Content uploaded by Juan José Igartua
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Juan José Igartua on Nov 17, 2020
Content may be subject to copyright.
International Journal of
Environmental Research
and Public Health
Article
Narrative Voice Matters! Improving Smoking
Prevention with Testimonial Messages through
Identification and Cognitive Processes
Juan-JoséIgartua * and Laura Rodríguez-Contreras
Department of Sociology and Communication, Faculty of Social Sciences, Campus Unamuno (Edificio FES),
37007 Salamanca, Spain; laurarodriguezcontreras@usal.es
*Correspondence: jigartua@usal.es; Tel.: +34-670-801-074
Received: 5 September 2020; Accepted: 2 October 2020; Published: 5 October 2020
Abstract:
Narrative messages are increasingly being used in the field of tobacco prevention. Our study
is based on narrative persuasion and aims to analyze the psychological mechanisms that explain
why the narrative voice is relevant to promote persuasive impact. An online experiment with a 2
(narrative voice)
×
2 (message) factorial design was carried out. Participants (525 adult smokers)
were randomly assigned to two experimental conditions (first-person versus third-person narrative
message). To increase the external validity of the study, two different messages were used within
each condition. After reading the narrative message the mediating and dependent variables were
evaluated. Participants who read the narrative in the first person experienced greater identification.
Moreover, mediational analysis showed that both counterarguing and cognitive elaboration played a
significant role in the relationship between narrative voice, identification, and persuasive impact.
This study confirm that narrative voice is not only an anecdotal formal choice but that it indirectly
affects variables related to tobacco prevention, due to the fact that first-person messages activate a
mechanism of affective connection with the message (increasing the identification with the protagonist)
that decreases resistance to prevention (the counterarguing process) while simultaneously stimulating
reflection or cognitive elaboration.
Keywords:
narrative persuasion; smoking prevention; testimonial messages; narrative voice;
identification with the protagonist; cognitive processes
1. Introduction
Tobacco is one of the main public health problems facing humanity. The consumption of this
substance has been linked to multiple health problems (such as respiratory and cardiovascular diseases
and various forms of cancer), being responsible for more than 8 million deaths a year [
1
,
2
]. In Spain,
18.8% of women and 25.6% of men smoke daily, constituting the second most widespread psychoactive
substance in the population and being responsible for 50,000 deaths per year (13% of all deaths) [
3
].
However, smoking is also the largest preventable cause of premature death and illness [
4
]. Therefore,
improving the effectiveness of smoking prevention campaigns or campaigns aimed at helping smokers
to quit is an important health communication goal.
The design of campaigns for the prevention of smoking requires innovative approaches that
consider both the characteristics of the messages and the psychological processes they trigger. In this
sense, narrative messages are increasingly being used in the field of tobacco prevention [
5
,
6
].
(An example of narrative intervention for tobacco prevention is the Tips from Former Smokers
campaign, developed by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) in the USA
(https://www.cdc.gov/tobacco/campaign/tips/index.html). In this context, our research focusses on
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 7281; doi:10.3390/ijerph17197281 www.mdpi.com/journal/ijerph
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 7281 2 of 15
testimonial messages or personal narratives, delivered by an adult smoker who tells their story
regarding successful tobacco cessation. This type of narrative message focusses on specific individual
cases, adopts an experiential style and does not include general, abstract or statistical information [
7
].
The ultimate goal of these types of messages is to cause a persuasive impact. In the present work,
we use the expression persuasive impact to refer to four outcome measures: intention to quit smoking,
perceived effectiveness of the message, expectations of self-efficacy (confidence in one’s ability to
abstain from smoking) and expectations of the efficacy of the preventive response (response efficacy
or outcome expectations). These measures constitute relevant variables in the theoretical models on
health communication and behavioral change (e.g., theory of planned behavior, theory of reasoned
action, health belief model, social cognitive theory and extended parallel process model; for a review,
see [
8
]). The mentioned theoretical models “are frequently applied to the crafting of persuasive health
messages and campaigns” [
9
]. Furthermore, these measures have been used as dependent variables in
several previous studies on smoking prevention using narrative messages [5,6,10].
Meta-analyses have shown that narrative interventions produce significant effects, but it is also
observed that not all narratives are effective [
11
,
12
]. Therefore, it is important to determine which
ingredients of such narratives are most effective from a persuasive point of view. In this sense, in the
present work, we focus on one characteristic of narrative messages that can condition their persuasive
efficacy: narrative voice [13,14].
The narrative voice is a property of the text, being defined as the perspective adopted when telling
a story, among which messages written in the first, second or third person can be differentiated [
15
–
18
].
In first-person narratives, the narrator is inside the story and directly expresses their views on a
subject or their experience. In this way, the audience has access to the protagonist’s internal life,
which facilitates engagement with the protagonist. On the other hand, messages written in the third
person include a narrator who relates the experience of the protagonist from an external or spectator’s
perspective. Therefore, third-person messages establish a kind of mental firewall that separates the
reader from the psychological life of the protagonist of the story. Finally, second-person narratives
identify the reader of the narrative as the protagonist, but they are rarely used in health campaigns [
19
].
Despite being a very relevant formal resource for the design of this type of message [
19
,
20
],
analysis of the effect of narrative voice has received insufficient attention in research on narrative
persuasion for smoking control. For example, in a review of 153 experimental studies on health-related
narrative persuasion, only 4 manipulated this attribute of the message, and only 1 of them focused
on smoking prevention [
19
]. Our work therefore aims to contribute to knowledge on the impact of
narrative voice in smoking prevention, as this line of research has been developed very little to date.
Narrative voice constitutes a formal characteristic of the message that determines the relationship
between the audience and the protagonist of the story. In this context, a second objective of this
work is to analyze the psychological mechanisms that explain why narrative voice constitutes a
relevant element to promote persuasive impact. Based on the main theoretical models on narrative
persuasion [21–23], several relevant mechanisms are established in the current study.
Previous research has found that people better imagine the thought processes of protagonists
(e.g., “Did you see what the main character was thinking and seeing?”) when narrative messages
are written in the first compared with the third person [
24
]. Therefore, it has been proposed that
first-person narratives make it easier for the audience to identify with the protagonist [
16
,
20
,
25
]. In this
context, identification with the protagonist (or the capacity to feel and adopt the point of view of the
protagonist of the narrative; [
14
,
26
,
27
] is proposed as a primary mediator. It is assumed that messages in
the first person enhance the aforementioned process, to a greater extent than messages in the third
person, by helping the individual to adopt the protagonist’s perspective and better imagine their
thought processes, thus leading to our first hypothesis:
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 7281 3 of 15
Hypothesis H1:
Compared with a third-person message, a first-person narrative message will induce greater
identification with the protagonist.
The main theoretical models of narrative persuasion consider that identification fosters persuasive
impact, since this process inhibits resistance to the persuasive proposal of the message, facilitating
attitudinal impact [
22
,
23
]. However, the empirical evidence in this regard is inconclusive [
28
].
Furthermore, since identification constitutes a process of temporal involvement with the message [
26
], it
is also possible that it will increase the cognitive elaboration during the reception of the message [
29
].
In this context, it is assumed that identification can facilitate persuasive impact through
three cognitive processes that would act as secondary mediating mechanisms. Consistent with the
Entertainment Overcoming Resistance Model (EORM) [
22
], identification can be expected to reduce
counterarguing (the production of critical cognitive responses that refute the content of the message; [
30
])
and reactance (negative reactions to the message when perceiving that freedom of choice or opinion is
being threatened [
31
]). Furthermore, it may be expected that identification will be associated with an
increase in cognitive elaboration (reflecting on the topic of the message during its processing) [29,32].
However, to date, the role of counterarguing, reactance and cognitive elaboration has not been analyzed
simultaneously within a single mediational model, so our study constitutes an original contribution to
this field. Therefore, our work tries to contrast the role of these three cognitive mechanisms to explain
the indirect effect of narrative voice on measures related to the prevention of smoking, leading to the
following mediational hypothesis (Figure 1):
Figure 1. Hypothesized serial–parallel mediation model (H2).
Hypothesis H2:
The indirect effect of narrative voice on the perceived effectiveness of the message, on the
intention to quit smoking, on the self-efficacy expectations and on the response efficacy expectations will be
serially mediated by the identification with the protagonist and by the cognitive processes of counterarguing
(H2a), reactance (H2b) and cognitive elaboration (H2c) (Figure 1).
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Design and Participants Subsection
An online experiment with a 2 (narrative voice)
×
2 (message) between-subjects factorial design
was carried out. The participants were randomly assigned to two experimental conditions such that
half read a narrative message written in the first person whereas the other half a message in the third
person. Furthermore, to increase the external validity of the study, two different messages were used
within each condition [33,34].
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 7281 4 of 15
The online experiment was carried out using QUALTRICS to access an initial sample of 568
adult smokers. Of these, 43 were removed for failing the manipulation test (correctly remembering
which type of message they had read: first or third person). The remaining sample consisted of 525
participants (50.9% women). Participant age ranged from 18 to 55 years old (M=35.27, SD =10.97)
(see Table 1).
Table 1. Characteristics of the study participants (n=525).
Mean (SD) or Percentage Range
Age M=35.27 18–55
SD =10.97
Sex Male: 258 (49.1%)
Female: 267 (50.9%)
Fagerström test M=4.65 0–10
SD =2.23
The applied questionnaire comprised three blocks: pre-test measures, reading of the narrative
message (experimental manipulation) and post-test measures. Sociodemographic information was
collected in the pre-test measures, and three screener questions were included to select the participants,
such that only those who declared that they were current smokers, indicated having smoked more than
100 cigarettes during their life and had smoked 5 or more cigarettes every day during the previous
week, were included. These eligibility criteria were used with reference to previous studies [
6
,
35
,
36
].
Moreover, the degree of tobacco dependence of the participants was also evaluated using the Fagerström
test [
37
], revealing a moderate dependence on this substance (M=4.65, SD =2.23, Md =5.00, on a
scale with a theoretical range from 0 to 10). At the end of this block of questions, the participants
were randomized to the experimental conditions, after which the post-test measures on the mediating
processes (identification with the protagonist, counterarguing, reactance and cognitive elaboration) and
the dependent variables (perceived effectiveness of the message, intention to quit smoking, self-efficacy
expectations and response efficacy expectations) were presented.
QUALTRICS allows the implementation of a series of quality controls. The questionnaire was
designed in such a way that it could only be completed in a single session. In addition, only those
participants who took 6–45 min to complete the questionnaire (M=10.95 min, SD =4.99), took between
60 and 420 s to read the narrative (M=112.88, SD =53.58) and correctly answered an attention check
question included in the questionnaire were counted as valid cases.
2.2. Independent Variable and Stimulus Materials
Taking as examples the testimonies of former smokers on forums and web pages as well as
narratives used in previous studies [
35
], a written narrative delivered by a 45-year-old woman who
indicated that she had quit smoking more than a year ago was constructed (narrative messages
are available in the Supplementary Materials file online, Supplementary File S1). In her story, the
protagonist alluded to topics such as the age at which she started smoking, why she considered quitting
smoking (after having being diagnosed with periodontitis) and how she managed to quit tobacco,
commenting in the final part of the narrative that she no longer wanted to start smoking again, that
she had noted how the negative consequences associated with tobacco use had disappeared and that
she was experiencing a number of benefits from quitting smoking.
In order to manipulate the narrative voice, the elements that mark the grammatical person in the
written narratives were modified, such as the choice of personal pronouns [15,20]. In addition, in the
first-person narrative, the protagonist introduced herself with her own voice and with her name at the
beginning (“My name is Teresa, and I am 45 years old. I have been a smoker for 20 years but have not
smoked for over a year.”) In contrast, in the third-person narrative, an external observer introduced
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 7281 5 of 15
the protagonist and told the story (“Teresa is 45 years old. She has been a smoker for 20 years but has
not smoked for more than a year.”)
The two versions of the narrative message were identical in all other aspects of their content.
Moreover, within each experimental condition (first versus third person), two different messages were
used, differing from each other only regarding the information provided on the number of attempts the
protagonist had made before quitting smoking. Thus, the messages emphasized that the protagonist
had quit smoking on her first attempt (e.g., “She had not tried to quit before; this was her first time”)
or fourth attempt (e.g., “She had already tried to quit smoking before, three times”). This subtle
modification to the text allowed us to include more than one narrative message per experimental
condition and thereby increase the external validity of the study, considering that people are often
exposed to testimonies from former smokers with different profiles.
The four narratives used in the experiment had a similar length (between 425 and 428 words),
which is the most common length used in this type of work [
38
]. A pilot study in which 105 people
participated (67.6% women, M=42.05 years, SD =12.39) was carried out. The results showed that the
designed narratives were perceived as clear and easy to understand, credible, interesting and realistic,
with no statistically significant differences being observed between the four versions.
2.3. Measures
Identification with the protagonist. Identification was assessed using an 11-item scale [
27
] that
measures the degree of identification with a specific character (e.g., “I felt as if I were Teresa”; from
1=not at all to 5 =very much; α=0.93, M=3.60, SD =0.84).
Counterarguing. A scale consisting of three items created from the counterarguing scale [
28
] was
used (e.g., “While reading the story, I thought that I did not agree with some of the things said by
Teresa”; from 1 =strongly disagree to 7 =strongly agree; α=0.73, M=2.72, SD =1.26).
Reactance. This was evaluated using the perceived threat to freedom scale [39] consisting of four
items (e.g., “The message tried to manipulate me”; from 1 =strongly disagree to 7 =strongly agree;
α=0.83, M=2.64, SD =1.37).
Cognitive elaboration. An adapted version of the cognitive elaboration scale [
29
] was used, consisting
of three items (“While reading the narrative, I reflected intensely on the topic of tobacco use and its
consequences”; from 1 =strongly disagree to 7 =strongly agree; α=0.85, M=5.33, SD =1.26).
Perceived effectiveness of the message. This was evaluated using a scale constructed from previous
work [
6
,
10
,
40
] and composed of four items (e.g., “the message was convincing”; from 1 =strongly
disagree to 7 =strongly agree; α=0.86, M=5.27, SD =1.14).
Intention to quit smoking. This was evaluated using a scale created from previous studies [
5
,
6
,
35
]
and composed of three items (e.g., “it is very likely that I will quit smoking in the next 3 months”;
from 1 =strongly disagree to 7 =strongly agree; α=0.84, M=4.72, SD =1.46).
Self-efficacy. To measure expectations of self-efficacy, that is, the confidence of the participants
in abstaining from smoking, a scale constructed from previous studies [
41
,
42
] and consisting of six
items was used (e.g., “I think I have the capacity to stop smoking when I want to”; from 1 =strongly
disagree to 7 =strongly agree; α=0.87, M=4.78, SD =1.25).
Response efficacy. The expectations of efficacy of the preventive response (quitting smoking) were
measured using a scale [
25
] comprising five items (e.g., “a life without tobacco reduces the risk of
health problems”; from 1 =strongly disagree to 7 =strongly agree;
α
=0.85, M=5.91, SD =0.97)
(see Table 2).
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 7281 6 of 15
Table 2. Key measures.
Measure Response Options Reliability (Cronbach’s alpha)
Identification with the protagonist 1 (not at all)–5 (very much) 0.93
•I felt emotionally involved with Teresa’s feelings
•I felt as if I were Teresa
•I imagined how I would act if I were Teresa
•I was concerned about what was happening
to Teresa
•I understood how Teresa acts, thinks and feels
•
I experienced Teresa’s emotional reactions myself
•I tried to imagine Teresa’s feelings, thoughts
and reactions
•I had the impression of living Teresa’s
story myself
•I understood Teresa’s feelings or emotions
•I tried to see things from Teresa’s point of view
•I identified with Teresa
Counterarguing 1 (strongly disagree)–7 (strongly agree) 0.73
•While reading the story, I thought that I did not
agree with some of the things said by Teresa
•While reading the story, I thought that the
information given by Teresa was inaccurate,
misleading or exaggerated
•While reading the story, I tried to determine
whether there were errors in Teresa’s conclusions
on some topics
Reactance 1 (strongly disagree)–7 (strongly agree) 0.83
•The message threatened my freedom of choice
•The message tried to make a decision for me
•The message tried to manipulate me
•The message tried to pressure me
Cognitive elaboration 1 (strongly disagree)–7 (strongly agree) 0.85
•While reading the narrative, I reflected intensely
on the issue of tobacco use and its consequences
•As I progressed through the narrative, I tried to
draw conclusions to adjust my views on tobacco
•Reading the message has made me think deeply
about what a life without tobacco would be like
Perceived effectiveness of the message 1 (strongly disagree)–7 (strongly agree) 0.86
•The message was believable
•The message was convincing
•This message has been very important to me
•Reading this message helped me feel more
confident about dealing with tobacco
•Reading the message, I have been concerned
about my smoking habit
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 7281 7 of 15
Table 2. Cont.
Measure Response Options Reliability (Cronbach’s alpha)
Intention to quit smoking 1 (strongly disagree)–7 (strongly agree) 0.84
•I’m thinking I’m going to make an effort to
quit smoking
•
It is very likely that I will quit smoking in the next
3 months
•I will definitely quit smoking in the future
Self-efficacy 1 (strongly disagree)–7 (strongly agree) 0.87
•I think I have the ability to quit smoking when I
want to
•I’m sure I can quit smoking
•I know what I should do to quit smoking
•If I quit smoking and someone offered me a
cigarette, I would know how to resist and would
not smoke
•If I quit smoking and attended a party with
friends or family, I would know how to act in
order not to smoke
•
If I have already decided not to smoke again, I am
sure I would not take a cigarette, even if I felt sad
or anxious
Response efficacy 1 (strongly disagree)–7 (strongly agree) 0.85
•I am convinced that, if I stop smoking, my health
will improve shortly thereafter
•I am sure that, if I stop smoking, my body will
soon recover from the harmful effects of tobacco
•I am convinced that, if I stop smoking, it will
decrease my risk of serious illnesses in the future
•
Even if you have been smoking for many years, it
is possible to become healthy again if you stop
smoking in time
•A life without tobacco reduces the risk of
health problems
2.4. Statistical Analysis
Data analyses were conducted using SPSS version 25, statistical software (IBM Company, Armonk,
NY, USA). Descriptive analysis (means and standard deviations) were calculated to examine sample
demographics (see Table 1). Reliability (Cronbach’s alpha) was calculated for all the measures (see
Table 2). The correlations between the mediating and dependent variables were analyzed by using the
Pearson correlation coefficient. One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and chi-squared test were
used to test the success of randomization. Factorial ANOVA was performed to determine the impact of
narrative voice on identification with the protagonist (H1), including the type of message as a second
independent variable. Effect size in ANOVA test was calculated using partial eta-squared (partial
η2
);
for nonsignificant results (p-values higher than 0.05), effect size was substituted by observed power (or
post hoc power), as recommended by many statisticians (e.g., [
43
]; but also see [
44
]). To test hypothesis
2, the PROCESS macro (version 3.5) for SPSS developed by Hayes was used [
45
]. This macro makes it
possible to test different mediational models based on the bootstrapping technique. According to the
bootstrapping method, an indirect effect is statistically significant if the confidence interval established
(CI at 95%) does not include the value 0. If the value 0 is included in the CI, the indirect effect is equal
to 0, that is, there is no association between the variables considered.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 7281 8 of 15
3. Results
3.1. Preliminary Analysis
Randomization was successful: the conditions did not differ significantly on gender (
χ2
(3,
n=525) =0.25, p=0.969), age (F(3, 521) =0.32, p=0.809) or the degree of tobacco dependence (F(3,
521) =0.67, p=0.571).
The correlations between the mediating variables and the dependent variables were also analyzed.
This analysis confirmed that the mediating processes showed convergent correlations with the proposed
hypotheses (for example, between identification and counterarguing). In addition, it was also verified
that the mediating processes showed statistically significant relationships with the dependent variables.
These results justify the proposed mediational model (see Table 3).
Table 3. Descriptive analysis and correlations between mediating and dependent variables.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1 Identification - - - - - - - -
2 Counterarguing −0.29 *** - - - - - - -
3 Reactance −0.21 *** 0.43 *** - - - - - -
4 Cognitive elaboration 0.71 *** −0.25 *** −0.15 *** - - - - -
5 Perceived message effectiveness 0.77 *** −0.35 *** −0.24 *** 0.74 *** - - - -
6 Intention to quit smoking 0.54 *** −0.17 *** −0.12 ** 0.59 *** 0.60 *** - - -
7 Self-efficacy 0.11 ** 0.03 0.01 0.15 *** 0.17 *** 0.36 *** - -
8 Response efficacy 0.41 *** −0.22 *** −0.22 *** 0.43 *** 0.47 *** 0.58 *** 0.21 *** -
Mean 3.60 2.72 2.64 5.33 5.27 4.72 4.78 5.91
Standard deviation 0.84 1.26 1.37 1.26 1.14 1.46 1.25 0.97
Note. n=525. For all the variables, a higher score indicates a greater intensity of the considered process, from 1
for “low” to 7 to “high” (except for the identification scale, which has a theoretical range from 1 for “low” to 5 for
“high”). ** p<0.01, *** p<0.001.
3.2. Effect of Narrative Voice on Identification with the Protagonist (H1)
Analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed that narrative voice significantly influenced the
identification with the protagonist (F
narrative voice
(1, 521) =6.59, p
=
0.011, partial
η2
=0.013), while
no statistically significant effects were observed for the type of message (F
message
(1, 521) =2.16,
p=0.142, observer power =0.31), nor interaction effects between narrative voice and message type
(F
narrative voice ×message
(1, 521) =0.07, p=0.781, observed power =0.05). The results showed that people
who read the narrative in the first person experienced greater identification with the protagonist
(M=3.69, SD =0.81) than those who were exposed to the narrative in the third person (M=3.51,
SD =0.85), thus confirming H1.
3.3. Testing a Serial-Parallel, Mediation Model (H2)
To test the second hypothesis, the PROCESS macro for SPSS (model 81, with 10,000 bootstrapping
samples to generate 95% confidence intervals by the percentile method) was used [
45
]. The independent
variable (narrative voice) was coded as a dummy variable (first-person message =1, third-person
message =0), and the message-type variable was included in the analysis as a covariate. This
procedure allowed the evaluation of the specific indirect effect of the experimental condition on
the dependent variables through identification (as primary mediator) and cognitive processes (as
secondary mediators).
It was observed that the narrative voice in the first person increased identification (B=0.18,
SE =0.07, p=0.010), and this in turn was associated with less counterarguing (B=
−
0.44, SE =0.07,
p=0.000) and lower reactance (B=
−
0.34, SE =0.07, p=0.000), as well as greater cognitive elaboration
(B=1.08, SE =0.04, p=0.000). Lesser counterarguing was associated with higher perceived effectiveness
of the message (B=-0.08, SE =0.02, p=0.000) and higher response efficacy expectations (B=
−
0.13,
SE =0.03, p=0.000). Reactance was not statistically significantly associated with any of the dependent
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 7281 9 of 15
variables. In contrast, cognitive elaboration showed positive and statistically significant associations
with the perceived effectiveness of the message (B=0.35, SE =0.03, p=0.000), the intention to quit
smoking (B=0.48, SE =0.05, p=0.000), self-efficacy expectations (B=0.15, SE =0.06, p=0.015) and
response efficacy (B=0.21, SE =0.04, p=0.000) (Figure 2).
Figure 2. Cont.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 7281 10 of 15
Figure 2.
Results of the mediation analysis (H2). The figures show the non-standardized regression
coefficients, B. The coefficients of the direct effects appear in parentheses. The dashed line represents
nonsignificant coefficients. +p<0.10, * p<0.05, ** p<0.01, *** p<0.001. (
a
) Dependent variable:
perceived message effectiveness. (
b
) Dependent variable: intention to quit. (
c
) Dependent variable:
self-efficacy. (d) Dependent variable: response efficacy.
Three statistically significant specific indirect effects of narrative voice were observed through
identification on the perceived effectiveness of the message (Effect =0.1154, SE =0.0474, 95% CI (0.0271,
0.2119)), the intention to quit smoking (Effect =0.0796, SE=0.0376, 95% CI (0.0153, 0.1617)) and the
response efficacy expectations (Effect =0.0317, SE =0.0187, 95% CI (0.0023, 0.0740)) (Table 3). In
addition, two specific indirect effects were also observed, through the serial mediation of identification
and counterarguing, on the perceived effectiveness of the message (Effect =0.0074, SE =0.0042, 95% CI
(0.0009, 0.0171)) and the response efficacy expectations (Effect =0.0114, SE =0.0061, 95% CI (0.0020,
0.0255)), thus providing partial support to H2a. However, no significant specific indirect effects were
observed through the serial mediation of identification and reactance on any of the dependent variables,
which implies a rejection of H2b. Finally, four specific indirect effects were observed, through the serial
mediation of identification and cognitive elaboration, on the perceived effectiveness of the message
(Effect =0.0725, SE=0.0300, 95% CI (0.0167, 0.1348)), the intention to quit smoking (Effect =0.0982, SE=
0.0414, 95% CI (0.0230, 0.1849)), self-efficacy expectations (Effect =0.0312, SE=0.0203, 95% CI ( 0.0004,
0.0776)) and response efficacy (Effect =0.0439, SE=0.0225, 95% CI (0.0080, 0.0954)), thus H2c receives
empirical support (see Table 4).
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 7281 11 of 15
Table 4.
Specific indirect effects of narrative voice on perceived message effectiveness, intention to quit smoking, self-efficacy and response efficacy through
identification and cognitive processes (H2). Mediation models with PROCESS.
(a) Dependent variable: perceived message effectiveness
Specific indirect effects (mediators) Effect Boot SE Boot 95% CI
Narrative voice →Identification →Perceived message effectiveness 0.1154 0.0474 [0.0271, 0.2119]
Narrative voice →Counterarguing →Perceived message effectiveness −0.0028 0.0101 [−0.0241, 0.0169]
Narrative voice →Reactance →Perceived message effectiveness −0.0018 0.0058 [−0.0147, 0.0092]
Narrative voice →Cognitive elaboration →Perceived message effectiveness −0.0384 0.0275 [−0.0925, 0.0168]
Narrative voice →Identification →Counterarguing →Perceived message effectiveness (H2a) 0.0074 0.0042 [0.0009, 0.0171]
Narrative voice →Identification →Reactance →Perceived message effectiveness (H2b) 0.0028 0.0023 [−0.0002, 0.0085]
Narrative voice
→
Identification
→
Cognitive elaboration
→
Perceived message effectiveness (H2c)
0.0725 0.0300 [0.0167, 0.1348]
(b) Dependent variable: intention to quit smoking
Specific indirect effects (mediators) Effect Boot SE Boot 95% CI
Narrative voice →Identification →Intention to quit smoking 0.0796 0.0376 [0.0153, 0.1617]
Narrative voice →Counterarguing →Intention to quit smoking 0.0002 0.0053 [−0.0102, 0.0126]
Narrative voice →Reactance →Intention to quit smoking −0.0007 0.0060 [−0.0143, 0.0120]
Narrative voice →Cognitive elaboration →Intention to quit smoking −0.0520 0.0390 [−0.1334, 0.0209]
Narrative voice →Identification →Counterarguing →Intention to quit smoking (H2a) −0.0006 0.0044 [−0.0095, 0.0087]
Narrative voice →Identification →Reactance →Intention to quit smoking (H2b) 0.0010 0.0034 [−0.0056, 0.0085]
Narrative voice →Identification →Cognitive elaboration →Intention to quit smoking (H2c) 0.0082 0.0414 [0.0230, 0.1849]
(c) Dependent variable: self-efficacy
Specific indirect effects (mediators) Effect Boot SE Boot 95% CI
Narrative voice →Identification →Self-efficacy 0.0079 0.0226 [−0.0374, 0.0557]
Narrative voice →Counterarguing →Self-efficacy 0.0024 0.0097 [−0.0161, 0.0245]
Narrative voice →Reactance →Self-efficacy 0.0005 0.0063 [−0.0124, 0.0147]
Narrative voice →Cognitive elaboration →Self-efficacy −0.0165 0.0156 [−0.0536, 0.0062]
Narrative voice →Identification →Counterarguing →Self-efficacy (H2a) −0.0063 0.0056 [−0.0200, 0.0019]
Narrative voice →Identification →Reactance →Self-efficacy (H2b) −0.0008 0.0035 [−0.0081, 0.0065]
Narrative voice →Identification →Cognitive elaboration →Self-efficacy (H2c) 0.0032 0.0203 [0.0004, 0.0776]
(d) Dependent variable: response efficacy
Specific indirect effects (mediators) Effect Boot SE Boot 95% CI
Narrative voice →Identification →Response efficacy 0.0317 0.0187 [0.0023, 0.0740]
Narrative voice →Counterarguing →Response efficacy −0.0043 0.0148 [−0.0330, 0.0268]
Narrative voice →Reactance →Response efficacy −0.0024 0.0077 [−0.0205, 0.0117]
Narrative voice →Cognitive elaboration →Response efficacy −0.0232 0.0175 [−0.0599, 0.0098]
Narrative voice →Identification →Counterarguing →Response efficacy (H2a) 0.0114 0.0061 [0.0020, 0.0025]
Narrative voice →Identification →Reactance →Response efficacy (H2b) 0.0036 0.0028 [−0.0002, 0.0106]
Narrative voice →Identification →Cognitive elaboration →Response efficacy (H2c) 0.0439 0.0225 [0.0080, 0.0954]
Note. Narrative voice (independent variable) was dummy coded (0 =third-person narrative, 1 =first-person narrative). We used 95% percentile bootstrap confidence intervals based on
10,000 bootstrap samples for statistical inference of the conditional indirect effects. A specific indirect effect is considered to be statistically significant if the established confidence interval
(95% CI) does not include the value 0. If the value 0 is included in the confidence interval, the specific indirect effect is equal to 0, that is, there is no association between the variables
involved [45]. Significant specific indirect effects are shown in bold.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 7281 12 of 15
4. Discussion
The results of the present experiment shed light on the mechanisms of narrative persuasion and
highlighted the relevance of narrative voice for increasing the effectiveness of tobacco prevention
interventions. In particular, the present work makes a significant contribution in this field and clarifies
two important questions.
First, it is verified that it is possible to increase the identification with the protagonist by
manipulating a formal mechanism in the message: the first-person narrative voice. Research in
the field of narrative persuasion to analyze the effect of narrative voice has not received sufficient
attention, despite its being a very relevant formal device for the design of narrative messages on
tobacco prevention [
20
]. Thus, in the review presented by de Graaf et al. [
19
], from a total of 153
experimental studies on narrative persuasion related to health, only 4 that manipulated this attribute
of the message were identified, and only 1 of them focused on smoking prevention. Furthermore,
previous studies on the effect of narrative voice have not provided significant evidence in favor of
messages written in the first person [
15
,
17
,
20
,
25
] or found that the effects were conditional (that is,
other additional “ingredients” were needed in the message for the effect to occur [
13
]). For all these
reasons, our work makes a significant contribution to the study of the effect of narrative voice in the
context of smoking prevention.
Secondly, a serial mediation process is demonstrated, providing new knowledge about the
relationship between identification and the cognitive processes of counterarguing and cognitive
elaboration. On the one hand, through reduced counterarguing, the participants who identified
more with the protagonist showed a more favorable reaction to the message and manifested a greater
perceived efficacy of the preventive response (considering that quitting smoking would improve
their personal health in the short and long term). This result is convergent with the postulates of the
EORM model [
22
] and with the Extended Elaboration Likelihood Model [
23
] as well as some previous
empirical studies [
28
,
46
,
47
]. In this way, our study reinforces the idea that the experience of fusion
with the protagonist of the message becomes a process that is incompatible with a state of negative
assessment (that would hinder prevention). However, it should be considered that reactance did not
act as a significant mediating mechanism, despite its negative correlation with identification (r=
−
0.21,
p=0.000) and with cognitive elaboration (r=- 0.15, p=0.000), and a positive, strong and statistically
significant correlation with counterarguing (r=0.43, p=0.000).
However, our research reveals that identification, which is conceived as a state of temporal
involvement [
26
], is also associated with greater cognitive elaboration and that this reflective process
increases the persuasive efficacy of the message. It should be noted that the role of cognitive elaboration
in narrative persuasion processes has been less widely investigated to date compared with the role of
message resistance processes (such as counterarguing or reactance) [
48
]. Therefore, our work makes
a significant contribution to understanding the role of cognitive processes in smoking prevention
using testimonial messages or personal narratives. Thus, it is verified that identification can stimulate
dual cognitive processing, so that both processes (the reduction of counterarguing and the increase in
elaboration) can act in tandem to achieve a persuasive impact through testimonial narrative messages.
The most important limitation of this work is that the proposed mediators were measured rather
than being experimentally manipulated, which prevents conclusions with total certainty regarding the
proposed causal sequence (identification
→
counterarguing
→
outcomes; identification
→
reactance
→
outcomes; identification
→
cognitive elaboration
→
outcomes). This problem is present in studies that
contrast serial mediational models in this field [
5
,
36
]. Although temporal precedence is an important
element to establish a causal inference, it is also necessary to propose a theoretical argument for
the relationship between the mediating mechanisms, a condition that our work fulfils by relying on
theoretical models of narrative persuasion. Indeed, future research should use other methodological
approaches to deal with such causal inference problems [49].
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 7281 13 of 15
5. Conclusions
The results of the present study lead us to raise two issues with important theoretical implications.
First, identification with a positive role model who is the protagonist of a testimonial message designed
to prevent smoking makes it difficult to produce negative responses to the message. Indeed, it is
perhaps difficult to counterargue against the biography of a person who reports having successfully
quit tobacco. Secondly, the experience narrated by this positive role model in the message can inspire
and stimulate deep cognitive processing in people, so that they question and adjust their opinions
about tobacco.
Beyond the theoretical implications, our work shows that certain characteristics of testimonial
messages aimed at smoking prevention play a primary role in increasing their persuasive impact. In
this work, it is shown that narrative voice is not only an anecdotal formal choice, but that it indirectly
affects variables related to tobacco prevention, due to the fact that first-person messages activate a
mechanism of affective connection with the message (identification with the protagonist) that reduces
resistance to prevention while simultaneously stimulating reflection.
The results obtained in our research suggest various applications in the field of prevention and
treatment of smoking. We consider that a narrative intervention such as that proposed (through
narrative messages written in the first person, with testimonies of former smokers who relate their
successful experience) could constitute a primary prevention tool, since any attempt to reduce tobacco
consumption indirectly seeks to avoid the development of associated health problems. This type of
testimonial messages (relating the experience of someone who has overcome tobacco addiction) would
thus serve both to prevent smoking (stopping people from starting to use tobacco) and to help active
smokers to quit tobacco, thus avoiding the damage that it may cause to them. In this sense, testimonial
messages of former smokers could be used in prevention campaigns and be disseminated through
social networks (for example, Instagram or Facebook), or as support materials in broader campaigns
disseminated on web pages.
Supplementary Materials:
The following are available online at http://www.mdpi.com/1660-4601/17/19/7281/s1,
File S1: Narratives.
Author Contributions:
Conceptualization, J.-J.I; formal analysis, J-J.I.; funding acquisition, J-J.I.; investigation,
J-J.I and L.R.-C.; methodology, J-J.I. and L.R.-C.; supervision, J-J.I and L.R.-C.; writing—original draft, J-J.I and
L.R.-C.; writing—review and editing, J.-J.I. and L.R.-C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version
of the manuscript.
Funding:
The present study was carried out within the framework of the research project “If you want, you can
quit. Narrative tools for smoking prevention in adults. Effects of similarity to the audience and narrative voice”,
funded by the Board of Education of the Autonomous Government of Castilla and Leon, Spain (reference no.
SA032G18).
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
References
1.
American Cancer Society. Health Risks of Smoking Tobacco. Available online: https://www.cancer.org/
cancer/cancer-causes/tobacco-and-cancer/health-risks-of- smoking-tobacco.html#references (accessed on
18 September 2020).
2.
World Health Organization. Tobacco. Available online: http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs339/en/
(accessed on 18 September 2020).
3.
AECC. Tabaquismo y C
á
ncer En España. Situaci
ó
n Actual [Smoking and Cancer in Spain. Current situation.].
Available online: https://www.aecc.es/sites/default/files/content-file/Informe-tabaquisimo-cancer-20182.pdf
(accessed on 18 September 2020).
4.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. The Health Consequences of Smoking: 50 Years of Progress.
A Report of the Surgeon General. Available online: https://www.hhs.gov/sites/default/files/consequences-
smoking-exec-summary.pdf (accessed on 18 September 2020).
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 7281 14 of 15
5.
Dunlop, S.M.; Wakefield, M.; Kashima, Y. Pathways to persuasion: Cognitive and experiential responses to
health-promoting mass media messages. Commun. Res. 2010,37, 133–164. [CrossRef]
6.
Kim, M. When similarity strikes back: Conditional persuasive effects of character-audience similarity in
anti-smoking campaign. Hum. Commun. Res. 2019,45, 52–77. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
7.
Braverman, J. Testimonials versus informational persuasive messages: The moderating effect of delivery
mode and personal involvement. Commun. Res. 2008,35, 666–694. [CrossRef]
8.
Viswanath, K.; Willington, S.F.; Blake, K.K. Media effects and population health. In The SAGE Handbook of
Media Processes and Effects; Nabi, R.L., Oliver, M.B., Eds.; Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2009; pp. 313–329.
9.
Myrick, J.G. Media effects and health. In Media Effects. Advances in Theory and Research; Oliver, M.B.,
Raney, A.A., Bryant, J., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2019; pp. 308–323.
10.
Kim, M.; Shi, R.; Cappella, J.N. Effect of character–audience similarity on the perceived effectiveness of
antismoking PSAs via engagement. Health Commun. 2016,31, 1193–1204. [CrossRef]
11.
Braddock, K.; Dillard, J.P. Meta-analytic evidence for the persuasive effect of narratives on beliefs, attitudes,
intentions, and behaviors. Commun. Monogr. 2016,83, 446–467. [CrossRef]
12.
Zebregs, S.; Van den Putte, B.; Neijens, P.; de Graaf, A. The differential impact of statistical and narrative
evidence on beliefs, attitude, and intention: A meta-analysis. Health Commun.
2015
,30, 282–289. [CrossRef]
13.
Kaufman, G.F.; Libby, L.K. Changing beliefs and behavior through experience-taking. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.
2012,103, 1–19. [CrossRef]
14.
Van Krieken, K.; Hoeken, H.; Sanders, J. Evoking and measuring identification with narrative characters:
A linguistic cues framework. Front. Psychol. 2017,8, 1–16. [CrossRef]
15.
Banerjee, S.C.; Greene, K. Role of transportation in the persuasion process: Cognitive and affective responses
to antidrug narratives. J. Health Commun. 2012,17, 564–581. [CrossRef]
16.
Chen, M.; Bell, R.A.; Taylor, L.D. Persuasive effects of point of view, protagonist competence, and similarity
in a health narrative about type 2 diabetes. J. Health Commun. 2017,22, 702–712. [CrossRef]
17.
Christy, K.R. I, you, or he: Examining the impact of point of view on narrative persuasion. Media Psychol.
2018,21, 700–718. [CrossRef]
18.
Nan, X.; Dahlstrom, M.F.; Richards, A.; Rangarajan, S. Influence of evidence type and narrative type
on HPV risk perception and intention to obtain the HPV vaccine. Health Commun.
2015
,30, 301–308.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
19.
De Graaf, A.; Sanders, J.; Hoeken, H. Characteristics of narrative interventions and health effects: A review of
the content, form, and context of narratives in health-related narrative persuasion research.
Rev. Commun. Res.
2016,4, 88–131. [CrossRef]
20.
Chen, M.; Bell, R.A.; Taylor, L.D. Narrator point of view and persuasion in health narratives: The role
of protagonist–reader similarity, identification, and self-referencing. J. Health Commun.
2016
,21,
908–918. [CrossRef]
21.
Green, M.C.; Brock, T.C. In the mind’s eye: Transportation-imagery model of narrative persuasion. In Narrative
Impact. Social and Cognitive Foundations; Green, M.C., Strange, J.J., Brock, T.C., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2002; pp. 315–341.
22.
Moyer-Gus
é
, E. Toward a theory of entertainment persuasion: Explaining the persuasive effects of
entertainment-education messages. Commun. Theory 2008,18, 407–425. [CrossRef]
23.
Slater, M.D.; Rouner, D. Entertainment-education and elaboration likelihood: Understanding the processing
of narrative persuasion. Commun. Theory 2002,12, 173–191. [CrossRef]
24.
Segal, E.M.; Miller, G.; Hosenfeld, C.; Mendelsohn, A.; Russell, W.; James, J.; Greene, A.; Joseph, D. Person
and tense in narrative interpretation. Discourse Process. 1997,24, 271–307. [CrossRef]
25.
Chen, M.; McGlone, M.S.; Bell, R.A. Persuasive effects of linguistic agency assignments and point of view in
narrative health messages about colon cancer. J. Health Commun. 2015,20, 977–988. [CrossRef]
26.
Cohen, J. Defining identification: A theoretical look at the identification of audiences with media characters.
Mass Commun. Soc. 2001,4, 245–264. [CrossRef]
27.
Igartua, J.J.; Barrios, I. Changing real-world beliefs with controversial movies: Processes and mechanisms of
narrative persuasion. J. Commun. 2012,62, 514–531. [CrossRef]
28.
Moyer-Gus
é
, E.; Nabi, R.L. Explaining the effects of narrative in an entertainment television program:
Overcoming resistance to persuasion. Hum. Commun. Res. 2010,36, 26–52. [CrossRef]
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020,17, 7281 15 of 15
29.
Igartua, J.J.; Vega, J. Identification with characters, elaboration, and counterarguing in
entertainment-education interventions through audiovisual fiction. J. Health Commun.
2016
,2, 293–300.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
30.
Niederdeppe, J.; Kim, H.K.; Lundell, H.; Fazili, F.; Frazier, B. Beyond counterarguing: Simple elaboration,
complex integration, and counterelaboration in response to variations in narrative focus and sidedness.
J. Commun. 2012,62, 758–777. [CrossRef]
31.
Rains, S.A. The nature of psychological reactance revisited: A meta-analytic review. Hum. Commun. Res.
2013,39, 47–73. [CrossRef]
32.
Petty, R.E.; Cacioppo, J.T. Communication and Persuasion: Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitude Change;
Springer: Wiesbaden, German, 1987.
33.
Reeves, B.; Yeykelis, L.; Cummings, J.J. The use of media in media psychology. Media Psychol.
2017
,19,
49–71. [CrossRef]
34.
Slater, M.; Peter, J.; Valkenburg, P.M. Message variability and heterogeneity: A core challenge for
communication research. Ann. Int. Commun. Assoc. 2015,39, 3–31. [CrossRef]
35.
Kim, H.S.; Bigman, C.A.; Leader, A.E.; Lerman, C.; Cappella, J.N. Narrative health communication and
behavior change: The influence of exemplars in the news on intention to quit smoking. J. Commun.
2012
,62,
473–492. [CrossRef]
36.
Kim, H.K.; Lee, T.K. Conditional effects of gain–loss-framed narratives among current smokers at different
stages of change. J. Health Commun. 2017,22, 990–998. [CrossRef]
37.
Heatherton, T.F.; Kozlowski, L.T.; Frecker, R.C.; Fagerstrom, K.O. The Fagerstrom test for nicotine dependence:
A revision of the Fagerstrom tolerance questionnaire. Br. J. Addict. 1991,86, 1119–1127. [CrossRef]
38.
Dahlstrom, M.F.; Niederdeppe, J.; Gao, L.; Zhu, X. Operational and conceptual trends in narrative persuasion
research: Comparing health-and non-health-related contexts. Int. J. Commun. 2017,11, 4865–4885.
39.
Shen, L. Targeting smokers with empathy appeal antismoking public service announcements: A field
experiment. J. Health Commun. 2015,20, 573–580. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
40.
O’Keefe, D.J. Message pretesting using assessments of expected or perceived persuasiveness: Evidence
about diagnosticity of relative actual persuasiveness. J. Commun. 2018,68, 120–142. [CrossRef]
41.
McQueen, A.; Waters, E.A.; Kaphingst, K.A.; Caburnay, C.A.; Sanders Thompson, V.L.; Boyum, S.;
Kreuter, M.W. Examining interpretations of graphic cigarette warning labels among us youth and adults.
J. Health Commun. 2016,21, 855–867. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
42.
Spek, V.; Lemmens, F.; Chatrou, M.; Kempen, S.; Pouwer, F.; Pop, V. Development of a smoking abstinence
self-efficacy questionnaire. Int. J. Behav. Med. 2013,3, 444–449. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
43.
Onwuegbuzie, A.J.; Leech, N.L. Post hoc power: A concept whose time has come. Understandg. Stat.
2004
,3,
201–230. [CrossRef]
44.
O’Keefe, D.J. Brief report: Post hoc power, observed power, a priori power, retrospective power, prospective
power, achieved power: Sorting out appropriate uses of statistical power analyses. Commun. Methods Meas.
2007,1, 291–299. [CrossRef]
45.
Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis, 2nd ed.; The Guilford Press:
New York, NY, USA, 2018.
46.
Banerjee, S.C.; Greene, K. Examining narrative transportation to anti-alcohol narratives. J. Subst. Use
2013
,
18, 196–210. [CrossRef]
47.
Moyer-Gus
é
, E.; Chung, A.H.; Jain, P. Identification with characters and discussion of taboo topics after
exposure to an entertainment narrative about sexual health. J. Commun. 2011,61, 387–406. [CrossRef]
48.
De Graaf, A.; Van Leeuwen, L. The role of absorption processes in narrative health communication.
In Narrative Absorption; Hakemulder, F., Kuipers, M.M., Tan, E.S., B
á
lint, K., Doicaru, M.M., Eds.; John
Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2018; pp. 271–292.
49.
Pirlott, A.G.; MacKinnon, D.P. Design approaches to experimental mediation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.
2016
,66,
29–38. [CrossRef]
©
2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Supplementary File S1
Narratives
(a) First-person narrative [message type]
My name is Teresa, and I am 45 years old. I have been a smoker for 20 years but have not smoked
for over a year. I have tried to quit smoking before, three times [I have not tried to quit before, this
was my first time].
A little over a year ago, I began to notice a strong pain when chewing and even felt as if my teeth
were moving. Also, my gums were very swollen, and when I brushed my teeth they always bled. I
decided to go to the dentist, and he said that I had periodontitis, better known as pyorrhoea. He
explained that it was not very advanced and that, if I stopped smoking, I could fully recover. The
truth is that I didn’t think it was that serious until he told me that, if I didn’t stop smoking, I could
lose all my teeth.
I will not lie, those were very hard days, with very strange feelings. I remember that during the
first few weeks I had a lot of anxiety, to the point that sometimes I felt like I was drowning. The
worry, nerves and lack of concentration were terrible, but the worst was the sadness, the exhaustion
and the constant crying. I often thought about smoking a cigarette. “One and that’s it,” I said to
myself, but then I remembered that I was sick because of tobacco, and my desire to smoke went away.
After all, this was my fourth attempt to quit smoking [After all, this was my first attempt to quit
smoking]. Everything improved after the third or fourth month. My symptoms had almost
disappeared thanks to the antibiotics, but my dentist recommended that I undergo treatment to
remove the tartar deeper under my gums, to get rid of the disease completely. When I quit smoking,
my body responded perfectly: the inflammation, bleeding and discomfort when chewing
disappeared, my breath no longer smelled bad and my teeth were much whiter. Now even food tastes
much better to me! Also, at the beginning, I bought a piggy bank, and every day I put in what I was
going to spend on tobacco and used it to go out or to buy myself treats; I was spurred on by seeing
how my health improved while I also saved a lot of money.
I was able to quit tobacco after several attempts [I was able to quit tobacco on my first attempt].
Although it was hard, now I can only say that it was worth it. I can no longer imagine puffing on a
cigar, not after seeing how good I feel after quitting. The urge to smoke has disappeared, and my
gums and health have improved. All this makes me not want to smoke again: I won’t let tobacco
harm my health again.
(b) Third-person narrative [message type]
Teresa is 45 years old. She has been a smoker for 20 years but has not smoked for more than a
year. She had already tried to quit smoking before, three times [She had not tried to quit before; this
was her first time].
A little over a year ago, she began to notice a strong pain when chewing and even felt as if her
teeth were moving. Also, her gums were very swollen, and when she brushed her teeth they always
bled. Teresa decided to go to the dentist, who told her that she had periodontitis, better known as
pyorrhoea. He explained that it was not very advanced and that, if she stopped smoking, she could
fully recover. The truth is that Teresa did not think it was so serious until she told him that, if she did
not stop smoking, she could lose all her teeth.
Those were very hard days for Teresa, with very strange feelings. For the first few weeks, she
was very anxious, to the point that sometimes she felt like she was drowning. The worry, nerves and
lack of concentration were terrible, but the worst was the sadness, the exhaustion and the constant
crying. On many occasions she thought about smoking a cigarette. “One and that’s it,” she told
herself, but then she remembered that she was ill because of tobacco, and her desire to smoke went
away. After all, this was her fourth attempt to quit smoking [After all, this was her first attempt to
quit smoking]. Everything improved from the third or fourth month. The symptoms had almost
disappeared thanks to the antibiotics, but her dentist recommended that she undergo treatment to
remove the tartar deeper under the gums, to get rid of the disease completely. When she quit
smoking, her body responded perfectly: the inflammation, bleeding and discomfort when chewing
disappeared, her breath no longer smelled bad and her teeth were much whiter. Now even food tastes
much better to her! In addition, at the beginning, she bought a piggy bank and every day put in what
she was going to spend on tobacco and used it to go out or buy herself treats; This spurred her on as
she saw how her health improved while at the same time saving a lot of money.
She was able to quit tobacco after several attempts [She was able to quit tobacco on her first
attempt]. Although it was hard, now she can only say that it was worth it. She no longer imagines
puffing on a cigar, not after seeing how good she feels after quitting. Teresa’s desire to smoke has
disappeared, and her gums and health have improved. All this makes her not want to smoke again:
she won’t let tobacco harm her health again.