Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
LETTER
Have Europeans Grown Tired of Democracy? New
Evidence from Eighteen Consolidated Democracies,
1981–2018
Alexander Wuttke* , Konstantin Gavras and Harald Schoen
University of Mannheim, Mannheim, Baden-Württemberg, Germany
*Corresponding author. E-mail: alexander.wuttke@uni-mannheim.de
(Received 1 March 2019; revised 17 January 2020; accepted 20 February 2020)
Abstract
Democracies without democrats are not sustainable. Yet, recent studies have argued that Western citizens
are turning their backs on the system of self-governance, thereby eroding the societal foundations of con-
solidated democracies. This study contributes to discussions about citizen support of democracy by
(1) analyzing new cross-national survey data in 18 European countries that facilitate assessments of the
temporal and geographical generalizability of previous findings, (2) disentangling age, cohort and period
effects, thereby aligning the analytical methods with the theoretical arguments and (3) transparently
reporting all evidence derived from pre-registered analyses to avoid cherry-picked findings. The findings
show that citizens of consolidated democracies continue to endorse self-governance. Yet in some (but not
all) countries, there is evidence of a growing number of ‘democrats in name only’, particularly among the
young generation. These findings suggest a second phase in research on democratic fatigue that broadens
the analytical scope for the multi-faceted nature of democratic support.
Keywords: illiberal democracy; populism; APC analysis; regime preferences; institutional trust; political interest; open science;
civic culture
Taking seriously the notion that ‘one of the greatest threats to democracy is the idea that it is
unassailable’(Carey et al. 2019), scholars have responded to contemporary challenges to democratic
principles and practices by revisiting a foundational question of modern political science (Easton,
Gunnell and Stein 1995): what are the prerequisites for a stable and healthy liberal democracy, and
to what extent are these conditions currently met? As any democratic system requires a sufficiently
large number of citizens who want to govern themselves (Almond and Verba 1963; Claassen 2019),
one strand of the literature focuses on citizen attitudes. Examining ‘democracy’s fading allure’
(Plattner 2015), scholars in this line of research investigate whether Western citizens are still sup-
portive of the democratic system they live in (Mattes 2018). Given the rise of populist and authori-
tarian leaders, scholars fear that ideas might have taken root that are incompatible with core
democratic components (Canovan 1999;Caramani2017). More and more citizens might consider
democracy as merely one of several viable options rather than the only legitimate form of govern-
ment, thereby challenging democracy’s role as ‘the only game in town’(Linz and Stepan 1996,15).
Against this backdrop, several scholars (Denemark, Donovan and Niemi 2016a;FoaandMounk
2017a;Mounk2018; Wike and Fetterolf 2018) claim to have identified a turning point in the his-
torical development of democracy: as consolidated democracies can no longer rely on the unshaken
support of their citizens, it once again seems conceivable that long-established democracies could
regress into some kind of nondemocratic regime.
© The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press. This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction
in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
British Journal of Political Science (2020), page 1 of 13
doi:10.1017/S0007123420000149
Most prominently, Foa and Mounk (2016,2017a,2017b) argued that citizens of consolidated
democracies –in particular, the youngest generations –are turning their back on liberal democ-
racy. The democratic deconsolidation hypothesis has sparked a ferocious debate. Critics have
challenged the hypothesis’validity on theoretical and empirical grounds, objecting that the argu-
ments and evidence were unconvincing and cherry picked. According to critics, when considered
in its entirety the data did not indicate an erosion of support for democracy (Alexander and
Welzel 2017; Inglehart 2016; Kirsch and Welzel 2019; Norris 2017; Voeten 2017; Zilinsky
2019). This state of affairs leaves those interested in reliable answers about whether democracy’s
attitudinal foundations are eroding with considerable uncertainty about what political science can
tell about this important question of contemporary politics.
In an attempt to provide timely and unbiased evidence about the dynamics of democratic sup-
port in Western democracies, this study makes three contributions to help overcome shortcomings
in the ongoing scholarly debate. First, it is the first study in this literature to make serious attempts
to disentangle period, life-cycle and cohort effects, thereby examining the proposed generation-
based arguments using adequate analytical methods. Secondly, this study relies on a recently pub-
lished dataset, thereby responding to calls to enhance the slim body of longitudinal evidence on
the development of democratic support. Finally, by following a pre-registered protocol and report-
ing the study’s entire evidence in an interactive companion website, we attempt to ensure that the
reported findings are neither selectively reported nor polished in one direction or the other.
In a nutshell, our analysis of survey data from eighteen European democracies suggests that atti-
tudes toward democracy remain stable and at a high level when queried as a generic term. However,
in some countries there is evidence of growing susceptibility to regime types that are incompatible
with established notions of liberal democracy, particularly among the young generation. In the con-
cluding section, we discuss how these seemingly contradictory findings can be reconciled.
How Trends in Democratic Support Come About
Proponents of the democratic deconsolidation hypothesis allude to contemporary economic and
socio-cultural transformations as origins of the alleged growing disenchantment with democracy
in Western societies. These propositions suggest the young generation is the vanguard of demo-
cratic decline (Denemark, Donovan and Niemi 2016a; Foa and Mounk 2016; Foa and Mounk
2017a). Recent birth cohorts are seen to be the most susceptible to growing democratic disaffec-
tion because they have been hit the hardest by recent economic crises in several European coun-
tries, which may have undermined their confidence in the prevailing political system (Mounk
2018). Given their socialization experiences in the age of individualization and digitalization,
the natural properties of democracy’s decision-making processes might be at odds with the pre-
ferences of today’s young generation. Whereas recent birth cohorts have grown accustomed to
highly individualized consumer products and a fast-reacting media environment, democracy’s
institutions are slow by design and do not necessarily respond to individual preferences; they
instead prescribe collectively binding decisions (Gurri 2018; Streeck 2016). As a consequence,
today’s young generation may become more disenchanted with democratic politics and poten-
tially more open to alternative forms of governing.
However, the expectation of growing democratic fatigue, especially among the young, contra-
dicts modernization theory, which posits that democracy’s attitudinal foundations will grow
stronger from one generation to the next (Alexander and Welzel 2017; Kirsch and Welzel
2019; Norris 2017; Norris and Inglehart 2019). According to modernization scholars, in the
wake of a universal value change, assertive orientations that are more prevalent among older
cohorts shift toward self-expressive, emancipative orientations that are more prevalent among
younger generations who grew up in secure material environments. As a result, the young gen-
eration may be more critical of political authorities and institutions but more steadfast in their
support for the principles of self-governance.
2 Alexander Wuttke et al.
Note that, in these discussions, scholars occasionally refer to the ‘young generation’without
clearly defining generational boundaries or work with differing sets of cohort operationalizations
(for example, Foa and Mounk 2017a, 6; Foa and Mounk 2017b; 10; Norris and Inglehart 2019,
36). This vagueness partly reflects the fact that various macro-level mechanisms are at play.
Influences such as the 2008 economic crisis hit a well-defined cohort of individuals during
their formative phase, but other influences such as the ‘fifth wave’of information technology
(Gurri 2018) characterize the formative years of a larger and less clearly defined cohort. While
democracy-undermining influences may be more forceful the later a person was born, there is
no clear criterion to delineate younger from older birth cohorts. Nonetheless, we can conclude
that skeptics and advocates of the democratic deconsolidation hypothesis disagree about the
differences in democratic support between older and younger birth cohorts and, by implication
and more fundamentally, about the prospects that generational replacement holds for mass
support for the democratic system of governing.
Less controversial than the presence of generational disparities is the claim that democracy is
facing challenges that may shake confidence in the democratic polity, regardless of generational
affiliation. Hence, another conceivable pathway of eroding support for democracy is that the
recent economic and political crises and other processes, such as the diminishing steering capaci-
ties of nationalized democracies, may have undermined democratic support in the form of period
effects –that is, uniform decreases from one point in time to the next irrespective of an indivi-
dual’s position in the life cycle and generational affiliation (Mounk 2018; Streeck 2016). What
remains disputed, however, is whether these period effects merely affect short-term political
orientations such as party preferences or whether they have begun to erode foundational attitudes
toward democracy such as support for the principles of self-governing. This dispute is partly
because the basic empirical facts on crucial aspects of Western societies are contested. This uncer-
tainty reflects various shortcomings in the literature.
Strengthening the Reliability and Validity of Findings in a Contested Field of Research
The topic’s longitudinal nature calls for survey questions that were asked repeatedly over a rela-
tively long period of time. Yet, time-series data on democracy-related questions are scarce. Hence,
it remains unclear whether the findings presented by prior research indicate short-term fluctua-
tions or long-term trends (van der Meer 2017). Responding to calls from all sides of the debate
for more comprehensive survey data, this study uses recently published cross-national data that
allow us to properly identify how democratic support has developed over time and across genera-
tions (Foa and Mounk 2016, 10; Foa and Mounk 2017a, 10; Voeten 2017, 1).
Although most time series do not cover long time periods, the respective surveys often contain
multiple instruments to assess individual-level political support (Mattes 2018). While a large
number of indicators reflects the complex nature of democracy, it also hinders the comparability
of studies, thereby contributing to the ambiguity of existing research. Moreover, considering the
vast array of analytical options, each with possibly different outcomes, it is no wonder that authors
frequently suspect that some findings in the scholarly controversy were reported selectively in a
way that fits the authors’claim (Alexander and Welzel 2017; Norris 2017, 5; Voeten 2017, 1).
In this study, we respond to the challenges for knowledge accumulation that result from
researchers’ample degrees of freedom in a contested field of social inquiry in the following
way. Before obtaining the data, we documented our analytical strategy and all indicators we con-
sidered relevant for assessing democratic support in a public pre-analysis plan.
1
What is more,
although limitations of space prohibit reporting the entire body of evidence in this study, we
1
We pre-registered our research questions, selection and priority of indicators, analytical strategy and scripts, robustness
tests, and inferential rules. In the Shiny Web Application, we describe and justify (mostly minor) deviations from the pre-
analysis plan. The pre-analysis plan can be obtained at: https://osf.io/bw5j3/registrations.
British Journal of Political Science 3
transparently report all our findings in an easy-to-use interactive companion website. These pro-
cedures bind our hands to intentional or unintentional misuses of analytical discretions and pro-
vide the reader with unbiased and unrestricted access to the collected evidence (Wuttke 2019).
To strengthen the validity of the substantive findings and to increase the informational value
of testing the mechanisms that may underlie particular patterns of attitude shifts, this study
employs statistical techniques to separate distinct attitudinal trends. Attitudinal trends may
stem from period effects that reflect shifts in the marginal distribution of attitudes, for example
in response to external shocks that do not vary by age or generational affiliation. Alternatively,
such trends may reflect generational disparities that affect the distribution of attitudes within a
society over time through generational replacement. Time may also play a role as life-cycle effects
that are hard to disentangle from generational effects from a cross-sectional perspective. Yet, life-
cycle effects usually have no ramifications for attitudinal trends at the societal level and are thus of
limited relevance to the state of democracy (Norris 2017, 9f; Voeten 2017, 5).
2
Hence, each temporal effect has distinct implications for the future distribution of attitudes.
Because these temporal effects relate to distinct theoretical arguments, disentangling cohort,
period and life-cycle effects advances testing the theoretical mechanisms proposed in the debate
on democratic deconsolidation. Finally, aggregate-level stability when observed in descriptive
studies may overshadow countervailing attitudinal dynamics of period and cohort effects that
could be revealed when separating these effects. While prior studies have often used less demand-
ing methods (for example, Denemark et al. 2016b; Foa and Mounk 2017b; Norris 2017; Norris
and Inglehart 2019; Voeten 2017) that run the risk of conflating temporal effects, there are good
reasons to employ statistical techniques that discern the distinct temporal pathways that may
underlie attitudinal trends toward democratic deconsolidation.
Data and Research Design
We analyze data from the European Values Survey, including the recently released fifth wave
(EVS 2019). We consider all European countries in the analyses that are classified as consolidated
democracies according to the Polity IV index plus France. Because attitude trends may differ
across countries, we report the results for each country separately. Evidence on seven additional
European democracies is reported in an interactive Shiny Web Application that accompanies this
study (http://democracy.alexander-wuttke.de).
3
Bearing in mind the wealth of relevant indicators and the space limitations, we only report the
development of selected indicators in the main text. Indicator selection was guided by data avail-
ability (indicators must have been surveyed in at least three EVS survey waves) and substantive
arguments. Given their character as the ‘backbone of the measurement of support for democracy’
(Mattes 2018), it does not come as a surprise that citizens’regime preferences are central to cur-
rent scholarly debates (Foa and Mounk 2016; Voeten 2017). EVS employs the following questions
to capture them: ‘I’m going to describe various types of political systems and ask what you think
about each as a way of governing this country. For each one, would you say it is a very good, fairly
good, fairly bad or very bad way of governing this country? (1) Having a democratic political
system (2) Having a strong leader who does not have to bother with parliament and elections
(3) Having experts, not government, make decisions according to what they think is best for
the country (4) Having the army rule the country.’In the main text, we use this question to report
on respondents’preferences for democratic and authoritarian systems of government,
2
Significant changes in the age composition of the electorate may lend some importance to life-cycle effects.
3
In addition to the established data collection mode via personal interviews, EVS Wave 5 has an additional, experimental
self-administered data collection mode. In line with EVS recommendations, responses from these modes are not included in
the analysis because mode effects might impair comparabilityacross survey waves. Analyses on the extended dataset including
the self-administered data collection modes are reported in the Appendix and suggest slightly weaker support for democracy
among the youngest generation.
4 Alexander Wuttke et al.
respectively. As the second concept of key interest, we analyze the development of institutional
trust, which has also played a role in the current debate (Voeten 2017). For compact reporting,
we created an unweighted summary index of confidence in institutions that are affected by or
involved in the process of democratic decision making: parliaments, justice systems and civil ser-
vice bureaucracies (see the interactive Appendix for results on single indicators).
The main text focuses on these primary indicators, but we complement the reported evidence
with condensed conclusions on substantively related secondary variables that tap into more spe-
cific attitudes towards democracy and other cultural and political orientations that are relevant to
democratic stability. The full results on the latter are available in the interactive Appendix. The
Appendix also reports tabulated results and findings on (mostly statistically insignificant or
small) life-cycle effects. The main text focuses on period and generational disparities because
they are critical to the over-time development of aggregate popular support for democracy.
As a main analytical strategy for discerning age, period and cohort effects, we employ general-
ized additive models (GAM, Grasso 2014). We include the survey years as fixed effects for the
period effects and recode age to a three-level categorical variable (15–29, 30–59, 60+).
4
Often
referred to as the ‘problem of generations’(Mannheim 1970), operationalizing birth cohorts is
rarely straightforward. Generational boundaries are seldom self-evident, and their specification
is even more complex in cross-national analyses since formative phases and events may differ
across countries. Because ill-specified thresholds run the risk of hiding meaningful patterns,
we refrain from deriving a comprehensive categorization of cohorts. Instead, we estimate
smoothed nonlinear cohort effects that retrieve any cohort commonalities among groups of indi-
viduals born in temporal proximity. This exploratory approach is particularly useful for the pur-
pose of this study, as we focus on the attitudes of the young as one select birth cohort. Doing so
allows us to examine whether recent birth cohorts differ from older birth cohorts without setting
rigid a priori boundary specifications, and thus minimizes the risk of overlooking meaningful dis-
parities related to a person’s time of birth.
To assess the magnitude of attitudinal dynamics, we visualize predicted cohort and period
effects with simultaneous intervals (Simpson 2018) based on the observed values approach
(Hanmer and Kalkan 2013). Because age, period and cohort (APC) analyses are sensitive to mod-
eling choices, we replicated the analyses using the robust hierarchical APC method (rHAPC),
which are reported in the Appendix (Bell and Jones 2015). The rHAPC is more conservative con-
cerning generational effects than the GAM and tends to underestimate generational effects (Bell
and Jones 2018), particularly when they are nonlinear such as when one cohort stands apart from
preceding generations. We consider GAM to be the primary model and consider the results as
less robust when they are not supported by both models. Hence, when interpreting the results,
we focus on cases in which the evidence is consistent across models, and we mention substantial
inconsistencies.
Results
We begin our inquiry of changes in support for democracy with a brief descriptive overview of the
marginal distribution of the study’s central indicators (Figure 1). Inspecting the development of
citizens’self-reported preferences for a democratic system over the past decade, the overall trend
suggests stronger support for democracy in the vast majority of European societies, from an already
high level. A small decline in democratic regime preferences is visible in Denmark, but notable
increases can be observed in countries such as Germany, Poland and the United Kingdom.
The dynamics of institutional trust show similar patterns. Although confidence in the institu-
tions of democracy diminished in a few countries (for example, Slovenia), by and large, the past
decade was characterized by stability or rebounds of institutional trust in most countries.
4
Additional information on the method is provided in the Appendix.
British Journal of Political Science 5
Regime: Democracy Trust Institutions Regime: Strong Leader
Austria
Denmark
Finland
France
Germany
Hungary
Iceland
Italy
Lithuania
Netherlands
Norway
Poland
Slovakia
Slovenia
Spain
Sweden
Switzerland
United Kingdom
0.00 0.25 0.50 0.75 1.00 0.00 0.25 0.50 0.75 1.00 0.00 0.25 0.50 0.75 1.00
2018
2008
1999
1990
1981
2018
2008
1999
1990
1981
2018
2008
1999
1990
1981
2018
2008
1999
1990
1981
2018
2008
1999
1990
1981
2018
2008
1999
1990
1981
2018
2008
1999
1990
1981
2018
2008
1999
1990
1981
2018
2008
1999
1990
1981
2018
2008
1999
1990
1981
2018
2008
1999
1990
1981
2018
2008
1999
1990
1981
2018
2008
1999
1990
1981
2018
2008
1999
1990
1981
2018
2008
1999
1990
1981
2018
2008
1999
1990
1981
2018
2008
1999
1990
1981
2018
2008
1999
1990
1981
Figure 1. Trends in average levels of democratic support over time
Note: “Regime: democracy”: eva luation of “h aving a democratic political system”as a good or bad way of governing a country.
“Trust institutions:”Summary index of trust in national parliament, justice system and ci vil servi ce. “Regime: strong leader”:evalu-
ation of “having a strong leader who does not have to bother with parliament and elections”as a good or bad way of governing a
country.
6 Alexander Wuttke et al.
Democratic institutions bolstered citizens’trust in a diverse set of countries such as Germany,
Hungary, Norway and the United Kingdom.
The picture is more mixed with regard to authoritarian regime preferences. On average, the
citizens of Italy, Slovakia and Spain now more strongly support a strong leader who does not
have to bother with parliament than they did a decade ago. Still, in the majority of countries,
undemocratic strongman governance has not grown more popular or has even lost support
(for example, Hungary, the Netherlands and Poland).
Taken together, the raw distribution of indicators of democratic support reveals little evidence
in favor of the democratic deconsolidation hypothesis. None of the reported indicators consist-
ently declines across most countries; nor does any country consistently exhibit a decline in all
indicators. Still, the descriptive evidence cannot conclusively refute the democratic deconsolida-
tion hypothesis. For instance, if only the very recent birth cohort differs from previous genera-
tions, then –due to their small share of the population –even a strong decline in democratic
support among the young might not lead to discernible trends in overall levels of support.
Also, it is conceivable that period and cohort effects point in different directions and thereby
offset each other. Therefore, in the following analysis, we disentangle these temporal effects to
separately report period and generational effects.
Figure 2 enables us to take a deeper dive into the dynamics underlying the preferences for a
democratic system of government. Cohort effects are represented by the purple curve, which
shows predicted attitude levels for cohort members who turned 18 years of age at the respective
time shown on the x-axis. Visualizing period-specific dynamics from one survey wave to the next
while controlling for life-cycle and cohort effects, the red dot shows how period effects affect
mean attitude levels over time.
The GAM results presented in Figure 2 show that the recent rise in democratic regime prefer-
ences is mainly due to period effects. After controlling for life-cycle and cohort effects, the aver-
age respondent in virtually all European countries favors the democratic system of government
more strongly today than in the preceding survey wave a decade ago. The generational effects
on democratic regime preferences are less pronounced. Most European citizens strongly prefer
a democratic system of government, independent of their cohort affiliation. In some countries,
such as Spain and the Netherlands, democracy is more strongly endorsed by the most recent
birth cohorts than by earlier generations. Importantly, the more sophisticated GAM analysis pro-
vides no evidence that cohort and period effects might have worked in different directions, which
could have created the false impression of stability in the descriptive analysis. Challenging the
democratic deconsolidation hypothesis, period and cohort effects instead indicate stability or
even a strengthening of democratic regime preferences.
Although these findings do not reinforce the notion that support for democracy has been
eroding, subtle, yet consequential changes in citizens’attitudes toward the democratic system
might still have taken place. To begin with, regime preferences may remain unchanged, but
respondents could attach lower importance to living in a democracy. However, an analysis of
the importance respondents ascribe to living in a democratic country provides little evidence
of shifts in perceived importance between time periods or across generations (except for a mod-
erate generational decline in Sweden and the United Kingdom, see the Appendix).
5
In another scenario of subtle changes, abstract democratic regime preferences remain stable
while citizens withdraw support for the very institutions that embody this form of government.
Traditionally, one line of research has interpreted low levels of institutional trust in an optimistic
way –as signaling the vitality of a critical citizenry that holds politicians accountable while
remaining steadfast supporters of democratic principles (for example, Norris 2011). More
recently, however, some scholars expect growing distrust as a result of disenchantment with
5
Question wording: ‘How important is it for you to live in a country that is governed democratically?’, 10-point scale ran-
ging from ‘not at all important’to ‘absolutely important’.
British Journal of Political Science 7
the democratic process more generally. In this vein, it is argued that an increasing number of
populist citizens adhere to the idea of self-governance but reject the checks and balances on
the popular will that courts and parliaments occasionally impose (Mounk 2018; Wike and
Fetterolf 2018). Also, recent generations may perceive democracy’s deliberately slow place and
their mutually restraining institutions as inferior to the hyper-responsive media and technology
young citizens experience in other life domains (Gurri 2018; Mounk 2018).
However, Figure 3 shows that the dynamics in institutional trust mirror the evidence on demo-
cratic regime preferences: Generational effects are fairly modest, and period effects often point
upward. In a broad set of countries, period effects fostered confidence over the past decade,
often substantially (for example, Austria, Finland, Germany, Lithuania, Sweden, UK). Hence,
starting from a rather low level of confidence, democracy’s institutions could regain trust in
many European countries. For instance, for the average Austrian citizen, period effects led to
an increase of confidence in democratic institutions from 0.47 [95 per cent CI 0.44–0.50] to
0.55 [0.52–0.57] scale points over the past decade. All in all, the findings on confidence in democ-
racy’s institutions is further assurance that support for democracy remains firm.
Thus far, the findings have lent little credence to the claim that democratic deconsolidation has
been taking place in advanced democracies. This does not rule out the possibility, however, that
alternative forms of government have gained traction among the public. Stronger support for
other regime types diminishes democracy’s relative advantage over potential competitors, and
may thereby call into question the defining criterion of consolidated democracies as societies
where ‘support for antisystem alternatives is quite small’and, therefore, ‘democracy is the only
game in town’(Linz and Stepan 1996,14–15).
In order to find out whether citizens consider democracy to be the only legitimate regime type
or merely one of various viable options, we relied on the EVS indicators that measure evaluations
of different types of government, led by the military, by experts instead of the government, or by a
strong leader who does not have to bother with parliament or parties. Because support for
authoritarian forms of government is at the center of contemporary scholarly attention, we report
the results on anti-democratic preferences for a strong leader in the main text and the remaining
indicators in the Appendix. Notably, these indicators show inconsistent, but significant, dynamics
in democracy-related attitudes.
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Austri a
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Denmark
1999 2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Finland
1999 2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
France
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Germany
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Hungary
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Iceland
1999 2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Italy
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Lithuania
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Netherlands
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Norway
1999 2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Poland
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Slovakia
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Slovenia
1999 2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Spain
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Sweden
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Switzerland
1999 2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
United Kingdom
Figure 2. Period and cohort effects on democratic regime preferences
Note: the figure shows predicted mean values for period and cohort effects derived from GAM analyses using an observed value
approach with simultaneous confidence intervals. For the cohort plots in blue, smoothing splines are overlaid on the yearly predictions
displayed in the background. Red dots represent period effects, showing predicted mean levels in the respective survey year.
8 Alexander Wuttke et al.
To begin with, the citizenries of most democracies under study have not substantially changed
their opposition to strongman government over the past decade and continue to flatly reject this
way of governing (Figure 4). Yet, some countries do exhibit considerable period effects. Most not-
ably, in light of the country’s political turmoil and instability, Italians have become more open to
authoritarian government; in 2008, the predicted attitude level in Italy was at a low estimate of
0.21 [95 per cent CI 0.16–0.25] and increased to 0.32 [0.28–0.36] in 2018. However, the patterns
are not consistent across Europe; in some countries such as the Netherlands, Norway and Poland,
period effects on support for authoritarian regime types point downward.
The generational patterns reveal surprising results. The democratic deconsolidation hypothesis
suggests the highest susceptibility to authoritarian regime alternatives among the youngest cohort
(Foa and Mounk 2017b) whereas modernization theory would predict the strongest support
among older cohorts, particularly the interwar generations (Norris and Inglehart 2019). Across
countries, there is no consistent evidence for either prediction. Generational disparities are nar-
row in most cases. Notably, in a handful of countries, the observed patterns are compatible with
both of the seemingly rival models. In places such as Norway and Sweden, we observe U-shaped
curves; cohorts that came of age in the protest-ridden 1960s and 1970s are strongly opposed to
authoritarian government, but both older and the youngest birth cohorts are more open to a
strong leader who does not have to bother with parliament. (U-shaped curves are even more
prevalent in generational effects on the cultural role of authorities and, as an inverted-U curve,
also on political interest, see the Appendix). Notably, even these generational disparities suggest
that generational replacement is unlikely to lead to an erosion of democratic support in the near
future. Instead, older generations with above-average inclinations for authoritarian regimes will
be replaced by young cohorts that show similar orientations, leaving the average support for
this regime type at the societal level virtually unchanged. Altogether, regardless of whether gen-
erational patterns show flat or U-shaped curves, in the medium term generational replacement is
not on track to fundamentally shift support for strongmen government in Europe.
Do other indicators of regime preference show more worrying signs of increasing openness to
non-democratic alternatives? The preference for experts instead of governments making political
decisions is subject to some intertemporal dynamics, but they do not show a consistent pattern.
1990
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Austri a
1981
1990
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Denmark
1990
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Finland
1981
1990
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
France
1981
1990
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Germany
1990
1999 2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Hungary
1981
1990
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Iceland
1981
1990
1999 2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Italy
1999 2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Lithuania
1981
1990
1999 2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Netherlands
1981
1990 2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Norway
1990
1999 2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Polan d
1990
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Slovakia
1990
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Slovenia
1981
1990
1999 2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Spain
1981
1990
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Sweden
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Switzerland
1981
1990
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
United Kingdom
Figure 3. Period and cohort effects effects on trust in democratic institutions
Note: the figure shows predicted mean values for period and cohort effects derived from GAM analyses using an observed value
approach with simultaneous confidence intervals. For the cohort plots in blue, smoothing splines are overlaid on the yearly predictions
displayed in the background. Red dots represent period effects, showing predicted mean levels in the respective survey year.
British Journal of Political Science 9
Expert governments have become more widely accepted among the younger generations in
Denmark, Sweden and the United Kingdom. Yet period effects show decreasing levels of support
over time in Germany, Lithuania, Poland and Switzerland (see the Appendix). Military rule
remains disavowed as a form of government throughout most of the continent. Neither period
nor cohort effects point toward greater acceptance in the majority of countries. Yet, again in a
few societies –France, Norway, Slovenia and the United Kingdom –the favorability of a military
takeover has increased as of late, particularly among the most recent generation (see the
Appendix for full results). Further supporting the idea that the young generation in selected
countries has grown more comfortable with a political role of the military, an increasing share
of the young generation in the named countries tends to consider army takeover a legitimate
element of democracy where governments are incompetent.
6
What do our findings imply for the regime preferences of the most recent generation that have
received much attention in scholarly debates? The results suggest that members of this cohort remain
committed to democracy as a viable system of government. At the same time, in some countries, the
youngest cohorts are also more receptive to other regime alternatives that promise clear and fast deci-
sions. However, we observe increasing openness toward non-democratic regime types only in a
minority of European societies, and these trends are limited in magnitude. The findings thus attest
to the importance of monitoring generational disparities in regime preferences. However, while the
dynamics presented here give reason for attentiveness, these modest attitude changes alone unlikely
pose a severe threat to the societal underpinnings of democracy in European societies.
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Austri a
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Denmark
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Finland
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
France
1999 2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Germany
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Hungary
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Iceland
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Italy
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Lithuania
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Netherlands
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Norway
1999 2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Polan d
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Slovakia
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Slovenia
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Spain
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Sweden
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
Switzerland
1999
2008
2018
1980 2000 2020
0.00
0.25
0.50
0.75
1.00
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
1920
1940
1960
1980
2000
2020
United Kingdom
Figure 4. Period and cohort effects on authoritarian regime preferences
Note: the figure shows predicted mean values for period and cohort effects derived from GAM analyses using an observed value
approach with simultaneous confidence intervals. For the cohort plots in blue, smoothing splines are overlaid on the yearly predictions
displayed in the background. Red dots represent period effects, showing predicted mean levels in the respective survey year.
6
Question wording: ‘Many things are desirable, but not all of them are essential characteristics of democracy. Please tell me
for each of the following things how essential you think it is as a characteristic of democracy. Use this scale where 1 means
‘not at all an essential characteristic of democracy’and 10 means it definitely is ‘an essential characteristic of democracy’’.
Analyzed answer options: ‘people choose their leaders in free elections’,‘the army takes over when government is incompe-
tent’,‘civil rights protect people from state oppression’. Note that time-series data on these indicators are only available on a
few survey waves and only when merging the EVS and WVS data sets, which could introduce sampling and measurement
biases and thus impair over-time comparability. The findings indicate no period or cohort effects on perceiving civil rights
and free elections as elements of democracy. The generational disparities concerning military takeover were indicated by
GAM analysis but not by the rHAPC analysis, rendering these generational effects less robust.
10 Alexander Wuttke et al.
Conclusion
A thriving democracy must command the support of its people. Yet no free society can force
its citizens to accept the prevailing political order. Therefore liberal democracies live by prere-
quisites they cannot guarantee themselves (Böckenförde 1976, 60), rendering the self-rule of
sovereign citizens an inherently ambitious and precarious form of government. In times of
growing pressure on long-standing institutions and the principles of liberal democracy, it is
worth taking seriously recent evidence that suggests a significant decline of support for dem-
ocracy among citizens of Western societies. In order to examine the validity of this claim, we
analyzed recent survey data from eighteen European countries on various indicators of demo-
cratic support, employing adequate statistical techniques to disentangle cohort, life-cycle and
period effects.
The most consistent finding throughout the entire sample of advanced European democracies
indicates strong and continuing support for the democratic system of government. Preferences for
democratic government remain stable, as do levels of self-reported importance of living in demo-
cratic polities; confidence in democratic institutions has even grown recently. These results thus
cast doubt on far-reaching claims about widespread and increasing democratic fatigue, as there is
no evidence that citizens of democratic societies are growing tired of governing themselves.
In some (but not nearly all) European societies, however, we found changes in what ‘democ-
racy’means to citizens and evidence of increased susceptibility to alternative systems of govern-
ing, particularly among the youngest generations. Hence, recently born citizens in some countries
are less decisively opposed even to regime types that are clearly at odds with fundamental prin-
ciples of liberal democracy, especially when compared to the protest generation that came of age
in the 1960s and 1970s, which appears to champion self-governance.
To understand how we can reconcile the co-occurrence of these two findings –support for
democracy remains stable while support for undemocratic alternatives rises in some populations –
it is instructive to recall the nature of the political challenges that democracies currently face.
Whereas in previous decades the Western form of democracy was challenged by systems of an
entirely different type, critiques and assaults on democratic politics are now brought forward
in the name of democracy itself (Krastev and Holmes 2019; Runciman 2018). In this vein, it
may not be surprising that our findings align with recent studies that also showed stable support
for ‘democracy’as an abstract term (Zilinsky 2019) but substantial variation in what citizens asso-
ciate with this concept (Kirsch and Welzel 2019). Hence, some citizens may exhibit a growing
but somewhat indeterminate openness to trying other than the established forms of political gov-
ernance, which these citizens do not see as incompatible with their unshaken preference for a
democratic system of government.
Yet considering that some of these tentatively considered regime preferences are at odds with
established principles of (liberal) democracy, the bourgeoning evidence of democrats in name
only may introduce a second phase in research on democratic fatigue. For future research, the
relevant question seems less about whether citizens of consolidated democracies support the gen-
eric concept of democracy. The pressing question instead concerns the liberal-democratic quality
of citizens’regime and process preferences (for example, Carey et al. 2019; Graham and Svolik
2020; Wuttke, Gavras and Schoen 2020). Building on the idea that how we want to govern our-
selves is a multi-faceted concept, future research should examine whether the values, norms and
attitudes of self-proclaimed democrats are compatible with normatively derived notions of dem-
ocracy, particularly its liberal variant.
A comprehensive approach to the study of democratic support that accounts for the complex-
ity of multi-faceted regime and process preferences may help assess today’s political realities and
promote the integration of those theoretical perspectives on democratic deconsolidation that we
considered as competing in this study. For instance, Blühdorn (2020) recently suggested that –
consistent with post-modernist propositions –contemporary value change may indeed foster
British Journal of Political Science 11
pro-democratic attitudes, but considered these dynamics more complex than being for or against
democracy. Specifically, the tenet of ‘dialectic of democracy’posits that the growing emphasis on
self-realization strengthens demands for the recognition of one’s identity, which fosters demo-
cratic proclivities; but in the age of individualization and digitalized consumer capitalism,
these highly fragmented and volatile demands also pose a challenge for traditional democratic
institutions (Blühdorn 2020; Fukuyama 2018; Gurri 2018).
The specific type of democratic politics envisaged by this line of reasoning is not only hard to
reconcile with representative democracy in its current form. It is also hard to capture with current
survey measures. With a different focus, the theory of pernicious polarization (Somer and McCoy
2019) also describes phenomena that have implications for citizens’process preferences that are
too complex for standard survey indicators to capture. Therefore, established measures show us
that citizens have not given up on the idea of democracy. However, integrating insights from mul-
tiple perspectives may help to devise measures and analytical designs that move beyond a unidi-
mensional conception of democratic support as either high or low. In effect, it may prove useful
to grasping the complexities of how the multi-faceted regime and process preferences of demo-
cratic citizens change in the wake of current social transformations.
For political science, it thus remains a key task to continuously assess the state of consolidated
democracies by observing a broad array of individual-level indicators. This study contributes add-
itional evidence to this enterprise in a transparent and impartial way. It suggests caution against
reports about the breakdown of public support for democracy in Europe. However, the evidence
does not rule out the possibility that conceptions and preferences concerning the political order
will undergo significant and worrisome changes in the years and decades to come. Political scien-
tists are thus well advised to closely monitor the health of democratic support in order to avoid
the embarrassment of being forced to announce that the patient died years before without any-
body noticing.
Supplementary material. Replication material is available in Harvard Davaverse at: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/
Y5Y6VD, pre-analysis plans are available at https://osf.io/bw5j3/registrations/ and the interactive, online appendices are avail-
able at http://democracy.alexander-wuttke.de/ and at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123420000149
Acknowledgments. We thank Richard Traunmüller, Gavin Simpson, Marcel Neunhoeffer, Verena Kunz and Guido Ropers
for valuable comments, and Lukas Isermann and Marcel Klemm for help with the code review. We acknowledge the
European Values Survey’s timely publication of the questionnaire before publishing the survey data that enabled us to pre-
register this study.
References
Alexander AC and Welzel C (2017) The myth of deconsolidation: rising liberalism and the populist reaction. Journal of
Democracy Web Exchange.
Almond GA and Verba S (1963) The Civic Culture: Political Attitudes and Democracy in Five Nations. Princeton, NJ:
Princeton University Press.
Bell A and Jones K (2015) Age, period and cohort processes in longitudinal and life course analysis: a multilevel perspective.
In Burton-Jeangros C, Cullati S, Sacker A and Blane D (eds), A Life Course Perspective on Health Trajectories and
Transitions. Wiesbaden: Springer-Verlag, pp. 197–213.
Bell A and Jones K (2018) The hierarchical age–period–cohort model: why does it find the results that it finds? Quality &
Quantity 52(2), 783–799.
Blühdorn I (2020) The dialectic of democracy: modernization, emancipation and the great regression. Democratization
29(1), 38–57.
Böckenförde E-W (1976) Staat, Gesellschaft, Freiheit: Studien zur Staatstheorie und zum Verfassungsrecht [State, Society,
Liberal: Studies on Constitutional Law]. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp.
Canovan M (1999) Trust the people! Populism and the two faces of democracy. Political Studies 47(1), 2–16.
Caramani D (2017) Will vs. reason: the populist and technocratic forms of political representation and their critique to party
government. American Political Science Review 111(1), 54–67.
Carey JM, et al. (2019) Party, policy, democracy and candidate choice in U.S. elections, Bright Line Watch. Available from
http://brightlinewatch.org/us-elections/ (accessed 5 January 2019).
12 Alexander Wuttke et al.
Claassen C (2019) Does public support help democracy survive? American Journal of Political Science online first.
Denemark D, Donovan T and Niemi RG (2016a) The advanced democracies: the erosion of traditional democratic citizen-
ship. In Denemark D, Mattes RB and Niemi RG (eds), Growing up Democratic: Does it Make a Difference? Boulder, CO:
Lynne Rienner Publishers, pp. 181–206.
Denemark D, Mattes RB and Niemi RG (eds) (2016b) Growing up Democratic: Does it Make a Difference? Boulder, CO:
Lynne Rienner Publishers.
Easton D, Gunnell JG and Stein MB (eds) (1995) Regime and Discipline: Democracy and the Development of Political
Science. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
EVS (2019) European Values Study 2017: Integrated Dataset: ZA7500 Data file Version 2.0.0. Cologne. doi: 10.4232/1.13314.
Foa RS and Mounk Y (2016) The democratic disconnect. Journal of Democracy 27(3), 5–17.
Foa RS and Mounk Y (2017a) The end of the consolidation paradigm: a response to our critics. Journal of Democracy Web
Exchange.
Foa RS and Mounk Y (2017b) The signs of deconsolidation. Journal of Democracy 28(1), 5–15.
Fukuyama F (2018) Identity: Contemporary Identity Politics and the Struggle for Recognition. London: Profile Books.
Graham M and Svolik M (2020) Democracy in America? Partisanship, Polarization, and the Robustness of Support for
Democracy in the United States. American Political Science Review 114(2), 392–409.
Grasso MT (2014) Age, period and cohort analysis in a comparative context: political generations and political participation
repertoires in Western Europe. Electoral Studies 33,63–76.
Gurri M (2018) The Revolt of the Public and the Crisis of Authority in the new Millennium. San Francisco, CA: Stripe Press.
Hanmer MJ and Kalkan KO (2013) Behind the curve: clarifying the best approach to calculating predicted probabilities and
marginal effects from limited dependent variable models. American Journal of Political Science 57(1), 263–277.
Inglehart RF (2016) The danger of deconsolidation: how much should we worry? Journal of Democracy 27(3), 18–23.
Kirsch H and Welzel C (2019) Democracy misunderstood: authoritarian notions of democracy around the globe. Social
Forces 98(1), 59–92.
Krastev I and Holmes S (2019) Light That Failed: A Reckoning. Colchester: Allen Lane.
Linz JJ and Stepan AC (1996) Toward consolidated democracies. Journal of Democracy 7(2), 14–33.
Mannheim K (1970) The problem of generations. The Psychoanalytic Review 57(3), 378–404.
Mattes R (2018) Support for Democracy: Oxford Research Encyclopedia in Politics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Mounk Y (2018) The People vs. Democracy: Why Our Freedom Is in Danger and How to Save It. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Norris P (2011) Democratic Deficit: Critical Citizens Revisited. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Norris P (2017) Is western democracy backsliding? Diagnosing the risks. Journal of Democracy Web Exchange.
Norris P and Inglehart R (2019) Cultural Backlash: Trump, Brexit, and Authoritarian Populism.
Plattner MF (2015) Is democracy in decline? Journal of Democracy 26(1), 5–10.
Runciman D (2018) How Democracy Ends. New York: Basic Books.
Simpson GL (2018) Modelling palaeoecological time series using generalised additive models. Frontiers in Ecology and
Evolution 6.
Somer M and McCoy J (2019) Transformations through polarizations and global threats to democracy. The ANNALS of the
American Academy of Political and Social Science 681(1), 8–22.
Streeck W (2016) How Will Capitalism end? Essays on a Failing System. London: Verso.
van der Meer TWG (2017) Political Trust and the ‘Crisis of Democracy’. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Voeten E (2017) Are people really turning away from democracy? Journal of Democracy Web Exchange.
Wike R and Fetterolf J (2018) Liberal democracy’s crisis of confidence. Journal of Democracy 29(4), 136–150.
Wuttke A (2019) Why too many political science findings cannot be trusted and what we can do about it: a review of
meta-scientific research and a call for academic reform. Political Science Quarterly 60(1), 1–19.
Wuttke A, Gavras K and Schoen H (2020), “Replication Data for: ‘Have Europeans Grown Tired of Democracy?’”,https://
doi.org/10.7910/DVN/Y5Y6VD, Harvard Dataverse, V1, UNF:6:TcZ3LdDA6xWeUXeerhH98g== [fileUNF]
Wuttke A, Gavras K and Schoen H (2020) Leader of the free world or pioneer in democracy’s decline? Examining the demo-
cratic deconsolidation hypothesis on the mass level in East and West Germany. Research & Politics.
Zilinsky J (2019) Democratic deconsolidation revisited: young Europeans are not dissatisfied with democracy. Research & Politics.
Cite this article: Wuttke A, Gavras K, Schoen H (2020). Have Europeans Grown Tired of Democracy? New Evidence from
Eighteen Consolidated Democracies, 1981–2018. British Journal of Political Science 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1017/
S0007123420000149
British Journal of Political Science 13