Available via license: CC BY-NC-ND 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rfan20
Anthrozoös
A multidisciplinary journal of the interactions of people and animals
ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rfan20
Social-Cognitive Processes Before Dog Acquisition
Associated with Future Relationship Satisfaction of
Dog Owners and Canine Behavior Problems
Esther M. C. Bouma , Lonneke M. Vink & Arie Dijkstra
To cite this article: Esther M. C. Bouma , Lonneke M. Vink & Arie Dijkstra (2020) Social-Cognitive
Processes Before Dog Acquisition Associated with Future Relationship Satisfaction of Dog Owners
and Canine Behavior Problems, Anthrozoös, 33:5, 659-672, DOI: 10.1080/08927936.2020.1799561
To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.2020.1799561
© 2020 The Author(s). Published by Informa
UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis
Group
Published online: 08 Sep 2020.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 218
View related articles
View Crossmark data
ANTHROZOÖS VOLUME 33, ISSUE 5 REPRINTS AVAILABLE PHOTOCOPYING
PP. 659–672 DIRECTLY FROM PERMITTED
PRINTED IN THE UK
THE PUBLISHERS BY LICENSE ONLY
Address for correspondence:
Esther M. C. Bouma PhD,
University of Groningen,
Faculty of Behavioural and
Social Sciences,
Grote Kruisstraat 2/1,
9712 TS Groningen,
The Netherlands.
E-mail: e.m.c.bouma@rug.nl
*Deceased
13 December 2017
Supplemental files for this
paper are available online.
659 Anthrozoös DOI: 10.1080/08927936.2020.1799561
Social-Cognitive Processes
Before Dog Acquisition
Associated with Future
Relationship Satisfaction
of Dog Owners and Canine
Behavior Problems
Esther M. C. Bouma, Lonneke M. Vink*, and Arie Dijkstra
Faculty of Behavioural and Social Sciences, University of Groningen,
The Netherlands
ABSTRACT Making the right decision before acquiring a dog may help prevent
the development of canine behavior problems and increase the likelihood of a
satisfactory relationship. In this study, social-cognitive factors in decision-making
were assessed in the phase before the acquisition of a dog (the motivational
phase) to see whether they were associated with later canine behavior problems
and satisfaction with the dog after formation of a relationship (the experience
phase). Respondents (n = 193) filled in an online questionnaire several months
before acquiring a dog and six and 18 months thereafter. Results indicate that
the confidence of dog owners in their ability to train and care for a dog—assessed
before acquisition—was associated with fewer canine behavior problems, more
satisfaction with the dog, and lower perceived costs in the experience phase.
Self-efficacy had no effect on consistency between planned and actual acquisi-
tion. People scoring high on social norms were more likely to adhere to their plans,
while those who expected many advantages were less likely to do so. Moreover,
several preparation activities were associated with consistency, with more frequent
doubt in the motivational phase being related to greater inconsistency. People
who frequently read books about owning dogs and who often talked about this
with others were more likely to adhere to their initial plans. In contrast, those who
often visited websites offering/selling dogs were less consistent, possibly due to
impulse buying. In conclusion, the quality of the relationship with a dog seems
partly related to human decision-making factors occurring before the dog has
been acquired. Longitudinal studies of human–animal relationships could enhance
knowledge concerning the social-cognitive processes underlying our relationships
with animals, possibly providing starting points for interventions aimed at improving
the welfare of both animals and humans.
AZ 33(5).qxp_Layout 1 9/1/20 2:14 PM Page 659
This is an Open Access article
distributed under the terms of
the Creative Commons
Attribution-NonCommercial-
NoDerivatives License
(http://creativecommons.org/
licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/),
which permits
non-commercial
re-use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium,
provided the original work is
properly cited, and is not
altered, transformed, or built
upon in any way.
© 2020 THE AUTHOR(S). PUBLISHED BY
TAYLOR & FRANCIS GROUP
INFORMA UK LIMITED, TRADING AS
Social-Cognitive Processes Before Dog Acquisition Associated with Future Relationship Satisfaction…
660 Anthrozoös
Keywords: behavior problems, decision-making, (dis)advantages, dog satisfaction, human–
animal interaction, impulse buying, self-efficacy
Dogs are the most popular companion animal in almost all European countries, the
USA, and Australia (Global survey on pet ownership, 2016). Dog ownership has been
associated with several benefits to human physical and psychological health,
including increased physical activity, reduced stress, companionship, social support, and
increased social interactions with people (O’Haire, 2010).
Unfortunately, not all human–dog relationships are healthy and pleasant. Problems asso-
ciated with dogs (e.g., difficulties in training, soiling, and aggressive behavior, Greenebaum,
2006; O’Farrell, 1997) cause inconvenience and suffering for humans. A sub-optimal relation-
ship might also cause suffering for the dog owing to maltreatment (e.g., neglect, Arluke, 2006),
relinquishment to a shelter (Coe et al., 2014; Lambert, Coe, Niel, Dewey, & Sargeant, 2015),
or disposal in other ways (e.g., through a trading website).These undesired situations are at
least partly related to a mismatch between characteristics of the dog (e.g., size, age, breed,
health, and behavior), the owner’s knowledge and capabilities concerning the dog’s needs, and
the owner’s expectations of the relationship with the dog. Mismatches are more likely when
dogs are acquired impulsively (Jagoe & Serpell, 1996).
Impulse buying involves an instantaneous, overpowering, and persistent desire to buy a prod-
uct when exposed to it. Such purchases are unintended and non-reflective (Rook, 1987). Rook
and Gardner (1993) define impulse buying as unplanned behavior involving quick decision- making
and a tendency toward immediate acquisition of a product. Several factors have been associated
with impulse buying (Muruganantham & Bhakat, 2013). For example, impulse buyers are more
social and status-conscious, and they do not spend much time considering their purchases.
When they feel a connection with a product, they simply must have it. Such connections can
occur when the product is physically close, seen on a website, or associated with a person that
one admires (de Veirman, Cauberghe, & Hudders, 2017). Impulse buying can be regarded as
sub-optimal decision-making. The social-cognitive factors involved in decision-making are
described in the Theory of Planned Behavior (Ajzen, 1991). In a previous study (Vink, Dijkstra, &
Epstude, 2019), we demonstrate that certain aspects of the decision-making process at time
zero (T0) are related to the actual acquisition of a dog (T1; yes/no). In the present study, we examine
how aspects of the decision-making process at T0 are related to canine behavior problems and
owner satisfaction, as measured in two follow-up questionnaires, six (T1) and 18 months (T2)
after acquiring a dog. The variables addressed in the present study call for a different theoretical
framework: the two-phase model of owner–dog relationship establishment.
Weighing advantages and disadvantages is central to making decisions (Dwyer, Bennett, &
Coleman, 2006; Janis & Mann, 1977; Trope, Liberman, & Wakslak, 2007). To do this properly,
a prospective dog owner needs knowledge about the general and specific needs of a dog.
Failure to consider possible disadvantages of a dog (e.g., time investment and financial costs)
could result in less satisfaction and more canine behavior problems once a relationship has
developed. Self-efficacy is important as well. Those who are confident they can fulfill a task are
likely to be more motivated and exhibit more perseverance with the task (Bandura, 1977;
Berget, Ekeberg, & Braastad, 2008). People who believe they are able to handle and care for
a dog are likely to invest more and be more successful in achieving and maintaining a
satisfactory relationship with the dog. The human’s social environment also figures into the
decision-making process. As social beings, people tend to be sensitive to the opinions of others,
❖
AZ 33(5).qxp_Layout 1 9/1/20 2:14 PM Page 660
particularly those who are close and important to them (Ajzen, 1991). More specifically, the
social norms of others could function as an extrinsic motivation to acquire a dog, possibly
resulting in more canine behavior problems and disappointment with the dog.
The aforementioned social-cognitive factors can change in light of relevant information, as
people prepare for their choices by gathering and contemplating information from various
sources (Holland, 2019), including books, the internet, or other people. People who are
uncertain about their decisions may engage in frequent preparation behavior. At some point,
however, they may feel that they know enough about the type of dog they want, as well as
about caring for and handling a dog. At this point, they may conclude the decision-making and
stop searching and contemplating. In contrast, a strong desire or longing for a dog (or a
particular breed) or frequently visiting websites offering dogs might increase the likelihood of
impulse buying, which may lead to undesired consequences.
Aim of the Study
As concluded by Holland (2019) in a review of factors associated with dog acquisition, little is
known about the underlying drivers of human behavior. In the present study, we addressed this
issue by assessing social-cognitive factors involved in the decision-making and preparation
activities of future dog owners before acquiring a dog, and examining whether these factors pre-
dict problems and satisfaction once they have developed a relationship with the dog they acquired.
Inspired by Health Action Process Approach theory (HAPA, Schwarzer & Luszczynska, 2015), we
proposed a two-phase model (see Figure 1) consisting of: 1) a motivational phase, in which
information is gathered and a decision is made to acquire the dog; and 2) an experience phase,
in which the relationship is experienced and, when needed, adjusted. The longitudinal design of
our study allowed us to examine social-cognitive factors and preparation activities in the motiva-
tional phase and to relate them to consistency between planned and actual purchase, as well as
to desired and undesired consequences in the future. These consequences are operationalized
as satisfaction with the dog, perceived costs of the dog, canine behavior problems, and satis-
faction with the decision to acquire the dog (all measured in the experience phase). Data from three
waves of the longitudinal data—baseline (T0) and two follow-ups at six (T1) and 18 (T2) months
after acquisition of the dog)—were analyzed.
Methods
The Institutional Review Board of the University of Groningen Faculty of Behavioural and Social
Sciences reviewed and approved the research (ppo-014-265). For additional information, see
Vink and colleagues (2019).
Bouma et al.
661 Anthrozoös
Figure 1. Two-phase model of owner–dog relationship establishment and the research design
of the present study.
AZ 33(5).qxp_Layout 1 9/1/20 2:14 PM Page 661
Social-Cognitive Processes Before Dog Acquisition Associated with Future Relationship Satisfaction…
662 Anthrozoös
Recruitment and Procedure
A call was published on several websites inviting people who were “planning to acquire a dog
within one year” to complete an online questionnaire. Participants were informed that they would
also be asked to complete another questionnaire six and 18 months after acquiring a dog. The
call was placed on websites and Facebook sites of Dutch organizations that provide informa-
tion about dogs and dog ownership. The researchers were assisted in contacting these organ-
izations by the Dutch Royal Association for the Protection of Dogs [de Hondenbescherming], an
organization dedicated to the welfare of dogs in the Netherlands. One Belgian organization
posted the appeal on its website. We approached 92 pet shops in the Netherlands with the
request to place flyers containing the same call on their counter; 82 agreed to do so.
The questionnaire was created on the Qualtrics platform. A link distributed to participants
directed them to the online questionnaire, which contained a notice that, by continuing to the
next page, participants automatically gave informed consent. The notice also stated that the
results of the questionnaire would be processed anonymously. The questionnaire was
administered in Dutch.
The present data comprise a baseline measurement and two follow-up measurements
(six and 18 months after acquiring a dog). Of the 1,418 people who started the questionnaire
at T0, 44.2% (n = 627) completed it. To time the second measurement (T1) at six months after
acquisition of the dog, participants were contacted three times (between 6–14 months) after
the baseline measurement, to ask whether they had acquired a dog. Respondents acquiring
a dog within 14 months after the first questionnaire (T0) were invited to participate in the ques-
tionnaires at T1 and T2. Of those completing the baseline questionnaire, 44.2% (n = 277)
completed the T1 questionnaire and 30.8% (n = 233) completed the T2 questionnaire. Ten
people got a new dog between T1 and T2; eight discarded the dog they acquired after T0 and
two lost their dog by death. To facilitate the interpretation of our results, we report only about
participants who completed all three questionnaires for the same dog (n = 183).
Attrition Analyses
The dropout rates from T0 to T1 and from T1 to T2 were substantial. Of the 627 participants
at T0, only 183 provided full data at both T1 and T2. Characteristics of these 183 participants
were compared with those of the 444 participants who were not included in the study
according to gender (2(1,627) = 0.059, p = 0.808), educational level1 (intermediate/low versus
high: 2(1,627) = 2.234, p = 0.135), experience with dogs (previous/current versus never:
2(1,627) = 0.464, p = 0.496), age (F(1,619) = 7.108, p = 0.008), advantages (F(1,626) = 0.129,
p = 0.719), disadvantages (F(1,626) = 1.100, p = 0.295), social norm (F(1,626) = 3.340,
p = 0.068), and self-efficacy (F(1,626) = 2.192, p = 0.139). Participants dropping out before T2
were slightly but significantly younger (M = 39.9 years, SD = 13.9 versus M = 43.1 years,
SD = 12.7) than those completing all three questionnaires.
Predictive Variables (T0)
The participants completed a baseline questionnaire containing questions about gender, age,
educational level, household composition, previous dog ownership, experience working with
dogs, whether they had considered a puppy or an adult dog, where they wanted to acquire
the dog (e.g., shelter, breeder), and which breed they wanted.
Preparation activities in which people could engage prior to acquiring the dog were assessed
according to 14 questions on searching and contemplating. Examples include: “How often are
you insecure about owning a dog?”; “How often do you think, ‘I want that too’ when you see
AZ 33(5).qxp_Layout 1 9/1/20 2:14 PM Page 662
somebody walking a dog on the street?”; “How often do you feel the desire to acquire a dog?”;
“How often do you think this is the right time to acquire a dog?”; “How often do you talk to oth-
ers about the investments related to dog ownership?”; “How often do you read books about
dogs?”; and “How often do you visit websites offering dogs?” Respondents used a 5-point scale
to rate their engagement in these activities in the last month: “never” (1), “sometimes” (2),
“regularly” (3), “often” (4), and “very often” (5). Although the reliability of the scale was good
(Cronbach’s : 0.77; Cronbach’s alpha is a measure of scale reliability where a Cronbach’s alpha
around 0.80 is considered good), we did not use the combined scale score, given the diverse
nature of the activities. We did include all 14 activities together in an exploratory analysis.
Advantages of owning a dog were assessed according to 25 items about the expected pos-
itive effects of dog ownership, based on existing literature (Endenburg, ’t Hart, & Bouw, 1994;
McConnell, Brown, Shoda, Stayton, & Martin, 2011; Staats, Wallace, & Anderson, 2008), on 10
in-depth interviews, and observations of how dog owners talked about their dog(s). Examples
include: “My dog will make sure that I have company” and “My dog will make sure that I get more
exercise.” Disadvantages of owning a dog were assessed according to 10 items about the
expected negative effects of dog ownership, based on existing literature and previously men-
tioned interviews and observations. Examples include: “Because of the dog, I will have to plan
my life more” and “Because of the dog, I will have more expenses.” Items on advantages and
disadvantages were rated along a 5-point scale: “completely disagree” (1), “disagree” (2), “nei-
ther disagree nor agree” (3), “agree” (4), “completely agree” (5). The average item scores were
used in the analyses (Cronbach : 0.92 for advantages and 0.73 for disadvantages). Higher
scale scores indicate greater expected advantages or disadvantages.
Self-efficacy (Bandura, 1977) was assessed according to two items. Participants used a
10-point scale to indicate their certainty about their ability to 1) raise/train a dog and 2) care
for a dog: “not certain at all” (1) to “very certain” (10). Average item scores were used in the
analyses. The two items were highly correlated (r = 0.71), with higher scores indicating greater
confidence in the ability to handle a dog satisfactorily.
Social norms assessed people’s estimation of what others might think that they should do
(Ajzen, 2002), based on three 5-point items: “My family/friends/neighbors think: “I definitely
should not acquire a dog” (1) to “I definitely should acquire a dog” (5). Average item scores were
used in the analyses (Cronbach’s = 0.74), with higher scores indicating more positive
opinions about the participant’s dog-acquisition plan.
Outcome Variables (T1 and T2)
Behavior problems were assessed at T1 and at T2 according to 16 items, including “bad
manners while eating,” “aggression to other people,” “aggression to other dogs,” “inappropriate
soiling,” and “inappropriate biting” (a full list of the 16 behaviors is provided in the online supple-
mental file Table S1). Items were inspired by the C-BARQ (Hsu & Serpell, 2003) and the behavior-
problem categories described by Neilson and Horwitz (2007). Respondents were asked about
the frequency of each behavior: “never” (1), “sometimes” (2), “regularly” (3), “often” (4), and “very
often” (5). In a subsequent question, they were asked whether they perceived the behavior as
problematic: “no” (1), “sometimes” (2), “yes” (3). Behaviors that were sometimes, or always, per-
ceived as problematic were considered behavior problems and included in the summation of the
behavior-problem score, with higher scores indicating more behavior problems perceived by the
owner. At T1, 9.2% of the participants did not perceive any behavior as problematic. At T2, this
was 11.7%. The highest behavior-problem score was 13 at T1 and 12 at T2.
Bouma et al.
663 Anthrozoös
AZ 33(5).qxp_Layout 1 9/1/20 2:14 PM Page 663
Satisfaction with the dog was assessed at T1 and T2 according to four items (Curb,
Abramson, Grice, & Kennison, 2015), based on a 7-point scale: “completely disagree” (1) to
“completely agree” (7). The items are: 1) “In general, I am very satisfied with my experiences
with my dog,” 2) “There are moments when I regret my decision to acquire this dog,” 3) “There
are several things I would like to change about my dog,” and 4) “I am satisfied with my dog
the way he/she is.” Items 2 and 3 were recoded to ensure a similar valence for all of the items.
Average item scores were used in the analyses (Cronbach : 0.49 at T1 and 0.73 at T2), with
higher scale scores indicating greater satisfaction with the dog.
Perceived costs were assessed at T1 and T2 according to the “Perceived Costs” sub-
scale of the Monash Dog Owner Relationship Scale (MDORS, Dwyer et al., 2006). This scale
consists of nine items scored on a 5-point scale: “completely disagree” (1) to “completely
agree” (5). Examples include: “I often feel that caring for my dog is a heavy duty,” “My dog costs
too much money,” “I often feel that having a dog is more effort than pleasure.” Average item
scores were used in the analyses (Cronbach : 0.86 at T1 and 0.87 at T2), with higher scale
scores indicating higher costs of dog ownership, as perceived by participants.
Satisfaction with the decision to acquire the dog was assessed at T2 according to seven items
rated on a 5-point scale: “completely disagree” (1) to “completely agree” (5). The seven items were
inspired by the Decision Satisfaction Inventory (Brehaut et al., 2003), which is used to assess
patients’ healthcare decisions: “I am satisfied with my decision,” “I am confident I made a deliber-
ative decision,” “It would have been useful if I had asked for more advice before acquiring my dog,”
“If I were to acquire another dog in the future, I would prepare myself differently,” “The decision to
acquire my dog was the right decision for my current situation,” “The decision to acquire my dog
did not play out as expected,” and “I sometimes have mixed feelings about the decision to acquire
my dog.” Items 3, 4, 6, and 7 were recoded to ensure a similar valence for all of the items. Average
item scores were used in the analyses (Cronbach = 0.77), with higher scores indicating that the
respondents were more satisfied with their decisions to acquire their dogs.
Statistical Analyses
We examined relationships between 1) preparation activities and 2) social-cognitive variables
at T0 and a) inconsistency between plan and purchase (inconsistency no = 0/yes = 1) in logistic
regression models, and b) desired and undesired outcomes at T1 and T2 in multivariate linear
regression models. The result tables display both B (estimated regression coefficient) and beta
(standardized regression coefficient) statistics. Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) and Pearson’s
2 (two-side) tests were used to identify significant differences between groups. Gender, age
(continuous), and educational level (low/medium versus high) were included as covariates in
all models. We used IBM SPSS 25.0 (Armonk, NY, USA) to analyze our data; p-values below
0.05 were considered statistically significant.
Results
Participants’ Characteristics
Participants who had acquired a dog 5–14 months after the baseline questionnaire (T0) and
who were still in possession of the dog at T2 were included in the study. The final sample con-
sisted of 183 participants, most of whom were female (87%), with an average age of 43 years
(SD = 12; 21% were below 30 years; 36% between 30 and 45; an additional 36% were
between 46 and 60 years and 7% were over 60 years); 59% had a high level of education.
Respondents were mainly part of a couple without (at home living) children (45%) or part of a
Social-Cognitive Processes Before Dog Acquisition Associated with Future Relationship Satisfaction…
664 Anthrozoös
AZ 33(5).qxp_Layout 1 9/1/20 2:14 PM Page 664
family (35%); only 15% were single and 6% shared a household with other adults. The majority
(76%) obtained a puppy and not an adult dog.
When asked who would make the final decision to acquire the dog, the majority of respon-
dents who were part of a couple (82%) or part of a family (93%) answered “myself and others.”
When asked who would bear primary responsibility for the dog, the majority again answered
“myself and others” (couple: 76%; family: 79%). Regarding previous experience with dogs, 16%
had never owned a dog before while 44% already owned one or more dogs at T0. About 80%
of respondents did attend a dog-training course (mostly obedience training and/or puppy
courses). Almost three-quarter (70%) did not work with dogs professionally.
Descriptive Statistics
Preparation Activities: Table 1 displays the means and standard deviations for all 14 prepara-
tion activities. Pearson’s correlations ranged from 0.00 to 0.65 (see online supplemental file
Table S2a), with 30% being non-significant. Based on means (see Table 1), with scores around
Bouma et al.
665 Anthrozoös
Table 1. Means and standard deviations for variables assessed at baseline (T0) and at six
months (T1) and 18 months (T2) after acquiring the dog.
Variable M (SD)
How Often Do You…
1. Think about acquiring a dog? 4.15 (0.86)
2. Speak to others about acquiring a dog? 3.73 (0.94)
3. Think this is the right moment to acquire a dog? 3.40 (1.05)
4. Feel the desire to acquire a dog? 3.91 (0.90)
5. Talk to others about the investments related to dog ownership? 2.86 (1.15)
6. Think “I want that too” when seeing somebody walking a dog? 2.88 (1.32)
7. Visit internet websites that offer dogs (e.g., breeders, animal shelters, trading websites)? 3.17 (1.29)
8. Long for a dog? 3.92 (0.95)
9. Read books about dogs? 2.97 (1.32)
10. Feel insecure about dog ownership? 1.75 (0.95)
11. Search the internet for information about dogs? 3.14 (1.24)
12. Doubt the decision to acquire a dog? 1.93 (1.06)
13. Think about the advantages of having a dog? 3.69 (0.92)
14. Think about the disadvantages of having a dog? 2.84 (1.08)
Self-efficacy T0 8.90 (0.10)
Social norm T0 3.95 (0.76)
Advantages T0 3.43 (0.55)
Disadvantages T0 3.58 (0.50)
Number of behavior problems T1 4.86 (3.21)
Number of behavior problems T2 4.16 (2.94)
Perceived costs T1 1.75 (0.55)
Perceived costs T2 1.77 (0.56)
Pet satisfaction T1 6.14 (0.81)
Pet satisfaction T2 6.11 (0.98)
Decision satisfaction T2 4.49 (0.58)
Note: Data presented in the table concern 183 participants.
AZ 33(5).qxp_Layout 1 9/1/20 2:14 PM Page 665
4 corresponding to “often,” the data indicate that, before acquiring the dog, respondents often
thought about acquiring the dog, felt a desire to acquire a dog, and longed for the dog. With
scores around 2 corresponding to “sometimes,” the data indicate that, on average, participants
only sometimes had doubts concerning whether it was the right decision to acquire a dog.
Socio-Cognitive Variables: Table 1 displays the means and standard deviations for the predic-
tive variables at T0. Pearson’s correlations between the socio-cognitive variables range from
0.01 to 0.36 and are all in the expected direction (see online supplemental file Table S2a). The
table also contains the means and standard deviations for the outcome variables at T1 and T2.
Pearson’s correlations range from 0.19 to 0.60 and are all in the expected direction (see online
supplemental file Table S2b). Correlations from the same variables measured at two different
times (T1 and T2) are small to moderate: problems (r = 0.59), pet satisfaction (r = 0.46), and
perceived costs (r = 0.60), suggesting changes in these variables over time.
Consistency Between Plan and Outcome
For all 183 participants we had information about the dog they acquired at T1: 51% acquired
a female dog, 76% acquired a puppy, and the majority of dogs (59%) came from breeders. For
the majority of the sample (88%, n = 161), we had some information about the plan at T0 and
the actual purchase at T1 (regarding breed (n = 125), purchase source (n = 161), and age
(n = 151; due to participants answering “I do not know yet” at T0). Most respondents (94%) pur-
chased the breed they had planned, and most (91%) adhered to their plans to purchase either
a puppy or an adult dog. Plans differed from actions regarding the source of the dog, with only
76% adhering to their plans. Fewer people who had planned to acquire a dog from a shelter
did so, instead acquiring their dogs from trading websites or foreign dog-welfare organizations.
This discrepancy between intending to adopt a dog from a shelter and actually purchasing the
dog elsewhere is also reported in a study by Garrison and Weiss (2015).
For 106 participants (58%) we had information for all three aspects (breed, source, and
age); for 48 participants (26%) there was information about two aspects; for 23 participants
(13%) there was only about one aspect; and for six participants (3%) no information at T0 was
available to determine consistency. We only included participants in our consistency analyses
for whom we had information about at least two aspects (n = 154, 84% of the total sample).
Of the 106 participants for which we have information about the three aspects of consistency
between plan and actual purchase, 87% were consistent for all three aspects, while all 48
participants were inconsistent for one of the two. Participants who were included in the
(in)consistency analyses did not differ with regard to gender, educational level, age, and
experience with dogs from those who were excluded.
No differences were present between consistent and inconsistent participants with regard
to the covariates gender (2(1,154) = 3.28, p = 0.070), educational level (2(1,154) = 1.521,
p = 0.217), experience with dogs (2(1,154) = 2.753, p = 0.097), and age (F(1,152) = 2.542,
p = 0.113). Also no differences were present between the socio-cognitive measures: advan-
tages (F(1,153) = 1.657, p = 0.200), disadvantages (F(1,160) = 0.001, p = 0.970), social norm
(F(1,160) = 3.274, p = 0.072), and self-efficacy (F(1,160) = 0.014, p = 0.907).
Predicting Decision Consistency
All 14 preparation activities were entered into the logistic regression model. Table 2 displays
the results for the 14 predictors and their covariates. Participants exhibiting inconsistency
between the initial plan (at T0) and the actual purchase engaged significantly more in two
Social-Cognitive Processes Before Dog Acquisition Associated with Future Relationship Satisfaction…
666 Anthrozoös
AZ 33(5).qxp_Layout 1 9/1/20 2:14 PM Page 666
activities and significantly less in two other activities, compared with those adhering to their
plans. Inconsistent people were more likely to visit websites offering dogs and to doubt their
decision to acquire a dog. Inconsistent people spent less time reading books about dogs and
dog acquisition and talked less to others about the investments related to dog ownership.
The four socio-cognitive variables were also entered into the logistic regression model
together (results in Table 3) but none had a significant effect on inconsistency. Covariate age
had a significant effect, with inconsistent participants tending to be older than those adhering
to their plan.
Predicting Problems, Satisfaction, and Perceived Costs
Tables S3a–S3g (online supplemental file) present results of statistical regression models
including all activities. Only two of the 14 activities were associated with future outcomes:
Bouma et al.
667 Anthrozoös
Table 2. Results of the prediction of plan inconsistency (T1), by preparation activities (T0).
Predictor BSEWald 2p-value Exp(B) 95%CI
for EXP(B)
Gender –1,279 0.682 3.514 0.061 0.278 0.073–1.060
Age 0.02 0.019 1.156 0.282 1.020 0.984–1.058
Education –0.136 0.46 0.088 0.767 0.872 0.354–2.151
1. Think about acquisition –0.427 0.373 1.309 0.253 0.653 0.314–1.355
2. Speak about acquisition –0.026 0.341 0.006 0.940 0.975 0.500–1.900
3. Doubt right moment –0.357 0.262 1.859 0.173 0.700 0.419–1.169
4. Feel desire 0.478 0.326 2.153 0.142 1.613 0.852–3.056
5. Talk about investments –0.452 0.22 4.203 0.040 0.636 0.413–0.980
6. “I want that too” 0.03 0.195 0.023 0.879 1.030 0.703–1.510
7. Visit websites offering dogs 0.547 0.211 6.733 0.009 1.729 1.143–2.614
8. Long for a dog –0.036 0.33 0.012 0.914 0.965 0.506–1.841
9. Read books –0.537 0.196 7.477 0.006 0.585 0.398–0.859
10. Feel insecure –0.29 0.301 0.927 0.336 0.748 0.415–1.351
11. Search online information 0.015 0.247 0.004 0.951 1.015 0.626–1.648
12. Doubt decision 0.804 0.298 7.303 0.007 2.235 1.24– 4.006
13. Think about advantages 0.144 0.284 0.257 0.612 1.155 0.662–2.015
14. Think about disadvantages –0.082 0.245 0.113 0.736 0.921 0.570–1.488
Note: Numbers in bold = significant (< 0.05). Numbers correspond to numbers and items in Table 1 (n = 154).
Table 3. Results of the prediction of plan inconsistency (T1), by socio-cognitive variables (T0).
Predictor BSEWald 2p-value Exp(B) 95% CI for
EXP(B)
Gender –1.062 0.591 3.227 0.072 0.364 0.109–1.112
Age 0.034 0.015 5.052 0.025 1.034 1.004–1.065
Education 0.540 0.389 1.931 0.165 1.716 0.801–3.677
Self-efficacy 0.141 0.190 0.548 0.459 1.151 0.793–1.670
Social Norm –0.439 0.259 2.873 0.090 0.645 0.388–1.071
Advantages 0.568 0.357 2.532 0.112 1.764 0.877–3.549
Disadvantages –0.207 0.345 0.361 0.548 0.813 0.414–1.598
Note: Number in bold = significant (p < 0.05, n = 154).
AZ 33(5).qxp_Layout 1 9/1/20 2:14 PM Page 667
Social-Cognitive Processes Before Dog Acquisition Associated with Future Relationship Satisfaction…
668 Anthrozoös
“talking about dogs” and “visiting websites that offer/sell dogs.” People who frequently talked
to others about dog acquisition perceived lower costs at T1 ( = –0.321, p = 0.003; see Table
S3b). People who frequently visited websites offering dogs perceived higher costs at T2
( = 0.177, p = 0.040; see Table S3e) and were less satisfied with their dogs at T2 ( = –0.246,
p = 0.005; see Table S3f). We included these three significant activities (and their covariates) in
three separate models. According to the results, the activity “talking to others” was still signifi-
cantly associated with perceived costs at T1 ( = –0.246, p = 0.001). This model explained
6.4% of the variance (R2 unadjusted) in perceived costs, with 5.7% (R2 change) being due to
the preparation activity “talking to others.” “Visiting websites” was significantly associated with
pet satisfaction at T2 ( = –0.164, p = 0.029). This model explained 3.0% of the variance
(R2 unadjusted) in pet satisfaction, with 2.6% (R2 change) due to the preparation activity “visiting
websites offering dogs.” The effect of “visiting websites” on perceived costs was no longer
significant ( = 0.096, p = 0.201) when included as single predictor in a model with covariates.
The socio-cognitive factors assessed at T0 were related to behavior problems, perceived
costs, and satisfaction with the dog at both T1 and T2. An overview of statistics from the
regression models is provided in Tables S4a–S4g (online supplemental file). Self-efficacy at T0
was associated with six of the seven future outcomes. The more confident respondents were
at T0 regarding their ability to raise and care for a dog, the fewer the problems they reported
after living with their dogs for six (T1) ( = –0.243, p = 0.003; Table S4a) and 18 months (T2)
( = –0.264, p = 0.001; Table S4d). Respondents with high levels of self-efficacy perceived
lower costs at T1 ( = –0.218, p = 0.005; Table S4b) and T2 ( = –0.170, p = 0.034; Table
S4e), and they were more satisfied with their dogs at T1 ( = 0.288, p < 0.001; Table S4c) and
T2 ( = 0.188, p = 0.020; Table S4f). In addition to self-efficacy, expecting relatively more dis-
advantages was significantly related to higher perceived costs at T1 ( = 0.195, p = 0.007;
Table S4b) and less satisfaction with the dog at T1 ( = –0.214, p = 0.003; Table S4c).
Effects of Covariates in Analyses
In the analyses of both the preparation activities and the socio-cognitive factors, age and
educational level were found to have significant effects on satisfaction with the decision to
acquire the dog (see online Table S3g and S4g). Older people and those with high educa-
tional levels were more satisfied with their decisions. There was also a significant association
between age and perceived costs after six months of living with the dog (T1); older participant’s
perceived lower costs than younger participants did (see Table S4b).
Post-hoc Analyses
The effects remained almost unchanged after including the level of experience with dogs (never
versus previous/current owners) as a covariate in the regression models.
Discussion
In a recent review, Holland (2019) concludes that more information is needed concerning the
underlying processes driving behavior when humans acquire pets. Our results indicate that
soci0-cognitive factors and preparation processes in the period before acquiring a dog (the
motivational phase) are related to desired and undesired consequences six and 18 months
later. Overall, the data indicate that the experience phase of the owner–dog relationship may
be determined partly during the motivational phase, with self-efficacy being the most consis-
tent predictor of later consequences. Bandura (1977) defines self-efficacy as “the confidence
one has to execute certain courses of action to deal with prospective situations.” In our study,
AZ 33(5).qxp_Layout 1 9/1/20 2:14 PM Page 668
self-efficacy refers to the ability to train and care for a dog. Greater confidence was associated
with fewer behavior problems, lower perceived costs, and greater future satisfaction. Greater
confidence in one’s ability to do what is needed enhances willingness to exert the required
effort, possibly resulting in fewer problems with the dog, greater satisfaction, and lower
perceived costs.
The results revealed few relationships between the other socio-cognitive factors (social
norms and expected (dis)advantages) in the motivational phase with desired and undesired
consequences. The motivation to acquire a dog centers on the expected advantages of dog
ownership. If these expectations are high before acquiring a dog, actual dog ownership is
more likely to fall short of these expectations, thus generating a disappointing reality. Our r esults
provide no support for this assumption, as there were no associations between expected
advantages and satisfaction or between costs and problems. At the same time, however, our
results indicate that expecting more disadvantages is associated with less satisfaction and
higher perceived costs after six months. This might indicate that disadvantages perceived
beforehand indeed played out in the reality of dog ownership, thus indicating that their esti-
mates were somewhat realistic. Another, related explanation might be that these people were
less confident about their decisions. Our data did indeed indicate that participants expecting
high levels of disadvantages were also likely to doubt their decisions concerning acquiring a
dog and the proper time to do so. Moreover, these respondents often thought about the dis-
advantages of having a dog. Given that social norms are positively associated with these
activities, participants perceiving many disadvantages before acquiring a dog had experienced
some social pressure to acquire the dog.
In conclusion, of the socio-cognitive factors assessed in the motivational phase (before
actually acquiring a dog), only self-efficacy was a robust predictor of desired and undesired
consequences in the experience phase. Social norms and advantages/disadvantages had hardly
any influence on the behavior of dog owners and the development of their relationships with
their dogs in the experience phase. According to the Theory of Planned Behavior, self-efficacy
(i.e., perceived behavioral control) is indeed the only socio-cognitive factor that is directly linked
to the execution of behavior (Ajzen, 1991). The other factors (social norms and advantages/
disadvantages) are conceptualized as influencing only the decision; not the later behavior.
The socio-cognitive factors (due primarily to self-efficacy) explained 8–13% of the variance,
thus indicating that their potential influence is modest but possibly highly relevant (Rosenthal
& Rubin, 1979). Low self-efficacy can be regarded as a risk factor for undesired future situa-
tions. Importantly, this factor can be assessed before acquisition of a dog, thereby allowing the
prevention of canine behavior problems and disappointment with the relationship or the deci-
sion. Reducing behavior problems by even a small amount is of great importance. Interestingly,
self-efficacy was not related to inconsistency between plans and actual purchase (which can
be regarded as a proxy for impulse buying).
Only a few preparation activities were related to undesired consequences, explaining 3–5%
of the variance. In the motivational stage, these activities apparently have little to do with what
happens during the development of the owner–dog relationship in the experience phase.
Some activities were nevertheless related to inconsistency between plans and actual purchase
(a proxy for impulse buying). According to the data, more frequent doubt in the motivational
phase was related to more inconsistency. Doubt may thus reflect an incomplete decision
process, as complete decisions might protect against impulse buying. Furthermore, reading
books about dogs and acquiring dogs increased adherence to initial plans, as did talking with
Bouma et al.
669 Anthrozoös
AZ 33(5).qxp_Layout 1 9/1/20 2:14 PM Page 669
others about dog acquisition. Talking to others might reflect the association that we found be-
tween social norms and consistency. Gathering information by reading and talking to others
might also reinforce confidence in one’s ability to care for and train a dog. Talking to others about
the decision to acquire a dog was related to lower perceived costs after six months. Talking to
others might facilitate the decision process, and frequent engagement might reflect a more thor-
ough decision process. In contrast, frequency of visiting websites offering dogs (e.g., breeders,
shelters, private advertisements) was inversely related to plan consistency. People who visit these
websites frequently might be more prone to impulse buying owing to a tendency to “fall in love”
with a picture or a story of a dog, even though the dog does not resemble their initial plan (as
shaped through a thorough consideration of the advantages and disadvantages of dog owner-
ship). Such impulsive decisions could result in disappointment with the dog, as indicated by our
data: frequent visitors to websites offering dogs perceived their dogs as more costly after 18
months. Unfortunately, we have no detailed information about the actual websites that were vis-
ited. Taken together, although preparation activities were related to the decision process, they had
hardly any influence on the development of the owner–dog relationship.
Limitations
Our study had several limitations. Our sample was not entirely representative of the popula-
tion of dog owners in the Netherlands (Facts and Figures on the Dutch Companion Animal
Sector [Feiten en cijfers Gezelschapsdierensector], 2015). First, while most dogs in the
Netherlands are owned by families with children, only one-third of our respondents were part
of a family with children. Second, although most dogs are mongrels, almost three-quarters of
our respondents owned purebred dogs. Also, highly educated women were over-represented
in our sample. This is common in animal-related research, partly owing to the general ten-
dency of women to have a more positive attitude toward animals than men (Herzog, Betchart,
& Pittman, 1991). In addition, people with low or intermediate levels of education were under-
represented in our sample. Another aspect that should be considered when interpreting our
findings is that they may have been influenced by over-recruitment. Our aim to hear from “peo-
ple planning to acquire a dog within one year” most probably led to the exclusion of people
with negative scores on advantages, social norms, and self-efficacy regarding dogs, as well
as those with little preparation activity. This may have led to less variance and, consequently,
less covariance between variables. Finally, the fact that three-quarters of the sample obtained
puppies might have influenced the results concerning behavior problems, given that some
behaviors are more common among puppies than among adult dogs, and vice versa.
Conclusion
The longitudinal design of this study allowed for testing whether factors involved in the deci-
sion-making process concerning the acquisition of a dog (assessed before actual acquisition)
could predict undesired consequences at a later stage. The results indeed indicated that
factors in the motivational phase—particularly self-efficacy—were related to factors in the
experience phase. Furthermore, preparing by reading books about dogs and talking to peo-
ple about the decision to acquire a dog might promote “well-considered” decisions. Frequently
visiting websites offering dogs could promote impulse buying, with negative consequences for
the future dog–owner relationship. Further studies are needed on the process of impulse
decision-making concerning pets and how early factors can lead to later problems. This could
provide information that helps to prevent problems developing between owners and their dogs,
and thus benefitting both humans and dogs.
Social-Cognitive Processes Before Dog Acquisition Associated with Future Relationship Satisfaction…
670 Anthrozoös
AZ 33(5).qxp_Layout 1 9/1/20 2:14 PM Page 670
Acknowledgements
This study received financial support from the Dutch Royal Association for the Protection of
Dogs. The authors thank all of the dog owners who participated in our research.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Note
1. In the Dutch educational system, a low level of education refers to vocational training, an intermediate level
refers to advanced vocational training, and a high level refers to college/university level.
References
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
50(2), 179–211.
Ajzen, I. (2002). Constructing a TPB questionnaire: Conceptual and methodological considerations. Retrieved
from https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/0574/b20bd58130dd5a961f1a2db10fd1fcbae95d.pdf?_ga=2.572
88040.332563760.1574688820–1517229358.1574152014.
Arluke, A. (2006). Just a dog: Understanding animal cruelty and ourselves. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University
Press.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84(2),
191–215.
Berget, B., Ekeberg, Ø., & Braastad, B. O. (2008). Animal-assisted therapy with farm animals for persons with
psychiatric disorders: Effects on self-efficacy, coping ability and quality of life, a randomized controlled trial.
Clinical Practice and Epidemiology in Mental Health, 4(9), 50–64.
Brehaut, J. C., O’Connor, A. M., Wood, T. J., Hack, T. F., Siminoff, L., Gordon, E., & Feldman-Stewart, D. (2003).
Validation of a Decision Regret Scale. Medical Decision Making, 23(4), 281–292.
Coe, J. B., Young, I., Lambert, K., Dysart, L., Nogueira Borden, L., & Raji , A. (2014). A scoping review of
published research on the relinquishment of companion animals. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science,
17(3), 253–273.
Curb, L. A., Abramson, C. I., Grice, J. W., & Kennison, S. M. (2003). The relationship between personality match
and pet satisfaction among dog owners. Anthrozoös, 26(3), 395–404.
Dwyer, F., Bennett, P. C., & Coleman, G. J. (2006). Development of the Monash Dog Owner Relationship Scale
(MDORS). Anthrozoös, 19(3), 243–256.
Endenburg, N., ‘t Hart, H., & Bouw, J. (1994). Motives for acquiring companion animals. Journal of Economic
Psychology, 15(1), 191–206.
Feiten en cijfers Gezelschapsdierensector [Facts and Figures Dutch Companion Animal sector]. (2015). HAS
University of Applied Sciences and University of Utrecht, The Netherlands. Retrieved from https://
edepot.wur.nl/361828 (in Dutch).
Garrison, L., & Weiss, E. (2015). What do people want? Factors people consider when acquiring dogs, the
complexity of the choices they make, and implications for nonhuman animal relocation programs. Journal of
Applied Animal Welfare Science, 18(1), 57–73.
Global survey on pet ownership. (2016). Growth from knowledge. Retrieved from https://www.gfk.com/global-
studies/global-studies-pet-ownership/.
Greenebaum, J. (2006). The throw-away society and the family dog: An exploration of the consumption and
dispossession of companion animals. Journal of Social and Ecological Boundaries, 2(2), 34–55.
Herzog, H. A., Betchart, N. S., & Pittman, R. B. (1991). Gender, sex role orientation, and attitudes toward
animals. Anthrozoös, 4(3), 184–191.
Holland, K. E. (2019). Acquiring a pet dog: A review of factors affecting the decision-making of prospective dog
owners. Animals, 9(4), doi:10.3390/ani9040124.
Hsu, Y., & Serpell, J. A. (2003). Development and validation of a questionnaire for measuring behavior and
temperament traits in pet dogs. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association, 223(9), 1293–1300.
Jagoe, A., & Serpell, J. (1996). Owner characteristics and interactions and the prevalence of canine behaviour
problems. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 47(1–2), 31–42.
Bouma et al.
671 Anthrozoös
AZ 33(5).qxp_Layout 1 9/1/20 2:14 PM Page 671
Janis, I., & Mann, L. (1977). Decision making: A psychological analysis of conflict, choice and commitment.
New York, NY: Free Press.
Lambert, K., Coe, J., Niel, L., Dewey, C., & Sargeant, J. M. (2015). A systematic review and meta-analysis of
the proportion of dogs surrendered for dog-related and owner-related reasons. Preventive Veterinary
Medicine, 118(1), 148–160.
McConnell, A. R., Brown, C. M., Shoda, T. M., Stayton, L. E., & Martin, C. E. (2011). Friends with benefits:
On the positive consequences of pet ownership. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(6),
1239–1252.
Muruganantham, G., & Bhakat, R. S. (2013). A review of impulse buying behavior. International Journal of
Marketing Studies, 5(3), 149–160.
Neilson, J. C., & Horwitz, D. F. (2007). Blackwell’s five minute veterinary consult clinical companion: Canine
and feline behavior. Ames, AI: Blackwell.
O’Farrell, V. (1997). Owner attitudes and dog behaviour problems. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 52(3–
4), 205–213.
O’Haire, M. (2010). Companion animals and human health: Benefits, challenges, and the road ahead. Journal
of Veterinary Behavior, 5(5), 226–234.
Rook, D. W. (1987). The buying impulse. Journal of Consumer Research, 14(2), 189–99.
Rook, D. W., & Gardner, M. P. (1993). Research in consumer behavior. In In the mood: Impulse buyings’affective
antecedents. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1979). Comparing significance levels of independent studies. Psychological
Bulletin, 86(5), 1165–1168.
Schwarzer, R., & Luszczynska, A. (2015). Health action process approach. In Predicting health behaviours.
Maidenhead, UK: McGraw Hill Open University Press.
Staats, S., Wallace, H., & Anderson, T. (2008). Reasons for companion animal guardianship (pet ownership) from
two populations. Society & Animals, 16(3), 279–291.
Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Wakslak, C. (2007). Construal levels and psychological distance: Effects on
representation, prediction, evaluation, and behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 17(2), 83–95.
Veirman, M. de., Cauberghe, V., & Hudders, L. (2017). Marketing through Instagram influencers: The impact of
number of followers and product divergence on brand attitude. International Journal of Advertising, 36(5),
798–828.
Vink, L., Dijkstra, A., & Epstude, K. (2019). A longitudinal study of decision making in the process of acquiring
a dog. Anthrozoös, 32(4), 489–501.
Social-Cognitive Processes Before Dog Acquisition Associated with Future Relationship Satisfaction…
672 Anthrozoös
AZ 33(5).qxp_Layout 1 9/1/20 2:14 PM Page 672