ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Air traffic controller workload is considered to be a limiting factor for further air traffic growth. To reduce workload, increased automation levels and novel decision-support tools are being investigated. This Paper describes the adaptation and evaluation of a previously developed interface, called the Solution Space Diagram, in a route merging task. It portrays both constrained and unconstrained speed and heading combinations and enables the controller, by means of direct manipulation, to safely vector aircraft. The authors hypothesized that this interface enables controllers to use it in their own preferred way, supporting their skills and strategies, reducing their workload. A preliminary experiment was conducted in which 12 participants, grouped according to expertise level, controlled a sector and were faced with different levels of traffic in a route merging task. Results show that the interface aids in finding merging solutions faster; a significant reduction in the number of commands and in perceived workload was observed. The participants changed their strategy to perform less vectoring and issue route interceptions at an earlier stage, without affecting aircraft separation. These changes were also observed with the professional controllers, although they showed to be more conservative to the use of the diagram. This Paper justifies experimentation with a larger number of participants and in a setup of higher operational realism.
Solution Space Decision Support for Reducing
Controller Workload in a Route Merging Task
G. A. Mercado Velasco*, C. Borst, M. M. van Paassen, M. Mulder§
Faculty of Aerospace Engineering, Delft University of Technology,
2600 GB Delft, The Netherlands
Air trac controller workload is considered to be a limiting factor for further air
trac growth. To reduce workload, increased automation levels and novel decision-
support tools are being investigated. This paper describes the adaptation and eval-
uation of a previously-developed interface, called the “Solution Space Diagram”, in
a route merging task. It portrays both constrained and unconstrained speed and
heading combinations and enables the controller, by means of direct manipulation,
to safely vector aircraft. We hypothesized that this interface enables controllers to
use it in their own preferred way, supporting their skills and strategies, reducing
their workload. A preliminary experiment was conducted in which twelve partici-
pants, grouped according to expertise level, controlled a sector and were faced with
dierent levels of trac in a route merging task. Results show that the interface
aids in finding merging solutions faster: a significant reduction in the number of
commands and in perceived workload was observed. Our participants changed
their strategy to perform less vectoring and issue route interceptions at an earlier
stage, without aecting aircraft separation. These changes were also observed with
the professional controllers, although they showed to be more conservative to the
use of the diagram. This study justifies experimentation with a larger number of
participants and in a setup of higher operational realism.
I. Introduction
Predicted air trac growth and pressing economic and environmental concerns are forcing a
fundamental redesign of the air trac management (ATM) system, to increase airspace capacity
*Research Associate, Control and Simulation; g.a.mercadovelasco@tudelft.nl
Assistant Professor, Control and Simulation; c.borst@tudelft.nl
Associate Professor, Control and Simulation; m.m.vanpaassen@tudelft.nl
§Professor, Control and Simulation; m.mulder@tudelft.nl. Associate Fellow AIAA
1
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
PLEASE REFER TO THIS PAPER AS
G. A. Mercado-Velasco, C. Borst, M. M. Van Paassen, and M. Mulder, “Solution Space Decision Support for
Reducing Controller Workload in Route Merging Task,” Journal of Aircraft, vol. 58, no. 1, pp. 125–137, 2021.
with higher levels of flight eciency and operational safety. Air trac controller workload is
one of the main factors that limits the safe and expeditious growth of air transport [1, 2, 3]. Un-
derstanding how task complexities (e.g., airspace organization) and decision-support tools (e.g.,
automation, including the human-machine interface) can aect controller workload is one of the
main research questions addressed in current ATM modernization eorts [4, 5, 6].
Hilburn and Jorna [7] stated that controller workload depends on several system factors as well
as on operator factors, see Figure 1. Whereas many studies are addressing workload by manipulat-
ing the task demand load [8] by, for example, clever dynamic sectorization [9] and airspace design
(e.g., change existing routing structures and sector geometries) [10], other studies investigate ways
to exploit new technologies that better support controllers in their tasks [11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 15, 16,
17, 18]. Especially the latter is most challenging in the Air Trac Control (ATC) domain, [19]
given the many failed attempts in the past (see Ref. [20] for an extensive overview).
According to Westin et al. [20], controller acceptance plays a critical role in embracing new
technologies, where acceptance is driven by how much the support tool conforms or “matches”
with the skills and strategies of humans. Research on cockpit automation, amongst others, high-
lights also several other factors, such as the reliability, ease of use, false alarm rates, intuitiveness
vs. opaqueness, that play a role in an operator’s trust and acceptance of automation [21].
A successful decision support tool that can keep workload at acceptable levels would be one
that not only allows controllers to cope with task complexities, but also takes into account their
skills, experience and strategies, see Figure 1. In contrast, a support tool that works against a
controller will likely lead to more cognitive load in trying to understand what it is doing and why,
increasing workload. Thus the challenge is how to design a decision support tool that leverages
human and automation abilities, whilst accounting for the controllers’ skills and strategies. To
tackle this challenge, two research paths can be distinguished.
On the one hand, decision support tools that provide conflict resolution advisories have hinted
to be eective in reducing workload [22, 23]. Some initiatives (for a survey of many studies, see
Ref. [20]) have incorporated human heuristics as an “externalized mental library” that can be used
to provide solution recommendations after an algorithm has identified a potential conflict. Since
the suggested solutions might follow the controller’s expectation, it allows her to infer why the so-
2
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
Taskload
Workload
System
dynamics
Interface
demands
Task
complexity
etc.
Skill
Strategy
Experience
etc.
System factors Operator factors
Figure 1. Factors aecting controller workload (adapted from Hilburn and Jorna, [7]).
lution was proposed, which ultimately increases the acceptance of the support tool. Nevertheless,
it is challenging to identify a strategy that is acceptable to all controllers in scenarios where non-
homogeneous problem-solving strategies are being used [24]. Tailoring the advisories to controller
preferences may lead to a conservative system, reinforcing sub-optimal strategies. And by provid-
ing a limited set of possible solutions, the system may hinder users in exploring other alternatives
and preclude the full exploitation of the human unique problem-solving capabilities. Under-using
these capabilities can ultimately lead to complacency, over-trust, and skill loss.
On the other hand, support tools that show the constraints imposed by the work domain, often
in the form of “no-go zones” [25], can have a positive eect on reducing workload, while avoiding
the adverse eects of complacency. Since operators are encouraged to think through multiple
possibilities and are not confined to focus on a limited subset of relevant information, they can
remain purposely engaged in problem-solving, and complacency-related issues are likely to be
avoided [26]. The problem, however, lies in the complexity that results from integrating several
variables to present an overview of all possible solutions to the user.
The Highly Interactive Problem Solver (HIPS), for example, developed by Eurocontrol in the
1990’s, made use of no-go zones in three decision support tools that displayed conflict situations
relative to one selected aircraft, to help the controller find solutions to these conflicts without
presenting explicit resolution advisories. [27, 28] These three tools were projections of conflict
situations on horizontal, altitude, and time/speed displays. The altitude display is now known as
the Level Assessment Display (the LAD, currently implemented in the London Area Control man-
agement system,[29, 30]) but the other two displays were not adopted as initially designed.
Possibly, the way in which information was provided in the three displays resulted in a concep-
3
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
tual and usability issue. In the face of a trac conflict, controllers had to decide for an instruction
that might be a change in altitude, speed, heading, or a combination of these. If users must acquire,
process, and integrate all the information needed to provide a resolution from dierent information
displays, they might not be able to comprehend the system status directly. More information dis-
plays mean more possible data relationships that need to be adequately understood, or the operator
may not always be capable (e.g., in high workload situations) to perceive the direct connection
with the physical system (the airspace, aircraft, routes, etc.). Some evaluations of HIPS identified
that controllers needed more explanation to understand that they were working with constraints
and not trajectories. [27, 28, 31] Nonetheless, the successful adoption of one of the HIPS displays,
i.e., the LAD, is a merit of the design philosophy, and shows the importance of the integration of
information in a way that is transparent and easy to access.
This study evaluates the capabilities of a tool also based on the no-go zones principle, called
the Solution Space Diagram (SSD), to assess whether its potential as tactical support tool could
motivate further research eorts. The SSD provides a visualization of all conflict-free velocity
vectors within the performance envelope of a selected aircraft. [32]. It integrates several sources
of information to present no-go zones in a velocity display. By allowing direct manipulation, the
controller can use the SSD to formulate and implement speed and heading clearances that avoid or
resolve separation conflicts. The SSD does not present advisories to controllers (although it can do
so as well, see [33]), but rather shows controllers all possible solutions, supporting their individual
skills, strategies and preferences, which may positively impact their acceptance.
Previous studies in ATC [34, 35, 36] have evaluated the capabilities of the SSD concept for
estimating metrics for airspace complexity and controller workload. Several SSD-based interfaces,
in two and three dimensions, have emerged for ATC [37, 38] and some have been successfully
evaluated as a decision-support tool for pilots, in self-separation tasks [39, 40, 41]. Recently, the
basic, state-based, SSD has been evaluated in studies focusing on basic ATC conflict detection
and resolution (CD&R). [42, 43] Using a novel intent-based SSD as a decision-support tool in the
air trac controller’s merging task has not been reported before, and its eects on workload and
performance are yet unknown. These eects may not all be positive; the SSD integrates several
sources of information to depict the full “solution space”, including intent, in support of CD&R,
4
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
but in the merging task its novel presentation could induce more workload than it alleviates.
The research presented in this paper is a first investigation of the capabilities of the SSD to
support controllers in a route merging task. The main purpose was to assess the diagram’s eects
on controller workload and merging performance. A secondary purpose was to evaluate whether
the SSD allows controllers, with dierent levels of expertise, to use the tool in their own preferred
way. An analysis of the impact of the SSD on the dierent controllers’ control strategies could
provide unique insights, possibly explaining any observed changes in workload.
The evaluation of the SSD as ATC interface is performed in a medium-fidelity simulator in
which controllers performed two-dimensional CD&R in the context of a spacing and route merging
task. Our study does not benchmark the tool’s performance with other support interfaces, and does
not evaluate the eects that could be measured after a prolongued use of the tool, such us overtrust,
complacency, or skill loss. Controller acceptance will also not be a part of the current evaluation,
but is likely to become a major dependent measure when the evaluation is successful, allowing for
a larger group of (experienced) controllers to be invited.
In this context, a series of human-in-the-loop simulations were performed, in which participants
with three dierent levels of expertise in ATC (ranging from university students to experienced air
trac controllers) performed an aircraft merging task, with and without the help of the SSD. The
main hypothesis was that, because the SSD makes the limitations imposed by the work domain
visible to the operator, she can in turn directly perceive the “space of solutions”, leading to a lower
workload. Second, the evaluation investigated whether the SSD is able to support the control strate-
gies adopted by the three groups of controllers. Third, we investigated whether showing the SSD
led to changes in these control strategies which could account for the observed changes in work-
load. The analysis of the eects on workload is performed with instantaneous self-assessments,
measured throughout the simulation runs, and also using quantitative performance-related metrics.
The paper is structured as follows. Section II describes the rationale of the SSD and motivates
its use as a decision aid. The simulator used in the evaluation is discussed in Section III, followed
by a description of the experiment in Section IV. Results are described in Section V, followed by
an extensive discussion in Section VI; the paper ends with conclusions in Section VII.
5
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
II. The solution space diagram as a decision aid
A. Construction of the solution space diagram
Having its foundations in the Velocity Obstacle theory [44], the solution space diagram (SSD) is a
two-dimensional representation that covers all heading/speed combinations possible for a specific
aircraft, indicating which velocity vectors oer safe solutions and which velocity vectors lead to a
loss of separation with another aircraft [32]. A description of the classical VO theory in its simplest
form (two vehicles following two-dimensional rectilinear trajectories at constant speed) provides
sucient background information for understanding the construction of the diagram.
VOA|B
PZB
pA
pB
vA
vB
vB
vrel
a) Velocity Obstacle for aircraft A(the controlled
aircraft), imposed by aircraft B(VOA|B).
VOA|B
Vmin
Vmax
vA
vB
b) Solution space diagram for aircraft A.
Figure 2. Basic solution space construction.
A Velocity Obstacle (VO) is defined as the set of all velocity vectors of a moving vehicle that
will result in a collision (or a loss of separation) with a moving obstacle at some moment in time,
assuming that the moving obstacle maintains a constant velocity vector (adapted from Ref. [45]).
Consider a controlled vehicle A, at position pA, and an observed (moving) obstacle B, at posi-
tion pB, with circular protected zone PZB, see Figure 2a. If a ray starting at pA, coinciding with the
relative velocity vector of Awith respect to B(vrel =vA-vB) intersects PZB, then vAis in the Veloc-
ity Obstacle of vehicle A, imposed by obstacle B(adapted from Ref. [46]). This velocity obstacle,
denoted VOA|B, represents the set of velocities of vehicle A, that will result in a loss of separation
with obstacle B. Figure 2a shows graphically how VOA|Bcan be derived. A relative velocity cone
can be constructed from the trac geometry, with its edges originating in pA, and tangent to PZB.
VOA|Bis then determined by adding the velocity of Bto the relative velocity cone. Such cone can
then be drawn in the velocity plane of a solution space diagram, as depicted in Figure 2b. Note
6
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
that the large and small circles represent the aircraft performance limits, i.e., the maximum and
minimum speed, respectively, of the aircraft under control.
The SSD for the selected aircraft Ashows which combinations of heading and speed will lead
to a loss of separation with B, the shaded cone. Vice versa, it also shows which heading and
speed combinations are ‘free’ to select, when there is a reason to change the vehicle’s heading or
speed, such as in vectoring the aircraft as done by ATC. In the situation illustrated in Figure 2b the
selected aircraft Ais flying close to its minimum velocity (the end-point of the vector vAlies close
to the minimum velocity circle), and the aircraft will ‘pass behind’ the faster aircraft B. The reader
is referred to Refs. [44, 47] for a more detailed discussion of the SSD elements and use, and to
Refs. [32, 40, 48] for the theoretical underpinnings of our displays.
When intended trajectories are not rectilinear paths, or contain speed changes, the calculation of
the VO is not as straightforward. Whereas D’Engelbronner et al. [35] proposed an approximation,
Mercado et al. [44] derived a closed mathematical form for calculating VOs that take intended
trajectory changes (in piecewise or continuous form) into account. Figure 3 illustrates the eects
that such intent information induces on the VO, and, therefore, on the SSD. This latter, intent-
based SSD is the subject of the current paper. Note that what would initially be regarded as a
conflict under the classical VO theory (Figure 3b), is no longer a conflict when intent information
is incorporated (Figure 3c).
B
A
a) Aircraft Bsharing intent infor-
mation with A.
vA
b) SSD for Awithout the intent in-
formation of B.
vA
c) SSD for Awith the intent infor-
mation of B.
Figure 3. Eects of including intent information on the SSD.
For trac situations that include more than two aircraft, the SSD for a controlled aircraft must
contain the VOs induced by all other observed aircraft that are close. Figure 4 shows an example
trac situation with the corresponding SSD, drawn for one particular aircraft that is under control
of the ATCo. In this example, the aircraft under control (A) is surrounded by two other aircraft (B
and C). Two conflict zones are shown, and since aircraft Band Cmerge on the same trajectory,
7
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
perpendicular to the controlled aircraft’s flight, the conflict zones point to the same origin. This is
because, after the merge, aircraft Band Chave the same velocity vector, i.e., the same direction
and speed. Finally, note that the SSD shows the situation for just one aircraft; when another aircraft
is selected, like Bor C, the picture will generally look very dierent.
B
C
A
a) Trac situation with aircraft Band Csharing
their intent information with A.
Vmin
Vmax
vA
b) Solution space diagram for aircraft A.
Figure 4. The SSD for a multi-aircraft scenario.
B. Decision support
The SSD could be seen as a “what-if” aid for tactical CD&R. However, it diers from some other
tools of this sort, since it does not force the operator out of the loop for projecting and analyzing
user- (or computer-) initiated hypothetical situations until a satisfactory solution is found. Instead,
it makes use of no-go zones to communicate whether the selected aircraft’s current or tentative
velocity vector would conflict with the trajectories that the aircraft nearby intend to fly, leaving
the planning task (the basis of the strategy to undertake) completely up to the operator. With the
implementation presented in this study, the controller remains fully responsible for the decision-
making and action implementation tasks.
Leaving all decision-making in the hands of the operator would also mean that the operator
can develop her or his own preferred strategy to perform the task, such as the merging task in
this study, with or without the help of the SSD. For instance, operators with little experience
in doing the task could build their expertise and rely more upon the SSD, as it simplifies
the information collection and integration stages needed to understand the trac situation under
8
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
control. In contrast, highly experienced controllers, who have learned to perform the merging task
without the help of any tool, can be assumed to already have the required expertise, and would
rely less on the SSD but could use it as a tool that could “confirm” their view on the situation and
the expected eects of their control actions. Tentatively, one can expect that the SSD will have
dierent eects on the dierent control strategies by experienced and less-experienced controllers,
but its success could lie in the fact that it supports them all in doing their job in each individual’s
preferred way.
Nevertheless, supporting information analysis comes with a risk. Providing the user with more
information requires additional cognitive eort for its processing, and increased visual display de-
mands are known to also possibly induce an increment in workload [49]. The authors expected
the SSD not to take the controller out of the loop in a way that would demand more cognitive re-
sources, however, the eects that the diagram can have on controller workload need to be carefully
studied. For this purpose, and to study other elements of SSD usage in the future (such as operator
acceptance), a medium-fidelity simulator has been developed, described in the following section.
III. Simulator setup
The main hypothesis explored in this paper is that presenting the SSD to support CD&R and
merging tasks in the horizontal plane reduces controller workload. A medium-fidelity simulator
was developed (in Matlab) to test this central hypothesis. Figure 5 shows a screenshot of the
simulator layout. Note that the coloring and font sizes have been changed so that the image could
be printed out with more clarity.
The simulator included a Plan View Display (PVD) of the airspace on the left-hand side
(marked by 1) and the SSD at the top right (marked by 2). In some scenarios, the VOs of
the SSD were shown, in others they were hidden, but in both cases, the SSD interface served as
direct manipulation interface for heading and speed commands. At the lower right, the interface
provided a set of virtual buttons (marked by 3) that the participants could use to give instructions
to the aircraft within the sector (sector colored in white). These buttons served as “shortcuts” for
Direct-to and Intercept clearances. A mouse was used for all interactions.
9
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
Figure 5. Experimental simulator screenshot.
Participants were expected to interact with the simulator in the following way. They had to
select the aircraft they were interested in controlling by clicking on it, and the SSD would be
updated to contain all the VOs that all other aircraft were imposing on the selected aircraft at that
moment. The SSD was then updated every second until the aircraft was deselected, or another
aircraft was selected. While an aircraft remained selected, the participant could click on the SSD
to change the aircraft’s velocity vector, having a clear picture of which velocity vectors would or
would not create conflicts. Later, the participant could decide to merge the aircraft with the given
velocity vector by clicking on the Intercept button. Alternatively, the participant could observe if
changing the heading towards a particular waypoint was a conflict-free possibility or not, and then
possibly decide to click on one of the Direct-to buttons.
In scenarios in which no support was provided, the way of interacting with the simulator was
similar; the single dierence was that VOs were not displayed at all. That is, when selecting an
aircraft, the participant could still click on the SSD interface to change the aircraft’s velocity vector,
or use the Direct-to buttons, but no VOs were shown and the operator had to judge herself whether
the control actions were valid.
The setup explained here contains certain elements that may drive the experiment away from
10
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
the reality of the ATC task it was meant to emulate. The simplification has the benefit, however,
of reducing training time and learning eects. Since the objective of this study is to evaluate a
tool meant to support merging and separation in the horizontal plane, the simulator did not allow
participants to issue altitude changes. Furthermore, they did not have to maintain flight strips, deal
with aircraft deviating from their prescribed trajectories, or having to communicate verbally.
A. Airspace and aircraft
The airspace presented contained the sector to be controlled, inside which aircraft could receive
commands. Although also the aircraft surrounding the sector were shown, they could only be con-
trolled when they entered the sector. The route to which aircraft were to be merged was indicated
on the PVD, together with the route points’ names (see Figure 5). The last point of the route
(HOOKS) served as the sector’s exit point.
Two types of aircraft were present in the simulation, “heavy” and “light”, with speed ranges
160-250 knots and 120-250 knots, respectively. Dierent sizes of aircraft icons were displayed on
the PVD to provide the notion of dierent aircraft types.
Dierent colors were used for the aircraft symbols. When a loss of separation would take place,
the aircraft involved turned red. After a Direct-to or Intercept Route command was issued, the color
of the data tag would turn from yellow to green. In other words, a yellow tag meant that the aircraft
has still not received a route merging instruction; a green tag implied that the aircraft did receive
a merging instruction. The selected aircraft would also show a circle around it, representing the 5
NM separation radius assumed for this research. The aircraft tags contained the aircraft callsign,
the current and target speed, and the current and target heading.
B. The solution space diagram implementation
Two important visual dynamic elements were provided in the SSD: target and current velocity
vectors (to give the notion of the transition between states), and color-coded conflict areas (VOs).
Dierent tones of blue were used for VOs that corresponded to aircraft that had already received
merging instructions, and dierent tones of gray for the VOs that corresponded to aircraft that still
had not received such directions.
11
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
This color-coding was adopted to allow the controller to be more selective on which VOs to
consider when issuing merging clearances. The SSD can become almost entirely covered in high
trac scenarios and, without the color coding, the complex lumped area that results from all the
VOs may leave the controller with very few choices and possibly lead her or him to refrain from
using the aid. With the color coding, the controller can consider vector commands that would
result in conflicts with other aircraft that have not yet received merging instructions because these
other aircraft would eventually receive additional instructions as well. Note that several properties
of a VO (e.g., its orientation in the SSD, the location of its tip and the width of the VO) enable a
controller to link the dierent VOs to corresponding aircraft on the PVD.
The SSD also acted as direct manipulation interface for the controller. By a mouse click inside
the minimum and maximum speed circles, the corresponding vector command was issued to the
selected aircraft. In conditions when the VOs were not shown, the same command interface was
used. This interface setup was selected because other types of input would add an extra task to
the controller. It is of great importance to maintain interface and equipment demands low, since
the main task of the controller is to keep the “mental picture” of the trac situation, and, already
by including the diagram, a momentary diversion of the controller’s attention from the PVD is
introduced. Separate buttons were used to enter route intercept and direct to waypoint commands.
C. Route intercept buttons
The interface included four additional buttons in the lower right corner, see Figure 5. One of these
was the Intercept Route button and the other three were Direct-to buttons for dierent waypoints.
These could be used to direct aircraft either to intercept the route (i.e., maintain current heading
and speed until the route is crossed, then continue along the route) or to fly to a particular route
point and from then on stay on the route. When one of these buttons was clicked, the selected
aircraft tag would turn from yellow to green, to indicate that that particular aircraft had received
merging instructions.
12
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
D. Instantaneous Self Assessment
Every decision a controller takes has its consequences in the future, which means that the task,
and in fact the experiment as a whole, has a dynamic nature. The workload experienced by the
controller will vary in time, and each controller will experience a dierent workload. Hence, when
workload is measured, it must be done at various points in time (see also Ref. [35]).
From the many dierent developed techniques for subjective workload determination, the In-
stantaneous Self-Assessment (ISA) method is one of the simplest tools with which an estimate
of perceived workload can be obtained during real-time simulations or actual tasks [50]. This
method requires the operator to give a discrete rating between 1 (very low) and 5 (very high) of the
workload she/he perceives, either verbally or by using a keyboard.
This method has shown to be highly correlated with the NASA Task Load Index and other
workload measures [51]. It is also easy to implement and has low intrusiveness. It was hence
considered to be well suited for the current experiment. Every 60 seconds, a red blinking message
displayed on the PVD (in an area not taken up by the visualization of trac) requested the con-
troller to subjectively rate his/her workload by pressing the relevant number key on the keyboard.
IV. Experiment
A. Experiment goal
The capabilities of the SSD as decision support tool in the tactical CD&R and merging task were
explored through a series of human-in-the-loop simulations, including participants with three levels
of ATC expertise. The main goal of the experiment was to determine whether presenting the SSD
VOs to controllers can alleviate their workload when faced with the task of merging aircraft into a
single route, by giving heading and velocity commands. Additionally, to facilitate an analysis of
changes in the adopted control strategies of the three participant groups, the eects of the SSD on
performance indicators such as the number (and type) of issued commands, aircraft in the sector,
and separation violations were investigated.
13
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
B. Experiment Design
Two independent variables were present in the experiment, each with two levels: the solution space
display (On/O) and trac(Low/High). When the SSD was “On”, all the VOs imposed by aircraft
close to the controlled aircraft were shown, as illustrated in Figure 5. Participants could control
the aircraft using the SSD interface, as discussed in the previous section. When the SSD display
was “O”, the interaction functionality remained the same, but the VOs were not shown. The
dierences in trac levels are explained later on.
A mixed-design was used. Participants belonged to a specific population group (between-
subjects variable) and performed once in every experimental condition (within-subjects variable).
The sequence of the experimental runs was established by a Latin Square design for a four treat-
ment experiment to counterbalance carryover eects such as practice and fatigue. Table 1 shows
these sequences.
Table 1. Orthogonal Latin square design for four treatments.
Sequence Treatment
1234
1 A B C D
2 B A D C
3 C D A B
4 D C B A
5 A D B C
6 B C A D
7 C B D A
8 D A C B
9 A C D B
10 B D C A
11 C A B D
12 D B A C
It is evident from Table 1 that twelve (or a multiple of twelve) participants are needed to have
a balanced design for a four treatment experiment. Each of the twelve participants was randomly
assigned to a sequence from Table 1.
C. Scenarios
1. Simulation speed and time
In an attempt to gather more information on the dynamics of events, avoid under-achievement from
the controllers, and following the procedure of previous experiments [34, 35], a faster-than-real-
time simulation was run, i.e., four times as fast as real-time. With this simulation speed, every
14
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
scenario ran for 20 minutes of real time (80 minutes of simulated time), with some break between
scenarios.
2. Trac conditions
Incoming aircraft streams were located at three fixed locations outside the sector. As depicted in
Figure 5, aircraft came in from either the northwest, northeast, or southwest. The sector geometry
and trac routing structure are representative for a realistic en-route sector, except that all aircraft
fly at the same altitude. A new aircraft was created at one of these fixed locations (randomly
selected) every 200 seconds for the low trac condition, and every 150 seconds for the high trac
condition, expressed in simulated time. These aircraft influx rates also represent realistic values
for en-route sectors.
The initial trac condition was identical in scenarios of same trac level, showing 11 aircraft
in the low-intensity condition and 13 in the high-intensity condition. Preliminary tests showed
that these number of aircraft were appropriate to rapidly achieve a steady number of aircraft in the
sector, thus reducing the duration of transition eects.
3. Solution space prediction time and geometry of the sector
Sector and route geometry were kept constant throughout all scenarios. With this geometry setup,
and depending on the trac condition, the time an aircraft would stay inside the sector would on
average be around six or eight minutes, real time.
Previous research on SSD properties concluded that in simulations running four times as fast as
real time, operators would try to plan ahead the development of events approximately ten minutes
in real time [35].
With these reasons in mind, the displayed SSD was calculated considering a trajectory predic-
tion with a horizon of ten minutes (real time). This explains the rounded shape in the origin of the
VO cone in Figure 5, as the true origin of the VO angle point lies at infinite time. [44]
15
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
D. Participants and instructions
Twelve participants performed in the experiment. Four of them were professional air trac con-
trollers, four others had recently received an extensive, multiple-day, hands-on ATC instruction
course (facilitated by the Netherlands Aerospace Center (NLR)), and the final four were aerospace
engineering graduate students from Delft University of Technology. These three populations are
from now on referred to as ATCo’s, Experts and Students, respectively.
The ATCo’s ages ranged from 35 to 61 years (µ=49.25, σ=11.96), the Experts’ ages ranged
between 27 and 47 (µ=36.75, σ=9.32), and the Students’ ages ranged between 24 and 26
(µ=25.00, σ=1.16). The limited availability of subjects, especially in the ATCO group, did not
allow us to balance-out the experiment for age.
Participants were briefed on the experiment goal and on how they should interact with the
interface. They were instructed to merge all aircraft onto the single route, without being able to
make altitude changes, trying to avoid separation violations at all costs, and have all aircraft exit
the sector at 180 kts (to better emulate reality and prevent the excessive use of maximum speed
commands). There were no minimum performance thresholds set for the participants, as this turned
out to be unnecessary in pre-experimental checks.
Participants performed two training scenarios, during which they were able to get familiarized
with the interface. The purpose was to avoid learning eects during the experiment as much as
possible. Every training scenario lasted for ten minutes. The first scenario had low-intensity trac
and did not present the SSD so that participants could get familiarized with the simulator and focus
on learning how to control aircraft. The second scenario oered a higher level of trac intensity,
with the SSD available, and aimed to have participants learn how to interact with the SSD and how
to interpret it.
Given that the simulation was limited and all subjects had basic to expert knowledge in air
trac control, training time was considered sucient. After participants had confirmed their un-
derstanding of the procedure and the use of the ISA workload rating, four full scenarios were
performed.
16
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
E. Dependent measures
After each experimental run, participants had the opportunity to provide open comments.
During the experimental runs, besides the ISA ratings of workload performed every minute,
other variables were monitored in order to get more insight in the participants’ performance and
control strategy:
1. Number of Commands. Every click in either the solution space interface, or one of the
command buttons, was counted as one command. Despite the fact that a click on the solution
space interface can have the eect of giving two commands (heading and speed changes),
we assume that one click does not represent a cognitive eort worth of two commands.
2. Aircraft Count. The median number of aircraft inside the sector (i.e., not in the entire visu-
alized airspace), measured every second (simulated time).
3. Loss of Separation Count, defined as the number of times the distance between two aircraft
was smaller than 5 NM.
4. Extra Distance Ratio: For each aircraft, the most ecient trajectory would be a straight line
from the point at which the aircraft enters the sector to HOOKS (the minimum distance). Any
other trajectory adds to this distance. This dependent measure was calculated as the ratio
between the additional flown distance and the minimum distance.
5. Smallest Aircraft Separation. The median value of the smallest separation between all flights
during their passing through the sector.
6. Sector Time. The median value of the time aircraft needed to transit the sector.
7. Handling Time. The median value of time it took the controller to issue the final merging
command to every aircraft after their entry into the sector.
All dependent measures, except for Number of Commands and Loss of Separation Count, were
analyzed with a central tendency measure to provide a single measure per scenario and participant.
Especially with this experimental setup, in which the presence of learning eects is a possibility,
the occurrence of situations in which a flight requires significantly more vectoring than normal is
17
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
likely. These situations could result in outlier values, and to rule out these eects, the median was
preferred over the mean as central tendency measure.
F. Hypotheses
The main hypothesis was that showing the SSD’s VO’s to our participants would contribute to a
reduction of their experienced workload, irrespective of trac levels and operational expertise.
In case the VO’s were presented on the SSD, these were expected to aid participants in finding
“empty spaces” in the aircraft sequence. It would allow them to merge trac faster and easier,
compared to the situation where the participants had to predict the future trac flow by themselves.
Showing the VO’s was expected to result in participants to direct aircraft more towards the sector
exit (leading to a reduction in the Sector Time, Handling Time, and Extra Distance Ratio measures)
and to issue fewer clearances (reduction in the Number of Commands measure). As a consequence,
aircraft are expected to be flying closer together (reduction in the Smallest Separation measure)
when the VOs are presented.
V. Results
Three types of analyses were conducted on the data collected in the experiment. The perfor-
mance metrics analysis studied the dependent measures discussed in Subsection IV.E. This was
followed by a workload analysis based on ISA ratings and, finally, a control strategy analysis that
sought to identify the controller adaptions to the independent variables.
The population group variable showed varying levels of statistical significance in all three anal-
yses. In the performance and workload analyses, a non-parametric test (Kruskal-Wallis) was first
used to test for dierences between groups. Since all scenarios showed no significant dierences,
all participants were evaluated as a single population group. The control strategy analysis, on
the other side, was based on a series of multinomial logistic regressions that showed significant
dierences across population groups. A more elaborate discussion is provided next.
18
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
A. Performance metrics analysis
Every population group studied in this experiment contained four participants. Being a small
sample size, a non parametric test (Kruskal-Wallis) was first used to test for dierences across
population groups. For every dependent measure analyzed, results showed that, in all scenarios,
non-significant dierences were present across population groups. Although dierences in con-
trol strategy between the population groups were to be expected, we can conclude that the sample
size was not big enough for detecting significant dierences between these groups. Therefore, the
population group variable was disregarded in the performance metrics analysis, making the popu-
lation size of the single analysis group (twelve participants) large enough for parametric statistical
methods.
The population size used in this experiment provided enough power for detecting large eect
sizes (r=0.5) of the dependent measures. To detect medium (r=0.3) or small (r=0.1) eect
sizes, a population size of 32 or 274, respectively, would have been required.a
Dependent measures that satisfied the normality and homogeneity of variance assumptions
(Number of Commands, Aircraft Count, Extra Distance Ratio, Smallest Separation, Sector Time,
and Handling Time) were studied with a parametric hypothesis test (repeated measures ANOVA).
The rest of the dependent measures (Loss of Separation Count) were studied with a non-parametric
test (Friedman’s ANOVA and pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni correction). Boxplots of all
dependent measures are shown in Figure 6.
Table 2 provides the significance levels obtained with the factorial analyses. Significant eects
of Trac on all dependent measures are evident. Interestingly, Table 2 shows significant eects of
the SSD only on the ISA Ratings of Workload (discussed later on in Subsection V.B), the Number
of Commands, and Handling Time variables.
1. Number of Commands
There were significant main eects of Trac and the SSD on the Number of Commands (Ncom)
that were issued. Irrespective of other predictors, the increase of trac led to an averaged increase
aCalculated with the G*Power software, version 3.1.9.2. http://www.gpower.hhu.de/en.html.Accessed on March
1st, 2015. Inputs: α=0.05, β=0.2.
19
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
Low Tr. High Tr.
80
120
160
200
[-]
SSD O
SSD On
a) Number of commands.
Low Tr. High Tr.
6
8
10
12
14
[-]
SSD O
SSD On
b) Aircraft count.
Low Tr. High Tr.
0
2
4
6
8
10
[-]
SSD O
SSD On
c) Loss of separation
count.
Low Tr. High Tr.
0
0.2
0.4
0.6
0.8
[-]
SSD O
SSD On
d) Extra distance ratio.
Low Tr. High Tr.
6
7
8
9
[NM]
SSD O
SSD On
e) Smallest separation.
Low Tr. High Tr.
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
[min]
SSD O
SSD On
f) Sector time.
Low Tr. High Tr.
0
2
4
6
8
10
12
14
16
18
[min]
SSD O
SSD On
g) Handling time.
Low Tr. High Tr.
250
500
750
1,000
1,250
Sum of Ranks
SSD O
SSD On
h) ISA ratings of work-
load.
Figure 6. Boxplots for all dependent measures.
Table 2. Dependent measures’ ANOVA (parametric and non-parametric) results.
Metrics Factors
SSD T SSD*T
ISA Ratings of Workload *** *** -
Number of Commands *** *** -
Loss of Separation Count - ** N/A
Aircraft Count - *** -
Extra Distance Ratio - *** -
Smallest Separation - *** -
Sector Time - *** -
Handling Time * ** -
Note: SSD =Solution space diagram, T =Trac,
-=non significant, * =p<0.05, ** =p<0.01,
*** =p<0.001.
of 50 commands (F(1,11) =87.65, p<0.001, r=0.89) and the SSD, irrespective of other
predictors, led to an averaged decrease of 17 commands (F(1,11) =23.76, p<0.001, r=0.68).
Note that both Trac and the SSD had a large size eect (r>0.5).
An increase of Ncom at the high trac condition would be naturally expected. The decrease of
Ncom when the SSD aid was displayed was hypothesized prior to the experiment. Several studies
indicate that Aircraft Count and Number of Commands have the highest influence on subjective
ratings of workload [2]. Therefore, a reduction in Ncomwould indicate that the SSD has a potential
20
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
to reduce workload.
2. Handling Time
There were significant main eects of Trac and the SSD on the Handling Time variable. Ir-
respective of other predictors, the increase of trac led to an averaged increase of 2.66 minutes
(simulated time) in handling time (F(1,11) =12.92, p<0.01, r=0.54) and the SSD, irrespective
of other predictors, led to an averaged decrease of 1.57 minutes (simulated time) in handling time
(F(1,11) =5.37, p<0.05, r=0.33).
Note that Trac had a large size eect (r=0.54), and the SSD showed a medium size eect
(r=0.33). These results suggest that the participants were able to merge trac onto the route
sooner with the help of the SSD. This might also explain the reduction in the number of commands
observed when the SSD was displayed.
3. Other dependent measures
All other dependent measures introduced in Subsection IV.E were not aected by the SSD sig-
nificantly, see Table 2. Only the trac level had a significant eect on these measures, with a
higher aircraft count, longer travelled distances, more losses of separation, longer sector times,
and reduced separation between aircraft for the high trac level, as it would be expected.
The SSD was expected to help participants to “find holes” for merging trac in the aircraft
trains more easily (resulting in aircraft flying closer together), and to aid in directing the trac
more towards the sector exit. It was therefore hypothesized that the SSD would induce a reduction
in the Smallest Separation, Sector Time, and Extra Distance Ratio dependent measures. Results
showed, however, no significant influences of the SSD on these metrics.
B. Workload analysis
With 20 ratings per simulated scenario, 12 participants and 4 scenarios per participant, 960 sub-
jective ratings of workload were measured during the entire experiment. These subjective ratings
were all measured in an ordinal scale, i.e., a ranking in terms of degree that has no established
numerical dierence between rankings. For data analysis, an ordinal scale requires a permissible
21
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
transformation that preserves its ordinality [52], and one of them is a rank-based transformation.
For this reason, all 80 subjective ISA ratings provided by every participant (4 scenarios, each hav-
ing 20 ratings) were ranked, and then the sum of ranks for every scenario was calculated, summed
across participants.
Due to the small population size in every participating group, a non parametric test (Kruskal-
Wallis) was first used to test for dierences across population groups. Since all scenarios showed
no significant dierences between groups, all participants were considered to belong to a single
group.
After testing for the relevant parametric assumptions (normality and homogeneity of variance),
a repeated measures ANOVA was performed on this transformed metric. Figure 6h shows a boxplot
for this dependent measure.
Results showed significantly higher ISA ratings with an increase of trac (F(1,11) =118.83,
p<0.001, r=0.92), and significantly lower ratings when the SSD interface was available
(F(1,11) =5.02, p=0.047, r=0.31). Subsection V.A showed similar results for the Num-
ber of Commands metric. Note that eect of size shows that trac had a large eect on the total
variance, while the SSD aid had a medium eect.
C. Strategy analysis
In subsection V.A we showed that Trac and SSD had a significant eect on the number of com-
mands. This section analyzes the type of commands issued to obtain more insight into the strategy
the participants adopted.
Participants had three options for executing their task: clicking on the SSD interface, directing
trac to a specific waypoint, or issuing a route intercept command. It was expected that partici-
pants would only make use of the SSD interface for vectoring aircraft, but their behavior showed
dierently.
Every command the participant issued was classified as either being a “vectoring”, “speed
adjustment”, or “merging” command. Merging commands were identified as the last Direct-to
or Intercept Route command issued to every aircraft. Vectoring commands were identified as all
Direct-to commands issued before the final merge command, and any click on the SSD that would
22
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
produce a change in heading. Finally, any click on the SSD that would produce only a change in
speed but not in heading was identified as a speed adjustment command.
Within the merging commands, the participant had to choose between either a Direct-to or
an Intercept Route command. Further, the choice of a Direct-to command needed to specify one
of the three available waypoints. This setup, in which participants must make choices within a
finite number of possibilities, may be analyzed with a classification method such as the logistic
regression [53, pp. 264–265]. Since more than two possible discrete outcomes were available
to participants, the multinomial logistic regression was the method of choice for studying their
strategy adaptations.
1. Vector, speed adjustment, and merge commands
The coecients of the multinomial logistic regression model are shown in Table 3. As a goodness
of fit indicator, the deviance statistic was calculated as χ2(14) =14.621, p=0.405. Hence, the
null hypothesis of the goodness of fit test (the data follow the specified distribution) can not be
rejected.
Table 3. Multinomial logistic regression coecients for model of issued commands.
95% CI for Odds Ratio
Variable B(SE) Lower Odds Ratio Upper
Vectoring vs. Merging
Intercept 0.11 (0.08) 0.96 1.12 1.31
SSD -0.21 (0.07)** 0.71 0.81 0.93
Trac 0.34 (0.07)*** 1.23 1.41 1.62
Group =Experts 0.21 (0.09)* 1.04 1.23 1.46
Group =ATCo’s 0.35 (0.08)*** 1.20 1.42 1.67
Speed adjustments vs. Merging
Intercept 0.68 (0.07)*** 1.71 1.97 2.28
SSD -0.19 (0.07)** 0.73 0.83 0.94
Trac 0.14 (0.07)* 1.01 1.15 1.31
Group =Experts -0.07 (0.08) 0.80 0.93 1.09
Group =ATCo’s -0.09 (0.08) 0.78 0.92 1.08
Note: * =p<0.05, ** =p<0.01, *** =p<0.001.
Note that the analysis performs a series of comparisons between two categories. Having three
outcome categories (i.e., “Vectoring”, “Merging”, and “Speed adjustment”), the analysis consists
of two comparisons [53, p. 300], in which “Merging” was selected as the baseline category.
The increase of trac had a significant influence on whether a vector (p<0.001) or a speed
adjustment (p<0.05) command was preferred over a merge command. The odds ratio shows that
23
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
the change in odds of vectoring or adjusting speed rather than merging trac was 1.41 and 1.15,
respectively. Participants were therefore more likely to vector or adjust speed than merge flights
when confronted with the high trac level.
Displaying the SSD had a significant influence (p<0.01) on the type of command issued as
well. The odds ratio shows that the change in odds of vectoring or adjusting speed rather than
merging trac was 0.81 and 0.83, respectively. Participants were therefore less likely to vector or
adjust speed than merge flights when the SSD aid was displayed.
Based on the model, the probabilities of issuing all dierent types of commands are shown
in Figure 7. Note that, in this figure, the dashed and full lines represent the low and high trac
conditions, respectively.
SSD OSSD On
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
0.6
Vectoring
Speed adjustment
Merging
a) Student group.
SSD OSSD On
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
0.6
Vectoring
Speed adjustment
Merging
b) Expert group.
SSD OSSD On
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
0.6
Vectoring
Speed adjustment
Merging
c) ATCo group.
Figure 7. Modeled probabilities of either vectoring, adjusting speed, or merging trac.
2. Merge commands
Trac was to be merged with either a Direct-to or Intercept Route command. The coecients of
the multinomial logistic regression model are shown in Table 4. The regression model showed to
have an acceptable goodness of fit indicator (deviance): χ2(5) =6.087, p=0.298.
The increase of trac had a significant influence on the issued merge command (p<0.001).
The odds ratio show that the change in odds of having an aircraft intercept the route rather than
directing it to a specific waypoint was 1.63. Participants were therefore more likely to issue a
Route Intercept command when confronted with high trac.
Displaying the SSD had a significant influence (p<0.05) as well. The odds ratio show that
the change in odds of having an aircraft intercept the route rather than directing it to a specific
24
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
Table 4. Multinomial logistic regression coecients for the model of issued merge commands.
95% CI for Odds Ratio
Variable B(SE) Lower Odds Ratio Upper
Intercept Route vs. Direct-to
Intercept 0.01 (0.15) 0.76 1.01 1.35
SSD -0.45 (0.19)* 0.44 0.64 0.92
Trac 0.49 (0.11)*** 1.30 1.63 2.03
Group =Experts -0.55 (0.19)** 0.40 0.57 0.83
Group =ATCo’s -1.58 (0.21)*** 0.14 0.21 0.31
Group =Experts * SSD -0.13 (0.27) 0.52 0.88 1.49
Group =ATCo’s * SSD 1.18 (0.28)*** 1.87 3.24 5.61
Note: * =p<0.05, ** =p<0.01, *** =p<0.001.
waypoint was 0.64. Participants were therefore less likely to issue a Route Intercept command
when the SSD aid was displayed. This eect, though, was surpassed in size by the interaction with
the ATCo group ( p<0.001). For the ATCo’s, the change in odds of having an aircraft intercept
the route was 3.24. They were therefore much more likely to intercept trac with the SSD aid.
Based on the model, the probabilities of issuing an Intercept Route rather than a Direct-to
command are shown in Figure 8.
SSD OSSD On
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
0.6
0.7High trac
Low trac
a) Students group.
SSD OSSD On
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
0.6
0.7High trac
Low trac
b) Experts group.
SSD OSSD On
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
0.6
0.7High trac
Low trac
c) ATCo’s group.
Figure 8. Modeled probabilities of merging trac with an Intercept Route rather than a Direct-to command.
SSD OSSD On
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
0.6
0.7High trac
Low trac
a) Students group.
SSD OSSD On
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
0.6
0.7High trac
Low trac
b) Experts group.
SSD OSSD On
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
0.6
0.7
High trac
Low trac
c) ATCo’s group.
Figure 9. Modeled probabilities of directing trac to the sector’s exit waypoint.
25
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
3. Directing trac to the sector’s exit
Participants had three waypoints available for directing trac. Choosing the third waypoint was
equivalent to sending aircraft straight to the sector’s exit point, HOOKS, see Figure 5. The regres-
sion model was built to compare the possibilities of directing trac to the first or second waypoints
vs. directing trac to the sector’s exit point; regression coecients are shown in Table 5. The
model showed to have an acceptable goodness of fit (deviance): χ2(4) =1.999, p=0.736.
Table 5. Multinomial logistic regression coecients for the directing strategy model.
95% CI for Odds Ratio
Variable B(SE) Lower Odds Ratio Upper
Directing trac to 1st or 2nd waypoints vs Directing trac to the exit
Intercept -0.90 (0.18)*** 0.29 0.41 0.57
Trac 1.34 (0.23)*** 2.42 3.82 6.04
Group =Students 1.63 (0.31)*** 2.75 5.09 9.44
Group =Experts 0.89 (0.27)** 1.42 2.43 4.15
Group =Students * Trac -0.85 (0.38)* 0.20 0.43 0.90
Group =Experts * Trac -1.08 (0.33)** 0.18 0.34 0.65
Group =Students * SSD -0.03 (0.30) 0.54 0.97 1.73
Group =Experts * SSD 0.53 (0.24)* 1.06 1.70 2.72
Note: * =p<0.05, ** =p<0.01, *** =p<0.001.
The increase of trac had a significant influence on selection of waypoint for directing trac
(p<0.001). With such increase, all population groups were less likely to direct trac to the
sector’s exit. The SSD aid showed to have a significant eect only in the Experts group (p<0.05).
This group was less likely to direct trac to the sector’s exit when the aid was displayed. Based
on the model, the probabilities of directing trac to the sector’s exit are shown in Figure 9.
VI. Discussion
This study intends to shed light on the eects the solution space diagram (SSD) has on work-
load, performance, and control strategy when performing tactical merging and separation tasks. Due
to two major caveats, its results should be interpreted and used with caution.
The medium-fidelity simulation studied in this paper attempted to emulate a limited subset of
common ATC tasks. Participants were not required to keep track of altitude changes because the
goal was to evaluate the potential of the SSD as a tactical CD&R and merging tool in the horizontal
plane. A tool that supports controllers in the vertical plane by showing no-go zones (a design phi-
losophy similar to the SSD) already exists [25] and is being used in operational environments. As
26
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
additional limitations, participants did not have to maintain flight strips, deal with aircraft inad-
vertently deviating from their prescribed trajectories, or having to make verbal communicates. All
these additional tasks would increase the perceived workload. Their exclusion had the purpose of
simplifying the experimental setup and reducing training time and learning eects. Despite this last
consideration, some of the experienced controllers that participated in the experiment mentioned
that they felt they still had not passed over the full learning curve when it came to making use of
the SSD. Future experiments should consider this.
Another point of consideration is the statistical power that was available for the experiment. A
sample size of 12 participants was good enough for detecting large eects of the independent
variables. To detect medium size eects, the current experiment would have needed a total of 32
participants, ideally all professional air trac controllers. This number of highly-skilled profes-
sionals represents a large-scale endeavor, which was not feasible in the current study. Furthermore,
participants represented dierent population groups. The population group variable had no sig-
nificant eect on measures of performance or workload, but did significantly influence control
strategy. With a larger population sample, the impact that the control strategy has on performance
and workload should have noticeable eects.
A. Eects on workload
Several studies state that aircraft count and number of commands correlate with the most important
metrics for workload [2]. It was hypothesized that the SSD would induce the participants to send
aircraft more often towards the sector exit, which in turn would have reduced the average number
of aircraft inside the sector. This study showed that the SSD significantly reduced the number of
commands (large size eect) issued by the participants and the handling time per aircraft (medium
size eect). The perceived workload levels were also influenced by the SSD, showing a medium
sized reduction eect.
After the experimental trials, however, some participants reported that the aid had detrimen-
tal eects on their workload (especially during high trac conditions) because of the momentary
attention diversion required for accessing the information in the diagram (refer to Figure 5). This
must have influenced the Instantaneous Self-Assessments of workload performed during the sim-
27
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
ulation runs, and that would explain the medium size eect on the reduction of workload when
a large size eect on the reduction of the number of commands was observed. Possibly, some
changes in the interface might solve this issue, like overlaying the SSD on the aircraft icons of the
plan view display.
Our results show the potential of the SSD as an information analysis aid.bClamann et al. [55]
and Kaber et al. [49] have shown, through empirical studies, that when automation is applied
to support cognitive functions such as information analysis or decision-making, higher workload
levels are experienced. In contrast, automated support for lower-level sensory and psychomo-
tor functions, such as information acquisition and action implementation, results in better overall
human-machine system performance. Whether these findings hold on a general level, however,
is debated in the literature (see e.g., [56, 57]). Design rules for what levels of automation, in all
its possible dimensions, from information-gathering to decision-making, seem impossible, as all
depends on context, dynamics, operator strategies and tasks. Nevertheless, in our study the SSD
provided a low level of automation in decision-making, and a high level of automation in informa-
tion analysis, resulting in an overall reduction in the workload experienced by the participants.
In this study, participants were responsible for the acquisition of information; they had to mon-
itor all aircraft actively, and whenever they needed additional information for a specific aircraft,
by clicking on it they could have access to the SSD. They were also responsible for the imple-
mentation of actions; once they had decided on a plan of action, they had to issue the instructions
themselves. The SSD implementation seems to have induced an increase of performance (reduction
in the number of issued commands and the time needed for handling every aircraft) even though
information acquisition and action implementation were not automated.
The authors would further argue that the “what-if” analysis which the SSD supports resulted
in the active involvement of the user, and that the way in which the SSD presents information not
only allowed for a reduction in workload, but also helped participants to maintain their situation-
awareness by supporting the comprehension and projection of system status. Further studies that
evaluate the SSD’s influence on situation awareness are needed, however, to investigate this claim.
bAccording to Parasuraman et al. [54], a tool that projects aircraft positions to evaluate for conflicts is an informa-
tion analysis tool.
28
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
B. Eects on control strategy
The SSD had a significant influence on the strategy adopted by the controller. Irrespective of the
trac level, the reduction in the number of issued commands came together with a reduction in
the amount of vectoring and speed adjustments. This gave room to an increase in the number of
merging commands. Such a trend is known to have a positive eect on controller workload [58].
Moreover, merging was performed in dierent ways for the dierent population groups. While
the Students and Experts groups used the aid to reduce (by about 10%) the number of Intercept
Route commands and increase (by the same amount) the Direct-to clearances, the ATCo’s group
did the exact opposite (in the same proportion). However, the ATCo’s group always maintained
their overall preference for issuing Direct-to clearances (always higher than 50%) over Intercept
Route commands. The slight change in preference could be attributed to a trust and/or safety is-
sue. The diagram allowed ATCo’s to provide a merge clearance at an earlier stage, but they might
have been somewhat conservative when issuing a Direct-to clearance due to concerns about the
diagram’s accuracy. Furthermore, with the new information provided by the diagram, they might
have seen waypoints as possible bottlenecks and tried to avoid an excessive use of Direct-to clear-
ances.
In terms of a preference for issuing a Direct-to clearance to the exit waypoint over the other
sector waypoints, no clear trend was observed.
As it would be expected, trac had the eect of changing the controller’s strategy to perform
more vectoring and to issue route interceptions in a later stage in order to ensure separation. The
SSD, however, showed to have induced an opposite eect (less vectoring and earlier route inter-
cepts) irrespective of the trac level, and without significantly influencing separation. Since the
aiding tool did not suggest any conflict resolution commands, we conclude that the change in strat-
egy is most likely because the interface allowed the controller to acquire conflict information that
was otherwise not available.
The SSD had a significant influence in the strategy adopted by the controller. Even though
some participants indicated that it was slightly distracting and that they felt they were still in their
learning curve, important beneficial eects were observed, like less vectoring, earlier route inter-
cept, a reduction in workload and in the number of issued commands. Therefore, further research
29
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
seems justified of the eects the SSD has on controllers once they are more familiar with it.
C. Future research
This paper investigated whether the workload experienced by an ATCo performing a two-dimen-
sional aircraft merging task could be reduced by presenting the SSD. The results observed justify
experimentation with a larger group of experienced controllers in a higher fidelity simulation setup.
Introducing a new tool in Air Trac Control is a tedious eort, however. Case studies of the adop-
tion and adaptation of the User Request Evaluation Tool (URET), for instance, showed dierences
in adopting the tool from center to center, from controller teams to teams, and from controller to
controller. [59]. Other likely directions of future work are as follows.
It was observed during the experimental trials that there might be a trac threshold level above
which the diagram is eective in reducing workload. The existence of a threshold up to which
the diagram remains eective is also a possibility. This indicates that the SSD might show to be
a valuable tool in the area of adaptive automation, in which the allocation of aiding tools, based
on states of the collective human-machine system, has the purpose of reducing the complexity of
the control problem at hand [60]. Preliminary tests in using the SSD as a trigger mechanism [61]
or as a way to predict individual controller actions [62] show promising results, but more work is
needed to substantiate the use of an SSD-based automation trigger mechanism.
Some participants reported that the aiding diagram had a distracting eect in some high traf-
fic situations. Further reducing the eort required for accessing the diagram by, e.g., displaying
the diagram around the aircraft icon on the plan-view display whenever the controller selects the
aircraft, might further enhance the controller’s performance and reduce subjective workload.
The fact that the diagram aects the adopted control strategy is apparent. Evidence suggests
that the learning curve will require a significant degree of controller experience with the SSD
to predict its steady-state impact. Thus, another research direction is possibly the analysis of the
strategies undertaken by participants with more experience using the diagram. Training experi-
enced controllers in the use of the diagram may not be a feasible option, as the mental strategies
they have developed over the years may dominate whatever new strategy that the SSD may make
possible. Training other population groups, or even educating student air trac controllers, and
30
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
analyzing their strategies and performance improvements, is an avenue of further research [43].
Finally, the inclusion of the third dimension in the diagram is an important extension. Since
aircraft separation can also be achieved by changing altitude levels, the reduction of the cognitive
load for two-dimensional conflict resolution may lead to an overall reduction in the level of per-
ceived workload. Yet, it could also be the case that the interpretation of a three-dimensional SSD
demands additional cognitive resources from the controller, resulting in higher workload levels. In
this respect, two design concepts of a two-dimensional diagram that allow discrete altitude (level)
changes have been created [37, 38], but not yet extensively evaluated. Thus, it remains to be seen
whether the additional complexity resulting from including the altitude dimension renders the SSD
useless, or not.
VII. Conclusions
The solution space diagram (SSD) visualizes the functional constraints of an aircraft caused by
the trajectories of other aircraft nearby. A pilot study is presented in which novice and experienced
air trac controllers performed a two-dimensional aircraft merging task, at low and high trac
conditions, with and without the SSD. We investigated the diagram’s potential to reduce controller
workload and its eects on the adopted control strategy.
Results showed that, at both trac levels and irrespective of expertise group, the diagram
eectively reduced the number of commands the participants issued to fulfill the ATC task. Most
likely this reduction was caused by decrements in the time needed for handling every aircraft. The
reduction in the number of commands also reflected a significant trend in the reduction of the
workload levels perceived by the participants.
Observations suggest that participants were able to find a merging solution faster with than
without the SSD. With its use, participants changed their strategy to perform less vectoring and
to issue route intercepts at an earlier stage, without significantly influencing aircraft separation.
These changes were also observed with the professional controllers, even though they showed to
have been more conservative in using the diagram. The results justify experimentation with a larger
group of (experienced) controllers in a higher-fidelity simulation environment.
31
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank the twelve participants of the experiment, and also the review-
ers for their useful feedback on the two drafts of this manuscript.
References
[1] Majumdar, A. and Polak, J., “Estimating capacity of Europe’s airspace using a simulation model of air
trac controller workload, Transportation Research Record, Vol. 1744, 2001, pp. 30–43,
doi:10.3141/1744-05.
[2] Hilburn, B. G., “Cognitive Complexity in Air Trac Control a Literature Review, Tech. Rep. EEC
Note No. 04/04, EUROCONTROL, 2004.
[3] Erzberger, H., Lauderdale, T. A., and Chu, Y. C., “Automated conflict resolution, arrival management,
and weather avoidance for air trac management, in “Proceedings of the Institution of Mechanical
Engineers, Part G: Journal of Aerospace Engineering, Inst. of Mechanical Engineers, 2012, pp. 930–
949,
doi:10.1177/0954410011417347.
[4] “SESAR Concept of Operations,” Tech. rep., Brussels, Belgium, 2007,
doi:DLM-0612-001-02-00.
[5] “Concept of Operations for the Next Generation Air Transportation System, Version 3.2, Report, Joint
Planning and Development Oce, 2011.
[6] “Air Trac Control Decision Support Tool Design and Implementation Handbook,” Report
DOT/FAA/TC-19/37, US Department of Transportation, Federal Aviation Administration, 2019.
[7] Hilburn, B. G. and Jorna, P. G. A. M., “Workload and Air Trac Control,” in Hancock, P. A. and
Desmond, P. A., eds., “Stress, Workload and Fatigue: Theory, Research and Practice, Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, pp. 384–394, 2001,
doi:10.1002/hfm.1009.
32
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
[8] Stassen, H. G., Johannsen, G., and Moray, N., “Internal Representation, Internal Model, Human Per-
formance and Mental Workload,” Automatica, Vol. 26, No. 4, 1990, pp. 811–820,
doi:10.1016/S1474-6670(17)53877-X.
[9] Gra˜
na, M., “Dynamic Airspace Configuration: A Short Review of Computational Approaches, in
Nguyen, N. T., Chbeir, R., Exposito, E., Aniort´
e, P., and Trawi´
nski, B., eds., “Computational Collective
Intelligence, Springer International Publishing, 2019, pp. 486–497,
doi:10.1007/978-3-030-28377-3 40.
[10] “A Proposal for the Future Architecture of the European Airspace,” Report, SESAR Joint Undertaking,
2019,
doi:10.2829/309090.
[11] Prevot, T., Lee, P. U., Smith, N., and Palmer, E. A., “ATC Technologies for Controller-Managed and
Autonomous Flight Operations, AIAA Guidance, Navigation and Control Conference, 15-18 August,
San Francisco, California, USA,
doi:10.2514/6.2005-6043.
[12] Prevot, T., “NextGen technologies for Mid-Term and Far-Term Air Trac Control Operations,” IEEE-
AIAA Digital Avionics Systems Conference,
doi:10.1109/DASC.2009.5347556.
[13] Wong, B. L., Rozzi, S., Boccalatte, A., Gaukrodger, S., Amaldi, P., Fields, B., Loomes, M. J., and Mar-
tin, P., “3D-in-2D Displays for ATC,” Sixth Eurocontrol innovative research workshop &exhibition,
Bretigny, France, pp. 54–69.
[14] Wong, B. L., Rozzi, S., Gaukrodger, S., Boccalatte, A., Amaldi, P., Fields, B., Loomes, M. J., and
Martin, P., “Human-Centred Innovation: Developing 3D-in-2D Displays for ATC, Third International
conference on Research in Air Transportation, Fairfax (VA), USA, pp. 295–302.
[15] Prevot, T., Lee, P. U., Callantine, T., Mercer, J., Homola, J., Smith, N., and Palmer, E. A., “Human-In-
the-Loop Evaluation of NextGen Concepts in the Airspace Operations Laboratory, AIAA Modelling
and Simulation Technologies Conference, 2-5 August, Toronto, Canada,
doi:10.2514/6.2010-7609.
33
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
[16] Prevot, T., Homola, and Mercer, J., “An Integrated Tool Suite for En Route Radar Controllers in
NextGen, 27th International Congress of the Aeronautical Sciences (ICAS).
[17] Prevot, T., Homola, J. R., Martin, L. H., Mercer, J. S., and Cabrall, C. D., “Toward Automated Air
Trac Control—Investigating a Fundamental Paradigm Shift in Human/Systems Interaction, Inter-
national Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, Vol. 2, No. 28, 2012, pp. 77–98,
doi:10.1080/10447318.2012.634756.
[18] Lundberg, J., Johansson, J., Forsell, C., and Josefsson, B., “The Use of Conflict Detection Tools in Air
Trac Management an Unobtrusive Eye Tracking Field Experiment During Controller Competence
Assurance, in “Proceedings of the HCI-Aero Conference, July 30-August 1, Santa Clara (CA), USA,”
, 2014,
doi:10.1145/2669592.2669655.
[19] Bekier, M., Molesworth, B. R., and Williamson, A., “Tipping point: The narrow path between au-
tomation acceptance and rejection in air trac management, Safety Science, Vol. 50, No. 2, 2012, pp.
259–265,
doi:10.1016/j.ssci.2011.08.059.
[20] Westin, C., Borst, C., and Hilburn, B. G., “Strategic Conformance: Overcoming Acceptance Issues of
Decision Aiding Automation?” IEEE Transactions on Human-Machine Systems, Vol. 46, No. 1, 2016,
pp. 41–52,
doi:10.1109/THMS.2015.2482480.
[21] Sarter, N. B. and Woods, D. D., “Cognitive Engineering in Aerospace Application: Pilot Interaction
with Cockpit Automation, Tech. Rep. CR-177617, 1993.
[22] Hilburn, B. G., “Evaluating Human Interaction with Advanced Air Trac Management Automation,
in “RTO Meeting Proceedings,” Research and Technology Organisation, 2002, pp. 1–12.
[23] Brooker, P., “Air Trac Control automation: for humans or people?” Human Factors and Aerospace
Safety, Vol. 5, No. 1, 2005, pp. 23–42.
[24] Kirwan, B., Scaife, R., and Kennedy, R. L., “Investigating complexity factors in UK Air Trac Man-
agement, Human factors and aerospace safety, Vol. 1, No. 2, 2001, pp. 125–144.
34
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
[25] Irfan, M., Bull, M. J., Clinch, A. T., and Pember, S. J., Air Trac Control, U.S. Patent Application
Publication US 2012/0303253 A1, 2012.
[26] Endsley, M. R., “From Here to Autonomy: Lessons Learned from Human-Automation Research,”
Human Factors, Vol. 59, No. 1, 2017, pp. 5–27,
doi:10.1177/0018720816681350.
[27] Jorna, P. G. A. M., Pavet, D., van Blanken, M., and Pichancourt, I., “PHARE Ground Human Machine
Interface (GHMI) project: Summary Report,” Tech. Rep. DOC 99-70-02, EUROCONTROL, 1999.
[28] Whiteley, M. J. and Wilson, I., “PHARE Advanced Tools Problem Solver (Final Report),” Tech. Rep.
DOC 98-70-18, EUROCONTROL, 1999.
[29] NATS, “Validation Report (VALR),” Tech. Rep. Project ID 04.07.08 (D10: Validation Report (VALR)),
SESAR, 2011.
[30] Cort´
es-Obrero, G., “Work as done by Controllers: A Practical Approach in the OPS Room,” Hindsight,
Vol. 25, 2017, pp. 26–29.
[31] Van Gool, M. and Schroter, H., “PHARE final report,” Tech. Rep. DOC 99-70-09, EUROCONTROL,
1999.
[32] Van Dam, S. B. J., Mulder, M., and Van Paassen, M. M., “Ecological Interface Design of a Tactical
Airborne Separation Assistance Tool, IEEE Trans. on Systems, Man &Cybernetics, Part A, Vol. 38,
No. 6, 2008, pp. 1221–1233,
doi:10.1109/TSMCA.2008.2001069.
[33] Borst, C., Bijsterbosch, V. A., van Paassen, M. M., and Mulder, M., “Ecological interface design:
supporting fault diagnosis of automated advice in a supervisory air trac control task, Cognition,
Technology &Work, Vol. 19, No. 4, 2017, pp. 545–560,
doi:10.1007/s10111-017-0438-y.
[34] Hermes, P., Mulder, M., van Paassen, M. M., and Boering, J. H. L., “Solution-Space-Based Analysis
of the Diculty of Aircraft Merging Tasks,” Journal of Aircraft, Vol. 46, No. 6, 2009, pp. 1995–2015,
doi:10.2514/1.42886.
35
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
[35] D’Engelbronner, J. G., Borst, C., Ellerbroek, J., van Paassen, M. M., and Mulder, M., “Solution Space-
Based Analysis of Dynamic Air Trac Controller Workload,” Journal of Aircraft, Vol. 52, No. 4, 2015,
pp. 1146–1160,
doi:10.2514/1.C032847.
[36] Abdul Rahman, S. M., Borst, C., Mulder, M., and Van Paassen, M. M., “Sector Complexity Measures:
A Comparison, Jurnal Teknologi, Vol. 76, No. 11, 2015, pp. 131–139,
doi:10.11113/jt.v76.5923.
[37] Lodder, J., Comans, J., Van Paassen, M. M., and Mulder, M., “Altitude-Extended Solution Space
Diagram for Air Trac Controllers, in “Proc. of the 16th International Symposium on Aviation Psy-
chology (ISAP), Dayton (OH), May 2-5, Wright State University, 2011, pp. 345–350.
[38] Beernink, B., Borst, C., Ellerbroek, J., Van Paassen, M. M., and Mulder, M., “Toward an Integrated
Ecological Plan View Display for Air Trac Controllers, in “Proc. of the 18th International Sym-
posium on Aviation Psychology (ISAP), Dayton (OH), May 4-7,” Wright State University, 2015, pp.
55–60.
[39] Ellerbroek, J., Visser, S. B. J., M. van Dam, Mulder, M., and Van Paassen, M. M., “Design of an
Airborne Three-Dimensional Separation Assistance Display, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man &
Cybernetics, Part A, Vol. 41, No. 5, 2011, pp. 863–875,
doi:10.1109/TSMCA.2010.2093890.
[40] Ellerbroek, J., Brantegem, K. C. R., Van Paassen, M. M., and Mulder, M., “Design of a Coplanar
Airborne Separation Display, IEEE Transactions on Human-Machine Systems, Vol. 43, No. 3, 2013,
pp. 277–289,
doi:10.1109/TSMC.2013.2242888.
[41] Ellerbroek, J., Brantegem, K. C. R., Van Paassen, M. M., de Gelder, N., and Mulder, M., “Experimen-
tal Evaluation of a Coplanar Airborne Separation Display,” IEEE Transactions on Human-Machine
Systems, Vol. 43, No. 3, 2013, pp. 290–301,
doi:10.1109/TSMC.2013.2238925.
[42] Borst, C., Bijsterbosch, V. A., Van Paassen, M. M., and Mulder, M., “Ecological Interface Design:
Supporting Fault Diagnosis of Automated Advice in a Supervisory Air Trac Control Task, Cogni-
36
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
tion, Technology, and Work, Vol. 19, 2017, pp. 545–560,
doi:10.1007/s10111-017-0438-y.
[43] Borst, C., Visser, R. M., Van Paassen, M. M., and Mulder, M., “Exploring Short-Term Training Eects
of Ecological Interfaces: A Case Study in Air Trac Control,” IEEE Transactions on Human-Machine
Systems, Vol. 49, No. 6, 2019, pp. 623–632,
doi:10.1109/THMS.2019.2919742.
[44] Mercado-Velasco, G. A., Borst, C., Ellerbroek, J., Mulder, M., and van Paassen, M. M., “The Use of
Intent Information in Conflict Detection and Resolution Models Based on Dynamic Velocity Obsta-
cles, IEEE Transactions on Intelligent Transportation Systems, Vol. 16, No. 4, 2015, pp. 2297 2302,
doi:10.1109/TITS.2014.2376031.
[45] Fiorini, P. and Shiller, Z., “Motion Planning in Dynamic Environments Using Velocity Obstacles,” The
International Journal of Robotics Research, Vol. 17, No. 7, 1998, pp. 760–772,
doi:10.1177/027836499801700706.
[46] Snape, J., Berg, J. v. d., Guy, S. J., and Manocha, D., “The Hybrid Reciprocal Velocity Obstacle,
IEEE Transactions on Robotics, Vol. 27, No. 4, 2011, pp. 696–706,
doi:10.1109/TRO.2011.2120810.
[47] Mulder, M., Borst, C., and Van Paassen, M. M., “Improving Operator Situation Awareness through
Ecological Interfaces: Lessons from Aviation, in Holzinger, A., Pl´
acido da Silva, H., and Helfert,
M., eds., “Computer-Human Interaction Research and Applications, CHIRA 2017, Revised Selected
Papers, Springer International Publishing, pp. 20–44, 2019,
doi:10.1007/978-3-030-32965-5 2. Series: Communications in Computer and Information Science,
volume 654.
[48] van Paassen, M. M., Borst, C., Ellerbroek, J., Mulder, M., and Flach, J. M., “Ecological Interface
Design for Vehicle Locomotion Control,” IEEE Trans. on Human-Machine Systems, Vol. 48, No. 5,
2018, pp. 541–555.
[49] Kaber, D. B., Perry, C. M., Segall, N., McClernon, C. K., and Prinzel, L. J., “Situation awareness
implications of adaptive automation for information processing in an air trac control-related task,
37
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, Vol. 36, No. 5, 2006, pp. 447–462,
doi:10.1016/j.ergon.2006.01.008.
[50] Tattersall, A. and Foord, P., “An experimental evaluation of instantaneous self-assessment as a measure
of workload, Ergonomics, Vol. 39, No. 5, 1996, pp. 740–748,
doi:10.1080/00140139608964495.
[51] Farmer, E. and Brownson, A., “Review of Workload Measurement, Analysis and Interpretation Meth-
ods, Tech. Rep. CARE-Integra-TRS-130-02-WP2, Eurocontrol, 2003.
[52] Stevens, S., “On the theory of scales of measurement,” Science, Vol. 103, No. 2684, 1946, pp. 677–680.
[53] Field, A., Discovering Statistics Using SPSS, SAGE Publications, Thousand Oaks, CA, 3rd ed., 2009.
[54] Parasuraman, R., Sheridan, T. B., and Wickens, C. D., A model for types and levels of human inter-
action with automation. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics - Part A: Systems and
Humans, Vol. 30, No. 3, 2000, pp. 286–297,
doi:10.1109/3468.844354.
[55] Clamann, M. P., Wright, M., and Kaber, D. B., “Comparison of Performance Eects of Adaptive Au-
tomation Applied to Various Stages of Human-Machine System Information Processing,” Proceedings
of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, Vol. 46, No. 3, 2002, pp. 342–346,
doi:10.1177/154193120204600327.
[56] Roth, E. M. and Pritchett, A. R., “Preface to the Special Issue on Advancing Models of Hu-
man–Automation Interaction, Journal of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making, Vol. 12, No. 1,
2018, pp. 3–6,
doi:10.1177/1555343417749192.
[57] Sheridan, T. B., “Comments on “Issues in Human–Automation Interaction Modeling: Presumptive
Aspects of Frameworks of Types and Levels of Automation” by David B. Kaber, Journal of Cognitive
Engineering and Decision Making, Vol. 12, No. 1, 2018, pp. 25–28,
doi:10.1177/1555343417724964.
38
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
[58] Boursier, L., Favennec, B., Homan, E., Trzmiel, A., Vergne, F., and Zeghal, K., “Merging Arrival
Flows Without Heading Instructions, in “USA/Europe Air Trac Management Research and Devel-
opment Seminar, , 2007, pp. 1–8.
[59] Bolic, T. and Hansen, M., “User Request Evaluation Tool (URET) Adoption and Adaptation, Three
Center Case Study, in “Proc. of the 6th USA/Europe ATM Seminar, Baltimore (MD), June 27-30, ,
2005, pp. 1–7.
[60] Kaber, D. B., Riley, J. M., Tan, K.-W., and Endsley, M. R., “On the Design of Adaptive Automation for
Complex Systems, International Journal of Cognitive Ergonomics, Vol. 5, No. 1, 2001, pp. 37–57,
doi:10.1207/S15327566IJCE0501 3.
[61] IJtsma, M., Borst, C., Mercado-Velasco, G. A., Mulder, M., and van Paassen, M. M., “Adaptive Au-
tomation Based on Air Trac Controller Decision-Making, in “Proc. of the 19th International Sym-
posium on Aviation Psychology (ISAP), Dayton (OH), May 8-11,” Wright State University, 2017.
[62] van Rooijen, S. J., Ellerbroek, J., Borst, C., and van Kampen, E., “Toward Individual-Sensitive Au-
tomation for Air Trac Control Using Convolutional Neural Networks, Journal of Air Transportation,
Vol. 0, No. 0, 2020, pp. 1–9,
doi:10.2514/1.D0180. Available online: https://doi.org/10.2514/1.D0180.
39
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics
... The simulator was based on a prototype of the Solution Space Diagram (SSD) for ATC, which is currently under development at the Delft University of Technology. 102 The SSD is a tactical decision support tool that displays color coded "go" (safe) and "no-go" (conflict) regions to facilitate participants' use of heading and speed resolutions. Conflict regions indicate that if the velocity vector is within one such area, there will eventually be a loss of separation (defined in en-route airspace as two aircraft being within a 5 nmi and 1000 feet from each other) and possible collision with the aircraft to whom the conflict region depicts. ...
... Aircraft interaction and conflict solution was facilitated by means of a modified ATC Solution Space Diagram (SSD). 102 The SSD is a support tool that visualizes conflict regions ("no-go" regions) imposed by the relative position of other traffic. In doings so, the SSD explicitly show conflict-free regions ("go" regions) that represent the available solution space for avoiding conflict (i.e., loosing separation between two or more aircraft). ...
... The SSD has been shown to reduce novice controllers' workload during high traffic loads and increase separation without reducing sector throughput. 102 Experienced controllers using the SSD have been shown to implement more conservative conflict solutions that benefit overall sector robustness, albeit at the cost of efficiency measured by the additional track miles. 233 indicate time to separation loss, with the red color representing less time than the orange color. ...
Thesis
View online: http://www.publicatie-online.nl/uploaded/flipbook/14797-c-westin/ This thesis set out to obtain a fundamental understanding of how controllers' acceptance of ATC conflict resolution advisories were affected, depending on how well the decision aid's conflict-solving strategy matches that of the individual controller. A novel approach was developed that repeated and curtailed controllers' own solutions as automated advisories. Strategic conformance was varied by providing the same controller with either her/his own solution to the same conflict (conformal), or a colleague's contrasting solution to the same conflict (nonconformal). Taken together, this thesis has contributed to the knowledge of what drives controllers' acceptance of resolution advisories in particular, and human-automation collaboration and automation acceptance in general. Empirical results showed that conformal resolution advisories benefited acceptance and agreement of that system's advisories, as well as reducing response time. These benefits were observed across varying expertise levels, particularly in relation to expert operators. Results also revealed that controllers were consistent, but disagreed on how to solve conflicts. In conclusion, findings support the feasibility of developing more individual-sensitive automation. However, designers need to carefully consider the specific contextual goals and objectives, particularly in safety-critical domains, for which a personalized conformal system is considered. Many work domains may be more suitable for facilitating homogeneity that restricts individual differences in interaction and problem-solving.
... An interesting middle ground is found in the evaluation by Mercado Velasco et al. (2021), where a "solution space" display is added to support present-day tactical ATC. This display shows combined speed and heading solutions that are clear from surrounding traffic. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Expert participants may not always be available for evaluation of new displays or support systems, and in some cases, it might be better to use novice participants, particularly when the display or support significantly changes existing work practices. To provide tools and arguments for selecting the expertise level of participants, we propose the use of Rasmussen's decision ladder to analyze where and how a new visualization or a support tool changes the task, and identify steps where a novice participant may learn to perform the task to an acceptable level. A comparison to the support with the current operational interfaces then shows where an expert might have difficulty in stepping away from learned practice. This analysis is applied to the domain of air traffic control, and a selected set of relevant past research with both expert and novice participants is reviewed, revisiting the decision for a participant level in the study.
... A potentially more fruitful direction for adaptive systems is to support controllers with information integration, with full decision-making authority with the human. For example, visualizing functional constraints through ecological interface design [42] or showing the separation monitor as an overview of conflicting aircraft pairs could be types of support that are less intrusive to decision-making and their success relies less on inferring intent. ...
Article
Full-text available
Air traffic controller workload is a limiting factor in the current air traffic management system. Adaptive support systems have the potential to balance controller workload and gain acceptance as they provide support during times of need. Challenges in the design of adaptive support systems are to decide when and how to trigger support. The goal of this study is to gain empirical insights into these challenges through a human-in-the-loop experiment, featuring a simplified air traffic control environment in which a novel triggering mechanism uses the quality of the controller's decisions to determine when support is needed. The designed system seeks to prevent high workload conditions by providing resolution advisories when the controller exceeds a threshold of “self-complicating” decisions. Results indicate that the new system is indeed capable of increasing the efficiency and safety compared to full manual control without intervention. More adaptive support, however, increased the frustration of participants, decreased acceptance, and did not result in improved workload ratings. These findings suggest that, unless we can better infer human intent in complex work environments, adaptive support at the level of decision-making is problematic. A potentially more fruitful direction is to provide support at the level of information integration, with full decision-making authority with the human.
Article
On final approach, an approach controller is responsible for separating aircraft lining up on the instrument landing system. In an attempt to increase traffic throughput, especially in strong headwind conditions, European regulation advises all European airports to move from distance-based to time-based separation. This effectively changes the controller’s task from a distance-based to a time-based problem. Further complications arise because of the European recategorization of aircraft types initiative, and experts fear that the gains foreseen with time-based separation will not be realized. This paper presents a visual tool integrated into the radar screen to assist controllers in performing time-based separation, the ideal turn-in point (ITIP) display. To assist controllers in selecting optimal approach strategies, starting from the moment aircraft enter the terminal control area, the display shows the possibilities and restrictions in the system rather than giving (restricting) advisories. A proof-of-concept experiment was performed with people knowledgeable in air traffic control ([Formula: see text]) and compared the ITIP to a current industry state-of-the-art display designed by U.K.’s National Air Traffic Services in scenarios of varying difficulty. Results show that with the ITIP tool, efficiency improved with similar or higher levels of safety and similar or lower workload. These promising results justify testing the interface with professional air traffic controllers. Future work aims at reducing clutter, increasing simulation fidelity, and increasing the level of support in complex traffic situations.
Article
Full-text available
Lack of trust and lack of acceptance caused by strategic mismatches in problem-solving have been identified as obstacles in the introduction of workload-alleviating automation in air traffic control. One possible way to overcome these obstacles is by creating automation capable of providing personalized advisories conformal to the individual controller. This paper focuses on performing an exploratory investigation into the tools and methodology required for creating conformal automation. Central in the creation of individualized prediction models is the combination of a visual feature, capturing traffic situations, and a tailored convolutional neural network model trained on individual controller data recorded from a human-in-the-loop simulation. The main advantage of using a visual feature is that it could facilitate “transparency” of the machine learning model. Results show that the trained models can reasonably predict command type, direction, and magnitude. Furthermore, a correlation is found between controller consistency and achieved prediction performance. A comparison between individual-sensitive and general models showed a benefit of individually trained models, confirming the strategy heterogeneity of the population, which is a critical assumption for personalized automation. Future research should be done in refining the model architecture, finding richer visual features that capture the breadth of human decision-making behavior and feedback model outputs back to individuals for measuring controller agreement.
Article
Full-text available
In many work domains, the push toward higher levels of automation raises the concern of diminishing human expertise. Ecological interfaces could help operators in retaining and potentially even in acquiring expertise as they are hypothesized to lead to a deeper understanding of the work domain. This study explores the short-term impact of ecological interfaces on knowledge development and compares the results with an instruction-based training method. To monitor and compare students’ progress, their decision-making strategies, identified from verbal comments recorded in “think-aloud” simulator sessions, are mapped onto the decision ladder. This method has been applied to an experiment ( N=16 ) aimed at training novices in conflict detection and resolution (CD&R) within a simplified air traffic control context. Results show that the overall CD&R performance in the final measurement sessions, featuring a transfer manipulation, was not significantly different between the “ecological” and “instructional” groups. In terms of cognitive behavior, however, students in the ecological group exhibited more laborious rule- and knowledge-based behaviors that sparked goal-oriented thoughts and corresponding control performances beyond the CD&R task. These findings indicate that ecological interfaces can change how people think and approach a control problem, even after removing the support. It is therefore reasonable to believe that ecological interfaces can play an important role in the early stages of deep knowledge development.
Article
Full-text available
Ecological interface design (EID) was originally developed in the context of process control but has been extended into many domains where technology has resulted in both changing work demands and increased opportunities for improved interface applications. This paper gives an overview of the application of the EID to the control of vehicle locomotion, either from within the vehicle, as a driver or a pilot, or from the outside, as an operator or a (air traffic) controller. It discusses lessons learned from the application of the EID for the vehicle locomotion control task and focuses on how the methodology can be applied to this domain. Specific issues identified are that the planning and control of a vehicle simultaneously spans multiple time scales and that the interface must be designed considering the format in which the control input is defined. Also, due to the extensive standardization of the instrumentation and training certification, changes introduced by the new displays must initially be additional to the existing displays. Chosen representations must also be shown in a format that matches the current instrumentation and the directly observable outside world.
Article
Full-text available
Future air traffic control will have to rely on more advanced automation to support human controllers in their job of safely handling increased traffic volumes. A prerequisite for the success of such automation is that the data driving it are reliable. Current technology, however, still warrants human supervision in coping with (data) uncertainties and consequently in judging the quality and validity of machine decisions. In this study, ecological interface design was used to assist controllers in fault diagnosis of automated advice, using a prototype ecological interface (called the solution space diagram) for tactical conflict detection and resolution in the horizontal plane. Results from a human-in-the-loop simulation, in which sixteen participants were tasked with monitoring automation and intervening whenever required or desired, revealed a significant improvement in fault detection and diagnosis in a complex traffic scenario. Additionally, the experiment also exposed interesting interaction patterns between the participants and the advisory system, which seemed unrelated to the fault diagnosis task. Here, the explicit means-ends links appeared to have affected participants’ control strategy, which was geared toward taking over control from automation, regardless of the fault condition. This result suggests that in realizing effective human-automation teamwork, finding the right balance between offering more insight (e.g., through ecological interfaces) and striving for compliance with single (machine) advice is an avenue worth exploring further.
Chapter
The purpose of human-machine systems design is to develop interfaces and automation tools which support human operators in performing effective, efficient and safe work. An important prerequisite for the latter is that operators understand the process under control, are aware of what is happening, and have sufficient means to act on the process appropriately. In this chapter, which is an extension of our CHIRA’2017 paper [1], we discuss the ecological approach we adopted to design human-machine systems in aviation. We focus in particular on what, in our opinion, operator situation awareness actually means, and how to improve it. The aircraft separation task will be discussed, using two examples which show how novel visualizations and tools can support pilots and air traffic controllers in their decision making.
Article
Three aspects of Kaber’s paper are discussed: (a) the origins of the level-of-automation concept as related to various misconceptions in the literature regarding the intent of the original paper; (b) distinctions between descriptive, predictive, presumptive, and normative models; and (c) the difficulty, even impossibility, of making level-of-automation taxonomies into readily useful tools for system design.
Article
To supervise correctly a complex technological plant, the human supervisor must be very familiar with the plant, that is he must possess an INTERNAL REPRESENTATION, IR, of the plant, the tasks, and the statistics of disturbances, if he or she is to act in an optimal way. The researcher's understanding of the supervisor's IR is an INTERNAL MODEL, IM, which can be included in an overall model of the supervisor's behavior, called HUMAN PERFORMANCE MODEL, HPM. Over the last decades control, AI, verbal and psychological models have been proposed. The existence of an IM is a necessary but non-sufficient condition for building HPM's. Models have been developed at the skill-based and rule-based level, but not at the knowledge-based level, yet it is just the human supervisor's creativity that motivates the need to maintain the human supervisor's job; it is his knowledge-based behavior that cannot be modeled, and hence it is of great interest for scientists, engineers and product managers.
Conference Paper
Through smart scheduling and triggering of automation support, adaptive automation has the potential to balance air traffic controller workload. The challenge in the design of adaptive automation systems is to decide how and when the automation should provide support. This paper describes the design of a novel mechanism for adaptively invoking automation support. Whereas most adaptive automation support systems are reactive in that they invoke automation support after controller workload has increased, the aim of the designed mechanism is to proactively trigger automation support prior to workload increases. To do this, the mechanism assesses the quality of air traffic controller's decisions. The designed adaptive automation system has been tested in a human-in-the-loop experiment. Results indicate that the adaptive support helps to increase efficiency and safety as compared to manual control. However, lower triggering thresholds (resulting in more frequent automation intervention) increased the frustration level of participants (as measured with NASA TLX) and decreased acceptance of the support.