ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review

Developing and implementing guidelines on culturally adapting the Addenbrooke's cognitive examination version III (ACE-III): a qualitative illustration

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Background: Cognitive tests currently used in healthcare and research settings do not account for bias in performance that arises due to cultural context. At present there are no universally accepted steps or minimum criteria for culturally adapting cognitive tests. We propose a methodology for developing specific guidelines to culturally adapt a specific cognitive test and used this to develop guidelines for the ACE-III. We then demonstrated their implementation by using them to produce an ACE-III Urdu for a British South Asian population. Methods: This was a several stage qualitative study. We combined information from our systematic review on the translation and cultural adaptation of the ACE-III with feedback from previous ACE-III adaptors. This identified steps for cultural adaptation. We formatted these into question-by-question guidelines. These guidelines, along with feedback from focus groups with potential users were used to develop ACE-III Urdu questions. Clinical experts reviewed these questions to finalise an ACE-III Urdu. Results: Our systematic review found 32 adaptations and we received feedback from seven adaptors to develop the guidelines. With these guidelines and two focus groups with 12 participants a sample ACE-III Urdu was developed. A consensus meeting of two psychiatrists with a South Asian background and familiarity with cognitive tests and cultural adaptation finalised the ACE-III Urdu. Conclusions: We developed a set of guidelines for culturally adapting the ACE-III that can be used by future adaptors for their own language or cultural context. We demonstrated how guidelines on cultural adaptation can be developed for any cognitive test and how they can be used to adapt it.
Content may be subject to copyright.
R E S E A R C H A R T I C L E Open Access
Developing and implementing guidelines
on culturally adapting the Addenbrookes
cognitive examination version III (ACE-III): a
qualitative illustration
Waquas Waheed
1
, Nadine Mirza
1*
, Muhammed Wali Waheed
2
, Abid Malik
3
and Maria Panagioti
1
Abstract
Background: Cognitive tests currently used in healthcare and research settings do not account for bias in
performance that arises due to cultural context. At present there are no universally accepted steps or minimum
criteria for culturally adapting cognitive tests. We propose a methodology for developing specific guidelines to
culturally adapt a specific cognitive test and used this to develop guidelines for the ACE-III. We then demonstrated
their implementation by using them to produce an ACE-III Urdu for a British South Asian population.
Methods: This was a several stage qualitative study. We combined information from our systematic review on the
translation and cultural adaptation of the ACE-III with feedback from previous ACE-III adaptors. This identified steps
for cultural adaptation. We formatted these into question-by-question guidelines. These guidelines, along with
feedback from focus groups with potential users were used to develop ACE-III Urdu questions. Clinical experts
reviewed these questions to finalise an ACE-III Urdu.
Results: Our systematic review found 32 adaptations and we received feedback from seven adaptors to develop
the guidelines. With these guidelines and two focus groups with 12 participants a sample ACE-III Urdu was
developed. A consensus meeting of two psychiatrists with a South Asian background and familiarity with cognitive
tests and cultural adaptation finalised the ACE-III Urdu.
Conclusions: We developed a set of guidelines for culturally adapting the ACE-III that can be used by future
adaptors for their own language or cultural context. We demonstrated how guidelines on cultural adaptation can
be developed for any cognitive test and how they can be used to adapt it.
Keywords: Cross cultural, Ethnic minority, Non-English, Language, Psychometrics, Scale development, Transcultural,
Translation
© The Author(s). 2020 Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License,
which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give
appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if
changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons
licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons
licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain
permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the
data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
* Correspondence: nadine.mirza@postgrad.manchester.ac.uk
1
Centre for Primary Care and Health Services Research, The University of
Manchester, Suite 8, 5th Floor, Williamson Building Oxford Road, Manchester
M13 9PL, UK
Full list of author information is available at the end of the article
Waheed et al. BMC Psychiatry (2020) 20:492
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-020-02893-6
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Background
There is an increasing prevalence of dementia on a glo-
bal scale, with numbers showing there will be a rise from
50 million to 82 million in the next decade [1]. Within
the United Kingdom (UK) there was an estimated 850,
000 people with dementia in 2015 but this is expected to
increase to 1 million by 2021 and over 2 million by 2051
[2]. Thus, the need for early and accurate diagnosis con-
tinues to take precedence [3,4].
In addition to a physical examination and robust inter-
viewing [5] the diagnosis must be supplemented with
the administration of a cognitive test [6]. These cognitive
tests are characterised by their ability to either screen
for or diagnose cognitive impairment and dementia [3].
In many cases they will also investigate severity and the
potential subtype of dementia [7].
This is achieved through questions that assess individ-
ual cognitive capabilities- otherwise known as cognitive
domains- such as attention and orientation, fluency,
memory, language, and visuospatial abilities [5,7]. They
are often administered verbally, consisting of written,
verbal, and sometimes physical tasks, ideally taking
under 30 min [6].
Currently there are over 40 cognitive tests available
and under use in both healthcare settings and within the
context of research [8]. These range from brief screening
tests such as the Six Item Cognitive Impairment Test (6
CIT) [9], Clock Drawing [10], the Mini Cog Test [11],
and Test Your Memory (TYM) [12] to longer more ex-
tensive diagnostic tests such as the Mini Mental State
Examination (MMSE) [13], the Montreal Cognitive As-
sessment (MoCA) [14], and the Addenbrookes Cogni-
tive Examination Version III (ACE-III) [15].
These cognitive tests were originally designed for Eng-
lish speaking European countries [16], standardised on
well educated, male, Caucasian outpatients [17]. While
applicable to that demographic, the original versions of
these cognitive tests are not suitable for use with diverse
populations [18].
Within the UK alone, 88 main languages other than
English are spoken [19] and 8% of the population do not
have English as a first language [20]. It is also estimated
that over 864,000 struggle to or are unable to speak Eng-
lish [20]. Cognitive tests in their current state would not
be able to accommodate for these non-English speakers.
Thus, many cognitive tests have been translated into tar-
geted languages. However, these translated versions
havent necessarily been psychometrically validated and
translation alone does not address gaps in understanding
and acceptability that arise due to cultural context [4].
In the UK 14% of the population identifies as belong-
ing to an ethnic minority group [19] and this is pre-
dicted to rise to 20% by 2051 [21]. A significant
proportion of these ethnic minorities migrated to the
UK as young adults [22,23]. Therefore, they still adhere
in many ways to the culture of their home countries and
pass this culture on to their children [22,23].
This cultural context cannot be ignored when adminis-
tering cognitive tests as culture influences how a respond-
ent perceives test questions and how they respond to them,
if they are even able to do so [18,24]. This is due to many
cognitive test questions being reliant on a familiarity with
the western calendar and western names, and knowledge of
western history, objects and even wildlife [4]. When these
questions have not been adapted to account for cultural
context bias occurs. This equates to a loss of content
equivalence (the questions are not relevant to the cultural
context), criterion equivalence (the questions are unable
to accurately assess for dementia) and content equivalence
(the questions are no longer able to accurately assess the
individual cognitive domains they were designed to) [25].
This accounts for higher rates of false positive and
false negative scores across cognitive tests within non
English speakers and ethnic minority groups as
compared to their English speaking and Caucasian coun-
terparts [18,26,27]. It also compromises the generalis-
ability of the results of dementia research that
incorporate these cognitive tests [18,26,27].
To counter this, attempts at designing new cognitive
tests for specific groups have been tried but this was
deemed too time consuming and complex, reducing
feasibility [28]. Another suggestion has been to adjust
cut-off scores for different ethnic minority and non-
English speaking groups but this has been criticised for
reducing sensitivity, specificity and likelihood ratios [29].
Therefore, culturally adapting an existing cognitive test
has been regarded as a preferred alternative.
As mentioned, translating these tests does occur but this
does not overcome the influence of culture beyond flu-
ency in the target language. Existing cultural adaptations
of cognitive tests do exist and have been developed
through a variety of qualitative methods such as the use of
global guidelines, involving experts and potential users in
coproduction, and pilot testing [30]. However, at present
there is no universal standard procedure or minimum cri-
teria for culturally adapting these cognitive tests [31].
Therefore, there must be a global consensus on the
steps and procedures that are essential for undertaking
thorough cultural adaptation of cognitive tests [18].
These must be conducted before the adapted cognitive
tests can be administered in healthcare and research set-
tings. The final step would be a psychometric validation
of the adapted cognitive test within the target popula-
tion. However, prior to this there must be steps that
focus on the cultural adaptation process itself.
We propose that for each cognitive test there should be a
set of guidelines on how to culturally adapt the questions of
that specific test. These would be developed through an
Waheed et al. BMC Psychiatry (2020) 20:492 Page 2 of 13
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
incorporation of a review of previous literature on that test
[25] with feedback from those who have already adapted it.
These guidelines would provide step by step instructions
on how to culturally adapt every question of that cognitive
test in accordance with evidence to allow for the retention
of content, criterion and conceptual equivalence [25].
The guidelines would then be implemented by using
them to create culturally adapted versions of the cogni-
tive test questions for a particular target demographic.
These versions would be presented to potential users
from the target demographic and clinical experts in the
field. Their feedback would finalise which versions of the
test questions will be retained in the adapted version of
the cognitive test.
Following this there would be a cultural validation of
the adapted cognitive test. This would consist of adminis-
tering it to members of the target population and con-
ducting qualitative cognitive interviews to assess the
adapted cognitive tests understanding and acceptability
within this population [32]. Once this has occurred, as
mentioned, the psychometric validation would take place.
In this paper we will detail the process of developing
and implementing such a set of guidelines for a non-
English speaking ethnic minority group within the UK.
The cognitive test we selected was the ACE-III [15], a
gold standard tool for the diagnostic accuracy of cogni-
tive impairment and dementia [33], consisting of 19
questions that assess the cognitive domains attention,
Table 1 Questions of the Addenbrookes Cognitive Examination Version III
Question Number Task/Question
1: Attention Orientation Ask the day, date, month, year, season, floor, street/hospital, town, county and country.
2: Attention Registration Say the words lemon, key and ball and ask them to repeat and try to remember.
3. Attention Concentration Ask to take 7 away from 100 and keep taking 7 away from the new number for 5 trials (Serial 7s).
4. Memory Recall Ask for the three words from 2. Attention Registration.
5a. Fluency Letters Ask for as many words as they can think of starting with the letter P, not including names of pronouns, in
one minute.
5b. Fluency Animals Ask for the names of as many animals as they can think of in one minute.
6. Memory Anterograde Say the name and address Harry Barnes, 73, Orchard Close, Kingsbridge, Devonand ask them to repeat and
try to remember.
7. Memory Retrograde Ask for the name of the current Prime Minister, name of the woman who was Prime Minister, name of the
USA president and name of the USA president who was assassinated in the 1960s.
8. Language Comprehension Place a pencil and paper in front. Ask to place the paper on top of the pencil,pick up the pencil but not
the paperand pass me the pencil after touching the paper.
9. Language Writing Ask to write two or more complete sentences about their last holiday/weekend/Christmas, without using
abbreviations.
10. Language Repetition Say the words caterpillar, eccentricity, unintelligible and statistician and ask them to repeat.
11. Language Repetition Say the proverbs All that glitters is not goldand A stitch in time saves nineand ask them to repeat.
12. Language Naming Show 12 images and ask them to name each.
13. Language Comprehension Ask to point to the one which is associated with the monarchy,the one which is a marsupial,the one
which is found in the Antarcticand the one which has a nautical connectionfrom the 12 images provided.
14. Language Reading Ask them to read the words sew, pint, soot, dough and height.
15a. Visuospatial Abilities - Infinity
Diagram
Ask them to copy the following:
15b. Visuospatial Abilities Wire Cube Ask them to copy the following:
15c. Visuospatial Abilities Clock Ask them to draw a clock face with numbers and the hands at ten past five.
16. Visuospatial Abilities Ask them to count the number of dots without pointing.
17. Visuospatial Abilities Ask them to identify the fragmented letters K, M, A and T.
18. Memory Recall Ask for the three words from 6. Memory Anterograde.
19. Memory Recognition For each word of the name and address that could not be recalled, give the options listed and ask to identify
which word it was.
Waheed et al. BMC Psychiatry (2020) 20:492 Page 3 of 13
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
memory, fluency, language and visuospatial abilities (See
Table 1).
The ACE-III and its predecessors, the ACE [34] and
ACE-Revised (ACE-R) [35] have been translated into a
range of languages and incorporated into use across the
globe. English versions of the ACE-III have also been
adapted for the UK and the United States of America
(USA). However, the ACE-III was originally designed for
English speakers native to Australia, with a reliance on
knowledge of the cultural background [15] and although
cultural adaptation has been undertaken by adaptors to
produce suitable adaptations [3638] there are no exist-
ing standardised guidelines for the cultural adaptation of
this cognitive test.
As South Asians are the UKs largest ethnic minority
group, at over 6.3% of the overall population, we selected
them as our target population to culturally adapt for.
The language we chose to adapt in was Urdu [32], a
popular South Asian language and the 4th most com-
mon language spoken in the UK [19]. Prior to this there
was only one other Urdu version of the ACE-III avail-
able, which was culturally adapted for use within India
[39]. Due to this it was not applicable to the cultural
contexts of other South Asian countries where Urdu
speakers reside [40], nor to the Urdu speaking diaspora
within Canada, the Middle East, the US and the UK [41].
Though this paper only details the development and
implementation process of the guidelines, the ACE-III
Urdu we produced through the methods has also under-
gone the cultural validation process, described elsewhere
[32]. This process undertook 25 cognitive interviews
with cognitively healthy Urdu speaking British South
Asians over the age of 60. The ACE-III Urdu will now
need to undergo a psychometric validation before being
made available for widespread use.
Methods
A several stage qualitative approach was undertaken to
develop guidelines for translating and culturally adapting
the ACE-III (See Fig. 1) and implement them to develop
an ACE-III Urdu (See Fig. 2):
Step 1: A systematic review.
Step 2: Receiving feedback from previous ACE-III
adaptors.
Step 3: Collating the data to form guidelines.
Step 4: Implementing guidelines with feedback from
lay persons and clinical experts.
Step 5: Developing the ACE-III Urdu.
Step 1: systematic review
We conducted a systematic review of all existing primary
publications of translations and cultural adaptations of
the ACE-III as well as its predecessors [30].
The electronic databases EMBASE, Medline, and Psy-
chINFO were searched using the search terms adden-
brookes cognitive examination or ace-iii or screen* or
test or instrument or measure or tool or diagnos*,de-
mentia or Alzheimer* or cognitive*and sensitivity and
specificity or accuracy or cut-off or receiver operator or
ROC or Youden[30]. We searched from January 2013,
the year the ACE-III was introduced [15], to December
2016, which was when this review was conducted.
Fig. 1 Overview of the process of developing guidelines for translating and culturally adapting the ACE-III
Waheed et al. BMC Psychiatry (2020) 20:492 Page 4 of 13
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
SCOPUS was also searched for any publication that cited
the original paper [15]. Additionally, we also screened all
included and excluded publications of a meta-analysis of
the ACE and ACE-Revised (ACE-R) [42].
Publications, including validation studies, that incorpo-
rated the use of a translated or culturally adapted ver-
sion of the ACE, ACE-R and ACE-III, from English into
any other language, and were the primary source of that
version, were included. For each of the publications we
extracted data on the version of the ACE that was cul-
turally adapted, the language it was translated into, the
country it was culturally adapted for and the section of
the text that described how it was culturally adapted.
These reported processes were broken down into indi-
vidual steps and grouped by which ACE-III question
they described adapting. This allowed us to identify
which questions were dependent on culture, how they
had been culturally adapted, and what the rationale was
behind the changes.
We also assessed the quality of the reported cultural
adaptation of each publication with the Manchester Cul-
tural Adaptation Reporting Questionnaire (MCAR) [30].
This shows which publications reported their cultural
adaptation process in sufficient detail to be replicated by
future adaptors.
Step 2: feedback from adaptors of the ACE-III
We aimed to receive feedback from official adaptors of
the ACE-III; those who had translated and culturally
adapted it for their respective language and culture and
had made their adaptation available on the Neuroscience
Research Australia (NeuRA) website that hosted the ori-
ginal ACE-III and its implementation materials at the
time of this research [39].
We downloaded all available adaptations of the ACE-
III from the website. We translated them into English
through the use of an online translation application and,
when available, with the aid of postgraduate research
students at the University of Manchester who were na-
tive speakers of the languages. The translated adapta-
tions were read through to identify which questions in
each adaptation of the ACE-III had been culturally
adapted beyond a translation verbatim. For each cultural
adaptation we developed a questionnaire that
highlighted the questions that had been culturally
adapted along with the original ACE-III counterparts
(See Additional file 1for sample questionnaire). We
asked for the rationale behind changing the original
question and the development process of the culturally
adapted version of the question with the rationale.
We distributed the questionnaires to the correspond-
ing adaptors attached with a standardised email relaying
the purpose of the questionnaires and a request for their
completion. We also requested a check of our transla-
tion of their language version. After a two week period
adaptors were sent a follow up email to act as a re-
minder. If adaptors did not initiate any form of contact
after this no further contact was made.
Step 3: data analysis and synthesis
To develop guidelines for culturally adapting the ACE-
III we collated the information from our systematic re-
view [30] and the feedback from adaptors. We identified
sets of mutually exclusive steps for culturally adapting
each question of the ACE-III.
From our systematic review we had broken down the
cultural adaptation processes extracted from each publi-
cation according to ACE-III question. The adaptation
processes for each question across publications were
merged and duplicates removed to identify the mutually
exclusive steps that could be undertaken to adapt each
question.
Fig. 2 Overview of the process of utilising the guidelines to
culturally adapt the ACE-III for British South Asians
Waheed et al. BMC Psychiatry (2020) 20:492 Page 5 of 13
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
The questionnaires sent to adaptors were already orga-
nised by question. We merged the adaptorsfeedback on
the cultural adaptation process of each question. We re-
moved duplicating information so each question had
mutually exclusive steps that could be undertaken to
adapt that question along with the adaptorsaccompany-
ing rationale.
The cultural adaptation steps for each ACE-III ques-
tion identified from the systematic review and from the
adaptorsfeedback were merged. Duplicates were re-
moved to identify overall mutually exclusive cultural
adaptation steps for each question. Accompanying ra-
tionale was presented with these steps and the respective
publications and adapted versions of the ACE-III were
cited, resulting in a question-by-question set of
guidelines.
Step 4: implementation of the guidelines
We conducted two focus groups within the British South
Asian community of Greater Manchester. We aimed to
recruit 1214 laymen participants overall, fluent in
speaking and writing Urdu, over the age of 60, able to
give informed consent and who did not have a history of
cognitive impairment.
Participants were voluntarily recruited via convenience
sampling from the local Pakistani Community Day
Centre and provided with an information sheet, available
in English and Urdu. They were given 24 h to decide if
they wished to participate, after which they were con-
tacted by a liaison at the Centre to confirm their partici-
pation and let them know the date and time of the focus
group. These were also held at the centre. On the day of
the focus group participants would be provided with
consent forms and demographics sheets, available in
English and Urdu.
Using the guidelines we produced several culturally
adapted versions of all questions of the ACE-III, backed
up by rationale, for the British Urdu speaking popula-
tion. The most suitable option would need to be se-
lected. We presented these versions of the questions
within our focus groups to receive their feedback on the
questionscultural appropriateness, which versions
should be retained for a potential ACE-III Urdu and
whether they proposed any changes or suggestions of
their own. This feedback was audio recorded and
transcribed.
We also conducted a consensus meeting with clinical
experts in the relevant fields, local to the Greater Man-
chester area. We aimed to recruit 24 experts on de-
mentia, the cognitive testing process and the translation
and cultural adaptation of these tests. They would also
be familiar with the ACE-III, its rationale and how to ad-
minister it. These experts also had to be fluent in
speaking and writing both English and Urdu and familiar
with UK and South Asian cultures. The experts were
recruited voluntarily via convenience sampling. The con-
sensus meeting was held at the Centre for Primary Care
and Health Services Research, at the University of
Manchester, before which informed consent was
obtained.
We presented the focus group feedback within the
consensus meeting and experts determined which were
the most culturally suitable adaptations of each question
of the ACE-III. The consensus meeting was audio re-
corded, transcribed and the data was collated for each
question of the ACE-III to determine how each would
be culturally adapted.
Step 5: developing the ACE-III Urdu
To develop the ACE-III Urdu the template was acquired
through NeuRA, allowing the ACE-III Urdu to retain
the exact same format as the original ACE-III. Urdu is
read from right to left so the template was reversed hori-
zontally such that questions were presented on the right
side of the template and the scoring instructions on the
left side. Standard information requested prior to the ad-
ministration of the ACE-III and instructions for the im-
plementation of the questions were translated into Urdu
and typed out. Each ACE-III question was typed out and
designed according to the suggestions confirmed within
the consensus meeting.
Results
Step 1: systematic review
We identified 113 publications through our search, of
which 32 met out criteria for data extraction and ana-
lysis; 12 for the original ACE, 17 for the ACE Revised
and 3 for the ACE-III. Overall, these publications
spanned 18 languages; Arabic, Cantonese, Chinese,
Czech, Danish, French, German, Greek, Hebrew, Italian,
Japanese, Korean, Lithuanian, Malayalam, Persian,
Portuguese, Slovak and Spanish [30].
The full results of the systematic review are described
elsewhere [30].
Step 2: feedback from adaptors of the ACE-III
Our search of the NeuRA website identified 17 fully
adapted versions of the ACE-III for the languages
Egyptian Arabic, Saudi Arabian Arabic, Chinese,
Estonian, Hebrew, Hindi, Hungarian, Indian Kannada,
Italian, Japanese, Marathi, Polish, Portuguese, Spanish,
Tamil, Telugu and Indian Urdu. Three versions had
been retained in English but culturally adapted for India,
New Zealand and the USA, resulting in a total of 20
ACE-III adaptations.
Of these adaptations the Estonian, Indian Kannada,
Marathi, Japanese, Tamil and Telugu versions could not
Waheed et al. BMC Psychiatry (2020) 20:492 Page 6 of 13
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
be translated into English due to a lack of resources in
terms of translation applications and translators. They
excluded from our analysis (30%). Questionnaires were
developed for the remaining 14 adaptations (70%) and
distributed to their respective adaptors, of which a total
of seven questionnaires (35%) were returned to us fully
completed.
The original Australian ACE-III was used by the
Hindi, Hungarian and Spanish adaptors (15%) and the
UK version of the ACE-III was used by the Egyptian
Arabic, Hebrew and Welsh adaptors (15%) for their own
adaptations. Polish adaptors used both (5%).
Table 2summarises which questions of the ACE-III
were culturally adapted by which adaptors, thereby
showing the frequency of reported cultural adaptation
undertaken for each question. Table 3shows examples
of the culturally adapted versions of these questions
across a range of languages and cultural contexts. We
can see that all adaptors culturally adapted questions 6,
7, 18 and 19 for memory and questions 10, 11 and 14
for language, and the majority had adapted question 2
for attention. In contrast, none of the adaptors had cul-
turally adapted any of the questions assessing visuo-
spatial abilities. This highlights which cognitive domains,
and their respective questions, rely on culture and which
would suffice with a simple translation into the target
language.
Step 3: data analysis and synthesis
For each question of the ACE-III, the individual cultural
adaptation steps identified from our systematic review
and from adaptorsfeedback, along with rationale
undertaken, were tabulated to form the guidelines (See
example in Fig. 3and see Addtional file 2). For each
question the following was presented:
Table 2 Questions of the ACE-III that were culturally adapted by the adaptors
ACE-III Questions
Language of the adaptors 1 2 3 4 5a 5b 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15a 15b 15c 16 17 18 19
Egyptian Arabic x x x x x x x x x x x
Hebrew x x x x x x x x x
Hindi x x x x x x x x x x x
Hungarian x x x x x x x
Polish x x x x x x x x x x x
Spanish x x x x x x x x
x
x
Welsh x x x x x x x x x
x = cultural adaptation was undertaken
Table 3 Examples of cultural adaptation of adapted ACE-III questions
ACE-III Question Example of adapted version Language
of version
2: Attention Registration: Say the words lemon, ask them to repeat and try to
remember.
Lemon was changed to Sliwka (Plum). Polish
5a. Fluency Letters: Ask for as many words as they can think of starting with the
letter P, not including names of pronouns, in one minute.
Psound was changed to Shsound. Egyptian
Arabic
6. Memory Anterograde: Say the name and address Harry Barnes, 73, Orchard Close,
Kingsbridge, Devonand ask them to repeat and try to remember.
The name and address were changed to
Katona Péter, Tavasz utca 42., Gyöngyös,
Heves megye.
Hungarian
7. Memory Retrograde: Ask for the name of the current Prime Minister. Asked for the name of the current President. Spanish
10. Language Repetition: Say the word caterpillar. Caterpillar was changed to Colomennod
(Pigeons).
Welsh
11. Language Repetition: Say the proverb A stitch in time saves nineand ask them
to repeat.
The proverb was changed to The orchestra
played and the audience applauded
Hebrew
12. Language Naming: Show image of kangaroo and ask them to name each. The image was changed to a goat. Hindi
13. Language Comprehension: Ask to point to the one which is a marsupial, from
the 12 images provided.
The question was changed to point to the
one which flies.
Egyptian
Arabic
14. Language Reading: Ask them to read the word height. Height was changed to Zamarzniety (Frozen). Welsh
17. Visuospatial Abilities: Ask them to identify the fragmented English letters. The letters were changed to Hindi alphabet. Hindi
Waheed et al. BMC Psychiatry (2020) 20:492 Page 7 of 13
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1) How the question has been previously culturally
adapted with the steps undertaken.
2) Examples compiled from publications and the
questionnaires, citing the respective languages and
adaptors of the ACE-III
3) The rationale behind adapting the question and
choosing the adapted replacement.
Step 4: implementation of the guidelines
Our focus groups had 12 voluntary participants, five fe-
male (41%) and seven male (59%), from ages 6175 years
(M = 66.67, SD = 6.44), from the Greater Manchester
area. (See Table 4for a breakdown of participant demo-
graphics). Seven participants were married, three were
widowed, and two did not disclose. Six were retired,
three were housewives, two were unemployed, and one
did not disclose. Participants came from varied socio-
economic backgrounds, across a range of educational
backgrounds.
Through our proposed suggestions for each question
of the ACE-III Urdu, developed through the use of the
guidelines, we determined questions 5b, 8, 9, 15a, 15b,
Fig. 3 An example page of the guidelines
Table 4 Demographic details of focus group participants
PI Age Level of Education First Language Other
Language
1F 60s X Punjabi Urdu, English
2F 70s 10th Year Punjabi Urdu
3F 60s FA Punjabi Urdu
4F 60s FA Punjabi Urdu
5F 70s None Punjabi Urdu
1 M 70s Graduate Punjabi Urdu, English
2 M 60s BA Urdu English
3 M 70s MA Punjabi Urdu, English, Arabic
4 M 70s GCSEs Urdu English
5 M 60s GCSEs Urdu Punjabi, English
6 M 60s X
Graduate
Punjabi Urdu, Italian, English
7 M 70s Graduate Punjabi Urdu, English, Persian
Waheed et al. BMC Psychiatry (2020) 20:492 Page 8 of 13
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Table 5 ACE-III Urdu items determined from focus groups and consensus meeting
Suggestions Justification
1: Attention
i. Ask the question These days which of the four seasons is it?
ii. Hospitaland countywill be spelt using Urdu letters.
iii. Only accept dates in the English calendar.
Participants and experts agreed with the proposed suggestions
developed with rationale from the guidelines.
2: Attention
i. Lemonis directly translated into Urdu.
ii. Keyis replaced with bell, spelt using Urdu letters.
iii. Ballis directly translated into Urdu
Participants and experts agreed with the proposed suggestions
developed with rationale from the guidelines for i and ii.
For iii, participants said belland ballsound far too alike and it was
decided that ball will be directly translated as the Urdu word for ballis
also one syllable.
3: Attention
Use the word minus, spelt using Urdu letters and the Urdu translation
for take away.
Participants and experts agreed with the proposed suggestions
developed with rationale from the guidelines.
4: Memory
Refer to Question 2: Attention Refer to Question 2: Attention
5a: Fluency (Letters)
Replace the letter Pwith the Urdu letter چ(chay). Participants agreed with both proposed suggestions developed with
rationale from the guidelines.
Between the letters چ(chay) and گ(gaaf), the former was then chosen
by experts after debate due to its unique sound as the latter could be
mistaken for the similar sounding letter ک(kaaf).
6: Memory
i. The first name Haroon is used. The last name Butt is used.
ii. The original ACE-III address will be retained and spelt using Urdu
letters.
Participants agreed with all the proposed suggestions developed with
rationale from the guidelines.
The name Haroon Butt was settled upon by the experts due to it
retaining the sounds and length of the original name, Harry Barnes.
7: Memory
i. The first, second and third question of the original ACE-III, Name of the
current Prime Minister,Name of the first female Prime Ministerand
Name of the USA presidentare retained.
ii. The fourth question will be replaced with Name of the princess who
died in a car crash in the 1990s.
Participants agreed with the proposed suggestions developed with
rationale from the guidelines.
The second question was retained according to the guidelines, despite
what was said in the focus groups. Experts ruled that British Urdu
speaking elderly should be aware of the first female Prime Minister of the
UK due to her prominence. She would have been Prime Minister at the
time when many of the elderly would have initially immigrated to the
UK and would therefore know of her.
The fourth question was replaced with a new suggestion by the experts.
Despite following the rationale of the guidelines Name of the British
currencyand Name of the city where (a Wonder of the World)is
locatedwere deemed too easy. It was also agreed that these
replacements do not retain the conceptual equivalence of the question.
Name of the princess who died in a car crash in the 1990retains the
concept, relating to a well know historical death relevant to the UK.
10: Language
i. Eccentricityis replaced with گوشگزار
ii. Unintelligibleis replaced with
نشیبوفراز
iii. The Indian Urdu replacements for caterpillarand statisticianare
retained.
Participants agreed with the proposed suggestions developed with
rationale from the guidelines.
Eccentricitywas replaced with a new suggestion by experts as the
words proposed were deemed too easy in comparison to the original
counterparts.
11: Language
The first saying, All that glitters is not gold, is translated into Urdu. The
second saying is replaced with the saying that translated to You cannot
clap with one hand.
Participants and experts agreed with the proposed suggestions
developed with rationale from the guidelines.
12: Language
i. Spoon is retained.
ii. Book is retained.
iii. Kangaroo is replaced with a goat.
iv. Penguin is replaced with a peacock.
v. Anchor is replaced with scissors.
Participants agreed with the proposed suggestions developed with
rationale from the guidelines.
For iii, experts decided that though sheep are more common in the UK,
British Urdu speakers would be familiar and able to recognise a goat.
For iv, experts selected a peacock as British Urdu speakers would be
Waheed et al. BMC Psychiatry (2020) 20:492 Page 9 of 13
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
15c and 16 would suffice with a direct translation. The
remaining questions required further cultural adaptation
that were deliberated over during these focus groups
(See Additional file 3for the proposed suggestions, de-
veloped using the guidelines).
Our consensus meeting was attended by two experienced
old age psychiatrists who were both bilingual British Pakis-
tanis. They had lived in both the UK and Pakistan and were
familiar with the cultures of both countries. They were also
both involved in clinical and research work relevant to
South Asian populations and were knowledgeable about
cognitive assessments, the ACE-III and the translation and
cultural adaptation of cognitive tests.
Step 5: developing the ACE-III Urdu
The suggestions finalised within this consensus meeting
and incorporated to form the ACE-III Urdu [19] can be
seen in Table 5.
Discussion
In this paper we detailed the methods involved in devel-
oping a set of guidelines to culturally adapt a specific
cognitive test; in this instance, the ACE-III. Following
this we also demonstrated how these guidelines can be
implemented to develop a cultural adaptation of the test
it was designed for, for a specific target population.
The combination of the systematic review and adap-
torsfeedback provided us with detailed information on
the cultural adaptation of the ACE-III, which was used
to develop question-by-question guidelines. These
present culturally adapted versions of questions of the
ACE-III, backed up by rationale.
We could potentially have incorporated more cultures
and languages through translators but due to limited re-
sources we were unable to produce questionnaires for 6
of the existing adaptations. We also had a poor return
rate of questionnaires (35%), reducing the amount of
additional information we could have received and
Table 5 ACE-III Urdu items determined from focus groups and consensus meeting (Continued)
Suggestions Justification
vi. Camel is retained.
vii. Harp is replaced with a dohl.
viii. Rhino is replaced with a bear.
ix. Barrel is replaced with a suitcase.
x. Crown is replaced with a cap.
xi. Crocodile is replaced with a tortoise.
xii. Accordion is replaced with a trumpet.
more familiar with it as opposed to a parrot. For v, experts settled on
scissors as they are a common household object.
For vii, experts selected a dohl due to familiarity with it in the culture.
For viii, experts proposed the new suggestion of a bear to replace the
rhino as it is a better known wild animal but still unique in the UK.
Experts ruled that a lion could be confused with other big cats such as a
tiger and a monkey is not as relative to the cultural context of the UK.
For ix, experts selected a suitcase as it is a form of container with a
specific purpose.
For x, experts proposed the new suggestion of a cap as it is a better
known form of headwear.
For xi, experts proposed the new suggestion of a tortoise, because it is a
better known wild animal that would be better recognised by the British
Urdu speaking elderly.
For xii, a trumpet was selected out of the proposed instruments by
participants from the focus groups as it was considered the most
uniquely shaped and easily recognisable by British Urdu speaking elderly.
13: Language
The following questions were asked regarding the images: Which one is
related to the head,Which one is found in the desert,Which one has a
shell on itand Which one is related to travel.
All questions were developed by the authors NM and WW according to
the images that were finalised, following the guidelines.
Participants and experts agreed with the proposed questions.
14: Language
The words used in the Indian Urdu ACE-III were retained. Participants and experts agreed with the proposed suggestions
developed with rationale from the guidelines
17: Visuospatial Abilities
The letters ،،،were selected. Participants and experts agreed with the proposed suggestions
developed with rationale from the guidelines
18: Memory
Refer to Question 6: Memory. Refer to Question 6: Memory.
19: Memory
Refer to Question 6: Memory.
The names Jamal Butt and Haroon Khan replaced the original names for
recognition.
Refer to Question 6: Memory.
Participants agreed with the proposed suggestions developed with
rationale from the guidelines.
The alternative names chosen by the experts retained the length,
familiarity and number of syllables.
Waheed et al. BMC Psychiatry (2020) 20:492 Page 10 of 13
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
compiled into the guidelines. Since our review and this
research more cultural adaptations of the ACE-III have
also been released that could not be included at this
time.
Therefore we must acknowledge that the usefulness of
these guidelines, and any guidelines for cognitive tests
developed via these methods, is dependent on how many
language and cultural versions of that test have been de-
veloped and how accessible these versions are. They are
also limited by how many current adaptors of these ver-
sions provide rich data on the rationale behind culturally
adapting questions that is often not conveyed through
publications alone.
However, the methods for forming these guidelines
are designed to allow for necessary updates as more in-
formation on cultural adaptation is acquired. In this way
newer additions can be made by simply adding in any
additional cultural adaptation steps found from new
publications on adaptations of the ACE-III. The inclu-
sion of feedback from adaptors also ensures we are not
limited strictly to just published information.
In addition, we were still able to account for 22 inter-
national languages and cultural contexts in our guide-
lines. Through the frequency of cultural adaptation
across questions, evidenced in our systematic review
[30] and from the feedback of the adaptors, we were also
able to determine which questions would most likely re-
quire cultural adaptation and which could suffice with a
simple translation. The guidelines highlight this. With
the accompanying rationale these guidelines would allow
future adaptors to conduct their own cultural adaptation
of the ACE-III and we have demonstrated this through
our cultural adaptation of the ACE-III for a British Urdu
speaking population.
While utilising the guidelines to develop potential
questions for an ACE-III Urdu [32] we acknowledged
British South Asianspreferences for certain English
words that are spelt with Urdu letters, such as,
county,belland ball, as opposed to translating
words into Urdu. This is attributed to the mixing of
English and Urdu that occurs within British Urdu
speaking communities. We also noted the influence of
the structure in which sentences are presented in
Urdu, and proposed the rephrasing of questions to
avoid confusion. This can be seen with the elabor-
ation of What is the season?to Which of the four
seasons is it?due to the Urdu word for weather and
season being the same.
We presented these suggestions within our focus
groups, allowing us to gather feedback from men and
women of a vast array of educational backgrounds
within the British Urdu speaking community. Through-
out the discussion participants were able to follow the
rationale provided by the guidelines when proposing
suggestions for the ACE-III Urdu. There was a notable
insistence on cultural adaptation for questions assessing
memory and language, with little focus on fluency and
visuospatial abilities. This is not to say that questions of
fluency and visuospatial abilities may not require cul-
tural adaptation, only that for this target population they
did not.
Following this we conducted a consensus meeting with
experts to review suggestions for the ACE-III Urdu pro-
posed during the focus groups. There was a general con-
sensus with the suggestions proposed, barring a few
items. Within Question 2: Attentionexperts preferred
ballbe translated directly into Urdu and in the case of
Question 7: Memory Retrogradeexperts decided to
retain three questions as they were. The proposed sug-
gestions were deemed too easy and the original ques-
tions were at a specific level of difficulty that was
required to measure retrograde memory.
Overall this demonstrated the usability of the guide-
lines and their role in developing cultural adaptations
of cognitive test questions alongside both potential
user and clinical expert feedback. These methods also
allowed us to develop a familiarity for the cognitive
test we were working with, and the intricacies of the
various test questions, in a way that general guide-
lines do not allow.
Through our methods we also developed the first ver-
sion of the ACE-III Urdu that can be used in the cultural
context of the UK. As mentioned earlier the next step
was to conduct a cultural validation, which assessed the
ACE-III Urdus understanding and acceptability across
older Urdu speaking British South Asians [32]. Further
efforts will now be undertaken to determine its perform-
ance in the detection of dementia through a psychomet-
ric validation.
Overall, these methods can be replicated for any cog-
nitive test to develop guidelines for adaptation specific
to that test. Similarly, they can also be implemented in
the same manner for any target population.
Conclusions
The guidelines are the first of their kind, and we have
provided an in depth account of the approach we
undertook to develop them and implement them.
This was not restricted to published literature but in-
corporated the first hand experiences of cultural
adaptation by existing adaptors of the ACE-III. This
accounted for adaptations that may not have had cor-
responding publications. Furthermore, instead of ad-
hering to general guidelines on cultural adaptation,
developing guidelines designed for the cultural adap-
tation of the ACE-III allowed for familiarity with its
individual questions.
Waheed et al. BMC Psychiatry (2020) 20:492 Page 11 of 13
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
These guidelines for the ACE-III can now be imple-
mented in the same manner we conducted for other
language and cultural groups, with the added incorp-
oration of potential user and clinical expert feedback.
The implications of this methodology can also be
taken forward to develop guidelines in the same man-
ner for other existing cognitive tests. This would help
to alleviate issues with mismatched culture across di-
verse populations that we currently see within health-
care and research settings.
Supplementary information
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at https://doi.org/10.
1186/s12888-020-02893-6.
Additional file 1: Supplementary Material- Appendix A1. Sample
Questionnaire- Cultural adaptation process of the Italian ACE-III.
Additional file 2: Supplementary Material- Appendix A2. The first
edition of our guidelines on translating and culturally adapting the ACE-
III.
Additional file 3: Supplementary Material- Appendix A3. Table on
Proposed ACE-III questions developed from the guidelines.
Abbreviations
UK: United Kingdom; 6 CIT: six item cognitive impairment test; TYM: Test
Your Memory; MMSE: Mini Mental State Examination; MoCA: Montreal
Cognitive Aessment; ACE-III: Addenbrookes Cognitive Examination Version III;
USA: United States of America; ACE-R: ACE Revised; MCAR: Manchester
Cultural Adaptation Reporting Questionnaire; NeuRA: Neuroscience Research
Australia
Acknowledgements
Alex J Mitchell, Andrew J Larner, Emilia Sitek, Gwerfyl Roberts, JP Newman,
Jordia MA Antem, Norbert Kovacs, Shailaja Mekala, Shuvarna Alladi and Tarik
Qassem in providing completed questionnaires.
Ms. Nadia Siddiqui, who acted as our liase for recruitment, Womens Voices
and the Greater Manchester Pakistani Association (GMPA).
Authorscontributions
The research question was formulated and the study designed by WW and
NM. The study was carried out by NM, with assistance from AM and MP,
supervised by WW. The qualitative data analysis was conducted by NM, with
assistance from MWW, supervised by WW. NM wrote the manuscript. All
authors read ad approved the manuscript.
Funding
There were no sponsorship or funding arrangements relating to this
research.
Availability of data and materials
Not Applicable.
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Ethical approval was waived for this research by the University of Manchester
Research Ethics Committee (UREC). This was because the portion of the
research requiring participation of members of the public was an act of
public involvement for research on developing an Urdu version of the ACE-III
that was used in a cultural validation study [19]. However, informed consent
was still obtained from all participants through a signed consent form.
Consent for publication
Not Applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Author details
1
Centre for Primary Care and Health Services Research, The University of
Manchester, Suite 8, 5th Floor, Williamson Building Oxford Road, Manchester
M13 9PL, UK.
2
The University of Leicester, Leicester, UK.
3
Greater Manchester
Mental Health NHS Foundation Trust, Salford, UK.
Received: 13 August 2019 Accepted: 24 September 2020
References
1. World Health Organization. Dementia Key Facts. 2020.
2. Alzheimers Society. Alzheimers Societys view on demography. 2020.
3. Gracey DJ, Morris RG. Neuropsychological assessment in dementia.
Psychiatry. 2007;6(12):498502.
4. Parker C, Philp I. Screening for cognitive impairment among older people in
black and minority ethnic groups. Age Ageing. 2004;33(5):44752.
5. Kipps CM, Hodges JR. Cognitive assessment for clinicians. J Neurol
Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2005;11(2):2230.
6. Sheehan B. Assessment scales in dementia. Ther Adv Neurol Disord. 2012
Nov;5(6):34958.
7. Prince M, Knapp M, Guerchet M, McCrone P, Prina M, Comas-Herrera A,
Wittenberg R, Adelaja B, Hu B, King D, Rehill A, Salimkumar D. Dementia UK
update. Alzheimers Soc. 2014:179.
8. Tsoi KK, Chan JY, Hirai HW, Wong SY, Kwok TC. Cognitive tests to detect
dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Intern Med. 2015;
175(9):14508.
9. Brooke P, Bullock R. Validation of a 6 item cognitive impairment test with a
view to primary care usage. Int J Geriatric Psychiatry. 1999;14(11):93640.
10. Brodaty H, Moore CM. The clock drawing test for dementia of the
Alzheimer's type: a comparison of three scoring methods in a memory
disorders clinic. Int J Geriatric Psychiatry. 1997;12(6):61927..
11. Borson S, Scanlan J, Brush M, Vitaliano P, Dokmak A. The mini-cog: a
cognitive vital signsmeasure for dementia screening in multi-lingual
elderly. Int J Geriatric Psychiatry. 2000;15(11):10217.
12. Brown J, Pengas G, Dawson K, Brown LA, Clatworthy P. Self administered
cognitive screening test (TYM) for detection of Alzheimers disease: cross
sectional study. Bmj. 2009;338:b2030.
13. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state: a practical method
for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res.
1975;12(3):18998.
14. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I,
Cummings JL, Chertkow H. The Montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a
brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;
53(4):6959.
15. Hsieh S, Schubert S, Hoon C, Mioshi E, Hodges JR. Validation of the
Addenbrooke's cognitive examination III in frontotemporal dementia and
Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2013;36(34):24250.
16. Tuerk R, Sauer J. Dementia in a black and minority ethnic population:
characteristics of presentation to an inner London memory service. BJPsych
bulletin. 2015 Aug;39(4):1626.
17. Fillenbaum GA, Heyman A, Williams K, Prosnitz B, Burchett B. Sensitivity and
specificity of standardized screens of cognitive impairment and dementia
among elderly black and white community residents. J Clin Epidemiol. 1990
Jan 1;43(7):65160.
18. Ganguli M, Hendrie HC. Screening for cognitive impairment and depression
in ethnically diverse older populations. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2005;
19(4):2758.
19. Office for National Statistics, National Records of Scotland, Northern Ireland
Statistics and Research Agency. 2011 Census aggregate data. UK Data
Service June 2016..
20. Mirza N. Those Leaflets Clinical Psychology Forum; 2019. p. 318.
21. Rees P, Wohland P, Norman P, Boden P. A local analysis of ethnic group
population trends and projections for the UK. J Popul Res. 2011;28(23):14983.
22. Mukadam N, Cooper C, Livingston G. A systematic review of ethnicity and
pathways to care in dementia. Int J Geriatric Psychiatry. 2011;26(1):1220.
23. Rait G, Burns A, Baldwin R, Morley M, Chew-Graham C, St Leger AS.
Validating screening instruments for cognitive impairment in older south
Asians in the United Kingdom. Int J Geriatric Psychiatry. 2000;15(1):5462.
24. Beaton DE, Bombardier C, Guillemin F, Ferraz MB. Guidelines for the process of
cross-cultural adaptation of self-report measures. Spine. 2000;25(24):318691.
Waheed et al. BMC Psychiatry (2020) 20:492 Page 12 of 13
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
25. Flaherty JA, Gaviria FM, Pathak D, Mitchell T, Wintrob R, Richman JA, Birz S.
Developing instruments for cross-cultural psychiatric research. J Nervous
Mental Dis. 1988.
26. Khan F, Tadros G. Complexity in cognitive assessment of elderly British
minority ethnic groups: cultural perspective. Dementia. 2014;13(4):46782.
27. Richards MA, Abas ME. Cross-cultural approaches to dementia and
depression. In Ethnicity: An Agenda for Mental Health. (D. Bhugra and V.
Bahl, Eds). London: Gaskell.
28. Guillemin F, Bombardier C, Beaton D. Cross-cultural adaptation of health-
related quality of life measures: literature review and proposed guidelines. J
Clin Epidemiol. 1993;46(12):141732.
29. Bohnstedt M, Fox PJ, Kohatsu ND. Correlates of mini-mental status
examination scores among elderly demented patients: the influence of
race-ethnicity. J Clin Epidemiol. 1994;47(12):13817.
30. Mirza N, Panagioti M, Waheed MW, Waheed W. Reporting of the translation
and cultural adaptation procedures of the Addenbrookes cognitive
examination version III (ACE-III) and its predecessors: a systematic review.
BMC Med Res Methodol. 2017;17(1):141.
31. Maneesriwongul W, Dixon JK. Instrument translation process: a methods
review. J Adv Nurs. 2004;48(2):17586.
32. Mirza N, Panagioti M, Waheed W. Cultural validation of the Addenbrookes
cognitive examination version III Urdu for the British Urdu-speaking
population: a qualitative assessment using cognitive interviewing. BMJ
Open. 2018;8(12):e021057.
33. Cheung G, Clugston A, Croucher M, Malone D, Mau E, Sims A, Gee S.
Performance of three cognitive screening tools in a sample of older new
Zealanders. Int Psychogeriatr. 2015;27(6):9819.
34. Mathuranath PS, Nestor PJ, Berrios GE, Rakowicz W, Hodges JR. A brief
cognitive test battery to differentiate Alzheimer's disease and
frontotemporal dementia. Neurology. 2000;55(11):161320.
35. Mioshi E, Dawson K, Mitchell J, Arnold R, Hodges JR. The Addenbrooke's
cognitive examination revised (ACE-R): a brief cognitive test battery for
dementia screening. Int J Geriatric Psychiatry. 2006;21(11):107885.
36. Machado A, Baeta É, Pimentel P, Peixoto B. Psychometric and normative
indicators of the version of the Addenbrookes cognitive examination-III.
Preliminary study on a sample of health subjects Acta Neuropsychologica.
2015;13(2).
37. Guiu JM, de Bobadilla RF, Escudero G, Pérez JP, Cortés A, Rodríguez EM,
Salgado MV, Ramos TM, Kulisevsky J, Guiu JM. Validación de la versión
española del test Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination III para el
diagnóstico de demencia. Neurología: Publicación oficial de la Sociedad
Española de Neurología. 2015;30(9):54551.
38. Qassem T, Khater MS, Emara T, Tawfik HM, Rasheedy D, Mohammedin AS,
Tolba MF, Aziz KA. Translation and cross cultural adaptation of the
Addenbrookes cognitive examination III into Egyptian Arabic In Royal
College of Psychiatrists International Congress; 2014.
39. Neuroscience Research Australia. 2016. [online] Available at: https://www.neura.
edu.au/research-clinic/frontier/research/downloads/. [Accessed 10 Nov 2016].
40. Central Intelligence Agency Library. The World Factbook. 2016. [online]
Available at: https://www.cia.gov/library/publications/the-world-factbook/
wfbExt/region_sas.html [Accessed 10 Nov 2016].
41. South Asian Concern. Who are South Asians. 2016. [online] Available at:
http://southasianconcern.org/south-asians/who-are-south-asians/ [Accessed
10 Nov 2016].
42. Larner AJ, Mitchell AJ. A meta-analysis of the accuracy of the Addenbrooke's
cognitive examination (ACE) and the Addenbrooke's cognitive examination-
revised (ACE-R) in the detection of dementia. Int Psychogeriatr. 2014 Apr;
26(4):55563.
PublishersNote
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional affiliations.
Waheed et al. BMC Psychiatry (2020) 20:492 Page 13 of 13
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
... As regards cognitive testing, our team demonstrated that, for every cognitive test, incorporation of literature on that test and feedback from adaptors of that test allows the development of a set of guidelines on culturally adapting each of that test's questions. 33 These methods were illustrated through forming cultural adaptation guidelines for the Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination Version III 33 and we later replicated this for the Montreal Cognitive Assessment. 34 Following this methodology, a cognitive test can be translated and culturally adapted for any language and cultural context using its guidelines. ...
... As regards cognitive testing, our team demonstrated that, for every cognitive test, incorporation of literature on that test and feedback from adaptors of that test allows the development of a set of guidelines on culturally adapting each of that test's questions. 33 These methods were illustrated through forming cultural adaptation guidelines for the Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination Version III 33 and we later replicated this for the Montreal Cognitive Assessment. 34 Following this methodology, a cognitive test can be translated and culturally adapted for any language and cultural context using its guidelines. ...
... 34 Following this methodology, a cognitive test can be translated and culturally adapted for any language and cultural context using its guidelines. 33,34 Such newly adapted tests can then be culturally validated, a process in which they are administered to healthy controls within the target population and a cognitive interviewing approach is undertaken. 35 This determines whether the questions in the newly adapted version are both understandable and acceptable in those without cognitive impairment and ascertains that there is no bias in the language and cultural context that may cause poor performance. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Low- and middle-income countries contribute to the majority of dementia and mild cognitive impairment cases worldwide, yet cognitive tests for diagnosis are designed for Western cultures. Language and cultural discrepancies mean that translated tests are not always reliable or valid. We propose a model for culturally adapting cognitive tests, one step of which is to assess the quality of any translation and cultural adaptation undertaken. We developed the Manchester Translation Evaluation Checklist (MTEC) to act as a tool for quality assessment and demonstrated its use by assessing a popular cognitive test that had been adapted. Aims: Assess quality of the translation and cultural adaptation of the Urdu Mini-Mental State Examination developed for a Pakistani population. Method: Two raters completed the MTEC for the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) Urdu and compared feedback. All authors were fluent in English and Urdu and familiar with Pakistani culture. Results: Raters had 78.5% agreement across the MTEC. The MMSE Urdu was appropriately translated and retained grammar and verb tense, but three questions had spelling errors. Across 20 MMSE questions, 5 required further cultural adaptation because the questions were not understandable in daily use, comfortable to answer, relevant to the language and culture, and relevant to original concepts. Conclusions: The MTEC highlighted errors in the MMSE Urdu and demonstrated how this tool can be used to improve it. Future studies could employ the MTEC to improve existing translated measures of health assessment, particularly cognitive tests, and act as a quality check when developing new adaptations of tests and before psychometric validation.
... Waheed et al 38 proposed that every cognitive test should have its own set of specific guidelines on culturally adapting each of its questions. These would be developed through an incorporation of previous literature and feedback from those who have previously adapted it. ...
... These would be developed through an incorporation of previous literature and feedback from those who have previously adapted it. 38 At present, there are only one set of guidelines that have been developed for the cultural adaptation of a specific cognitive test. 38 These are for the Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination Version III (ACE-III), a gold standard for the diagnostic accuracy of cognitive impairment and dementia. ...
... 38 At present, there are only one set of guidelines that have been developed for the cultural adaptation of a specific cognitive test. 38 These are for the Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination Version III (ACE-III), a gold standard for the diagnostic accuracy of cognitive impairment and dementia. 39,40 These guidelines were developed through a multi-stage qualitative study combining findings from a systematic review on the translation and cultural adaptations of the ACE-III with feedback received via questionnaires from previous adaptors of the ACE-III. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Ethnic minorities in countries such as the UK are at increased risk of dementia or minor cognitive impairment. Despite this, cognitive tests used to provide a timely diagnosis for these conditions demonstrate performance bias in these groups, because of cultural context. They require adaptation that accounts for language and culture beyond translation. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) is one such test that has been adapted for multiple cultures. Aims We followed previously used methodology for culturally adapting cognitive tests to develop guidelines for translating and culturally adapting the MoCA. Method We conducted a scoping review of publications on different versions of the MoCA. We extracted their translation and cultural adaptation procedures. We also distributed questionnaires to adaptors of the MoCA for data on the procedures they undertook to culturally adapt their respective versions. Results Our scoping review found 52 publications and highlighted seven steps for translating the MoCA. We received 17 responses from adaptors on their cultural adaptation procedures, with rationale justifying them. We combined data from the scoping review and the adaptors’ feedback to form the guidelines that state how each question of the MoCA has been previously adapted for different cultural contexts and the reasoning behind it. Conclusions This paper details our development of cultural adaptation guidelines for the MoCA that future adaptors can use to adapt the MoCA for their own languages or cultures. It also replicates methods previously used and demonstrates how these methods can be used for the cultural adaptation of other cognitive tests.
... Administration time is about 15 min, and scoring time is about 5 min. The Urdu translation of the ACE-III has been culturally adapted and validated for Urdu speakers by N. Mirza et al. (2018; see also Waheed et al., 2020). This test is available in many languages online for clinical use. ...
Article
Purpose This aim of this tutorial was to review assessment tools for speech-language pathologists working with Urdu speakers in the United States. Method We outlined cultural and linguistic considerations for speech-language pathologists to consider when assessing Urdu speakers. We also reviewed available Urdu-language tests for pediatric and adult populations by their assessment area and evaluated whether they had been validated for Urdu speakers. Results Speech-language pathologists should consider the impact of cultural and linguistic differences when planning assessment. In particular, many Urdu speakers are Muslim; hence, clinicians unfamiliar with the religion should open communication with clients about assessment preferences. Testing instruments covering the major areas of speech-language pathology are available for Urdu speakers in the United States. Conclusions Speech-language pathologists can use the tools presented in this tutorial to evaluate both body impairments for Urdu speakers and the impact on an individual's participation. A summary of resources for Urdu speakers with links to assessments is provided in Supplemental Material S1 . Supplemental Material https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.24147564
... The second issue is that the Neurotrack battery's cultural adaptation did not cover all of the assessments' components (e.g., the items on display when taking the exam). It is worth noting that for adapting cognitive assessments to different cultural contexts, there are currently no established standards guidelines, or criteria (136). Nevertheless, an exact transcribing methodology was performed to consider any potential cultural context-related performance bias. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives: This study aimed to investigate the effects of Ramadan diurnal intermittent fasting (RDIF) on cognitive performance, sleep quality, daytime sleepiness, and insomnia in physically active and sedentary elderly individuals. Methods: A total of 58 participants (62.93±3.99 yrs) were assigned to one of the following two groups: a sedentary group (control group) who observed Ramadan (n=32) and a physically active group (n=26) who continued to train while observing Ramadan. Participants were assessed two weeks before Ramadan and during the fourth week of Ramadan. On each occasion, participants completed a digital assessment of their cognitive performance and responded to the Pittsburgh sleep quality index (PSQI), the insomnia severity index (ISI) and the Epworth sleepiness scale (ESS) questionnaires to assess sleep parameters. Results: Compared to before Ramadan, performance in executive function (p= 0.035), attention (p= 0.005), inhibition (p= 0.02), associative memory (p= 0.041), and recognition memory (p= 0.025) increased significantly during Ramadan in the physically active group. For the sedentary group, associative learning performance decreased (p= 0.041), whilst performances in the remaining domains remained unchanged during Ramadan. Global PSQI, ISI, and ESS scores indicated both groups suffered from poor sleep quality and excessive daytime sleepiness, with significantly higher negative effects of RDIF observed in the sedentary group. Conclusion: Older adults who continue to train at least three times per week during Ramadan may improve their cognitive performance, despite the impairment of sleep quality. Future studies in older adults during Ramadan including objective measures of sleep (e.g., polysomnography, actigraphy) and brain function (e.g., functional magnetic resonance imaging) are warranted. Keywords: Exercise; Fasting; Aging; Neuropsychological Tests; Sleep
... Specific challenges may arise when utilizing neuropsychological tests in many LMIC settings for bilingualism and dementia research. Traditionally these tests have been derived for educated and English-speaking western populations and may have limited applicability to other cultures (76,77). Although, Alladi and colleagues (68) successfully used culturally and linguistically amended versions of the Addenbrookes Cognitive Examination and Dementia Rating Scale there are other specific challenges to consider. ...
Article
Full-text available
Dementia is a global public health priority which cost global societies $818 billion in 2015 and is disproportionately impacting low and middle-income countries (LMICs). With limited availability of disease modifying drugs to treat Alzheimer's disease (AD), researchers have increasingly focused on preventative strategies which may promote healthy cognitive aging and mitigate the risk of cognitive impairment in aging. Lifelong bilingualism has been presented as both a highly debated and promising cognitive reserve factor which has been associated with better cognitive outcomes in aging. A recent metanalysis has suggested that bilingual individuals present on average 4.05 years later with the clinical features of AD than monolinguals. Bilinguals are also diagnosed with AD ~2.0 years later than monolingual counterparts. In this perspective piece we critically evaluate the findings of this metanalysis and consider the specific implications of these findings to LMICs. Furthermore, we appraise the major epidemiological studies conducted globally on bilingualism and the onset of dementia. We consider how both impactful and robust studies of bilingualism and cognition in older age may be conducted in LMICs. Given the limited expenditure and resources available in LMICs and minimal successes of clinical trials of disease modifying drugs we propose that bilingualism should be positioned as an important and specific public health strategy for maintaining healthy cognitive aging in LMICs. Finally, we reflect upon the scope of implementing bilingualism within the education systems of LMICs and the promotion of bilingualism as a healthy cognitive aging initiative within government policy.
Chapter
Full-text available
Dementia is a global health priority, with huge human and financial costs. Over 50 million people worldwide live with dementia, a figure which is set to rise to 130 million by 2050. The current cost of dementia to the global economy is a staggering $1 trillion per year. Around 60% of people with dementia live in low- and middle-income countries (LMICs), countries in which population ageing is occurring at the most rapid rate and which already have the least capacity to cope. Key challenges in these regions include limited reliable epidemiological data, poor public understanding of dementia, inadequate health and social care systems and a lack of coherent, national or transnational dementia strategies. These challenges require concerted effort towards innovative, collaborative and technology-driven solutions involving clinicians, academics, engineers, economists and policymakers. LMICs also present new opportunities for globally relevant dementia research: for example, the study of novel genetic and environmental factors that modify vulnerability and resilience to disease, as well as innovative approaches to dementia diagnosis and care in resource-poor situations. Whilst some established institutions in the developing world are already engaged in exploring these research opportunities, there is enormous potential for benefit from greater international exposure and collaboration. The 2015 World Health Organization First Ministerial Conference on dementia concluded that “A sustained global effort is required to promote action on dementia and address the challenges posed by dementia and its impacts. No single country, sector or organization can tackle these challenges alone.” The aim of this Research Topic is to bring together, in one volume, research addressing problems that specifically relate to the impact of dementia in the developing world. In doing so, we hope to identify new challenges for future investigation. We welcome submissions from a broad range of disciplines and are particularly interested in work that illustrates the collaborative coming together of disciplines and centers. Topics that will be considered include, but are not restricted to, epidemiology, diagnosis (clinical, neuropsychology, biomarkers, imaging), genetics, modifiable risk factors, treatment, diversity and under-represented populations, health economics, social science and public policy. Whilst submissions covering the full range of article types are encouraged, we are particularly interested in Original Research on dementia that specifically addresses issues relating to the LMIC context.
Article
Background: There has been increasing evidence and support for the use of digital technology in the cognitive health field. Despite the growing use of innovative digital technology to assess cognitive function, such technology remains scarce in Arabic countries, particularly in Tunisia. Objective: To investigate the effectiveness of a digitally delivered cognitive assessment battery in differentiating varying degrees of cognitive function in older Tunisian adults. Methods: One hundred fifty-five Tunisian older adults (age: 62.24±7.52 years) were assigned to one of four groups: healthy controls (HC), at-risk (AR), mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and Alzheimer's disease (AD). Participants completed a translated version of the Neurotrack digital cognitive battery. Results: The AD group performed significantly lower on the associative learning (p = 0.01) and associative memory assessments (p = 0.002), than the HC and AR groups. The AD group also performed worse on the inhibition measure (p = 0.008) than the HC, AR, and MCI groups. For recognition memory, the was a significant difference between all four groups (p < 0.0005), with AD having the lowest scores followed by the MCI, AR, and HC groups, respectively. There were no significant differences observed on attention, executive function and processing speed performance between the four groups (p > 0.05). Conclusion: The use of digital technology appears to be a viable solution to current cognitive assessment challenges for assessing cognitive function in a Tunisian population. These findings provide further support for the use of digital technology in cognitive assessment, particularly in understudied populations.
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive screening tests are culture bound and have been shown to perform differently depending on the culture, even with adequate translation. Khan et al examine in detail ways in which the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) has been modified for different languages and cultures and produce a systematic guide for future modifications. However, questions arise regarding the availability of the MoCA. Other important issues in the transcultural use and modification of neuropsychiatric tests include providing a culturally safe context for testing, understanding the cultural context in which screening takes place and assessing other neuropsychiatric conditions, which may manifest differently in different cultural contexts and which affect cognition.
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Our research determined whether the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination Version III (ACE-III) Urdu eliminated cultural bias through a qualitative assessment of its understanding and acceptability within the British Urdu-speaking population, employing cognitive interviews. Method We aimed to recruit 25 participants fluent in speaking and writing Urdu, over the age of 60 years, able to give informed consent and who did not have a history of cognitive impairment. Participants were administered the ACE-III Urdu, and cognitive interviews were conducted, which involve obtaining verbal data on the individual’s perception of the assessment overall, their understanding of the mental processes behind how they interpreted questions within the assessment and how they produced appropriate responses. This allows us to gauge the participants’ overall thoughts on the Urdu ACE-III before applying question-formatted prompts to every ACE-III Urdu item. Results We recruited 25 participants, 12 women (48%), ranging from ages 60 years to 85 years (M=69.12, SD=6.57), all from Greater Manchester. Participants came from varied socioeconomic backgrounds, with 22 identifying as Pakistani, one as British Pakistani and two as East African. Across 19 ACE-III Urdu items, 7 required changes based on participant feedback: item 5a: fluency; items 6, 18 and 19: memory; items 12 and 13: language; and item 17: visuospatial abilities. The need for some of these changes was realised after 21 participants, due to persistently reoccurring issues, and these were applied before the last four participants. Overall, the ACE-III Urdu was considered easy and straightforward by all 25 participants, who understood items and felt the ACE-III Urdu was appropriate, not just for them, but for British Urdu speakers in general. Conclusion Our cognitive interviews determined the ACE-III Urdu was acceptable, especially with regards to cultural context, but further changes were made to ensure understanding. Therefore, we adapted the ACE-III Urdu in accordance with feedback, resulting in our finalised version being culturally validated.
Article
Full-text available
Background The ACE-III, a gold standard for screening cognitive impairment, is restricted by language and culture, with no uniform set of guidelines for its adaptation. To develop guidelines a compilation of all the adaptation procedures undertaken by adapters of the ACE-III and its predecessors is needed. Methods We searched EMBASE, Medline and PsychINFO and screened publications from a previous review. We included publications on adapted versions of the ACE-III and its predecessors, extracting translation and cultural adaptation procedures and assessing their quality. ResultsWe deemed 32 papers suitable for analysis. 7 translation steps were identified and we determined which items of the ACE-III are culturally dependent. Conclusions This review lists all adaptations of the ACE, ACE-R and ACE-III, rates the reporting of their adaptation procedures and summarises adaptation procedures into steps that can be undertaken by adapters.
Article
Full-text available
Aims and method To examine data on referrals to an inner-city London memory service to explore any differences in referral rates, cognitive assessments and stages of dementia at presentation between ethnic groups. Results African–Caribbean patients were well represented in the memory service. They were diagnosed with dementia on average 4.5 years younger than their White British counterparts and were more likely to be diagnosed with a vascular or mixed type dementia. However, scores on initial cognitive testing were significantly lower in the African–Caribbean group, possibly representing more advanced disease at presentation. Clinical implications Initiatives to access Black and minority ethnic populations earlier in the course of their illness should be considered. Professionals need to consider the potential for cultural bias in memory testing and diagnosing dementia in these populations, and the importance of cultural competency in assessments.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Recently a third version of the Addenbrooke‘s Cognitive Examination (ACE-III) was developed in order to improve previous versions. The present work aims to determine some psychometric properties ofACE-III, such as: internal reliability of ACE-III validity evidence pertaining to the instrument’s internal structure, convergent and divergent validity evidence. Additionally, the influence of sociode mo graphic variables on the test’s performance was studied and normative tables for the Portuguese version of ACE-III were produced. Material/Methods: The study enrolled a convenience sample made up of healthy volunteers (n=100) without any subjective complain of memory loss and completely independent in daily life activities. Internal reliability of ACE-III was determined trough Cronbach’s alpha, validity evidence pertaining to the instrument’s internal structure was established by using inter domain correlations, convergent and divergent validity were determined by correlations between. Multiple linear regressions were performed to determine the predictive variables and to generate normative equations. Results: Results point to a good construct validity, with acceptable reliability (α=.732) and significant inter-domains correlations. The Portuguese version of ACE-III also revealed convergent and divergent validity. Multiple linear regression determined age and education as the sole predictors of the ACE-III results. Normative tables were created based on those variables. Conclusions: The availability of ACE-III normative equations based on a healthy sample according to age and education enables the use of a brief screening tool for cognitive functioning.
Article
Full-text available
Dementia is a global public health problem. The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) is a proprietary instrument for detecting dementia, but many other tests are also available. To evaluate the diagnostic performance of all cognitive tests for the detection of dementia. Literature searches were performed on the list of dementia screening tests in MEDLINE, EMBASE, and PsychoINFO from the earliest available dates stated in the individual databases until September 1, 2014. Because Google Scholar searches literature with a combined ranking algorithm on citation counts and keywords in each article, our literature search was extended to Google Scholar with individual test names and dementia screening as a supplementary search. Studies were eligible if participants were interviewed face to face with respective screening tests, and findings were compared with criterion standard diagnostic criteria for dementia. Bivariate random-effects models were used, and the area under the summary receiver-operating characteristic curve was used to present the overall performance. Sensitivity, specificity, and positive and negative likelihood ratios were the main outcomes. Eleven screening tests were identified among 149 studies with more than 49 000 participants. Most studies used the MMSE (n = 102) and included 10 263 patients with dementia. The combined sensitivity and specificity for detection of dementia were 0.81 (95% CI, 0.78-0.84) and 0.89 (95% CI, 0.87-0.91), respectively. Among the other 10 tests, the Mini-Cog test and Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination-Revised (ACE-R) had the best diagnostic performances, which were comparable to that of the MMSE (Mini-Cog, 0.91 sensitivity and 0.86 specificity; ACE-R, 0.92 sensitivity and 0.89 specificity). Subgroup analysis revealed that only the Montreal Cognitive Assessment had comparable performance to the MMSE on detection of mild cognitive impairment with 0.89 sensitivity and 0.75 specificity. Besides the MMSE, there are many other tests with comparable diagnostic performance for detecting dementia. The Mini-Cog test and the ACE-R are the best alternative screening tests for dementia, and the Montreal Cognitive Assessment is the best alternative for mild cognitive impairment.
Article
Background/aims: The aims of this study were to validate the newly developed version of the Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination (ACE-III) against standardised neuropsychological tests and its predecessor (ACE-R) in early dementia. Methods: A total of 61 patients with dementia (frontotemporal dementia, FTD, n = 33, and Alzheimer's disease, AD, n = 28) and 25 controls were included in the study. Results: ACE-III cognitive domains correlated significantly with standardised neuropsychological tests used in the assessment of attention, language, verbal memory and visuospatial function. The ACE-III also compared very favourably with its predecessor, the ACE-R, with similar levels of sensitivity and specificity. Conclusion: The results of this study provide objective validation of the ACE-III as a screening tool for cognitive deficits in FTD and AD.
Article
There is a clear need for brief, but sensitive and specific, cognitive screening instruments as evidenced by the popularity of the Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination (ACE). We aimed to validate an improved revision (the ACE-R) which incorporates five sub-domain scores (orientation/attention, memory, verbal fluency, language and visuo-spatial). Standard tests for evaluating dementia screening tests were applied. A total of 241 subjects participated in this study (Alzheimer's disease=67, frontotemporal dementia=55, dementia of Lewy Bodies=20; mild cognitive impairment-MCI=36; controls=63). Reliability of the ACE-R was very good (alpha coefficient=0.8). Correlation with the Clinical Dementia Scale was significant (r=-0.321, p<0.001). Two cut-offs were defined (88: sensitivity=0.94, specificity=0.89; 82: sensitivity=0.84, specificity=1.0). Likelihood ratios of dementia were generated for scores between 88 and 82: at a cut-off of 82 the likelihood of dementia is 100:1. A comparison of individual age and education matched groups of MCI, AD and controls placed the MCI group performance between controls and AD and revealed MCI patients to be impaired in areas other than memory (attention/orientation, verbal fluency and language). The ACE-R accomplishes standards of a valid dementia screening test, sensitive to early cognitive dysfunction.
Article
IMPORTANCE: Dementia is a global public health problem. The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) is a proprietary instrument for detecting dementia, but many other tests are also available. OBJECTIVE: To evaluate the diagnostic performance of all cognitive tests for the detection of dementia. DATA SOURCES:Literature searches were performed on the list of dementia screening tests in MEDLINE, EMBASE, and PsychoINFO from the earliest available dates stated in the individual databases until September 1, 2014. Because Google Scholar searches literature with a combined ranking algorithm on citation counts and keywords in each article, our literature search was extended to Google Scholar with individual test names and dementia screening as a supplementary search. STUDY SELECTION: Studies were eligible if participants were interviewed face to face with respective screening tests, and findings were compared with criterion standard diagnostic criteria for dementia. Bivariate random-effects models were used, and the area under the summary receiver-operating characteristic curve was used to present the overall performance. MAIN OUTCOMES AND MEASURES: Sensitivity, specificity, and positive and negative likelihood ratios were the main outcomes. RESULTS: Eleven screening tests were identified among 149 studies with more than 49 000 participants. Most studies used the MMSE (n = 102) and included 10 263 patients with dementia. The combined sensitivity and specificity for detection of dementia were 0.81 (95% CI, 0.78-0.84) and 0.89 (95%CI, 0.87-0.91), respectively. Among the other 10 tests, the Mini-Cog test and Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination–Revised (ACE-R) had the best diagnostic performances, which were comparable to that of the MMSE (Mini-Cog, 0.91 sensitivity and 0.86 specificity; ACE-R, 0.92 sensitivity and 0.89 specificity). Subgroup analysis revealed that only the Montreal Cognitive Assessment had comparable performance to the MMSE on detection of mild cognitive impairment with 0.89 sensitivity and 0.75 specificity. CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE: Besides the MMSE, there are many other tests with comparable diagnostic performance for detecting dementia. The Mini-Cog test and the ACE-R are the best alternative screening tests for dementia, and the Montreal Cognitive Assessment is the best alternative for mild cognitive impairment.