Access to this full-text is provided by PLOS.
Content available from PLOS ONE
This content is subject to copyright.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Framing COVID-19: How we conceptualize
and discuss the pandemic on Twitter
Philipp WickeID
1
*, Marianna M. BolognesiID
2
1Department of Computer Science, University College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland, 2Department of Modern
Languages, Literatures, and Cultures, University Bologna, Bologna, Italy
*philipp.wicke@ucdconnect.ie
Abstract
Doctors and nurses in these weeks and months are busy in the trenches, fighting against a
new invisible enemy: Covid-19. Cities are locked down and civilians are besieged in their
own homes, to prevent the spreading of the virus. War-related terminology is commonly
used to frame the discourse around epidemics and diseases. The discourse around the cur-
rent epidemic makes use of war-related metaphors too, not only in public discourse and in
the media, but also in the tweets written by non-experts of mass communication. We hereby
present an analysis of the discourse around #Covid-19, based on a large corpus tweets
posted on Twitter during March and April 2020. Using topic modelling we first analyze the
topics around which the discourse can be classified. Then, we show that the WAR framing
is used to talk about specific topics, such as the virus treatment, but not others, such as the
effects of social distancing on the population. We then measure and compare the popularity
of the WAR frame to three alternative figurative frames (MONSTER, STORM and TSU-
NAMI) and a literal frame used as control (FAMILY). The results show that while the FAMILY
frame covers a wider portion of the corpus, among the figurative frames WAR, a highly con-
ventional one, is the frame used most frequently. Yet, this frame does not seem to be apt to
elaborate the discourse around some aspects involved in the current situation. Therefore,
we conclude, in line with previous suggestions, a plethora of framing options—or a meta-
phor menu—may facilitate the communication of various aspects involved in the Covid-19-
related discourse on the social media, and thus support civilians in the expression of their
feelings, opinions and beliefs during the current pandemic.
Introduction
On December 31, 2019, Chinese authorities alerted the World Health Organization of pneu-
monia cases in Wuhan City, within the Hubei province in China. The cause, they initially said,
was unknown, and the disease was first referred to as 2019-nCoV and then named COVID-19.
The next day, the Huanan seafood market was closed, because it was suspected to be the source
of the unknown disease, as some of the patients presenting with the pneumonia-like illness
were dealers or vendors at that market. Since then, the disease has spread quickly throughout
China, and from there to the rest of the world. SARS-CoV-2 is the name of the virus
PLOS ONE
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 1 / 24
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Wicke P, Bolognesi MM (2020) Framing
COVID-19: How we conceptualize and discuss the
pandemic on Twitter. PLoS ONE 15(9): e0240010.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010
Editor: Panos Athanasopoulos, Lancaster
University, UNITED KINGDOM
Received: April 16, 2020
Accepted: September 17, 2020
Published: September 30, 2020
Peer Review History: PLOS recognizes the
benefits of transparency in the peer review
process; therefore, we enable the publication of
all of the content of peer review and author
responses alongside final, published articles. The
editorial history of this article is available here:
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010
Copyright: ©2020 Wicke, Bolognesi. This is an
open access article distributed under the terms of
the Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited.
Data Availability Statement: All data files are
available from the open science framework
repository (https://osf.io/bj5a6/?view_only=
b46ed9663a98461dac3a9430e3954e10).
Funding: The author(s) received no specific
funding for this work. PW’s affiliated University
responsible for this coronavirus pandemic that we are experiencing while the present article
was in writing. The virus has so far spread throughout all the inhabited continents and affected
millions of people, killing thousands of individuals. Schools have been shut down, kids are still
at home in many countries, many citizens are now working remotely, locked down in their
houses, and leaving only for reasons of primary necessity, such as shopping for groceries and
going to medical appointments.
With many countries implementing lock downs and promoting quarantines, suggesting or
forcing citizens to stay inside their homes in order to avoid spreading the virus, millions of
people are experiencing a global pandemic for the first time in their lives. The social distancing
enforced by various governments stimulated many internet users to use social media to com-
municate and express their own concerns, opinions, beliefs and feelings in relation to this new
reality. On Twitter, tweets with hashtags such as #coronavirus, #Covid-19 or #Covid pile up
quickly (for instance, we accumulated around 16,000 tweets per hour). A variety of issues are
debated on a daily basis on Twitter, in relation to the pandemic. These include, but are not lim-
ited to, the political and social consequences of various governmental decisions, the situations
in the hospitals getting increasingly more crowded every day, the interpretation of the num-
bers associated with the spreading of the pandemic, the problems that families face with home-
schooling their children while working from home, and so forth. Among these issues, the
discussion around the treatment and containment of the virus is surely a central topic.
The present article aims at describing how the discourse around Covid-19 is framed on
Twitter. In particular, we present a study that elucidates what the main topics are related to the
discourse around Covid-19 on Twitter and to what extent the treatment of the disease is
framed figuratively. Because previous research has shown that various social and political
issues addressed in public discourse are framed in terms of wars [1], we assumed that this ten-
dency may emerge also on Twitter, in relation to the discourse around Covid-19. Although
Twitter contains messages written by journalists and other experts in mass communication,
most tweets are provided by non-expert communicators. We investigated to what extent Twit-
ter users, and therefore non-expert communicators, frame Covid-19 in terms of a war, and
whether other figurative framings arise from automated analyses of our corpus of Tweets con-
taining virus-related hashtags.
In particular, we addressed the following research questions:
1. What type of topics are discussed on Twitter, in relation to Covid-19?
2. To what extent is the WAR figurative frame and the conventional metaphor DISEASE
TREATMENT IS WAR used to talk about Covid-19 on Twitter? Which lexical units are
used within this metaphorical frame and which lexical units are not?
3. Are there alternative figurative frames used to talk about Covid-19 on Twitter? And how
does their use compare to the use of the WAR frame?
These three questions are addressed in the remainder of this paper in this same order. For
each question we present methods, results and discussion of specific corpus-based analyses.
The innovative aspect of this paper lies in the quantitative nature of our observations and
analyses of figurative framings used in pandemic-related discourse, and in the methods used:
topic modelling. In particular, the WAR frame, which previous studies have identified as per-
vasive in many crisis-related texts, is hereby investigated by means of automated methods
(topic modelling) applied to real-world data. This is a new approach in cognitive linguistics
and metaphor studies, where the analysis of figurative frames is typically based on qualitative
observations or small-scale corpus analyses. By answering the research questions outlined
above our study opens the path to further investigations that may take a longitudinal
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 2 / 24
(UCD) provides the funding for the publication
fees.
Competing interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist.
perspective on the current reality, to investigate how the discourse around Covid-19 changes,
with the development of new phases of the pandemic. These present results and the future
developments provide important information in the field of opinion mining and can be used
to understand the current state of mind, beliefs and feelings of various communities.
Theoretical background
Mining the information encoded by private internet users in the short texts posted on Twitter
(the tweets) is becoming an increasingly fruitful field of research. In relation to health dis-
course, tweets have been used by epidemiologists to access supplementary data about epidem-
ics. For example, tweets about particular diseases have been compared to gold-standard
incidence data, showing that there are positive correlations between the number of tweets dis-
cussing flu-symptoms and official statistics about the virus spread such as those published by
the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and the Health Protection Agency [2]. Already
a decade ago, in Brazil, tweets have been used to track the spreading of the dengue fever, a
mosquito-transmitted virus [3]. More recently, Pruss and colleagues [4] used a topic model
applied to a large corpus of tweets to automatically identify and extract the key topics of discus-
sion about the Zika disease, a virus that spread mainly in the Americas in early 2015. The
authors also found that rises in tweeting activity tended to follow major events related to the
disease, and Zika-related discussions were moderately correlated with the virus incidence.
Moreover, it has been demonstrated that the combination of data collected from hospitals
about specific diseases, and data collected from social media, can improve surveillance and
forecasting about the disease more effectively [5,6].
Besides providing a valuable tool for tracking the spread of epidemics, and thus helping
experts to make more effective decisions, social media have been used to investigate public
awareness, attitudes and reactions about specific diseases [7,8]. As Pruss and colleagues report
in their review [4], the 2013 measles outbreak in the Netherlands, for example, has been ana-
lyzed in this perspective by Mollema and colleagues [9], who compared the number of tweets
(and other messages posted on social media) with the number of online news articles as well as
with the number of reported measles cases and found a strong correlation between social
media messages and news articles and a mild correlation between number of tweets and num-
ber of reported measles cases. Moreover, through a topic analysis and a sentiment analysis of
the tweets, they found that the most common opinion expressed in the tweets was frustration
regarding people who do not vaccinate because of religious reasons (the measles outbreak in
the Netherlands began among Orthodox Protestants who often refuse vaccination for religious
reasons).
The 2014 Ebola outbreak in Africa was also used as a case study to mine the attitudes, con-
cerns and opinions of the public, expressed on Twitter. For example, Lazard and colleagues
[10] analyzed user-generated tweets to understand what were the main topics that concerned
the American public, when widespread panic ensued on US soil after one case of Ebola was
detected. The authors found that the main topics of concern for the American public were the
symptoms and lifespan of the virus, the disease transfer and contraction, whether it was safe to
travel, and how they could protect their body from the disease. In relation to the same out-
break, Tran and Lee [11] built Ebola-related information propagation models to mine the
Ebola related tweets and the information encoded therein, focusing on the distribution over
six topics, broadly defined as: 1. Ebola cases in the US, 2. Ebola outbreak in the world, 3. fear
and prayer, 4. Ebola spread and warning, 5. jokes, swearing and disapproval of jokes and 6.
impact of Ebola on daily life. The authors found that the second topic had the lowest focus,
while the fifth and sixth had the highest.
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 3 / 24
More recently, tweets have been mined to understand the discussion around the Zika epi-
demics. Miller and colleagues [12] used a combination of natural language processing and
machine learning techniques to determine the distribution of topics in relation to four charac-
teristics of Zika: symptoms, transmission, prevention, and treatment. The authors managed to
outline the most persistent concerns or misconceptions regarding the Zika virus, and provided
a complex map of topics emerged from the tweets posted within each of the four categories.
For example, in relation to the issue of prevention they observed the emergence of the follow-
ing topics: need for control and prevention of spread, need for money, ways to prevent spread,
bill to get funds, and research. Vijaykumar and colleagues [13] analyzed how content related
to Zika disease spreads on Twitter, thanks to tweet amplifiers and retweets. The authors found
that, of the 12 themes taken into account, Zika transmission was the most frequently talked
about on Twitter. Finally, Pruss and colleagues [4] mined a corpus of tweets in three different
languages (Spanish, Portuguese and English) with a multilingual topic model and identified
key topics of discussion across the languages. The authors reported that the Zika outbreak was
discussed differently around the world, and the topics identified were distributed in different
ways across the three languages.
In cognitive linguistics, and in particular in metaphor studies, public discourse is often ana-
lyzed in relation to different figurative and literal communicative frames. We “frame” a topic
when we “select some aspects of a perceived reality and make them more salient in a commu-
nicating text, in such a way as to promote a particular problem definition, causal interpreta-
tion, moral evaluation, and/or treatment recommendation for the item described” [14, p.53].
Metaphors are often used to talk about different aspects of diseases, such as their treatment,
their outbreak and their symptoms. The framing power of metaphor is particularly relevant in
health-related discourse, because it has been shown that it can impact patients’ general well-
being. For example, in a seminal study Sontag [15] criticized the popular use of war metaphors
to talk about cancer, a topic of research recently investigated also by Semino and colleagues
[16]. As these authors explain, the military metaphor that we tend to use to talk about the
development, spreading and cure of cancer inside the human body has been repeatedly
rejected by cancer patients as well as by many relatives and doctors, who indicate that such
framing provokes anxiety and a sense of helplessness that can have negative implications for
cancer patients. In a series of experiments, for example, Hendricks and colleagues [17] found
that framing a person’s cancer situation within the war metaphor, and therefore as a battle, has
the consequence of making people believe that the patient may feel guilty in the case that the
treatment does not succeed. Conversely, framing the cancer situation as a journey encourages
the inference that the patient will experience less anxiety about her health condition.
The military metaphor commonly used to talk about diseases such as cancer is a very com-
mon one to be found in public discourse [1]. According to Karlberg and Buell [18] 17% of all
articles in Time Magazine published between 1981 and 2000, contained at least one war meta-
phor. The war metaphor is not used solely to frame the discourse around diseases, but also the
discussion around political campaigns, crime, drugs and poverty. As explained in [1], war met-
aphors are pervasive in public discourse and span a wide range of topics because they provide
a very effective structural framework for communicating and thinking about abstract and
complex topics. Moreover, this frame is characterized by a strong negative emotional valence.
In the special case of the diseases, the war metaphor is typically used to frame the situation rel-
atively to the treatment of the disease. As indicated in MetaNet, the Berkeley-based structured
repository of conceptual metaphors and frames [19], the metaphor can be formalized as DIS-
EASE TREATMENT IS WAR, or TREATING DISEASE IS WAGING WAR (https://
metaphor.icsi.berkeley.edu/pub/en/index.php/Metaphor:DISEASE_TREATMENT_IS_
WAR). Within this metaphor, a variety of mappings can be identified, including: the diseased
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 4 / 24
cells are enemy combatants, medical professionals are the army of allies, the body is the battle-
field, medical tools are weapons, and applying a treatment is fighting.
The figurative frame of WAR, used in discourses around diseases, is certainly a conven-
tional one, frequently used, often unconsciously. As argued by [1], such a frame is handy and
frequently used because it draws on basic knowledge that everyone has, even though for most
people this is not knowledge coming from first-hand experience. Moreover, this frame
expressed in an exemplary way the urgency associated with a very negative situation, and the
necessity for actions to be taken, in order to achieve a final outcome quickly. The outcome can
be either positive or negative, in a rather categorical way. The inner structure of the frame is
also relatively simple, with opposing forces clearly labelled as in-groups and out-groups, or
allies and enemies. Each force has a strategy to achieve a goal, which involves risks and can
potentially be lethal. For these reasons, this frame is arguably very well suited to appear in the
discourse around Covid-19, as previously observed in relation to other diseases. The adversar-
ial relationship between doctors and the virus, the different goals afforded by the two forces
and the human body as the battlefield for this operation, are possible mappings that we seek to
trace down, with our analysis on Covid-19 related tweets.
Despite the undebatable frequency by which public discourse around diseases uses war
metaphors, this frame is sometimes not well received, as mentioned above, and war-related
metaphors can be opposed for various reasons. In the last weeks an increasingly large amount
of blog posts and articles for the large public confronted and opposed the use of military lan-
guage to talk about the pandemic, providing different arguments that range from the blindness
that war metaphors generate toward alternative ways to solve problems, to the rise of xenopho-
bia and the increase of fear and anxiety in the population that these metaphors generate. For
example, [20] argued that “to adopt a wartime mentality is fundamentally to allow for an all-
bets-are-off, anything-goes approach to emerging victorious. And while there may very well be
a time for slapdash tactics in the course of weaponized encounters on the physical battlefield,
this is never how one should endeavor to practice medicine.” [21] claimed that “using a war
narrative to talk about COVID-19 plays into the hands of white supremacist groups. U.S. offi-
cials and the media should stop it.” [22] explained that using a WAR frame breeds fear and
anxiety, divides communities, compromises democracies and may legitimize the use of actual
military actions. [23] foreshadowed “shifts towards dangerous authoritarian power-grabs, as in
Hungary, where Prime Minister Viktor Orba
´n seized wide-ranging emergency powers and the
ability to rule by decree”, as a consequence of war-related language in the current situation.
As we will further elaborate in the Discussion section, in some cases the press opposes
deliberately the war frame, advancing alternative figurative frames. Tracking down alternative
frames to the war one in a qualitative manner has been a recent endeavor initiated by scholars
in cognitive linguistics and corpus analysis on Twitter. The hashtag #ReframeCovid (first pro-
posed, to the best of our knowledge, by two Spanish scholars, Ine
´s Olza and Paula Sobrino) has
been recently used to harvest texts such as articles, advertisements and notes showing how the
virus has been opposed and framed in alternative ways by a few journalists and writers. Nota-
bly, the discourse has been reframed using lexical units related to the domain of FOOTBALL,
of GAMES, of STORMS and so forth. In this paper, we explored the structure and functioning
of alternative frames too, in a corpus-based analysis of tweets about Covid-19, and compared
them to the WAR frame as well as a literal frame, that is the FAMILY frame. In this case, it
should be mentioned that although FAMILY may be used as a metaphorical frame to talk, for
example, about nations (“founding fathers”, “daughters of the American revolution”, “sending
our sons to war” and so forth, see [24] for an extensive discussion), in the discourse around
Covid-19, family-related words are typically used in their literal meaning, to talk for example
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 5 / 24
about family members affected by the virus and family-dynamics being disrupted by the mea-
sures taken in response to the pandemic (e.g., the lock down).
Study design
To address our three research questions, first we explored the range of topics addressed in
the discourse on Covid-19 on Twitter using a topic modelling technique. Consequently, we
explored the actual usage of the WAR frame, and explored which topics (among the topics
identified in the first part of the study) are more frequently framed within the metaphor of
WAR. To do so, we compiled a list of war-related lexical units and ran it against our corpus
of Covid-19 tweets, observing and discussing which lexical units of each frame were used in
the tweets, and within which topics. Finally, we explored alternative frames that could be
used to frame the discourse around Covid-19 on Twitter. To do so, we compiled lists of lexi-
cal units for selected alternative frames (three figurative frames and one literal one) and com-
pared the percentages by which they appear to be used in the corpus of tweets, against the
percentages by which the WAR frame is used. To conclude, we replicated our analysis on a
new corpus of tweets collected in the weeks that followed the collection of the first corpus,
following the same criteria, as well as on an existing resource “Coronavirus Tweets Dataset”
by Lamsal [25], which became available during the revision process. Lamsal’s dataset is a con-
stantly updated repository of twitter IDs. The collection of those IDs is based on English
tweets that include 90+ hashtags and keywords that are commonly used while referencing the
pandemic.
Constructing the corpus of Covid-19 tweets
In order to identify tweets that relate to the Covid-19 epidemic, we defined a set of relevant
hashtags used to talk about the virus: #covid19, #coronavirus, #ncov2019, #2019ncov,
#nCoV, #nCoV2019, #2019nCoV, #COVID19. Using Twitter’s official API in combination
with the Tweepy python library (tweepy.org) for 14 days we collected 25.000 tweets per day
that contain at least one of the hashtags and no retweets. The tweets were collected in accor-
dance with the Twitter terms of service. Two main restrictions of those terms and service
motivated our decision to limit the extent of our corpus: Firstly, the free streaming API only
allows access up to one week of Twitter’s history. Secondly, there is a limit of 180 requests per
15-minutes.
To balance our corpus, we needed to consider how a single tweet or a single user weights
on the overall corpus. For example, a scientific analysis of fake news spread during the 2016
US presidential election showed that about 1% of users accounted for 80% of fake news and
report that other research suggests that 80% of all tweets can be linked to the top 10% of most
tweeting users [26]. In other words, there are Twitter users who tweet a lot, and Twitter users
who tweet seldomly. This may be problematic when looking for the frequency distribution of
word uses. For example, a specific Twitter user who is very fond of sci-fi issues might use the
MONSTER framing very frequently in their tweets, also when tweeting about Covid-19. If we
kept all the tweets by this user, this might have biased the frequency distribution of the MON-
STER related words. For the purpose of our study, we were interested in exploring the relative
uses of different frames in the discourse around Covid-19 on Twitter, rather than in the abso-
lute percentages of use of the different frames in Twitter. Therefore, we constructed our corpus
to be representative and balanced, as well as manageable from a computational perspective. To
do so, we retained only one tweet per user and dropped retweets. Keeping only one tweet per
user allowed us to balance compulsive tweeters and less involved Twitter users. Table 1 accu-
mulates for each day how many tweets have been collected and the number of filtered tweets
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 6 / 24
after discarding tweets from users that have already tweeted. This implies that only the first
contribution of each user was retained: we have kept the first tweet on that day by users whose
tweets we have not collected yet. Consequently, all 203,756 tweets are from unique tweeters.
For example, Table 1 data column 3 shows that on 22.03.2020, we collected 75,000 tweets in
total and from those 57,073 were from unique tweeters.
The filtered tweets are single tweets per user, the total number of collected tweets is 25k per
day.
As the streaming API starts collecting tweets from 23:59 CET of each day and has been lim-
ited to English, our corpus encompasses mainly tweets produced by users residing in the USA,
where the time of data collection corresponds to awake hours, and the targeted language corre-
sponds to the first language of most US residents, according to the American Community Sur-
vey (ACS). The total number of collected tweets from unique tweeters over 14 days
(20.03.2020 to 02.04.2020) is 203,756. This resulted in 41.78% of the collected tweets being
tweets of a user tweeting more than once, thus being filtered out.
Given our research questions and our aims, we did not analyze the dynamics involved in
retweetings and mentions on Twitter and neither did we provide an analysis of usernames,
hashtags or URLs.
In compliance with the privacy rights of Twitter [27], we have only collected the
tweet along with a timestamp. In order to comply with Twitter’s content redistribution policy,
it is not possible to make any information other than the Tweet IDs publicly available. We
have therefore stored our data as tweet IDs. This dataset is publicly available in the online
repository on OSF, retrievable at the following url: https://osf.io/bj5a6/?view_only=
1644595a66dd4adebeeb6b2bb0449c89.
General corpus analytics
The corpus encompassed 203,756 tweets, in which the 30 most common words, excluding
stopwords and online tags (e.g. “&”, “https”) are:
people (19153), us (13368), get (11270), like (10451), time (10263), help (10091), need
(9993), cases (9205), home (9044), stay (8788), new (8752), one (8725), friends (8465),
please (8232), pandemic (7614), support (7255), know (6931), going (6788), realdonald-
trump (6659), times (6462), world (6451), health (6449), day (6153), family (6010), go
(5986), trump (5967), work (5862), would (5705), today (5602), take (5532)
In this list, the number in brackets represents the frequency of occurrence, also visualized
in Fig 1 (in which greater size of the word indicates greater occurrence in the corpus). In this
word cloud, the larger the word print, the more frequent its occurrence in the corpus. The
most frequent word is “people” with about 19k occurrences, followed by “us” with about 13k
occurrences. It should be mentioned that we cannot distinguish whether “us” means “United
States” or the pronoun “us”.
Table 1. Dates of collection for tweets containing hashtags related to the Covid-19 epidemic.
Date 20.03.2020 21.03.2020 22.03.2020 23.03.2020 24.03.2020 25.03.2020 26.03.2020
Filtered / Collected 20,316/25,000 39,284/50,000 57,073/75,000 73,346/100,000 89,785/125,000 103,614/150,000 118,866/175,000
Date 27.03.2020 28.03.2020 29.03.2020 30.03.2020 31.03.2020 01.04.2020 02.04.2020
Filtered / Collected 132,995/200,000 146,654/225,000 156,775/250,000 167,847/275,000 180,234/300,000 191,278/325,000 203,756/350,000
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010.t001
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 7 / 24
What type of topics are discussed on Twitter, in relation to Covid-
19?
Identifying topics in Covid-19 discourse on Twitter through topic
modeling
A topic model is a generative statistical model for discovering “topics” that occur in a collec-
tion of documents. The topics identified by topic modelling are based on the occurrences of
semantically related words. For example, “ball”, “strike”, “bat”, “catcher”, “hitter” and “dia-
mond” and “fastball” are likely to appear in documents about baseball. A document typically
concerns multiple topics in different proportions, and such proportions are reflected in the
probability of the words related to each topic. The topics produced by topic modeling tech-
niques are therefore probability clusters of related words, which are unlabeled (the model
returns the cluster of words, not the name of such cluster and thus of the topic). Topics there-
fore need to be interpreted and labeled by the analysts.
Conversely, in communication sciences a frame is typically defined as consisting of two ele-
ments [28]: elements in a text such as words, used as framing devices, and (latent) information
used as reasoning devices, through which a problem, cause, evaluation, and/or treatment is
implied. A topic operationalized by topic modelling, in this sense, corresponds to the first of
these two components of a frame, that is, a list of semantically coherent words used as framing
devices).
In order to extract and identify topics from the corpus, we used a Latent Dirichlet Alloca-
tion algorithm (henceforth, LDA) [29]. LDA is an unsupervised machine learning algorithm
that aims to describe samples of data in terms of heterogeneous categories. It is mostly used to
identify categories in documents of text and thus appropriate to identify topics within the
Covid-19 corpus of tweets. The study reported by Pruss and colleagues on the corpus of tweets
related to the Zika epidemics [4], for example, used the same algorithm to identify topics
within the corpus. For the purpose of our study we used the Gensim LDA-Multicore algorithm,
which allowed us to parallelize the training of our data on multiple CPUs. As an unsupervised
learner, LDA needs to be given the number of topics that it will try to divide the data into. Our
exploratory approach included the search space for several different amounts of topics, thus
Fig 1. Word cloud of the most common words in the corpus of over 200k collected tweets with at least one
hashtag relating to the covid19 epidemic.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010.g001
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 8 / 24
varying in the level of granularity represented within each topic. We hereby reported the
results obtained from the division of the data into a relatively small number of topics (N = 4)
and a relatively large number of topics (N = 16), to show and compare a less granular and a
more granular division of the data. We expected to find broader and more generic concepts
listed in the first analysis (N = 4) and more specific concepts in the fine-grained topic analysis
(N = 16). These two numbers of clusters were chosen by investigating the data and are backed
up by our post-hoc analysis of the LDA coherence measures. The preprocessing phase encom-
passed the six following steps:
• converting each tweet into a list of tokens (using Gensim’s simple_preprocess function)
• removing tokens with less than 3 characters (e.g. “aa”, “fo”, “#o”)
• removing stopwords from the list of tokens (including updated stopwords from Stone et al.
[30] and twitter specific stopwords.)
• removing Covid-19 words from the list of tokens (e.g. “covid”, “nCov”, “coronavirus” etc)
• turning the tokens into a bag-of-words, i.e. a list of tuples with the token and its number of
occurrences in the corpus
We excluded terms like “coronavirus”, “covid”, “corona”, “virus” or “nCov19” from the
topic modeling because these do not add information about the topics themselves. The prepro-
cessing resulted in a list of 415,329 tokens, that is, inflected word forms. We did not lemmatize
the corpus, nor pos-tagged it for the purpose of our study, because different forms of a lemma
can express different metaphor scenarios and therefore, they shall be preserved. Hence, for
example, gerundive forms of verbs, as well as plural forms of nouns are present in the corpus,
and the list of frame-related words is also composed by inflected word forms and not simple
lemmas. Additionally, we trained another LDA model with a tf-idf (term frequency-inverse
document frequency) version of the tokens. The tf-idf assigns a statistical relevance to each
token based on how many times the token occurs and the inverse document frequency (a mea-
sure of whether the token is rare or common in the corpus) of that token. As its results did not
add any further insight to our research, we included it in the online repository but do not dis-
cuss this model further in the current paper.
Topic model analysis
Dividing the corpus into four topics through the LDA, we obtained a list of words for each
topic and the weightage (importance). Fig 2 shows the word clouds with greater words signal-
ing greater significance. Except for topic #II, all of the other topics included the word “pan-
demic” among their most important words and show a strong overlap. The weights
(importance) and words for each topic allocated by the LDA model with N = 4 topics were the
following:
LDA (N = 4, 6 passes):
Topic #I: 0.008 pandemic, 0.005 news, 0.004 data, 0.004 update, 0.004 world, 0.004 youtube,
0.003 information, 0.003 latest, 0.003 today, 0.003 april
Topic #II: 0.029 times, 0.028 friends, 0.027 family, 0.022 share, 0.021 italy, 0.021 trying, 0.014
support, 0.014 sign, 0.013 stand, 0.011 colleagues
Topic #III: 0.014 people, 0.013 cases, 0.011 trump, 0.009 realdonaldtrump, 0.008 like, 0.006
china, 0.006 world, 0.005 deaths, 0.005 pandemic, 0.004 going
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 9 / 24
Topic #IV: 0.010 home, 0.008 help, 0.008 stay, 0.007 people, 0.007 time, 0.007 need, 0.007 pan-
demic, 0.006 health, 0.006 work, 0.005 safe
The results for 16 topics, displayed in Fig 3, showed a much greater diversity among the
classes.
Discussion
As previously mentioned, the LDA algorithm does not provide labels for the topics. The inter-
pretation of the topics is left to the analysts. The topics identified by LDA analyzed above and
visualized in Fig 2 can be labeled as follows:
Topic #I:Communications and Reporting.
Topic #II:Community and Social Compassion.
Fig 2. Word clouds form N = 4 LDA topic modeling with greater words signaling greater significance.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010.g002
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 10 / 24
Topic #III:Politics.
Topic #IV:Reacting to the epidemic.
The sixteen topic LDA model provided a more fine-grained view of topics that could be
related to the 4 general topics. In the field of Communication and Reporting, we observed finer
distinctions in topics #4, #11 and some in #15. Topic #11, in particular, is more focused on
“World”, “Trump” and “China”, while topic #4 specifically encompasses “News”, “Lockdown”,
“Press”, and “Media”. In the domain of Community and Social Compassion, topic #3 is very
close to topic #II. Whereas, topic #13, #16, #5 relate to topics around the quarantine, self-isola-
tion and in general Reacting to the Epidemic (#IV).
There are also some novel topics around treatment and medical needs (#1, #6, #7), around
testing (#10) or working/studying from home (#2, #9 and parts of #12). Rather unrelated to
the whole epidemic, a conglomerate of words can be found in topic #8 and parts in #12.
Fig 3. Depiction of the word clouds for each of the 16 topics clustered by the LDA.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010.g003
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 11 / 24
Finally, we provide an interactive online tool to explore the results of the LDA models for
the 4 topic LDA model (https://bit.ly/3dCczfr) and for the 16 topic LDA model (https://bit.ly/
3gUx5tU). These renderings have been produced using pyLDAvis [31].
To what extent is the WAR figurative frame used to talk about
Covid-19 on Twitter?
Determining lexical units associated with the WAR frame
To investigate to what extent users use the WAR frame to talk about Covid-19, we needed to
assess the amount of tweets that use war language in our corpus. To explore the lexical units
associated with the WAR frame we took a double approach, using two tools. The first tool was
the web-service relatedwords.org. This web-service provides a list of words (inflected word
forms, not lemmas) related to a target word. This list is ranked through competing scores by
several algorithms, one of which finds similar words in a word embedding [32] and another
one queries ConceptNet [33] to find words with meaningful relationships to the target word.
Choi and Lee [34] used the same web-service to expand the list of categories used to model
conceptual representations for crisis-related tweets [34]. The list of words retrieved on related-
words was adapted to our purpose. As a matter of fact, the list featured words such as “franco-
prussian war” or “aggression”. The former is a specific type of war and it includes the term
“war” itself. We dropped any kind of specific war or terms that include a compound of war,
e.g. “state of war”. The latter term “aggression” is too broad and not closely related to the target
word. Additionally, in case of doubt, we checked the term in an online dictionary to verify its
relation to the war framing. The second tool used to prepare the list of lexical units related to
the WAR frame was the MetaNet repository of conceptual metaphors and frames housed at
the International Computer Science Institute in Berkeley, California [18]. Here, from the
WAR frame (https://metaphor.icsi.berkeley.edu/pub/en/index.php/Frame:War) we selected
the 12 words that were not yet included in the selection of lexical units based on relatedwords.
Moreover, we dropped compound units that included words that we had already included in
the list (e.g., “combat zone”, because we featured already the word “combat”) and two mis-
spelled units (“seige” instead of “siege” and “beseige” instead of “besiege”). The total number
of lexical units for the WAR framing was 91:
WAR (91): allied, allies, armed, armies, army, attack, attacks, battle, battlefield, battle-
ground, battles, belligerent, bloodshed, bomb, captured, casualties, combat, combatant,
combative, conflict, conquer, conquering, conquest, crusade, defeat, defend, defenses,
destruction, disarmament, enemies, enemy, escalation, fight, fighter, fighting, foe, fortify,
fought, grenade, guerrilla, gunfight, holocaust, homeland, hostilities, hostility, insurgency,
invaded, invader, invaders, invasion, liberation, military, peace, peacetime, raider, rebellion,
resist, resistance, riot, siege, soldier, soldiers, struggle, tank, threat, treaty, trench, trenches,
troops, uprising, victory, violence, war, warfare, warrior, wars, wartime, warzone, weapon,
alliance, ally, arsenal, blitzkrieg, bombard, front, line, minefield, troop, vanquish,
vanquishment.
A methodological clarification shall be made explicit regarding the identification of lexical
units used metaphorically in our corpus. In cognitive linguistics and metaphor studies, the
procedure usually adopted for the reliable identification of words used metaphorically in lin-
guistic corpora is MIPVU [37]. This procedure is applied manually, by multiple annotators, in
content analyses where analysts make decisions about the metaphoricity of each lexical unit
encountered in the text, based on information retrieved from dictionaries. Despite the high
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 12 / 24
reliability of this method, because it is performed manually, it cannot be applied to large cor-
pora, like the corpus of tweets on which the current study is based. For this reason, we opted
for the following procedure: we assumed that war-related lexical entries would be used meta-
phorically rather than literally, within tweets about Covid-19, and we qualitatively confirmed
our intuition by manually looking at a subsample of tweets. We acknowledge the fact that in
the tweets that we haven’t manually checked it could be the case that some war-related words
were used literally, for example to talk about soldiers getting infected with Covid-19 while
being in service. However, we believe that this phenomenon may characterize a negligible
amount of tweets, compared to the number of tweets in which war-related lexical entries are
used metaphorically. Nonetheless, we acknowledge this limitation to our approach and we
wish further studies to account for this possibility, and possibly to compare the metaphorical
use to the literal use of war-related terms in Covid-19 discourse.
In order to understand where in relation to our predicted LDA topics the WAR frame was
located, we collected all tweets that mentioned at least one term of the WAR frame and asked
the LDA model to predict its topic. This way, we could identify the topics with the most or the
least terms related to WAR.
WAR framing results
Analyzing all tweets from the database, a total of 10,846 tweets contained at least one term
from the WAR framing, which is 5.32% of all tweets. Of these, 1,253 tweets had more than one
war-related term. The 20 most common war terms found in our database are hereby reported
with relative percentage to all war terms and number of occurrences:
WAR: fight (29.76%, 3228), fighting (10.65%, 1155), war (10.08%, 1093), combat (5.89%,
639), threat (5.13%, 556), battle (4.19%, 454), front line (3.82%, 414), military (3.61%, 392),
peace (3.43%, 372), attack (2.95%, 320), enemy (2.61%, 283), defeat (2.51%, 273), violence
(2.12%, 230), attacks (1.44%, 156), struggle (1,34%, 145), resist (1.23%, 133), soldiers
(1.23%, 133), weapon (1,20%, 130), victory (0.95%, 103), wars (0.95%, 103)
Words that were virtually absent (or had very limited usage) in the context of Covid-19 on
Twitter were: combatant (2x), combative (2x), disarmament (2x), gunfight (2x), invader (2x),
treaty (2x), bombard (2x), minefield (2x), belligerent (1x), guerilla (1x), insurgency (1x), van-
quish (1x), conquest (0x), blitzkrieg (0x), vanquishment (0x).
LDA topic prediction of WAR tweets
The LDA model can predict the probability that a document belongs to a certain topic of the
corpus. We therefore used this prediction method to investigate what topics are relevant for
those tweets that feature WAR terms. For this, we tokenized all tweets that contain at least one
WAR term and used both of our LDA models to suggest which of the four and sixteen topics
those WAR related tweets most likely belong to. For the four-ways topic model we report a dis-
tribution in Fig 4.
For the sixteen-ways topic model, we report a distribution in Fig 5. This image shows that
lexical units belonging to the WAR domain and therefore tweets that relate to the WAR frame
are most likely to be found in tweets that belong to topics IV and I, and partly III (in the macro
distinction of topics) and in tweets that belong to topics 2, 7 and 10 in the fine-grained
distinction.
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 13 / 24
Discussion
The results show that 5.32% of all tweets contain war-related terms and are therefore likely to
frame the discourse around Covid-19 metaphorically, in terms of a literal war. While it is hard
to evaluate in absolute terms the impact that this frame has on the overall discourse around
Covid-19, we show in the next sections how the WAR frame compares to the usage of 3 other
figurative frames, as well as to a literal frame.
The specific words within the WAR frame that appeared to be used in the tweets were
“fight” “fighting”, the very same word “war”, “combat”, “threat”, and “battle”. All these words
carry a very negative valence and denote aspects of the war that relate to actions and events.
This is probably due to the stage of the pandemic that we are in, that is, the emergency situa-
tion, and the related urgent need to take action and confront the negative situation. We cannot
exclude that this tendency may change, once the pandemic moves into a different stage. In par-
ticular, it could be the case that when the emergency has passed, and we will move toward the
next phase, in which we will leave the peak of the death and infection rates, the most frequent
words used in relation to the WAR frame might relate to the identification of strategies to keep
ourselves safe and to defend our community from potential new attacks.
In relation to the topic modelling of the war-related tweets, we showed that tweets that fea-
ture war-related terms are most likely to belong to topics IV, I and III, rather than to topic II.
Interestingly, topic IV addresses aspects related to the reactions to the epidemic, including the
measures proposed by the governments and taken by the people, such as self-isolating, staying
at home, protecting our bodies and so forth. Our analysis therefore suggests that using war-
Fig 4. LDA-predicted average probability of WAR term contributing to one of 4 topics.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010.g004
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 14 / 24
related words is a communicative phenomenon that we use to express aspects of the Covid-19
epidemic related to the measures needed to oppose (fight!) the virus. Moreover, tweets that fea-
ture war-related words are also often classified within the topics I and III, which include the
aspects related to communications and reports about the virus, and politics. We interpret
these results arguing that public communications and political messages are likely to frame the
discourse in the WAR framing. Finally, it might not come as a surprise the fact that topic II,
which encompasses aspects of the discourse related to the familiar sphere, the community and
the social compassion, does not relate well with the tweets containing war terms.
The fine-grained analysis into 16 topics shows some interesting trends as well. In particular,
tweets containing war-related terms are particularly well represented in topics 2, 7, and 10.
Topic 2 seems to relate to online learning and education, Topic 7 encompasses aspects related
to the treatment of the virus, with words such as “workers”, “health”, “care”, “help”, “thank”,
“need”, “support”. Similarly, topic 10 relates to the diagnostics and treatment of the virus, with
words such as “positive”, “death”, “cases”, “tested”, “people”, “confirmed”. Therefore, as the
MetaNet WAR metaphor suggests, and as we described in the Theoretical Background of this
paper, it is the discourse around the disease treatment and its diagnostics that are likely to be
framed figuratively in terms of a war. Conversely, topic 3, which is characterized by words like
“friends”, “share”, “trying”, “family”, “time” and therefore addresses intimate social relations
and personal affective aspects related to Covid-19, is not related to the WAR frame: tweets
addressing these aspects do not employ military lexical units.
Fig 5. LDA-predicted average probability of a WAR term contributing to one of 16 topics.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010.g005
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 15 / 24
We note that an LDA model uses randomness in its training and inference, therefore train-
ing a new model with the same parameters will always yield slightly different topic distribu-
tions and there are different ways and limitations to analyze those [35].
Are there alternative figurative frames used to talk about Covid-19
on Twitter?
Search method for alternative framings and relevant lexical units therein
In order to identify whether or not the war framing is particularly relevant, we explored alter-
native framings used in discourses on viruses. For this purpose we used the metaphor explora-
tion web services by [36], called MetaphorMagnet (http://bonnat.ucd.ie/metaphor-magnet-
acl). Using the keywords “virus” and “epidemic” we selected the following alternative frames,
which could be in principle used to frame the discourse around Covid-19: STORM, MON-
STER and TSUNAMI. These figurative frames have been reported also within the crowd-
sourced observations collected by the #ReframeCovid initiative on Twitter. Other possible
figurative frames are reported within this initiative too. These are, for example, GAME and the
sub-frame SOCCER GAME, used in the Spanish press according to the community of Spanish
cognitive linguists. However, lexical units such as “game”, “football”, “soccer”, “game season”,
and so on, are likely to be used literally in the tweets, to refer to the fact that all sport events
and thus all games have been suspended, due to the epidemic. Another frame that has been
observed in the press and tagged as #ReframeCovid is the FLOOD frame. However, through a
quick search on Metaphor Magnet, we realized that this frame has too many shared lexical
units with STORM and TSUNAMI, and was therefore discarded. Moreover, we observed that
the wordlists of the frames STORM and TSUNAMI contain shared words. However, dictio-
nary definitions of these two terms suggest that the two phenomena are quite different. For
example, the MacMillian online dictionary defines STORM as an occasion when a lot of rain
falls very quickly, often with very strong wind or thunder and lightning. Conversely, TSU-
NAMI is defined as a very large wave or series of waves caused when something such as an
earthquake moves a large quantity of water in the sea. Because the two concepts denote differ-
ent phenomena, the fact that they share a few words does not constitute a redundancy.
In order to select the lexical units within each of the alternative frames, we used the tool
relatedwords, already used for the WAR frame, for consistency. However, because these alter-
native frames are arguably less conventionalized, none of them is included in the list of frames
on MetaNet. Thus, relatedwords was the only tool we used to harvest lexical units for the alter-
native frames. We created three list of lexical units:
STORM (57): thunderstorm, rain, lightning, snowstorm, blizzard, wind, hurricane,
weather, rainstorm, typhoon, tempest, precipitation, beaufort, snow, cyclone, meteorology,
hail, hailstorm, windstorm, flooding, thunder, tornado, monsoon, rainfall, rage, force,
disaster, ice, storm, atmospheric, disturbance, wildfire, clouds, firestorm, ramp, tornadoes,
fog, winds, rains, waves, landfall, thunderhead, duststorm, tides, gusts, floodwaters, wave,
cloud, swells, cloudburst, anticyclone, downpour, sandstorm, stormy, whirlwinds, storms,
oceanographic.
MONSTER (51): freak, demon, devil, giant, ogre, fiend, zombie, frankenstein, bogeyman,
werewolf, horror, mutant, creature, dragon, superhero, goliath, behemoth, monstrosity,
colossus, legend, evil, lusus, naturae, mouse, beast, boogeyman, leviathan, dracula, mon-
strous, teratology, villain, killer, ghost, gigantic, siren, superman, vampire, undead, psycho,
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 16 / 24
monster, chimera, godzilla, fiction, mythology, mutation, demoniac, manatee, mermaid,
monsters, spider, bug.
TSUNAMI (50): earthquake, disaster, tide, oceans, calamity, catastrophe, tragedy, wave-
length, wind, period, cataclysm, flood, eruption, tidal, seiche, quake, thucydides, floods,
floodwater, cyclone, devastation, ocean, surface, wave, coastlines, typhoon, waves, hurri-
cane, magnitude, aftershock, mudslide, seafloor, richter, seawall, seismic, landslide, tsuna-
mis, aftershocks, flooding, torrential, earthquakes, deepwater, triggering, tsunami, tremors,
mudslides, riptide, rains, whirlpool, pacific.
As for the WAR frame, we ran these lists against our corpus and compared the frequency of
occurrences within the corpus across the different framings.
The literal frame of FAMILY used as control
To evaluate the relevance of the figurative frames in the corpus of tweets, we compared the
occurrence of the lexical units listed therein with those listed within a frame that we expected
to occur in the literal sense: the FAMILY frame. The word list of lexical entries related to this
frame encompasses the following words:
FAMILY (66): marriage, household, kin, house, kinfolk, home, lineage, kinship, parent, rel-
ative, clan, cousin, children, child, sister, mother, father, uncle, nephew, brother, grandson,
son, grandfather, grandmother, kinsfolk, ancestor, consanguinity, tribe, sibling, subfamily,
kindred, stepfamily, couple, family, sib, foster, parentage, menage, phratry, folk, daughter,
kinsperson, aunt, grandma, granddaughter, grandaunt, stepbrother, niece, stepson, dad,
stepdaughter, stepfather, wife, husband, daddy, parents, elder, daughters, mom, siblings,
stepmother, grandpa, grandparents, relatives, widow, spouse.
Alternative framing results
The terms belonging to the frame STORM were found in 3,036 tweets (1.49% of all tweets).
The terms in the MONSTER frame were found in 1,382 tweets (0.68% of all tweets). The terms
in the TSUNAMI frame were found in 2,304 tweets (1.13% of all tweets). The terms in the lit-
eral frame (FAMILY) were found in 24,568 tweets (12.06% of all tweets). The difference
between the frequency of occurrence of the frames, and in particular of the sets of words
related to each frame, is statistically significant (Cochran’s Q test statistic = 47,226.72, df = 4,
p<0.001). We then looked at the distribution of the frequencies by which the terms within
each framing were used and observed that they all tended to follow Zipf distributions (see Fig
6, where the term “fight” from the WAR frame has more than 3,000 occurrences in the tweets).
In other words, within each frame there were few words used very frequently, but many words
were rarely used. Moreover, although this is not visible on the plot in Fig 6, in the online repos-
itory we stored the full list of lexical units within each frame. Among the top ranked ones for
the FAMILY frame we found “home”, “family”, “house”, “children”, “parents”, “wife”, “son”,
and “mom”. For the STORM frame among the most frequently used lexical units we found
“force”, “disaster”, “weather”, “ice”, “wave”, “storm”, “cloud”, and “rain”. For the MONSTER
frame among the most frequently used lexical units we find “evil”, “horror”, “killer”, “giant”,
“monster”, “legend”, “ghost”, “zombie”, “devil”, “fiction”, “bug” and “beast”. Finally, for the
TSUNAMI frame among the most frequently used lexical units we found “period”, “disaster”,
“wave”, “tragedy”, “catastrophe”, and “waves”.
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 17 / 24
Also, the total number of words within each frame was different, with the WAR frame fea-
turing more words than the other figurative frames. In order to compare how frequently the
WAR frame was used in Covid-19 discourse, compared to other possible figurative frames
(and a literal frame), it was necessary to have lists of lexical units of the same length because
longer lists could have yielded larger numbers of tweets in the corpus than shorter lists, in
principle. Therefore, we decided to evaluate two subsets of the term lists for each framing, set-
ting two cutoffs at N = 30 and N = 50 terms on each list. In this way, we only considered the
top 30 and then 50 most relevant (i.e., most frequently used) terms within each frame. We
then compared the number of tweets featuring words from these lists, which were now compa-
rable in length.
Table 2 reports the number of tweets featuring at least one lexical unit related to a frame,
and the general percentage of tweets in the corpus that can be related to these frames. Results
showed that the literal frame FAMILY is substantially more frequently used in the discourse
Fig 6. Five histograms depicting the occurrences of terms for each frame within the corpus.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010.g006
Table 2. Proportions of tweets that contain at least one of the terms from each of the frames with term list size N = 30 and N = 50.
Frame # of tweets with at least 1
word from 30-item list
Percentage of tweets over the
whole corpus (30 terms)
# of tweets with at least 1
word from 50-item list
Percentage of tweets over the
whole corpus (50 terms)
Total
Tweets
WAR 10,107 4.96% 10,704 5.25% 203,756
FAMILY 24,269 11.91% 24,563 12.06% 203,756
STORM 3,017 1.48% 3,035 1.49% 203,756
MONSTER 1,348 0.66% 1,382 0.68% 203,756
TSUNAMI 2,217 1.09% 2,304 1.13% 203,756
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010.t002
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 18 / 24
on Covid-19 than the figurative frames. However, among figurative frames, the WAR frame
covered a higher portion of the tweets in our corpus than the other figurative frames. The table
also shows that there is no substantial difference between the coverages of the corpus obtained
using the 30 words and the 50 words lists of lexical units for each frame.
Replication studies
Given the timeliness of this study, our first analysis was based on a corpus of tweets covering
2-weeks’ time. During the submission and review process, more data (more tweets) obviously
became available. We therefore replicated our analysis in which different frames are compared,
with new data. First, we constructed an additional corpus of tweets like the first one, with
tweets produced in the two weeks that followed the timeframe of the first corpus. Second, we
replicated our study using an external dataset with more than 1.2 million tweets, which became
available during the revision process of the current article. The choice of tweets to be collected
from an external dataset has been limited by the factors that define our corpus: no retweets,
only unique tweeter’s tweets, English, from 20.03.2020–20.05.2020 and maximum memory
limit (due to hardware constraints). The external corpus included 1,213,420 tweets from Lam-
sal’s Coronavirus Tweets Dataset [25] over two months. Fig 7 presents an overview of the com-
parison and Table 3 provides the descriptive statistics and results of Cochran Q tests.
As Fig 7 shows, the distribution of the five frames across the first corpus of tweets is very
similar to the distributions observed in the replication studies. The increase of >0.22% in the
WAR framing from our first corpus (W&B 2 weeks) to the second corpus (W&B 2 months)
could be partially explained by new debates entering the discourse, with the development of
the epidemic.
Fig 7. Comparison of the two corpora for five frames and two time spans (2 weeks,2 months). Each bar indicates the percentage of a frame within
the respective corpus.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010.g007
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 19 / 24
The comparison between our data and the data extracted from the “Coronavirus Tweets
Dataset” by Lamsal over the same time-frame shows that overall the relative order of propor-
tions is the same as in our analysis: Family >War >Storm + Tsunami + Monster. The differ-
ences in these proportions (Family proportion decreased by 3.46%, War increased by 1.62%)
can be explained by different keywords that have been used to acquire the Lamsal dataset. This
dataset, in fact, has been constructed using keywords that we did not use to construct our data-
set, notably the keyword “Corona”. This is arguably a more colloquial expression that we chose
to not include in our set of keywords. Moreover, in Lamsal’s dataset keywords change multiple
times during the process of data mining, while we keep the same set of keywords, day after
day.
Discussion
Our results show that the literal frame used as control (FAMILY) covers a wider portion of the
tweets in the corpus while the figurative ones cover substantially less tweets. This is not partic-
ularly surprising, as previous literature shows that overall metaphor-related words cover only a
percentage of the discourse, and that literal language is still prevalent. Steen and colleagues
[37], for example, report that literal language covers 86.4% of the lexical units, while meta-
phor-related words cover just 13.6% of the lexical units. Their analysis is based on a sub-corpus
of the BNC that encompasses 187,570 lexical units extracted from academic texts, conversa-
tions, fiction and news texts. All parts of speech are included in their analyses, including func-
tion words (such as prepositions and articles). Based on these statistics, we would expect to
find around 13% of the lexical units in our corpus to be used metaphorically. This percentage
would need to include pervasive metaphorical uses of function words such as prepositions, as
well as all words used metaphorically, which can be related to any figurative frame. In this per-
spective, we believe that the percentage of use of the WAR frame reported in our study suggests
that this frame is particularly frequent. In our specific study, based however on a limited num-
ber of possible figurative frames, lexical entries related to the WAR frame cover more than one
third of all the words attributed to the metaphorical frames hereby investigated.
Within the FAMILY (literal) frame, the top words (i.e., most frequent words) that are used
in the tweets denote family members and family relations. Within the STORM frame, words
that suggest the most frequently used words seem to denote concrete entities that can be typi-
cally observed within a storm scenario. In general, from a qualitative standpoint, it can be
observed that the different frames are used to tackle different aspects associated with Covid-19.
Words in the STORM and in the TSUNAMI frames seem to relate to events and actions asso-
ciated with the arrival and spreading of the pandemic (e.g., “wave”, “storm”, “tide”, “tsunami”,
“disaster”, “tornado”). Words within the MONSTER framing, instead, are mostly nouns and
Table 3. Results of the comparison of the two corpora for five frames and two time spans.
WAR FAMILY STORM MONSTER TSUNAMI Tweets Tot. Cochran Q statistic (df = 4)
W&B 5.32% 12.06% 1.49% 0.68% 1.13% 203,756 Q = 47,226.72
2 weeks p<0.0001
Lamsal 6.94% 8.6% 1.06% 0.70% 1.05% 401,582 Q = 57,159.11
2 weeks p<0.0001
W&B 5.54% 9.95% 1.67% 0.67% 1.25% 654,354 Q = 110,616.87
2 months p<0.0001
Lamsal 6.55% 8.14% 1.24% 0.72% 1.24% 1,213,420 Q = 173,630.43
2 months p<0.0001
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010.t003
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 20 / 24
can be arguably used to frame the discourse about the behavior of the virus, in a rather person-
ified way, which is loaded with emotional content and extremely negative valence (e.g., “devil”,
“demon”, “horror”, “monster”, “killer”). This phenomenon, overall, supports the idea that dif-
ferent frames are apt to elaborate the discourse around different aspects, related to a topic, and
that therefore multiple frames are more likely to enable the effective description and discussion
of different aspects related to the Covid-19 reality.
Finally, the series of replication analyses conducted on new and alternative corpora show
that the results hereby reported are similar across corpora and therefore consistent. Small vari-
ations between our corpora and the resource provided by Lamsal may be due to the keywords
used to construct the datasets. Conversely, differences between the 2-weeks and the 4-weeks
corpora may be due to a change in the discourse, reflecting the natural evolvement of the pan-
demic. In this perspective, future research, which we are currently pursuing, will show in a lon-
gitudinal perspective the development of the different topics and figurative frames used in the
discourse around Covid-19 week after week.
General discussion and conclusion
In this study we explored the discourse around Covid-19 in its manifestation on Twitter. We
addressed three specific research questions: 1. What are the topics around which the Twitter
discourse revolves, in relation to Covid-19; 2. To what extent the WAR framing is used to
model the Covid-19 discourse on Twitter, and specifically in relation to which topics does this
figurative framing emerge; 3. To what extent does the WAR framing compare to other poten-
tially relevant figurative framings related to the discourse on viruses, and to the literal framing
FAMILY.
In general, we found that the topics around which most of the Twitter discourse revolves,
in relation to Covid-19, can be labelled as Communications and Reporting,Community and
Social Compassion;Politics and Reacting to the epidemic. A more fine-grained analysis brings
to light topics related to the treatment of the disease, mentioning people involved in this opera-
tion such as doctors and nurses, and topics related to the diagnostics of the virus. We also
found that these specific topics appear to be those in which the WAR frame is particularly rele-
vant: most lexical units within this frame are found in tweets that get automatically classified
within the specific topics of virus treatment and diagnostics. Moreover, in relation to the sec-
ond research question, we observed that there is a little number of lexical units related to war
that are very frequently used, while the majority of war-related words are not used to frame
the discourse around Covid-19. The more frequently used words refer to actions and events,
such as “fighting”, “fight”, “battle”, and “combat”. As we anticipated, this might be a peculiarity
of the stage of the pandemic we are currently living, which is the peak of the emergency. We
do not exclude that with the development of the pandemic and the passage to the next phase
(i.e., leaving the peak) also the most frequent words used within the WAR frame will change,
to exploit new aspects of this frame that are relevant to the new situation. Finally, in relation to
the third research question, we compared the frequency by which the WAR frame, the FAM-
ILY literal frame and three other figurative frames are used. We found that while the FAMILY
literal frame used as control covers a wider portion of the corpus, among the alternative figura-
tive frames analyzed (MONSTER, STORM and TSUNAMI), the WAR frame is the most fre-
quently used to talk about Covid-19, and thus, arguably, the most conventional one, as
previous literature also suggests.
It should be mentioned that the current study is based on a corpus of tweets (and then repli-
cated on other corpora of tweets) that has been constructed on the basis of precise methodo-
logical criteria. Notably, we dropped retweets from our corpus, and we retained only one tweet
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 21 / 24
per user, to avoid the bias introduced by super-tweeters, that is, users (sometimes bots) who
tweet many times a day and that may have monopolized the sample of tweets used for our
analyses. These two operations, which were motivated by methodological requirements, on
one hand made our corpus probably more robust, balanced and representative for the phe-
nomena to be investigated, but on the other hand neglected these peculiarities of Twitter. As a
matter of fact, Twitter, as a social network typically encompasses retweets (that is, duplicated
tweets, which can be retweeted sometimes thousands of times) and it features super-tweeters.
Additionally, information propagated by super-tweeters, can “go viral” or gain popularity,
measured by likes and retweets. Therefore, a limitation of our study is that our findings may
not reflect the actual distribution of topics and figurative frames on Twitter as a social media
network per se. Rather, we argue, our findings reflect the way in which a wide selection of
American-English speakers conceptualize and talk about Covid-19 on Twitter. In this sense,
the approach adopted in the present study is embedded in common practices used in cognitive
linguistics, discourse analysis and corpus linguistics. We acknowledge the fact that in scientific
fields such as social media monitoring, criteria such as preserving the dynamics that character-
ize specifically the Twitter platform, such as retweets and super-tweeters, are particularly
important. In these fields the construction of the sample of tweets (the corpus) would have
been performed differently, to include retweets and without controlling for super-tweeters.
Taken together our results suggest what has been previously argued in discourse analysis,
that is the relative pervasiveness of the WAR frame in shaping public discourse. In our study
we show that this tendency applies also to the discourse on Covid-19, as previous literature
would have predicted, given the frequent use of this frame in discourses on diseases and
viruses. However, we have also found that this frame is used to talk about specific aspects of
the current epidemic, such as its treatment and diagnostics. Other aspects involved in the epi-
demic are not typically framed within a WAR. This point is particularly important. The WAR
frame, like any other frame, is useful and apt to talk about some aspects of the pandemic, such
as the treatment of the virus and the operations performed by doctors and nurses in hospitals,
but not to talk about other aspects, such as the need to feel our family close to us, while respect-
ing the social distancing measures, or the collaborative efforts that we should undertake in
order to #flattenthecurve, that is, diluting the spreading of the virus over a longer period of
time, so that hospital ICU departments can work efficiently without getting saturated by
incoming patients. In this sense, future studies could focus on the systematic identification of
alternative figurative framings actually used in the Covid-19 discourse to tackle different
aspects of the epidemic, but could also focus on the generation of additional frames, which can
help communities to understand and express aspects of this situation that cannot be expressed
by the WAR frame. A collection of different frames and metaphors that tackle different aspects
of the current situation, or a Metaphor Menu (http://wp.lancs.ac.uk/melc/the-metaphor-
menu/), as Semino and colleagues proposed in relation to cancer discourse [15], is arguably
the most desirable set of communicative tools that, as language, communication, and com-
puter scientists, we shall aim to construct in these current times, as a service to our
communities.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank all doctors, nurses, health-care workers, grocery store workers
and anyone else at the front line of this epidemic.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: Philipp Wicke, Marianna M. Bolognesi.
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 22 / 24
Data curation: Philipp Wicke, Marianna M. Bolognesi.
Formal analysis: Philipp Wicke, Marianna M. Bolognesi.
Investigation: Philipp Wicke, Marianna M. Bolognesi.
Methodology: Philipp Wicke, Marianna M. Bolognesi.
Project administration: Philipp Wicke, Marianna M. Bolognesi.
Resources: Philipp Wicke, Marianna M. Bolognesi.
Software: Philipp Wicke.
Supervision: Marianna M. Bolognesi.
Validation: Marianna M. Bolognesi.
Visualization: Philipp Wicke.
Writing – original draft: Philipp Wicke, Marianna M. Bolognesi.
Writing – review & editing: Philipp Wicke, Marianna M. Bolognesi.
References
1. Flusberg SJ, Matlock T, Thibodeau PH. War metaphors in public discourse. Metaphor and Symbol
2018 33.1. 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2018.1407992
2. Culotta A. Towards Detecting Influenza Epidemics by Analyzing Twitter Messages. Proceedings of the
First Workshop on Social Media Analytics. SOMA’10. New York, NY, USA: ACM; 2010. p. 115–122.
3. Gomide J, Veloso A, Meira W Jr, Almeida V, Benevenuto F, Ferraz F, et al. Dengue Surveillance Based
on a Computational Model of Spatio-temporal Locality of Twitter. Proceedings of the 3rd International
Web Science Conference. WebSci’11. New York, NY, USA: ACM; 2011. p. 3:1–3:8.
4. Pruss D, Fujinuma Y, Daughton AR, Paul MJ, Arnot B, Szafir DA, et al. Zika discourse in the Americas:
A multilingual topic analysis of Twitter. PloS one, 2019 14(5). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.
0216922 PMID: 31120935
5. Paul MJ, Dredze M, Broniatowski D. Twitter Improves Influenza Forecasting. PLoS Currents Outbreaks.
2014. https://doi.org/10.1371/currents.outbreaks.90b9ed0f59bae4ccaa683a39865d9117 PMID:
25642377
6. Santillana M, Nguyen AT, Dredze M, Paul MJ, Nsoesie EO, Brownstein JS. Combining Search, Social
Media, and Traditional Data Sources to Improve Influenza Surveillance. PLOS Comput Biol. 2015; 11
(10):e1004513. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004513 PMID: 26513245
7. Ji X, Chun SA, Geller J. Monitoring public health concerns using Twitter sentiment classifications. IEEE
International Conference on Healthcare Informatics; 2013. pp. 335–344.
8. Smith M, Broniatowski DA, Paul MJ, Dredze M. Towards Real-Time Measurement of Public Epidemic
Awareness: Monitoring Influenza Awareness through Twitter. AAAI Spring Symposium on Observa-
tional Studies through Social Media and Other Human-Generated Content; 2016.
9. Mollema L, Harmsen IA, Broekhuizen E, Clijnk R, De Melker H, Paulussen T, et al. Disease detection or
public opinion reflection? Content analysis of tweets, other social media, and online newspapers during
the measles outbreak in the Netherlands in 2013. Journal of Medical Internet Research. 2015; 17(5):
e128. https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.3863 PMID: 26013683
10. Lazard AJ, Scheinfeld E, Bernhardt JM, Wilcox GB, Suran M. Detecting themes of public concern: A
text mining analysis of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention’s Ebola live Twitter chat. Ameri-
can Journal of Infection Control. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajic.2015.05.025 PMID: 26138998
11. Tran T, Lee K. Understanding citizen reactions and Ebola-related information propagation on social
media. IEEE/ACM International Conference on Advances in Social Networks Analysis and Mining
(ASONAM). 2016. pp. 106–111.
12. Miller M, Banerjee T, Muppalla R, Romine W, Sheth A. What Are People Tweeting About Zika? An
Exploratory Study Concerning Its Symptoms, Treatment, Transmission, and Prevention. JMIR public
health and surveillance. 2017; 3(2):e38. https://doi.org/10.2196/publichealth.7157 PMID: 28630032
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 23 / 24
13. Vijaykumar S, Nowak G, Himelboim I, Jin Y. Virtual Zika transmission after the first U.S. case: who said
what and how it spread on Twitter. American Journal of Infection Control. 2018; https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.ajic.2017.10.015 PMID: 29306490
14. Entman R. M. (1993). Framing: Toward clarification of a fractured paradigm. Journal of Communication,
43(4), 51–58. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1993.tb01304.x
15. Sontag S. Illness as Metaphor. Allen Lane, London. 1979.
16. Semino E, Demje
´n Z, Demmen J, Koller V, Payne S, Hardie A, et al. The online use of Violence and
Journey metaphors by patients with cancer, as compared with health professionals: a mixed methods
study. BMJ supportive & palliative care 2017 7.1: 60–66. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjspcare-2014-
000785 PMID: 25743439
17. Hendricks R. K., Demje
´n Z., Semino E. and Boroditsky L. Emotional implications of metaphor: Conse-
quences of metaphor framing for mindset about cancer, Metaphor and Symbol, 2018 33:4. 267–279.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2018.1549835
18. Karlberg M, Buell L. Deconstructing the ‘war of all against all’: The prevalence and implications of war
metaphors and other adversarial news schema in TIME, Newsweek, and Maclean’s. Journal of Peace
and Conflict Studies 2005. 12.1: 22–39.
19. Dodge EK, Hong J, Stickles E. MetaNet: Deep semantic automatic metaphor analysis. Proceedings of
the Third Workshop on Metaphor in NLP. 2015.
20. Wise A. Scientific American. 2020. https://blogs.scientificamerican.com/observations/military-
metaphors-distort-the-reality-of-covid-19/
21. Henderson K. Counterpunch. 2020. https://www.counterpunch.org/2020/04/24/trump-is-not-a-wartime-
president-and-covid-19-is-not-a-war/
22. Tisdall S. The Guardian. 2020 https://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2020/mar/21/donald-
trump-boris-johnson-coronavirus
23. Musu C. The Conversation. 2020. https://theconversation.com/war-metaphors-used-for-covid-19-are-
compelling-but-also-dangerous-135406
24. Lakoff G. 1996 Lakoff, G. Moral Politics: How Liberals and Conservatives Think. 1996
25. Lamsal R. Coronavirus (COVID-19) Tweets Dataset. IEEE Dataport. 2020. https://doi.org/10.21227/
781w-ef42
26. Grinberg N, Joseph K, Friedland L, Swire-Thompson B, Lazer D. Fake news on Twitter during the 2016
US presidential election. Science, 2019. 363(6425), 374–378. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.
aau2706 PMID: 30679368
27. Williams ML, Burnap P, Sloan L. Towards an ethical framework for publishing Twitter data in social
research: Taking into account users’ views, online context and algorithmic estimation. Sociology, 2017.
51(6), 1149–1168. https://doi.org/10.1177/0038038517708140 PMID: 29276313
28. Joris W, d’Haenens L, Van Gorp B. The euro crisis in metaphors and frames: Focus on the press in the
Low Countries. European Journal of Communication. 2014. 29(5), 608–617.
29. Blei DM, Ng AY, Jordan MI. Latent dirichlet allocation. Journal of machine Learning research, 2003 3
(Jan), 993–1022.
30. Stone B, Dennis S, Kwantes PJ. Comparing methods for single paragraph similarity analysis. Topics in
Cognitive Science 2011. 3(1), 92–122. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01108.x PMID:
25164176
31. Sievert C, Shirley K. LDAvis: A method for visualizing and interpreting topics. In Proceedings of the
workshop on interactive language learning, visualization, and interfaces. 2014. pp. 63–70.
32. Le Q, Mikolov T. Distributed representations of sentences and documents. In International conference
on machine learning 2014 (pp. 1188–1196).
33. Liu H, Singh P. ConceptNet—a practical commonsense reasoning tool-kit. BT technology journal, 2004
22(4), 211–226.
34. Choi WG, Lee KS. Conceptual Representation for Crisis-Related Tweet Classification. Computacio
´n y
Sistemas. 2019 23(4).
35. Chang J, Gerrish S, Wang C, Boyd-Graber JL, Blei DM. Reading tea leaves: How humans interpret
topic models. In Advances in neural information processing systems. 2009. pp. 288–296.
36. Veale T. A service-oriented architecture for metaphor processing. Proceedings of the Second Work-
shop on Metaphor in NLP. 2014. pp. 52–60.
37. Steen GJ, Dorst AG, Herrmann JB, Kaal AA, Krennmayr T, Pasma T. A method for linguistic metaphor
identification. From MIP to MIPVU. 2010. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
PLOS ONE
Framing COVID-19
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240010 September 30, 2020 24 / 24
Content uploaded by Marianna Bolognesi
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Marianna Bolognesi on Oct 01, 2020
Content may be subject to copyright.