1.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110379
Received 14 June 2020; Received in revised form 31 August 2020; Accepted 1 September 2020
Corresponding author at: Maastricht University, Universiteitssingel 40, 6229 ER Maastricht, Netherlands.
E-mail addresses: m.spix@maastrichtuniversity.nl (M. Spix), m.aan.het.rot@rug.nl (M. aan het Rot).
Personality and Individual Differences 168 (2021) 110379
Available online 25 September 2020
0191-8869/ © 2020 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/BY/4.0/).
T
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. 2D:4D ratios
Digit lengths of both hands were derived from hand scans, gener-
ated using an Aficio MP3351 digital copier (Ricoh, Japan), by four
coders using AutoMetric 2.2 (Face Research Lab, University of
Glasgow). Each second and fourth digit was measured from the middle
of the ventral base crease to the tip. Intra-class coefficients (Shrout &
Fleiss, 1979) were ≥0.89 for the right and left hand separately and for
male and female participants separately. Thus, the right and left 2D:4D
ratios from the four raters were averaged. The correlation between right
and left ratios was strongly positive, r(81) = 0.80, p< 0.0001, and
right vs. left ratios did not differ significantly, t(80) = 1.91, p= 0.06.
2.2.2. Interpersonal behavior
For event-contingent recording (ECR; Moskowitz & Sadikaj, 2013)
of social interactions, we installed a link to an app (TEMPEST; Batalas
et al., 2018) on participants' smartphones. Using the app, either online
or offline, participants recorded their interactions for 14 days. First,
they provided information on contextual variables, including the
gender and role of their primary interaction partner. Subsequently, they
checked which behaviors, of a list of 12 behaviors, they engaged in. For
each interaction, to prevent fixed response sets, participants randomly
saw one of four different but comparable behavior lists. These lists were
derived from the Social Behavior Inventory covering agreeableness,
quarrelsomeness, dominance, and submissiveness (Moskowitz, 1994).
Corresponding to prior studies (e.g. Moskowitz, 1994), for every
interaction we first calculated the mean frequency of all checked be-
haviors and then subtracted this mean frequency from the frequency of
a specific type of behavior. The resulting ipsatized scores represent the
rate at which each type of behavior was checked, adjusted for general
behavior checking.
2.3. Procedure
First-year psychology students at the University of Groningen enroll
into an online research participation platform at the start of the first
semester and provide personal information including their sexual
identity, relationship status, and contraceptive use. Participants in the
present study were recruited via this platform, after the Departmental
Ethics Committee approved the study.
In a first meeting with a research assistant, participants received
detailed study information, provided consent, and completed several
questionnaires. Subsequently, TEMPEST was installed, the ECR proce-
dure was explained, and a sample social interaction (i.e., a spoken
conversation of at least 5 min) was recorded together with the assistant.
In a second meeting, within a week after the ECR period, participants
completed additional questionnaires and provided hand scans.
2.4. Analyses
Data were analyzed in SAS 9.4 (SAS, Cary, NC) using multilevel
modeling with an unstructured covariance structure. Social interactions
were nested within individuals. Degrees of freedom were calculated
using the Satterthwaite method.
Analyses were identical to Moskowitz et al. (2015): social interac-
tions involving parents or siblings were omitted; each participant's
2D:4D ratio was standardized using the mean ratio for participant's
gender and treated as a continuous between-person factor; and analyses
were conducted separately for each dimension of behavior, for men and
women, and for the right and left hand. The gender of the primary
partner during each social interaction was the within-person factor. In
the first analysis step, main effects of Ratio and Partner gender were
entered. In the second step, the interaction term was added. Significant
interaction terms were examined by estimating simple slopes and cal-
culating point estimates.
3. Results
The mean number of social interactions available for analyses was
46, with no significant differences between men and women, see
Table 1. Analysis outcomes are in Supplementary Table S1.
3.1. Analyses in the men
After omitting family interactions, the mean number of interactions
was 36. The main effect for the 2D:4D ratio and the interaction term
were never significant.
3.2. Analyses in the women
After omitting family interactions, the mean number of interactions
was 42. Again, we found no significant main effects for the 2D:4D ratio
and no significant interaction terms for dominance and quarrelsome-
ness. The interaction effect was significant for submissiveness in the
analysis involving the right-hand ratio but simple slopes for male vs.
female partners were not significant.
However, the interaction term was significant for agreeableness in
the analysis involving the right-hand ratio, see Fig. 1. Women with
larger ratios acted more agreeably towards other women than towards
men, b= 6.24, t(1336) = 3.44, p< 0.001, d= 0.19. This was not
found among women with smaller ratios, b= −0.56, t
(1325) = −0.29, p= 0.77, d= 0.02. Similarly, while the analysis
involving the left-hand ratio did not yield a significant interaction term
(0.05 < p< 0.10), post-hoc simple contrasts revealed that women
with larger left-hand ratios were more agreeable towards other women
than towards men, b= 5.08, t(1361) = 2.94, p= 0.003, d= 0.16, and
this was not found among women with smaller ratios, b= 0.58, t
(1349) = 0.30, p= 0.76, d= 0.02.
4. Discussion
Between-person differences in 2D:4D ratios have previously been
associated with differences in interpersonal behavior among working
adults: Moskowitz et al. (2015) found that men in their late twenties
with smaller 2D:4D ratios, considered a marker of higher prenatal
Table 1
Relevant descriptive and social interaction data.
Men (n= 28) Women (n= 53) tor X
2
test
Age in years 21 (1) 20 (1) t(73.2) = 3.69, p= 0.002
In a romantic relationship when entering university 39% 44% X
2
(1) = 0.18, p= 0.67
Hormonal contraceptive use 70%
Right-hand preference 75% 89% X
2
(1) = 2.54, p= 0.11
Right-hand 2D:4D ratio 0.97 (0.03) 0.98 (0.04) t(79) = −0.51, p= 0.61
Left-hand 2D:4D ratio 0.97 (0.03) 0.98 (0.03) t(79) = −1.40, p= 0.17
Number of social interactions available for analyses 41 (20) 49 (17) t(79) = −1.91, p= 0.06
Note. Data represent M (SD) unless indicated otherwise.
M. Spix and M. aan het Rot Personality and Individual Differences 168 (2021) 110379
2
exposure to testosterone, were more agreeable and less quarrelsome
towards women than men with larger ratios. It was suggested that
higher prenatal testosterone exposure increases men's engagement with
behaviors that, to women, indicate a willingness to invest in opposite-
gender relationships.
We aimed to replicate these findings in a younger sample, i.e. stu-
dents in their early twenties. However, we found no significant asso-
ciations between 2D:4D ratios and interpersonal behavior among male
participants: men acted more agreeably and were less quarrelsome to-
wards women than towards men, regardless of their 2D:4D ratios. Thus,
we did not replicate Moskowitz et al. (2015).
Nonetheless, 2D:4D ratios were positively associated with agree-
ableness among female participants. Women with larger ratios were
more agreeable towards other women than to men, while women with
smaller ratios did not show this contextual variation in agreeableness.
Following Moskowitz et al. (2015), this might mean that relatively low
prenatal exposure to testosterone increases the engagement of young
women with behaviors that indicate a willingness to invest in same-
gender relationships.
The diverging results of both studies might be due to several
aforementioned sample differences. Participants studied by Moskowitz
et al. (2015) had a mean age of 29 years and were employed and in a
long-term relationship. Participants in our study were students, on
average nearly 10 years younger, and mostly not in a relationship.
Being in a relationship has been associated with a reduced motivation
to be agreeable (Schultheiss et al., 2003), particularly in women (Suh
et al., 2004). Possibly, our female participants varied more in agree-
ableness than the women studied by Moskowitz et al. (2015).
Between-study differences in the use of hormonal contraceptives
(HCs) might also explain the results. Most of our female participants
used HCs. In the (Canadian) study by Moskowitz et al. (2015), this
percentage may have been lower (United Nations Department of
Economic and Social Affairs Population Division, 2019). HCs often
contain estrogen and may alter opposite-gender interactions (Little
et al., 2013). The 2D:4D ratio has previously been shown to predict HC-
associated side effects (Oinonen, 2009), suggesting that the impact of
HC use might differ between women with higher vs. lower 2D:4D ratios.
As HC use might have influenced the observed association between
female agreeableness and 2D:4D ratios, our study may be limited in that
we did not exclude HC users. However, neither did Moskowitz et al.
(2015). Another limitation of our study was the relatively small sample
size, resulting in relatively low statistical power (which prevented us
from examining HC use as a potential moderator). Relatedly, the low
proportion of male participants reduced the likelihood of finding
significant results in men. This is why the results for men should be
interpreted with caution. A final limitation was that the result on
agreeableness in women was larger for the right-hand 2D:4D ratio than
for the left-hand ratio. However, it has been argued that right-hand
ratios are better marker of prenatal sex hormone levels (Hönekopp &
Watson, 2010).
In conclusion, we did not replicate Moskowitz et al. (2015). None-
theless, we found that young women with larger 2D:4D ratios were
more agreeable towards other women than towards men, whereas this
was not seen among women with smaller ratios. While this result may
seem to contradict Moskowitz et al. (2015), it might also complement
their work. We highlighted several factors that might help explain both
studies' findings, including being in a (long-term) romantic relationship
and using HCs. These factors could be examined in future studies.
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110379.
CRediT authorship contribution statement
Michelle Spix: Conceptualization, Validation, Writing - original
draft. Marije aan het Rot: Conceptualization, Methodology, Software,
Formal analysis, Writing - review & editing, Visualization, Supervision.
Declaration of competing interest
None.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Renske Bosman, Minita Franzen, Sophie
Lammertink, Jana Uppendahl, and Lenka Wichmann for data collec-
tion.
References
Batalas, N., aan het Rot, M., Khan, V. J., & Markopoulos, P. (2018). Using TEMPEST: End-
user programming of web-based ecological momentary assessment protocols.
Proceedings of the ACM on Human-Computer Interaction, 2(EICS), 1–24. https://doi.
org/10.1145/3179428.
Fink, B., Manning, J. T., & Neave, N. (2004). Second to fourth digit ratio and the “big five”
personality factors. Personality and Individual Differences, 37(3), 495–503. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.paid.2003.09.018.
Hönekopp, J., & Watson, S. (2010). Meta-analysis of digit ratio 2D:4D shows greater sex
difference in the right hand. American Journal of Human Biology, 22(5), 619–630.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.21054.
Hönekopp, J., & Watson, S. (2011). Meta-analysis of the relationship between digit-ratio
2D:4D and aggression. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(4), 381–386. https://
a. Right-hand ratio b. Left-hand ratio
Fig. 1. Interaction effect of 2D:4D ratio and partner gender for female agreeable behavior.
Note. *p< 0.05.
M. Spix and M. aan het Rot Personality and Individual Differences 168 (2021) 110379
3
doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.003.
Little, A. C., Burriss, R. P., Petrie, M., Jones, B. C., & Roberts, S. C. (2013). Oral contra-
ceptive use in women changes preferences for male facial masculinity and is asso-
ciated with partner facial masculinity. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(9), 1777–1785.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.02.014.
Manning, J. T. (2011). Resolving the role of prenatal sex steroids in the development of
digit ratio. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(39), Article 16143.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1113312108.
McEwen, B. S., & Milner, T. A. (2017). Understanding the broad influence of sex hor-
mones and sex differences in the brain. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 95(1–2),
24–39. https://doi.org/10.1002/jnr.23809.
Moskowitz, D. S. (1994). Cross-situational generality and the interpersonal circumplex.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(5), 921–933. https://doi.org/10.
1037//0022-3514.66.5.921.
Moskowitz, D. S., & Sadikaj, G. (2013). Event-contingent recording. In M. R. Mehl, & T. S.
Conner (Eds.). Handbook of research methods for studying daily life (pp. 160–175). New
York, NY: Guilford.
Moskowitz, D. S., Sutton, R., Zuroff, D. C., & Young, S. N. (2015). Fetal exposure to
androgens, as indicated by digit ratios (2D:4D), increases men’s agreeableness with
women. Personality and Individual Differences, 75, 97–101.
Oinonen, K. A. (2009). Putting a finger on potential predictors of oral contraceptive side
effects: 2D:4D and middle-phalangeal hair. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(5),
713–726. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.11.009.
Schultheiss, O. C., Dargel, A., & Rohde, W. (2003). Implicit motives and gonadal steroid
hormones: Effects of menstrual cycle phase, oral contraceptive use, and relationship
status. Hormones and Behavior, 43(2), 293–301. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0018-
506X(03)00003-5.
Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater relia-
bility. Psychological Bulletin, 86(2), 420–428. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.86.
2.420.
Suh, E. J., Moskowitz, D. S., Fournier, M. A., & Zuroff, D. C. (2004). Gender and re-
lationships: Influences on agentic and communal behaviors. Personal Relationships,
11(1), 41–60. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2004.00070.x.
United Nations Department of Economic and Social Affairs Population Division (2019).
Contraceptive Use by Method 2019: Data Booklet (Vol. ST/ESA/SER.A/435).
M. Spix and M. aan het Rot Personality and Individual Differences 168 (2021) 110379
4
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Four traits of the interpersonal circumplex, dominance, submissiveness, agreeableness, and quarrelsomeness, were measured using experience sampling. Participants monitored their behavior for 20 days. For each social interaction, they recorded behaviors and information about the situation. Two sets of situations, agentic and communal, were defined on the basis of information about the relationship of the other person with the participant. Results suggested general guidelines for expectations about magnitudes of cross-situational generality. For these broad traits of the interpersonal circumplex, there was modest to low generality across agentic situations in which individuals varied in power and status (supervisor and co-worker). Cross-situational generality was moderately high across communal situations (acquaintances and friends). Behavior toward a romantic partner was distinct from behavior toward close friends and acquaintances. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Males tend to be more aggressive than females and the organizational effects of prenatal testosterone (T) appear to contribute to this sex difference. Low second-to-fourth digit ratio (2D:4D) is thought to be a marker of high prenatal testosterone. For this reason, a number of studies have used 2D:4D to investigate a potential effect of prenatal T upon aggression in later life. Here we meta-analyse these studies to determine the true size of the relationship between 2D:4D and aggression. We find no evidence of 2D:4D better predicting aggression at different levels of risk nor do we find evidence for a relationship between 2D:4D and aggression in females. Regarding males we find some evidence of a small, negative relationship between 2D:4D and aggression (r ≈ −.06) and no indication that either hand would predict aggression better than the other. We contrast these findings with results regarding levels of aggression in females with elevated prenatal T levels due to Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia and we discuss implications for 2D:4D research.
Article
Researchers who perform Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) studies tend to rely on informatics experts to set up and administer their data collection protocols with digital media. Contrary to standard surveys and questionnaires that are supported by widely available tools, setting up an EMA protocol is a substantial programming task. Apart from constructing the survey items themselves, researchers also need to design, implement, and test the timing and the contingencies by which these items are presented to respondents. Furthermore, given the wide availability of smartphones, it is becoming increasingly important to execute EMA studies on user-owned devices, which presents a number of software engineering challenges pertaining to connectivity, platform independence, persistent storage, and back-end control. We discuss TEMPEST, a web-based platform that is designed to support non-programmers in specifying and executing EMA studies. We discuss the conceptual model it presents to end-users, through an example of use, and its evaluation by 18 researchers who have put it to real-life use in 13 distinct research studies.
Article
Sex hormones act throughout the entire brain of both males and females via both genomic and nongenomic receptors. Sex hormones can act through many cellular and molecular processes that alter structure and function of neural systems and influence behavior as well as providing neuroprotection. Within neurons, sex hormone receptors are found in nuclei and are also located near membranes, where they are associated with presynaptic terminals, mitochondria, spine apparatus, and postsynaptic densities. Sex hormone receptors also are found in glial cells. Hormonal regulation of a variety of signaling pathways as well as direct and indirect effects on gene expression induce spine synapses, up- or downregulate and alter the distribution of neurotransmitter receptors, and regulate neuropeptide expression and cholinergic and GABAergic activity as well as calcium sequestration and oxidative stress. Many neural and behavioral functions are affected, including mood, cognitive function, blood pressure regulation, motor coordination, pain, and opioid sensitivity. Subtle sex differences exist for many of these functions that are developmentally programmed by hormones and by not yet precisely defined genetic factors, including the mitochondrial genome. These sex differences and responses to sex hormones in brain regions, which influence functions not previously regarded as subject to such differences, indicate that we are entering a new era of our ability to understand and appreciate the diversity of gender-related behaviors and brain functions.
Article
The ratio of the length of the second finger, or digit, to the fourth finger (2D:4D) is influenced by fetal exposure to androgens; a smaller ratio indicates greater androgen exposure. We used event contingent recording to investigate the relation between the 2D:4D ratio and social behavior. Participants completed multiple records of their behavior in events in naturalistic settings; records included information about situational features such as the gender of the person with whom the person was interacting. Men were more agreeable towards women than men; this effect was significantly greater in those with smaller 2D:4D ratios. Men with smaller 2D:4D ratios were also less quarrelsome towards women than towards men. The 2D:4D ratio did not influence social behavior in women. The hormonal environment in which the male fetal brain develops may influence adult social behavior in specific contexts.
Article
The present research examined the moderating influence of situations involving friends and romantic partners on gender differences in interpersonal behaviors reflecting agency and communion. Behavior was studied in three situations varying in social role and dyadic gender composition: same-sex friendships, opposite-sex friendships, and romantic relationships. To obtain multiple events representing each relationship situation, participants recorded information about their interpersonal interactions during a 20-day period using an event-contingent recording procedure. Results indicated gender differences consistent with gender stereotypes when men and women were interacting with same-sex friends; men with men were more dominant and women with women were more agreeable. In interactions with romantic partners, gender differences in communal behavior were opposite to gender stereotypes; women were less agreeable and more quarrelsome than men with their romantic partners. Results are considered in reference to developmental socialization theory, social role theory, and studies of gender differences in marital relationships.
Article
Several studies have demonstrated that the ratio between the lengths of the second and fourth digits (2D:4D) is sexually dimorphic. On average males demonstrate lower digit ratios than do females. There is accumulating evidence that 2D:4D is established early in development and is negatively related to prenatal testosterone and positively with prenatal estrogen. In this present study, we investigated associations between digit ratio and the ‘big-five’ personality factors (extraversion, neuroticism, openness, conscientiousness and agreeableness). Only agreeableness showed a significant sex difference with females scoring higher. Neuroticism showed a significant positive correlation with right hand digit ratio for the whole sample, and in females only for right hand 2D:4D. Further, a significant negative association was found between females’ right hand digit ratio and agreeableness. Male 2D:4D did not correlate significantly with any of the personality factors. The results follow previous studies reporting that 2D:4D was a stronger correlate of personality traits in females than in males. In summary, the associations between digit ratio and personality factors are weak but provide some further support for the claim that gender-related behaviour is affected by the amount of steroid hormones present during critical periods of prenatal development.
Article
Many women experience emotional or physical side effects when taking oral contraceptives (OCs). Despite the potential impact on women's health and well-being, there are no valid methods to screen women for their risk of OC side effects. The present paper presents the results of two studies where anthropometric indicators of androgen exposure, 2D:4D and middle-phalangeal hair, were examined for their potential as predictors of OC side effects. In study 1, 2D:4D was associated with women's reports of a history of: (a) negative mood side effects; (b) discontinuation due to negative mood side effects; (c) specific mood side effects (i.e., crying, sadness, and altered trust in one's partners) and (d) specific physical side effects (i.e., headaches, fatigue, and decreased sex drive). In study 2, 2D:4D and/or middle-phalangeal hair was/were associated with a reported history of: (a) discontinuation due to negative mood side effects; (b) specific mood-related side effects (i.e., negative mood, disrupted sleep, increased aggression, and altered trust in one's partner) and (c) specific physical side effects (i.e., headaches, decreased menstrual cramps, and increased sex drive/arousal). The general pattern was that adverse OC side effects were experienced by women with lower 2D:4D and fewer middle-phalangeal hairs. Almost all relationships remained significant when response bias was controlled. These results suggest a possible role for prenatal testosterone exposure and both androgen action and sensitivity in women's experience of OC side effects. Furthermore, these two digit measures may be useful predictors of hormonal contraceptive side effects in women.